Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 10

Australian Occupational Therapy

Journal

Australian Occupational Therapy Journal (2011) 58, 231–240 doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1630.2011.00928.x

Research Article

A play-based intervention for children with ADHD:


A pilot study
Sarah Wilkes, Reinie Cordier, Anita Bundy, Kimberley Docking and
Natalie Munro
Faculty of Health Sciences, The University of Sydney, Lidcombe, New South Wales, Australia

Introduction: Many children with attention deficit Discussion: Results support the use of play, video feed-
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have serious social and forward ⁄ feedback techniques, therapist- and peer-modelling
peer difficulties that can lead to adverse outcomes in adoles- and parent involvement as an effective means to develop
cence and adulthood. To date, psychosocial treatments have the social play skills of children with ADHD. Further
produced poor outcomes in reducing social impairments larger-scale research is required.
commonly associated with ADHD. This study aimed to
KEY WORDS decentering, empathy, peer-modelling,
examine the efficacy of a new intervention designed to
social problems, video self-modelling.
improve the play and social skills of children with ADHD
and their playmates within the natural context of play.
Methods: Participants included children (aged 5–11 Introduction
years) diagnosed with ADHD, age-matched typically devel- Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a com-
oping playmates (n = 14 ⁄ group) and parents of children mon childhood neurobehavioural disorder characterised
with ADHD. The intervention involved seven weekly by developmentally inappropriate levels of inattention,
video-recorded free-play sessions; video feed-forward ⁄ hyperactivity and impulsivity (American Psychiatric
feedback and therapist- and peer-modelling were used to Association, 2000). Social dysfunction is common in chil-
promote social play. The Test of Playfulness was used as a dren with ADHD which frequently leads to peer rejection,
pre- ⁄ post-test measure. Data were subjected to Rasch analy- and having fewer meaningful friendships (Hoza et al.,
sis to calculate measure scores on interval level; dependant 2005; Wiener & Mak, 2008). Although stimulant medica-
sample t-test and Cohen-d calculations were used to mea- tion and psychosocial treatments are most commonly pre-
sure effect. scribed, these treatments have produced poor outcomes
Results: A dependant samples t-test revealed that both in reducing social impairments (Findling, 2008; MTA
children with ADHD (t = 8.1; d.f. = 13; P < 0.01) and Cooperative Group, 2008; Purdie, Hattie & Carroll, 2002).
their playmates (t = 6.9; d.f. = 13; P < 0.01) improved in This is concerning, as the escalating effect of continuing
their social play. Results demonstrated a large effect in social difficulties and peer rejection is likely to cause
improving the social play of children with ADHD adverse outcomes in later life (e.g. high-school drop-out,
antisocial behaviour) and can potentially lead to serious
(d = 1.5) and their playmates (d = 1.3).
psychiatric disorders (Bagwell, Molina, Pelham & Hoza,
2001; Barkley, Fischer, Smallish & Fletcher, 2002).
Most psychosocial treatments aim to reduce negative
Sarah Wilkes BAppSc (OT) Hons; Occupational Therapist.
Reinie Cordier PhD; Lecturer, Discipline of Occupational social behaviours by teaching children socially acceptable
Therapy. Anita Bundy ScD; Chair of Occupational Therapy. behaviours within a controlled therapeutic setting (Whalen
Kimberley Docking PhD; Lecturer, Discipline of Speech & Henker, 1991). The limited effectiveness of such interven-
Pathology. Natalie Munro PhD; Lecturer, Discipline of tions could in part be explained by a number of factors,
Speech Pathology. including delivering the treatment within a group context
Correspondence: Reinie Cordier, Faculty of Health Sciences, rather than in dyads, limited parental involvement and sim-
Cumberland Campus, The University of Sydney, Rm J121, ply targeting poor social skills, rather than the underlying
J Block C43J, 75 East Street, Lidcombe, NSW 2141, Australia. cause of the social impairment (Antshel & Remer, 2003).
Email: reinie.cordier@sydney.edu.au
Accepted for publication 7 February 2011. Lack of interpersonal empathy in children
C 2011 The Authors

with ADHD
C 2011 Occupational
Australian Occupational Therapy Journal Recent research indicates that the social impairments
Therapy Australia of children with ADHD may be attributed to lack of
232 S. WILKES ET AL.

interpersonal empathy, rather than simply poor social To facilitate the development of interpersonal empa-
skills (Cordier, Bundy, Hocking & Einfeld, 2010a; Marton, thy, the process of decentering was encouraged (i.e. dis-
Wiener, Rogers, Moore & Tannock, 2008). This finding is criminating and identifying the emotional states of
supported in Barkley’s (1997) model, suggesting that chil- playmates, taking on playmates’ perspectives or roles
dren with ADHD are less empathetic, and demonstrate a and evoking shared affect). Techniques including self-
lack of responsiveness to the needs, emotions and view- modelling (using adapted video feed-forward and video
points of others. Empathy is associated with prosocial feedback techniques) and therapist-modelling were used
behaviour and comprises both affective and cognitive to shift the behaviour of children with ADHD away from
components involving the ability to: (i) discriminate and their tendency towards domination, destruction and self-
identify the emotional states of another, (ii) take on the focus (Cordier et al., 2009).
perspective or role of another and (iii) evoke shared affec- Playmates were included to promote peer-modelling
tive responses in the context of mutually enjoyed, reci- and friendship development. Although there is limited
procal interactions (Feshbach, 1997). Furthermore, research on the playmates of children with ADHD, recent
childhood development theory denotes that pretend play findings suggest that to a lesser degree they too display
is central in developing emotional understanding and by negative behaviours (Cordier, Bundy, Hocking & Einfeld,
early school age, children are better able to take on oth- 2010b). Therefore, familiar playmates were included as
ers’ viewpoints and are less occupied with their own we postulate that they would also benefit from the inter-
viewpoint; they become more decentered (Piaget, 1962; vention and thus become better equipped to support chil-
Stagnitti, 2009). dren with ADHD. Parents were key in reinforcing the
Cordier et al. (2010a) observed that the play of children application of intervention techniques to different con-
with ADHD is characterised by a developmentally inap- texts and to facilitate the continuity of skill development.
propriate lack of empathetic responding. This is observed These techniques were incorporated into the intervention
in their difficulty to: support the play of others, respond as they have been found to be effective in improving play
to others’ play cues, share and interact in a cooperative and social skills in different population groups (Dowrick,
manner; highlighting their preoccupation with having 1999; Frankel, Myatt, Cantwell & Feinberg, 1997; Wolf-
their own play needs met. Cordier et al. offered lack of berg, 2003).
empathy as an explanation of how these play deficits For the purposes of this study, play was defined as a
impacted on the children’s ability to develop play trans- transaction between the individual and the environment
actions as well as form and maintain meaningful friend- that is intrinsically motivated, internally controlled, free
ships. Based on the results of their study, Cordier et al. of many of the constraints of objective reality and skills
developed a model for an intervention aimed at enhanc- related to framing (giving and responding to cues) (Bate-
ing the social play skills of children with ADHD (Cordier, son, 1972). Play manifests in children as playfulness (i.e.
Bundy, Hocking & Einfeld, 2009). The model for a play- the disposition to play) (Bundy, 2004; Neumann, 1971).
based intervention was developed on the assumptions Using this definition and the Test of Playfulness (ToP),
that play is an important childhood occupation and the which operationalises the definition, we set out to
natural context within which children develop complex develop and test the efficacy of a play-based intervention.
social behaviours and competence. Applying the previous four principles, the intervention
aimed to improve the social play skills of children with
Play-based intervention principles ADHD and their playmates. We tested the following
The model embraces four important principles: (i) captur- hypotheses:
ing the intrinsic motivation of children; (ii) facilitating the d Hypothesis 1: The mean overall ToP post-test score of
development of interpersonal empathy; (iii) including a children with ADHD will be significantly higher than
regular playmate and; (iv) active parent involvement. the mean overall pre-test score.
Furthermore, during the initial phase of the pilot inter- d Hypothesis 2: The mean overall ToP post-test score of
vention we noted the importance of therapist modelling playmates of children with ADHD will be signifi-
to reinforce all intervention strategies. Therefore, we cantly higher than the mean overall pre-test score.
added a fifth intervention principle: therapist-modelling. d Hypothesis 3: The mean post-test scores of children
The intervention was first concerned with capturing with ADHD will be significantly higher than their
the intrinsic motivation of children with ADHD. There- pre-test scores on ToP items that reflect interpersonal
fore, the environment was set in an inviting playroom empathy.
which the child perceived as emotionally and physically
safe, supportive of free play. When a child with ADHD
engages in play that is intrinsically motivated, they are Methods
more likely to experience an increased ability to maintain
Participants
attention and persist through difficult situations, allow-
ing a play transaction to be fully developed (Bundy, 2004; The participants recruited for the study included two
Cordier et al., 2009). groups of children (n = 15 ⁄ group) between the ages of 5

C 2011 The Authors



Australian Occupational Therapy Journal
C 2011 Occupational Therapy Australia
ADHD PLAY-BASED INTERVENTION 233

and 11 years. Group 1 involved children diagnosed as t-value ‡ 70 on the DSM-IV subscales of the CPRS-3 and
having ADHD and Group 2 involved typically develop- CBCL). To assist with the interpretation of the ToP (Bun-
ing children without ADHD. One child with ADHD was dy, 2004) results, the CPRS-3 subscale scores which pro-
paired with one typically developing child. The play vide a behavioural representation of participants are
pairs were known to each other to avoid unfamiliar peers summarised in Table 1. Children were included in the
influencing the results in unacceptable ways. Play pairs study regardless of their medication status. Parents were
were matched by age a priori to cater to developmental asked to maintain the consistency of the type and dose of
needs (i.e. the maximum age difference between play medication throughout the course of the intervention ses-
pairs was three years; mean age difference = 0.3 years; sions and to report any changes; deviations in medication
SD = 0.96). All participants were required to be proficient and health concerns were recorded on a weekly basis.
English speakers. Parents of children with ADHD also
participated in the study. One family discontinued their Typically developing playmates (Group 2)
participation because of pressing family commitments; Children in Group 2 were invited by a child with ADHD;
thus, the findings are reported on n = 14 ⁄ group. they were similar in age and a regular playmate (in some
cases a sibling). For the purposes of this study, a typically
Children with ADHD (Group 1) developing playmate was defined as a child who did not
Children with ADHD were recruited from local paediat- have ADHD as defined by the DSM-IV criteria for ADHD
ric services and primary schools and through a media (i.e. scored below the clinical cut-off for all of the CPRS-3
release. To be included in the study, children had a for- and CBCL subscales) and for whom no concerns were
mal and unambiguous diagnosis of ADHD made by a raised about development or behaviour by a teacher or
psychiatrist or paediatrician using recognised diagnostic health professional. Typically developing playmates who
procedures (i.e. meeting the criteria for ADHD as defined scored in the borderline clinical range for the CPRS-3 and
in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, the CBCL (t-values ‡ 65; t-values < 70) were excluded.
4th edition [DSM-IV]). Children with comorbid condi-
tions that commonly coexist with ADHD (e.g. learning Parents of children with ADHD
disorders, oppositional defiant disorder) were included, At least one parent or primary carer was present during
provided ADHD was the primary diagnosis. However, all intervention sessions. Where possible both parents
children diagnosed with major neurodevelopmental or attended. The demographic information of participants is
psychiatric disorders (e.g. autism spectrum disorder, summarised in Table 2.
intellectual disability and cerebral palsy) were excluded.
In addition to the formal diagnosis made by a paedia- Instruments
trician or psychiatrist, the presence of ADHD symptoms
Test of Playfulness
was confirmed a priori by parent ratings on the Conners’
Parent Rating Scale-3rd edition (CPRS-3) and the Child The ToP (Bundy, 2004) was the primary assessment tool
Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001; used to measure the children’s play. The ToP is a 30-item,
Conners, 2008; i.e. ratings above the clinical cut-off, observer-rated instrument suitable for children and teens

TABLE 1: Conners’ Parent Rating Scales-3rd edition subscale scores

ADHD Playmates

Subscale item Item description Mean† Mean

Hyperactivity symptoms Never sit still; act as if driven by a motor 80.1† 47.0
Inattention symptoms Forgetful; easily bored; sluggish 85.0† 49.1
Learning problems Cognitive problems; slow learning; distracted 76.0† 46.9
Executive functioning Lacks: problem-solving; insight; planning 76.2† 52.7
Aggression Loses temper; easily annoyed; resentful 84.4† 58.5
Peer relations Social problems; few friends; isolation 85.2† 54.8
Inattentive subtype Predominately inattentive symptoms 78.2† 49.8
Hyperactivity ⁄ impulsivity subtype Predominately hyperactive ⁄ impulsive symptoms 76.6† 46.5
Conduct behaviour Bullies; threatens; uses weapons; violent 78.1† 56.2
Oppositional behaviour Opposes authority; rejects, breaks rules 77.5† 50.3

†Conners’ Parent Rating Scales-3rd edition subscale mean scores are above the clinical cut-off (i.e. subscale scores > 70).
All subscale items for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and playmate groups: d.f. = 13; P < 0.01.


C2011 The Authors
Australian Occupational Therapy Journal
C 2011 Occupational Therapy Australia
234 S. WILKES ET AL.

TABLE 2: Participant demographics is a well-established measure for assessing behaviours


consistent with ADHD. The CPRS-3 has evidence for
Group 1 Group 2 excellent reliability and construct validity; internal con-
Variables ADHD playmates sistency (0.89), temporal stability (0.86) and inter-rater
reliability (0.83). The CPRS-3 was found to be a valid
Mean age (SD) (years) 7.6 (1.6) 7.3 (1.6) measure: (i) factorial validity proved to have adequate fit,
Gender (%) (ii) across-informant correlations were consistent
Male† 71.4 57.1 (r = 0.61) and (iii) discriminative validity was found
Female 28.6 42.9 to have high levels of classification accuracy (86.0%;
Ethnicity (%)
Conners, 2008).
Caucasian 64.3 57.1 Child Behaviour Checklist
Asian 21.4 28.6
The CBCL is a standardised parent questionnaire that is
Other ethnicities 14.3 14.3
commonly used in research to establish the frequency
Primary caregiver’s highest level of education (%)
and intensity of behavioural problems of children aged
Completed high school 14.3 21.4
6–18 years. The CBCL was administered to all partici-
Tertiary qualifications: 14.3 14.3 pants as a secondary screening measure to confirm the
Diploma ⁄ certificate absence or presence of current ADHD symptoms as rated
Tertiary qualifications: 71.4 64.3 in the CPRS-3. To be included, children had to meet the
University degree clinical cut-off criteria on both the CBCL and CPRS-3.
Primary caregiver’s occupation (%) The CBCL has proven to be a reliable and valid measure;
Jobs that do not require tertiary 57.1 64.3 internal consistency (0.86–0.91) and test–retest reliability
qualifications (0.85–0.93; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001).
Jobs that do require tertiary 42.9 35.7
qualifications Procedure
Once ethical approval was received, parents whose chil-
†Ratio 1:3, this is a close approximation to the 1:5 ration dren met the inclusion criteria were contacted and sched-
of boys to girls diagnosed with attention deficit uled to receive seven weekly 40-minute intervention
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) as reported in the literature sessions. The play-based intervention took place in a con-
(American Psychiatric Association, 2000). SD = standard sistent and inviting playroom (5 m · 5 m with a one-way
deviation. mirror), set up to be supportive of play (Cordier et al.,
2010a). The toys available in the playroom where study
observations were made catered to gender differences,
between the ages of 6 months and 18 years. Each item is the age range of the children and for their likely motiva-
rated on a 4-point (0–3) scale. Scores reflect extent (i.e. tions for engaging in free-play activities. A diversity of
proportion of time), intensity (i.e. degree of presence) or play materials was present in the room to support a range
skilfulness (i.e. ease of performance). The ToP measures of play. Examples of the toys included dress-up clothes,
the concept of playfulness as a reflection of the combined construction toys, a sandbox, and soft bat and ball games.
presence of four elements contributing to a single (unidi- The same toys were present during all play sessions.
mensional) construct of playfulness: perception of con- The children were allowed to choose play materials and
trol, freedom from constraints of reality, source of activities.
motivation and ability to give and read social cues. The
ToP has evidence for excellent inter-rater reliability (data Play-based intervention structure
from 96% of raters fit the expectations of the Rasch Each play pair was allocated a primary and secondary
model); moderate test–retest reliability (e.g. intraclass therapist to promote a positive therapist–child relation-
correlation 0.67 at P < .01; Brentnall, Bundy & Kay, 2008) ship. The primary therapist provided the children with
and construct validity (e.g. data from 93% items and 98% weekly feedback and modelled desired skills in the play-
of people fit Rasch expectations (Bundy, Nelson, Metzger room. Concurrently, the secondary therapist worked
& Bingaman, 2001). closely with the child’s parents. Figure 1 provides a
temporal, schematic representation of the intervention
Conners’ Parent Rating Scales-3 structure.
The CPRS-3 is a screening questionnaire completed by All intervention sessions were video-recorded to facili-
parents to identify children between the ages of 6 and tate self-modelling (using video feedback and feed-
18 years who have current symptoms consistent with a forward) and for ease of scoring. A single non-blinded
diagnosis of ADHD. The CPRS-3 was selected as the pri- therapist scored the video recording of the children’s
mary screening tool to confirm the presence or absence of play against the ToP following each intervention session.
current ADHD symptoms. The CPRS-3 was selected as it Prior to scoring the sessions, the therapist was calibrated

C 2011 The Authors



Australian Occupational Therapy Journal
C 2011 Occupational Therapy Australia
ADHD PLAY-BASED INTERVENTION 235

Intervention structure

Baseline play (pre-test) Therapist-modeling Therapist-, peer- & Final evaluation (post-test)
Only therapist & child with self-modeling
Orient to play environment 20 minute video feedback/feed- 20 minute video feedback/
Child with ADHD & playmate ADHD play (20-minutes)
forward feed-forward
Child

play (30 minutes - first 10 Build rapport with child with 20 minute free-play – post test
ADHD 20 minute free-play
minutes not included in pre-test Post-test scored without
Therapist demonstrates Graded therapist modeling -
score) therapist involvement to match
strategies to parents as skilled decreased as children
progressed pre-test
playmate

Children & primary therapist

Session 1 >>>>>>>>>>>>>>>>> Session 2 >>>>>>>>>>>>>>>> Sessions 3–6 >>>>>>>>>>>>>>>> Session 7

Parents & secondary therapist

Introduction & education Strategies for developing social Support and training Feedback & continuation
skills through play Observe video feedback & play
Gather client history Feedback: child’s strengths vs. Identify play areas that Topics, strategies, & play-tasks
Parent

Topics: understanding play; target areas requiring improved & areas requiring re-capped
play deficits in children with development further support Child’s overall improvement
ADHD; social skills; Play observation: strategies to Discuss therapist modeling and discussed
importance of friendship develop your child’s target how parents can implement Gather parent feedback
areas strategies at home

Play-tasks (home work): sessions 1 & 2 Play-tasks: sessions 3–6


Gathering information on observed play behaviours at home Implementing intervention strategies at home
Playing with your child Giving the child feedback and problems-solving alternative strategies
Including siblings/playmates in play Facilitating play-dates

FIGURE 1: Intervention structure (not source).

on the ToP. This means that the consistency of her ratings encouraging joint pretend play. In turn, the playmates
was compared with hundreds of other raters in a large modelled desired social skills and reciprocity.
ToP sample (n > 3000). The therapist’s calibration results
indicated that she was a reliable rater because her good- Parents and the secondary therapist. The secondary thera-
ness-of-fit statistics were within an acceptable range pist worked closely with the child’s parents by providing
(MnSq < 1.4; standardised value £ 2; Bond & Fox, 2007). education, support training and feedback (see Fig. 1). Par-
ents observed all self-modelling and playroom sessions
Children and the primary therapist. The primary therapist through a one-way mirror, completed weekly play tasks
facilitated the self-modelling and free-play sessions (see and reviewed their weekly take-home copy of the self-
Fig. 1). During the self-modelling session, children modelling DVD with their child before the next session.
observed and reflected on edited video footage of their Parents were encouraged to continue fostering dyadic
social play from the previous week. The therapist used friendship development by inviting the playmate or a
video feedback techniques by discussing the segments of peer to their home.
their past performance. The therapist then used video
feed-forward techniques by facilitating a problem-solving Data analysis
discussion with the children, helping them to develop To attain interval-level scores for each participant, ToP
strategies to preempt the changes required for future raw scores were entered into an existing database con-
social skill development to occur. To conclude, the thera- taining scores of children with ADHD and typically
pist presented the children with three key summary developing children (N = 378). The data were then sub-
points to remember and preempt as they entered the jected to Rasch analysis using the Winsteps program (ver-
playroom. While playing, the therapist supported both sion 3.70.0.2; Linacre, 2007).
children to engage in prosocial behaviours (e.g. sharing, The resulting measure scores of the sample were then
supporting and responding). This was facilitated by entered into SPSS (version 18; SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL,
engaging in mutually enjoyable social play and by USA) and t-tests for dependent samples were calculated


C2011 The Authors
Australian Occupational Therapy Journal
C 2011 Occupational Therapy Australia
236 S. WILKES ET AL.

to compare pre- and post-test mean ToP scores from


Sessions 1 and 7. All significance levels were set at Hypothesis 1: Effect of the intervention on
P < 0.05. A non-parametric one-sample Kolmogorov– the play of children with ADHD
Smirnov test was used to compare the observed cumula- The hypothesis that the mean overall ToP post-test score
tive distribution with the expected null hypothesis of children with ADHD will be significantly higher than
distribution. The goodness-of-fit tests if observations the mean overall pre-test scores was supported. A t-test
could reasonably have come from the specified distribu- for dependent samples revealed significance (t > 8.10;
tion; thus, parametric testing was deemed appropriate. P < 0.01). Furthermore, Cohen’s effect size value (d = 1.5)
Cohen-d values were calculated to examine effect size suggests a large effect size (see Table 3).
(Cohen, 1992). Cohen-d effect size sets a benchmark of
0.20 as small, 0.50 as medium and 0.80 as large (Rosnow, Hypothesis 2: Effect of the intervention on
Rosenthal & Rubin, 2000). the play of playmates
Prior to conducting t-tests, differential analysis (i.e. The hypothesis that the mean overall ToP post-test score
DIF, differential item functioning), generated by the Win- of playmates of children with ADHD will be significantly
steps program, was used to ensure equivalence of the higher than the mean overall pre-test scores was sup-
group with respect to potentially confounding variables ported (t > 6.86; P < 0.01). Furthermore, Cohen’s effect
(P £ 0.05). DIF analysis was used to examine the ToP size value (d = 1.3) suggests a large effect size (see
items to see whether the items have significantly different Table 3).
meanings for the two groups, indicated by any significant
differences in how children performed on each ToP item Hypothesis 3: Development of interpersonal
for each diagnostic group (ADHD vs. playmate). We empathy of children with ADHD
tested the effects of 12 such variables: (1) gender, (2) age The hypothesis that the mean post-test scores of children
(in three groups: 5–6, 7–8 and 9–11 years), (3) ethnicity, with ADHD will be significantly higher than their pre-
(4) socioeconomic status, (5) younger vs. older sibling test scores on ToP items that reflect interpersonal empa-
playmates, (6) age difference between playmate pairs, (7) thy were partially supported. Empathy, as described by
sibling vs. peer playmates, (8) clinically significant oppo- Feshbach (1997) and applied to the ToP items (see num-
sitional defiant disorder symptoms vs. non-clinically bers denoted in Table 4), comprises (i) the ability to dis-
significant oppositional defiant disorder symptoms, (9) criminate and identify the emotional states of another
clinically significant conduct disorder symptoms vs. (item 30); (ii) the capacity to take the perspective or role
non-clinically significant conduct disorder symptoms, of the other (items 5, 15, 10); and (iii) the evocation of a
(10) clinically significant anxiety symptoms vs. non-clini- shared affective response (items 6, 13, 12) (Cordier et al.,
cally significant anxiety symptoms, (11) parent-reported 2010a). In the remainder of the discussion, the ToP item
play time at home and (12) children receiving medication numbers, as shown in Table 4, are used in brackets for
vs. those who did not. reference (e.g. ToP item 5).
DIF analysis results and corresponding t-values are
shown in Table 4. Children with ADHD improved signif-
Results icantly in four of the seven ToP items that reflect the con-
No comparison of data for any potentially confounding structs of interpersonal empathy: skill of sharing ideas or
variable was significant (t < 1.96; P < .05). We interpreted objects (ToP item 5; t = 2.88), skill of supporting the play
this to indicate that none of the confounding variables of others (ToP item 6; t = 2.76), skill of transitioning
tested (e.g. medication use, age, oppositional defiant dis- between activities (ToP item 10) (t = 5.32) and skill
order and anxiety) accounted for the observed changes. of responding to play cues (ToP item 30; t = 2.29). No

TABLE 3: Effect sizes of the intervention

ADHD Playmates

Pre-test Post-test Pre-test Post-test


Mean S1 (range) Mean S7 (range) SD Cohen-d† ES Mean S1 (range) Mean S7 (range) SD Cohen-d† ES

41.8 (17.9–64.4) 69.2 (50.3–83.0) 18.2 1.5 Large 56.8 (42.5–75.2) 75.7 (55.3–90.8) 14.7 1.3 Large

†Cohen-d effect size (ES) for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and playmate groups were calculated
separately: Group (mean post-test ) mean pre-test) ⁄ spooled SD for group measure scores. ES was interpreted as large
(‡ 0.80), medium (‡ 0.50) or small (‡ 0.20) in magnitude (Cohen, 1992). Mean and standard deviation (SD) scores were
derived from interval-level measure scores. S1, Session 1 of the intervention; S7, Session 7 of the intervention.

C 2011 The Authors



Australian Occupational Therapy Journal
C 2011 Occupational Therapy Australia
ADHD PLAY-BASED INTERVENTION 237

TABLE 4: Differential item functioning analysis of all children with Test of Playfulness (ToP) item descriptions and corresponding t-
values

ADHD Playmates

Difference Difference
Item S1† S7† in t-value S1† S7† in t-value

Perception of control
1 Extent of playing safely 0.87 0.11 )0.76* )1.75 0.00 1.75*
2 Skill of initiating new activities 0.36 )0.32 )0.68* 0.97 )1.25 )2.22*
3 Skill of negotiating needs )2.08 )0.21 1.87* 0.92 1.48 0.56*
4 Extent of deciding what to do 0.69 )0.82 )1.51* )0.33 0.15 0.48*
5 Skill of sharing ideas or objects† )3.73 )0.85 2.88* 2.28 2.11 )0.17*
6 Skill of supporting the play of others† )3.68 )0.92 2.76* 2.33 2.21 )0.12*
7 Intensity of interacting with objects )0.07 0.89 0.96* )1.14 0.86 2.00*
8 Skill of interacting with objects 0.38 )0.06 )0.44* )0.44 0.12 0.56*
9 Skill of modifying task requirements )0.33 0.81 1.14* 0.14 )0.69 )0.83*
10 Skill of transitioning between activities† )2.76 2.56 5.32* )1.18 1.66 2.84*
11 Extent of playing with others 0.76 0.78 0.02* )0.87 )0.89 )0.02*
12 Intensity of playing with others† 0.64 0.55 )0.09* )0.75 )0.43 0.32*
13 Skill of playing with others† )1.7 )1.47 0.23* 1.36 1.84 0.48*
Freedom from constraints of reality
14 Extent of pretending )0.87 2.31 3.18* )2.10 0.94 3.04*
15 Skill of pretending† )0.98 0.00 0.98* 0.23 0.59 0.36*
16 Extent of using people or objects unconventionally 1.00 1.63 0.63* )2.60 0.20 2.80*
17 Skill of using people or objects unconventionally 0.70 0.27 )0.43* )0.61 )0.32 0.29*
18 Extent of using mischief ⁄ teasing 2.52 )2.17 )4.69* 1.03 )3.2 )4.23*
19 Skill of using mischief ⁄ teasing 0.74 )1.78 )2.52* 0.75 0.29 )0.46*
20 Extent of using clowning ⁄ joking 4.61 )1.24 )5.85* 0.86 )5.06 )5.92*
21 Skill of using clowning ⁄ joking 1.64 )0.46 )2.10* 0.30 )1.65 )1.95*
Source of motivation
22 Extent of being engaged 0.28 )0.12 )0.40* )0.58 0.60 1.18*
23 Intensity of being engaged 0.00 0.82 0.82* )0.64 )0.08 0.56*
24 Extent of being involved in the process )1.12 0.11 1.23* 0.70 0.00 )0.70*
25 Intensity of persistence 0.58 0.42 )0.16* )1.56 0.72 2.28*
26 Intensity of showing positive affect 2.45 0.7 )1.75* )1.60 )2.13 )0.53*
Framing (play cues)
27 Skill of being engaged 0.66 1.17 0.51* )1.54 )0.18 1.36*
28 Extent of giving cues 0.00 )1.00 )1.00* 1.27 )0.53 )1.80*
29 Skill of giving cues )0.83 )1.38 )0.55* 0.90 1.77 0.87*
30 Skill of responding to cues† )3.72 )1.43 2.29* 3.00 1.69 )1.31*

*Item denotes significance (t = > 1.96; > )1.96). †ToP items that represent the constructs of interpersonal empathy.
S1, Session 1; S7, Session 7.

significant improvements were observed for intensity of dren with ADHD. In particular, we attempted to improve
playing with others (ToP item 12), skill of playing with the social play of children with ADHD and their play-
others (ToP item 13) and skill of pretend play (ToP item mates as measured by the ToP. Furthermore, we aimed to
15). determine if the ToP items that reflect interpersonal
empathy improved significantly in children with ADHD.
The intervention was based on the model and key princi-
Discussion ples proposed by Cordier et al. (2009).
We set out to examine the efficacy of an innovative inter- The play-based intervention comprises a unique con-
vention aimed at developing the social play skills of chil- stellation of techniques aimed at addressing the particu-


C2011 The Authors
Australian Occupational Therapy Journal
C 2011 Occupational Therapy Australia
238 S. WILKES ET AL.

lar social play deficits associated with children with social skills is well documented (Guralnick, Hammond &
ADHD. The large effect size strongly supports the com- Connor, 2003). More so than other types of play, pretend
bined use of video self-modelling, peer- and therapist- play facilitated the process of decentering.
modelling and parent involvement within the context of In concordance with past research, our results support
play as an effective means to develop the play and social the integral role of parents in generalising newly
skills of children with ADHD and their playmates. acquired skills to home and other social environments
Although preliminary in nature, the results are promis- and for the continuity of skill development (Frankel et al.,
ing, as few psychosocial interventions have evidence of 1997). However, in this study, the evidence of general-
efficacy (Antshel & Remer, 2003; Purdie et al., 2002). isation to home is only anecdotal; further research is
Our results support previous research where video warranted.
self-modelling was effective in promoting the develop- The intervention also resulted in improvements to the
ment of social skills in different populations (Dowrick, social play of the playmates. This is an important finding
1999). We are not aware of any studies where video self- because by improving their social play skills, playmates
modelling has been used to improve the social skills of are likely to be better equipped to both meet their own
children with ADHD. We found that by viewing edited needs in play and support children with ADHD. The
video footage of themselves, children with ADHD were playmates’ ability to ‘persist’ (ToP item 25) during play
better able to reflect on their own performance. This transactions improved significantly, thereby supporting a
seemed to enable them to develop perspective-taking positive dyadic friendship to develop. Existing literature
and problem-solving skills. Moreover, seemingly strongly supports dyadic friendship development to coun-
because the technique was novel and captured their teract the negative effects of peer rejection (Bagwell et al.,
attention, the children were able to recall and demon- 2001; Hoza, Mrug, Pelham, Greiner & Gnagy, 2003).
strate changes in skills required to achieve future social Furthermore, we postulate that in fostering friendships,
development. we have assisted play pairs to have continued opportuni-
Our findings are similar to those reported by Wolfberg ties to develop their skills post-treatment. That, too,
(2003) for children with autism in response to peer- and requires further investigation.
sibling-mediated interventions for enhancing social skills. Our results support previous findings that children
We, too, found that the inclusion of a known playmate in with ADHD experience difficulty in ToP items that repre-
the context of play provided a novel, motivating and sent interpersonal empathy (Cordier et al., 2010a).
effective means for developing the social skills required Furthermore, post-test results demonstrate that the inter-
for forming and maintaining meaningful friendships (e.g. vention provides promise in counteracting the effects of
problem-solving, perspective-taking, supporting, shar- lack of interpersonal empathy, by supporting shift
ing, negotiating and creating shared enjoyed experi- towards prosocial skill development (decentering). This
ences). was reflected in significant improvement in several ToP
We realised early into the intervention that therapist- items reflective of empathy (ToP items 5, 6, 10, 30; see
modelling was required to promote high-quality social Table 4).
interactions. The therapist engaged in play with the chil- Somewhat surprisingly, ToP items extent of: ‘clowning’
dren, fostering mutually enjoyed joint play and guiding (ToP item 20) and ‘mischief and teasing’ (ToP item 18)
both children to engage in prosocial behaviours. That is, decreased significantly at post-test for both children with
the therapist supported the playmates and helped the ADHD and their playmates. We speculate that as the chil-
child with ADHD, directly and indirectly, to shift away dren developed more effective prosocial mechanisms for
from a tendency to control and dominate play. Our obser- interacting with each other (e.g. supporting, sharing and
vations suggest that therapist support is critical to the responding) that the use of less effective mechanisms
efficacy of the intervention given the severity of social (e.g. clowning and teasing) was no longer needed.
skill impairment of children with ADHD and their intru- At the end of Session 7, it was evident that some chil-
sive, disruptive style of social interaction (Wiener & Mak, dren might have benefitted from having more interven-
2008). tion sessions. We postulate that increasing the number of
Although children improved significantly in spontane- sessions offered would result in improvements to all ToP
ously initiating pretend play themes (ToP item 14), thera- items relating to interpersonal empathy. Therefore, it is
pist support was essential in evolving the themes by recommended that future research explore a criterion-
facilitating joint and cooperative forms of pretend play based approach.
between children (ToP item 15). Recent research estab-
lished links between pretend play and social competence,
denoting children who demonstrate less skill in complex
Limitations
forms of play (i.e. pretend play) are more likely to disrupt It was not feasible to draw a random sample. Hence, the
peers or have lower levels of engagement during play ability to generalise the results of this study to children
(Uren & Stagnitti, 2009). Furthermore, the importance of with ADHD in other populations is limited. Furthermore,
pretend play in developing emotional understanding and because of time and resource constraints associated with

C 2011 The Authors



Australian Occupational Therapy Journal
C 2011 Occupational Therapy Australia
ADHD PLAY-BASED INTERVENTION 239

preliminary research, the rater was not blinded to the into young adulthood as a function of reporting source
purposes of the study or to the phase of intervention. and definition of disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
Thus, further research is required. 111 (2), 279–289.
Bateson, G. (1972). Toward a theory of play and fantasy.
In: G. Bateson (Ed.), Steps to an ecology of the mind (pp.
Conclusions and implications for 14–20). New York: Bantam.
research Bond, T. & Fox, C. (2007). Applying the Rasch model:
Fundamental measurement in the human sciences (2nd ed.).
The purpose of this pilot study was to develop and test
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
the efficacy of a play-based intervention aimed at
Brentnall, J., Bundy, A. & Kay, F. (2008). The effect of the
improving the play and social skills of children with
length of observation on Test of Playfulness scores.
ADHD. Results support the use of play, self-modelling OTJR: Occupation, Participation, and Health, 28 (3), 133–
techniques (video feed-forward ⁄ feedback), therapist- and 139.
peer-modelling and parent involvement as an effective Bundy, A. (2004). Test of Playfulness (ToP), Version 4.0.
means to develop the social play skills of children with Sydney, Australia: The University of Sydney.
ADHD. Bundy, A., Nelson, L., Metzger, M. & Bingaman, K. (2001).
Future research should be geared towards refining Validity and reliability of a test of playfulness. OTJR:
features of the intervention such as: (i) developing an Occupation, Participation, and Health, 21 (4), 276–292.
adjunctive home programme to strengthen generalisation Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin,
to other environments; (ii) implementing parent-rated 112, 155–159.
measures to ascertain if gains are maintained post-treat- Conners, C. (2008). Conners 3rd Edition Manual. Toronto,
ment; and (iii) employing a criterion-based approach to Ontario: Multi-Health Systems Inc.
determine whether extending the number of intervention Cordier, R., Bundy, A., Hocking, C. & Einfeld, S. (2009).
sessions would further the skill development of children A model for play-based intervention for children with
more severely affected. Additionally, future research ADHD. Australian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 56 (5),
should ultimately be directed towards trialling the inter- 332–340.
vention with a larger, more representative sample to Cordier, R., Bundy, A., Hocking, C. & Einfeld, S. (2010a).
determine if the intervention demonstrates efficacy Empathy in the play of children with ADHD. OTJR,
Occupation, Participation, and Health, 30 (3), 122–132.
within the population at large.
Cordier, R., Bundy, A., Hocking, C. & Einfeld, S. (2010b).
Playing with a child with ADHD: A focus on the
Acknowledgements playmates. Scandinavian Journal of Occupational Therapy,
17 (3), 191–199.
The authors wish to extend their gratitude to the families
Dowrick, P. (1999). A review of self modeling and
who participated in the research and Dee Why Rotary related interventions. Applied and Preventive Psychology,
Club for providing financial support. 8, 23–39.
Feshbach, N. (1997). Empathy: The formative years:
References Implications for clinical practice. In: A. C. Bohart & L. S.
Greenberg (Eds.), Empathy reconsidered: New directions in
Achenbach, T. & Rescorla, L. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA psychotherapy (pp. 33–51). Washington, DC: American
school-age forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Psychological Association.
Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Findling, R. (2008). Evolution of the treatment of attention-
Families. deficit ⁄ hyperactivity disorder in children: A review.
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Clinical Therapeutics, 30 (5), 942–957.
statistical manual of mental disorders (revised 4th ed.). Frankel, F., Myatt, R., Cantwell, D. & Feinberg, D. (1997).
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Parent-assisted transfer of children’s social skills training:
Antshel, K. & Remer, R. (2003). Social skills training in Effects on children with and without attention-deficit
children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of
A randomized-controlled clinical trial. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1056–1064.
Child and Adolescent Psychology, 32 (1), 153–165. Guralnick, M., Hammond, M. & Connor, R. (2003).
Bagwell, C., Molina, B., Pelham, W. & Hoza, B. (2001). Subtypes of non-social play: Comparisons of children
Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and problems in with and without developmental delays. American Journal
peer relations: Predictions from childhood to adolescence. of Mental Retardation, 108 (5), 247–362.
Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 40 (11), 1285–1292. Hoza, B., Mrug, S., Pelham, W., Greiner, A. & Gnagy, E.
Barkley, R. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, (2003). A friendship intervention for children with
and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of attention-deficit ⁄ hyperactivity disorder: Preliminary
ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121 (1), 65–94. findings. Journal of Attention Disorders, 6 (3), 87–98.
Barkley, R., Fischer, M., Smallish, L. & Fletcher, K. (2002). Hoza, B., Mrug, S., Gerdes, A., Hinshaw, S., Bukowski, W.,
The persistence of attention-deficit ⁄ hyperactivity disorder Gold, J. et al. (2005). What aspects of peer relationships are


C2011 The Authors
Australian Occupational Therapy Journal
C 2011 Occupational Therapy Australia
240 S. WILKES ET AL.

impaired in children with attention-deficit ⁄ hyperactivity Rosnow, R., Rosenthal, R. & Rubin, D. (2000). Contrasts
disorder? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73 and correlations in effect size estimation. Psychological
(3), 411–423. Science, 11, 446–453.
Linacre, J. M. (2007). Winsteps Rasch measurement: Computer Stagnitti, K. (2009). Pretend play assessment. In: K. Stagnitti
software manual (version 3.70.0.2). Retrieved 20 September & R. Cooper (Eds.), Play as therapy: Assessment and
2010, from http://www.winsteps.com/index.htm therapeutic interventions (pp. 87–101). London: Jessica
Marton, I., Wiener, J., Rogers, M., Moore, C. & Tannock, R. Kingsley Publishers.
(2008). Empathy and social perspective taking in children Uren, N. & Stagnitti, K. (2009). Pretend play, social
with attention-deficit ⁄ hyperactivity disorder. Journal of competence and involvement in children aged 5–7 years:
Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 107–118. The concurrent validity of the child-initiated pretend
MTA Cooperative Group (2008). Evidence, interpretation, play assessment. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal,
and qualification from multiple reports of long-term 56, 33–40.
outcomes in the multimodal treatment study of children Whalen, C. & Henker, B. (1991). Therapies for hyperactive
with ADHD (MTA): Part II: Supporting details. Journal of children: Comparisons, combinations, and compromises.
Attention Disorders, 12 (1), 15–43. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 126–
Neumann, E. (1971). The elements of play. New York: MSS 137.
Information. Wiener, J. & Mak, M. (2008). Peer victimization in children
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New with attention-deficit ⁄ hyperactivity disorder. Psychology
York: W.W. Norton. in the Schools, 46 (2), 116–131.
Purdie, N., Hattie, J. & Carroll, A. (2002). A review of the Wolfberg, P. (2003). Peer play and the autism spectrum: The
research on interventions for attention deficit hyperactivity art of guiding children’s socialization and imagination.
disorder: What works best? Review of Educational Research, Shawnee Mission, KS: Autism Asperger Publishing.
72 (1), 61–99.

C 2011 The Authors



Australian Occupational Therapy Journal
C 2011 Occupational Therapy Australia

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi