Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 8

I N T E R N A T I O N A L J O U R N A L OF G E R I A T R I C PSYCHIATRY, VOL.

2: 83-90 (1987)

LATE PARAPHRENIA: NEUROPSYCHO-


LOGICAL IMPAIRMENT AND STRUCTURAL
BRAIN ABNORMALITIES ON COMPUTED
TOMOGRAPHY
MOHSEN N A G U I B , ” MRCPsych, DPM A N D R A Y M O N D LEVY, FRCPsych, FRCP, PhD
Section of Old Age Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, CIK

SUMMARY
Forty-three patients with ‘late paraphrenia’ were subjected to detailed clinical and psychological assessment and
computed tomography (CT). The paraphrenics had significantly larger lateral cerebral ventricles and greater
cognitive deficits than a group of 40 age-matched normal volunteers. Group comparisons within the patients,
between those with particularly large ventricular brain ratio (VBR) (equal or more than two standard deviations
above the control mean) and the rest of the group, showed that the extent of ventricular enlargement had no direct
bearing on the clinical presentation of the paraphrenic syndrome. The results suggest that ventricular enlargement
may have preceded the onset of overt symptoms by a period of years and probably served as a non-specific risk
factor for the development of the disorder. The unimodal distribution of VBR values suggests that paraphrenia, at
least at a brain structural level, is a unitary condition. It also indicates the presence of subcortical pathology which
might be in part responsible for the cognitive dysfunction elicited.

KEY woms-Paraphrenia psychological impairments, C A T scan, Abnormalities

INTRODUCTION zophrenia had variable degrees of increased


ventricular size and/or sulcal enlargement. These
Kraeplin (1919), who first introduced the term are usually interpreted as evidence for central and
paraphrenia to describe patients with late onset surface cerebral atrophy respectively. Similar
paranoid delusions and hallucinations, considered results have been demonstrated using CT scan-
that intellectual functions and memory were not ning (Johnstone et al., 1976; Kingsley and
impaired in this disorder. He believed that the Trimble, 1978; Weinberger et al., 1979a, b, 1982).
later age of onset of florid symptoms and the Patients with chronic schizophrenia are almost
failure to progress -in the majority of cases -to invariably reported to perform poorly on various
defect states set paraphrenia apart from dementia neuropsychological tests (Klonoff et al., 1970;
praecox. The two conditions were presumed to be Watson et al., 1968; Golden et al., 1980). On the
free from any morbid anatomical brain changes basis of their test performance alone they are
and permanent cognitive impairment. However, frequently indistinguishable from patients with
there is now a growing body of knowledge which organic brain damage (Golden e f af., 1978;
suggests that at least in chronic schizophrenia Watson, 1971). Furthermore, a number of studies
there are morphological brain changes, neurop- have reported the presence of motor disorders
sychoiogical deficits and neurological symptoms. and other features suggestive of neurological
Structural brain abnormalities were first de- dysfunction in unselected groups of psychiatric
scribed in pneumoencepholographic studies patients and particularly those with severe and
(Moore et al., 1933; Matthews and Booker, 1972) chronic forms of schizophrenia (Rogers, 1985;
which showed that patients with chronic schi- Pollin et al., 1966; Rochford et af., 1970).
In the case of schizophrenia of late onset Hi!l
(1956) stated that: ... ‘in the involutional period
*Now at St Mary’s Hospital Medical School and St Charles of life organic cerebral factors usually act as
Hospital. Exmoor Street. London WIO precipitants of the psychosis and inevitably

0885-6230/87/02008W8$05 .OO Received 28 November 1986


01987 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd Accepted 2 December 1986
84 M . NAGUIB AND R. LEVY

determine an unfavourable progress’. While Kay fantastic, persecutory or grandiose delusions,


and Roth (1961) found a pathological degree of delusions of reference with or without hallucina-
cerebral degeneration -which they postulated to tions; (2) the absence of a primary affective
be related to the onset of late paraphrenia - in disorder; (3) intellectual capacity in keeping with
5% of their cases, Post (1966), aiming to estimate that of normal ageing; (4) the phenomena
the contribution made by cerebral disease to the occurred in the setting of clear consciousness; (5)
causation of paranoid and paraphrenic syn- the first onset of symptoms was at or over the age
dromes, judged that about 26% of his patients of 60; (6) absence of a history or physical signs of
had an associated cerebral disease. Herbert and cerebrovascular accidents, neurological disorders
Jacobson (1967) found specific E E G abnormali- or alcoholism.
ties in over half the patients studied, but they
considered these to be age related. Similarly, Clinical data
Maggs and Turton (1956) in a survey of EEGs in
people over the age of 60 suffering from delusions In addition to obtaining detailed personal and
found the same number of specific abnormalities demographic data all subjects were assessed
as in normal controls. There is a great deal of using:
controversy in the literature about the relevance 1. The Geriatric Mental State Schedule (GMS)
of organic cerebral disease to the genesis of late (Copeland et al., 1976).
paraphrenia. Whilst Fish (1960) in his Edinburgh 2. The Mental Test Score (MTS) (Hodkinson,
Series of senile paranoid states found organic 1973), a test of memory and orientation.
factors to be very important, other authors 3. The Digit Copying Test (DCT) (Kendrick,
tended to place greater emphasis on elements like 1965), a test of psychomotor speed and
social isolation, personality traits and sensory concentration.
defects. 4. The Digit Symbol Substitution Sub-Test
In the present study, we have attempted to (DST) of the WAIS (Wechsler, 1955). Scores
investigate the role of these factors in the on this test probably reflect a variety of
causation of paraphrenia. We report here the functions including both psychomotor speed
results of CT investigations and the neuropsycho- and learning ability. The learning component
logical assessment. E E G findings will be given in may be dependent on both visuo-spatial
a separate report. discrimination and memory (Glosser et al.,
1977).

SUBJECTS AND METHODS Radiological data


CT scans were obtained using the EM1 CT 1010
The subjects consisted of 43 patients suffering
head scanner. A minimum of eight cuts were
from late paraphrenia who were hospital inpa-
made parallel to the orbito-meatal line. Each slice
tients, outpatients or day patients. Thirty-nine
represented a thickness of 10 mm. The two slices
subjects were current patients of the Joint
showing the biggest lateral ventricular areas were
Hospital and five came from other sources. The
chosen and the ventricular-brain ratio (VBR) was
control group consisted of 40 normal elderly
determined using a planimeter. The largest value
volunteers who were living independently in the
is given here for both groups. A linear measure of
community and who showed no evidence of
the maximum width of the frontal horns of the
dementia or psychiatric disorder. They were
lateral ventricles to the maximum intenral dia-
matched for age and sex. The latter were derived
from Age Concern centres, local day centres and meter of the skull (bifrontal-intracranial ratio)
was also determined for each subject.
Salvation Army Homes for elderly officers. Both
groups were broadly comparable in terms of their
previous occupational status.
The diagnosis of paraphrenia was first estab- RESULTS
lished by the consultant psychiatrist in charge of
the case, but before being included in the study These concern comparisons between paraphre-
patients were required to fulfill the following nics and normal controls.
criteria (Naguib et al., 1987). (1) The presence of 1. Age. Both groups were well matched for age
LATE PARAPHRENIA 85

Table 1. Psychological tests and CT measurements

Paraphrenia Normals Significance


n = 43 n = 40 level
Mean f SD Mean k SD

Age 75.27f 6.29 75.85f 8.64 N.S.


Mental Test Score (MTS) 28.48f 4.03 31.72t 3.08 p<O.OOI
Digit Copying Test 59.62 f 32.96 93.59 k 30.86 p < 0.002

Digit Symbol Substitution Test 9.93 f 7.03 11.52 f 3.22 N.S.


(DST)
Ventricular-brain ratio 13.09f 4.34 9.75f 4.35 p<O.O01
( V W
Bifrontal-intracranial 32.09k 6.39 31.91 f 4.33 N.S.
ratio
~~

f-test (two-tailed).

(paraphrenics 75.85 k 8.6 vs controls 75-28 on the MTS and GMS symptom factors
k 6-2). ‘depression’, ‘memory impairment’, ‘retarded
2. Sex. The female to male ratio was somewhat speech’ and ‘disorientation’. Results are given
higher in the paraphrenic group as compared in Table 2.
to the controls (37/6 vs 31/9). 5. Radiological data. Patients with paraphrenia
3. Sensory defects. Sixteen (37%) of the para- differed significantly from normal controls on
phrenics were ‘socially deaf‘ while this was so planimetric measures of cortical atrophy VBR
in only two (5%) of the normal controls. Three (p < 0.001). The same trend was seen for the
(7%) paraphrenics had variable degrees of bifrontal-intracranial ratio although it did not
visual impairment and four (10%) of the reach significance level. Results are given in
controls had similar defects. Table 1. There were no significant correlations
4. Psychological tests. When compared with the between VBR (or bifrontal-intracranial ratio)
normal controls paraphrenics performed less and the various symptoms elicited by the GMS
well on all tests. The differences were signifi- (see Table 2).
cant for the MTS (p<O.OOl) and the DCT 6. Distribution of Mental Test Score and ven-
(p < 0.002). The DST differences were in the tricular-brain ratio by age decade. The VBR
same direction but were not significant. values increased with age in both paraphrenics
Results are given in Table 1. Subjects who and normal controls indicating greater degree
were visually impaired have been excluded of cerebral atrophy. This was paralleled by a
from the analysis of the DCT and DST. similar fall in MTS scores for both groups
Although there was a highly significant cor- (Table 3).
relation between scores on neuropsychological 7. Six patients had VBR values equal to or more
tests and measures of cerebral atrophy (VBR than two standard deviations above the con-
and bifrontal-intracranial ratio) in the normal trols’ mean (18.45). Those subjects did not
control group, amongst the paraphrenics no differ from the rest of the patient group in age,
such significant associations emerged. There age of first onset, duration of illness or its
were also no significant correlations between severity, the presence of positive or negative
performance on the neuropsychological tests symptoms, or in MTS, DCT and DST scores.
or the measures of cerebral atrophy and the 8. The mean duration of paraphrenia was 47
duration of illness. Scores on psychological months with a range of 3-240. An arbitrary
tests showed a non-significant association, with duration of two years was taken to divide the
the presence of hearing impairment (MTS, patients into a group with a relatively acute
p < 0.06; DCT, p < 0.07). Statistically signifi- form of the illness ( N = 15) and one with a
cant correlations were found between scores more chronic form ( N = 27).
Table 2. Intercorrelation of clinical, psychological and CT variables

GMS FACTORS

Paraphrenia months 1 1.00


Memory/orientation 2 -0.04 1 .00
Digit copying 3 -0.14 0.51*** 1.00
Digit symbol 4 -0.15 0.48 * * * 0.31% I.00
Depression 5 0.04 -0.32* 0.12 -0.30* 1 40
Anxiety 6 0.30* 0.15 0.03 0.009 0.20 1 .oo
Memory impaired 7 -0.004 -0.7 I * * * * -0.5 I**** -0.20 0.13 -0.22 1.00
Retarded speech 8 -0.04 -0.31* 0.13 0.12 0.38* -0.06 0.32** 1.00
Disorientation 9 0.12 -0.31* -0.09 -0.13 0. I3 -0.12 0.44** * 0.51**** 1 .00
Insight impaired 10 0.37** 0.08 0.07 0.03 -0.12 -0.31 -0.14 -0.13 -0.22 1.00
11 -0.01 -0.04 - ().'j6*il '? :v
Paranoid delusions 0.30 0.09 -0.21 -0.06 -0.17 0.01 0.43*** 1.00
Visual hallucination 12 -0.07 0.05 0.50*** 0.10 0.18 ().37*:i- * -0.25* -0.05 -0.09 -0.03 -0.19 1.00
Auditory hallucination 13 0.16 0.16 - 0.0 1 -0.06 0.05 0.06 -0.10 0.21 0.02 -0.09 -0.02 -0.16 1.00
VBR 14 0.11 -0.16 0.25 0.07 0.05 -0.02 0.08 -0.06 0.11 0.008 -0.21 0.19 0.07 1.00
Bifron tal-intracranial 15 0.01 -0.16 -0.08 -0 4 2 0.01 0.07 0.20 -041 0.20 -0.10 -0.12 -0.05 0.17 0.75*'" 1.00
ratio
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 I? 14 15

* p < 0.05
* * p <0.01
* * * p < 0.005
* * * * p < 0.00 1
LATE PARAPHRENIA 87

Table 3. Distribution of Mental Test Score and ventricular-brain


ratio by age decade

Age decade MTS VBR


Paraphenia Controls Paraphrenia Controls

6&69 30.00 ? 3.4 33.22 f 1.3 11.46 f 4.3 6.33 f 2.3


70-79 29.07 f 3.4 32.00 f 3.4 12.96 f 3.7 10.38 f 4.9
80-89 26.30 f 4.8 29.75 f 3.01 14.04 f 3.8 11.12 f 3.6

The two subgroups did not significantly differ on the lack of significant difference in VBR between
any of the psychological or radiological indices subjects with a relatively short history and those
(VBR 12.6 vs 13-3; MTS 28.2 vs 28.6; DCT 58.0 who had been ill for more than two years. VBR
vs 60.8; DST 10.7 vs 9-2). values were in fact normally distributed in the
patient group which, cannot be meaningfully
subdivided in terms of ventricular size.
DISCUSSION The role of medication is somewhat unclear. Of
10 patients who were drug-free some showed
This investigation demonstrated for the first time ventricular enlargement. The same is true for
that patients with late paraphrenia had lateral younger patients with schizophrenia. Abnormal
cerebral ventricles which were larger than those ventricles were found by pneumoencephalogra-
of an age-matched normal control group. This phy in patients who had never received neurolep-
was associated with a mild but appreciable tics (Huber, 1957; Asano, 1967). More recently
impairment on a number of psychological tests. Johnstone et al. (1976, 1978) using CT techniques
Whilst the degree of ventricular enlargement was also found enlarged ventricles in patients who had
much less than that reported in patients with never received phenothiazines.
dementia (Jacoby and Levy, 1980; Naguib and Institutionalization and its long-term effects
Levy, 1982), it was of a similar magnitude to that have been suggested as causes of ventricular
seen in elderly depressives (Jacoby and Levy, enlargement. The lack of correlation between
1980). Similar results have been found in younger ventricular size and duration of illness refutes this
patients with schizophrenia (Johnstone et al., proposition in this patient group. Moreover,
1976; Weinberger et al., 1979a, 1982; Tanaka et Weinberger et al. (1979b) studying patients with
al., 1981; Okasha and Madkour, 1982; Nasrallah schizophrenia found that neither duration of
et al., 1982). illness nor length of hospitalization correlated
This finding appears to challenge the widely with ventricular size.
held belief that structural brain abnormalities do Our own data and those reported in most
not occur in paraphrenia and raises a number of studies in schizophrenia, notably that of Weinber-
interesting questions. Does ventricular enlarge- ger et al. (1982), support the notion that
ment precede or follow the development of the ventricular enlargement may precede the de-
condition? Is there a causal connection between velopment of the psychosis both in young patients
the two? What bearing does ventricular enlarge- and in late life. Large ventricles probably serve as
ment have on the type of clinical presentation a non-specific risk factor for later development of
seen? Is the cognitive impairment a direct result paraphrenia in vulnerable individuals. This is
of ventricular enlargement or does it reflect some consistent with the notion of a multifactorial
other illness-related variables? genetic-environmental continuum of liability
The answers to many of these questions must (Reich et al., 1972), with paraphrenics having a
remain inferential. The lack of correlation be- smaller genetic loading as compared with schi-
tween ventricular enlargement and duration of zophrenics and requiring more environmental
illness suggests that ventricular enlargement may events before manifesting the disorder (Naguib et
have preceded the development of late paraphre- al., 1987). The additive effects of various
nia. Further supporting evidence for this view was environmental events such as ventricular enlarge-
88 M . NAGUIB A N D R . LEVY

ment (which might be an expression of past or Patients with paraphrenia have historically
present cerebral insult), prolonged social isola- been considered to show little or no intellectual
tion, longstanding sensory defects, superimposed deterioration. The finding of a significant cogni-
on an abnormal personality of paranoid or tive impairment amongst those in this study is
schizoid type may lead to the psychosis becoming important and interesting. The significant rela-
overt for the first time late in life. This view is tionship between measures of brain atrophy and
supported by a report by Reveley et a / . (1984), those of psychological impairment in the normal
who studied schizophrenia in twins’ birth and control group and their gradual progression with
found that later age of onset was associated with a advancing age indicate that the two processes are
negative family history and enlarged cerebral a feature of normal ageing (Barron et al., 1976;
ventricles. The latter, they suggested, may be just Wood, 1982). The lack of such correlation in the
one non-specific indicator of cerebral pathology patient group may signify that other variables
necessary to produce psychiatric illness in those such as psychotic experiences, hearing impair-
with a low genetic predisposition. Preexisting ment, medication, lack of motivation or other
cerebral abnormalities were also reported by Kay related difficulties may account for more of the
and Roth (1961) and McClelland et al. (1966) in variance in the patient group.
relation to late paraphrenia. Furthermore, Davi- These results merit further investigation. Fu-
son and Bagley (1969) who examined the role of ture studies should exploit the greater resolution
focal CNS lesions in producing psychosis pointed of more advanced CT scanners. PET scanning
out that there was a lapse of some six years (with may provide valuable dynamic information about
a range of 0-31 years) between the development cerebral function and magnetic resonance imag-
of the lesion and the onset of psychiatric ing (MRI) may provide greater information about
symptoms. possible changes in white matter. In the mean-
The evidence also suggested that the degree of time a follow-up study is likely to yield useful
ventricular enlargement appeared to have n o information about the prognostic significance of
direct bearing on the presentation of the clinical the changes reported.
syndrome. There was no difference between
those with particularly large VBRs and the rest on
any of the clinical or psychological parameters. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This accords with the views of Nasrallah et al.
(1983) studying young and middle aged patients. We would like to thank Dr Klaus Bergmann, Dr
However, other investigators attempting to iden- George Stein and D r Marisa Silverman for
tify variables which appear to be differentially allowing us to study their patients. We are also
associated with enlarged ventricles have reported most grateful to D r Felix Post, Professors Alwyn
associations between increased ventricular size Lishman and Brice Pitt and Dr Robin Jacoby for
and chronic schizophrenia, the presence of reading earlier drafts and making constructive
neuropsychological impairment, negative symp- suggestions and the staff of the neuroradiology
toms, poor premorbid adjustment and poor department of the Maudsley Hospital for patient-
response to neuroleptic drugs (Johnstone et al., ly carrying out the scans. The work was supported
1978; Crow, 1980; Weinberger et al., 1980). by a grant from the Special Health Authority of
As in the studies of dementia (Jacoby and the Bethlem Royal Hospital and the Maudsley
Levy, 1980; Naguib and Levy, 1982a,b), we Hospital.
found that the memory and orientation test
correlated with the greatest number of variables,
confirming its clinical value. The negative correla- REFERENCES
tions between GMS factors ‘depression’ and
‘retarded speech’ with scores on the memory and Asano, N. (1967) Pneumoencephalographic study of
orientation test confirm the observation that schizophrenia In Clinical Genetics in Psychiatry.
Problems in Nosological Classification. (H. Mitsuda
depression andlor depressive symptoms which are and 0. Takasuk Eds). Igaku-Skoin, Tokyo, pp.
prominent features of paranoid disorders may 20%219.
produce cognitive change. It seems likely that the Barron, S. A., Jacobs, L. and Kinkel, W. R. (1976)
relationship between Digit Copying Test and the Change in size of normal lateral ventricles showing
GMS factor ‘visual hallucination’ is a chance aging determined by computed tomographyn.
finding in a large matrix. Neurology 26, 1011-1013.
LATE P A R A P H R E N I A 89

Copeland, J. R. M., Kelleher, M. J., Kellett, J. M., Ment. Sci. 107, 649-686.
Gourlay, A. J., Gurland, B. J., Fleiss, J. L. and Kendrick, D. C. (1965) Speed and learning in the
Sharpe, L. (1976) A semi-structured clinical inter- diagnosis of diffuse brain damage in elderly subjects:
view for the assessment of diagnosis and mental state a Bayesian statistical approach. Brit. J . SOC. Clin.
in the elderly: The Geriatric Mental State Schedule: Psychol. 4, 141-148.
1. Development and reality. Psychol. Med. 6, Kingsley, D . and Trimble, K. (1978) Cerebral ventricu-
439449. lar size in chronic schizophrenia. Lancet 1, 278-279.
Crow, T. J. (1980) Molecular pathology of schizophre- Klonoff, H . , Fibiger, C. and Hutton, G . H . (1970)
nia: more than one disease process? Brit. Med. J . 78, Neuropsychological patterns in chronic schizophre-
6M8. nia. J . Nerv. Ment. Dis. 150, 291-300.
Davison, K. and Bagley, C. R. (1969) Current Kraeplin, E. (1919) Dementia Praecox and Paraphre-
problems in neuropsychiatry (ed. R. N. Herrington). nia. Krieger, New York, 1971.
Brit. J . Psychiat., Special Publication No. 4 (for the Maggs, R. and Turton, E. C. (1956) Some E E G
Royal Medico-Psychological Associaton). Headley , findings in old age and their relationship to affective
Ashford, Kent. disorder. J . Ment. Sci. 102, 812.
Fish, F. (1960) Senile schizophrenia. J . Ment. Sci. 106, Matthews, C. G. and Booker, H . E. (1972)
938-946. Pneumoencephalographic measurements and
Glosser, G . , Butters, N. and Kaplan, E. (1977) neuropsychological test performance in human
Visuo-perceptual processes in brain damaged pa- adults. Cortex 8, 69-92.
tients on the Digit Symbol Substitution Test. Brit. J . McClelland, H. A . , Roth, M., Neubauer, H. and
Psychiat. 7, 5946. Garside, R. (1966) Proceedings of the 4th World
Golden, C . J. Moses, J. A. Jr., Zelazowski, R., Congress of Psychiatry, Madrid, Vol. 4. ICS 150,
Graber, B., Zatz, L. M., Horvath, T . B. and Berger, Excerpta Medica 2955.
P. A . (1980) Cerebral ventricular size and neuro- Moore, M. T., Nathan, D., Elliot, A. R. et al. (1933)
psychological impairment in young chronic Encephalographic studies in schizophrenia dementia
schizophrenics: Measurement by the standardised praecox. A m . J . Psychiat. 89, 801-810.
Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery. Arch. Naguib, M. and Levy, R. (1982a) Prediction of
General Psychiatry, 37, 619423. outcome in senile dementia - a computed tornogra-
Golden, C. J., Hammeke, T. A. and Purisch, A. D. phy study. Brit. J . Psychiat. 140, 263-267.
(1978) Diagnostic validity of a standardised neuro- Naguib, M. and Levy, R. (1982b) C T scanning in senile
psychological battery derived from Luria’s neuro- dementia - a follow-up of survivors. Brit. J .
psychological tests. J . Consult. Clin. Psychol. 46, Psychiat. 141, 618-620.
1258-1265. Naguib, M., McGuffin, P., Levy, R., Festeneshine. H.
Herbert, M. E. and Jacobson, S. (1967) Late and Alonso, A. C. (1987) Genetic markers in late
paraphrenia. Brit. J . Psychiat. 113, 461469. paraphrenia - a study of HLA antigens. Brit. J .
Hill, D. (1956) Schizophrenia - Somatic Aspects. (ed. Psychiat. 150, 124-127.
D . Richter). Pergamon Press, London. Nasrallah, A., Jacoby, C. G . , McCalley-Whitters, K.
Hodkinson, H. M. (1973) Mental impairment in the and Kuperman, S. (1982) Cerebral ventricular
elderly. J . Roy. Coll. Physicians Lond. 7, 305-317. enlargement in subtypes of chronic schizophrenia.
Huber, G. (1957) Pneumoencephalographische und Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 39, 774-777.
Psychopathologische Bilder bei Endogenen Nasrallah, H. A , , Kuperman, S . , Hamra, B. J. and
Psychosen. Springer-Verlag, Berlin. McCalley-Whitters, M. (1983) Clinical differences
Jacoby, R. and Levy, R. (1980) Computed tomography between schizophrenia patients with and without
in the elderly: 2 Senile dementia: diagnosis and large cerebral ventricles. J . Clin. Psychiat. 44, 11,
functional impairment. Brit. J . Psychiat. 136, 25& 407409.
269. Okasha, A . and Madkour, 0. (1982) Cortical and
Johnstone, E . C., Crow, T. J., Frith, C. D., Husband, central atrophy in chronic schizophrenia. Acta
J. and Kreel, L. (1976) Cerebral ventricular size and Psychiat. Scand. 65, 29-34.
cognitive impairment in chronic schizophrenia. Lan- Pollin, W., Stabenan, J . R., Mosher, L. and Tupin, J.
cet 2, 924-926. (1966) Life history differences in identical twins
Johnstone, E. C . , Crow, T. J., Frith, C. D., Stevens, discordant for schizophrenia. Am. J . Orthopsychiat.
M., Kreel, L. and Husband, J. (1978) The dementia 36, 492-509.
of dementia praecox. Acta Psychiat. Scand. 57, Post, F. (1966) Persistent Persecutory State of the
305-324. Elderly. Pergamon Press, Oxford.
Kay, D. W. K. and Roth, M. (1961) Environmental Reich, T., James, J. W. and Morris, C. A. (1972) The
and hereditary factors in the schizophrenias of old use of multiple thresholds in determining the mode
age (‘late paraphrenia’) and their bearing on the of transmission of semi-continuous traits. A n n . Hum.
general problem of causation in schizophrenia. J . Genet. 36, 163-184.
90 M. NAGUIB A N D R. LEVY

Reveley, A., Reveley, M. and Murray, R. (1984) Wechsler, D., (1955) The Wechsler Manual. The
Cerebral ventricular enlargement in non-genetic Psychological Co-operation, New York.
schizophrenics: a controlled study. Brit. J . Psychiat. Weinberger, D. R., Bigelow, L. B., Kleinman, J. E.,
144, 89-93. Klein, S. T., Rosenblatt, J . E. and Wyatt, R. J.
Rochford, J. M., Detre, T., Tucker, G. J . and Harrow, (1980) Cerebral ventricular enlargement in chronic
M. (1970) Neuropsychological impairment in func- schizophrenia - an association with poor response
tional psychiatric diseases. Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 22, to treatment. Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 37, 11-13.
114-119. Weinberger, D. R., Torrey, E. F., Neophytides, A. M .
Rogers, D. (1985) The motor disorders of severe and Wyatt, R. J. (1979a) Lateral cerebral ventricular
psychiatric illness: a conflict of paradigms. Brit. J . enlargement in chronic schizophrenia. Arch. Gen.
Psychiat. 147, 221-232. Psychiat. 36, 735-739.
Tanaka, Y., Hazama, H., Kawahara, R. and Weinberger, D. R., Torrey, E. F., Neophytides, A. N.
Kobayashi, K. (1981) Computerized tomography of and Wyatt, R. J. (1979b) Structural abnormalities in
the brain in schizophrenic patients. Acta Psychint. the cerebral cortex of chronic schizophrenic patients.
Scand. 63, 191-197. Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 36, 935-939.
Watson, C. G. (1971) Separation of brain-damaged Weinberger, D. R., DeLisi, L. E., Perman, G. P.,
from schizophrenic patients, Reitan-Halstead pat- Targum, S. and Wyatt, R. J. (1982) Computed
tern analysis: an unsuccessful replication. Psychology tomography in schizophreniform disorder and other
29, 1343-1346. acute psychiatric disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 39,
Watson, C. G., Thomas, R . W., Andersen, D. et al. 778-783.
(1968) Differentiation of organics from schizophre- Wood, R. (1982) The psychology of ageing: assessment
nics at two chronicity levels by use of the Reitman- of defects and their management. In The Psychiatry
Halstead organic test battery. J . Consult. Clin. of Late Life. (Raymond Levy and Felix Post Eds),
Psychol. 32, 679-684. Blackwell, Oxford.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi