Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 3

Commentaries 14!

Cognitive benefits of nicotine: fact or performance of habitual users who are given
fiction? nicotine with the performance of non-users who
Geoi^e J. Spilich are not given nicotine. Oddly enou^, many
researchers who investigate the effects of smok-
ing upon cognitive performance still insist on
Newton tells us that a body in motion tends to comparing the performance of active smokers
remain in motion unless acted upon by some only with that of deprived smokers. In a recent
outside force. Ideas also enjoy inertia; opinions issue of Psychopkarmacology devoted to nicotine
when stated frequently and with an air of self-as- research (1992, 108, No. 4), five papers dis-
surance tend to go unchallenged and even resist cussed the effects of smokir^ upon cognitive
the effects of disconfirming evidence. performance but only Prichard, Robinson &
West (1993) challenges addiction researchers Guy, (1992) and Sherwood, Kerr & Hindmarch,
to question critically the dogma that nicotine (1992) compared the performance of smokers
generally facilitates cognitive performance. He with a non-smoking control group. The ex-
notes that while smokers tend to report that clusion of a contrast between the performance of
smoking reduces stress and aids their cognitive smokers and non-smokers might be warranted if
performance, the data do not unanimously sup- the focus of the article was to investigate the
port such beliefs. How can experts in the fields of effects of smoking or nicotine deprivation upon
addiction medicine, psychopharmacology and the performance of smokers. Although Sherwood
cognitive neuroscience all investigate the eEfert of et at. (1992) and Prichard et al. (1992) use the
nicotine upon cognitive performance and yet ar- word 'smoker' in the title of their article and so
rive at different conclusions? imply that their conclusions refer only to the
One answer lies in the variance in methods effects of smoking upon smokers' behavior, oth-
used in research which investigates the effects of ers are not so careful in either drawing their own
nicotine upon performance. Some of this vari- conclusions or in interpreting the work of others.
ance in methodology is common to all drug Comparing the cognitive performance of smok-
research; differences in drug dosage, method of ers with deprived smokers allows us to discuss
administration, subject selection, and other dis- deprivation effects but does not allow us to gen-
parities in design and procedure lead to different eralize unblushingly that "Tvlicotine can improve
patterns of results. Differences in results associ- memory by enhancing attention." (Warburton,
ated with this type of procedural variance Rusted & Fowler, 1992, p. 446). To clearly
provide important information about the separate deprivation effects from drug effects,
'boundary conditions' of phenomena. researchers must compare the performance of
active smokers with non-smokers; an even better
Nicotine research seems to have additional model would be to compare the performance of
and perhaps unique procedural sources of vari- active smokers, deprived smokers and non-
ance, and I hope that by highlighting the most smokers (Spilich, June & Renner, 1992).
troublesome of these errors in procedure, I can
help to clarify the muddy waters of discussion.
The most serious and yet easily avoidable of
these methodological errors are: (1) failing to Restricting the range of tasks
distinguish deprivation effects from drug effects; One of the essential characteristics of a depen-
and (2) over-generalization from a restricted dent variable is that it be valid; that is, it
range of performance tasks to global cognitive measures what it is supposed to measure. Within
effects. this context, it is important for addiction re-
searchers to choose measures of mental function
which reflect the type of processing encountered
Deprivation or drug effects? in everyday tasks.
Many addiction researchers are interested in the Much of the research which investigates the
effects of a specific drug upon mental processes; effects of nicotine upon cognitive performance
simply put, does the use of a particular drug has been condurted with a very restrictive set of
affect our mental performance in any regular tasks such as the Mackworth clock, the RVIP
way? In order to evaluate the chronic effects of task, finger tapping tasks, all of which involve a
nicotine upon cognition, one must compare the rapid response to a simple perceptual demand.
142 Commentaries

Consider for a moment the Mackworth clock. In ing on memory consolidation, Psychopharmacohgyt
the typical appiication of this task, a subject 108, pp. 448-451.
PRICHARD, W . S., RoBmsoN, J. H. & GUY, T . 0 .
stares for more than an hour at a clock face (1992) Enhancement of continuous performance
without numbers, watching for the second hand task reaction time by smoking in non-deprived
to occasionaJly stuner ahead two 'seconds' rather smokers, Psydiopharmaccdosy, 108, pp. 437-442.
than 'one'. Psychopharmacologists who view this SHERWOOD, N . KERR, J. S. & HINDMARCH, I. (1992)
Psychomotor performance in smokers following
task as a model of the cognitive demands of single and repeated doses of nicotine gum,
every day life must lead an excruciatingly tedious P^chopharmacohgy, 308, pp. 432—436.
existence. SFIUCH, G . J., JUNE, L. & RENNER, J. (1992) Cigarette
I am not arguing that one type of task is smoking and cognitive performance, British Journal
oj Addiction, 87, pp. 1313-1326.
intrinsically better than another nor that every
US DHEW (1964) Smoking and health, Repon of the
study should employ a battery of tasks, but gen- advisory committee to the Surgeon General of the
eralizing about the effea of a drug upon Public Health Service (Washington, DC, US
something as broad as cognitive performance Government Printing Office).
from the results of only one task or one type of US DHHS (J984) Drug abuse and drug abuse research,
DHHS Publication No. (ADM) 85-1374 (Washing-
task is a recipe for disaster. Tlieorists in behav- ton, DC US Government Printing Office).
ioral pharmacology need to base their WARBURTON, D . (1992) Nicotine issues, Psychophar-
generalizations about the cognitive effects of a macology, 108, pp. 393-396.
drug upon a wide range of tasks which differ in WAREURTON, D . M . , RUSTED, J. M. & FO-BTJJR, J.
task demands and complexity. (1992) A comparison of the anentiona! and consoli-
dation hypotheses for the facilitation of memory by
nicotine, Psychopharmacology, 108, pp. 443-447.
WEST, R. (1993) Beneficial effects of nicotine: fact or
Conclusions fiction? Addiction, 88, pp. 589-590.
To the outsider, this discussion concerning
proper methodology for assessing the effects of
smoking and nicotine upon human cognitive Does cigarette smoking increase stress?
performance might appear to be an esoteric dis- Andy Parrott
pute among scientists. In fact, the implications of
methodological errors which lead researchers to One of the issues raised in West's (1993) edi-
conclude incorrectly that smoking benefits cog- torial concerns the role of stress in smoking.
nitive performance in some general fashion are The relationship between cigarette smoking
both far-reaching and costly. For example, the and feelings of stress has long been a subject for
1964 US Surgeon General's Report (US controversy. This relates to the cotnplex mood
DHEW, 1964), the 1978 WHO report (WHO, state changes which accompany both cigarette
Technical Report Series, No. 618, 1978, p. 8) as smoking, and periods without cigarettes. The act
well as the 1984 NIDA repon (US DHHS, of smoking is generally associated with positive
1984, pp. 19-20) concur that disturbances in moods; "Individuals report that they smoke be-
cognitive performance are a hallmark of an ad- cause it improves their affective state, reducing
dictive substance. If the effects of smoking upon negative and/or increasing positive affect"
human cognitive performance are not clearly (Gilbert & Wesler, 1989, p. 188). Whereas vrith-
understood due to a narrow and confined man- out access to cigarettes, regular smokers often
ner in which research is conducted, our experience a range of negative moods, including
knowledge of addictive processes will suffer. irritability, tension, and nervousness. These dele-
terious feeling state changes have been widely
GEORGE J. SPUJCH
documented dtiring long-term smoking cessation
Psychology Department,
(Hughes et al., 1990), but it has become increas-
Dunning Science Center,
ingly apparent that they also occur quite rapidly
Washington College,
after cigarette extinction. Thus Perkins et al.
300 Washington Avenue,
(1992) noted that the positive mood effects of
Chestertovm, Maryland 21620, USA.
smoking had disappeared 10 minutes after the
last inhalation, in subjects performing a high
COLRAIN, I. M., MANGAN, G . L., PEIXET, O . L. & challenge (high stress) task.
BATES, T. C. (1992) Effects of post-learning smok- Various models have been proposed for the

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi