Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 10

Behavioural Processes 70 (2005) 122–131

The role of edges in the selection of a jump


target in Mantis religiosa
Karin Hyden, Karl Kral ∗
Institute of Zoology, Karl-Franzens University Graz, Universitaetsplatz 2, A-8010 Graz, Austria

Received 28 February 2005; received in revised form 19 May 2005; accepted 20 May 2005

Abstract

Before jumping to a landing object, praying mantids determine the distance, using information obtained from retinal image
motion resulting from horizontal peering movements.
The present study investigates the peering-jump behaviour of Mantis religiosa larvae with regard to jump targets differing in
shape and size. The experimental animals were presented with square, triangular and round target objects with visual extensions
of 20◦ and 40◦ . The cardboard objects, presented against a uniform white background, were solid black or shaded with a gradation
from white to black.
It was found that larger objects were preferred to smaller ones as jump targets, and that the square and triangle were preferred to
the round disk. When two objects were presented, no preference was exhibited between square and triangular objects. However,
when three objects were presented, the square was preferred.
For targets with a visual angle of 40◦ , the amplitude and velocity of the horizontal peering movements were greater for the
round disk than for the square or triangle. This amplification of the peering movements suggests that weaker motion signals are
generated in the case of curved edges. This may help to account for the preference for the square and triangle as jump targets.
© 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Behaviour; Visual target; Image motion; Shape determination; Range determination; Mantis religiosa

1. Introduction body slowly from side to side in the horizontal plane,


counter-rotating its head while moving so as to keep
Before jumping to a stationary landing target, it oriented straight ahead. In the case of a station-
locusts and praying mantids engage in a distinctive ary target, the lateral shift in the insect’s eye position
type of behaviour referred to as “peering”. While star- shifts the angular position of the image of the target on
ing straight ahead, the insect shifts its head and upper the retina. Manipulation experiments, where the tar-
get is moved, indicate that the neural system of the
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +43 316 380 5614; insect can determine the jump distance from the veloc-
fax: +43 316 380 9875. ity of retinal image motion (Wallace, 1959; Sobel,
E-mail address: karl.kral@uni-graz.at (K. Kral). 1990; Poteser and Kral, 1995; for review, see Kral and

0376-6357/$ – see front matter © 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2005.05.003
K. Hyden, K. Kral / Behavioural Processes 70 (2005) 122–131 123

Poteser, 1997; Poteser et al., 1998; Kral, 1998a, 1998b, at a temperature of 21 ◦ C and a relative humidity
1999, 2003a, 2003b; Kral and Prete, 2004). These find- of 50–60%. The animals were housed individually
ings are confirmed by the linear relationship between in numbered plastic cages with a height of 10.5 cm
the jump speed of the animal and the target distance and a diameter of 4.5 cm. The mantids were sprayed
(Sobel, 1990; Poteser, 1998). This implies not only that daily with water and were fed wingless adult fruitflies,
an absolute value for the respective object distance is Drosophila melanogaster.
available as data at the time of the jump, but also that
this can be translated into an appropriate degree of mus- 2.2. Arena and behavioural experiments
cle activity.
Behavioural studies in the natural environment show Between 8:00 and 16:00, individual mantid larvae
that a freely moving praying mantis does not jump were placed on a round island with a diameter of
blindly, but rather appears to look first for the most 20 mm. This island platform was positioned in the mid-
accessible object (Horridge, 1986; Kral and Devetak, dle of a Petri dish with a diameter of 91 mm and a height
1999). The animal seems to execute peering move- of 15 mm. The Petri dish was filled with water to form
ments directed toward several objects before deciding a moat surrounding the platform and was placed in the
on a target. Clearly, it is important for a mantid to middle of a round arena made of white cardboard. The
select an object on which a safe landing is guaran- arena had a diameter of 30 cm and was surrounded by a
teed. Under controlled laboratory conditions it has been wall 20 cm high (see Fig. 1). The arena was uniformly
found that in the case of objects at different distances illuminated directly from above. The illumination in
from the mantid, a stimulus is assessed as a suitable the arena was between 800 and 1000 lx.
“jump target” if it is determined to be safely accessible Two or three objects differing in shape were pre-
to the mantid (Walcher and Kral, 1994). The stimu- sented simultaneously to each larva as potential jump
lus interpreted as a jump target was always the nearest targets. In the first part of the investigations targets
object, indicating that the highest peak velocity of reti- made of black cardboard sheets were employed, shaped
nal image motion of the target arising from peering as squares, equilateral triangles and round disks. These
movements is the relevant stimulus for a jump (Poteser,
1998). The present study examines whether stimuli dif-
fering in shape and size presented at the same distance
from the experimental animals attract equal numbers
of jumps. In addition, the amplitude and velocity of the
horizontal peering movements are investigated in order
to determine whether there is a behavioural correlation
with the shape and size of the jump targets. The objects
were positioned at a sufficient distance so that the man-
tid would not be able to determine their characteristics
with the aid of other cues, such as binocular cues (Köck
et al., 1993).

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Animals

In the present study, second- and third-stage lar- Fig. 1. Schematic drawing of the arena used to study the peering-
jump behaviour of mantid larvae (Mantis religiosa). In the middle of
vae of the praying mantis Mantis religiosa, with body
the arena is the round island surrounded by water, where the larva
lengths ranging from 7 to 10 mm, were examined. is placed. Cardboard objects are then presented to the experimental
The larvae were held in an incubator with a 12 h animal as potential landing targets. On the right-hand side of the
light-darkness cycle (with a brightness of 500–800 lx), figure the targets used are shown. For more information, see text.
124 K. Hyden, K. Kral / Behavioural Processes 70 (2005) 122–131

particular shapes were chosen so that the objects would jumps were included in the statistical analysis only if
possess vertical, horizontal, curved and oblique edges. they were preceded by significant object-related peer-
For each shape two sizes of object were used, with ing movements directed toward each of the potential
visual angles which measured 20◦ and 40◦ , assuming landing targets (first toward one object, then another,
that the eyes of the experimental animal were posi- etc.) (see Walcher and Kral, 1994). It could thus be
tioned at the edge of the island. Preliminary studies guaranteed that all of the objects were included in the
showed that a visual extension of 20◦ represents the target selection decision. If a mantid did not show an
threshold visual angle for the jumping behaviour, while interest in the objects by moving to the edge of the
a visual angle of 40◦ corresponds to the central frontal island after an adaptation period of 15–20 min and
part of the eye, where the ommatidia and photoreceptor beginning object-related peering movements, it was
cells are directed forward (see Köck, 1992). The pre- returned to the incubator and was not tested again
liminary studies also showed that black objects were that day.
always preferred to grey ones as jump targets, i.e.,
objects with the greatest contrast to the background 2.3. Video analysis of peering-jump behaviour
were preferred (K. Kral, unpublished observations). In
the second series of experiments a similar set of objects Peering movements occurring immediately prior to
was presented, but instead of being black the objects jumping were analysed. They were taped with the aid of
were shaded with a gradation from white at the top to a CCD video camera (SONY, DXC-107 A/107 AP) and
black at the bottom (see Fig. 1). an S-VHS recorder (SONY, SV09620P). The camera
Objects presented simultaneously were equally far was aimed at the animal from directly above or from the
from the edge of the island, in the optimum jumping side. The video recordings were stored in a computer
range of 15 mm for mantids in the second larval stage with the aid of a Cinergy video card and the program
or 17 mm for mantids in the third larval stage. At these Cinergy TV. The program VirtualDub 1.6.5 was used to
distances, the objects were far enough away that dis- store the film sequences as single frames. In each case,
tance estimation on the basis of binocular cues would the two frames were selected that most clearly showed
not be possible (Köck et al., 1993; see also Leitinger, the starting point and end point of the peering move-
1994). The objects were presented against a uniformly ment. With the aid of the graphic program CorelDraw 8,
white, unstructured background. The angle between the the distance between the starting point and end point of
near edges of the objects was 120◦ when two objects each peering movement was measured in terms of the
were presented, and 80◦ (for objects with a visual angle displacement normal to vertical straight lines. With its
of 40◦ ) to 100◦ (for objects with a visual angle of known diameter, the island in the arena, which was also
20◦ ) when three objects were presented. The vertical recorded on single video frames, served as a reference
position of each object was chosen in such a way that for calculating the absolute distance. Once the peer-
the object was aligned with the centre of the mantid’s ing amplitude was known, the velocity of the peering
frontal visual field, provided that the mantid assumed movement was determined. The duration of the peering
an ordinary straight posture, which was usually movement was found by counting the individual frames
the case. of the video recording from the starting point to the end
During a trial with an individual animal, the objects point. From the amplitude and duration of the peer-
were positioned according to a random sequence. This ing movement the velocity could then be determined
eliminated the possibility that the mantid might select a (in mm/s).
particular target based on its position. The trial for each
experimental animal lasted 1–2 h. In this time period
an average of two to four target selections with jumps 3. Results
could be observed. However, a trial was concluded
when the total number of jumps for the particular tar- 3.1. Black square and round black disk
get combination reached 100. Following a trial, the
experimental animal was not examined again that day. In the first experiment, mantid larvae (Mantis
All of the targets selected were recorded; however, religiosa) were presented with a black square and a
K. Hyden, K. Kral / Behavioural Processes 70 (2005) 122–131 125

Fig. 2. Graphs indicating the extent to which mantid larvae distinguish between a black square and a round black disk with the same (A, B) and
different (C, D) visual angles, when selecting a jump target. For significance levels, see text.

round black disk, with the same or different angular jump target significantly more often (see Fig. 2C and
extensions, as potential jump targets. In preliminary D; chi-square test: P < 0.001; n = 100).
studies, it was found that with a visual angle of 10◦ ,
the mantids generally exhibited no peering-jump 3.2. Black square and black triangle
behaviour, regardless of the shape of the object. In
the experiments, peering-jump behaviour occurred In the next experiment, the mantid larvae were pre-
with objects having a visual angle of at least 20◦ . sented with a black square and a black triangle, with
In Fig. 2A it can be seen that the mantids did not the same or different angular extensions. Preliminary
make a distinction between the two objects when both studies showed that the peering behaviour was the same
had visual angles of 20◦ (n = 100). When both had regardless of whether the edges of the triangle had a
visual angles of 40◦ , the experimental animals jumped slope of 45◦ or 60◦ (K. Kral, unpublished observations).
significantly more often to the square than to the disk An equilateral triangle with angles of 60◦ was used in
(see Fig. 2B; unpaired t-test: P < 0.05; n = 100). If one the experiments, so that the visual expansion would be
of the targets had a visual angle of 20◦ and the other similar to that of the other target objects. The length of
had a visual angle of 40◦ , the larger object was the each side of the triangle was equal to the length of each
126 K. Hyden, K. Kral / Behavioural Processes 70 (2005) 122–131

Fig. 3. Graphs indicating the extent to which mantid larvae distinguish between a black square and a black triangle with the same (A, B) and
different (C, D) visual angles, when selecting a jump target. For significance levels, see text.

side of the square and to the diameter of the disk. Man- If both the triangle and the disk had visual angles of
tids jumped equally frequently to the square and the 20◦ , there was no significant difference in the jump
triangle when both objects had the same visual angle frequency (see Fig. 4A; n = 100). However, if both tar-
of 20◦ or 40◦ (see Fig. 3A and B; n = 100). If the trian- gets had visual angles of 40◦ , the triangle was the jump
gle was larger than the square, the mantids jumped to it target significantly more often than the disk (unpaired
significantly more frequently. However, if the triangle t-test, P < 0.05; see Fig. 4B; n = 100). In this experi-
had a visual angle of 20◦ and the square had a visual ment as well, the larger of the two objects was more
angle of 40◦ , the square was the jump target signifi- frequently selected as the jump target (see Fig. 4C and
cantly more often (see Fig. 3C and D; chi-square test: D; chi-square test: P < 0.001; n = 100).
P < 0.001; n = 100).
3.4. Black square, black triangle and round black
3.3. Black triangle and round black disk disk

In the third experiment the mantid larvae were pre- In a further experiment each mantid larva was
sented with a black triangle and a round black disk. simultaneously presented with a black square, a black
K. Hyden, K. Kral / Behavioural Processes 70 (2005) 122–131 127

Fig. 4. Graphs indicating the extent to which mantid larvae distinguish between a black triangle and a round black disk with the same (A, B)
and different (C, D) visual angles, when selecting a jump target. For significance levels, see text.

triangle and a round black disk, all having the same 3.5. Square, triangle and round disk shaded with a
visual angle. If all of the objects had visual angles of gradation from white to black
20◦ , the square was the jump target more frequently
than the triangle, and significantly more frequently In an additional experiment a square, a triangle and
than the disk (unpaired t-test, P < 0.05); the triangle a round disk, all with the same visual angle and shaded
was more frequently the jump target than the disk, with a gradation from white at the top to black at the bot-
but the difference was not significant (see Fig. 5A; tom, were presented to the mantid larvae. With visual
n = 100). Similar results were found for objects with angles of 20◦ , the square was more often the jump target
visual angles of 40◦ (see Fig. 5B; n = 100). Here, the than the triangle (unpaired t-test, P < 0.05) or the disk
disk was less frequently the jump target than the square (P < 0.05). The difference between the triangle and the
(P < 0.01) or the triangle (P < 0.05), and there was also disk was not significant (see Fig. 6A; n = 100). With a
a significant difference between the square and the tri- visual angle of 40◦ , the square was likewise more often
angle (P < 0.05). The tendency of the mantids to exhibit the jump target than the triangle (P < 0.001) or the disk
preferences among the three shapes was thus more (P < 0.001), while the difference between triangle and
pronounced than in the case of the smaller objects. disk was not significant (see Fig. 6B; n = 100).
128 K. Hyden, K. Kral / Behavioural Processes 70 (2005) 122–131

Fig. 6. Graphs indicating the extent to which mantid larvae distin-


guish between a square, a triangle and a round disk shaded with a
gradation from white to black, with visual angles of 20◦ (A) and 40◦
(B), when selecting a jump target. For significance levels, see text.

Fig. 5. Graphs indicating the extent to which mantid larvae distin- forward in such a way that the line of movement of the
guish between a black square, a black triangle and a round black disk
eyes was always normal to the vertical part of the target
with visual angles of 20◦ (A) and 40◦ (B), when selecting a jump
target. For significance levels, see text. edges. It was found that the shape and size of the target
had no influence on the general course of the peering
movements.
3.6. Peering behaviour Fig. 7 shows the amplitude and velocity of peering
movements executed prior to jumps. A comparison of
For each size and shape of target, video recordings the amplitude (mean ± S.D.) of the peering movements
of 10 peering sequences were made directly from above showed no difference for targets of different shapes
or from the side. Analyses of these recordings of object- with a visual angle of 20◦ (see Fig. 7A). For targets
related peering movements preceding jumps showed a with a visual angle of 40◦ , there was no significant dif-
linear side-to-side shift of the head once or twice for ference in the peering amplitudes for the square and the
all targets. By means of compensatory counter-rotation triangle (see Fig. 7B). For the round disk, however, the
about the yaw axis, the head was constantly directed magnitude of the peering amplitude was significantly
K. Hyden, K. Kral / Behavioural Processes 70 (2005) 122–131 129

Fig. 7. Graphs indicating the amplitude (A, B) and velocity (C, D) of peering movements immediately prior to a jump, for different object sizes
and shapes. Results of peering movements oriented toward targets where jumps did not take place are not shown, because they are very similar.
For significance levels, see text.

greater than in the case of the other objects (unpaired squares and triangles, but not for the disks. The veloc-
t-test, P < 0.001). A comparison of the peering ampli- ity was greater in the case of the smaller targets (t-test,
tudes for targets having the same shape, but with visual P < 0.01). It should be noted that due to the temporal
angles of 20◦ and 40◦ , showed a difference only for resolution of the video recordings (30 frames per sec-
squares. The peering amplitude was greater for squares ond), the estimated error for the values could vary from
of 20◦ than for those of 40◦ (t-test, P < 0.01). approximately ±4 to ±7%; however, the trends could
The velocity of peering movements (mean ± S.D.) be readily discerned despite this degree of error.
did not differ significantly in the case of the square,
triangle and round disk, with a visual angle of 20◦ (see 3.7. Jumping behaviour
Fig. 7C). However, for objects with a visual angle of
40◦ , the peering velocity was greater for the disk than More than 1600 jumps to targets were recorded, and
for the triangle or square (t-test, P < 0.01) (see Fig. 7D). an additional 100 jumps were examined by means of
A comparison of the peering velocity for targets hav- video analysis. Landing was successful in every case,
ing the same shape, but with visual angles of 20◦ and and took place primarily on the vertical, oblique, or
40◦ , showed a significant difference in the values for curved lateral part of the edges. In some cases, landing
130 K. Hyden, K. Kral / Behavioural Processes 70 (2005) 122–131

occurred on horizontal edges, but never in the middle expected to result in a greater flow of information con-
of an object. cerning the retinal image motion.
However, the experiments with two targets show
that with edges greater than a certain length, i.e. in
4. Discussion the case of objects with a visual angle of 40◦ , whether
or not the edges are straight or curved plays a role. It
The behavioural experiments with mantid larvae was found that the square and triangle were selected as
(Mantis religiosa) in the present study show that, with jump targets more frequently than the round disk. It is
the exception of mantids that do not attempt to leave noteworthy that the amplitude and velocity of peering
the island, each animal begins object-related peering movements oriented toward the round disk are greater
movements as soon as it faces a stationary object so than is the case for the square and triangle. Since the
that the object is within its visual field. The target edges peering movements are executed in such a way that the
appear to be the significant visual factor, rather than line of movement of the eyes is always precisely hori-
the surface of the target. Peering movements seem to zontal, it may be expected that the retinal image motion
be directed only toward the edges, and landing occurs associated with vertical and oblique edges could result
only on the edges, never on the surface of the target. in greater motion contrasts or in data that are more
The target selection behaviour also indicates that the readily processed than is the case with curved or hori-
precise size of the surface is not a determining factor. zontal edges. It is possible that the peering movements
For instance, in the case of a visual angle of 20◦ , all of mantids are accentuated in the case of curved edges
shapes are chosen equally often, although the size of because such edges generate weaker motion signals
the surface area varies from 23 to 48%; this is also than straight edges of a corresponding length. It has also
the case for square and triangular targets with a visual been found that peering movements can be adjusted to
angle of 40◦ . The present findings correspond to those distance, and that they are accentuated with increasing
for other insects, where visual orientation behaviour distance (see Collett, 1978; Poteser and Kral, 1995).
is found to be determined by the extension of edges The amplification of the peering movements in the case
and contrast lines, rather than by the size of the sur- of disks suggests that curved edges generate weaker
face area of objects (e.g. Wehner, 1981; Srinivasan et motion signals than is the case with squares or tri-
al., 1990). In freely walking flies, detection of edges angles. Mantids may therefore perceive the disks as
occurs as soon as the edges have a visual extension being further away, which could account for the pref-
larger than approximately 10◦ (Wehner, 1972). In the erence for square and triangular jump targets. Thus, it
peering-jump behaviour of mantids, it has been found is possible that the attraction of an object as a jump
that edges are detected if at least approximately 9 target may be determined simply by the strength of the
vertically arranged or 11 obliquely arranged forward- motion signals, without involving explicit shape deter-
looking ommatidia (<1% of all the ommatidia of mination. Further work is needed to investigate whether
each compound eye), or at least 42–54 photorecep- the system is this simple, or whether the mantid has
tor cells (blue-green receptors; see e.g. Heisenberg and independent shape determination mechanisms.
Buchner, 1977), are stimulated simultaneously in each It should be noted that an evolutionary (rather than a
of the two compound eyes (see Köck, 1992; Köck mechanistic) aspect may be associated with the present
et al., 1993). findings. In previous studies, it has been found that ver-
Furthermore, it appears that an object is more read- tical structures are predominant in the natural visual
ily identified as a jump target as the length of its edges surroundings of Mantis religiosa (Kral and Devetak,
increases. Longer edges result in the stimulation of 1999; see also Coppola et al., 1998). The precisely
more ommatidia and photoreceptor cells. With a visual horizontal movement of the eyes during peering move-
angle of 40◦ , the largest possible number of forward- ments might thus be an adaptation for distance determi-
looking ommatidia (18–22) and photoreceptor cells nation of the vertical edges that predominate in the nat-
(108–132) are stimulated simultaneously in each of ural environment. This might likewise help to account
the two compound eyes (see Köck, 1992). The larger for the fact that vertical edges are preferred as jump tar-
number of photoreceptors involved in peering can be gets. This preference is apparent in the selection among
K. Hyden, K. Kral / Behavioural Processes 70 (2005) 122–131 131

three objects, particularly in the case of the objects Köck, A., Jakobs, A.-K., Kral, K., 1993. Visual prey discrimination
shaded from white to black, which may more closely in monocular and binocular praying mantis Tenodera sinensis
approximate natural conditions. It is possible that three during postembryonic development. J. Insect Physiol. 39, 485–
491.
objects positioned equidistantly around the arena may Kral, K., Poteser, M., 1997. Motion parallax as a source of distance
be easier to compare, because two objects are always information in locusts and mantids. J. Insect Behav. 10, 145–
within view; however, in the case of two objects posi- 163.
tioned at an angle of 120◦ from one another, only one Kral, K., 1998a. Side-to-side movements to obtain motion depth
cues: a short review of research on the praying mantis. Behav.
may be seen at a time. One reason why the preference
Proc. 43, 71–77.
for the square is more apparent for objects shaded from Kral, K., 1998b. Spatial vision in the course of an insect’s life. Brain
white to black than in the case of solid black objects Behav. Evol. 52, 1–6.
may be that the shaded objects offer less contrast to the Kral, K., 1999. Binocular vision and distance estimation. In: Prete,
white background, thus making distance determination F.R., Wells, H., Wells, P.H., Hurd, L.E. (Eds.), The Praying Man-
tids: Research Perspectives. Johns Hopkins University Press,
more difficult. Whereas in the case of the solid black
Baltimore, pp. 114–140.
objects, the greater overall size of the square does not Kral, K., Devetak, D., 1999. The visual orientation strategies of
appear to play a role, in the case of the objects shaded Mantis religiosa and Empusa fasciata reflect differences in the
from white to black, the fact that the square has the structure of their visual surroundings. J. Insect Behav. 12, 737–
largest size and the greatest total edge length may be 752.
significant. It is possible that the greater length of the Kral, K., 2003a. Behavioural-analytical studies of the role of head
movements in depth perception in insects, birds and mammals.
contrasting boundaries could facilitate distance deter- Behav. Proc. 64, 1–12.
mination in this case. Kral, K., 2003b. Perspectives on the role of head movements in depth
perception. Behav. Proc. 64, 21–22.
Kral, K., Prete, F.R., 2004. In the mind of a hunter: the visual world of
Acknowledgements praying mantis. In: Prete, F.R. (Ed.), Complex Worlds from Sim-
pler Nervous Systems. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 75–115.
Leitinger, G., 1994. Frühe postembryonale Entwicklung des Kom-
We would like to thank Mary Ansell for correcting plexauges und der Lamina ganglionaris der Gottesanbeterin nach
the English text and Gerlinde Pauluzzi for the breeding Photodegeneration der akuten Zone mit Sulforhodamin. Master’s
of Drosophila. We would also like to express our appre- Thesis. University of Graz.
ciation of the valuable comments on the manuscript Poteser, M., Kral, K., 1995. Visual distance discrimination in praying
mantis larvae: an index of the use of motion parallax. J. Exp. Biol.
made by Prof. Tom Collett. This study has been sup- 198, 2127–2137.
ported by a research grant from the Austrian Science Poteser, M., 1998. Die Bedeutung der Bewegungsparallaxe für die
Foundation (grant no. P14697-Bio to K.K.). räumliche Orientierung von Insekten am Beispiel der Gottesan-
beterin Tenodera sinensis. Doctoral Thesis. University of Graz.
Poteser, M., Pabst, M.-A., Kral, K., 1998. Proprioceptive contribution
to distance estimation by motion parallax in a praying mantid. J.
References Exp. Biol. 201, 1483–1491.
Sobel, E.C., 1990. The locust’s use of motion parallax to measure
Collett, T.S., 1978. Peering—a locust behaviour pattern for obtaining distance. J. Comp. Physiol. 167, 579–588.
motion parallax information. J. Exp. Biol. 76, 237–241. Srinivasan, M.V., Lehrer, M., Horridge, G.A., 1990. Visual figure-
Coppola, D.M., Purves, H.R., McCoy, A.N., Purves, D., 1998. The ground discrimination in the honeybee: the role of motion paral-
distribution of orientation contours in the real world. Proc. Natl. lax at boundaries. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 238, 331–350.
Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 4002–4006. Walcher, F., Kral, K., 1994. Visual deprivation and distance estima-
Heisenberg, M., Buchner, E., 1977. The role of retinula cell types in tion in the praying mantis larvae. Physiol. Entomol. 19, 230–240.
visual behavior of Drosophila melanogaster. J. Comp. Physiol. Wallace, G.K., 1959. Visual scanning in the desert locust Schisto-
117, 127–162. cerca gregaria Forskål. J. Exp. Biol. 36, 512–525.
Horridge, G.A., 1986. A theory of insect vision: velocity parallax. Wehner, R., 1972. Spontaneous pattern preferences of Drosophila
Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 229, 13–27. melanogaster to black areas in various parts of the visual field.
Köck, A., 1992. Morphologische und optische Indizien für eine J. Insect Physiol. 18, 1250–1268.
postembryonale Entwicklung des binokularen Mechanismus der Wehner, R., 1981. Spatial vision in arthropods. In: Autrum, H. (Ed.),
Entfernungsmessung bei der Gottesanbeterin Tenodera sinensis. Handbook of Sensory Physiology, vol. VII/6C. Springer Verlag,
Master’s Thesis. University of Graz. Berlin, pp. 287–616.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi