Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 15

Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Acta Psychologica
j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s ev i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / a c t p s y

Emotional valence of spoken words influences the spatial orienting of attention☆


Julie Bertels a,b,⁎, Régine Kolinsky a,b, José Morais c
a
Université Libre de Bruxelles (U.L.B.), Belgium
b
Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique – FNRS (F.R.S.–FNRS), Belgium
c
Université Libre de Bruxelles (U.L.B.), Belgium

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The influence of the affective content of speech on the spatial orienting of auditory attention was examined
Received 18 December 2008 by adapting the dot probe task. Two words, one of which was emotional in one quarter of the trials, were
Received in revised form 23 February 2010 played simultaneously from a left- and a right-located loudspeaker, respectively, and followed (or not) by a
Accepted 24 February 2010
lateralized beep to be detected (Experiments 1 and 2) or localized (Experiment 3). Taboo words induced
Available online 26 March 2010
attentional biases towards their spatial location in all experiments, as did negative words in Experiment 3,
PsycINFO classification:
but not positive words. Thus, in audition, the identification of an emotional word automatically activates the
2346 Attention information about its spatial origin. Moreover, for both word types, attentional biases were only observed
2360 Motivation & Emotion when the emotional word was presented on the participant's right side, suggesting that the dominant left
hemisphere processing of words constrains the occurrence of spatial congruency effects.
Keywords: © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Auditory attention
Cognitive bias
Emotional content
Spoken words

1. Introduction “where” subsystem on the medial, posterior auditory cortex (Clarke,


Bellmann, Meuli, Assal, & Steck, 2000; Rauschecker, 1997, 1998a,b).
The present work examines whether the affective content of speech In audition, as perceptual form and organization are not processed
influences the spatial orienting of auditory attention. If so, an emotional in relation to space, possible links between the “what” and the
word might facilitate or interfere with the processing of an unrelated, “where” auditory subsystems have been underestimated (see Kubovy,
simple auditory signal subsequently presented, depending on whether 1988). Still, it can be argued that such functional links afford us an
this signal shares the spatial origin of the speech stimulus or not. ecological advantage. In peripheral space, beyond a 20° visual angle,
It has been proposed that the auditory system, like the visual one, auditory targets elicit shorter orientation latencies than visual targets
is composed of two parallel streams of processing: the “what” and the (Goldring, Dorris, Corneil, Ballantyne, & Munoz, 1996) and, at night,
“where” subsystems (Kubovy & Van Valkenburg, 2001). The dissoci- the identification of sounds signalling either danger or urgent
ation of these two subsystems is supported, for example, by the requests for help may require auditory localization. Consequently,
observation of an illusion in which a sequence of pitches is perceived identification of an auditory object relevant for survival (or at least for
correctly but in the wrong ear location (Deutsch & Roll, 1976), and by ecological adaptation) might automatically activate information
the fact that adaptation may have a detrimental effect on localization about the spatial location of its source in order to orient spatial
while leaving pitch perception unaltered (Hafter, 1997). The two attention to the same location. This would put the listener in a better
auditory subsystems seem to rely on distinct cerebral structures: the attentional state to process subsequent auditory (or visual) informa-
“what” subsystem relying on the superior temporal cortex and the tion coming from the same source. For example, identifying the kind
of animal that produced a particular cry should, in principle, influence
the level of attention devoted to its position in space and therefore the
accuracy and/or rapidity of detection or localization of subsequent
☆ The first author was a Research Fellow of the Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique signals coming from the same direction.
(F.R.S.–FNRS) and is presently a Scientific Research Worker of the F.R.S.–FNRS. The second Obviously, not all auditory objects convey shallow, non-symbolic,
author is a Senior Research Associate of the F.R.S.–FNRS. All authors were affiliated to the information. Spoken words are auditory objects that transmit highly
Research Unit in Cognitive Neurosciences, Université Libre de Bruxelles.
⁎ Corresponding author. UNESCOG, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Avenue F.D.
abstract information, including semantics. As regards spoken words,
Roosevelt, 50, C.P.191, 1050 Brussels, Belgium. some refer to emotional states or are emotionally loaded. Although
E-mail address: jbertels@ulb.ac.be (J. Bertels). emotional words are not more directly relevant for survival than

0001-6918/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.008
J. Bertels et al. / Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278 265

neutral words (especially when uttered in a neutral intonation), their when the emotional content of the word is irrelevant to the task.
identification might evoke emotional feelings, boost the attentional Furthermore, as Kinsbourne (1970) argued, the presentation of verbal
system and trigger the “where” auditory subsystem, influencing the material could activate the left hemisphere preponderantly, thus
orientation of spatial attention. leading to involuntary orientational biases to the right side of space.
Many authors examined the spatial orienting of auditory attention, To this hemispheric imbalance hypothesis one may add the
namely the characteristics of the auditory “where” subsystem, assumption that an emotional word catches more attentional
without considering the involvement of the “what” subsystem (e.g., resources than its paired neutral word, and therefore activates the
Buchtel & Butter, 1988; Rhodes, 1987; Spence & Driver, 1994). Most of hemispheric contralateral to its side of presentation to a greater
these studies used an auditory variant of the cuing paradigm (Posner, extent than the ipsilateral one. Thus, taking these two assumptions
1980), in which a peripherally-presented auditory cue is followed by together, one may predict the attentional bias to be stronger when the
an auditory target, presented either at the same location as the cue or emotional word is presented on the right side. Indeed, in this case, the
at the opposite location (in valid and invalid trials, respectively). processing of a right-sided beep would be favoured by both
Exogenous attentional orienting to the spatial location of the cue is attentional mechanisms, the “hemispheric” and the “emotional”
reflected by the validity effect, namely the reaction time (henceforth, one, whereas processing of a left-side beep would be disadvantaged
RT) difference between invalid and valid trials. by both. In contrast, if the emotional word is presented on the left side,
In the present study, we used a variant of the auditory cuing processing a beep presented on either the left or the right side would
paradigm to attempt to assess the impact of the emotional content of benefit from one of the two mechanisms, either the “emotional” or the
word cues, relying on the “what” subsystem, on the spatial orienting “hemispheric” one, respectively, but would be disadvantaged by the
of auditory attention. other mechanism.
This variant actually consists of an adaptation to the auditory The influence of the emotional content of a word on attentional
modality of the dot probe task (also called attentional deployment orienting could depend on its specific emotional valence. Because the
task, MacLeod, Mathews, & Tata, 1986) used in the visual modality rapid processing of positive information does not seem to be as crucial
to examine the role of the emotional content of written words on for the organism's well-being as is the processing of negative
the spatial orienting of visual attention. In the dot probe task, two information (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001;
written words, either one emotionally loaded and the other not, or Grühn, Smith & Baltes, 2005; Rozin & Royzman, 2001; Taylor, 1991),
both neutral, are presented one above the other. In half of the we hypothesized the “where” auditory subsystem to be more likely
trials, the words are followed by an unrelated visual target (a dot) triggered by negative and taboo words (namely vulgar, shocking or
in the location that was previously occupied by one of the words. obscene words, as well as insults) than by positive ones. Hence, we
On congruent trials (i.e. valid trials, in the terminology of the cuing expected taboo and negative spoken words to facilitate the processing
paradigm), the dot is presented at the same location as the one of the beep presented at the same spatial location (compared to its
previously occupied by the emotional word, while on incongruent presentation at the opposite side) to a greater extent than positive
(i.e. invalid) trials the dot is presented at the opposite location. In words. This effect would be larger for right-presented emotional
our auditory adaptation of the dot probe task (named the beep words, as argued above.
probe task), during each trial two different spoken words were Experiment 1 consisted of an auditory adaptation of the original
presented through loudspeakers in a diotic way, i.e. simulta- version of the dot probe task. As in this task, the participants were
neously, one on the left and the other on the right side of the required to detect the presence of the non-linguistic target in each
listener. The use of loudspeakers instead of headphones was trial, here the beep. Also, given that in the visual paradigm
motivated by the ecological advantage of making sounds come participants were asked to read the word presented in the upper
from the external world. In the relevant trials, one of the words position in order to be sure they paid attention to the linguistic
was emotional and the other was neutral. In half of the trials, a stimuli, we asked them to repeat the word presented through a pre-
lateralized single beep target followed the presentation of the pair specified loudspeaker after responding to the beep in Experiment 1.
of words. The use of an auditory target instead of a visual one, as Given that only effects of taboo words emerged, we eliminated this
in the dot probe task, allowed us to keep the unimodal aspect of repetition task in a second experiment, speculating that it might have
the original paradigm. monopolized some attentional resources, making it difficult for
The congruence or incongruence between the presentation side negative words to facilitate selectively the detection of a same-side
of the emotional word and the presentation side of the beep was beep. In addition, the attentional biases observed in Experiment 1 for
critical. Consistent with the underlying idea of the cuing paradigm taboo words could have been due to the fact that participants had to
that the faster the participant reacts to a given location, the most pronounce these shocking words in front of the experimenter.
attention was attracted in this location (Posner, Snyder, & Elimination of the repetition task thus allowed us to both reduce
Davidson, 1980), the prediction derived from the auditory “what– the cognitive load and remove this potential bias. Finally, in a third
where” connection hypothesis was that the spatial congruence experiment, we used a localization rather than a detection task in
between the emotional word and the beep would lead to shorter order to introduce a spatial component in the task, given that spatial
RTs than their spatial incongruence, hence creating what is called validity effects have been observed more frequently in the auditory
an attentional bias, following MacLeod and Mathews' (1988) cuing paradigm when spatial representations are somehow relevant
terminology. for the participant's goal (Rhodes, 1987; Schmitt, Postma, & De Haan,
It must be noted that the auditory “what–where” connection 2000; Spence & Driver, 1994).
hypothesized here needs not be emotion-dependent. However, as in
the present situation only one of the diotic stimuli was emotional, any 2. Experiment 1: Beep probe detection and word repetition
spatial congruency effect implies that the “what–where” connection is
driven (or at least is reinforced) by the emotional member of the This experiment was intended to be a close auditory analogue of
diotic pair, not by the neutral one. the classic version of the dot probe task (MacLeod et al., 1986, see also
Given that both the words and beeps were presented in a e.g., Fox, 1993 and Salemink, van den Hout, & Kindt, 2007), using
lateralized way, perceptual laterality effects related to hemispheric lateral stimulations. We did not use up–down stimulations because
specialization were likely to be observed. Indeed, as shown by these proved to be exceedingly hard to discriminate. Neutral,
Demakis and Harrison (1994), both neutral and affective words are negative, positive and taboo words were presented. Participants
mainly or more efficiently processed by the left hemisphere, at least were asked to detect the target — here, a beep — and to then repeat a
266 J. Bertels et al. / Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278

word of the pair presented on a pre-specified side, in the same way as willingness to be socially desirable (the Marlowe–Crowne Scale
that they had to read aloud the upper word of the pair in the original of Social Desirability, SDS; Crowne & Marlowe, 1960) in order to
version of the dot probe task in order to make sure they paid attention examine whether these measures would correlate with the
to the linguistic stimuli. emotional effects. Table 1 displays the average scores obtained in
Following the auditory cuing literature, the use of a detection these questionnaires.
task would prevent the occurrence of any validity effect (Rhodes, Participants were randomly assigned to one of four groups (8 par-
1987; Schmitt et al., 2000), at least when loudspeakers and ticipants in each group) created on the basis of the pair-beep interval
uninformative cues are used (Spence & Driver, 1994). Indeed, the length (short/long) and the side of the auditory space (the
lack of any spatial representation linked to the task would probably loudspeaker) to which they had to pay attention in order to repeat
preclude spatial orienting towards the location of the auditory cue. the word presented on that side (left/right), hereafter called repetition
Nevertheless, Buchtel, Butter, and Ayvasik (1996) argued that side.
validity effects are observed with a detection task when there is
an orienting reaction towards the source of the sound, namely in
conditions favouring the intention to orient the head to a sound 2.1.2. Material and apparatus
source. Following this idea, given the instruction to repeat a word Words were orally presented. They were spoken by a French-
presented on a pre-specified loudspeaker on each trial (i.e. to orient speaking female theatre student in a neutral tone of voice and were
attention voluntarily towards one side of the auditory space) and in digitally recorded on a Sony MiniDisc. Stimuli were then transferred
spite of the fact that a detection task and uninformative cues were on a Macintosh Powerbook G3 via the Digidesign DIGI 002 Rack
used in Experiment 1, we expected to observe congruency effects interface and were cleaned and normalized with the Protools
between the position of the emotional word in the pair and the Digidesign 6.2.2. software. Mean word duration was 719 ms (standard
position of the following beep. deviation: 135 ms).
As any activation of the “what–where” connections is presumably The words used in this experiment (as in the following ones)
due to the need to register relatively rapid changes in the were chosen as described below. There were 480 pairs of mono- or
environment, we assumed that it would be transient. To test this bi-syllabic words: 120 emotional pairs, in which one of the words
idea we used two word-beep intervals here, one short and one long, was emotionally neutral and the other was emotionally charged (see
and expected the predicted attentional bias (i.e. facilitated processing Appendix A), and 360 neutral pairs, consisting of two emotionally
of the beep presented at the same spatial location as the emotional neutral words. Emotional pairs included 40 positive (e.g., amour/
word of the pair, compared to the beep presented at the opposite side) agence — love/agency), 40 negative (e.g., bombe/bulle — bomb/
to occur with the short interval, not — or at least less so — with the bubble) and 40 taboo pairs (e.g., salope/serrure — bitch/lock), defined
long one. Moreover, if an attentional bias were to occur in the long as such because the emotional word in the pair was positive,
interval condition, we predicted it to be negative. Namely, partici- negative or taboo. These words were generated by collaborators or
pants would detect the beep faster if presented at the opposite chosen from the relevant literature (Bonin et al., 2003; Hermans &
location (i.e. at the location of the neutral word) than if it was De Houwer, 1994; Messina, Morais & Cantraine, 1989; Van der
presented at the same location as the emotional word. As a matter of Goten, De Vooght & Kemps, 1999). In accordance with the original
fact, given the lengthening of the SOA (Stimulus Onset Asynchrony, version of the dot probe task (e.g. MacLeod et al., 1986, see also Fox,
i.e. the time between the onset of the word and the onset of the beep) 1993 and Salemink et al., 2007), all the 120 emotional pairs as well
in this condition, one could expect an inhibition of return phenom- as 120 neutral pairs (i.e., as many neutral as emotional pairs) were
enon (IOR, cf. Posner & Cohen, 1984) to occur: with long SOAs, critical pairs, as they were followed by a beep. The others were filler
attention oriented to the location of the cue would come back to a pairs, also considered as catch trials, since there was no beep to
central location for the application of inhibitory processes to the detect.
location previously explored, making it easier to process any On the basis of the database Lexique 2 (New, Pallier, Ferrand, &
information coming at the opposite rather than at the same location Matos, 2001), each category of emotional words was matched to
as the cue. the set of neutral words associated with them in a pair, according
to the number of phonological neighbours, literary frequency and
2.1. Method web frequency. The use of web frequency was crucial for taboo
words given that it better reflects their frequency of use than the
2.1.1. Participants literary frequency. Hence, these matchings ensure that attentional
Participants were 35 first-year students of the Université Libre orienting towards the spatial location of either type of word is not
de Bruxelles (31 women; 3 left-handed), ranging from 17 to due to these lexical factors. According to the same criteria, we also
40 years (mean: 20.3). They were given course credits for their matched all emotional words to all associated neutral words, and
participation. The results of three women, all right-handed, were the two series of neutral words constituting the neutral critical
discarded from the analyses: two because their average RTs (all pairs. Finally, we matched these four sets of material. These
experimental conditions mingled) were superior to two standard matching sets led to non-significant difference at 0.05. Within a
deviations above the overall average performance, and one because
of her inability to repeat correctly the presented words. All
participants had spoken French for at least the last 10 years.
Previous studies investigating attentional biases in the visual Table 1
modality pointed to the robustness of this phenomenon only in Average standard scores obtained on the STAI-Y (Spielberger, 1983) and on the SDS
selected groups of participants such as anxious subjects (e.g., Bar- (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960) and average raw scores obtained on the BDI–II (Beck et al.,
1996) in Experiments 1, 2 and 3 (standard deviations are in parentheses).
Haim, Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans–Kranenburg, & van Ijzendoorn,
2007), repressors (Mogg et al., 2000) and depressed people (e.g., Exp. 1 (n = 32) Exp. 2 (n = 24) Exp. 3 (n = 20)
Mogg, Bradley & Williams, 1995). Therefore, we used personality STAI-Y
questionnaires (filled in after the beep probe task) evaluating State anxiety 48.09 (10.80) 46.38 (7.46) 48.05 (6.79)
anxiety and depression levels (the Spielberger Trait-State Anxiety Trait anxiety 48.84 (10.36) 49.58 (10.81) 46.75 (9.54)
Inventory, STAI-Y; Spielberger, 1983, and the Beck Depression SDS 47.72 (22.35) 47.83 (12.01) 46.96 (7.64)
BDI-II 10.56 (7.8) 12.08 (8.54) 9.4 (5.59)
Inventory, BDI–II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996, respectively), as well
J. Bertels et al. / Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278 267

pair, the first phoneme, the syllabic structure and the phonological of the taboo words was positively correlated with their arousal
uniqueness point of the two words were identical. Words of the level, r = 0.56, p b 0.001.
filler pairs were only matched in terms of first phoneme and In the fourth post-hoc study, 24 participants rated the same
syllabic structure. words as in study 3 on a 1 (unfamiliar) to 5 (very familiar) scale for
Four post-hoc control studies were carried out on these words or familiarity. Regarding words presented in the non-excluded pairs,
part of them. statistical analyses revealed that familiarity differed between the
In the first post-hoc study, the 480 words constituting the critical emotional types of words, F(2, 109) = 66.679, p b 0.001, η2 = 0.555,
pairs, a priori classified as neutral (360), positive (40), negative (40) with positive words more familiar than the negative and taboo
and taboo (40), were presented to 24 listeners, who were asked to words, which also differed from each other (all p ≤ 0.001,
rate each word on a 1 (very negative) to 7 (very positive) scale for Bonferroni corrected). We also calculated for each pair the
emotionality. The difference between the emotional means of the two difference in familiarity between the emotional word and its
words of each pair was calculated from these ratings. For emotional neutral associate. The effect of type of emotional pair was
pairs of words the minimal acceptable difference was one point, significant, F(2, 109) = 3.36, p = 0.04, η2 = 0.059, reflecting that
whereas for neutral pairs the difference had to be smaller than one positive pairs led to a greater difference than negative (0.45 vs.
point. Following this criterion, two negative, seven positive and 14 0.08, p = 0.04, Bonferroni corrected), but not taboo pairs (0.34,
neutral pairs were removed from analyses. F b 1). The difference between negative and taboo pairs did not
In the second post-hoc study, a list of 120 words a priori reach significance (p N 0.10).
classified as neutral (80) and taboo (40) was presented to 24 In the beep probe task, each pair of words was presented
listeners, who were asked to rate each word on a 1 (normal) to 5 diotically, namely with the two words constituting the pair
(very taboo, shocking) scale for obscenity. No word was removed presented simultaneously with each different member spoken
from analyses on the basis of these ratings, as the confidence through a different, lateralized, loudspeaker. Words within each
intervals of the two word categories did not overlap and as the pair were synchronized at their onset and offset, which was
minimum and the maximum values corresponding to one category facilitated by the fact that they had the same syllabic structure, and
did not enter into the confidence interval of the other category; were uttered at a constant rate. This synchronisation was made by
however, one taboo word was removed because more than the half operating very short excisions using the Protools Digidesign 6.2.2.,
of the participants did not understand its shocking meaning and was perceptually evaluated by the first author. Critical pairs
(biroute). were followed by a 100-ms beep. Filler pairs were followed by a
The characteristics of the emotional pairs that were taken into silence of identical duration.
account in the statistical analyses are presented in Table 2. Since the
shock value was only assessed for the taboo words and part of the 2.1.3. Procedure
neutral words, only data concerning the shock value of the taboo Participants sat in front of a computer. Two loudspeakers were
words are mentioned in Table 1. It should be noted that the exclusion located at 45 cm on their left and right, with an anterior deviation
of 24 pairs on the basis of their emotional valence or the of 60° in relation to the sagittal plane. Stimulus presentation and
understanding of their meaning (see above) did not affect the timing as well as data collection were controlled using the
matching of the three categories of emotional pairs, on literary Psyscope button box and 1.2.5. PPC software (Cohen, MacWhinney,
frequency, web frequency or the number of phonological neighbours. Flatt, & Provost, 1993) running on a Macintosh Performa 6320.
Results from the two subsequent control studies were indicative Each session began with detailed instructions. Participants were
and did not serve to exclude pairs from further analyses. In the told that they would hear pairs of different words, presented
third post-hoc study, 24 participants were asked to rate the 240 simultaneously, one on the left and one on the right, and that in
words from the emotional pairs, a priori classified as neutral (120), half of the trials, a beep would be presented after the pair of words,
negative (40), positive (40) and taboo (40), on a 1 (very quiet) to 7 either on the left or the right. They had to perform two tasks on
(very arousing) arousal scale. Regarding words presented in the each trial: firstly, to detect the beep by pressing the right key of a
non-excluded pairs, statistical analysis showed that the arousal button box as quickly and accurately as possible with the preferred
level differed between the emotional types of words, F(2, 109) = hand, without caring about its side of presentation; and secondly,
450.29, p b 0.001, η2 = 0.894, each type differing from the others, to repeat as accurately as possible the word presented at a pre-
p b 0.001 for all (Bonferroni corrected), with negative words specified side (the same throughout the testing), to ensure they
producing the highest arousal, followed by the taboo and the paid attention to the presented words.
positive words. For each pair we also calculated the difference in Each trial started with the presentation of a 750 ms fixation
arousal level between the emotional word and its neutral associate. cross in the middle of the screen. Then, a pair of auditory words
The effect of the type of emotional pair was significant, F(2, 109) = was presented. In half of the trials, immediately or 250 ms after the
258.2, p b 0.001, η2 = 0.828, each type differing from all the others offset of the words (depending on the participants), a beep was
at p b 0.001 (Bonferroni corrected). Negative pairs led to the presented, at the location of one of the words. The participant had
greatest difference (mean: 2.04), followed by taboo (1.37) and 2500 ms to answer. The interval between the response and the
positive pairs (−1.64).1 It is also worth noting that the shock value next trial was 2000 ms. Trials were pseudo-randomly presented: a
beep was presented in not more than three trials in a row, and
similarly a word of the same emotional type was presented in not
more than three trials in a row whatever the presentation side, and
never more than in two trials in a row on the same presentation
side.
1
As we developed in a normative study (Bertels, Kolinsky, & Morais, 2009), it is not The experiment started with a 36-trial practice block, during
surprising that positive words were judged as less arousing than their associate which participants received feedback regarding their performance.
neutral words given that in the 7-point scale we used the “1” level depicting a very Next, each participant was presented with five 96-trial blocks,
calm, relaxing feeling; the “7” level reflecting a very arousing, exciting feeling; and the without any feedback. Thus, a participant was presented with each
“4” level referring to no particular feeling. This actually means that while neutral
words were judged (on average) as not evoking any particular arousing feeling,
pair only once insofar as assignment of a specific word to one side
positive words were in fact judged as more calming and relaxing than neutral words (e.g., “beauté–bateau” vs. “bateau–beauté”) and beep location were
(see Table 2). counterbalanced between participants.
268 J. Bertels et al. / Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278

Table 2
Characteristics of the emotional pairs taken into account in the statistical analyses, following post-hoc control studies.

n Literary frequency Web frequency Number of phonological neighbours Emotional valence Shock value Arousal Familiarity

Negative pairs 38
Negative words 32.163 4261.413 7.053 2.08 – 5.81 3.83
Neutral words 30.046 9021.363 6.658 4.445 – 3.774 3.751
Positive pairs 33
Positive words 82.42 15,059.057 10.061 6.16 – 2.191 4.467
Neutral words 248.035 16,487.881 11.364 4.433 – 3.828 4.013
Taboo pairs 39
Taboo words 5.676 363.245 8.769 2.877 2.984 5.052 3.549
Neutral words 5.195 1385.998 7.41 4.386 – 3.686 3.209

2.2. Results adjusted post-hoc comparisons showed that taboo pairs led to longer
RTs than negative and positive pairs, both p b 0.001, which did not
Errors (misses and false alarms) were rare, 0.28% on average. differ between each other, p N 0.10.
Therefore, the analyses focused on RT data only. RTs on erroneous More crucially, there was a significant three-way interaction
trials as well as on trials in which the participant had not correctly between type of emotional pair, beep location and emotional word
repeated the word (in total, 11.18% of the observations) were location, F(2, 56) = 5.07, p b 0.01, partial η2 = 0.153, superimposed by
excluded from further analyses. This was also the case for RTs on a significant four-way interaction between type of emotional pair,
the 24 pairs for which (a) the emotional word did not differ beep location, emotional word location and interval length, F(2, 56) =
considerably on emotional valence from the neutral word; or 3.27, p b 0.05, partial η2 = 0.104. Considering each interval condition
(b) one neutral word did differ on emotional valence from the separately, we found the effect of type of emotional pair to be
other neutral word of the pair; or (c) the shocking meaning was significant in both, F(2, 28) = 14.27 and 6.53, both p b 0.01, partial
not straightforward (see Method).2 Table 3 displays the mean η2 = 0.505 and 0.318. In contrast, the three-way interaction between
correct RTs. type of emotional pair, beep location and emotional word location
Firstly, we examined whether the presence of an emotional word was significant in the short interval condition, F(2, 28) = 7.97,
of a particular valence affected the response to the beep. For this p = 0.002, partial η2 = 0.363, but not in the long one, F b 1.
purpose, a 2 (interval length: short/long) × 2 (repetition side: left/ In the short interval condition, we analysed separately the data for
right) × 4 (type of emotional pair: neutral/negative/positive/taboo) each type of emotional pair, given that we predicted the occurrence of
× 2 (beep location: left/right) repeated measures analysis of variance attentional biases with taboo and negative pairs, but not with positive
(ANOVA) design was applied on correct response latencies. The type pairs. The interaction between beep location and emotional word
of emotional pair and the beep location were within-subject factors, location was significant for the taboo pairs, F(1, 14) = 9.46, p b 0.01,
interval length and repetition side were between-subject factors. partial η2 = 0.403, but not for the negative pairs, F(1, 14) = 1.8, p N 0.10,
The only significant factor was the type of emotional pair, F(3, 84) = partial η2 = 0.114; for positive pairs there was only a trend, F(1, 14) =
17.8, p b 0.01, partial η2 = 0.389: taboo pairs led to significantly longer 3.7, p = 0.08, partial η2 = 0.208. This interaction reflects the fact that,
RTs (463 ms, on the average) than neutral (416 ms), positive (415 ms) when the taboo word was right-presented, participants detected the
and negative (414 ms) pairs respectively, all p b 0.01 (Bonferroni beep faster when this was right-presented (434 ms) than when it was
corrected). No other comparison was significant, all p N 0.10. The left-presented (515 ms), p b 0.001 (Bonferroni corrected). No effect
interaction between type of emotional pair and interval length did not was observed when the taboo word was left-presented, p N 0.10 (see
reach significance, F b 1. Fig. 1). Thus, the attentional bias reflected by the RT difference (81 ms)
The second important point was to assess whether emotional
words led to attentional biases, namely if they facilitated the detection
of the beep when this was presented at the same spatial location (in Table 3
Mean correct detection times observed in Experiment 1 for all types of pairs (standard
spatially congruent trials) as the emotional word in comparison to the
errors are in parentheses).
situation where it was presented at the opposite spatial location (in
spatially incongruent trials). As in other studies that looked for Type of emotional pair
attentional biases linked to the emotional valence of the stimuli, the Beep and emotional word Neutral Negative Positive Taboo
analyses only took the results on emotional pairs into account, given locations
that, by definition, emotional word location is not available for neutral Short interval condition
pairs. A 2 (interval length: short/long) × 2 (repetition side: left/ Beep Left 436 (21)
right) × 3 (type of emotional pair: positive/negative/taboo) × 2 (beep Emotional word Left 448 (28) 435 (21) 468 (27)
Emotional word Right 402 (17) 407 (19) 515 (29)
location: left/right) × 2 (emotional word location: left/right) repeated
Beep Right 424 (20)
measures ANOVA design was applied on response latencies. Interval Emotional word Left 429 (23) 407 (22) 489 (33)
length and repetition side were between-subject factors, the other Emotional word Right 431 (24) 431 (29) 434 (27)
variables were within-subject factors. The only main effect that Average 430 (17) 427 (18) 420 (18) 477 (24)
reached significance was the type of emotional pair, F(2, 56) = 18.46,
Long interval condition
p b 0.001, partial η2 = 0.397. As with the previous analysis, Bonferroni Beep Left 409 (17)
Emotional word Left 394 (16) 409 (17) 428 (18)
Emotional word Right 397 (20) 398 (20) 459 (33)
2 Beep Right 395 (21)
Note that results from the statistical analyses on the whole set of data were similar
Emotional word Left 406 (25) 410 (23) 444 (28)
to the ones obtained after exclusion of these pairs. In particular, the four-way
Emotional word Right 403 (22) 422 (33) 467 (37)
interaction between type of emotional pair, beep location, emotional word location
Average 402 (17) 400 (18) 410 (18) 450 (24)
and interval length was significant, F(2, 56) = 4.37, p b 0.02, partial η2 = 0.135.
J. Bertels et al. / Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278 269

To put it in a formula: x̅ RTs taboo R-beep R b x̅ RTs taboo L (beep L&R).


In contrast, difficulties to disengage attention from right-presented
taboo words would be reflected in increased RTs on left-located beeps
following right-presented taboo words compared to RTs on beeps
following left-presented taboo words, i.e. x̅ RTs taboo R-beep L N x̅ RTs
taboo L (beep L&R). Given that general, non-spatial effects of taboo
words do not depend on the location of the taboo word in the pair, any
difference between these RTs can only be allocated to spatial effects of
right-presented taboo words.
As can be seen in Fig. 1, in the short interval condition of the present
experiment, RTs on trials in which both the taboo word and the beep
were right-presented were shorter than RTs on trials in which the
taboo word was left-presented (434 vs. 479 ms, t(15) = 2.35,
p = 0.035). In contrast, RTs on trials in which the right-presented
Fig. 1. Target detection latencies observed for taboo pairs in Experiment 1, in the short taboo word was followed by a left-located beep did not differ from RTs
interval length condition, as a function of the emotional word and beep locations. on trials in which the taboo word was left-presented (515 vs. 479 ms, t
(15) = 1.75, p = 0.10). Hence, these results suggest that attention
would be engaged preferentially to the spatial location of a right-
presented taboo word relative to the spatial location of the neutral
between the spatially incongruent and congruent taboo trials was word presented simultaneously in the pair.
restricted to the condition in which the taboo word was right-
presented. This bias was not correlated with any of the scores on the
2.3. Discussion
personality questionnaires (STAI-state: r = −0.15; STAI-trait: r = 0.11;
BDI–II: r = −0.05; SDS: r = 0.07, all p N 0.50).
In Experiment 1, participants had to detect a beep as quickly as
Moreover, the attentional bias linked to right-presented taboo
possible and to repeat on each trial a word presented at a pre-
words in the short interval condition was significantly larger than the
specified side. We observed an attentional bias: the detection of a
non-significant biases associated with right-presented negative and
beep was faster if it appeared at the same spatial location as a just
positive words (−30 and −24 ms, respectively), t(15) = 3.753 and
presented taboo word than if it appeared at the opposite location. As
4.123, both p b 0.005 (Bonferroni corrected).
predicted, this attentional bias was observed only when the beep
It is of theoretical interest to consider on which spatial attentional
followed immediately the presentation of the pair of words,
orienting component(s) taboo words had an effect. A positive attentional
suggesting that the phenomenon is very fleeting. Moreover, this
bias like the one observed here for right-presented taboo words (i.e., a
only held true when the taboo word originated from the right side of
positive difference between RTs on incongruent and congruent trials for
the listener, a specificity that allowed us to establish that the observed
these words) could be due either to vigilance to the taboo words,
attentional bias reflected attentional vigilance towards taboo words
namely an effect on the engagement component of attentional
rather than difficulties to disengage from these words.
orienting, or to difficulties in disengaging attention from these words
In addition to producing an attentional bias, taboo words also seem
once attention has been normally engaged. In the former case, the
to lead to a general, non-spatial effect. Indeed, whatever the interval
attentional bias would result from a RT advantage on congruent
length this time, taboo pairs yielded longer RTs to the beep than the
compared to incongruent trials, whereas in the latter case it would
other types of pairs, be they emotional or neutral ones.
proceed from increased RTs on incongruent trials. However, merely
Contrary to our prediction, there was no attentional bias linked to
comparing RTs on congruent and incongruent trials does not disentan-
the spatial source of negative words. However, in this experiment, the
gle these possibilities given the absence of a baseline in the emotional
concurrent repetition task may have monopolized some attentional
trials. To solve this problem, Koster, Crombez, Verschuere, and De
resources, making it difficult for negative words to facilitate
Houwer (2004) proposed to use the neutral trials (i.e., trials in which
selectively the detection of a same-side beep. This explanation was
both words are emotionally neutral) as baseline and to compare them
checked in Experiment 2 by removing the repetition task.
systematically with the RTs on congruent and incongruent trials, but this
approach has been recently criticized (Mogg, Holmes, Garner, & Bradley,
2008). Given that neutral trials do not, by definition, contain emotional 3. Experiment 2: Beep probe detection
words, these authors argued that potential non-spatial interference
effects of emotional words could contribute to the RT difference This experiment was similar to Experiment 1, except that
between neutral and spatially (in)congruent trials. As such general, participants only had to detect the beep, without having to pay
non-spatial effects of taboo words occurred in the present experiment, attention to one side of presentation in order to repeat the presented
the RT difference with neutral words would not be a pure index of the word. Moreover, given the results of the first experiment, we only
attentional orienting processes influenced by the presentation of taboo used the short interval condition for all participants.
words. In addition to reducing the cognitive load of the task, the present
Nevertheless, the specific results obtained here allow us to use experiment could also help us to discard an alternative explanation of
another baseline, to which the criticism addressed by Mogg et al. the effects observed on taboo words in Experiment 1. Indeed, in the
(2008) does not apply. Since attentional biases towards taboo words former experiment, the observed bias and delayed RTs could be linked
were only observed when the taboo word was right-presented, RTs on to the participants' emotional reaction to the requirement of repeating
left-presented taboo words can serve as a baseline to determine a taboo word in front of the experimenter, in a similar way as the taboo
whether participants were particularly vigilant to right-presented nature of the verbal response made by the subject could be responsible
taboo words or, instead, had difficulties to disengage from these for the higher recognition thresholds and galvanic skin response
words. Attentional vigilance to right-presented taboo words would be observed for those words (Zajonc, 1962). If this were the case, no
reflected in RTs on right-located beeps presented after right- specific effect of taboo words should be observed in the present
presented taboo words being shorter than the average RTs on left- experiment. On the contrary, if the effects observed in Experiment 1
and right-located beeps presented after left-presented taboo words. reflected attentional influences of taboo words, the same attentional
270 J. Bertels et al. / Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278

bias and delayed RTs should be observed here, because the present Table 4
situation no longer requires a verbal response. Mean correct detection times observed in Experiment 2 for all types of pairs (standard
errors are in parentheses).

3.1. Method Type of emotional pair

Beep and emotional word Neutral Negative Positive Taboo


3.1.1. Participants locations
Participants were 27 undergraduate students of the Université Beep Left 297 (12)
Libre de Bruxelles (24 women; 3 left-handed), ranging in age from 18 Emotional word Left 292 (11) 301 (15) 294 (13)
to 25 (mean: 18.97). They were given course credits for their Emotional word Right 282 (11) 294 (14) 303 (14)
Beep Right 294 (11)
participation. The results of three participants (3 women; 1 left-
Emotional word Left 285 (13) 291 (14) 295 (14)
handed) were discarded from further analyses, because their RTs Emotional word Right 286 (12) 301 (13) 286 (12)
were superior to two standard deviations above the mean. All Average 295 (12) 286 (12) 297 (14) 294 (13)
participants had spoken French for at least 10 years. Average scores
on three personality questionnaires evaluating anxiety and depres-
sion levels as well as willingness to be socially desirable are presented
in Table 1.
when the taboo word was right-presented, participants detected the
3.1.2. Material and apparatus beep faster when it was also right-presented than when it was left-
The material and the apparatus were the same as in Experiment 1. presented (286 vs. 303 ms), p = 0.05. No difference was observed when
the taboo word was left-presented, p N 0.10 (see Fig. 2). These
3.1.3. Procedure comparisons were not significant for negative and positive pairs, all
The procedure was exactly the same as in Experiment 1 except p N 0.10. We thus observed an attentional bias linked to taboo words
that there was no repetition task and we only used the short interval (17 ms) when these were right-presented. This bias was not correlated
condition: for all participants on critical pairs, the beep appeared with any of the scores on the personality questionnaires (STAI-state:
immediately after the offset of the words. r = 0.04; STAI-trait: r = −0.016; BDI–II: r = −0.076; SDS: r = −0.057,
all p N 0.50).
3.2. Results Moreover, the attentional bias linked to right-presented taboo
words was significantly larger than the non-significant bias associated
Errors (misses and false alarms) were again very infrequent (0.2%, with right-presented positive words (−7 ms), t(23) = 2.969, p b 0.015
on the average) so that the analyses focused only on the RT data. As in (Bonferroni corrected). However, the difference between the atten-
Experiment 1, RTs on erroneous trials as well as RTs on 24 pairs were tional biases linked to right-presented taboo and negative words
excluded from the analysis (see Method of Experiment 1).3 Table 4 (−4 ms) was not significant, p N 0.10.
displayed the mean correct RTs. Given that, as in Experiment 1, an attentional bias was only
As in the previous experiment, we first applied a 4 (type of observed when the taboo word was right-presented, we used RTs on
emotional pair: neutral/negative/positive/taboo) × 2 (beep location: trials in which taboo words were left-presented as a baseline in order
left/right) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) design on to determine which spatial orienting component was affected by the
correct response latencies to examine whether the presence of an presentation of a right-presented taboo word. However, RTs on trials
emotional word in the pair influenced the processing of an ensuing in which both the taboo word and the beep were right-presented did
beep. The type of emotional pair and the beep location were within- not significantly differ from RTs on trials in which the taboo word was
subject factors. The type of emotional pair was the only significant left-presented (286 vs. 295 ms, t(23) = 1.36, p N 0.10). Similarly, RTs
factor, F(3, 69) = 3.51, p = 0.02, partial η2 = 0.132. Bonferroni adjust- on trials in which the right-presented taboo word was followed by a
ed post-hoc comparisons showed that RTs on taboo pairs (294 ms) left-located beep did not differ from RTs on trials in which the taboo
tended to be longer than RTs on negative pairs (286 ms), p = 0.09. word was left-presented (303 vs. 295 ms, t(23) = 1.16, p N 0.10).
This was not the case for taboo pairs relative to positive ones Hence, these results do not allow us to conclude about the nature of
(297 ms), p N 0.10. the spatial component affected by the presentation of taboo words.
The same effect was observed in the 3 (type of emotional pair:
negative/positive/taboo) × 2 (beep location: left/right) × 2 (emotional
word location: left/right) repeated measures ANOVA design applied on
correct response latencies, F(2, 46) = 4.41, p b 0.02, partial η22 = 0.161.
Taboo pairs led to significantly longer RTs than negative pairs, p b 0.05.
No difference was observed for taboo pairs compared to positive ones,
p N 0.10 (Bonferroni corrected). No other effect or interaction was
significant. In particular, the three-way interaction between type of
emotional pair, beep location and emotional word location felt short of
significance, F(2, 46) = 2.71, p = 0.078, partial η2 = 0.105. Nevertheless,
Bonferroni adjusted comparisons revealed that, as in Experiment 1,

3
Results from the statistical analyses on the whole set of data were similar to the ones
obtained without the excluded pairs: the three-way interaction between type of emotional
pair, beep location and emotional word location was not significant, F(2, 46) =2.29, pN 0.10,
partial η2 = 0.09. Nevertheless, right-presented beeps following right-presented taboo
words tended to lead to shorter RTs than left-presented beeps following right-presented Fig. 2. Target detection latencies observed for taboo pairs in Experiment 2, as a function
taboo words, p= 0.065 (Bonferroni corrected). of the emotional word and beep locations.
J. Bertels et al. / Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278 271

3.3. Discussion Table 5


Mean correct localization times observed in Experiment 3 for all types of pairs (standard
errors are in parentheses).
As in Experiment 1, we observed an attentional bias linked to the
taboo nature of words. Taboo pairs also yielded longer RTs than Type of emotional pair
negative pairs. Given that participants did not now have to repeat Beep and emotional Neutral Negative Positive Taboo
words, the effects observed in Experiment 1 cannot be attributed to an word locations
emotional reaction when repeating a taboo word. The fact that these Beep Left 358 (18)
effects were larger in Experiment 1 than in the present experiment is Emotional word Left 349 (18) 359 (19) 374 (23)
most probably due to the longest RTs associated to the double task Emotional word Right 359 (19) 360 (21) 384 (22)
condition of our first experiment. Consistent with this idea, for Beep Right 349 (17)
Emotional word Left 352 (16) 329 (15) 367 (20)
Experiments 1 and 2 considered together, we observed a significant Emotional word Right 335 (16) 354 (18) 348 (19)
correlation between attentional biases linked to right-presented Average 353 (17) 349 (17) 350 (18) 368 (20)
taboo words and the average RTs, r = .461, p b 0.01.
Given that no attentional bias was observed for negative words,
contrary to what we supposed, it does not seem that the presence or
absence of such an effect of these words is, at least exclusively, related
to the extent to which attentional resources are mobilized by a as possible by pressing the left or right key of a button box. Again,
concurrent task. It may be the case that a spatial dimension needs to there was no repetition task.
be involved, in addition to a low cognitive load, in order to observe
attentional biases towards the spatial location of negative words.
Indeed, as already mentioned in the Introduction of Experiment 1, 4.2. Results
some authors argued that spatial attentional orienting towards the
location of a single peripheral cue would only occur when a spatial As in Experiments 1 and 2, errors (misses, false alarms and wrong
representation is activated by the task (Rhodes, 1987; Schmitt et al., localizations) were very infrequent (0.4%, on the average) so that the
2000; Spence & Driver, 1994). Supporting this idea, they observed analyses focused only on the RT data. Erroneous responses were
validity effects (i.e. spatial attentional orienting) with a localization excluded from further analyses. Also, we excluded from the analyses
task but not with a detection task. Considering that the presence of an the RTs on 24 pairs (see Method of Experiment 1).4 Mean correct RTs
emotional word in the pair can act as a single peripheral cue, one are presented in Table 5.
possibility is that attentional orienting towards its spatial location (i.e. We first applied a 4 (type of emotional pair: neutral/negative/
attentional biases) occurs more easily if a localization task is used, positive/taboo) × 2 (beep location: left/right) repeated measures
because this task strongly activates a spatial representation. This analysis of variance (ANOVA) on response latencies. Both variables
possibility was investigated in Experiment 3. were treated as within-subject factors. The effect of type of emotional
pair was significant, F(3, 57) = 8.57, p b 0.01, partial η2 = 0.311, with
4. Experiment 3: Beep probe localization taboo pairs leading to overall longer RTs (368 ms) than neutral
(353 ms), positive (350 ms) and negative (349 ms) pairs, all p b 0.03
This experiment was exactly the same as Experiment 2, except that (Bonferroni corrected). Also significant was the effect of beep location,
participants had to localize the beep when it was presented, instead of F(1, 19) = 9.48, p b 0.01, partial η2 = 0.333, which reflects the fact that
simply detecting it. Word repetition was not required, so that, participants localized the beep faster when it was right-presented
contrary to Experiment 1, participants' attentional resources were not than when it was left-presented (348 vs. 363 ms, respectively).
mobilized by a concurrent task. In doing so, we predicted that we Further analyses only took the results on emotional pairs into
would observe not only attentional biases linked to the taboo nature account. A 3 (type of emotional pair: positive/negative/taboo) × 2
of the words (as was the case in the two previous experiments) (beep location: left/right) × 2 (emotional word location: left/right)
but also attentional biases linked to the spatial location of negative repeated measures ANOVA design was applied on response latencies.
words. All variables were within-subjects factors. The main effects that
reached significance were the type of emotional pair, F(2, 38) = 9.32,
4.1. Method p = 0.001, partial η2 = 0.329, and the beep location, F(1, 19) = 9.64,
p b 0.01, partial η2 = 0.336, both reflecting the same differences as in
4.1.1. Participants the analysis including neutral words (see above). The three-way
Participants were 23 undergraduate students of the Université interaction between the type of emotional pair, beep location and
Libre de Bruxelles (23 women; 3 left-handed), ranging in age from 18 emotional word location was also significant, F(2, 38) = 6.01,
to 22 (mean: 19.35). They were given course credits for their p = 0.005, partial η2 = 0.24.
participation. The results of three subjects (3 women, 1 left-handed) To further examine this interaction, we analyzed separately the results
were discarded from further analyses because their error rate was for each type of emotional pair. The interaction between beep location and
superior to two standard deviations above the mean. All participants emotional word location was significant for the taboo and negative
had spoken French for at least 10 years. Average scores on three pairs, F(1, 19)=4.45, pb 0.05, partial η2 =0.19 and F(1, 19)=9.33,
personality questionnaires evaluating anxiety and depression levels pb 0.01, partial η2 =0.329, not for the positive pairs, F(1, 19)=2.92,
as well as willingness to be socially desirable are presented in Table 1. pN 0.10, partial η2 =0.133. The interaction between beep location and
emotional word location for taboo pairs reflected the fact that when
the taboo word was right-presented, participants localized a right-
4.1.2. Material and apparatus
The material and the apparatus were the same as in Experiments 1
and 2.
4
Note that results from the statistical analyses including the excluded pairs were similar
4.1.3. Procedure to those obtained without these: the three-way interaction between type of emotional
The procedure was exactly the same as in Experiment 2 except pair, beep location and emotional word location is significant, F(2, 38) = 5.25, p = 0.01,
that the participants had to localize the beep as quickly and accurately partial η2 = 0.216.
272 J. Bertels et al. / Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278

presented beep faster (348 ms) than a left-presented beep (384 ms), followed a right-presented negative word and trials in which a negative
p=0.004. There was no difference when the taboo word was left- word was left-presented (359 vs. 335 ms, t(19) = 1.194, p N 0.10).
presented, pN 0.10 (see Fig. 3a). This was also the case for negative
pairs: when the negative word was right-presented, participants 4.3. Discussion
localized a right-presented beep faster (335 ms) than a left-presented
one (359 ms), p=0.002, while there was no difference when the word As in Experiments 1 and 2, we observed that participants
was left-presented, pN 0.10 (see Fig. 3b). responded faster to a beep presented at the same location as a
We thus observed attentional biases linked to taboo (36 ms) and to taboo word than presented at the same location as a neutral word.
negative (24 ms) words when they were right-presented. None of these Again, this attentional bias was only observed when the taboo word
attentional biases was correlated with any of the scores on the was right-presented. Moreover, as in Experiment 1, results suggest
personality questionnaires (For taboo words: STAI-state: r = 0.07, that participants did not have difficulties in disengaging attention
STAI-trait: r = 0.23, BDI-II: r = −0.13, SDS: r = −0.10, all p N 0.30; for from taboo words but were, rather, particularly vigilant to these
negative words: STAI-state: r = −0.06, STAI-trait: r = 0.15, BDI–II: words. This spatial effect of taboo words thus appears to be robust. In
r = 0.21, SDS: r = −0.30, all p N 0.15 ). addition, as in the previous experiments, trials in which a taboo word
The difference between the attentional biases linked to right- was presented led to overall longer RTs to a following beep than other
presented negative and taboo words and the attentional bias linked to types of trials, whatever the side of presentation of the beep.
right-presented positive words (6 ms) did not reach significance, both Importantly, in Experiment 3, an attentional bias was also
p ≥ 0.10 (Bonferroni corrected). As expected, the biases linked to right- observed towards negative words. As with the attentional biases
presented negative and taboo words did not differ from each other, t b 1. observed for taboo words, this bias for negative words was only
As can be seen in Fig. 3a, RTs on trials in which both the taboo word observed when the emotional word was right-presented and most
and the beep were right-presented were shorter than RTs on trials in probably reflects attentional vigilance towards these words.
which the taboo word was left-presented (348 vs. 370 ms, t(19) =
2.34, p = 0.03). In contrast, RTs on trials in which the right-presented 5. General discussion
taboo word was followed by a left-located beep did not differ from RTs
on trials in which the taboo word was left-presented (384 vs. 370 ms, While the distinction between the “what” and the “where” auditory
t(19) = 1.2, p N 0.10). Similarly, as depicted by Fig. 3b, RTs on trials in subsystems is largely accepted (e.g., Kubovy & Van Valkenburg,
which both the negative word and the beep were right-presented 2001), the functional link between these subsystems has been
were shorter than RTs on trials in which the negative word was left- relatively neglected. Yet such a link would be advantageous for
presented (335 vs. 350 ms, t(19) = 2.34, p = 0.03). On the contrary, no ecological adaptation: if the identification of an auditory object that
RT difference was found between trials in which a left-located beep conveys emotional information leads to activate the information about
its spatial location, then one would be in a better attentional state to
react to subsequent auditory information coming from the same spatial
location.
In the present study, we investigated the existence of a functional
link between the auditory “what” and “where” subsystems by
examining the influence of the emotional content of spoken words,
linked to the “what” subsystem, on the spatial orienting of auditory
attention, relying on the “where” subsystem. This was done by using a
specific kind of cuing situation (Posner, 1980), actually an auditory
adaptation of the attentional deployment task (MacLeod et al., 1986)
in which a pair of spoken words — one potentially emotional — was
followed by a beep target. Considering that the presence of an
emotional word of the pair can act as a single peripheral cue, we
expected to observe spatial congruency effects, namely attentional
biases linked to the spatial location of taboo and negative words, but
no specific effect of positive words was anticipated. Moreover,
according to Kinsbourne's (1970) theory, as the presentation of
verbal material could prime the left hemisphere, leading to an
orientational bias to the contralateral hemifield, and following the
idea that an emotional word catches more attentional resources than
its paired neutral word (therefore activating to a greater extent the
hemispheric contralateral to its side of presentation), we predicted
attentional biases linked to the emotional valence of the words to
occur largely when the emotional word was right-presented.
An overview of the experiments and their associated results is
presented in Table 6. Crucially, in addition to non-spatial inhibitory
effects of taboo words, attentional biases linked to the spatial location
of taboo and negative spoken words were observed. Namely,
participants responded faster to a beep presented at the same spatial
location as the taboo or negative word of the pair than to the neutral
word. In particular, attentional biases towards taboo words were
consistently observed in three experiments, regardless of whether
attention to the words and a spatial response were required or not. In
Fig. 3. a and b. Target localization latencies observed in Experiment 3, as a function of
contrast, attentional biases towards negative words were only
the emotional word and beep locations, for taboo (a) and negative (b) pairs, observed when the attentional load was low, that is when attention
respectively. to the words was not required, and when participants had to indicate
J. Bertels et al. / Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278 273

Table 6 emotional trials exclusively. Hence, if there were any influence of


Overview of the three experiments. the predictive nature of the occurrence of an emotional word, this
Experiment Interval Task(s) Results influence marred all these reaction times, making them comparable
length (ms) and warranting that any difference between them is due to their
Spatial effects Non-spatial effects specific emotional content.
(attentional biases)
Finally, in Experiment 3, although target beeps were also
1 0 Beep Right taboo words RTs taboo N RTs presented more often after emotional than after neutral trials, the
detection negative, positive,
participants' task was to localize the beep. Thus, an adequate response
then neutral words
repetition could not be prepared on the basis of the occurrence of an emotional
250 Beep – RTs taboo N RTs word in the pair, given that the location of the emotional word of the
detection negative, positive, pair (left/right) was not predictive of the location of the beep. Still, we
then neutral words observed results similar to those of Experiments 1 and 2, indicating
repetition
that the predictability of the relation between the presence of an
2 0 Beep Right taboo words RTs taboo N RTs
detection negative words emotional word and the occurrence of the beep, even if it were noted
3 0 Beep Right taboo and RTs taboo N RTs by the participants (which is unwarranted), could not be responsible
localization negative words negative, positive, for the observation of spatial and non-spatial effects for taboo and
neutral words
negative words, respectively.

5.1. Attentional biases to taboo words

the spatial location of the beep by pressing one of two response An attentional bias linked to the spatial source of right-presented
buttons (as in Experiment 3). Importantly, these attentional biases taboo words was consistently observed in three experiments. This is
occurred only when the emotional word of the pair was right- consistent with previous results from visual studies that used the
presented, which is coherent with Kinsbourne's (1970) theory. taboo variant of the emotional Stroop task (the taboo Stroop task), in
Notably, these attentional biases were observed in unselected which it has been repeatedly shown that healthy, unselected
volunteers and were not correlated with any of the scores in the participants took longer to name the ink colour of taboo words
personality questionnaires. Our data are thus in accordance with the compared to neutral words (MacKay & Ahmetzanov, 2005; MacKay
idea of an emotion-dependent functional link between the “what” and et al., 2004; Siegrist, 1995; Taylor, Kornblum, Lauber, Minoshima, &
the “where” auditory subsystems. Koeppe, 1997). These interference effects are generally considered to
Before discussing the conditions under which this link is observed, a be attentional biases. However, as in the Stroop setting the two
relevant methodological point should be addressed. In all experiments, dimensions of the stimulus, relevant and irrelevant for the task (the
we adopted MacLeod et al.'s (1986) original design of the dot probe task ink colour and the emotional valence, respectively), are part of the
that was largely used in other visual studies (e.g., Fox, 1993; Salemink same stimulus, no spatial attentional shift is expected to occur. Given
et al., 2007). As previously commented on by Mogg and Bradley (1998, that taboo words have never been used in paradigms involving spatial
1999), in this design, the occurrence of an emotional word is predictive attentional orienting such as the dot probe task (MacLeod et al., 1986)
of the occurrence of the target. More specifically, in the present and the emotional cuing paradigm (Stormark, Nordby, & Hugdahl,
experiments the beep was presented after all emotional pairs but only 1995), the present results extend the findings already reported in the
after one third of the neutral pairs. Stroop situation for visually presented taboo words by showing
Nevertheless, the present results cannot be accounted for by the spatial attentional biases for auditorily presented taboo words. In
predictability of the occurrence of the beep. As a matter of fact, in the addition, in comparison to most visual studies using taboo words (but
present study, we used several types of emotional words (i.e., negative, see Nielsen & Sarason, 1981), the present results may have more
positive, and taboo). Thus, if the predictability of the occurrence of the ecological validity, given that such words are more common in oral
beep following an emotional word were responsible for the observed than in written language.
spatial and non-spatial effects, then these effects would be observed for We also examined, more specifically, on which spatial atten-
all emotional words. On the contrary, when a detection task was used tional orienting component(s) taboo words had an effect. For this
(Experiments 1 and 2), only taboo words influenced the spatial purpose, we compared RTs on congruent and incongruent right-
orienting of attention, only negative words led to non-spatial effects, presented taboo trials with RTs on left-presented taboo trials
and positive words did not elicit either type of effect. Moreover, (acting as baseline, given that no attentional bias was observed on
attentional biases were only observed when the emotional word was these trials). In Experiments 1 and 3, we observed an RT advantage
right-presented in the linguistic pair. This cannot be a consequence for congruent right-presented taboo trials compared to baseline
of the fact that the presence of an emotional word in the pair was left-presented taboo trials, but found no difference between RTs on
predictive of the occurrence of the beep, given that the side of presen- incongruent right-presented taboo trials and left-presented taboo
tation of the emotional word was unpredictable. Indeed, it does not trials. This is more consistent with the hypothesis of an attentional
seem reasonable to admit that listeners only attended to taboo words on vigilance towards taboo words than with the idea of difficulties to
their right side or that they decided that only taboo words presented on disengage attention from these words once attention has been
their right side were predictive of the occurrence of the beep. However, normally engaged. Attention would be engaged preferentially to
even if the present results eliminate the possibility that the attentional the spatial location of a right-presented taboo word relative to the
effects would result from the detection of the predictability of the spatial location of the neutral word presented simultaneously in
occurrence of the target, further studies should rather present a to-be- the pair. However, further studies using a different methodology
localized beep on each (emotional or neutral) trial, as it is often the case should confirm these findings. Indeed, as suggested by an
in dot probe studies (e.g., Mogg, Bradley, De Bono, & Painter, 1997; Mogg anonymous Reviewer, the fact that no attentional bias was
& Bradley, 1999). observed for left-presented taboo words does not ensure that
It ought also to be taken into account that the occurrence of they did not have any influence on RTs (e.g., they could be
attentional biases linked to the presentation of a specific type of antagonistic). As such, RTs on emotional trials do not appear to be
emotional word was investigated comparing reaction times to the most proper baseline.
274 J. Bertels et al. / Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278

Further work should aim at clarifying the basis of this attentional link with the emotional information than spatial location. However,
vigilance towards spoken taboo words. Nevertheless, it is worth as the effect of two intervals were compared only in Experiment 1
noting that the set of taboo words we used in the experiments was (given that no attentional bias was observed with the long one)
matched in literary and web frequencies with the set of neutral words and as no other study using an attentional deployment task
with which they were associated in the pairs. The infrequent nature of compared different conditions of intervals between the pair of
taboo words is therefore unlikely to be responsible for the occurrence words and the probe, our observation remains isolated, and hence
of the attentional bias. Moreover, one of the post-hoc control studies should be considered with caution.
confirmed that our taboo words were not less familiar than the
associated neutral words (see Method section of Experiment 1). 5.2. Attentional biases towards negative words
Rather, two potential factors could have held the participants'
attention: the arousing and shocking nature of the taboo words. In addition to attentional biases to right-presented taboo words,
According to various studies, arousal would account for the we also observed attentional biases to right-presented negative words
observed effects on attention (e.g., Anderson, 2005; Schimmack, in Experiment 3, but this was not the case in Experiments 1 and 2.
2005) as well as on memory (e.g., Buchanan, Etzel, Adolphs, & Tranel, Therefore, the attentional bias to negative words appears to be less
2006; Kensinger & Corkin, 2003; Mather et al., 2006). Using the robust than the attentional biases to taboo words or, instead, to occur
attentional blink paradigm, recent studies showed preferential under specific conditions.
attentional processing of taboo stimuli (Arnell, Killman, & Fijavz, A crucial factor could be cognitive load, with spatial effects of the
2007; Mathewson, Arnell, & Mansfield, 2008; Most, Smith, Cooter, negative valence observable only under low-load conditions. Indeed,
Levy, & Zald, 2007), and some researchers argued that the arousal in Experiment 3, the average RTs were far shorter than in
nature of these stimuli in fact influences their encoding in memory Experiment 1 (344 vs. 439 ms). The idea that task-irrelevant
(Arnell et al., 2007; Mathewson et al., 2008). However, the whole set emotional stimuli are subject to the effects of processing load (as
of data does not seem to support the idea that arousal is the only are neutral stimuli, cf. Lavie, 1995) is supported by studies showing
factor leading to attentional capture: in Mathewson et al.'s (2008) that brain responses usually observed to emotional stimuli disappear
study, the threatening words used in the experiments were as when cognitive load increases (e.g., Doallo, Holguín, & Cadaveira,
arousing as taboo words, but had no effect. Hence, even if arousal is a 2006; Mitchell et al., 2007; Pessoa, McKenna, Gutierrez, & Ungerlei-
crucial dimension, taboo words seem to have a particular quality der, 2002; Pessoa, Padmala, & Morland, 2005). Also, at the
which accounts for this different effect. behavioural level, interference effects of emotional stimuli on
Although our study was not initially designed to assess the impact performance vanish with the increase of task demands, suggesting
of arousal, two results of our post-hoc control study (see Method that interference only occurs when sufficient resources are available
section of Experiment 1) enable us to discard this factor as the main to process task-irrelevant emotional information (Okon-Singer,
basis of the observed effects. Firstly, no correlation was found Tzelgov, & Henik, 2007; Smith Erthal et al., 2005). Similarly, in our
between the attentional bias when the emotional word was right- study, participants were particularly vigilant to negative stimuli
presented and the difference in arousal between the emotional and when cognitive load was low (Experiment 3), not at all when it was
the neutral word of the pair in any of the experiments (all p N 0.10). high (Experiment 1).
Hence, this difference does not seem to be a good predictor of the size However, cognitive load does not explain the whole phenomenon.
of the attentional bias (highest R2: 0.041). This was the case both As a matter of fact, no attentional bias to negative words was observed
when the correlation analyses included all emotional words irrespec- in Experiment 2, although participants only had to detect the presence
tive of valence and when separate correlation analyses were of a beep. As already developed in the Discussion section of
performed for positive, negative and taboo words. Secondly, the Experiment 2, one possibility is that negative words would only
arousal level of taboo words was lower than the arousal level of the influence the spatial orienting of auditory attention when the spatial
negative words used in the experiments. Also, the difference in location of the beep is task-relevant, as was the case in Experiment 3,
arousal between the emotional and the neutral word of the pair was but not in Experiment 2. Actually, when negative words are
lower for taboo than for negative pairs. If arousal were the crucial presented, the involvement of a spatial orienting component would
factor in the occurrence of the effect, we should have observed robust be critical to permit “what”-related dimensions to influence “where”-
attentional biases towards negative words as well. Thus, rather than related ones, in addition to a low cognitive load. The “where”
arousal, it seems that it is their shocking quality (perhaps combined subsystem would be influenced by the “what” subsystem that detects
with arousal) that is the relevant factor of the attentional bias they a negative emotional content only when it is previously activated,
provoke. As MacKay et al. (2004) suggested, the particular type of namely when it is somehow relevant to the participant's goal. These
emotional reaction could play a crucial role in the culmination of the assumptions require verification in further studies.
process. For example, taboo words may create sexual arousal or Interestingly, as was the case with taboo words, attentional biases
embarrassment, which would be responsible for this particular to right-presented negative words would be due to participants being
involvement of attentional processes. particularly vigilant to these words: when presented together with a
Attentional biases linked to taboo words appear to be short- neutral word, attention would be oriented preferentially to the spatial
lived, suggesting that the activation of the “what-where” connec- location of the negative word.
tions would be transient. As a matter of fact, in Experiment 1,
attentional biases to taboo words were only observed when the 5.3. Laterality effects in attention
interval between the offset of the words and the onset of the beep
was null, not when it was 250 ms long. The fact that an attentional Following Kinsbourne's (1970) theory, we assumed that the use of
bias only occurs towards events (here, the beep) which are verbal material would prime the left hemisphere, leading to
concomitant (e.g., in the emotional Stroop task) or which orientational biases to the contralateral side (the right one).
immediately follow the emotional information (taboo, in our Ordinarily, one would expect left rather than right orientational
case) seems adaptive: given their transitory nature, emotional biases, given the emotional nature of the words and the fact that
auditory stimuli would create reactions having an influence on the emotions are processed in the right hemisphere (e.g., Bryden &
processing of immediately subsequent events, not on the proces- MacRae, 1989). However, this assumption comes from studies in
sing of later ones. This would be the case particularly when which participants had to process the emotional dimension of the
subsequent events are not emotional and when they have no other stimuli, which was not the case in our experiments. In Experiments 2
J. Bertels et al. / Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278 275

and 3 they were not even required to process the words at all. 5.4. Non-spatial effects of emotional words
Moreover, not all words were emotionally charged. Hence, the mere
presentation of words seems to activate the left hemisphere, and this In all experiments, taboo pairs led to longer RTs to the beep. These
activation could have been reinforced by the expectations of the inhibitory effects of taboo words ought to be considered as non-
participants as to the verbal nature of the major part of the material, spatial, because (a) they do not depend on the spatial congruence
leading to a right orientational bias. This idea resonates with the between the presentation sides of the emotional word and the beep
earlier findings of Demakis and Harrison (1994) who showed that the and (b) they are observed even when no spatial dimension is involved
rehearsal of affective words primed the left hemisphere, as neutral (Experiment 2), namely when participants did not have to attend one
words did, suggesting that even affective words are processed by the side of space in order to repeat a word (as in Experiment 1) nor to
left hemisphere. Moreover, Russell and Voyer (2004) showed that the localize the beep on each trial (as in Experiment 3).
magnitude of the REA is similar in studies where an emotional These effects can be related to the interference effects of taboo
component was present and in studies where it was not. Finally, it is words observed in taboo Stroop tasks (e.g., MacKay & Ahmetzanov,
worth noting that, according to Bryden (1978) and Cheatham and 2005) and to their slowing effects in lexical decision tasks (Thomas &
Herbig (1992), there is a generalized rightward orientational bias in LaBar, 2005; but see MacKay et al., 2004), and interpreted as a freezing
right-handed people. reaction, namely an inhibition of motor responses provoking a generic
As indicated in the Introduction, we predicted the processing of slowdown, similar to the interpretation of the interference and
right-presented congruent beeps to benefit not only from the slowing effects of negative words (Algom, Chajut, & Lev, 2004). The
attraction of attentional resources to the spatial source of the fact that these non-spatial effects were also observed when there was
emotional word (which would be the case also of left-presented a 250 ms delay between the offset of the words and the onset of the
congruent beeps), but also from the left-hemisphere activation beep (long interval condition of the Experiment 1) supports this
associated with the recurrent processing of verbal material. The interpretation: indeed, Flykt (2006) suggested that if an effect of a
results of these three experiments confirmed this prediction. threatening stimulus is still present after detection of this stimulus
Moreover, only right-presented emotional words influenced signif- (namely during the withholding of the response), this effect can be
icantly the spatial orienting of attention, and consequently the located at the level of the response. Alternatively, some authors
processing of the beep. Thus, the emotional valence of the words consider that general non-spatial effects of emotional words would
only has a systematic (in the case of taboo words) and reliable actually reflect difficulties to disengage attention from their task-
influence if it is presented at the side of the auditory space primed irrelevant emotional content (e.g., Estes & Verges, 2008; McKenna &
by left-hemisphere activation, hence locking the connections Sharma, 2004) or the triggering of binding mechanisms which
between the “what” and “where” systems only in the right side of associate the source of an emotion (here, the word) with its context
the space. of occurrence (see MacKay et al., 2004 and MacKay et al., 2005).
This seems to constitute a partial failure of evolutionary Further studies should aim at disentangling these interpretations and
adaptation. It could indeed be so. Non-adaptive behaviours could identifying the basis of the non-spatial effects we observed.
in some circumstances result from the conjunction of two or more For the time being, the fact that for taboo words general non-
mechanisms that are, by themselves, adaptive. The adaptive value spatial effects were observed in both the short and the long interval
of hemisphere specialisation, particularly for language, is largely conditions of Experiment 1 but attentional biases only in the short
recognized (for recent reviews, see e.g., Hopkins, 2008; Josse & interval condition, indicates that these two effects are distinct and
Tzourio-Mazoyer, 2004). This is also the case for the anatomical independent from each other. The general non-spatial effects are
and functional decussation of the central nervous system relating, more long-lasting and less dependent on the immediate temporal
mostly in the vertebrates, each half of the brain to the opposite succession of the emotional stimulus and the beep than the spatial
half of the body and of the environmental space, although its attentional biases are, at least for taboo words.
precise adaptive origin has been matter for debate for many years
(see, e.g., Ramon y Cajal, 1898, and Suttie, 1926, vs. Braitenberg,
5.5. Conclusion
1965, for quite opposite views). The combination of two distinct
adaptive mechanisms may not always be adaptive itself, such as
The present data are in agreement with the idea that the functional
when an auditory non-verbal stimulus is presented on the
link between the “what” and the “where” auditory subsystems is
opposite side of a previous word (it being particularly relevant
reinforced by the emotional characteristics of the stimuli. The
for some reason) which is processed in the left hemisphere and
identification of a spoken emotional word automatically activates
engages attention to the right of environmental space. Moreover,
information about its spatial location, influencing the processing of a
the asymmetrical pattern of attentional biases observed here could
subsequent extraneous auditory stimulus. Nevertheless, only emo-
also depend on the specific experimental setting that we designed.
tional words presented at the automatically primed side of the
If one considers sounds to be usually delivered by an object, and
auditory space (in our case, the right side) affected the orienting of
that they serve to attract visual attention towards this object, our
attention. This held true for taboo and, to a lesser extent, for negative
experiments may have lacked ecological validity to some extent. A
words. This suggests that the “what”–“where” connections are locked
cross-modal setting, with an auditory emotional word followed by
exclusively to the right side of the space in a purely auditory situation
a visual target, could not lock the “what”–“where” connections
using a linguistic material. Further work will aim to assess whether
exclusively to the right side and might then give rise to a fully
such a limitation disappears in audio-visual settings or with non-
adaptive attentional behaviour. Such experiments are currently in
linguistic emotional auditory materials.
progress.
In comparison to the unimodal setting used here, the cross-modal
setting will offer a further advantage in allowing reducing the SOA Acknowledgements
(more precisely, the word-beep interval) without masking (through
noise effects) the spoken words. This will allow better comparison This work was supported by two grants of the F.R.S.–FNRS
with studies investigating attentional biases in the visual modality, (convention FRFC 2.4579.02 F and FNRS 1.5705.06). Many thanks to
since written words are generally briefly presented. In addition, Marcha Van Boven for the help in preparing the material, to Olivia
observations would not be limited to a relatively late and limited Gosseries and Ana Franco for testing the participants, as well as to
snapshot of the influence of emotional stimuli on attentional orienting. Pascale Lidji and Chotiga Pattamadilok for their technical assistance.
276 J. Bertels et al. / Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278

Appendix A Appendix A (continued)


Emotional word Neutral word

French word English translation French word English translation


Words constituting the emotional pairs (included and excluded from the statistical
analyses on the basis of the post hoc control studies), and their English translation. Positive pairs
Included in the statistical analyses
Emotional word Neutral word Rêve Dream Robe Dress
Rire Laugh Rôle Role
French word English translation French word English translation
Romance Romance Raquette Racket
Negative pairs Sincère Sincere Salade Salad
Included in the statistical analyses Sourire Smile Seconde Second
Attaque Attack Allure Speed Succès Success Sûrement Surely
Bombe Bomb Bulle Bubble Tendre Tender Tourne Turn
Bourreau Torturer Baigner Bathe Trésor Treasure Trafic Traffic
Cadavre Corpse Convaincre Convince Vacances Holiday Visite Visit
Cancer Cancer Cadence Rhythm Vie Life Va Go
Carnage Slaughter Corniche Cornice Excluded from the analyses
Cercueil Coffin Citrouille Pumpkin Argent Money Aspect Appearance
Chagrin Grief Charbon Coal Désir Desire Décor Decor
Colère Anger Quarante Forty Gagnant Winner Gazon Lawn
Coupable Culprit Quatorze Fourteen Gaieté Cheerfulness Galop Gallop
Crime Crime Cloche Bell Partage Share Parcours Way
Danger Danger Dessin Drawing Piscine Swimming pool Pilote Pilot
Echec Failure Etoffe Cloth Santé Health Salon Lounge
Haine Hate Hall Hall
Hurler Scream Hardi Bold Taboo pairs
Malheur Misfortune Mélange Blend Included in the statistical analyses
Massacre Massacre Moutarde Mustard Bander Have a hard-on Bambou Bamboo
Menace Threat Minute Minute Bite Dick Bêche Spade
Menottes Handcuffs Mésange Chickadee Blondasse Dull blond Brasseur Brewer
Meurtre Murder Muscle Muscle Bonasse Hottie Balise Beacon
Misère Misery Message Message Bonniche Skivvy Banquise Ice field
Morgue Morgue Merle Blackbird Bordel Brothel Bourgogne Burgundy
Mort Death Mise Putting Catin Trollop Coupon Coupon
Otage Hostage Augure Oracle Chiant Damn Chiot Puppy
Panique Panic Pendule Pendulum Clodo Bum Cresson Cress
Peur Fear Poche Pocket Connasse Bitch Canine Canine
Prison Jail Plateau Tray Conne Twat Quinte Quintain
Ravage Devastation Régate Regatta Crapule Scoundrel Crevette Shrimp
Sanglot Sob Sourcil Eyebrow Crétin Moron Croquis Sketch
Seringue Syringe Cerise Cherry Débile Moronic Décade Decade
Sévices Abuse Centime Cent Encule Bugger Amphore Amphora
Sida Aids Semer Sow Foireux Yellow-bellied Foison Abundance
Souffrir Suffer Secteur District Foutre Cum Firme Firm
Tank Tank Taupe Mole Gerber Throw up Jongler Juggle
Tombe Grave Tâche Task Gueule Trap Gare Station
Torture Torture Tartine Slice of bread Ignare Ignorant Hippique Horse
Tragique Tragic Transit Transit Merde Shit Marque Brand
Violence Violence Voiture Car Merdeux Squirt Matelot Sailor
Excluded from the analyses Morveux Jerk Merlan Whiting
Famine Starvation Fissure Crack Nichon Tit Napper Coat
Hargne Aggressiveness Harpe Harp Niquer Fuck Neiger Snow
Pédé Queer Piquet Peg
Positive pairs Pétasse Slut Pudding Pudding
Included in the statistical analyses Péter Fart Pommeau Pommel
Amour Love Agence Agency Pine Cock Pull Pullover
Beauté Beauty Bateau Boat Pisser Pee Pigeon Pigeon
Bon Good Bas Low Pouffiasse Tart Pochoir Stencil
Cadeau Gift Coton Cotton Putain Whore Panneau Panel
Câlin Cuddle Caddie Trolley Roulure Trollop Rameur Rower
Chance Luck Chaise Chair Salope Bitch Serrure Lock
Charme Charm Chiffre Digit Tapette Queer Tisseur Weaver
Confiance Trust Conduire Drive Taré Cretin Tympan Eardrum
Confort Comfort Canard Duck Taulard Con Teckel Dachshund
Content Glad Carré Square Traînée Slut Trousseau Bunch
Couple Couple Course Race Troncher Shag Tréma Dieresis
Courage Courage Commune Town Excluded from the analyses
Extase Ecstasy Estrade Platform Biroute Prick Benzène Benzene
Fête Party Phase Phase
Gentil Kind Genou Knee
Guérir Recover Garage Garage
Intime Intimate Indice Clue
Joie Joy Juin June
References
Merci Thanks Montrer Show
Algom, D., Chajut, E., & Lev, S. (2004). A rational look at the emotional Stroop
Parfait Perfect Poster Post
phenomenon: A generic slowdown, not a Stroop effect. Journal of Experimental
Passion Passion Panier Basket Psychology: General, 133(3), 323−338.
Plage Beach Presse Press Anderson, A. K. (2005). Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting
Respect Respect Rentrer Return awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(2), 258−281.
Arnell, K. M., Killman, K. V., & Fijavz, D. (2007). Blinded by emotion: Target misses
follow attention capture by arousing distractors in RSVP. Emotion, 7(3), 465−477.
J. Bertels et al. / Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278 277

Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. MacKay, D. G., Hadley, C. B., & Schwartz, J. H. (2005). Relations between emotion,
(2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and non-anxious individuals: A illusory word perception, and orthographic repetition blindness: Tests of binding
meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1−24. theory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychol-
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger ogy, 58(8), 1514−1533.
than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323−370. MacKay, D. G., Shafto, M., Taylor, J. K., Marian, D. E., Abrams, L., & Dyer, J. R. (2004).
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory Relations between emotion, memory, and attention: Evidence from taboo Stroop,
(2ème édition). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. lexical decision, and immediate memory tasks. Memory and Cognition, 32(3),
Bertels, J., Kolinsky, R., & Morais, J. (2009). Norms of emotional valence, arousal, threat 474−488.
value and shock value for 80 spoken French words: Comparison between neutral MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1988). Anxiety and the allocation of attention to threat.
and emotional tones of voice. Psychologica Belgica, 49(1), 19−40. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A(4), 653−670.
Bonin, P., Méot, A., Aubert, L., Malardier, N., Niedenthal, P., & Capelle-Toczek, M. -C. MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders.
(2003). Normes de concrétude, de valeur d'imagerie, de fréquence subjective et de Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(1), 15−20.
valence émotionnelle pour 866 mots. L'Année Psychologique, 104, 655−694. Mather, M., Mitchell, K. J., Raye, C. L., Novak, D. L., Greene, E. J., & Johnson, M. K. (2006).
Braitenberg, V. (1965). Taxis, kinesis and decussation. Progress in Brain Research, 17, Emotional arousal can impair feature binding in working memory. Journal of
210−222. Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(4), 614−625.
Bryden, M. P. (1978). Strategy effects in the assessment of hemispheric asymmetry. In Mathewson, K. J., Arnell, K. M., & Mansfield, C. A. (2008). Capturing and holding
G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies of information processing. London: Academic Press. attention: The impact of emotional words in rapid serial visual presentation.
Bryden, M. P., & MacRae, L. (1989). Dichotic laterality effects obtained with emotional Memory and Cognition, 36(1), 182−200.
words. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 1(3), 171−176. McKenna, F. P., & Sharma, D. (2004). Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals that
Buchanan, T. W., Etzel, J. A., Adolphs, R., & Tranel, D. (2006). The influence of autonomic it is not what it seems: The role of fast and slow components. Journal of
arousal and semantic relatedness on memory for emotional words. International Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(2), 382−392.
Journal of Psychophysiology, 61(1), 26−33. Messina, D., Morais, J., & Cantraine, F. (1989). Valeur affective de 904 mots de la langue
Buchtel, H. A., & Butter, Ch. M. (1988). Spatial attentional shifts. Implications for the role française. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, European Bulletin of Cognitive Psychology,
of polysensory mechanisms. Neuropsychologia, 26(4), 499−509. 9(2), 165−187.
Buchtel, H. A., Butter, Ch. M., & Ayvasik, B. (1996). Effects of stimulus source and intensity Mitchell, D. G. V., Nakic, M., Fridberg, D., Kamel, N., Pine, D. S., & Blair, R. J. R. (2007). The
on covert orientation to auditory stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 34(10), 979−985. impact of processing load on emotion. NeuroImage, 34, 1299−1309.
Cheatham, B. H., & Herbig, R. (1992). Spatial localization and auditory lateralization: Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour
Binaural cues and their absence. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75(3), 919−922. Research and Therapy, 36, 809−848.
Clarke, S., Bellmann, A., Meuli, R. A., Assal, G., & Steck, A. J. (2000). Auditory agnosia and Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1999). Some methodological issues in assessing attentional
spatial deficits following left hemispheric lesions: Evidence for distinct processing biases for threatening faces in anxiety: A replication study using a modified version
pathways. Neuropsychologia, 38, 797−807. of the probe detection task. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 595−604.
Cohen, J., MacWhinney, B., Flatt, M., & Provost, J. (1993). Psyscope — An interactive Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., De Bono, J., & Painter, M. (1997). Time course of attentional bias
graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the Psychology for threat information in non-clinical anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35
laboratory using Macintosh computers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & (4), 297−303.
Computers, 25(2), 257−271. Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Dixon, C., Fisher, S., Twelftree, H., & McWilliams, A. (2000). Trait
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of anxiety, defensiveness and selective processing of threat: An investigation using
psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24(4), 349−354. two measures of attentional bias. Personality and Individual Differences, 28,
Demakis, G. J., & Harrison, D. W. (1994). Subvocal rehearsal of neutral and affective 1063−1077.
words interferes with left-hemisphere performance and facilitates right-hemi- Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Williams, R. (1995). Attentional bias in anxiety and
sphere performance. Psychobiology, 22(3), 238−243. depression: The role of awareness. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34, 17−36.
Deutsch, D., & Roll, P. (1976). Separate “what” and “where” decision mechanisms in Mogg, K., Holmes, A., Garner, M., & Bradley, B. P. (2008). Effects of threat cues on
processing a dichotic tonal sequence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human attentional shifting, disengagement and response slowing in anxious individuals.
Perception and Performance, 2(1), 23−29. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 656−667.
Doallo, S., Holguín, S. R., & Cadaveira, F. (2006). Attentional load affects automatic Most, S. B., Smith, S. D., Cooter, A. B., Levy, B. N., & Zald, D. H. (2007). The naked truth:
emotional processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. NeuroReport, 17 Positive, arousing distractors impair rapid target perception. Cognition and Emotion,
(17), 1797−1801. 21(5), 964−981.
Estes, Z., & Verges, M. (2008). Freeze or flee? Negative stimuli elicit selective New, B., Pallier, C., Ferrand, L., & Matos, R. (2001). Une base de données lexicales du
responding. Cognition, 108, 557−565. français contemporain sur internet: LEXIQUE.L'Année Psychologique, 101, 447−462
Flykt, A. (2006). Preparedness for action: Responding to the snake in the grass. http://www.lexique.org.
American Journal of Psychology, 119(1), 29−43. Nielsen, S. L., & Sarason, I. G. (1981). Emotion, personality, and selective attention.
Fox, E. (1993). Allocation of visual attention and anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 7(2), Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(5), 945−960.
207−215. Okon-Singer, H., Tzelgov, J., & Henik, A. (2007). Distinguishing between automaticity
Goldring, J. E., Dorris, M. C., Corneil, B. D., Ballantyne, P. A., & Munoz, D. P. (1996). and attention in the processing of emotionally significant stimuli. Emotion, 7(1),
Combined eye-head gaze shifts to visual and auditory targets in humans. 147−157.
Experimental Brain Research, 111(1), 68−78. Pessoa, L., McKenna, M., Gutierrez, E., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Neural processing of
Grühn, D., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (2005). No aging bias favouring memory for positive emotional faces requires attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
material: Evidence from a heterogeneity-homogeneity list paradigm using 99(17), 11458−11463.
emotionally toned words. Psychology and Aging, 20(4), 579−588. Pessoa, L., Padmala, S., & Morland, T. (2005). Fate of unattended fearful faces in the
Hafter, E. R. (1997). Binaural adaptation and the effectiveness of a stimulus beyond its amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation.
onset. In R. H. Gilkey, & T. R. Anderson (Eds.), Binaural and spatial hearing in real and NeuroImage, 28, 249−255.
virtual environments (pp. 211−232). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,
Hermans, D., & De Houwer, J. (1994). Affective and subjective familiarity ratings of 740 32, 3−25.
Dutch words. Psychologica Belgica, 34(2–3), 115−139. Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. A. (1984). Components of Visual Orienting. In H. Bouma, & D. G.
Hopkins, W. D. (2008). Theoretical speculations on the evolutionary origins of Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and Performance X (pp. 531−554). Hillsdale, NJ:
hemispheric specialization. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, Erlbaum.
233−237. Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of
Josse, G., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2004). Hemispheric specialization for language. Brain signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160−174.
Research Reviews, 44(1), 1−12. Ramon y Cajal, S. (1898). Estructura del kiasma optico y teoria general de los
Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2003). Memory enhancement for emotional words: Are entrecruzamientos de las vias nerviosas. Revista Trimestral Micrografica, 3, 15−65.
emotional words more vividly remembered than neutral words? Memory and Rauschecker, J. P. (1997). Processing of complex sounds in the auditory cortex of cat,
Cognition, 31(8), 1169−1180. monkey, and man. Acta Oto-Laryngologica — Supplement, 532, 34−38.
Kinsbourne, M. (1970). The cerebral basis of lateral asymmetries in attention. Acta Rauschecker, J. P. (1998). Cortical processing of complex sounds. Current Opinions in
Psychologica, 33, 193−201. Neurobiology, 288, 516−521.
Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Selective attention Rauschecker, J. P. (1998). Parallel processing in the auditory cortex of primates.
to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to Audiology and Neuro-otology, 3, 86−103.
disengage. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(10), 1183−1192. Rhodes, G. (1987). Auditory attention and the representation of spatial information.
Kubovy, M. (1988). Should we resist the seductiveness of the space-time, vision- Perception & Psychophysics, 42(1), 1−14.
audition analogy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and
Performance, 14(2), 318−320. contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 296−320.
Kubovy, M., & Van Valkenburg, D. (2001). Auditory and visual objects. Cognition, 80(1–2), Russell, N. L., & Voyer, D. (2004). Reliability of laterality effects in a dichotic listening
97−126. task with words and syllables. Brain and Cognition, 54(3), 266−267.
Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal Salemink, E., van den Hout, M. A., & Kindt, M. (2007). Selective attention and threat:
of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 451−468. Quick orienting versus slow disengagement and two versions of the dot probe task.
MacKay, D. G., & Ahmetzanov, M. V. (2005). Emotion, memory, and attention in the Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(3), 607−615.
taboo Stroop paradigm. An experimental analogue of flashbulb memories. Psycho- Schimmack, U. (2005). Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: Threat,
logical Science, 16(1), 25−32. negativity or arousal? Emotion, 5(1), 55−66.
278 J. Bertels et al. / Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) 264–278

Schmitt, M., Postma, A., & De Haan, E. (2000). Interactions between exogenous auditory Suttie, I. D. (1926). A theory of decussation. Note on a possible adaptive significance of
and visual spatial attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A the translateration of the upper motor and sensory neurones. The Journal of
(1), 105−130. Neurology and Psychopathology, VI, 267−280.
Siegrist, M. (1995). Effects of taboo words on colour-naming performance on a Stroop Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The
test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81(3), 1119−1122. mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 67−85.
Smith Erthal, F., De Oliveira, L., Mocaiber, I., Garcia Pereira, M., Machado-Pinheiro, W., Taylor, S. F., Kornblum, S., Lauber, E. J., Minoshima, S., & Koeppe, R. A. (1997). Isolation of
Volchan, E., et al. (2005). Load-dependent modulation of affective picture specific interference processing in the Stroop task: PET activation studies.
processing. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(4), 388−395. NeuroImage, 6(2), 81−92.
Spence, C. J., & Driver, J. (1994). Covert spatial orienting in audition: Exogenous and Thomas, L. A., & LaBar, K. S. (2005). Emotional arousal enhances word repetition
endogenous mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception priming. Cognition and Emotion, 19(7), 107−1047.
and Performance, 20(3), 555−574. Van der Goten, K., De Vooght, G., & Kemps, E. (1999). Concreteness and emotional
Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y) (“Self- valence ratings of 399 Dutch nouns. Psychologica Belgica, 39(1), 49−70.
evaluation questionnaire”). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Zajonc, R. B. (1962). Response suppression in perceptual defense. Journal of
Stormark, K. M., Nordby, H., & Hugdahl, K. (1995). Attentional shifts to emotionally Experimental Psychology, 64(3), 206−214.
charged cues: Behavioural and ERP data. Cognition and Emotion, 9(5), 507−523.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi