Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 17

LSHSS

Tutorial

Laying a Firm Foundation: Embedding


Evidence-Based Emergent Literacy
Practices Into Early Intervention
and Preschool Environments
Pamela Terrella and Maggie Watsona

Purpose: As part of this clinical forum on curriculum-based are discussed. These include phonemic awareness, print /
intervention, the goal of this tutorial is to share research alphabet awareness, oral language skills, and embedded /
about the importance of language and literacy foundations explicit literacy.
in natural environments during emergent literacy skill Results: Research indicates that rich home literacy
development, from infancy through preschool. Following environments and exposure to rich oral language provide
an overview of intervention models in schools by Powell an important foundation for the more structured literacy
(2018), best practices at home, in child care, and in preschool environments of school. Furthermore, there is a wealth
settings are discussed. Speech-language pathologists in of evidence to support a variety of direct and indirect
these settings will be provided a toolbox of best emergent intervention practices in the home, child care, and preschool
literacy practices. contexts to support and enhance all aspects of oral and
Method: A review of published literature in speech-language written literacy.
pathology, early intervention, early childhood education, and Conclusions: Application of this “toolbox” of strategies
literacy was completed. Subsequently, an overview of the should enable speech-language pathologists to address
impact of early home and preschool literacy experiences are the prevention and intervention of literacy deficits within
described. Research-based implementation of best practice multiple environments during book and play activities.
is supported with examples of shared book reading and Additionally, clinicians will have techniques to share with
child-led literacy embedded in play within the coaching parents, child care providers, and preschool teachers for
model of early intervention. Finally, various aspects of evidence-based literacy instruction within all settings during
emergent literacy skill development in the preschool years typical daily activities.

“speech-to-print” continuum of development, acknowledging

T
he purpose of this tutorial is to help speech-language
pathologists (SLPs) develop a “toolbox” of tech- the foundational skills of oral language to written language.
niques, strategies, and activities designed to help The information in this tutorial focuses on the development
children develop emergent and early literacy skills. As of emergent literacy (EL) skills, such as print awareness
Powell (2018) noted in the lead article of this clinical forum, a and phonemic awareness (PA), alongside the development
great deal of research has demonstrated strong links between of pretend play, and oral language skills, such as vocabu-
early oral language skills and subsequent literacy achieve- lary, narration, and conversational skills.
ments. These connections have been referred to as the This tutorial is organized to first provide the theoret-
ical base for the connection between oral and written lan-
guage, as well as how to enhance development in both.
a
School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Next, we will describe how oral and written literacy skills
Wisconsin–Stevens Point can be targeted in tandem across environments, including
Correspondence to Pamela Terrell: pterrell@uwsp.edu home, child care settings, and preschools. Then, we will
Editor-in-Chief: Shelley Gray present information regarding the multitude of linguistic
Editor: Ashley Meaux skills that can be developed while reading with children,
Received June 12, 2017 followed by information on how to develop critical skills,
Revision received August 19, 2017 such as print and PA with examples and explanations
Accepted October 24, 2017
https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_LSHSS-17-0053
Publisher Note: This article is part of the Clinical Forum: Exploring Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the time
Curriculum-Based Language Assessment and Interventions. of publication.

148 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 148–164 • April 2018 • Copyright © 2018 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association

Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
offered in Appendixes A and B. Finally, a timely review the pages, look at the pictures and, perhaps, act upon the
of the impact of electronic media on oral language and book by petting a picture of an animal. However, as a care-
EL skills of infants, toddlers, and preschoolers will be dis- giver models pointing to a picture and saying the name of
cussed. Throughout, specific suggestions and examples will the object (e.g., ball and cow), the same child might point to
be presented so that clinicians can create their own toolbox pictures, label pictures, vocalize appropriate sounds (such
designed to assist children’s development of critical language as animal noises), and answer simple questions with the
and early literacy skills across a variety of settings. adult mediating the literacy experience. If the caregiver ex-
ceeds the child’s ZPD and asks more abstract and linguisti-
cally complex questions (e.g., “Why do you think the boy
EL and Oral Language is weeping?”), the child may become frustrated. Conversely,
Saracho (2017) defined EL as “literacy-like” behaviors if the caregiver stays below the child’s ZPD level and does
that are acquired prior to formal instruction in reading and not provide enough scaffolding (e.g., turns pages with no
writing. Some of those behaviors are developed during the linguistic input), the child may become bored and inatten-
first 3 years of life, such as book handling, labeling pictures, tive. The ZPD is ever-changing as the child learns and
and listening to stories. As children age, more advanced develops new skills and interests, so caregivers must be
EL skills emerge. These emergent behaviors simulate actual closely attuned to the child’s disposition, attention, and
reading and writing activities, such as pretending to read, engagement. Scaffolded instruction within the child’s ZPD
scribbling a “note” as part of play, and connecting stories has been shown to occur in mother–child pairings as mothers
to real-life events. It is important to remember that, during labeled pictures following a child’s point, expanded a child’s
this time, children are still advancing oral language skills verbalization, asked questions, tracked print with a finger,
that are also essential for the eventual development of read- pointed out print conventions, and recruited attention to
ing and writing. Children incorporate new vocabulary in text, pictures, or story grammar (Bus & van Ijzendoorn, 1988,
their own speech and begin to use complex morphological 1997; Sénéchal, Cornell, & Broda, 1995). This type of in-
and syntactic structures. Developmentally, both EL and teraction influences both oral and written language skills.
oral language skills progress simultaneously and typically As caregivers learn to work within a child’s ZPD
within social environments. Saracho (2017) further stated, when engaging with print, the child may develop increased
“…emergent literacy shows the way written literacy abilities understanding about EL. This incorporates another impor-
surface progressively in the framework of oral language tant aspect of Vygotsky’s theory, exposing children to
development” (p. 306). Just as being exposed to “language- slightly more complex information and concepts. For exam-
rich environments” is important for the development of ple, as adults track print, they might verbalize “that’s a long
oral language abilities, literacy-rich environments provide word,” which provides more advanced information about
children with opportunities to learn about, interact with, the connection between print and words. This may help chil-
and experiment with print. dren understand that sentences are composed of words and
that words are separate entities as compared with individual
letters or sentences. Ultimately, through mediated instruc-
Connecting Theory With Practice tion, the child discovers that words are formed with letters
One of the most powerful tools an educator can have on the basis of auditory speech sounds. While this is occur-
is the ability to understand theories associated with language ring, the child’s understanding changes from the initial
acquisition along with the ability to translate theory to adult-directed social process to internalizing the concepts
practice. This implies knowing the continuum of development for himself (Vygotsky, 1986). Scaffolding can occur across
for both oral and written language, as well as how those activities, including shared storybook reading, pretend
linguistic domains interact over time. SLPs will need strate- play, and daily activities, such as grocery shopping and
gies and techniques to help caregivers apply theoretical con- following recipes while cooking.
structs in their daily interactions with children, as well as Snow, Tabors, and Dickinson (2001) discussed the
when providing direct intervention services. importance of early language skills that “replicate the
Vygotsky’s theories have been embraced by the educa- demands of literacy” and labeled those as “extended dis-
tional community and form a foundation for many peda- course.” Examples of extended discourse include narra-
gogical beliefs and current practices, which are especially tives, explanations, and dialogue that accompany pretend
relevant for the development of emergent and early literacy play. This type of discourse offers children the opportunity
skills. One of the key constructs of Vygotsky’s theories is the to learn new vocabulary words and use language in a non-
notion of the zone of proximal development (ZPD). ZPD literal manner and for a variety of social functions, such
may be described as the gap between a child’s actual devel- as explaining, problem solving, perspective taking, and
opmental level when completing a task independently and negotiating. Katz (2001) specifically noted the importance
the level of potential development with advanced guidance of preschool children interacting with adults while playing
from adults. The ZPD can vary depending on the kind of pretend and developing the language skills necessary to
task, the type of instruction given, and the child’s current participate in extended dialogues and convey meaning that
developmental level in that area. (Vygotsky, 1986). For goes beyond the immediate context, stating “it is this ability
example, an 18-month-old given a board book might turn to build and understand larger language structures that

Terrell & Watson: Laying a Firm Foundation 149


Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018
Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
contributes to children’s later literacy” (p. 68). Katz (2001) learn about picture and symbol representation. Shared
reported that the frequency of using and being exposed to reading also exposes them to print and print concepts,
“pretend talk” (e.g., enacting roles and scripts and describ- such as directionality in a decontextualized format (Bus
ing how to use objects symbolically) by 3- and 4-year-olds & van Ijzendoorn, 1988, 1997; Lawhon & Cobb, 2002;
positively correlated with language and literacy skills when Sénéchal et al., 1995). In their study of infants’ and pre-
they were in kindergarten. schoolers’ EL experiences, Bus and van Ijzendoorn (1988)
Numerous research studies have shown a strong con- found that most mothers indicated that they did not pro-
nection between early literacy attainments with subsequent vide reading instruction to their children during storybook
academic achievements (Bodrova, Leong, & Shore, 2004; activities. However, the results of their investigation observ-
Invernizzi, Landrum, Teichman, & Townsend, 2010; ing middle-class mothers and their children (three groups
National Early Literacy Panel [NELP], 2008). As Powell of 1.5-, 3.5-, and 5.5-year-olds) engaged in shared reading
(2018) noted in the previous article of this forum, the skills demonstrated that the mothers were naming and talking
of phonological awareness, vocabulary, and comprehen- about letters and drawing connections between oral words
sion are among the five key components of effective read- and their alphabetic representation without realizing it.
ing instruction. Children with good EL exposure and skills Mothers tended to follow the child’s lead if the child showed
enter kindergarten primed to begin explicit reading educa- some interest in letters and reading.
tion. Authors of a report from the NELP (2008) reviewed A literate home environment begins with the presence
over 500 research articles to determine those EL and oral of easily accessible reading and writing materials in the
language skills critical for reading development. The emergent home and is shaped by the child’s experiences with print
skills that best predicted subsequent literacy development materials, family attitudes about literacy, and adult modeling
included alphabet knowledge (both name and letter–sound of reading and writing activities (Bus & van Ijzendoorn,
recognition), phonological awareness (the ability to analyze 1988; Lawhon & Cobb, 2002; Roberts, Jurgens, & Burchinal,
spoken language into smaller components), rapid automatic 2005; Sénéchal, LeFevre, Thomas, & Daley, 1998). Activities
naming of letters/digits and objects/colors, writing letters/ that develop an awareness of rhyme, prosody, and rhythm,
own name, and remembering verbal information (phono- such as nursery rhymes, songs, and fingerplays help build
logical memory). Other variables cited in the NELP report a foundation for reading as well (Lawhon & Cobb, 2002).
as important were concepts about print, print awareness, Investigations of literacy environments have been conducted
and early word-decoding skills. Expressive language skills using a variety of checklists and criterion-based instruments.
were also found to play a role in predicting literacy skills, They were designed to examine the amount and type of
especially those involving more complexity or composite books in the home, the child’s access to books and writing/
measures of oral language that included grammar, word art implements, other literacy artifacts (e.g., recipes, receipts,
definitions, and listening comprehension (NELP, 2008). and mail), and family reading behaviors and routines and
Thus, an important addition to the SLPs’ literacy community literacy experiences, such as the library and story
toolbox is an understanding of the strong relationship time (Caldwell & Bradley, 2003; Hanna, Hinrichs, Mahar,
between oral language attainments with subsequent liter- DeFrain, & Durden, 2010). Research has indicated that a
acy skills (Dickinson & Porche, 2011; NELP, 2008; Pelatti, home that is rich with literacy experiences and adult models
Piasta, Justice, & O’Connell, 2014). This information is with many types of reading materials readily accessible to
necessary when interacting with young children to help cir- young children positively influences the EL skills of young
cumvent potential oral and written language delays. That children and impacts school-age literacy skills (Edwards, 2012;
is, both written and oral language skill development should Pinto, Pessanha, & Aguiar, 2013; Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2014).
be addressed when working with young children through Children living in poverty are especially vulnerable
activities that provide social interaction and opportunities to not receiving early literacy experiences and form the
for adult-mediated instruction. largest group of children demonstrating delayed literacy
development (Tichnor-Wagner, Garwood, Bratsch-Hines,
& Vernon-Feagans, 2015). Thus, they are often not ready
EL Development in Various Environments for the literacy instruction offered within preschools and
(Birth to 5 Years) child care centers. Children living in poverty had less expo-
sure to books in their homes and were often not exposed
Home Literacy Environment to public libraries as compared with children from higher
The first experiences with print and early reading socioeconomic groups (Baker, 2003; Farver, Xu, Lonigan,
activities typically occur in the home within parent–child & Eppe, 2013; Slates, Alexander, Entwisle, & Olson, 2012).
interactions (Edwards, 2012). Early exposures to letters Adults may not readily expose their children to literacy due
and print happen through picture and story books, children’s to a lack of education and literacy skills (Boyce, Innocenti,
television programming (Bus & van Ijzendoorn, 1988), and Roggman, Norman, & Ortiz, 2010; Li & Christ, 2007).
other electronic media (e.g., smart phones and tablets), as Farver et al. (2013) further stated that children from low-
well as through language play in the form of songs and income families and those whose first language is not
nursery rhymes. Shared picture book reading is a corner- English face “considerable challenges” in acquiring read-
stone of EL because, during those interactions, young children ing skills.

150 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 148–164 • April 2018

Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Thus, additional needs for the SLP’s toolbox include space and materials for literary activities than those staffed
strategies and techniques to help prevent the effects of by teachers with high school degrees and a larger adult–
limited home literacy experiences (Appendix A). Tichnor- child ratio (2:12–16). Pinto et al. (2013) found similar
Wagner et al. (2015) provided suggestions to help parents results when examining child care centers in Portugal. Using
bolster their ability to increase and improve their children’s a longitudinal design, the researchers rated Portuguese
EL experiences. These included advocating for programs child care centers on factors, including room arrangement
that provide literacy resources and educational programs and furnishings, listening and talking, program format,
for parents, including direct instruction through classes, and interactions. Pinto et al. (2013) found that “child care
providing books for the home, and increasing access to classroom quality” was associated with children’s develop-
educational digital resources. These programs require fund- ment of language and literacy skills.
ing and personnel devoted to improving the EL experiences Child-led literacy experiences have also been shown
of young children along with educational opportunities for to be beneficial for EL skills. In focus group interviews
their caregivers. However, SLPs can help parents make with preschool (birth to age 3 years) teachers in Sweden,
some simple adjustments to take advantage of environmental teachers did not feel like they were engaging in specific
print that exists in most homes. This can include artifacts, literacy instruction (Hvit, 2014). However, as the teachers
such as food packaging, mail, and the textbooks /homework began to describe activities and routines within the day,
of older siblings. Yet, another avenue to help improve young it became apparent that they were “doing literacy” frequently,
children’s early literacy experiences is through interactions with less explicit literacy teaching. These teachers were
with older siblings. Older siblings who have developed some following the lead of the children and embedding early
reading skill can be taught or encouraged to engage in shared literacy experiences throughout the day. For example, when
reading and other literacy-focused activities (e.g., playing a young boy was drawing circles with a stick in a sandbox,
school) with the younger children in the home. This may his teacher commented how he made an “O” like the first
be especially useful for families whose primary caregivers letter in his name. During shared book reading, children
do not speak the language of instruction (Kibler, Palacios, were encouraged to dramatize and retell stories, using photos
Simpson Baird, Bergey, & Yoder, 2016). of their play as visual support. Additionally, these toddlers
Heath (1983) urged educators to capitalize on were encouraged to “read” books to dolls and each other
the “authentic literacy experiences” that may be occur- and to write (scribble) their names. While the teachers
ring in the homes of children not living in mainstream thought they were lagging in literacy instruction, the oppo-
society. For example, Boyce et al. (2010) developed a site was true. Literacy experiences were playful, sponta-
“strengths-based” program for increasing literacy skills neous, hands-on, and child-led within child care routines
of Spanish-speaking migrant preschoolers. The authors (Hvit, 2014). However, this study underscores the need
helped Spanish-speaking migrant families create oral nar- for child care and preschool teachers to have explicit in-
ratives about everyday events and, then, turned those into struction in EL development and strategies to facilitate
books the families could use repeatedly. The books were language and literacy in group and classroom settings
relevant, functional, and culturally appropriate. Parents because the teachers were unaware that literacy experiences
were given strategies to use while sharing the books with could be serendipitous and occur during play and in simple
their children. Mothers improved quality of language and narratives.
literacy support compared with the control group; children Pretend play centers in classrooms (e.g., home living,
increased the number of words and different words from grocery store, and office) can provide embedded literacy learn-
pretest to posttest. On the other hand, some families may ing opportunities, thus bridging the gap between home and
have a strong cultural storytelling heritage, and they should school reading exposure (Lawhon & Cobb, 2002; Whitehurst
be encouraged to continue that tradition. Their young & Lonigan, 1998). A study by Neuman and Roskos (1993)
children may be exposed to descriptive language and story found that children in Head Start classrooms equipped with
grammar concepts through listening to stories. an office play center containing print artifacts, such as calen-
dars, phone books, and writing materials, scored higher
than control classrooms on environmental print tasks. When
Child Care and Early Preschool Literacy Environments a trained adult volunteer modeled and scaffolded the chil-
According to the U.S. Census Bureau (2013) results, dren’s play by referencing print (e.g., “Let me look up that
61% of American children ages birth to 4 years were cared price.”), the children performed even higher on environmental
for by someone other than their parents while the parents print tasks than children in the rooms with office centers
worked or were involved in other activities or obligations. and no adult volunteer. These types of creative learning
Twenty-seven percent were in the care of a relative, whereas experiences can foster cohesion between the home and pre-
25% attended organized child care facilities. Because approx- school literacy environments. In a separate study, Neuman
imately one out of every four children attends a child care and Roskos (1997) took the embedded preschool literacy
center, the literacy environments of those centers also bear concept a bit further. This time, they created a post office,
consideration. For example, Norris (2017) found that a restaurant, and a doctor’s office setting within Early Start
child care centers staffed with teachers who had bachelor classrooms and provided related literacy tools, such as
degrees and low adult–child ratios (2:8) also had more stamps, envelopes, menus, receipts, eye charts, pill bottles,

Terrell & Watson: Laying a Firm Foundation 151


Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018
Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
credit cards, and so forth. Students also went on a field trip comments and questions, and follows the child’s utterances
to a post office, and teachers modeled how to play “post with feedback, such as praise, expansion, or repetition
office” in the classroom. Gradually, the teachers faded out (Hargrave & Sénéchal, 2000; Institute of Educational
of the play to become observers. The 3- and 4-year-old pre- Sciences, 2015; Whitehurst et al., 1994). Shared book read-
schoolers learned literacy procedures (e.g., writing and ing provides a natural environment for literacy exposure,
mailing a letter), names and purposes of literacy materials and as children snuggle in the lap or sit beside the adult
(e.g., stamp, envelope, and menu), literacy routines (e.g., reader, positive emotions may also be associated with the
ordering from a menu while server writes down information), experience.
and print knowledge (e.g., open/closed sign on restaurant) In 12 mother–child or father–child dyads from White
all within natural pretend play (Neuman & Roskos, 1997). middle-class to upper-class families in Canada, Sénéchal
Although most child care centers rarely have a formal literacy et al. (1995) found significant increases in the amount of
curriculum for children younger than age 3 years, they do parent questioning and the number of children’s vocaliza-
often have dramatic play centers. Thus, adding a few literacy tions as the ages of the children increased. Across all age
items to each play center and showing teachers and para- groups (9–27-month-olds), parents were consistently percep-
professionals how to model and scaffold would be an easy, tive and responsive to a child’s gaze. Parents used pointing
proactive way of including literacy into play. to follow and maintain a child’s attention to a particular
page or picture and incorporated attention-recruiting com-
ments (e.g., “See the ball?”) and elaborations (e.g., “That’s
Shared Book Reading her favorite toy.”) with infants and more questions (e.g.,
In the most general sense, shared book reading in this “What’s he doing?”) and feedback (e.g., “Yes! That is a
tutorial refers to any time an adult and young child share big dog!”) with older toddlers (Sénéchal et al., 1995).
a book experience together, typically with the adult reading Edwards (2012) found mothers (middle to upper socio-
to the child or talking about the book. During shared book economic status; M = 19 years of education) tended to
reading experiences, infants and toddlers begin to progress reference written language skills, such as story grammar
through preliterate developmental stages (see Table 1). Inter- and book conventions (and, to a lesser extent, print), but
active book reading is a specific type of shared book reading not phonological awareness, while reading to their toddler/
in which an adult uses techniques to engage a small child preschool children (18–34 months). Other research has
(or children) in the text (Institute of Educational Sciences, indicated that most adult readers do not naturally incorporate
2015), and it is a hallmark of early literacy experiences in print referencing (PR) nor do young children spontaneously
terms of frequency and benefit. Interactive book reading attend to it (Justice & Ezell, 2000; Justice, Kaderavek, Fan,
may include strategies, such as pointing to pictures, asking Sofka, & Hunt, 2009). Therefore, parents, caregivers, and
questions, acting upon the pictures (e.g., petting the kitty), teachers could benefit from some education regarding the
labeling pictures, and making associated sounds (e.g., importance of early phonological awareness (e.g., rhyming)
“meow”). Another category of shared book reading is dia- and PR (e.g., following print with finger).
logic reading in which the adult reads to the child and asks Shared story book reading also benefits preschoolers,
open-ended questions, is highly responsive to the child’s both in a caregiver–child dyad at home and in small
groups within the classroom and warrants consideration
as part of a child care/early childhood (EC) center curricu-
Table 1. Preliterate development of book handling and interaction. lum. Dialogic reading in particular, which includes asking
wh-questions, following the child’s lead, repeating and
Approximate age Preliterate book skill expanding child comments, and praising the child, is effec-
tive in increasing vocabulary (Hargrave & Sénéchal, 2000;
Emerges younger than Book handling
Whitehurst et al., 1994) and increasing frequency and
12 months • Mouthing books
• Patting or grabbing pictures length of book-related conversations (Milburn, Girolametto,
• Holding books Weitzman, & Greenberg, 2013). These benefits were noted
• Turning pages with 3- to 5-year-old children who engaged in dialogic book
Emerges 12–18 months Verbal and nonverbal referencing reading at home and at school, but strongest effects were
• Making sounds and gestures
in response to pictures observed with a combined home and school approach or
• Responding to adult comments/ home only (Milburn et al., 2013). Blewitt, Rump, Shealy,
questions by pointing, making and Cook (2009) found that the most effective way to
sounds, or labeling incorporate dialogic reading with preschoolers was to
Emerges 12+ months Behavioral
• Increased attention to books start with low demand questions (e.g., Who? What?) and
• Staying still during reading gradually scaffold up to more complex “How?” and “Why?”
experiences questions, while incorporating more complex vocabulary
from the book (“Where does the marsupial live?”). Finally,
Note. Based on Bus and van Ijzendoorn (1997); DeLoache,
Pierroutsakos, Uttal, Rosengren, and Gottlieb (1998); and Sénéchal it should be noted that the studies above included children
et al. (1995). from a variety of socioeconomic levels, as well as children
exposed to two languages at home.

152 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 148–164 • April 2018

Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Although enjoyment of shared book reading comes Guidelines by ASHA (2008) on the roles and respon-
naturally for most children, those with language impairments sibilities of SLPs in early intervention state that due to docu-
are often difficult to engage in shared book experiences. mented connections between early literacy experiences (e.g.,
Some simple adjustments in both one-on-one and group book shared book reading) and later literacy development, lit-
reading may improve the child’s engagement, the reader’s eracy should be addressed in the early intervention con-
language facilitation, and the benefit of shared book reading text as a prevention, if not intervention, measure. Current
(Kaderavek, Pentimonti, & Justice, 2014; van Kleeck, Vander best practice in early intervention service provision is a coach-
Woude, & Hammett, 2006). Manipulative books, such as ing model (Rush & Shelden, 2011). In this team-based,
lift-the-flap books, and books with less text and shorter sen- transdisciplinary model, there is a primary service provider
tences have been demonstrated to increase the attentiveness (PSP), who may or may not be the SLP. The PSP would
and responsiveness of children with language impairments provide literacy instruction, model facilitative literacy tech-
(Kaderavek & Pakulski, 2007; Kaderavek et al., 2014). niques and, then, provide feedback to parents, caregivers, and
If the book has more text, child engagement may be en- child care teachers as they implement the strategies. If the
hanced by the adult talking about the book using shorter SLP is not the primary provider, then the SLP would be
utterances and not feeling compelled to read the text verba- additionally tasked with educating and consulting with the
tim. Additionally, dialogic reading and scripts, with an em- PSP (typically a physical therapist, occupational therapist,
phasis on literal language (e.g., “What is the dog doing?”) or EC educator) about evidence-based EL techniques (Rush
before scaffolding to inferential and abstract language & Shelden, 2011; Shelden & Rush, 2013).
(e.g., “How do you think the dog feels?”) has been dem- Literacy coaching might involve modeling a technique
onstrated to increase receptive vocabulary with smaller in Appendix A, such as dialogic reading, to the caregivers.
gains in abstract language for children with language For example, the PSP would explain a basic type of dialogic
impairments (van Kleeck et al., 2006). Caregiver and reading, such as asking simple wh-questions about the pic-
teacher responsiveness, sensitivity to the child’s ZPD for tures and story. The PSP might then place the toddler in
book reading, and allowing additional response time may her lap and model the strategy by reading the text and/or
also help children with language impairments benefit more talking about the pictures while interspersing questions
from shared book experiences (Dale, Crain-Thoreson, occasionally, such as “Where is the puppy going?” “Uh-oh!
Notari-Syverson, & Cole, 1996; Kaderavek et al., 2014; The baby looks sad, doesn’t he?” or “Who is driving the
van Kleeck et al., 2006). car?” In the next stage of coaching, the adult caregiver
would engage the child in reading while the PSP observed
and offered feedback. Asking reflective questions often
The SLP’s Role leads to self-discovery and better feedback than telling the
adult the pros and cons of the interaction. Questions such
Birth-to-Three Intervention as “What do you feel went well while you were reading?
Research has led to some effective methods to in- How did you feel when your child lost interest and started
corporate literacy awareness and skills in everyday routines squirming? What could you do differently to help your child
and activities at home, at child care, and in the community. pay attention longer next time?” are more constructive than
Often, the focus of early intervention is on eliciting first telling a mother she asked too many questions or forgot to
words and phrases, increasing the phonetic inventory and point to the pictures. Recording and replaying brief videos
speech complexity, and developing skills, such as play and on a tablet or smart phone can be very helpful during this
joint attention (American Speech-Language-Hearing Asso- self-evaluation process. It is also crucial to provide only one
ciation [ASHA], 2008). However, because early literacy or two tools from the toolbox initially so that parents are
exposure can have a significant impact on infants’ and not overwhelmed. The PSP should reinforce the concept that
toddlers’ later literacy and academic development, SLPs the focus of the activity is language enrichment/facilitation
in birth-to-three practice should attune to the home and/or and literacy, not accurate speech sound production. It is
child care literacy environments of the families that they important for the caregiver to understand that the goal is
serve. Instruments, such as home literacy questionnaires building vocabulary, narrative skills, and “grammar” (i.e.,
during the routines-based interview and assessment process, morphosyntax) skills and not precise articulation. Finally,
may be helpful for determining the extent of a child’s early the PSP should always find something to praise about the
language and literacy exposure during daily routines. Because interaction.
children with speech and language impairments are already
at risk for written language problems, addressing literacy
within routines in home, in child care, and in community Preschool Intervention (Age 3–5 Years)
settings is an intervention priority. By helping parents and EC education is delivered in a variety of settings,
child care providers embed literacy learning opportunities, including prekindergarten classrooms, Head Start, and
the impact of language impairment on EL development child care centers. Across those environments, children from
may be lessened. Additionally, children with language defi- diverse ethnic, social, and economic backgrounds are pro-
cits will be better prepared for future learning environments vided with foundational skills to prepare them for more for-
where EL skills will be explicitly taught. mal schooling. Some of those children enter those programs

Terrell & Watson: Laying a Firm Foundation 153


Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018
Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
with well-developed EL skills, while others may have had Children With Language Impairments
limited exposure to print. Other students may lag far behind
Because the development of EL is dependent on many
their peers due to disabilities or limited exposure to the lan-
oral language skills, children with language impairments
guage of instruction. Nevertheless, teachers are given the
are at risk for developmental reading difficulties. Numerous
charge to ensure that their students enter kindergarten
studies have shown that many children with oral language
with the necessary skills to develop the ability to read and
deficits have concomitant reading difficulties (Catts, Fey,
write. In fact, the Common Core State Standards (National
Governors Association Center for Best Practices, 2010) Zhang, & Tomblin, 2001, 2002; Murphy, Justice, O’Connell,
specify that children in kindergarten should recognize/name Pentimonti, & Kaderavek, 2016). These delays may be docu-
alphabet letters, associate sounds with letters, and be profi- mented in kindergarten, a time when more formal reading
cient in a variety of phonological awareness tasks, including instruction often begins. For example, Catts et al. (2001)
identifying phonemes in simple words and substitute phonemes showed that language problems exhibited by children in
in single words to create new words (see Appendix B). kindergarten predicted reading problems at subsequent ages.
Early literacy skill development in EC education has Catts et al. (2002) also showed that children identified as
received a great deal of attention due to the link between language impaired as kindergarteners exhibited reading
EL skills and later academic achievements (Invernizzi et al., difficulties in second and fourth grade at a rate of six times
2010). Many researchers have shown that educators in EC greater than their nonimpaired peers did. In a retrospective
classrooms may not be providing satisfactory support or study, Murphy et al. (2016) found that reading difficulties
learning opportunities for their students to develop EL skills demonstrated by children in kindergarten was predicted by
(Girard, Girolametto, Weitzman, & Greenberg, 2013; Mihai, oral language skills, alphabet knowledge, and print knowl-
Butera, & Friesen, 2017; Pelatti et al., 2014). In their review, edge evidenced when the participants were enrolled in EC
Mihai et al. (2017) discussed that EC teachers often do not special education classrooms. Thus, differences between good
always foster the development of vocabulary and phono- and poor readers can often be discerned prior to formal
logical skills, nor do they conduct shared book reading in literacy instruction. Murphy et al. (2016) found that print
a way that encourages the development of more complex concept knowledge and oral language skills were related
language. For example, Pelatti et al. (2014) observed 81 EC in their group of subjects. The authors discussed that print
teachers and their students during a time that was “repre- concept tasks often use complex instructions, such as “point
sentative of typical classroom procedures.” The mean length to the longest word on the page,” thus providing further
of each observation was 93 min (range = 24.33–151.42 min). support of the link between oral language and early reading
Results showed that the average amount of instruction development.
time devoted to EL skill development was 18 min (range = Justice (2005) described children with language impair-
0.73–47.80 min). The authors noted that “only a handful” ments as “vulnerable” for acquiring reading skills. She
of the teachers in their study provided specific learning further stated that the lack of adequate reading skills would
opportunities for critical skills, such as vocabulary devel- be the “most significant handicap these children will face”
opment, print concepts, and PA. Rohde (2015) also discussed (p. 7). Justice then delineated the roles of SLPs regarding
the existence of a gap between what research has shown to intervention for and prevention of reading difficulties in
be important for high-quality reading instruction and what children. Thus, it is imperative that children with language
actually happens in many classrooms. Reasons for this gap impairments be identified early so that intervention for both
include preschool educators not having adequate training oral language development and EL skills can commence as
on literacy development/instruction, as well as a lack of soon as possible. This directive puts SLPs in a position to
understanding of the component skills essential for literacy effect change in both domains, as well as fill a collaborative
development and how those skills interact. role with classroom teachers in the delivery of services.
In their investigations of EC teaching environments, Therefore, tools SLPs may find useful include methods for
Justice, Mashburn, Hamre, and Pianta (2008) showed that early identification of those children at risk for difficulty
teachers’ use of “high-quality literacy interactions” with developing literacy skills, which includes collaborating with
children was more important than the materials used for EC teachers. This collaboration should involve both early
instruction. Thus, SLPs may need to add strategies or adapt identification and intervention.
a curriculum to collaborate with EC teachers on best prac- The SLP’s role in helping children develop reading
tices for developing children’s oral and written language and writing skills has been documented in a position state-
skills. These strategies should include arrangement of the ment and technical report (ASHA, 2001). Rather than
physical space for easy access of books and writing utensils being seen as prescriptive, these documents can help SLPs
and how teachers can intentionally support children’s use determine how to best assist the acquisition of EL skills
of such materials throughout the day. Methods to embed and continued development of oral language of children
written language development in a variety of contexts can enrolled in EC classrooms. Schuele and Larrivee (2004)
also be demonstrated. Finally, explicit instruction of develop- suggested that the SLP’s role is not to replace what the
mentally appropriate print and phonological awareness classroom teacher is doing but rather to “supplement and
skills can be modeled through co-teaching and coaching augment” instruction. However, this advice is tempered
using service delivery models, as described later in this tutorial. by the previous discussion on the premise that many EC

154 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 148–164 • April 2018

Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
teachers are not offering best practices in EL instruction educational programs that encompass both oral language
on a consistent basis. Thus, one role the SLP may take on and EL learning (Gillam & Ukrainetz, 2006). Although
is that of mentor and coach. For example, positive effects SLPs can enhance literacy development in many ways, two
have been reported when SLPs have served as coaches important avenues are PA and print concept knowledge.
and mentors to EC educators in an effort to improve EL As stated by Powell (2018) in the lead article, “teachers lack
instruction (Girolametto, Weitzman, & Greenberg, 2012; the fundamental knowledge of linguistic principles that are
Milburn et al., 2015). Coaching with child care and pre- the foundation for reading instruction” (p. 11). Thus, addi-
school teachers can follow the birth-to-three paradigm as tional critical components of the SLP’s toolbox include
described earlier, with the SLP explaining a strategy from strategies and techniques to foster children’s development
the EL toolbox, modeling the strategy in the classroom, of phoneme awareness and print concept knowledge.
then observing the teacher and providing supportive feed-
back. For example, the SLP would explain that identifying
initial sounds in words is an important skill for phonological Phonological Awareness Instruction
awareness. During circle time, the SLP would model how Phonological awareness is the ability to identify and
talking about initial sounds could be embedded in the manipulate units of oral language, such as counting the
calendar routine, such as “It’s November. What sound number of words in a sentence or syllables in a word. PA,
does November (slight emphasis on /n/) start with? /n/–that’s a subset of phonological awareness, specifically involves
right! And it’s the fourth. Does ‘fourth’ start with /m/ showing an understanding of individual sounds in words,
or /f/?” The SLP then gives constructive feedback to the such as identifying the first sound in a spoken word or
teacher once the teacher begins to implement the strategy. deciding if two words begin with the same phoneme (Yopp,
1992). The connections between spoken and written language
should be explicitly taught, then that knowledge needs to
Response-to-Intervention be transferred to reading and writing tasks. Children most
The response-to-intervention (RTI) framework also has at risk for difficulties developing PA skills include those
implications for the SLP’s role in helping children develop with language impairments, children whose first language
literacy skills, as described in Powell’s (2018) lead article is not English, and those from low-income socioeconomic
for this forum. The use of RTI in EC classrooms can be groups.
helpful for both prevention and early identification of EL PA skills progress along a continuum and with care
difficulties (Justice, 2005, 2006). Specifically, for Tier 1 and to ensure that children receive developmentally appropriate
Tier 2 instruction, SLPs can help classroom teachers focus instruction. Gillon (2003) provided a list of phonological
on critical EL concepts, including print concepts, alphabet and phonemic skills, along with suggestions for adjusting
skills, and phonological awareness. Justice (2006) suggested task difficulty. Those skills most appropriate for preschool
that SLPs can make a strong impact on reading potential children are presented in Appendix B. This information
by influencing the scope and content of Tier 1 EL instruc- can also be used to create differentiated instruction within
tion. By helping develop and implement scientifically-based lessons. For example, some children may be ready for more
curricula designed to foster EL skills, SLPs can be a part challenging segmenting activities (e.g., segmenting words
of an educational team charged with preventing reading containing consonant clusters into individual phonemes),
difficulties. For example, SLPs may assist with designing whereas others still need to work on separating words into
instructional practices, learning opportunities, and physical onsets and rimes (e.g., “p—ig”).
environment to help children acquire EL skills (NELP, Callaghan and Madelaine (2012) recommended provid-
2008). Using a collaborative model of instruction, SLPs ing short but frequent PA lessons to preschoolers. One way
may be able to identify those children at risk and require this can be accomplished is by integrating PA instruction
more intensive instruction by systematic monitoring of into classroom activities. For example, during a lesson on
children’s skill development. If needed, the SLP can then farm animals, children could be asked to identify animals
provide Tier 2 instruction to supplement learning opportuni- whose name starts with a “k” sound. Those words could
ties already provided to all of the children in the classroom. then be a part of both blending and segmenting activities.
These Tier 2 interventions should be designed to accelerate Providing instruction at varying levels of challenge (see
children’s EL skill development because research has shown Appendix B) within the same lesson can meet the needs of
that, without adequate instruction, poor readers are not diverse learners. On the other hand, dedicated time could
likely to catch up with their peers (Juel, 1988). Through be devoted to providing specific PA instruction in the context
proactive collaborative instructional design and delivery, of small groups. This offers an opportunity for the SLP to
SLPs may help reduce the number of children with reading conduct in-classroom intervention, especially for children
delays in elementary school by focusing efforts to provide already receiving speech and language services. Children
a strong foundation for oral language and EL skills for pre- could be grouped according to ability levels in order to spe-
school children. cifically focus instruction as advocated by the RTI model.
The training that SLPs receive in speech-language For example, the SLP could work only with children who
development and disorders, phonetics, and language inter- may be struggling with lower level PA skills. Another option
vention techniques provides a solid foundation to build EC is that children with varying levels of PA abilities could be

Terrell & Watson: Laying a Firm Foundation 155


Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018
Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
grouped together, with the SLP differentiating instruction. Print Knowledge Instruction
That way, children with lower abilities could be exposed to
Justice, Bowles, and Skibbe (2006) described print
“what is yet to be learned” and benefit from the modeling
knowledge as an “umbrella term” encompassing both print
and social interaction more skilled children may provide.
concept knowledge and alphabet knowledge. Print concept
The content, or “what to teach” for PA instruction,
knowledge involves knowing that print is meaningful and
can be derived from specific curricular materials (Blachman,
how it functions, whereas alphabet knowledge is shown by
Ball, Black, & Tangel, 2000) or can be “teacher-made,”
if on the basis of scientific teaching principles. Koutsoftas, the ability to identify and name alphabet letters (Justice
Harmon, and Gray (2009) created their own teaching et al., 2006; Justice & Ezell, 2004). PR has been presented
sequence for PA instruction for preschoolers, including as a method to help preschool children attain print con-
probes for monitoring progress, scripts for instructors, cepts (Justice et al., 2009; Justice & Ezell, 2004; Lovelace
and a suggested sequence of teaching objectives. Gillon & Stewart, 2007) and should be included in the SLP’s liter-
(n.d.) also provided extensive resources that can be used acy toolbox. PR strategies include verbal and nonverbal
in PA instruction. Those resources include both a handbook cues to help children attend to the “forms, features, and func-
that provides background information and teaching instruc- tions of written language” (Justice & Ezell, 2004, p. 186).
tions, along with printables (e.g., pictures and worksheets) These strategies may be easily embedded while reading
that can be used to facilitate teaching. Goldstein (2016) to children and include tracking print, pointing to specific
created “PAth to Literacy” to help children with delays in letters or words on a page, asking the child to identify let-
EL skills. ters or words, providing specific comments about print,
The recommended teaching strategies for PA develop- and asking the child to find specific letters or words. The
ment include explicit descriptions about each specific task. goal of instruction is for children to develop the knowledge
This instruction should include modeling and providing of how print functions and is organized. An understanding
multiple cues as needed. For example, success with PA of print forms can allow students to distinguish alphabet
activities is highly dependent on the ability to process audi- letters and realize that letters can be combined in a variety
tory information; however, teachers can also exploit other of ways to convey meaning.
cues, such as pointing out the visual (e.g., lips together for Like PA training, print knowledge instruction can
bilabials) and physical characteristics (e.g., continued airflow be embedded throughout the day within EC classrooms or
for fricatives) of phoneme production. PA training should presented in specific instructional contexts, such as during
also offer children many opportunities to learn and practice shared book reading with children (Justice, Logan, &
each PA skill. Scaffolding should be used to provide children Kaderavek, 2017; Justice, McGinty, Piasta, Kaderavek, &
a means to increase their ability to respond, and correc- Fan, 2010). Results demonstrated that using a PR strategy
tive feedback needs to be provided as often as required. while reading to children can help improve their ability to
Olszewski, Soto, and Goldstein (2017) also showed that write their names, as well as increase their alphabet and
“instructive feedback” (feedback telling the child that their print concept knowledge. Reading to children using a PR
response was correct and why) also supported gains in PA style has been shown to improve the print knowledge of
skills. Importantly, the concept of “sound” or “phoneme” children with language impairments and those from lower
must be separated from the concept of “letter.” For example, socioeconomic households (Justice et al., 2017; Lovelace &
the letter “m” (pronounced / m/) makes the /m/ sound. Stewart, 2007).
Further, the report from NELP (2008) indicated that PA However, research has shown that many EC educators
instruction was most effective when combined with training do not often reference print while reading to their students,
on letter–sound relationships. This additional training may thus missing valuable learning opportunities (Zucker, Justice,
help children associate PA skills with word decoding, as & Piasta, 2009). This provides yet another opportunity for
well as their own attempts at writing words. SLPs to model, mentor, and collaborate with EC teachers
Appendix B provides suggestions for direct and indi- in the provision of EL skill development. Justice and Ezell
rect instruction for a variety of PA tasks. Numerous research (2000, 2004) have shown the benefits of training parents
articles have demonstrated the value of PA training for and teachers in Head Start programs and how to effectively
children with delays in early literacy skills (Goldstein et. al., reference print, thus improving EL skills.
2017), speech impairments (Gillon, 2003; Kirk & Gillon, SLPs can assist EC teachers in helping their students
2007), language delays (van Kleeck, Gillam, & McFadden, develop print knowledge in many ways. For example, SLPs
1998), and childhood apraxia of speech (Moriarty & Gillon, can work alongside classroom teachers and model PR when
2006). Other research has shown the benefit of combining reading to groups of children. SLPs can also recommend
PA training along with alphabet instruction (Olszewski books that contain high print saliency. Books with speech
et al., 2017; Piasta & Wagner, 2010; Whitehurst & Lonigan, bubbles, changes in font size and style, and print embedded
1998). Readers are encouraged to use the teaching strategies within illustrations provide opportunities to point out and
described above when modeling, eliciting responses, and discuss print. SLPs can also help teachers develop appropri-
providing feedback. In addition, the suggestions provided ate print concept objectives. Justice et al. (2017) provided
should be used as a catalyst for additional ideas and activi- a list of objectives teachers can use to structure shared book
ties, relevant to each educational environment. reading sessions. These objectives encompass four categories,

156 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 148–164 • April 2018

Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
including print organization, the meaning of print, letters, younger, therefore underscoring the AAP recommendation
and words. When teaching about print organization, informa- for no screen use in infants and toddlers (AAP, 2016).
tion, such as the title/author, would be specifically mentioned. In addition to television, infants, toddlers, and pre-
The adult may also track print showing that books are read schoolers are now interacting more with handheld technol-
top to bottom, as well as from left to right. To encourage ogy, such as smart phones and tablets. The notion of digital
the development of print meaning, the adult would point to literacy is a recent concept and defined as “the ability to
specific units of print and announce, “That says….” Speech use information and communication technologies to find,
bubbles and print embedded within illustrations are espe- evaluate, create, and communicate information, requiring
cially good stimuli to develop print meaning. Letter knowl- both cognitive and technical skills” (Visser, 2012, para. 2).
edge can be taught by specifically pointing to and naming Common Sense Media (2013) noted that 38% of children
letters, pointing out the difference between uppercase and under age 2 years have used a tablet or smartphone, and
lowercase letters, or asking children to find specific letters that number jumps to 80% for children age 2 to 4 years old.
on a page. Finally, knowledge of words can be facilitated Tablets and smartphones differ from books in that they
by pointing out the difference between long and short words offer a haptic interface (interaction through touch), which
on a page, drawing attention to highly frequent words, and modifies the narrative experience, changes how text is navi-
showing how each printed word represents a spoken word. gated and shared, and impacts the creation of meaning, as
Similar to PA, instruction can be differentiated in order to literacy moves beyond printed text and into an increasingly
meet the needs and abilities of diverse learners. connected world (Harrison & McTavish, 2016; Merchant,
2015). Hourcade, Mascher, Wu, and Pantoja (2015) analyzed
208 YouTube videos of infants and toddlers interacting
with tablets. Just as there is predictable progression in print
Digital and Electronic Media book-handling skills, children seem to acquire skills in screen
An article about birth-to-five literacy in 2017 would not navigation in a consistent developmental manner. These
be complete without a mention of digital literacy. Although type of screen skills included transitioning from whole hand
technology seems omnipresent, and it is difficult to remember or multiple fingers to a single finger to manipulate a tablet,
life without laptops, smartphones, and tablets, the discussion then learning to swipe and drag the screen to access content
of children’s interaction with literacy other than print is (Hourcade et al., 2015). Additionally, toddlers handled
not new. Children’s television programming began a new tablets the same as holding books, often with the tablet
trend in 1969 with the debut of Sesame Street. Sesame Street in the child’s lap and being held with the child’s hands
was specifically created to appeal to an inner-city audience (Hourcade et al., 2015; Merchant, 2015).
with its urban set, diverse cast of human and Muppet char- SLPs in early intervention and EC special education
acters, and its focus on letters, numbers, and other preschool may be tempted to digital applications (apps) in practice
concepts (van Kleeck & Schuele, 2010; Sesame Workshop, because tablets offer the benefit and ease of portability and
n.d.). It set the stage for other preschool and children’s pro- the potential to expose children to new settings, such as a
gramming from the vintage Mister Rogers’ Neighborhood zoo or park through dynamic apps (Artemenko, 2014). For
to the more modern Daniel Tiger (http://www.pbskids.org). early intervention home visits, Artemenko (2014) suggested
The preschool programming boom led to the American using picture apps to build vocabulary and interactive
Academy of Pediatrics (AAP, 2016) issuing a policy state- apps for play sequences and to model scenes from everyday
ment in 1999 discouraging any television viewing for children experiences, such as setting the table, baking, and playing
under 2 years of age. However, this was revised in 2016 to with an interactive dollhouse. However, current best prac-
address the now-ubiquitous digital media. New recom- tice in early intervention requires that providers do not
mendations for infants through preschoolers are located bring any toys, books, or other materials into the home
in Appendix A. because early interventionists should use toys and manipula-
In the past 20 years, there has been an increase in pro- tives already present in the environment so that parents can
gramming aimed at infants and toddlers with the advent follow through during the week (Rush & Shelden, 2011).
of videos like Baby Einstein (http://www.kidsii.com/brands/ Even if there are no toys or books in the home, the parents
babyeinstein) geared for children ages 6 months to 3 years. can be coached to create rich play out of sofa cushions,
There may not be much learning happening with these bowls, and boxes and literacy experiences out of mail and
“educational” shows for the youngest children. Anderson food packaging. Bringing a tablet into the home that the
and Pempek (2005) found that young children have a “video parents may not have access to between visits is contrary
deficit,” which means that they learn less from television to early intervention principles. It is important to remember
than from actual experiences with other people, particu- that children younger than 3 years of age prefer and learn
larly children younger than 3 years old. Toddlers learn play more from adult interaction than from screens. This corre-
schemes, imitation, object retrieval, and vocabulary better sponds to Vygotsky’s socially mediated language theory
from real-life interactions than watching a video of a similar in which adult-mediated interactions within the children’s
scenario or play sequence (Anderson & Pempek, 2005; ZPD improve learning, as well as Piaget’s description of
Kirkorian et al., 2016; Kremar, Grela, & Lin, 2007). This the sensorimotor stage from birth to age 2 years (Piaget,
video deficit is most significant in children age 2 years and 1936; Vygotsky, 1986). In the sensorimotor stage, young

Terrell & Watson: Laying a Firm Foundation 157


Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018
Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
children experience and learn about the world through all of conversation and parent responsiveness (Sosa, 2016). When
their senses and motor movements. Although they can see playing with electronic toys, parents often sat back and let
and hear apps, toddlers cannot grasp, taste, feel, or manip- the toy do the talking. As discussed above, young children
ulate them with their whole bodies (Piaget, 1936). It is im- do not learn as efficiently or comprehensively from non-
perative that SLPs serve in a preventative role and educate human interactions, so this is an area for ongoing parent
parents, teachers, child care providers, pediatricians, and education and modeling. For parents who may not be read-
other educational and health care workers about screen in- ing to young prelinguistic children or whose children do
teractions with young children. not sit still for books, play with nonelectronic toys still offers
Edwards and Dukhovny (2017) found that most SLPs a significant linguistic advantage over electronic toys (Sosa,
select apps on the basis of recommendations from peers 2016).
and colleagues, and they posit that SLPs need to engage in Reich, Yau, and Warschauer (2016) conducted a meta-
more critical thinking about app usage in therapy, even those analysis of research on young children’s use of e-books. They
described as educational. One reason for this is that apps suggested that time away from screens was most beneficial
and technology platforms change so quickly that the research for infants/toddlers as the bulk of research indicated that
may be out of date by the time it is published. This is why face-to-face interaction was superior for learning than screen
SLPs should question “evidenced based” apps. While some time. However, results from studies were mixed when pre-
apps have been tested and reported via the peer review pro- schoolers and e-books were considered. Some studies indi-
cess, other apps may be loosely based on an evidence-based cated increased parent–child conversation during print,
model, such as behaviorism or video modeling (Edwards & whereas others noted increased speech during e-book reading.
Dukhovny, 2017). More importantly, there is currently not It should be noted that, often, the conversations centered
a strong evidence base for the use of apps in intervention or on how to “work” the book by pushing buttons or swiping
casual home use. Because of the potential negative impact versus focusing on the story itself (Reich et al., 2016). They
of handheld devices on infants and toddlers, SLPs, parents, recommended that any e-books for preschoolers should
and educators must be especially cautious and thoughtful be designed to support and scaffold learning (as compared
as they select apps and mobile technology for young children with including games and sounds irrelevant to the story)
with developmental disabilities. Although some apps provide and that e-books have the most impact when mediated
access to augmentative and alternative communication at a and shared with an adult (Reich et al., 2016).
lower cost, such as Proloquo2go, Proctor and Wang (2015)
encourage parents and professionals to consider app quality
because app developers may not have expertise in speech Concluding Thoughts
and language development or understanding of how young
children learn. Additionally, SLPs and teachers must think In summary, a rich home literacy environment, pre-
about the relationship of language and literacy goals and tend play embedded with a variety of literacy tools, shared
curriculum to an app or tablet function. Is using an app interactive book reading, and explicit instruction all serve
the best practice for supporting learning, or is it being used important functions in creating a strong foundation of EL
because it is shiny and new? Finally, Proctor and Wang for infants, toddlers, and preschoolers. Implementing EL
(2015) suggest handheld devices only when they have the strategies into the home, child care, and preschool settings is
potential to enhance communication rather than providing relatively low cost, can be fun, and strengthens the oral–
an opportunity for children to withdraw and engage in a written language connection. Children with exposure to
solitary activity. diverse oral language of varied complexity, exposure to print
With the popularity of handheld electronics, electronic materials, access to writing implements, and an awareness
toys and electronic books (e-books) have also increased of the function and purpose environmental print enter
in use among young children. Generally, parent–child in- kindergarten primed for literacy learning. Shared book
teractive play with electronic toys, such as electronic shape reading experiences, accompanied by PR and extratextual
sorters and barn/animals versus traditional nonelectronic talk, also create a strong EL base that provides more linguis-
versions of the same toys, tended to decrease the amount tic benefit than electronic toys, e-books, tablets, and smart
and quality of the parents’ verbal input with toddlers (Sosa, phones. Therefore, SLPs have a valuable role to play in
2016; Wooldridge & Shapka, 2012; Zosh et al., 2015). Sosa partnering with parents, caregivers, child care providers,
(2016) found that, when interacting with young toddlers, and preschool teachers to educate, share, model, and sup-
age 10–16 months old, mothers said fewer words, and port a variety of evidence-based practices for the develop-
there were fewer conversational turns, parental responses, ment of crucial EL skills in all environments for infants,
and productions of content-specific words when playing toddlers, and preschoolers.
with electronic toys as compared with playing with tradi-
tional toys and books. Children also vocalized less when
interacting with parents in the electronic toy condition. Acknowledgments
Play with books resulted in richer language exposure and The authors wish to thank graduate students Anna Weisbrod,
interaction in all measures as compared with play with elec- Moriah Bemke, and Abigail Wallace for their assistance in proof-
tronic toys, and play with traditional toys led to increased reading this tutorial.

158 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 148–164 • April 2018

Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
References for young children with language delays. Topics in Early Child-
hood Special Education, 16, 213–235.
American Academy of Pediatrics. (2016). American Academy of DeLoache, J. S., Pierroutsakos, S. L., Uttal, D. H., Rosengren,
Pediatrics announces new recommendations for children’s media K. S., & Gottlieb, A. (1998). Grasping the nature of pictures.
use. Retrieved from http://www.aap.org/en-us/about-the-aap/ Psychological Science, 9, 205–211.
aap-press-room/pages/american-academy-of-pediatrics-announces- Dickinson, D., & Porche, M. (2011). Relation between language
new-recommendations-for-childrens-media-use.aspx experiences in preschool classrooms and children’s kindergarten
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2001). Roles and and fourth-grade language and reading abilities. Child Develop-
responsibilities of speech-language pathologists with respect to read- ment, 82, 870–886.
ing and writing in children and adolescents [Position statement Edwards, C. M. (2012). Maternal literacy practices and toddlers’
and technical report]. Retrieved from http://www.asha.org/policy emergent literacy skills. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy,
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2008). Roles 14, 53–79.
and responsibilities of speech-language pathologists in early inter- Edwards, J., & Dukhovny, E. (2017). Technology training in speech-
vention: Guidelines. Retrieved from http://www.asha.org/policy/ language pathology: A focus on tablets and apps. Perspectives
gl2008-00293.htm of the ASHA Special Interest Groups, 2(SIG10), 33–48.
Anderson, D. R., & Pempek, T. A. (2005). Television and very Farver, J. A., Xu, Y., Lonigan, C. J., & Eppe, S. (2013). The home
young children. American Behavioral Scientist, 48(5), 505–522. literacy environment and Latino Head Start children’s emergent
Artemenko, S. (2014). App-titude: Apps that excite our youngest literacy skills. Developmental Psychology, 49, 775–791.
clients. The ASHA Leader, 19(2), 38–39. Gillam, R., & Ukrainetz, T. (2006). Language intervention through
Baker, L. (2003). The role of parents in motivating struggling literature-based units. In T. Ukrainetz (Ed.), Contextualized
readers. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 19, 87–106. language intervention (pp 59–94). Greenville, SC: Thinking
Blachman, B., Ball, E. W., Black, R., & Tangel, D. (2000). Road Publications.
to the code: A phonological awareness program for young chil- Gillon, G. (n.d.). Phonological analysis resources. Retrieved from
dren. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. http://www.canterbury.ac.nz/education/research/phonological-
Blewitt, P., Rump, K. M., Shealy, S. E., & Cook, S. A. (2009). awareness-resources/
Shared book reading: When and how questions affect young Gillon, G. T. (2003). Phonological awareness: From research to
children’s word learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, practice. New York, NY: Guilford.
101(2), 294–304. Girard, L., Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., & Greenberg, J. (2013).
Bodrova, E., Leong, D., & Shore, R. (2004, March). Child outcome Educators’ literacy practices in two emergent literacy contexts.
standards in pre-K programs: What are standards; what is Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 27(1), 46–60.
needed to make them work? Preschool Policy Matters, 5, 1–12. Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., & Greenberg, J. (2012). Facilitating
Retrieved from http://nieer.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/08/5.pdf emergent literacy: Efficacy of a model that partners speech-
Boyce, L. K., Innocenti, M. S., Roggman, L. A., Norman, V. K., language pathologists and educators. American Journal of
& Ortiz, E. (2010). Telling stories and making books: Evidence Speech-Language Pathology, 21(1), 47–63.
for an intervention to help parents in immigrant Head Start Goldstein, H. (2016). Path to literacy. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
families support their children’s language and literacy. Early Goldstein, H., Olszewski, A., Haring, C., Greenwood, C. R.,
Education and Development, 21, 343–371. McCune, L., Carta, J., . . . Kelley, E. S. (2017). Efficacy of a
Bus, A. G., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1988). Mother–children inter- supplemental phonemic awareness curriculum to instruct pre-
actions, attachment, and emergent literacy: A cross-sectional schoolers with delays in early literacy development. Journal of
study. Child Development, 39, 1262–1272. Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(1), 89–103.
Bus, A. G., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Affective dimension Hanna, J. S., Hinrichs, K. M., Mahar, C. J., DeFrain, J. D., &
of mother–infant picture book reading. Journal of School Durden, T. R. (2010, January). Early literacy checklist—in
Psychology, 35, 47–60. the home. Retrieved from http://extensionpublications.unl.edu/
Caldwell, B. M., & Bradley, R. H. (2003). Home observation for assets/pdf/g1991.pdf
measurement of the environment: Administration manual. Tempe, Hargrave, A. C., & Sénéchal, M. (2000). A book reading inter-
AZ: Family and Human Dynamics Research Institute, Arizona vention with preschool children who have limited vocabularies:
State University. The benefits of regular reading and dialogic reading. Early
Callaghan, G., & Madelaine, A. (2012). Levelling the playing field Childhood Research Quarterly, 15(1), 75–90.
for kindergarten entry: Research implications for preschool Harrison, E., & McTavish, M. (2016). ‘i’Babies: Infants’ and tod-
early literacy instruction. Australasian Journal of Early Child- dlers’ emergent language and literacy in a digital culture of
hood, 37(1), 13–22. iDevices. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy. Advance online
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2002). publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468798416653175
A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children Heath, H. (1983). Ways with words. Cambridge, United Kingdom:
with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Cambridge University Press.
Hearing Research, 45(6), 1142–1157. Hourcade, J. P., Mascher, S. L., Wu, D., & Pantoja, L. (2015, April).
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (2001). Look, my baby is using an iPad! An analysis of YouTube videos
Estimating the risk of future reading difficulties in kindergarten of infants and toddlers using tablets. In Proceedings of the 33rd
children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, annual ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Sys-
32(1), 38–50. tems (1915–1924). New York, NY: ACM.
Common Sense Media. (2013). Zero to eight: Children’s media use in Hvit, S. (2014). Literacy events in toddler groups: Preschool edu-
America 2013. Retrieved from http://www.commonsensemedia. cators’ talk about their work with literacy among toddlers.
org/file/zero-to-eight-2013pdf-0/ Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 15, 311–330.
Dale, P. S., Crain-Thoreson, C., Notari-Syverson, A., & Cole, K. Institute of Educational Sciences. (2015). What Works Clearing-
(1996). Parent-child book reading as an intervention technique house Intervention Report. Retrieved from IES website: https://

Terrell & Watson: Laying a Firm Foundation 159


Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018
Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/Docs/InterventionReports/wwc_shared- Kirkorian, H. L., Lavigne, H. J., Hanson, K. G., Troseth, G. L.,
book_041415.pdf Demers, L. B., & Anderson, D. R. (2016). Video deficit in tod-
Invernizzi, M., Landrum, T. J., Teichman, A., & Townsend, M. dlers’ object retrieval: What eye movements reveal about on-
(2010). Increased implementation of emergent literacy screen- line cognition. Infancy, 21(1), 37–64.
ing in pre-kindergarten. Early Childhood Education Journal, Koutsoftas, A. D., Harmon, M. T., & Gray, S. (2009). The effect
37(6), 437–446. of tier 2 intervention for phonemic awareness in a response-
Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study to-intervention model in low-income preschool classrooms.
of 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 40(2),
Educational Psychology, 80(4), 437–447. 116–130.
Justice, L. (2005). Influence of research and policy on practice Kremar, M., Grela, B., & Lin, K. (2007). Can toddlers learn
in today’s schools: Reading, evidence and speech-language vocabulary from television? An experimental approach. Media
pathology. SIG 11 Perspectives on Administration and Super- Psychology, 10(1), 41–63.
vision, 15, 6–10. Lawhon, T., & Cobb, J. B. (2002). Routines that build emergent
Justice, L. (2006). Evidence-based practice, response to intervention, literacy skills in infants, toddlers, and preschoolers. Early
and the prevention of reading difficulties. Language, Speech, Childhood Education Journal, 30, 113–118.
and Hearing Services in Schools, 37(4), 284–297. Li, G., & Christ, T. (2007). Social capital and home literacy engage-
Justice, L. M., Bowles, R. P., & Skibbe, L. E. (2006). Measuring ment: Case studies of low-SES single mothers’ access to literacy
preschool attainment of print-concept knowledge: A study of resources. English in Education, 41, 21–36.
typical and at risk 3- to 5-year-old children using item response Lovelace, S., & Stewart, S. R. (2007). Increasing print awareness
theory. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 37, in preschoolers with language impairment using non-evocative
224–235. print referencing. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
Justice, L. M., & Ezell, H. K. (2000). Enhancing children’s print Schools, 38(1), 16–30.
and word awareness through home-based parent intervention. Merchant, G. (2015). Keep taking the tablets: iPads, story apps, and
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9(3), 257–269. early literacy. Australian Journal of Language and Literacy, 38,
Justice, L. M., & Ezell, H. L. (2004). Print referencing: An emer- 3–11.
gent literacy enhancement strategy and its clinical applications. Mihai, A., Butera, G., & Friesen, A. (2017). Examining the use
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 185–193. of curriculum to support early literacy instruction: A multiple
Justice, L. M., Kaderavek, J. N., Fan, X., Sofka, A., & Hunt, A. case study of head start teachers. Early Education and Develop-
(2009). Accelerating preschoolers’ early literacy development ment, 28(3), 323–342.
through classroom-based teacher—child storybook reading Milburn, T. F., Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., & Greenberg, J.
and explicit print referencing. Language, Speech, and Hearing (2013). Enhancing preschool educators’ ability to facilitate
Services in Schools, 40, 67–85. conversations during shared book reading. Journal of Early
Justice, L. M., Logan, J., & Kaderavek, J. N. (2017). Longitudinal Childhood Literacy, 14(1), 105–140.
impacts of print-focused read-alouds for children with language Milburn, T. F., Hipfner-Boucher, K., Weitzman, E., Greenberg, J.,
impairment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, Pelletier, J., & Girolametto, L. (2015). Effects of coaching
26(2), 383–396. on educators’ and preschoolers’ use of references to print and
Justice, L. M., Mashburn, A., Hamre, B., & Pianta, R. (2008). phonological awareness during a small-group craft/writing
Quality of language and literacy instruction in preschool class- activity. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,
rooms serving at risk pupils. Early Childhood Research Quar- 46(2), 94–111.
terly, 23(1), 51–68. Moriarty, B., & Gillon, G. (2006). Phonological awareness inter-
Justice, L. M., McGinty, A. S., Piasta, S. B., Kaderavek, J. N., & vention for children with childhood apraxia of speech. Inter-
Fan, X. (2010). Print-focused read-alouds in preschool class- national Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41,
rooms: Intervention effectiveness and moderators of child out- 713–734.
comes. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, Murphy, K. A., Justice, L. M., O’Connell, A. A., Pentimonti, J. M.,
41(4), 504–520. & Kaderavek, J. N. (2016). Understanding risk for reading
Kaderavek, J. N., & Pakulski, L. N. (2007). Mother–child story difficulties in children with language impairment. Journal of
book interactions: Literacy orientation of preschoolers with hear- Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59(6), 1436–1447.
ing impairment. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 7, 49–72. National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy:
Kaderavek, J. N., Pentimonti, J. M., & Justice, L. M. (2014). Chil- Report of the National Early Literacy Panel. Washington, DC:
dren with communication impairments: Caregivers’ and teachers’ National Institute for Literacy.
shared book-reading quality and children’s level of engagement. National Governors Association Center for Best Practices, & Council
Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 30, 289–302. of Chief State School Officers. (2010). Common core state stan-
Katz, J. R. (2001). Playing at home: The talk of pretend play. In dards. Washington, D.C.: National Governors Association Center
D. K. Dickinson & P. O. Tabors, Beginning literacy with lan- for Best Practices, Council of Chief State School Officers.
guage: Young children learning at home and school (pp. 53–74). Neuman, S. B., & Roskos, K. (1993). Access to print for children
Baltimore, MD: Brookes. of poverty: Differential effects of adult mediation and literacy-
Kibler, A., Palacios, N., Simpson Baird, A., Bergey, R., & Yoder, M. enriched play settings on environmental and functional print
(2016). Bilingual Latin children’s exposure to language and tasks. American Educational Research Journal, 30, 95–122.
literacy practices through older siblings in immigrant families. Neuman, S. B., & Roskos, K. (1997). Literacy knowledge in prac-
Linguistics and Education, 35, 63–77. tice: Contexts of participation for young writers and readers.
Kirk, C., & Gillon, G. T. (2007). Longitudinal effects of phono- Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 10–32.
logical awareness intervention on morphological awareness in Norris, D. J. (2017). Comparing language and literacy environ-
children with speech impairment. Language, Speech, and Hear- ments in two types of infant–toddler child care centers. Early
ing Services in the Schools, 38, 342–352. Childhood Education Journal, 45, 95–101.

160 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 148–164 • April 2018

Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Olszewski, A., Soto, X., & Goldstein, H. (2017). Modeling alphabet Slates, S. L., Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Olson, L. S.
skills as instructive feedback within a phonological awareness (2012). Counteracting summer slide: Social capital resources
intervention. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, within socioeconomically disadvantaged families. Journal of
26, 769–790. Education for Students Placed at Risk, 17, 165–185.
Pelatti, C. Y., Piasta, S. B., Justice, L. M., & O’Connell, A. (2014). Snow, C., Tabors, P., & Dickinson, D. K. (2001). Language
Language- and literacy-learning opportunities in early childhood development in the preschool years. In D. K. Dickinson &
classrooms: Children’s typical experiences and within-classroom P. O. Tabors (Eds.), Beginning literacy with language: Young
variability. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 445–456. children learning at home and school (pp. 1–26). Baltimore, MD:
Piaget, J. (1936). Origin of intelligence in the child. London, Brookes.
England: Penguin Books. Sosa, A. V. (2016). Association of the type of toy used during play
Piasta, S. B., & Wagner, R. K. (2010). Developing early literacy with the quantity and quality of parent-infant communication.
skills: A meta-analysis of alphabet learning and instruction. JAMA Pediatrics, 170(2), 132–137.
Reading Research Quarterly, 45(1), 8–38. Tichnor-Wagner, A., Garwood, J. D., Bratsch-Hines, M., &
Pinto, A. I., Pessanha, M., & Aguiar, C. (2013). Effects of home Vernon-Feagans, L. (2015). Home literacy environments and
environment and center-based child care quality on children’s foundational literacy skills for struggling and nonstruggling
language, communication, and literacy outcomes. Early Child- readers in rural early elementary schools, Learning Disabilities
hood Research Quarterly, 28, 94–101. Research and Practice, 31, 6–21.
Powell, R. (2018). Unique contributors to the curriculum: From U.S. Census Bureau. (2013). How do we know? Child care an impor-
research to practice for speech-language pathologists in schools. tant part of American life. Retrieved from http://www.census.
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 49, 140–147. gov/library/visualizations/2013/comm/child_care.html
Proctor, L. A., & Wang, Y. (2015). Using iPads and mobile tech- van Kleeck, A., Gillam, R. B., & McFadden, T. U. (1998). A
nology for children with developmental disabilities: Facilitating study of classroom-based phonological awareness training for
language and literacy development. In N. R. Silton (Ed.), preschoolers with speech and/or language disorders. American
Recent advances in assistive technologies to support children with Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 7, 65–76.
developmental disorders (pp. 45–78). Hershey, PA: IGI Global. van Kleeck, A., & Schuele, C. M. (2010). Historical perspectives
Reich, S. M., Yau, J. C., & Warschauer, M. (2016). Tablet-based on literacy in early childhood. American Journal of Speech-
eBooks for young children: What does the research say? Journal Language Pathology, 19, 341–355.
of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 37(7), 858–591. van Kleeck, A., Vander Woude, J., & Hammett, L. (2006). Fostering
Roberts, J., Jurgens, J., & Burchinal, M. (2005). The role of home literal and inferential language skills in Head Start preschoolers
literacy practices in preschool children’s language and emer- with language impairment using scripted book-sharing dis-
gent literacy skills. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing cussions. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
Research, 48, 345–359. 15, 85–95.
Rohde, L. (2015). The comprehensive emergent literacy model: Visser, M. (2012, September). ALA connect: Digital literacy
Early literacy in context. SAGE Open, 5(1), 1–11. https://doi. definition. Retrieved from http://connect.ala.org/node/
org/10.1177/2158244015577664 181197
Rush, D. D., & Shelden, M. L. (2011). The early intervention coach- Vygotsky, L. (1986). The development of scientific concepts in
ing handbook. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. childhood: The design of a working hypothesis. In A. Kozulin
Saracho, O. N. (2017). Research, policy, and practice in early child- (Ed.), Thought and language (pp. 146–209). Cambridge, MA:
hood literacy. Early Child Development and Care, 187(3–4), MIT.
305–321. Whitehurst, G. J., Arnold, D. S., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L.,
Schuele, M. C., & Larrivee, L. S. (2004). What’s my job? Differ- Smith, M., & Fischel, J. E. (1994). A picture book reading
ential diagnosis of the speech-language pathologist’s role in intervention in day care and home for children from low-income
literacy learning. SIG 1 Perspectives on Language Learning and families. Developmental Psychology, 30(5), 679–689.
Education, 11, 4–8. Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development
Sénéchal, M., Cornell, E. H., & Broda, L. S. (1995). Age-related and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848–872.
differences in the organization of parent-infant interactions Wooldridge, M. B., & Shapka, J. (2012). Playing with technology:
during picture-book reading. Early Childhood Research Quar- Mother–toddler interaction scores lower during play with
terly, 10, 317–337. electronic toys. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,
Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. (2014). Continuity and change in the 33, 211–218.
home literacy environment as predictors of growth in vocabu- Yopp, H. K. (1992). Developing phonemic awareness in young
lary and reading. Child Development, 85, 1552–1568. children. Reading Teacher, 45(9), 696–703.
Sénéchal, M., LeFevre, J., Thomas, E. M., & Daley, K. D. (1998). Zosh, J. M., Verdine, B. N., Filipowicz, A., Golinkoff, R. M.,
Differential effects of home literacy experiences on the devel- Hirsch-Pasek, K., & Newcombe, N. S. (2015). Talking shape:
opment of oral and written language. Reading Research Quar- Parental language with electronic versus traditional shape
terly, 33, 96–116. sorters. Mind, Brain, and Education, 9(3), 136–144.
Sesame Workshop. (n.d.). 40 years and counting. Retrieved from Zucker, T. A., Justice, L. M., & Piasta, S. B. (2009). Prekinder-
http://www.sesameworkshop.org/about-us/40-years-and-counting/ garten teachers’ verbal references to print during classroom-
Shelden, M. L., & Rush, D. D. (2013). The early intervention team- based, large-group shared reading. Language, Speech, and
ing handbook. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. Hearing Services in Schools, 40(4), 376–392.

Terrell & Watson: Laying a Firm Foundation 161


Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018
Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Appendix A ( p. 1 of 2)
Interactive Book Reading, Environmental Print, and Digital Media Strategies

Dialogic reading strategies


Description Example Evidence base

Dialogic reading: Background knowledge: Blewitt, Rump, Shealy, & Cook, 2009; Dale
• Adults should be reminded that they are This little girl has a kitty just like you. et al., 1996; Hargrave & Sénéchal, 2000;
not compelled to stick to the text. Books Remember when we went to the zoo? Institute of Educational Sciences, 2015;
with limited text and/or short sentences Making predictions: Kaderavek & Pakulski, 2007; Whitehurst
can be more engaging for children with What will the puppy do? et al., 1994
communication impairments. Uh-oh! What happened?
• Igniting background knowledge, making Asking questions: Where’s the ball? What
predictions, asking questions, is the baby eating? (scaffold to more
commenting, and pointing to pictures and difficult how/when/why questions, such as
text are important ways to interact with “How does the bear feel?”)
books, keep young children engaged, and Commenting:
scaffold language. That’s a yellow flower.
• Above all, follow the child’s lead. If the That cookie looks yummy!
child points, then the adult should label or
describe what the child is pointing to.
• Allow children with language impairments
more time to respond.
Playing in the book: Model petting the pony or popping the Bus & van Ijzendoorn, 1997; Kaderavek &
• Interactive books, such as touch-and-feel bubbles in the bathtub in a picture and Pakulski, 2007; van Kleeck et al., 2006
and lift the flap books, naturally use gentle hand-over-hand if necessary.
encourage manipulating the book. Shake a finger at a naughty puppy in a story
• Standard board books and picture books with an exaggerated, “No, no!,” pat a
can be made interactive by the reader. sleepy baby in a book while saying, “Shh.
• Increases child’s attention and makes Night-night,” or pretend to pick cupcakes
literacy experiences more interactive and off the page and eat them “Mmmm.
enjoyable, which is especially important Yummy.”
for children with communication
impairments.
• Vary vocal intonation and use facial
expressions to indicate emotion and
different characters.
Following child’s lead: Sometimes when an adult allows a child Bus & van Ijzendoorn, 1988, 1997; Kaderavek
• Never hold a child and force a reading to get up and wander, but still reads the & Pakulski, 2007; Lawhon & Cobb, 2002;
interaction. Reading together should be a story with an animated voice and lively Sénéchal et al., 1995
pleasant experience with physical expressions, the child will be drawn back
closeness, such as the child in the adult’s into the narrative or other book experience.
lap or by the adult’s side, invoking Keep utterances short and simple and choose
positive interactions, and warmth. books that relate to child’s interests.

Print referencing/book-handling strategies

Description Example Evidence base

Include print referencing into literacy Verbal references while pointing to: Bus & van Ijzendoorn, 1988, 1997; Justice
experiences by using a finger to follow the Words as distinct units: “There are 1, 2, 3, & Ezell, 2004; Justice et al., 2009;
print while reading aloud, as well as making 4 words on this page.” Justice et al., 2017; Lawhon & Cobb,
comments that refer to the print. Long versus short words: “This word is a 2002; Sénéchal et al., 1995
long word” while pointing to “elephant.”
Salient print on a page: “This writing on the
bus says, ‘school bus.’”
Letters: “Puppy starts with ‘p’ like in your
name, ‘Paul.’”
Directionality of print: “We starting reading
here at the top of this page.”
Young children can be taught book Demonstrate turning pages right to left, Bus & van Ijzendoorn, 1997; DeLoache,
conventions, such as how to hold a book reading top to bottom, holding the book Pierroutsakos, Uttal, Rosengren, &
and turn pages, as well as book and print right side-up. Talk about the parts of the Gottlieb, 1998; Sénéchal et al., 1995
directionality. They can learn to care for book, such as title, author, illustrator,
books and treat them differently than toys. page, pictures, and words. (“Here’s the
title. It tells us the name of the book.”)
(table continues)

162 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 148–164 • April 2018

Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Appendix A ( p. 2 of 2)
Interactive Book Reading, Environmental Print, and Digital Media Strategies.
Literacy environments

Description Example Evidence base

Create a home or child care environment in Encourage parents and teachers at home Caldwell & Bradley, 2003; Edwards, 2012;
which infants and toddlers have easy and in child care settings to create some Hanna, Hinrichs, Mahar, DeFrain, &
access to age-appropriate books, such as natural play areas that include literacy Durden, 2010; Pinto, Pessanha, & Aguiar,
cloth or board books and pictures books. scripts/routines and materials, such as 2013; Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2014
Also, have writing implements, such as envelopes, menus, bills, and coupons.
chalk, markers, crayons, pencils, and paint, Show adult how to embed and scaffold
available to toddlers with supervision. literacy within natural play contexts, for
example, playing restaurant with a menu
or taking a short child-friendly (words and
pictures) grocery list along when
shopping.
Consider using a home or classroom
literacy checklist to identify strengths
and weaknesses of literacy environments.
Point out environmental print in the home, Stop signs, exit signs, coupons, TV shows, Caldwell & Bradley, 2003; Hanna, Hinrichs,
child care setting, and the community. cookbooks, tabloids and magazines at Mahar, DeFrain, & Durden, 2010; Justice
the checkout lane, menus at restaurants, & Ezell, 2000
names on child care cubbies—the
opportunities to notice and discuss print
are endless.
“See that red sign? It says ‘STOP.’”
“Oh! Look at those words. It’s Paw Patrol.”
“Look. It says ‘Emma.’ That’s where you hang
your coat.”
Take advantage of the public or school library, Most libraries now have board books for the Baker, 2003; Farver, Xu, Lonigan, & Eppe,
if possible. If transportation is a problem for youngest readers and have group play 2013; Slates, Alexander, Entwisle, &
families, look for bookmobile routes or times, story hour, and other interactive Olson, 2012
“Little Free Libraries” in the area literacy events for children from infancy
neighborhoods. through school age. More and more
libraries are including a variety of toys and
manipulatives for young children as well,
both to be played with at the library and
some available for checkout.
Note: Dolly Parton’s imaginationlibrary.com
website provides free books each month
for children ages birth to 5 years.

Digital toys and media

Description Example Evidence base

Encourage families to create media plan and AAP (2016) recommends: AAP, 2016; Anderson & Pempek, 2005;
refrain from using electronic and digital • No screen media except video chatting Kirkorian et al., 2016; Kremar, Grela, &
media as a babysitter. Carefully monitor for children age < 18 months Lin, 2007
screen usage (TV, computer, tablet, and • 1 hr/day of high-quality programming
smartphone). Engage in prevention and for children ages 2–5 years
educate families/educators about potential A family media plan can be found at http://
risks/downfalls of digital media, especially www.healthychildren.org
with infants and toddlers.
Engage in dialogic reading with electronic “The boy laughed. I think he’s happy” (in Reich et al., 2016
books, as discussed above. Also remember response to a child pushing a button/
to discuss the story, not the device. picture in an interactive e-book).
When children do use tablets and watch TV, Describe what is happening on TV, ask Anderson & Pempek, 2005; Kirkorian et al.,
engage with them. questions related to the story, and make 2016; Kremar, Grela, & Lin, 2007; Reich
comments. Talk about letters and words; et al., 2016
point out symbols and icons (“This button
with the big, red ‘N’ is for Netflix. That’s
the one for movies”).
Help parents, child care providers, and Shape sorters, puzzles, vehicles, farm sets, Sosa, 2016; Wooldridge & Shapka, 2012;
educators select traditional toys that blocks, dolls, toy food, and so forth offer Zosh et al., 2015
promote oral language and pretend play. more opportunities for creative play and
Electronic toys tend to decrease adult and language.
child language and limit symbolic play.

Terrell & Watson: Laying a Firm Foundation 163


Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018
Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Appendix B
Tasks to Develop a Variety of Phonemic Awareness Skills

Task Explicit task Embedded task Connect with print

Syllabication Orally present a list of words of When reading or playing with children, Point to printed words in books and
varying lengths and clap out occasionally draw attention to longer other materials and mention their
the number of syllables for each, words as they occur spontaneously, length or number of “parts” in
for example, “monkey,” “cat,” and clap out the number of syllables; each word.
“bicycle.” for example, “caterpillar.”
Rhyming Offer word pairs and ask if they Sing songs and lead finger plays; for Print pairs of rhyming words and
rhyme (e.g., cat/bat; cat/log). example, “Down by the Bay.” compare and contrast the letters
Ask children to generate their own Read books that have a strong rhyme with each other; for example,
rhymes; for example, “what words component, such as Dr. Seuss compare and contrast the letters
rhyme with pig?” books. in “hop” versus “mop.”
Play guessing games, “what drink While playing, create silly words that Use print to show how different
rhymes with silk?” rhyme (e.g., “honeybunny”). initial sounds can be added to
rhymes to create many rhyming
words.
Phoneme Ask children to say the whole Occasionally call on children by Use print or letter tiles to show how
blending word when present with words segmenting the phonemes in their to use individual letters can be
separated by onset and rime name, for example, “J-on” and combined to create words. Tiles
(“p-ig”). “S-a-m.” can include both individual letters
Ask children to say the whole word and common rhymes, such as
when given individual phonemes “at” and “ed.”
(“p-i-g”; “p-l-u-m”).
Syllable and Ask children to omit a syllable from When reading with children, Print compound words and words
sound longer words, (e.g., “say railroad occasionally produce a word of varying syllabic length on a
deletion without the ‘road,’” “say bicycle without one of the phonemes whiteboard and demonstrate what
without the ‘bi.’” and see if they can provide the happens when certain parts of
Ask children to omit a single phoneme correct phoneme to complete those words are erased; for
from words (e.g., “say bone without the word (e.g., “he rolled up example, erase the last syllable
the /b/”; “say smoke without the his ‘at’/mat”). from “butterfly” to show that
/s/”). “butter” is left.
Ask children to omit a phoneme from Break down words, such as
a cluster in CCVC or CVCC words “cupcake” to “cup” “cake”
(e.g., “say black without the /b/” and “up.”
and “say mask without the /k/”).
Syllable and Ask children to produce compound Occasionally, while playing, point Print words on cards and show
phoneme words when provided with out the components of compound how they can be manipulated
segmentation component words (e.g., “what words, (e.g., “racecar and racetrack and combined with each other to
word does grape plus fruit make?”). both contain the word ‘race’”). create a variety of different words;
Ask children to produce longer words Help children create “menus” while for example combining “bed” with
when provided component syllables playing restaurant, emphasizing the other words to create “bedroom”
(e.g., “what word does ham-bur-ger syllables of some of the longer words or “bedtime.”
make?”). (e.g., “wa-ter-mel-on”). Print the word “key” on a card and
Ask children to produce the whole While playing restaurant, order items on separate cards print syllables
words when given those words in by producing one phoneme at a time such as “tur,” “mon,” and “don”
parts; for example, “p-ig,” “b-e-d,” and see how quickly the children can to show how those parts can
“s-l-e-d,” “f-a-s-t,” and so forth. guess your order (e.g., “m-i-l-k”). be combined to create “turkey,”
“monkey,” and “donkey.”
Phoneme Introduce a phoneme such as /b/, When pretending to cook, call attention Provide children with a list of items
identification recite a list of words, and have the to foods that begin with the same in the classroom along with
children raise their hands when sounds and (e.g., “bacon and bread picture icons if necessary, and
they hear a word beginning with both start with /b/”). ask them to find objects whose
that sound. When reading books with alliterative names begin with a specific
Have children group pictures based text, occasionally call attention to sound and check off each one
on initial sounds (e.g., foods that the repetitiveness (e.g., “Sheep in a they find (e.g., desk, dog, dot,
begin with /p/). shop, I hear the ‘sh’ sound twice”). and door). If possible, encourage
Create memory games where children to write the initial letter
matching pictures begin with the next to the appropriate item.
same sound (e.g., “sink and sun”).

164 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 148–164 • April 2018

Downloaded From: https://lshss.pubs.asha.org/ by a University Library Utrecht User on 04/26/2018


Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi