Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 6

Pain and Sensory Dysfunction 6 to 12 Months After

Inguinal Herniotomy
Trine Mikkelsen, MS*, Mads U. Werner, MD, PhD†, Birgit Lassen, RNA†, and
Henrik Kehlet, MD, PhD*
*Department of Surgical Gastroenterology and †Acute Pain Service, Department of Anesthesiology, Hvidovre University
Hospital, Copenhagen, Denmark

Inguinal hernia repair is associated with a 5%–30% incidence and nonpain groups, except for a small but significant in-
of chronic pain, but the pathogenesis remains unknown. We crease in pain response to von Frey hair and brush stimula-
therefore evaluated pain and sensory dysfunction by quan- tion in the pain group. Hypoesthesia, or tactile allodynia, in
titative sensory testing 6–12 mo after open herniorrhaphy. the incisional area was observed in 51% (37 of 72) of the pa-
Before sensory testing, all patients (n ⫽ 72) completed a tients, but the incidence did not differ significantly between
short-form McGill Pain Questionnaire and a functional im- the pain group and the nonpain group (14 of 20 versus 23 of
pairment questionnaire. Sensory dysfunction in the inci- 52; P ⬎ 0.3). We concluded that cutaneous hypoesthesia, or
sional area was evaluated by quantification of thermal and tactile allodynia, is common after inguinal herniotomy but
mechanical thresholds, by mechanical pain responses (von has a low specificity for chronic postherniotomy pain. Fac-
Frey/pressure algometry), and by areas of pinprick hypoes- tors other than nerve damage may be involved in the devel-
thesia and tactile allodynia. The incidence of chronic pain opment of chronic postherniotomy pain.
was28%(20of72).Quantitativesensorytestingandpressure
algometry did not demonstrate differences between the pain (Anesth Analg 2004;99:146–51)

I
nguinal hernia repair is a common procedure with disturbances and pain. However, in both these stud-
infrequent postoperative morbidity, but it may be ies, the examination technique was not presented, and
followed by complaints of chronic pain in 5%–30% patients without pain were not examined, thereby
of patients (1). However, most studies have not as- hindering interpretation regarding the association be-
sessed chronic pain as the primary aim of the study; tween chronic pain and sensory dysfunction as an
therefore, assessment of the functional consequences indicator of neuropathic pain. Therefore, the aim of
of chronic pain after inguinal herniotomy have only this study was to evaluate in detail the association
recently been reported (2– 4). In these studies, approx- between chronic pain and sensory dysfunction in her-
imately 10% stated that pain was interfering with nia repair patients with and without pain 6 –12 mo
work or leisure activity (2– 4), thus emphasizing the after surgery.
importance of the problem.
The development of chronic pain after inguinal her-
niotomy has been attributed to several pathogenic Methods
mechanisms, including damage to the well defined After approval from the regional ethical committee, all
sensory nerves in the groin area: the iliohypogastric patients were included who had undergone elective
nerve, the ilioinguinal nerve, and the genitofemoral uncomplicated inguinal herniotomy by the Lichten-
nerve. Only two studies (5,6) have discussed the asso- stein procedure 6 –12 mo before the start of the study
ciation between late postoperative cutaneous sensory at our institution. Exclusion criteria were female sex, a
bilateral procedure, surgery for recurrent hernia, age
This study was supported by Grant 22010160 from the Danish younger than 18 yr, chronic opioid medication, lan-
Medical Research Council. guage barriers, cognitive dysfunction, or known neu-
Accepted for publication December 11, 2003. rological disease. Questionnaires with prestamped re-
Address correspondence to Mads U. Werner, MD, PhD, Depart-
ment of Oncology, University Hospital, SE 221 85 Lund, Sweden. turn envelopes were mailed to 171 patients (Fig. 1).
Address e-mail to madswerner@medscape.com. Reprints will not be After verbal and written consent, the patients were
available from the authors. asked to complete two questionnaires relating to pain
DOI: 10.1213/01.ANE.0000115147.14626.C5 and functional impairment (3). All patients were given

©2004 by the International Anesthesia Research Society


146 Anesth Analg 2004;99:146–51 0003-2999/04
ANESTH ANALG PAIN MEDICINE MIKKELSEN ET AL. 147
2004;99:146 –51 SENSORY DYSFUNCTION AFTER HERNIOTOMY

care not to include scar tissue. This area and an area


immediately outside were used for sensory thresholds
and pain assessments.
Hypoesthesia was determined by stimulating with a
von Frey monofilament (Senselab Aesthesiometer; So-
medic AB, Sweden; No. 17; nominal buckling force,
468 mN) well outside the sensory testing area along
eight linear paths angled at 45° and converging to-
ward the center of the area. The patients were in-
structed to report a definite decrease in tactile sensa-
tion. Tactile allodynia was evaluated by gently
stroking the skin with a soft brush perpendicular to
the linear paths, and the patients were instructed to
report any sensation of discomfort or pain.
Mechanical pain threshold was evaluated by stim-
ulation with 11 progressively rigid calibrated von Frey
monofilaments (No. 7, 2 mN; No. 8, 3 mN; No. 9, 6
mN; No. 10, 12 mN; No. 11, 22 mN; No. 12, 34 mN; No.
13, 58 mN; No. 14, 89 mN; No. 15, 212 mN; No. 16, 309
mN; and No. 17, 468 mN [calibrated at 23°C and a
relative humidity of 35%]) as previously described (7).
The mechanical pain threshold was defined as the
least force that elicited a sensation of pain or discom-
fort. If von Frey filament No. 17 did not elicit a sen-
sation of pain or discomfort, the observation was as-
signed a value of 18. Pain responses (VAS 0 –100) to
mechanical stimuli were assessed by five stimuli of a
Figure 1. Chart depicting patient flow. HIV ⫽ human immunode-
ficiency virus. rigid von Frey filament (No. 17) with a stimulation
rate of 0.5 Hz.
Brush (dynamic) allodynia was evaluated by appli-
detailed verbal instructions for correct completion. cation of an electric toothbrush (Oral-B 4713; Braun,
The first questionnaire is a modified Danish short Germany; 127-Hz oscillations) on the skin for 30 s in
form of the McGill Pain Questionnaire (3) that con- the incisional area. Brush-induced pain or discomfort
tains a visual analog scale (VAS; 0 –100) for assessment was evaluated by VAS ratings after 5 and 25 s of
of pain intensity, a list of adjectives describing pain stimulation.
quality, and an anatomical chart for depiction of pain The mechanical pain threshold from deep somatic
localization. In the second questionnaire, the patients
tissues was evaluated by pressure algometry (Bridge
were asked to assess how the pain was interfering
amplifier; Somedic AB, Sverige) with a circular probe
with daily living and leisure activity (3).
(0.18 cm2) applied directly over the middle of the
The quantitative sensory testing was performed
inguinal ligament. Pressure was applied at a constant
with the patients resting in a comfortable semireclined
position. To get the patients well acquainted with the rate (10 kPa/s), and the patient was instructed to press
testing paradigm, a preparatory testing sequence was a button when a sensation of pain or discomfort ap-
performed at the patient’s forearm, followed by test- peared. Assessments were made in triplicate, with an
ing in the groin area. During the examination, the interstimulus interval of 30 s, and the median value
patient was asked to keep his eyes closed and to was used in the analysis.
concentrate on the evoked sensations. The patient was Thermal thresholds were assessed by a computer-
unaware of the test results. The quantitative sensory ized 25 ⫻ 50 mm contact thermode (Modular Sensory
testing was done by two investigators (TM and BL). Analyzer; Somedic AB, Hörby, Sweden) applied in the
The actual sensory testing was performed starting incisional area. The patients were instructed to imme-
in a reference area (25 ⫻ 50 mm) 4 cm cephalad for the diately push a button when a change of temperature
incision. The reference area was used for all sensory was perceived. Assessments of warm detection
thresholds and pain assessments in patients without threshold, cold detection threshold, and heat pain
any demonstrable skin hyperesthesia. In patients with threshold (HPT) were made in triplicate with random-
hyperesthesia, a corresponding area (25 ⫻ 50 mm) was ized interstimulus intervals of 4 – 6 s, starting from a
demarcated centrally in the hyperesthesia zone, taking baseline temperature of 32°C and with a ramp rate of
148 PAIN MEDICINE MIKKELSEN ET AL. ANESTH ANALG
SENSORY DYSFUNCTION AFTER HERNIOTOMY 2004;99:146 –51

⫾1°C/s. The cutoff limits for warm and cold meas-


urements were 50°C and 25°C, respectively. The cre-
master reflex was elicited by stroking the inside of the
thigh with a von Frey monofilament No. 18 (1584
mN), and retraction was noted.
The chronological order of testing was assessment
of hypoesthesia and tactile allodynia, cold detection
threshold, warm detection threshold, mechanical pain
threshold (skin), HPT, mechanical pain response,
brush allodynia, cremaster reflexes, and mechanical
pain threshold (pressure algometry). All patients were
examined clinically with palpation of both groin areas
with the patient in the standing position, at rest, and
during coughing.
Data were analyzed for normality by the
Kolmogorov-Smirnov test (SPSS 10.0.7; SPSS Inc., Chi- Figure 2. Pain descriptors reported from the pain questionnaire
cago, IL). Because several of the test data were not (short form of the McGill Pain Questionnaire) (12). Categories are 1,
evaluative; 2–14, sensory; and 15–18, affective. The most commonly
normally distributed, only nonparametric tests were chosen descriptors were 1, 3, 5, and 13.
used: the Mann-Whitney test for nonpaired data and
Wilcoxon’s signed rank test for paired data. The ␹2 test
with Yates’ continuity correction or Fisher’s exact test, There was no significant difference between the
as appropriate (8), was used to assess differences in pain group and the nonpain group in mechanical pain
frequencies between groups. Values are median and thresholds for punctate stimuli (P ⬎ 0.4; Mann-
interquartile range (25%–75%) unless otherwise Whitney test; Table 1). In contrast, the pain response to
stated. A P value ⬍0.05 was considered to indicate five repeated von Frey hair stimuli was significantly
statistical significance. higher (P ⬍ 0.002) in the pain patients, but the VAS
scores were very low (10 versus 2; Table 1). Pain to
brush-evoked stimulation (5 and 25 s) was also more
frequent in the pain group than in the nonpain group
Results (P ⬍ 0.02 and P ⬍ 0.03; Mann-Whitney test; Table 1),
Between January 1 and December 31, 2000, 171 male but again with very low VAS scores, between 2 and 5
patients had undergone elective primary inguinal her- of 100 (Table 1). There was no difference in mechanical
niotomy by the Lichtenstein procedure at our institu- pain perception (von Frey or brush) among the pain
tion. Questionnaires with prestamped return enve- patients with or without sensory dysfunction (P ⬎
lopes were mailed to all patients (Fig. 1). The median 0.3).
age of the patients was 64 yr (interquartile range, Mechanical pain threshold assessed by pressure al-
48 –73 yr). The investigation period started April 3 and gometry in the incisional area did not differ between
ended August 10, 2001, and the median follow-up the pain and the nonpain groups (P ⬎ 0.1; Mann-
time was 9.5 mo (7–13 mo). Whitney test; Table 1). Warm detection, cold detec-
Twenty (28%) of the 72 patients had experienced
tion, and HPT did not differ between the pain group
pain in the area, and 52 patients (72%) had not. Eleven
and the nonpain group (P ⬎ 0.6; Mann-Whitney test;
(15%) of the patients with pain indicated that pain
Table 1).
significantly interfered with work or leisure activity.
The most commonly chosen descriptors of pain were The cremaster reflex was elicitable on the incisional
pricking (13 of 20; Fig. 2), annoying/irritating (11 of side in 14 of 20 patients in the pain group and in 44 of
20), tender (10 of 20), and shooting/jolting (10 of 20). 52 patients in the nonpain group (P ⬎ 0.2; ␹2 Yates’
The number of descriptors was 4 (interquartile range, correction; Table 1). The physical examination did not
3– 8). The spontaneous VAS pain intensity in the pain reveal any recurrent or new hernias. Anatomical pain
group was 22 (interquartile range, 12–30). charts for the 20 pain patients are illustrated in Figure
Mechanical hypoesthesia or tactile allodynia in the 3. In addition, two patients (Patients 29 and 52) spon-
incisional area was observed in 51% (37 of 72) of the taneously reported that they experienced severe ejac-
patients. Three patients in the pain group and six ulatory pain that led to sexual dysfunction and im-
patients in the nonpain group had areas with both pairment. Neither of these patients reported pain in
hypoesthesia and tactile allodynia in the incisional the incisional area.
area. The overall incidence of sensory dysfunction was Post hoc statistical analysis showed that the power of
not different among pain patients (14 of 20) compared the study (n ⫽ 72) ranged between 80% and 95% (␣ ⫽
with nonpain patients (23 of 52) (P ⬎ 0.3). 0.05; ␤ ⫽ 0.05– 0.20) in regard to detection of a 2.5⫻
ANESTH ANALG PAIN MEDICINE MIKKELSEN ET AL. 149
2004;99:146 –51 SENSORY DYSFUNCTION AFTER HERNIOTOMY

Table 1. Summary of Results from Quantitative Sensory Testing in the Incisional Area
Variable Pain versus nonpain group
Incidence of hypoesthesia (fraction of patients) 7/20 vs 12/52: P ⬎ 0.2
Incidence of tactile allodynia (fraction of patients) 10/20 vs 17/52: P ⬎ 0.2
Mechanical pain threshold (von Frey) 18 vs 18: P ⬎ 0.4
Mechanical pain response (von Frey ⫻ 5) (VAS 0–100) 10 vs 2: P ⬍ 0.002
Brush allodynia after 5 s of stimulation (VAS 0–100) 2 vs 0: P ⬍ 0.02
Brush allodynia after 25 s of stimulation (VAS 0–100) 5 vs 0: P ⬍ 0.03
Mechanical pain threshold in area (pressure algometry; kPa) 145 vs 178: P ⬎ 0.1
Warm detection threshold (°C) 36.9 vs 37.0: P ⬎ 0.9
Cold detection threshold (°C) 28.5 vs 28.4: P ⬎ 0.7
Heat pain threshold (°C) 46.9 vs 47.2: P ⬎ 0.6
Cremaster reflex (elicitable/total number) 14/20 vs 44/52: P ⬎ 0.2
VAS ⫽ visual analog scale.

hernia repair patients 6 –12 months after elective un-


complicated surgery. A total of 28% of the patients
reported pain, and 15% stated that the pain interfered
with work or social activities; this is in agreement with
previous follow-up studies (2– 4,9). The quantitative
sensory testing showed that sensory disturbances, i.e.,
mechanical hypoesthesia and tactile allodynia, were a
common finding (37 of 72 patients; 51%) in the her-
niotomy area, but these sensory aberrations did not
seem to occur more often in patients with pain than in
patients without pain. Also, detailed, quantified sen-
sory testing did not demonstrate differences between
pain and nonpain patients except for minor, but sig-
nificant, increased VAS responses in pain patients to
von Frey and brush stimulation.
Chronic pain after herniotomy has primarily been
assumed to be of neuropathic origin (1), and it has
been hypothesized that the nerve injury could be at-
tributed either to direct surgical trauma or to delayed
injury caused by postoperative inflammatory changes.
A previously observed relationship between chronic
pain and late postoperative sensory dysfunction sup-
ports this interpretation (5,6), but in these studies no
detailed sensory assessments were performed, and no
information was provided from patients without pain.
In the Cunningham et al. study (5), pain and sensory
disturbances were followed up in 276 patients with
Figure 3. Anatomical charts with patients’ localization of pain (n ⫽
20).
315 hernia repairs. At 12 months control, 31% of the
patients reported numbness in the incisional area, and
a significant correlation with pain was observed. On
increased incidence of hypoesthesia or tactile allo- physical examination, 90% of patients with sensory
dynia, a 10% change in thermal thresholds, a 40% impairment had hypoesthesia, whereas dysesthesia
change in mechanical pain threshold (pressure algom- was uncommon and allodynia was not noted. In a
etry), or a 40% change in the incidence of elicitation of large long-term follow-up questionnaire-based study,
the cremaster reflex in the pain group compared with 8% of the patients indicated hypoesthesia and 2%
the nonpain group. indicated touch- or movement-related paresthesia in
the incisional area (6).
In the study by Cunningham et al. (5), three distinct
Discussion types of pain were suggested but not analyzed: a
This study is the first to investigate chronic pain and somatic pain localized to the common ligamentous
sensory dysfunction by quantified sensory testing in insertion to the pubic tubercle, a neuropathic pain
150 PAIN MEDICINE MIKKELSEN ET AL. ANESTH ANALG
SENSORY DYSFUNCTION AFTER HERNIOTOMY 2004;99:146 –51

referable to the ilioinguinal or genitofemoral nerve compared with control. Repetitive pinprick stimula-
distribution, and a visceral, ejaculatory-related pain. tion was associated with higher pain ratings in the
In the anatomical chart (Fig. 3) in this study, 9 of the 20 pain group, indicating a prominent sensitization com-
pain patients indicated pain near the incision, 6 indi- ponent. Data from this study corroborate these find-
cated discrete pain points near the inguinal ligament, ings, bearing in mind that all pain patients in the study
and 6 indicated a more circumscribed pain area (Fig. by Gottrup et al. (19) had an abnormal hypersensitiv-
3). As previously mentioned, two patients reported ity in the incisional area. In this area we observed no
ejaculatory pain. The discrete pain markings could difference in thermal detection thresholds and HPTs
represent a somatic pain component or a neuroma between pain and nonpain patients, but we did ob-
formation, whereas the circumscribed areas would serve an increased pain response to pinprick and
seem to suggest involvement of one or more nerve brush stimulation in patients with pain compared
branches. An anatomical study indicated a complex with those without pain, although these differences
innervation pattern of the three cutaneous nerves sup- were quantitatively very minor (VAS ⬍10 on a 100-
plying the groin area (10), and a detailed nerve lesion point scale).
interpretation is therefore not possible. In support of a The lack of a clear relationship in sensory distur-
neuropathic component, we observed that 10 of the bances between pain and nonpain patients may at first
pain patients, including 3 of 6 patients with circum- seem surprising. However, sensory disturbances may
scribed pain areas, used a combination of 2 or more be related to damage to the ilioinguinal and iliohypo-
neuropathic pain descriptors (11–13), i.e., shooting, gastric nerves, and studies with intraoperative cryo-
pricking, burning, or tender (Fig. 2). The most fre- analgesia of these nerves have demonstrated sensory
quent pain descriptors belonged to the sensory or dysfunction but no effect on acute pain for up to four
evaluative category (pricking, annoying/irritating, weeks after herniotomy (22). Therefore, chronic pain
tender, and shooting/jolting), which is in close agree- after inguinal herniotomy may be related to damage to
ment with a previous study (3). deeper nerve structures (musculofascial layer) rather
A number of studies have evaluated chronic cuta- than damage to the nerves traversing the surgical
neous sensory impairment after tissue injury, by using field. Findings after herniotomy may therefore differ
quantitative testing methods: in postherpetic neural- from those after herpes zoster and mastectomy, both
gia (14 –17), in neuropathy (18), in postmastectomy of which include superficial tissue damage and there-
pain (19), in osteoarthritis (20), and in whiplash injury fore may be more closely related to cutaneous sensory
(21). In a prospective study of herpes zoster patients, disturbances.
the presence in the acute stage of brush-induced allo- In conclusion, we studied pain and sensory dys-
dynia (gain of sensory function) and pinprick hypoes- function in 72 patients 6 –12 months after herniotomy
thesia (loss of sensory function) correlated with devel- with a quantitative sensory testing technique. The in-
opment of postherpetic neuralgia (15). In a recent cidence of chronic pain was 28% (20 of 72) and that of
study, it was suggested that two distinct mechanisms sensory dysfunction was 51% (37 of 72), but there were
may be operational in neuropathic pain: central sensi- no differences between the pain group and the non-
tization in patients with minor cutaneous sensory dys- pain group. Quantitative sensory testing had a low
function (painful hyperalgesia) and partial nociceptive specificity for chronic pain after inguinal herniotomy.
deafferentation in patients with major sensory dys- Future studies should therefore include a detailed as-
function (painful hypoalgesia) (18). In this study of 20 sessment of all preoperative, intraoperative, and post-
pain patients, 14 had tactile allodynia or hypoesthesia, operative factors to elucidate the pathogenesis of
and 3 of these presented with a combination of allo- chronic postherniotomy pain (23).
dynia and hypoesthesia.
In a study of postmastectomy pain, sensory abnor-
malities were studied with a quantitative sensory test-
ing technique in pain patients (n ⫽ 15) and nonpain References
patients (n ⫽ 11). The inclusion criterion was abnor- 1. Perkins FM, Kehlet H. Chronic pain as an outcome of surgery:
a review of predictive factors. Anesthesiology 2000;93:1123–33.
mal sensitivity, painful (allodynia or dysesthesia) or 2. Callesen T, Bech K, Kehlet H. Prospective study of chronic pain
nonpainful (hypoesthesia or dysesthesia), in or after groin hernia repair. Br J Surg 1999;86:1528 –31.
around the incisional area. Sensory thresholds to pin- 3. Bay-Nielsen M, Perkins FM, Kehlet H. Pain and functional
prick and thermal stimuli were significantly increased impairment one year after inguinal herniorrhaphy: a nation-
wide questionnaire study. Ann Surg 2001;233:1–7.
on the operated side, with no difference between 4. Haapaniemi S, Nilsson E. Recurrence and pain three years after
groups. There was no difference in HPT in the pain groin hernia repair: validation of postal questionnaire and se-
group, but a significant decrease was observed in the lective physical examination as a method of follow-up. Eur
nonpain group in the hypoesthetic/dysesthetic area. J Surg 2002;168:22– 8.
5. Cunningham J, Temple WJ, Mitchell P, et al. Cooperative hernia
The mechanical pain threshold for pinprick was sig- study: pain in the postrepair patient. Ann Surg 1996;224:
nificantly lower in pain patients on the operated side 598 – 602.
ANESTH ANALG PAIN MEDICINE MIKKELSEN ET AL. 151
2004;99:146 –51 SENSORY DYSFUNCTION AFTER HERNIOTOMY

6. Gillion JF, Ganiez PL. Chronic pain and cutaneous sensory 16. Rowbotham MC, Fields HL. The relationship of pain, allodynia
changes after inguinal hernia repair: comparison between open and thermal sensation in post-herpetic neuralgia. Brain 1996;
and laparoscopic techniques. Hernia 1999;3:75– 80. 119:347–54.
7. Pedersen JL, Kehlet H. Hyperalgesia in a human model of acute 17. Pappagallo M, Oaklander AL, Quatrano-Piacentini AL, et al.
inflammatory pain: a methodological study. Pain 1998;74:139–51. Heterogenous patterns of sensory dysfunction in postherpetic
8. Altman DG. Practical statistics for medical research. London: neuralgia suggest multiple pathophysiologic mechanisms. An-
Chapman & Hall, 1991. esthesiology 2000;92:691– 8.
9. Poobalan AS, Bruce J, King PM, et al. Chronic pain and quality 18. Baumgartner U, Magerl W, Klein T et al. Neurogenic hyperal-
of life following open inguinal hernia repair. Br J Surg 2001;88: gesia versus painful hypoalgesia: two distinct mechanisms of
1122– 6. neuropathic pain. Pain 2002;96:141–51.
10. Akita S, Niga S, Yamato Y, et al. Anatomic basis of chronic pain 19. Gottrup H, Andersen J, Arendt-Nielsen L, Jensen TS. Psycho-
with special reference to sports hernia. Surg Radiol Anat 1999;
physical examination in patients with post-mastectomy pain.
21:1–5.
Pain 2000;87:275– 84.
11. Boureau F, Doubrere JF, Luu M. Study of verbal description in
20. Kosek E, Ordeberg G. Lack of pressure pain modulation by
neuropathic pain. Pain 1990;42:145–52.
12. Melzack R. The short-form McGill Pain Questionnaire. Pain heterotopic noxious conditioning stimulation in patients with
1987;30:191–7. painful osteoarthritis before, but not following, surgical pain
13. Melzack R, Katz J. Pain measurement in persons in pain. In: relief. Pain 2000;88:69 –78.
Wall PD, Melzack R, eds. Textbook of pain. London: Churchill 21. Kasch H, Stengaard-Pedersen K, Arendt-Nielsen L, Staehelin JT.
Livingstone, 1999:1341–52. Headache, neck pain, and neck mobility after acute whiplash
14. Rowbotham MC, Fields HL. Post-herpetic neuralgia: the rela- injury: a prospective study. Spine 2001;26:1246 –51.
tion of pain complaint, sensory disturbance, and skin tempera- 22. Callesen T, Bech K, Thorup J, et al. Cryoanalgesia: effect on
ture. Pain 1989;39:129 – 44. postherniorrhaphy pain. Anesth Analg 1998;87:896 –9.
15. Haanpaa M, Laippala P, Nurmikko T. Allodynia and pinprick 23. Kehlet H, Bay-Nielsen M, Kingsnorth A. Chronic posthernior-
hypesthesia in acute herpes zoster, and the development of rhaphy pain: a call for uniform assessment. Hernia 2002;6:
postherpetic neuralgia. J Pain Symptom Manage 2000;20:50 – 8. 178 – 81.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi