Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 10

Brain and Cognition 124 (2018) 47–56

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Brain and Cognition


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c

The neural correlates of attachment security in typically developing children T


a,b b,c,d e f a,⁎
Eun Jung Choi , Margot J. Taylor , Soon-Beom Hong , Changdai Kim , Soon-Hyung Yi
a
Department of Child and Family Studies, College of Human Ecology, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea
b
Department of Diagnostic Imaging, Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Canada
c
Neuroscience & Mental Health Program, Hospital for Sick Children Research Institute, Toronto, Canada
d
Department of Psychology and Medical Imaging, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
e
Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, Seoul National University Hospital, Seoul, South Korea
f
Department of Education, College of Education, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: This study investigated neural correlates of children’s attachment security using functional magnetic resonance
Attachment security imaging. Fifty-one boys’ attachment styles (age mean = 9.5 years, SD = 0.61) were assessed with the Separation
fMRI Anxiety Test (SAT). We created an fMRI version of the SAT to activate children’s attachment system in fMRI
Striatum environment and contrasted two conditions in which children were instructed to infer the specific feeling of the
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
boy in the picture or to identify objects or physical activities. In the final fMRI analysis (N = 21), attachment
Amygdala
security could be detected at the neural level corresponding to the behavioural differences in the attachment
interview. Securely attached children showed greater activation in the frontal, limbic and basal ganglia area
which included the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, amygdala, cingulate cortex and striatum, compared to other
children who had lower quality of attachment. These regions have a key role in socio-emotional information
processing and also represent a brain network related to approach and avoidance motivation in humans.
Especially the striatum, strongly linked to reward processing underpinning social approach and avoidance
motivation, showed the largest effects in these differences and also positively correlated with emotional open-
ness scores in SAT. This suggests that the quality of attachment configures the approach and avoidance moti-
vational system in our brain mediated by the striatum.

1. Introduction that children rely on the caregiver’s availability as a source of comfort


and protection when they feel unsettled or fearful of something in the
1.1. Attachment security environment, while those with insecure attachment have not experi-
enced consistent availability from their caregivers and become anxious,
Attachment is a deep and strong emotional bond that connects an expressing fear or anger that their caregivers are not responsive when
infant to a caregiver at an early stage of life and it is characterized by needed (Ainsworth et al., 2015). Insecure attachment style can take the
specific approach behaviours in children to seek proximity with the form of anxious attachment and avoidant attachment (Ainsworth et al.,
attachment figure, especially when upset or threatened (Ainsworth, 2015). Attachment anxiety is predicted by the receipt of unreliable or
1973; Bowlby, 1969). It is almost universal that infants develop at- unpredictable caregiving, whereas experiences of rejection by care-
tachment relationships, but there are individual differences in how ef- givers predict the development of an avoidant attachment style. In-
fectively the infants and children can use their caregivers as a source of dividuals high in avoidant attachment style dismiss the importance of
comfort in the face of danger or threats from the environment and how attachment bonds, while anxiously attached individuals are hypervi-
balanced they are between exploration and seeking proximity to the gilant for signs of social rejection, and readily admit their longing for
caregiver in various situations. Attachment security, that refers to these improved attachment relationships. Over time, each group tends to
individual differences in the quality of the attachment relationship, is develop a diverse and mixed set of profiles, but a number of long-
well-established at a behavioural level and has been classically divided itudinal studies on attachment have shown that these differences in
into two categories: “secure” and “insecure” attachment relationships attachment security are stable across the life span from infancy to
(Ainsworth et al., 2015; Bowlby, 1973). Secure attachment indicates adulthood (Waters, Merrick, Treboux, Crowell, & Albersheim, 2000)


Corresponding author at: Dept. of Child Development & Family Studies, Seoul National University, Seoul, South Korea and Dept. of Diagnostic Imaging, Hospital for Sick Children,
Toronto, Canada.
E-mail addresses: eunjchoi3@gmail.com (E.J. Choi), ysh@snu.ac.kr (S.-H. Yi).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2018.04.003
Received 11 December 2017; Received in revised form 14 April 2018; Accepted 15 April 2018
0278-2626/ © 2018 Published by Elsevier Inc.
E.J. Choi et al. Brain and Cognition 124 (2018) 47–56

and even extend across generations (Benoit & Parker, 1994). expression in individuals who have a different styles of attachment.
One reason that attachment security has a prolonged effect on an One research group studied the attachment system specifically in
individual’s life is that attachment is basically supported by a physio- the fMRI environment, to examine the functional neuroanatomy of at-
logical system that is biologically predisposed and selected during tachment security. Buchheim et al. (2006) used attachment-related
evolution for survival (Bowlby, 1969, 1982). The attachment beha- scenes to activate the attachment system in adult patients with bor-
vioural system can be viewed as a physiological adaptation and derline personality disorder (BPD). The BPD patients showed sig-
homeostasis, such that attachment security can be understood as a re- nificantly more anterior midcingulate cortex activity in response to
sult of adapting the attachment behavioural system to the caregiving pictures which depicted characters facing attachment threats alone and
environment. A second reason that attachment security is stable over their unresolved attachment was associated with increasing amygdala
time is that from interactions with primary caregivers, children develop activation (Buchheim et al., 2006). Here we employed this paradigm
mental representations of the self and others, called “internal working with healthy children to determine the functional neuroanatomy of
models”, which are internalized in one’s mind and utilized as a source attachment security and to investigate differences in brain activity in
of how to react to socio-emotional cues in close relationships and more children with different levels of attachment security.
generalized social contexts (Bretherton & Munholland, 2008). How-
ever, the neural substrates underlying this stable attachment security 2. Material and methods
remain largely unknown.
2.1. Participants
1.2. The neural basis of attachment security
Fifty-one boys were recruited from elementary schools (age
There is growing empirical evidence that individual differences in mean = 9.5 years, SD = 0.61). After receiving informed consent,
attachment security are associated with different neurobiological trained examiners visited their home and assessed attachment security
functions in the processing of perceptual information (Cohen & Shaver, using the Separation Anxiety Test (SAT; Hansburg, 1972; Resnick,
2004; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007), especially in emotional information. 1993). The SAT is a semi-projective interview using separation pictures
Regions in prefrontal, limbic and basal ganglia are thought to be re- to assess attachment from preschool-aged children to adolescents. All
sponsible for individual differences in attachment security. However, interviews were audiotaped, transcribed verbatim, and coded according
the amygdalae also are often related to attachment security. During to the SAT manual (Resnick, 1993). The SAT rating scales include nine
processing various emotional information such as social feedback in subscales (e.g., Emotional openness, Devaluing of attachment, Self-
gaming situations (Vrticka, Lordier, Bediou, & Sander, 2014), emo- blame, Resistance/Withholding, Preoccupied anger, Displacement of
tional faces (Norman, Lawrence, Iles, Benattayallah, & Karl, 2014; feelings, Anxiety, Coherence of transcript, Solutions), and assign each
Redlich et al., 2015; Vrticka, Andersson, Grandjean, Sander, & child to one of the attachment classifications based on the profile in
Vuilleumier, 2008), attachment-related stress (Lemche et al., 2006; Liu, each subscale (e.g., Secure, Insecure_avoidant, Insecure_preoccupied).
Ding, Lu, & Chen, 2017), emotional adjectives (Debbané et al., 2017), As a result, twenty-nine boys (56.9%) were classified as secure at-
emotional regulation (Moutsiana et al., 2014), or mother’s face tachment and twenty-two boys (43.1%) were classified as insecure at-
(Tottenham, Shapiro, Telzer, & Humphreys, 2012), amygdalae showed tachment. Secure attachment is prevalent in diverse populations and
different patterns of activity in individuals with different attachment children in this group show a diverse set of profiles that are considered
styles. These results suggested that the different sensitivity for emo- more qualitative rather than quantitative (Diamond & Marrone, 2003).
tional cues may be modulated by attachment security in the amygdalae. Based on the SAT coding system, only fourteen boys in secure group
The basal ganglia serve a wide range of functions, including motor, could be assigned into the prototypical “securely attached” category
cognitive, motivational, and emotional processes (Arsalidou, Duerden, with very high scores for emotional openness compared to the others in
& Taylor, 2013). Consistent with this broad-reaching involvement in secure group who revealed restricted feelings. Insecure attachment is
brain function, basal ganglia dysfunction has been implicated in nu- represented by two distinct patterns, avoidant or ambivalent type. In
merous neurological and psychiatric disorders. Recent research re- our study, nineteen out of twenty-two in the insecure group showed
vealed a critical role for brain reward systems, involving the basal avoidant attachment style. All subjects who declined the brain scan or
ganglia, in the biological underpinnings of attachment security, as had a history of psychiatric or neurological illness, left handedness, or
neural responses to facial expression (Donges et al., 2012; Vrticka et al., metal in their body were excluded. Additionally, eight subjects were
2008), reward processing (Takiguchi et al., 2015), or the perception of also excluded from more than 4 mm head motion during the scan. Fi-
mother’s face (Minagawa-Kawai et al., 2008). There is mounting evi- nally, 21 boys were included: seven boys were in the prototypical se-
dence for a role of reward circuits and reinforcing processes in social curely attached group, another seven were securely attached but re-
approach and bonding in maternal and romantic love (Aron et al., stricted in expressing feelings and seven boys were in the insecurely
2005; Fisher, Aron, & Brown, 2005; Lenzi et al., 2008; Nitschke et al., attached group, six of whom had avoidant attachment type. Demo-
2004; Ranote et al., 2004; Sander, Frome, & Scheich, 2007). graphic information is presented in Table 1. Parents’ educational level
Most of research on the neural basis of attachment security, how- and social economic status were similar across all participants. All
ever, has studied adult populations, although attachment security is parents reported that the main caregiver of the child during the first
formed at an early stage of life. Investigating the neural underpinnings three years was the mother. This study was approved by the institu-
of attachment security in childhood would allow us to determine if the tional review board for human subjects at the Seoul National Uni-
adult model applies to younger cohorts, as well as determining possible versity. All children and their parents provided written informed con-
developmental differences in this complex social behaviour. sent prior to study entry.
Furthermore, most research has used relatively generalized emotional
stimuli such as facial expressions to examine functional regions related 2.2. Stimulus materials and procedure
to attachment security, but attachment security is classically assessed
by activating one’s attachment system through presenting negative at- We constructed an fMRI version of the SAT to activate children’s
tachment-related stimuli, such as strange situations in infancy and se- attachment system and to elicit mental engagement with attachment-
paration pictures in childhood and adolescence. This leaves open the related experiences in the MRI environment. The stimulus material
question of whether or not the differences in brain activity during consisted of attachment-related pictures which originally came from
processing facial expressions would reflect the neural basis of attach- other versions of SAT, the Adult Attachment Projective Picture System
ment security, or would be simply distinct neural responses to facial (AAP), or neuroimaging studies that measured neural response of the

48
E.J. Choi et al. Brain and Cognition 124 (2018) 47–56

Table 1
Demographics and neuropsychological assessments.
Attachment security type Test statistics

Prototypical secure (Mean ± SD) Secure but restricted (Mean ± SD) Insecure (Mean ± SD)

Children’s age 9.60 ± 0.52 9.71 ± 0.41 9.42 ± 0.78 F = 0.45

Father’s education level


University or more 7 (100%) 5 (71.4%) 4 (57.1%) χ2(2)=3.68
High school graduation 0 (0%) 2 (28.6%) 3 (42.9%)

Mother’s education level


University or more 7 (100%) 4 (5731%) 5 (71.4%) χ2(2)=3.68
High school graduation 0 (0%) 3 (42.9%) 2 (28.6%)
SES
High 1 (14.3%) 0 (0%) 1 (14.3%) χ2(2)=2.12
Middle 5 (71.4%) 6 (85.7%) 6 (85.7%)
Low 1 (14.3%) 1 (14.3%) 0 (0%)

Attachment Security Score 52.43 ± 2.70 51.29 ± 6.63 47.29 ± 7.57 F = 1.41

IQ (K-WISC-IV)
VCI 123.43 ± 12.38 116.71 ± 18.48 115.00 ± 12.95 F = 0.59
PRI 122.71 ± 12.72 112.86 ± 16.43 106.00 ± 20.42 F = 1.68
WMI 116.14 ± 12.93 117.57 ± 15.13 95.00 ± 12.15 F = 5.50*
PSI 94.43 ± 2.88 107.86 ± 12.84 96.50 ± 13.53 F = 3.14
FSIQ 121.43 ± 10.77 118.71 ± 15.06 105.83 ± 14.73 F = 2.37

CBCL total score 22.86 ± 19.44 22.00 ± 12.88 15.14 ± 10.06 F = 0.42

*
p < .05.

Table 2
Examples of the experimental conditions.
Attachment condition Control condition

Target stimuli

Neutral stimuli

activation of attachment system for adults (Buchheim et al., 2006, scenes. The result of these ranking showed a significant difference be-
2008). These pictures depicted attachment-related distress such as se- tween two sets of stimuli (t = −17.78, p < .001).
paration, loneliness, death, or threat using black and white line drawing In the attachment condition, children were instructed to infer the
and contained the essential components to recognize the context specific feeling of the boy in the picture while the scene was presented,
without facial expressions. Each picture was combined with a question and then answer using a button box when the response screen came up.
“How does the child feel now?” to establish an attachment condition, In the control condition, children were instructed to identify objects or
designed for subjects to focus on the emotional component. For the activities according to the questions while the scene was presented, and
control condition, the same pictures were combined with a different then to answer using button box when the response screen came up as
question, asking about physical characteristics (e.g., “Where is the child well. They had a training session prior to entering the scanner, so the
sitting now?”, “What is that beside the child?”, etc.). Thus, we could task and instructions were clearly understood. Each block was assigned
compare directly the neural activations when children’s attachment either attachment condition or control condition and it started with an
system was activated or not. Neutral stimuli were included as filler instruction screen for 5.4 s and then one of two targets and one neutral
items and consisted of pictures which portrayed adult-child dyads but stimuli were presented in random order for 6 s each followed by a re-
without an emotional component and were also combined with two sponse screen for 3.8 s and a fixation cross for 1 s (see Fig. 1). Each run
different questions in each condition (Table 2). To validate the stimuli, had six blocks and it took about 5 min. We repeated this paradigm 3
a separate group of twenty-nine after-school-program students con- times so the entire session lasted 15.4 min.
ducted valence ratings between attachment-related scenes and neutral

49
E.J. Choi et al. Brain and Cognition 124 (2018) 47–56

Fig. 1. Experimental design.

2.3. Neuropsychological assessment ion,ucl.ac.uk/spm), all functional images for each participant were: (a)
realigned to correct for head motion; (b) spatially normalized into an
Neuropsychological measures were used to assess children’s IQ, EPI image in SPM template for between-group analysis; and (c)
behavioural problems, and self-reported attachment security. A mea- smoothed using a 8-mm full-width, half-maximum isotropic Gaussian
sure of IQ was obtained using the Korean version of Wechsler kernel. Following image conversion and preprocessing, the imaging
Intelligence Scale for Children Fourth Edition (K-WISC-IV: Gwak, Oh, & data were analyzed using SPM8. For each participant, condition effects
Kim, 2011), which included Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), Per- (attachment targets vs. control targets) were estimated according to the
ceptual Reasoning Index (PRI), Working Memory Index (WMI), Pro- general linear model, using a canonical hemodynamic response func-
cessing Speed Index (PSI). The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) was tion. The resulting contrast images were entered into second-level
also completed by a caregiver to identify behavioural problems in analyses using a random effects model to allow for inferences to be
children (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). We also used the Attachment made at the population level (Friston, Holmes, Price, Büchel, &
Security Scale (Kerns, Aspelmeier, Gentzler, & Grabill, 2001), a self- Worsley, 1999). We investigated the overall brain activation in each
reported questionnaire from children, to assess children’s perceptions of group by one-sample t-test; (1) prototypical secure (PS) (2) secure but
attachment quality. restricted (RS) (3) insecure (IS). Then we used SPM full factorial model
with group (3) by condition (2). Comparisons between conditions in
2.4. fMRI data acquisition whole-brain activation maps were first thresholded at uncorrected
p < .001 to see overall tendencies and we then applied a family wise
Images were acquired using Siemens Tim Trio 3.0 Tesla with 32 error correction p < .05. To examine any correlations between brain
channel head coil. fMRI data were obtained using T2-weighted gra- activity and attachment rating scores, the ROIs were created using peak
dient-echo EPI pulse sequences. Sponge padding was placed around the MNI coordinates based on the results which showed group differences.
head to minimize head movement and the lower jaw was fixed with A 5-mm-diameter sphere was drawn surrounding the peak MNI co-
tape. Participants watched the monitor through a mirror on the head ordinates.
coil, and held the button box in their right hand. MRI data were ac-
quired using the following parameters (T2-weighted gradient-echo EPI; 3. Results
TR = 2700 ms; TE = 30 ms; flip angle = 90°, matrix = 64 × 64). Each
volume was constructed by 3 mm thick interleaved slices parallel to 3.1. Group characteristics
anterior-posterior commissure line. High resolution T1-weighted
(MPRAGE) whole brain images were also obtained as an anatomical Table 1 contains the neuropsychological information for the three
underlay (T1-weighted images; voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm; groups of children. Children who were classified in secure attachment
TR = 1900 ms; TE = 2.36 ms; flip angel = 9°; matrix = 256 × 256). tended to have higher score in the self-reported attachment security
ratings, but there was no significant group difference. Children who
2.5. fMRI data analysis were classified in insecure attachment showed significantly lower
working memory index (F = 5.50, p = 0.014) (see Table 1), but the full
Using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8: Wellcome Department scale IQ was not significantly different from other groups. There were
of Cognitive Neurology, Institute of Neurology, London, UK; www.fil. no significant group differences in behavioural problems.

50
E.J. Choi et al. Brain and Cognition 124 (2018) 47–56

Table 3 Table 4
Overall brain activation to the contrast conditions in prototypical secure group. Overall brain activation to the contrast conditions in insecure group.
Regions BA L/R Peak voxel (x,y,z) k z Regions BA L/R Peak voxel (x,y,z) k z

Attachment > Control Control > Attachment


Inferior frontal gyrus 45 L −50 20 2 27 4.37 Precuneus 19 R 22 −80 38 67 4.55
Inferior frontal gyrus 47 L −42 24 −6 4.02 Precuneus 7 R 22 −68 38 4.28
Caudate R 10 12 −2 33 4.24 Middle temporal gyrus 39 R 30 −68 26 4.26
Caudate R 10 4 2 3.72 Precuneus 31 L −26 −76 14 116 4.41
Putamen R 18 4 6 3.48 Middle temporal gyrus 19 L −34 −80 22 4.31
Anterior cingulate gyrus 24 L −6 36 14 117 4.05 Cuneus 7 L −22 −80 34 4.04
Dorsolateral prefrontal gyrus 8 L −6 48 46 4.03 Inferior temporal gyrus 37 L −54 −56 −14 8 3.59
Dorsolateral prefrontal gyrus 8 R 6 40 46 4.00
Amygdala 34 L −18 −8 −22 5 4.00 p < .001(uncorrected), k5, BA: Brodmann area. L/R:left/right, peak
Anterior cingulate gyrus 24 R 6 32 6 8 3.52 voxel = MNI xyz co-ordinates, k = cluster size.
Midcingulate gyrus 31 R 10 −40 30 5 3.43
Middle frontal gyrus 8 R 38 16 46 6 3.25

p < .001(uncorrected), k > 5, BA: Brodmann area. L/R:left/right, peak


voxel = MNI xyz co-ordinates, k = cluster size.

3.2. Overall neural patterns in each group

First, we examined BOLD responses to the contrast images (at-


tachment targets > control targets vs control targets > attachment
targets) in each group.

3.2.1. Prototypical secure group


The prototypical secure group showed activations in left inferior
frontal gyrus, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the caudate and putamen
in basal ganglia area, and the anterior cingulate gyrus and amygdala to
attachment targets compared to control targets; no activation was sig-
nificantly greater to control targets compared to attachment targets
(Table 3 and Fig. 2).

3.2.2. Secure but restricted group


Fig. 3. Brain activation in insecure group: peak activations in precuneus.
The children who were assessed as the group of secure but restricted
showed no significant differences between the two conditions, in-
dicating that the neural activations of attachment targets and control main effects of conditions and the interactions between condition and
targets were processed similarly in this group. group (pcorr < 0.05). The main effects of condition were found mostly
in superior parietal lobule, superior frontal gyrus, and precuneus. Post-
3.2.3. Insecure group hoc analysis showed the attachment targets activated the superior
The insecure group showed the opposite pattern of brain activation frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, and medial frontal gyrus compared
compared the prototypical secure group. There were no significantly to control targets, but control targets were strongly related to the ac-
greater activations to attachment than control targets, but the pre- tivation of precuneus (Table 5 and Fig. 4).
cuneus, middle temporal gyrus, and inferior temporal gyrus were sig- The interaction of groups and conditions revealed significant
nificantly more activated to control compared attachment targets (pcorr < 0.05) activity in the basal ganglia including the globus pal-
(Table 4 and Fig. 3). lidus, putamen, subthalamic nucleus, and claustrum (Table 6 and
Fig. 5).
3.3. Group differences In a post-hoc analysis, the prototypical secure group children
showed greater activation (pcorr < 0.05) in regions of basal ganglia
In the factorial model of conditions (2) and groups (3), we found the during processing of the attachment targets compared to other two

Fig. 2. Brain activation in prototypical secure group: peak activations in inferior frontal gyrus (A), basal ganglia and striatum area (B), and anterior cingulate gyrus
and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (C).

51
E.J. Choi et al. Brain and Cognition 124 (2018) 47–56

Table 5 Table 6
Main effects of condition. Interaction between conditions and groups.
Regions BA L/R Peak voxel (x,y,z) k z Regions BA L/R Peak voxel k z

Main effects of conditions Globus pallidus L −14 0 −10 53 5.60


Superior parietal lobule 7 L −26 −72 34 215 7.22 Putamen L −22 8 −14 4.68
Precuneus 7 L −22 −72 58 4.44 Subthalamic nucleus L −6 −12 −10 4.67
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex 8 R 10 36 62 18 5.38 Globus pallidus R 10 0 −6 25 5.42
Precuneus 19 R 30 −76 38 15 4.75 Parahippocampal gyrus R 22 −4 −22 4.82
Precuneus 7 R 26 −68 30 4.42 Globus pallidus R 18 −4 −10 4.38
Precentral gyrus 6 L −38 4 30 10 4.40 Claustrum R 38 −8 6 23 4.93
Putamen R 22 −8 10 4.50
Post-hoc: Attachment > Control
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex 8 R 10 36 62 25 5.50
Middle temporal gyrus 21 L −58 0 −22 6 4.90
FWE p < .05, k5, BA: Brodmann area. L/R:left/right, peak voxel = MNI xyz
Inferior frontal gyrus 47 L −34 20 −18 7 4.78 co-ordinates, k = cluster size.
Medial frontal gyrus 10 L −10 44 −10 7 4.59

Post-hoc: Control > Attachment


Precuneus 19 L −26 −72 34 247 7.30
Precuneus 7 L −22 −72 58 4.58
Precuneus 7 L −10 −76 50 4.22
Precuneus 19 R 30 −76 38 41 4.87
Precuneus 7 R 26 −68 30 4.56
Middle occipital gyrus 19 R 34 −80 22 4.26
Precental gyrus 6 L −38 4 30 17 4.55

FWE p < .05, k5, BA: Brodmann area. L/R:left/right, peak voxel = MNI xyz
co-ordinates, k = cluster size.

groups (Table 7 and Fig. 6).


We also examined the correlation between the basal ganglia activity
and attachment rating scores. The ROIs were created using peak MNI
coordinates which showed significant effects on group by condition
interaction. We did not find any correlation with children’s reports for
attachment security (ASS scores) in basal ganglia activity, but did find
significantly positive correlations with the emotional openness score,
one of the most important subscales in the Separation Anxiety Test
rating scales, in the left (x,y,z coordinates = −14, 0, 10; dia- Fig. 5. Interaction between condition and group: peak activations in basal
meter = 5 mm; r = 0.559; p < .008) and right globus pallidus (x,y,z ganglia.
coordinates = 10, 0, −6; diameter = 5 mm; r = 0.502; p < .020).
4.1. Frontal areas
4. Discussion
Inferior, medial and dorsolateral prefrontal areas were active during
In this study, we investigated the neural bases of attachment se- the processing of emotions in attachment-related scenes in children
curity in normally developing children by activating their attachment with prototypical secure attachment. These frontal cortical areas are
system in the fMRI environment. The results showed that children ex- related to modulating evaluative judgments (Cunningham, Johnson,
hibited different patterns of neural activity according to their level of Gatenby, Gore, & Banaji, 2003). The prefrontal contributions to affec-
attachment security in several frontal, limbic, and basal ganglia areas. tive evaluation are known for integrating information regarding

Fig. 4. Post-hoc analysis results for the main effect of condition brain activity of: (A) the medial prefrontal cortex in the attachment condition, (B) the precuneus in
the control condition.

52
E.J. Choi et al. Brain and Cognition 124 (2018) 47–56

Table 7 attachment-related scenes in this study depicted a boy, who was being
Comparisons between groups for brain activation in attachment condition. treated unfairly by his mother, and participants were asked to infer the
Regions BA L/R Peak voxel (x,y,z) k z boy’s emotional state. One of the most distinguishable features of the
prototypical securely attached children was a sense that they had al-
Prototypical secure > secure but restricted ready given thought to their feelings about parents and separation is-
globus pallidus R 10 0 −6 31 5.30
sues in general (Resnick, 1993). This did not seem to be a new topic to
amygdala R 22 −4 −22 4.96
putamen R 26 8 −10 52 5.11
them because they knew how they would feel and they had access to
claustrum R 38 −4 2 5.03 these feelings in a very consistent way. Another feature of this group
putamen R 26 −8 10 4.72 was that they displayed a strong use of self in their answers, that is, they
globus pallidus L −14 0 −6 13 4.82 could talk about feelings in an open and vulnerable way and related the
subthalamic nucleus L −6 −12 −10 5 4.65
pictured situation to their own experiences (i.e., “This boy feels sad
anterior cingulate gyrus 24 R 6 32 10 4.31
because he missed his parents… this happened to me and I was really
Prototypical secure > insecure
sad and lonely.”). Therefore, our findings in frontal areas suggest that
globus pallidus L −14 0 −10 92 7.68
globus pallidus R 10 0 −6 59 6.45 attachment security is functionally correlated with frontal areas which
claustrum R 38 −8 6 5.61 support the skill of understanding the mental states of others.
putamen R 26 8 −10 5.32
precuneus 31 R 26 −76 22 15 5.77
4.2. Limbic areas
cuneus 18 R 18 −80 18 5.46
cuneus 18 L −14 −88 18 9 5.76
amygdala R 22 −4 −22 5 4.40 The limbic system is well-known as the emotional network in our
brain and activation of these areas was not surprising as the task di-
FWE p < .05, k5, BA: Brodmann area. L/R:left/right, peak voxel = MNI xyz rectly contrasted two types of information, emotional or physical in-
co-ordinates, k = cluster size. formation. The anterior/midcingulate cortex and amygdala were acti-
vated during the processing of emotions in attachment-related scenes in
contextual variables, personal values and memories with information children with prototypical secure attachment.
provided by perceptual evaluative system. The medial prefrontal cortex Previous neuroimaging studies support the theory of dorsal-cogni-
has been found to be active in mentalizing tasks where participants tive vs. ventral-emotional functional dissociation within the anterior
infer the state of others (Frith & Frith, 2006), especially in making in- cingulate cortex (Bush, Luu, & Posner, 2000; Whalen et al., 1998), and
ferences about affective aspects (Völlm et al., 2006). especially the ventral ACC which was related to social feedback such as
Recently, attachment theory has expanded and connected with the being accepted or rejected (Somerville, Heatherton, & Kelley, 2006),
term “mentalization”. The ability to mentalize, the ability to reflect and social pain (Tracey & Mantyh, 2007; Vogt & Sikes, 2009). The ACC
upon and understand one’s state of mind and to have insight into what is specialized for affective processes, whereas the midcingulate cortex
one is feeling, is closely linked with attachment security. Fonagy and (MCC) is more specialized for cognitive processes. More recent imaging
Bateman (2005) proposed that caregivers’ insightful understanding of studies implicate the MCC in the regulation of autonomic activity and
children’s experience with appropriate feedback helps children learn the perception and production of emotion (Kober et al., 2008), and
how to understand what they are experiencing. This modeling ulti- especially the anterior subdivision of MCC was linked to the experience
mately culminates in children learning to reflect upon, and understand of more intense states of negative affect (Mechias, Etkin, & Kalisch,
their own states of mind. Importantly, this progression depends on a 2010; Sehlmeyer et al., 2009), delivery of pain (Farrell, Laird, & Egan,
healthy and consistent emotional interaction between children and 2005; Rainville, Duncan, Price, Carrier, & Bushnell, 1997) and other
caregivers, which supports the building of a secure attachment. How- aversive stimuli (Peyron, Laurent, & Garcia-Larrea, 2000; Vogt, 2005).
ever, when early caregivers are unable to reflect on children’s state of Amygdala activity is also critical for fear learning (LeDoux, 2003), but
mind, children do not receive the instruction they require, via caregiver there is substantial evidence that amygdala activity codes more gen-
modeling, to develop this important capacity. Therefore, their learning erally for relevant and arousing stimuli (Sanders, Wiltgen, & Fanselow,
how to understand their own thoughts and feelings or thoughts and 2003), and this may be especially true during childhood, when arousing
feeling of others is negatively impacted. stimuli, regardless of valence, effectively recruit amygdala activity
We found connections between attachment security and the ability (Heatherton & Wagner, 2011).
of mentalization during attachment interviews with children. The Emotional openness and regulation are linked to the quality of

Fig. 6. Comparison between groups of brain activation in the attachment condition (6A: Prototypical secure > secure but restricted; 6B: Prototypical secure >
insecure).

53
E.J. Choi et al. Brain and Cognition 124 (2018) 47–56

attachment (Cassidy & Shaver, 2002). Being emotionally open and able study had avoidant attachment type and a key feature of avoidant at-
to verbalize feelings of vulnerability means that children can imagine tachment is to minimize or ignore their vulnerability of being attached
feelings of sadness, loneliness, anger and fear without experiencing to close others, and to overestimate their own ability to be independent.
overwhelming tension or conflict. The secure style of attachment When we directly compared the groups, basal ganglia was the most
manifests as open and flexible regulation whereas the avoidant style of important region to show the group differences according to the level of
attachment tends to minimize affect and thus can be understood in attachment including globus pallidus, claustrum, subthalamic nucleus.
terms of the overregulation of affect. These problems in emotional The globus pallidus and claustrum are implicated in initiating motiva-
dysregulation can be tied to various types of pathology (Gergely et al., tional behaviours and the computation of affective value (Arkadir,
2002). In our study, securely attached children tended to be surprised Morris, Vaadia, & Bergman, 2004; Wager, Davidson, Hughes, Lindquist,
when the mother was dealing with her son unfairly, and they could also & Ochsner, 2008). Especially the striatum, comprising of globus pal-
access feelings of needing and missing those close to them. For example, lidus, putamen, and caudate nucleus, has been shown to mediate be-
they verbalized some specific negative feelings clearly and sponta- tween socio-emotional information processing and attachment security
neously in a coherent manner (i.e., sad, angry, afraid, missing someone, in adult studies (Schneider et al., 2012; Vrtička & Vuilleumier, 2012;
etc.). In contrast, vague feelings such as ‘not good’, ‘strange’, or the Vrticka et al., 2008; Vrticka, 2012). However there is little research
failure to provide appropriate justifications are indicative of a lack of which demonstrates the role of the striatum as a functional basis of
emotional openness. attachment security in typically developing children. Our results sug-
A recent neuroimaging study showed that during childhood and gest that this pattern emerges in young childhood. Furthermore, we
adolescence, amygdala activity was preferentially recruited by images found an association of the globus pallidus activity and children’s
of the child’s mother and this activity correlated with a greater ten- emotional openness scores, during emotional processing in attachment-
dency to approach the mother, such as faster reaction times to maternal related stimuli. Emotional openness, one of the most important sub-
stimuli during an in-scanner behavioural task or an out-of scanner scales in the Separation Anxiety Test ratings, represents the extent to
child-report of attachment related behaviour (Tottenham et al., 2012). which the child is able to express feeling such as loneliness, sadness,
However it was not clear if amygdala activity was related to attachment fear, or anger and is a key aspect of secure attachment. This finding
or just in response to a familiar face or emotional arousal. In this study builds on the growing attention on the striatum as a neural basis of
we determined that amygdala activity differed according to the level of secure attachment by showing for the first time the association of the
attachment security, and thus could represent the neurobiology of at- globus pallidus activity, attachment classification, and emotional
tachment security. openness rating. Notably, we did not find correlations between brain
activity and children’s perception of attachment quality. There is evi-
4.3. Basal ganglia dence that at the beginning of middle childhood insecure patterns be-
come sex biased, with boys showing more avoidance and girls showing
The caudate and putamen are major components of the basal more anxiety (Del Giudice & Belsky, 2010). Thus, middle childhood
ganglia, and make up the dorsal striatum which is highly related to boys start to show traits such as aggression, independence and inflated
initiating motivationally relevant behaviours and reward processing. self-esteem and these traits may affect their perception of attachment
The caudate is known as a key region to control approach-attachment quality. Our data also showed that self-reported attachment security
behaviours in animal models (Burkett, Spiegel, Inoue, Murphy, & from children was not significantly different among the groups even
Young, 2011; Villablanca, 2010). These areas of the dopamine-rich though securely attached children tended to report higher attachment
basal ganglia system are widely accepted to be associated with in- security scores.
itiating motivational behaviours necessary for survival such as food
(Knutson and Greer, 2008), but also are activated by strong social sti- 4.4. Precuneus
muli such as maternal and long-term intense romantic love (Acevedo,
Aron, Fisher, & Brown, 2012; Bartels & Zeki, 2004), which means that In this study, the precuneus was the region most implicated for
social attachment in a close relationship would be considered as a physical vs. emotional information processing in attachment-related
survival-related and highly-rewarding cue in our brain. The child-mo- scenes; insecurely attached children showed precuneus activity only for
ther relationship, classically viewed as the first and strongest love physical compared to emotional information processing. Recent neu-
(Freud, 1949) and as the prototype of all later love relationships, not roimaging findings in healthy subjects suggest a central role for the
only forms the matrix on which subsequent personality development precuneus in a wide range of highly integrated tasks, including visuo-
builds, but also provides the motivational core of social behaviour spatial imagery, episodic memory retrieval and self-processing opera-
throughout the lifespan. tions, such as first-person perspective taking and experience of agency
Our findings showed that these areas were only activated by emo- (Cavanna & Trimble, 2006). Our control condition was designed to
tional processing of attachment-related scenes in the prototypical se- control the attachment system by guiding the focus to physical aspects
cure group. In the attachment interview, securely attached children had of the stimuli, such as identifying the objects beside the child, the
a strong sense of motivation for being reunited with the attachment spatial relationship of the objects, or the activities of the child. These
figure and optimism for the outcome of the separation. That is, they questions seemed to lead to an effective attentional shift to visuo-spatial
were obviously trying to regain the attachment figures or maintain imagery. Importantly, only insecurely attached children showed the
contact with them or seek comfort and support from them. They often strong activity in precuneus during the processing of physical in-
talked not only about their negative feelings during separation but also formation. Attachment-related scenes used in the attachment interview
that they would feel happy when reunited with their parents. This contain negative and aversive emotions, as their roles typically activate
finding can be interpreted that securely attached children responded to subjects’ attachment system. However, children in the avoidant at-
attachment-related cues at an automatic processing level, as those cues tachment group were generally unable to identify feelings, especially in
had been useful for their survival and rewarding. This was reflected at the more stressful pictures. They could provide understanding of what
the neural level by greater activation to attachment-related cues. was going on in the situation, hence the precuneus activation, but they
However, children who were lower on attachment security may lose were not able to connect these situations with specific and open feel-
their biological motivation to attachment-related cues as those cues had ings. There was a quality of “shutting-off” of attachment among these
not been useful for their survival or not rewarding. They seem to have children (Resnick, 1993). Some children in the avoidant attachment
developed a mechanism to not pay attention to these cues. This inter- group showed explicit refusal or resistance toward completing inter-
pretation is plausible as most of the insecurely attached children in our views or gave sparse or one-word answers. Some other of these children

54
E.J. Choi et al. Brain and Cognition 124 (2018) 47–56

seemed to express some open feelings, but immediately followed by (2006). Measuring attachment representation in an fMRI environment: A pilot study.
discounting, devaluing or minimizing these feelings. Psychopathology, 39(3), 144–152.
Burkett, J. P., Spiegel, L. L., Inoue, K., Murphy, A. Z., & Young, L. J. (2011). Activation of
μ-opioid receptors in the dorsal striatum is necessary for adult social attachment in
5. Conclusions monogamous prairie voles. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(11), 2200.
Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior
cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 215–222.
The present study determined the neural basis of attachment se- Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). (2002). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and
curity in typically developing children. We identified the neural activity clinical applications. Rough Guides.
when children’s attachment system was engaged in an fMRI environ- Cavanna, A. E., & Trimble, M. R. (2006). The precuneus: A review of its functional
anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain, 129(3), 564–583.
ment. Our data support the idea that attachment security could be Cohen, M., & Shaver, P. (2004). Avoidant attachment and hemispheric lateralisation of
detected at the neural level corresponding to the behavioural differ- the processing of attachment-and emotion-related words. Cognition and Emotion,
ences in the attachment interview. Securely and insecurely attached 18(6), 799–813.
Cunningham, W. A., Johnson, M. K., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., & Banaji, M. R. (2003).
children showed distinct patterns of neural activity in the frontal,
Neural components of social evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
limbic and basal ganglia area which included the dorsolateral pre- 85(4), 639.
frontal cortex, amygdala, cingulate cortex and striatum. These regions Debbané, M., Badoud, D., Sander, D., Eliez, S., Luyten, P., & Vrtička, P. (2017). Brain
have a key role in socio-emotional information processing and also activity underlying negative self-and other-perception in adolescents: The role of
attachment-derived self-representations. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral
represent a brain network related to approach and avoidance motiva- Neuroscience, 17(3), 554–576.
tion in humans (Spielberg et al., 2012). Especially the striatum, strongly Del Giudice, M., & Belsky, J. (2010). Sex differences in attachment emerge in middle
linked to reward processing underpinning social approach and avoid- childhood: An evolutionary hypothesis. Child Development Perspectives, 4(2), 97–105.
Diamond, N., & Marrone, M. (2003). Attachment and intersubjectivity. Whurr Publishers.
ance motivation, showed the largest effects in these differences. The Donges, U. S., Kugel, H., Stuhrmann, A., Grotegerd, D., Redlich, R., Lichev, V., ...
model of attachment is understood as a motivational and behavioural Dannlowski, U. (2012). Adult attachment anxiety is associated with enhanced auto-
system that directs children to seek proximity with a familiar caregiver matic neural response to positive facial expression. Neuroscience, 220, 149–157.
Farrell, M. J., Laird, A. R., & Egan, G. F. (2005). Brain activity associated with painfully
when they are alarmed. Our results indicate that the quality of at- hot stimuli applied to the upper limb: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1),
tachment configures the approach and avoidance motivational system 129–139.
in our brain, mediated by the striatum. Fisher, H., Aron, A., & Brown, L. L. (2005). Romantic love: An fMRI study of a neural
mechanism for mate choice. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 493(1), 58–62.
Fonagy, P., & Bateman, A. (2005). Attachment theory and mentalization-oriented model
Acknowledgments of borderline personality disorder. The American Psychiatric Publishing Textbook of
Personality Disorders, 187–207.
Freud, S. (1949). An outline of psycho-analysis (J. Strachey, Ed. & Trans.). The standard
For generous support, the authors thank Brain Imaging Center of
edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud, 23.
department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences in Seoul National Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Price, C. J., Büchel, C., & Worsley, K. J. (1999). Multisubject
University. fMRI studies and conjunction analyses. Neuroimage, 10(4), 385–396.
Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron, 50(4), 531–534.
Gergely, G., Fonagy, P., Jurist, E., & Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation, mentalization,
Financial disclosures and the development of the self.
Gwak, G., Oh, S., & Kim, C. (2011). K-WISC-IV (Korean Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
This research was supported by National Research Foundation of Children-IV). Manual for experts. Seoul: Hakjisa.
Hansburg, H. G. (1972). Adolescent separation anxiety: A method for the study of adolescent
Korea (NRF-2013S1A5A2A01019722/ NRF-2014S1A5B6038081). separation problems. Springfield, IL: Thomas.
Heatherton, T. F., & Wagner, D. D. (2011). Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation
References failure. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(3), 132–139.
Kerns, K. A., Aspelmeier, J. E., Gentzler, A. L., & Grabill, C. M. (2001). Parent-child at-
tachment and monitoring in middle childhood. Journal of Family Psychology, 15(1),
Acevedo, B. P., Aron, A., Fisher, H. E., & Brown, L. L. (2012). Neural correlates of marital 69–81.
satisfactions and well-being: Reward, empathy, and affect. Clinical Knutson, B., & Greer, S. M. (2008). Anticipatory affect: Neural correlates and con-
Neuropsychiatry, 9(1). sequences for choice. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Sciences, 363(1511), 3771–3786.
Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, Kober, H., Barrett, L. F., Joseph, J., Bliss-Moreau, E., Lindquist, K., & Wager, T. D. (2008).
& Families. Functional grouping and cortical–subcortical interactions in emotion: A meta-ana-
Ainsworth, M. (1973). The development of infant-mother attachment [w:] B. Caldwell, lysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage, 42(2), 998–1031.
HN Ricciuti (red.). Review of Child Development Research, 1–94. LeDoux, J. (2003). The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cellular and Molecular
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. N. (2015). Patterns of attach- Neurobiology, 23(4–5), 727–738.
ment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Psychology Press. Lenzi, D., Trentini, C., Pantano, P., Macaluso, E., Iacoboni, M., Lenzi, G. L., & Ammaniti,
Arkadir, D., Morris, G., Vaadia, E., & Bergman, H. (2004). Independent coding of M. (2008). Neural basis of maternal communication and emotional expression pro-
movement direction and reward prediction by single pallidal neurons. Journal of cessing during infant preverbal stage. Cerebral Cortex, 19(5), 1124–1133.
Neuroscience, 24(45), 10047–10056. Lemche, E., Giampietro, V. P., Surguladze, S. A., Amaro, E. J., Andrew, C. M., Williams, S.
Aron, A., Fisher, H., Mashek, D. J., Strong, G., Li, H., & Brown, L. L. (2005). Reward, C., ... Simmons, A. (2006). Human attachment security is mediated by the amygdala:
motivation, and emotion systems associated with early-stage intense romantic love. Evidence from combined fMRI and psychophysiological measures. Human Brain
Journal of Neurophysiology, 94(1), 327–337. Mapping, 27(8), 623–635.
Arsalidou, M., Duerden, E. G., & Taylor, M. J. (2013). The centre of the brain: Liu, Y., Ding, Y., Lu, L., & Chen, X. (2017). Attention bias of avoidant individuals to
Topographical model of functions of the basal ganglia. Human Brain Mapping, 34, attachment emotion pictures. Scientific Reports, 7, 41631.
3031–3054. Mechias, M. L., Etkin, A., & Kalisch, R. (2010). A meta-analysis of instructed fear studies:
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. Implications for conscious appraisal of threat. Neuroimage, 49(2), 1760–1768.
Neuroimage, 21(3), 1155–1166. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and
Benoit, D., & Parker, K. C. (1994). Stability and transmission of attachment across three change. Guilford Press.
generations. Child Development, 65(5), 1444–1456. Minagawa-Kawai, Y., Matsuoka, S., Dan, I., Naoi, N., Nakamura, K., & Kojima, S. (2008).
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Retrospect and prospect. American Journal of Prefrontal activation associated with social attachment: Facial-emotion recognition
Orthopsychiatry, 52(4), 664. in mothers and infants. Cerebral Cortex, 19(2), 284–292.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Vol. 1 Loss. New York: Basic Books. Moutsiana, C., Fearon, P., Murray, L., Cooper, P., Goodyer, I., Johnstone, T., & Halligan,
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss, Vol. 2: Separation. New York: Basic Books. S. (2014). Making an effort to feel positive: Insecure attachment in infancy predicts
Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (2008). Internal working models in attachment re- the neural underpinnings of emotion regulation in adulthood. Journal of Child
lationships: Elaborating a central construct in attachment theory. In I. J. Cassidy & P. Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(9), 999–1008.
R. Shaver (Red.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications Nitschke, J. B., Nelson, E. E., Rusch, B. D., Fox, A. S., Oakes, T. R., & Davidson, R. J.
(s. 102–127). (2004). Orbitofrontal cortex tracks positive mood in mothers viewing pictures of their
Buchheim, A., Erk, S., George, C., Kächele, H., Kircher, T., Martius, P., ... Walter, H. newborn infants. Neuroimage, 21(2), 583–592.
(2008). Neural correlates of attachment trauma in borderline personality disorder: A Norman, L., Lawrence, N., Iles, A., Benattayallah, A., & Karl, A. (2014). Attachment-se-
functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, curity priming attenuates amygdala activation to social and linguistic threat. Social
163(3), 223–235. Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(6), 832–839.
Buchheim, A., Erk, S., George, C., Kächele, H., Ruchsow, M., Spitzer, M., ... Walter, H. Peyron, R., Laurent, B., & Garcia-Larrea, L. (2000). Functional imaging of brain responses

55
E.J. Choi et al. Brain and Cognition 124 (2018) 47–56

to pain. A review and meta-analysis (2000). Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Open, 1(2), 121–128.
Neurophysiology, 30(5), 263–288. Tottenham, N., Shapiro, M., Telzer, E. H., & Humphreys, K. L. (2012). Amygdala response
Rainville, P., Duncan, G. H., Price, D. D., Carrier, B., & Bushnell, M. C. (1997). Pain affect to mother. Developmental Science, 15(3), 307–319.
encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. Science, Tracey, I., & Mantyh, P. W. (2007). The cerebral signature for pain perception and its
277(5328), 968–971. modulation. Neuron, 55(3), 377–391.
Ranote, S., Elliott, R., Abel, K. M., Mitchell, R., Deakin, J. F. W., & Appleby, L. (2004). The Villablanca, J. R. (2010). Why do we have a caudate nucleus. Acta Neurobiologiae
neural basis of maternal responsiveness to infants: An fMRI study. Neuroreport, Experimentalis (Wars), 70(1), 95–105.
15(11), 1825–1829. Vogt, B. A. (2005). Pain and emotion interactions in subregions of the cingulate gyrus.
Redlich, R., Grotegerd, D., Opel, N., Kaufmann, C., Zwitserlood, P., Kugel, H., ... Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 6(7), 533.
Dannlowski, U. (2015). Are you gonna leave me? Separation anxiety is associated Vogt, B. A., & Sikes, R. W. (2009). Cingulate nociceptive circuitry and roles in pain
with increased amygdala responsiveness and volume. Social Cognitive and Affective processing: The cingulate premotor pain model. Cingulate Neurobiology and Disease,
Neuroscience, 10(2), 278–284. 312–339.
Resnick, G. (1993). Measuring attachment in early adolescence: A manual for the adminis- Völlm, B. A., Taylor, A. N., Richardson, P., Corcoran, R., Stirling, J., McKie, S., ... Elliott,
tration, coding and interpretation of the SAT for 11 to 14 year olds. Rockville, MD: R. (2006). Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic
Westat. resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. Neuroimage, 29(1), 90–98.
Sander, K., Frome, Y., & Scheich, H. (2007). FMRI activations of amygdala, cingulate Vrticka, P. (2012). Interpersonal closeness and social reward processing. Journal of
cortex, and auditory cortex by infant laughing and crying. Human Brain Mapping, Neuroscience, 32(37), 12649–12650.
28(10), 1007–1022. Vrtička, P., & Vuilleumier, P. (2012). Neuroscience of human social interactions and adult
Sanders, M. J., Wiltgen, B. J., & Fanselow, M. S. (2003). The place of the hippocampus in attachment style. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6.
fear conditioning. European Journal of Pharmacology, 463(1), 217–223. Vrticka, P., Andersson, F., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2008). Individual
Schneider, S., Brassen, S., Bromberg, U., Banaschewski, T., Conrod, P., Flor, H., ... Nees, F. attachment style modulates human amygdala and striatum activation during social
(2012). Maternal interpersonal affiliation is associated with adolescents' brain appraisal. PLoS One, 3, 11.
structure and reward processing. Translational Psychiatry, 2(11), e182. Vrticka, P., Lordier, L., Bediou, B., & Sander, D. (2014). Human amygdala response to
Sehlmeyer, C., Schöning, S., Zwitserlood, P., Pfleiderer, B., Kircher, T., Arolt, V., & dynamic facial expressions of positive and negative surprise. Emotion, 14(1), 161.
Konrad, C. (2009). Human fear conditioning and extinction in neuroimaging: A Wager, T. D., Davidson, M. L., Hughes, B. L., Lindquist, M. A., & Ochsner, K. N. (2008).
systematic review. PloS one, 4(6), e5865. Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron,
Somerville, L. H., Heatherton, T. F., & Kelley, W. M. (2006). Anterior cingulate cortex 59(6), 1037–1050.
responds differentially to expectancy violation and social rejection. Nature Waters, E., Merrick, S., Treboux, D., Crowell, J., & Albersheim, L. (2000). Attachment
Neuroscience, 9(8), 1007. security in infancy and early adulthood: A twenty-year longitudinal study. Child
Spielberg, J. M., Miller, G. A., Warren, S. L., Engels, A. S., Crocker, L. D., Banich, M. T., ... Development, 71(3), 684–689.
Heller, W. (2012). A brain network instantiating approach and avoidance motivation. Whalen, P. J., Bush, G., McNally, R. J., Wilhelm, S., McInerney, S. C., Jenike, M. A., &
Psychophysiology, 49(9), 1200–1214. Rauch, S. L. (1998). The emotional counting Stroop paradigm: A functional magnetic
Takiguchi, S., Fujisawa, T. X., Mizushima, S., Saito, D. N., Okamoto, Y., Shimada, K., ... resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division. Biological
Hiratani, M. (2015). Ventral striatum dysfunction in children and adolescents with Psychiatry, 44(12), 1219–1228.
reactive attachment disorder: Functional MRI study. British Journal of Psychiatry

56

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi