Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 17

The Role of Formal Structure in Liking for Popular Music

Author(s): Jonathan J. Rolison and Judy Edworthy


Source: Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, Vol. 29, No. 3 (Feb. 1, 2012), pp.
269-284
Published by: University of California Press
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1525/mp.2012.29.3.269
Accessed: 05-06-2018 18:08 UTC

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms

University of California Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend
access to Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
The Role of Form in Popular Music 269

T he R ole of F ormal S tructure in L iking for P opular M usic

J onathan J. R olison & J udy E dworthy of a piece of music can take. The arrangement of themes
University of Plymouth, Plymouth, United Kingdom and musical ideas, key structure, and changes of texture
and instrumentation are all important determinants of
the effect of restructuring the form of three form (Bonds, 1991; Carpenter, 1983; Reti, 1951). However,
unfamiliar pop/rock songs was investigated in two the salience of form and its influence on listeners’ enjoy-
experiments. In the first experiment, listeners’ judge- ment of compositions is a matter of some debate. Several
ments of the likely location of sections of novel popular studies have taken pieces of music with a discernible form
songs were explored by requiring participants to place and have reordered sections and presented them to listen-
the eight sections (Intro – Verse 1 – Chorus 1 – Verse 2 ers in a new form, only to find that the listener is largely
– Chorus 2 – Bridge (solo) – Chorus 3 – Extro) of the indifferent to that reordering. Not only does reordering
songs into the locations they thought them most likely to musical sections have little effect on people’s enjoyment of
occur within the song. Results revealed that participants the new version of the piece, listeners usually seem not to
were able to place the sections in approximately the right have noticed the restructuring (Eitan & Granot, 2008;
location with some accuracy, though they were unable to Gotlieb & Koneçni, 1985; Karno & Koneçni, 1992; Koneçni,
differentiate between choruses. In Experiment 2, three 1984, Tillman & Bigand, 1996; but see Heyduk, 1975). For
versions of each of the songs were presented in three example, Eitan and Granot (2008) created hybrid versions
different structures: intact (original form), medium of two of Mozart’s piano sonatas such that sections of one
restructured (the sections in a moderately changed were heard within the other, and found that participants
order), and highly restructured (more severe did not prefer the original versions of the sonatas over
restructuring). The results show that listeners’ judgments hybrids (where the two sonatas were combined). In fact,
of predictability and liking were largely uninfluenced by listeners with music training preferred the hybrids over
the restructuring of the songs, in line with findings for the original sonatas. Tillman and Bigand (1996) found
classical music. Moment-by-moment liking judgements that participants’ ratings of coherence were unaffected
of the songs demonstrated a change in liking judgements even when the segments of a piano solo were played in
with repeated exposure, though the trend was downwards reverse. Tan and Spackman (2005) went one stage further
with repeated exposure rather than upwards. Detailed by rearranging musical segments taken from completely
analysis of moment-by-moment judgements at the ends different compositions. Under these conditions, untrained
and beginnings of sections showed that listeners were participants did rate the “patchwork” versions as less uni-
able to respond quickly to intact songs, but not to fied than their intact versions but ultimately preferred
restructured songs. The results suggest that concatenism them to the intact versions. However, by rearranging
prevails in listening to popular song at the expense of musical segments in this way, adjacent segments often
paying attention to larger structural features. differed in key and tempo, as well as volume. As a whole,
Received May 26, 2010, accepted June 2, 2011. these findings suggest that without musical training
people are often insensitive to overall form.
Key words: concatenism, restructured songs, repeated If people’s perceptual experiences are unrelated to the
exposure, liking, predictability overall form of a composition, one may ask whether
form has any real importance in listeners’ experience of
a composition. Several studies indicate that listeners at-
tend to the surface features of music rather than the

T
 he issue of formal structure in music is an structural elements (Deliège, 1987; Deliège, Melen,
important one for composers, musicologists, and Stammers, & Cross, 1996; Pollard-Gott, 1983). Indeed,
listeners alike. In both classical and contemporary supporters of concatenism in music (Gurney, 1880/1966;
music there are many forms that the overarching structure Levinson, 1997) argue that larger structures in music are

Music Perception volume 29, issue 3, pp . 269–284 issn 0730-7829, electronic issn 1533-8312. © 2012 by the regents of the university of california all
rights reserved . please direct all requests for permission to photocopy or reproduce article content through the university of california press ’ s
rights and permissions website , http :// www . ucpressjournals . com / reprintinfo . asp .DOI: 10.1525/ mp .2012.29.3.269

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
270 Jonathan J. Rolison & Judy Edworthy

all but irrelevant to the listeners’ enjoyment of a piece, as an introduction, followed by two or three verses and
which is very much “in the moment” and depends more choruses (usually the chorus follows the verse, but there
on local features than on the overall structure and form. are famous songs that begin with the chorus), often also
Of course, pieces of music are written with a specific with some kind of contrasting section such as a solo or
form in mind and so violations of that form, whether or a bridge, usually occurring quite late into the song. Songs
not the listener is consciously attending to it, can cause often end with some kind of ‘extro’ as well.
surprising effects in rearranged music (such as sudden In the present studies we tested whether people’s per-
changes of key, loudness, and other features one might ceptual experiences of form and liking for compositions
not expect if the music were presented in the order in- can be influenced by disrupting the form of popular
tended by the composer), thus disrupting the moment- music that follows a familiar verse-chorus form. We
by-moment listening. There is ample previous research might expect that rearranging the sections of a pop-rock
indicating that untrained listeners have expectations song would elicit changes in people’s perceptions of
about local sequences of events in Western music structure and other judgments that might follow, such
(Tillmann, Bharucha, & Bigand, 2000; Tillmann & as liking. A pop-rock song that starts, for example, with
Lebrun-Guillaud, 2006), whereas the evidence that these its extro followed by its third chorus (which, even though
expectations generalize to expectations about the choruses are generally similar to one another, might be
sequences of sections that determine the form is, at the louder and more repetitive than earlier choruses) would
very least, ambiguous. seem to violate its true structure. We would expect that
Almost all of the studies looking at listeners’ percep- rearranging these sections will appear both unpleasing
tion of form and the effect of changing that form on initially and highly unpredictable.
liking and other judgments are concerned with classical Intriguingly, even in cases where restructured compo-
music. However, it is important to ascertain the extent sitions are rated as less well structured initially, altered
to which these effects might occur in listening to versions are sometimes perceived as more pleasing with
popular music. As an indication of its popularity and repeated hearings, in contrast to intact versions of those
familiarity in modern Western culture, pop-rock music compositions. Tan, Spackman, and Peaslee (2006) found
accounted for 58% of British record sales in 2009 (BPI that while people initially preferred intact versions over
Statistical Handbook, 2009), and is, in general, under- “patchwork” versions, they instead preferred the “patch-
studied in the music psychology literature. Popular work” compositions after only four hearings. The
songs are relatively short, usually about three minutes authors reasoned that while the new arrangements were
in length, and are typically much shorter than the perceived as more complex and unpredictable initially,
classical pieces investigated in other studies (though in they came to be liked more over repeated hearings as
many of these studies short excerpts were used). The they became more familiar to the listener. As the authors
subject of form in popular music is one of considerable note, their findings are readily understood when the
interest to musicologists, popular musicians, music relation between liking and complexity is viewed as an
writers, and the like (e.g., Covach, 2005; Everett, 2009; inverted U curve. According to this account, people have
Stephenson, 2002). a preference for moderately complex stimuli (Berlyne,
While there is much discussion and some disagree- 1971; Hargreaves, 1984; North & Hargreaves, 1995;
ment regarding this topic, writers (e.g., Everett, 2009; Smith & Cuddy, 1986; Szpunar, Schellenberg, & Pliner,
Stephenson, 2002) agree that a basic principle of much 2004). A composition that is initially perceived as highly
popular music is an alternation between verses and cho- complex may be viewed as unpredictable and unpleas-
ruses. While verses tend to have the same music for each ing. After repeated hearings, however, the composition
verse but vary in terms of their lyrical content, choruses would appear more familiar and predictable. Hence, a
are usually either very similar or identical to one another composition that is initially unpredictable, such as one
in terms of their lyrics as well as melody. The repetition whose segments have been rearranged, may be perceived
of the same, usually short lyrics is a defining feature of as more pleasing with each hearing. In contrast, an intact
a chorus. Choruses also often tend to be louder (as more composition that is more balanced may become predict-
instruments are typically used) and have higher notes in able and less pleasing over time. This is in contrast,
them. Songwriters also use other methods to announce however, with a number of findings that are consistent
the start of new sections, for example “stop time” is often with a “mere exposure” effect of repetition, whereby
used to announce the start of a new verse (Stephenson, music compositions of any kind are expected to be
2002). Typically in verse-chorus form and its variations, perceived as more pleasing over time simply due to
a song will have a number of discernible sections such exposure to the stimulus (Bornstein, 1989; Peretz,

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
The Role of Form in Popular Music 271

Gaudreau, & Bonnel, 1998; Zajonc, 1968). Hence, in line sections. One was intended to be somewhat reordered
with the findings of Tan et al. (2006) we can expect (“medium restructured”) and the other was intended
people’s liking for popular music to change over repeated to be more significantly reordered (“high restruc-
hearings, but this may depend on whether song sections tured”). The results of Experiment 1 allowed us to
are intact or rearranged. Intact versions of pop-rock gauge the validity of our two restructured versions of
songs may be preferred initially over restructured each song.
versions, but this may reverse with repeated hearings.
One of the central issues of concern with listening to Method
restructured pieces of music is that of the interplay
between the listener’s experience of the larger structural Participants
and formal features and the local, note-by-note features. Sixteen undergraduate students at the University of
The methodology used in all experiments in this area Plymouth participated for course credits. Participants
until now has been to take judgments at the end of the completed the task in a single sitting lasting up to
piece (or usually at the end of an excerpt). This approach 25 min.
does not necessarily allow insight into the interplay
between the moment-by-moment features of the music Design
and the broader structural elements. In Experiment 2, All participants listened to the individual sections of
we use a moment-by-moment methodology where the each of the three songs used in Experiment 2. The song
listener tracks his or her liking during the playing of the sections were heard in a different random order for
piece. Such a methodology has not been used in music each participant and all sections of a song were heard
preference studies but has been used in other areas in the before moving on to the next song. While hearing each
psychology of music (e.g., Krumhansl, 1997). section, participants were asked to adjust a slider
displayed on a computer monitor to indicate where in
Experiment 1 the song they expected to hear the section. The slider
ranged from “the beginning of the song” on the left, to
Since different section types (e.g., verse, chorus, bridge) “the end of the song” on the right, with labels appearing
tend to have different characteristics, we might expect above the slider at each end. Slider values ranged from
that listeners could use these characteristics to guess the 0 (on the left) to 100 (on the right). After hearing each
identity of a section and (therefore) its probable loca- section, the slider remained on screen until participants
tion within the song. For example, we might expect that clicked a “continue” button to move on to the next
after hearing a chorus section in isolation, listeners section of a song, and were informed on screen before
could identify it as a chorus (from its internal features) hearing the sections of the next song. The slider
and would therefore expect it to follow a verse. Similarly, appeared initially in the left most position, under the
they might expect a solo or bridge to occur during the label “beginning of the song.”
second half rather than the first half of a song. We do
not know the extent to which listeners might be able to Materials
differentiate between individual verses and choruses, Three songs were selected for use in the studies. These
but that is in part the purpose of Experiment 1. In were by unsigned artists/bands that were very unlikely
Experiment 1 we asked participants to make judgments to be familiar to the participants. The three songs used
about the likely positions of the individual sections of in the experiments were made up of eight sections cor-
the three unfamiliar songs used in Experiment 2. Each responding to an Intro – Verse 1 – Chorus 1 – Verse 2
song followed a similar Intro – Verse 1 – Chorus 1 – – Chorus 2 – Solo (or Bridge) – Chorus 3 – Extro
Verse 2 – Chorus 2 – Solo – Chorus 3 – Extro sequence layout; Stephenson (2002) describes this as a variant of
in its original form. Participants judged where in the what he calls “verse-chorus-bridge strophic form.” The
songs they expected each of the sections to be posi- sections were of different lengths for the three songs,
tioned. They heard only the sections and not the entire and the songs were of different lengths overall. The sec-
songs. This allowed us to both gauge the extent to tions of the songs demonstrate many of the principles
which listeners understood the intended structure of of rock form as delineated by Stephenson (2002,
the original songs and to form a basis for generating Chapter 6): The delineation of the different sections
alternative versions of the songs where the sections largely comes from the way text, instrumentation,
were reordered. For Experiment 2, we generated two rhythm, and harmony were used across the songs. For
alternative versions of the songs by reordering the example, all of the songs had a short instrumental

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
272 Jonathan J. Rolison & Judy Edworthy

introduction that formed the intro. Verses tended to intro). Solos were indicated by the absence of the voice,
start on the tonic, and in all cases had repeated melodic and usually the introduction of some new or modified
patterns with different text on each iteration. Choruses material played only on instruments.
were indicated by one-line long, immediately repeated In relation to the second focus of interest, the key fea-
text and recurred numerous times throughout the ture was that the choruses of all three songs used the
songs. The choruses sometimes started on the domi- same lyrics throughout, whereas the words of the verses
nant or subdominant harmony, though in one song the changed as the songs progressed. In terms of the verses,
chorus started on the tonic (Song 2). The solo or bridge there was very little variation in the accompanying music
for the selected songs was an instrumental section across the two verses of each song, though occasionally
following the second chorus, in place of a third verse, there was added instrumentation in the second verse (for
and usually with some new musical material such as a example, Song 2 adds a piano to the accompaniment).
new melody (with new accompanying harmony), or an In all three songs the lyrics were different across the two
instrumental development of music material that verses. In all three songs, the words of the choruses were
appeared earlier in the song. Differences between the the same throughout. Choruses 1 and 2 tended to be
sections were further delineated by changes in instru- almost identical, with greater variation in Chorus 3 (for
mentation, the use of stop-time, introduction of back- example, in Song 1 the melodic line was different and
ing singers and other devices typical of rock songs. The higher in pitch on the third iteration).
focus of interest for the purposes of the research
reported here was the degree to which 1) the main Procedure
sections of the songs were typical of songs of this type; Participants operated a personal computer custom pro-
and 2) the extent to which similar, repeated sections grammed in Visual Studio. They were informed that
(for example verses and choruses) differed from one they would hear the sections of a set of songs in random
another as the song progressed. In relation to the first order one at a time before moving on to the next song.
focus of interest, the verses were typically “verse-like” in While listening to each section, participants were asked
that they tended to start on the tonic, be lower in pitch to decide where in the song they thought the section
overall than the choruses, and be accompanied by more should go, and to make their judgment by adjusting a
sparse instrumentation than the choruses (typically slider presented on the computer screen to some point
there would be no drum for at least a section of each between the “beginning of the song” and the “end of the
verse, and on occasion only a single instrument, usually song.” To ensure that participants’ judgments were
a guitar, would be heard). Stop-time was sometimes based solely on the characteristics of the song sections,
used in some of the verses. Most importantly, the verses they were not told how many sections they would hear
in all cases carried the narrative of the song, which for each song, and were not told anything about the
unfolded from verse to verse. The melodic line of the sections they heard (i.e., whether they were listening to
choruses was typically higher in pitch (at least for parts a verse or chorus) or anything about the structure of
of the choruses) than the verses and the instrumenta- the songs (e.g., whether they followed a verse-chorus
tion was generally richer than in verses, and in some form or otherwise). Participants heard each section of
cases a backing chorus was used. For all three songs each song only once and were informed on screen
there were clear indications that transitions from verses before moving on to the next song.
to choruses were about to occur through the build-up
to each chorus. For example, in Song 1, Chorus 1 is Results and Discussion
clearly indicated by an introduction of the drums and a
vocal backing that was not present during Verse 1. Our first concern was whether participants had expec-
The other parts of the songs were typically indicated tations about section sequences that were consistent
in ways that would be expected; intros tended to be iden- with the true structure of the songs. Figure 1 shows the
tifiable by simple repeated chord sequences before the mean group judgments made by participants on the
voice entered, and extros tended to be indicated by tonal slider for each section averaged across the three songs
resolution (except Song 1) and fade-out. In all cases the used in Experiment 2. Numeric values adjacent to the
intro was different in some way to the extro, though this section labels provide the mean group slider values
varied across the songs (for example, the intro and extro from 1 to 100. These judgments demonstrate a number
for Song 1 were completely different from one another of features that the listeners appeared to understand on
whereas the extro for Song 3, though different from the a single hearing; namely, listeners understood where
intro, contained some material originally heard in the the intro and extro should go, and that they were

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
The Role of Form in Popular Music 273

Verse 2 (30.00)

Chorus 2 (61.38)
Intro (19 54) Solo (65.77)

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Chorus 1 (61.27) Extro (92.85)

Verse 1 (22.48) Chorus 3 (57.77)


FIGURE 1. Overall mean group judgments for each of the song sections for songs used in Experiment 2.

different from one another, that Verse 1 preceded Verse discrepancy depends on the number of positions that
2, that choruses came after verses, and that solos (or each section is out of place. Verse 2 in our example is two
bridges) tended to come in the second half of a song. positions out of place with respect to its actual position
What the results also demonstrated is that listeners, on in the song. The number of positions that each section
average across the three songs, were unable to put the is out of place for each incorrectly placed section can
choruses in the correct order but located all three cho- then be summed for each song as a measure of the total
ruses in approximately the same position. Though inconsistency between a participants’ expectations and
there were some differences between the choruses the actual structure of a song. If the order in which a
within each of the three songs, the cues available did participant places the sections is identical to the song
not allow listeners to order them. structure, then no sections will be out of place and the
However, note that judgments for the sections showed sum score will equal zero. If the score is a value other
a noticeable resemblance to the actual order of sections, than zero then one or more of the sections are out of
indicating that participants’ expectations were broadly place to some degree, indicating some inconsistency
consistent with the musical structure of the songs. This between the individual’s expectations and the actual
is even more striking when we consider that participants structure of the song. We refer to these summed scores as
heard each section of each song only once, and the sec- inconsistency scores. Inconsistency scores were calculated
tions of the songs were heard in random order. We may for each of the songs used in Experiment 2 separately for
expect that judgments about the order of sections in a each participant. Mean group scores are provided in
song would be further improved with repeated hearings, Table 1. Note that these were positive for all three songs,
as people begin to discern more about the structure of indicating some inconsistency between participants’
the song. expectations and the true structure of the songs.
Although the results do indicate that listeners had In Experiment 2 the eight song sections were heard
some degree of competence at ordering the sections, in their original order in an “intact” condition, but in
there were also inconsistencies between participants’ ex- rearranged orders for “medium” and “high” restructured
pectations and the actual order of sections in the songs. conditions. In the medium restructured condition, the
This discrepancy can be calculated separately for each sections instead followed an Intro – Chorus 2 – Verse
participant for each song. This is done by first ordering 1 – Solo – Chorus 1 – Verse 2 – Chorus 3 – Extro
the sections by the positions that the participant placed sequence, and in the high restructured condition an
them on the slider. For example, a sequence indicated by Extro – Chorus 3 – Verse 2 – Solo – Chorus 1 – Verse 1
a participant could be Intro – Verse 2 – Chorus 2 – Verse – Chorus 2 – Intro sequence. We hence also calculated
1 – Chorus 3 – Chorus 1 – Solo – Extro, where some of
the sections are out of place with respect to the actual Table 1.   Mean Group Inconsistency Scores for Songs Used in
sequence of sections in the song. In this example, the Experiment 2
intro is correctly placed at the beginning of the song, but Song 1 Song 2 Song 3
Verse 2 is incorrectly placed second in the sequence.
Sections that are placed in incorrect positions in a song Intact 8.88 12.31 10.88
indicate the discrepancy between participants’ expecta- Medium Restructured 11.73 17.56 12.63
tions and the true structure of the song. The size of the High Restructured 26.63 22.56 24.63

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
274 Jonathan J. Rolison & Judy Edworthy

participants’ inconsistency scores in relation to the that the effects of restructuring on people’s perceptions of
three songs in medium and high restructured condi- music may depend in part on the song that is restructured.
tions. This provides a measure of the degree to which This is to be expected and will be due to a number of
restructured songs fit with people’s expectations com- reasons, for example, the level of narrative in the song, the
pared with when the songs are heard intact. These were degree to which the choruses differ from one another, and
calculated separately for each participant. The highest so on. But our results show that for two of the songs there
inconsistency score possible is 32, which would indicate is a clear distinction between all three levels of restructuring,
that all eight sections are out of place with respect to and for the third there is at least one clear difference and
the individual’s expectations by the greatest possible one difference approaching significance. Thus, the
amount. By viewing Table 1 it can be seen that the restructuring of the versions, from intact to medium to
mean group inconsistency scores are substantially high, show a clearly increasing deviation from how the
higher for medium and high restructured songs than listener would expect the song to be ordered.
for intact songs, suggesting that the songs are less con- In light of the results of Experiment 1, we can say with
sistent with participants’ expectations about song some confidence that listeners had quite significant
structure when their sections are rearranged. Note that awareness of the intended structure of the songs.
inconsistency scores in the three conditions are esti- Restructured songs should therefore violate their expec-
mated from participants’ expectations about the tations, which may or may not influence other important
sequence of sections in a song. Participants did not aesthetic judgements about the songs. In Experiment 2
themselves rate the structure of the intact and we test whether restructuring the songs influences
restructured songs. people’s perceptions of predictability and, in turn, their
A two-way repeated-measures analysis of variance liking for the songs over several hearings. We can also
(ANOVA) using condition (intact, medium, and high test whether or not listeners are sensitive to song form
restructured) and song (Song 1, Song 2, and Song 3) as by observing their immediate reactions to new sections.
repeated measures and inconsistency scores as the depen- If, for example, a new section is not what the listeners
dent variable confirmed that inconsistency scores differed were expecting (because the song has been restructured)
significantly between conditions, F(2, 30) = 75.39, p < .001, then we might expect people’s reactions to differ in some
indicating that restructured music conflicts with people’s way from reactions to expected sections. Differences in
expectations about song structure. Follow-up one-way participants’ immediate reactions to new sections can
ANOVAs confirmed that inconsistency scores were higher give further insight into the degree to which concatenism
for medium restructured (13.97) than for intact (10.69) is figuring in the listeners’ responses to songs.
songs, F(1, 15) = 52.45, p < .001, and larger for high (24.61)
compared with medium restructured songs, F(1, 15) = Experiment 2
100.73, p < .001. There was also an effect of song, F(2, 30) =
5.88, p = .007, and an interaction between condition and Experiment 2 was designed to investigate the effects of
song, F(4, 60) = 4.88, p = .002. This suggests that the effects rearranging song sections on people’s perceptions of
of restructuring may depend on which song is restruc- structure and liking for a set of popular songs. In
tured. Follow-up ANOVAs revealed differences between previous published studies participants have rated com-
medium and intact, F(1, 15) = 23.57, p < .001, and high positions or excerpts only after hearing each stimulus in
and medium, F(1, 15) = 103.45, p < .001 conditions for its entirety. Retrospective reports may be influenced by
Song 1. This confirms that inconsistency scores differed memory decay, and also not necessarily tap into the
between all three conditions for Song 1. For Song 2, moment-by-moment processing of a song. This may
differences were also found between medium and intact especially be the case for restructured versions of
conditions, F(1, 15) = 58.54, p < .001, and a difference that compositions; unpredictable or complex compositions
was close to significant between high and medium, F(1, 15) may quickly be forgotten before the rating task is
= 2.36, p = .15, conditions. Finally, for song 3, differences administered. For this reason, in Experiment 2 partici-
between medium and intact conditions were marginally pants also rated their liking continuously on a
significant, F(1, 15) = 4.40, p = .05, and highly significant moment-by-moment basis while listening to the compo-
between high and medium conditions, F(1, 15) = 59.18, sitions using a methodology similar to that used in other
p <.001. These results confirm that restructuring these areas of music psychology (Krumhansl, 1997). Three
songs by rearranging their sections violates people’s versions of each of the three songs were used. In the
expectations about the intended song structure. The intact version, the original version of the song was used.
interaction between song and condition, however, suggests In the medium restructured versions, the songs were

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
The Role of Form in Popular Music 275

reordered in the following way: Intro – Chorus 2 – Verse rated their liking for each song on a 7-point scale, rang-
1 – Solo – Chorus 1 – Verse 2 – Chorus 3 – Extro and in ing from “I did not like this song at all” (on the left;
the high restructured condition they were reordered as with a value of 1) to “I liked this song a lot” (on the
follows: Extro – Chorus 3 – Verse 2 – Solo – Chorus 1 – right; with a value of 7). As our measure of perceptions
Verse 1 – Chorus 2 – Intro. However, it is important to of song structure, participants also rated whether the
note that the increase in inconsistency from the medium song followed a “predictable structure” on the same
to the high restructured versions is largely due to the scale, this time ranging from “the song did not follow
placement of the verses, intro, and extro as the data from any predictable structure” (on the left) to “the song fol-
Experiment 1 showed that while listeners were able to lowed a highly predictable structure” (on the right).
place Verse 1, Verse 2, the intro, and the extro in an Importantly, participants were not made aware that the
appropriate position, they were only able to discern that songs had been structurally altered in restructured
the choruses on average should follow verses and were conditions.
unable to place the choruses in any particular order. Slider rating. A mechanical slider was developed spe-
cifically for the present study as a tool for recording
Method participants’ liking ratings on a moment-by-moment
basis while listening to the songs. This consisted of a
Participants slider secured in a box that could be moved by the par-
Fifty-seven undergraduate students at the University of ticipant from left to right. A slider also appeared on the
Plymouth participated for course credits. None of these computer monitor and tracked the position of the me-
participants participated in Experiment 1. chanical slider. This allowed participants to monitor
their liking ratings on screen while adjusting the me-
Design chanical slider. The slider on screen ranged from “I do
All participants listened to three of the pop-rock songs not like this bit at all” on the left (the labels appeared
tested in Experiment 1 (Songs 1, 2, and 3), six times just above the slider) to “I like this bit a lot” on the right.
each across two sessions, generating 18 hearings in total The numerical values of the slider ranged from 0 to 255
for each participant. For each session, the 9 hearings from left to right. Participants were instructed to hold
were presented in random order for each participant. one hand on the slider while listening to the songs to
Participants were divided into three independent ensure that they could respond to changes in their liking
groups of 19 participants depending on whether they as fast as possible. At the beginning of each hearing,
heard the three songs in their original ‘intact’ form, or both the mechanical slider and the slider displayed on
under ‘medium’ or ‘high’ restructured conditions the computer monitor appeared initially in the center
(with a value of 127.5). This was ensured by requiring
Materials participants to return the slider to its center point before
The average song length was 3 min and 11 s, ranging beginning the next hearing (albeit within a tolerance of
from two min and 50 s to three min and 54 s. The 2 points).
Audacity 1.3 Beta program was used to divide and rear-
range the song sections for medium and high Results and Discussion
restructured conditions.
Overall Liking Ratings
Procedure After the 1st (Session 1) and 6th (Session 2) hearings,
All participants listened to the three songs three times participants rated on a 7-point scale their liking for
each in each of the two sessions completed on each of the songs. Mean group ratings for each song in
consecutive days, lasting up to 40 min. For each hearing the intact, medium, and high restructured conditions
participants operated a personal computer custom pro- are displayed in Table 2. We expected that participants’
grammed in Visual Basic for the present study. During liking for the compositions would change over repeated
each hearing participants were instructed to rate their hearings, but that this would depend on whether they
liking for each song on a moment-by-moment basis by heard intact or altered versions of the compositions. In
adjusting the position of a mechanical slider. Following order to test for effects of restructuring over repeated
each hearing, they clicked a ‘continue’ then a ‘play’ but- hearings we performed a three-way mixed ANOVA on
ton to begin the next hearing. Following the 1st and 6th participants’ overall liking ratings, using condition (in-
hearing of each song, an additional display appeared on tact, medium restructured, or high restructured) as a
screen for participants to rate the song. Participants between-subjects factor, and song (Song 1, Song 2, and

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
276 Jonathan J. Rolison & Judy Edworthy

Table 2. Overall Liking and Predictability Ratings in Experiment 2

Song 1 Song 2 Song 3


Session 1 Session 2 Session 1 Session 2 Session 1 Session 2
Overall Liking Rating
Intact 4.79 4.74 3.21 3.11 3.47 2.84
Medium Restructured 4.18 3.89 3.18 3.79 3.59 3.47
High Restructured 5.17 4.63 3.72 3.53 3.11 3.58
Overall Predictability Rating
Intact 5.05 5.26 5.16 5.42 4.21 4.11
Medium Restructured 4.65 5.58 4.29 5.42 4.59 4.89
High Restructured 5.06 4.74 3.94 4.89 4.44 4.63

Song 3) and hearing (Sessions 1 and 2) as repeated Song 1 (mean rating = 148.77) more than Song 2
measures. There was no effect of rearranging song sec- (104.44), F(1, 54) = 24.65, p < .001, and Song 3 (106.18),
tions on participants’ overall liking ratings, F(2, 54) = F(1, 54) = 24.09, p < .001, but rated Song 2 and Song 3
0.60, p = .56, and their liking for the songs did not ap- similarly, F(1, 54) = 0.03, p = .86. This confirms that
pear to change from the 1st to the 2nd session, F(1, 54) = participants’ overall ratings were consistent with those
.45, p = .51. The only significant effect was for song, recorded on a moment-by-moment basis.
F(2, 108) = 11.53, p < .001, and there were no signifi- In contrast with our analysis of overall liking ratings,
cant interactions. Follow-up ANOVAs confirmed that however, there was also an effect of hearing, F(2, 270) =
participants liked Song 1 (mean rating = 4.57) more 4.40, p = .001. A trend analysis confirmed that this effect
than Song 2 (3.42), F(1, 54) = 16.12, p < .001, and Song was cubic, F(1, 54) = 13.57, p = .001, whereby participants’
3 (3.34), F(1, 54) = 21.20, p < .001, but rated Song 2 and liking for the songs decreased from the 1st (127.46) to the
Song 3 similarly, F(1, 54) = 0.07, p = .80. Hence, par- 3rd (115.46) hearing, but returned almost to their initial
ticipants’ overall liking for the songs did not appear to rating during the 4th (122.70) hearing before decreasing
be affected by rearranging the song sections in medium again by the 6th (114.90). This effect is illustrated in Figure
and high restructured conditions, and did not differ 2. Recall that participants heard the songs three times in
from the 1st to the 2nd session. each session. The two sessions were completed on
consecutive days, leaving a gap of an entire day between
Slider Ratings the 3rd hearing (Session 1) and 4th hearing (Session 2). It
While listening to the songs, participants rated continu- appears that while participants found the songs less
ously how much they liked what they were listening to by pleasing over time within each session, their ratings
adjusting a mechanical slider, ranging from 0 to 255 changed little from session 1 to session 2 (see Figure 2).
(mid point = 127.5). Recall that recording participants’ There was also an effect of section, F(7, 378) = 10.87,
likings for songs on a moment-by-moment basis avoids p < .001, in which participants appeared to like the extro
relying on retrospective reports. One possibility, therefore, of the compositions least. However, there were a number
is that participants liked the song sections to differing of interactions involving section, including an interac-
degrees. Hence, we averaged participants’ slider ratings tion between song and section, F(14, 756) = 5.90, p <
for each section within each hearing for each song. We .001, and between song, section, and condition, F(28,
then performed a four-way mixed ANOVA on the average 756) = 1.89, p = .004. We thus conducted a number of
slider ratings for each section using condition (intact, follow-up ANOVAs on slider ratings separately for each
medium restructured, high restructured) as a between- condition. These analyses revealed interactions between
subjects factor and song (Song 1, Song 2, Song 3), hearing song and section in the intact, F(14, 252) = 2.12, p = .012,
(1-6), and song-section (1-8) as repeated measures. As and high restructured condition, F(14, 252) = 6.61, p <
with our analysis of overall liking ratings, there was no .001, but not in the medium restructured condition,
effect of rearranging song section, F(2, 54) = 0.24, p = .79. F(14, 252) = 1.51, p = .11. This suggests that the
However, consistent with our earlier findings there was an interaction between song and section on slider ratings
effect of song, F(2, 108) = 14.75, p < .001. Follow-up was apparent only for the intact and high restructured
ANOVAs confirmed that participants generally liked conditions. For this reason, we then conducted follow-up

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
The Role of Form in Popular Music 277

180

160

140
Mean Liking Rating

120

100
Song 1
80
Song 2
60
Song 3
40

20

0
Hearing 1 Hearing 2 Hearing 3 Hearing 4 Hearing 5 Hearing 6

Session 1 Session 2

FIGURE 2.  Mean group slider ratings for each song across the six hearings in Experiment 2.

analyses separately for each song for these two conditions. hearings. For Song 1 and Song 2, however, some sections
This revealed that the effect of section was significantly followed flatter patterns. Specifically, Verse 2 and Chorus
higher for Song 1 compared with Song 2 and Song 3 in 2 of Song 1 did not appear to jump up during the 4th
both the intact, F(7, 126) = 3.83, p = .002, and F(7, 126) hearing. For Song 2, participants’ liking for the solo ap-
= 2.02, p = .06, respectively, and the high restructured peared to generally increase, whereas their liking for
conditions, F(7, 126) = 5.34, p < .001, and F(7, 126) = Chorus 2, Chorus 3, and the extro appeared relatively flat.
11.45, p < .001. These effects as a whole suggest that par- While participants’ liking for the songs did not appear
ticipants’ ratings for the song sections were generally to differ between each of the sections, one possibility is
more dispersed for Song 1 than for Songs 2 and 3, but that participants were sensitive to transitions between
only in the intact and high restructured conditions. This sections. If their listening is directed more to the
effect appears to be carried largely by participants’ moment-by-moment progress of the songs than to their
ratings for the extro section. This suggests that there may overall structure, then we might expect them to be sensi-
have been something about the extro of Song 1 that tive to those transitions, and also for their responses to
participants particularly disliked. Analysis of this song transitions to differ depending on whether they are hear-
indicates that the extro contained some harsh and hith- ing an intact or restructured version of the song. We
erto unheard harmonies, and ended with a single falling might expect adjustments made to the slider to be more
bass guitar, that participants appeared not to have liked. abrupt as a song moves from one section to another. This
Finally, there were interactions between hearing and is because the listener is likely to have expectations about
section, F(35, 1890) = 1.89, p = .001, and between song, upcoming changes in song section, anticipating a transi-
hearing, and section, F(70, 3780) = 1.31, p = .05. tion. But this may depend on whether songs have been
Follow-up ANOVAs conducted on each song separately structurally altered. When song sections are rearranged
revealed significant interactions between section and in restructured conditions, transitions between sections
hearing for Song 1, F(35, 1890) = 1.43, p = .05, and for may be more surprising to the listener. As an example,
Song 2, F(35, 1890) = 2.32, p < .001, but not for Song 3, plotted in Figure 4 are the mean group moment-
F(35, 1890) = 0.83, p = .75. By viewing Figure 3 it can be by-moment slider ratings recorded every 100 ms across
seen that participants’ ratings for all eight sections of the entire length of Song 1 in the intact and high
Song 3 broadly followed similar (cubic) patterns over restructured conditions on the first hearing. Viewing the

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
278 Jonathan J. Rolison & Judy Edworthy

A B
Song 1 Song 2
180 180
160 160
140 140
Mean Liking Rating

Mean Liking Rating


120 120
100 100
80 80
60 60

40 40

20 20

0 0
Hearing 1 Hearing 2 Hearing 3 Hearing 4 Hearing 5 Hearing 6 Hearing 1 Hearing 2 Hearing 3 Hearing 4 Hearing 5 Hearing 6
Session 1 Session 2 Session 1 Session 2

C
Song 3
180
160
140 Intro
Mean Liking Rating

120 Verse 1
100 Chorus 1
80 Verse 2

60 Chorus 2

40 Solo
Chorus 3
20
Extro
0
Hearing 1 Hearing 2 Hearing 3 Hearing 4 Hearing 5 Hearing 6
Session 1 Session 2

FIGURE 3. Interaction effect between song, hearing, and section on slider ratings in Experiment 2.

transitions between each section (marked by the vertical suggest that adjustments to the slider are more abrupt
bars) provides some preliminary support that slider ad- during a transition than before a transition. We con-
justments were more abrupt during transitions between ducted the following analysis only on the 1st hearing of
sections. This appears most apparent after Verse 2, the each song in the three conditions. This is because par-
solo, and Chorus 3 in the intact version of the song and ticipants’ reactions to changes in sections should be most
after the extro, the solo, and Chorus 3 in its highly abrupt when the songs are still relatively unfamiliar. We
restructured form (See Figure 4). reversed the sign of negative values for this analysis in
In order to examine participants’ responses in more order to investigate overall change, rather than its direc-
detail, we compared average slider ratings 200 ms before tion. We then conducted a three-way mixed ANOVA on
a transition into a new section with their average ratings difference scores, using condition as a between-subjects
200 ms after a transition. This provides a measure of factor, and song and type of change (before transition
participants’ reaction to the transition from one section and after transition) as repeated measures. This analysis
to another. The crucial test is whether this difference is yielded some interesting results. While there were no
larger for adjustments made in the 200 ms after a transi- significant main effects, there was an interaction between
tion than for the preceding 200 ms. If adjustments to the type of change and condition, F(2, 52) = 4.70, p = .01.
slider are larger between sections, then this would Follow-up ANOVAs conducted on each condition

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
The Role of Form in Popular Music 279

Intact
250

200
Mean Liking Rating

150

100

50

0
Intro Verse 1 Chorus 1 Verse 2 Chorus 2 Solo Chorus 3 Extro

High Restructured
250

200
Mean Liking Rating

150

100

50

0
Extro Chorus 3 Verse 2 Solo Chorus 1 Verse 1 Chorus 2 Intro
FIGURE 4. Overall mean slider ratings for the first hearing of Song 1 for intact and high restructured conditions.

separately revealed that while average adjustments to the seems to be entirely eliminated when the order of
slider were significantly more abrupt during transitions sections is rearranged from their intended order.
(mean group adjustment = 1.02) than before a transition Participants’ sensitivity to transitions between sections
(0.67) for intact songs, F(1, 18) = 6.26, p = .02, this was for intact songs must occur within a strikingly short tem-
not the case for restructured songs. Instead, for both poral window. In the above analysis, we compared slider
medium and high restructured songs, adjustments to the ratings that were made just 200 ms before and after a
slider before a transition were similar (0.96 and 0.43, for section change. This would seem too early for the listener
medium and high restructured groups respectively) to to consciously reflect on changes in music, and far too
the adjustments made to the slider during transitions early for the individual to begin to reflect on their liking
(0.86 and 0.45), F(1, 17) = 1.08, p = .31, and F(1, 17) = for the new section. Instead, what these results seem to
0.10, p = .75, respectively. This indicates that intact and imply is a knee jerk reaction to transitions between
restructured songs differed in the way that participants sections for intact songs and a clear focus on the immedi-
responded to transitions between sections. The abrupt ate thrust of the song. Intriguingly, this reaction is
changes that participants made to the slider as they eliminated by rearranging their sections in restructured
entered a new section of the songs in their intact form songs. Hence, at some level of processing people do appear

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
280 Jonathan J. Rolison & Judy Edworthy

sensitive to violations in song structure because they are at least on participants’ ratings of predictability. However,
able, in a very short period of time, to respond to the again there was no effect of restructuring, F(2, 54) = 0.63,
change in section and to begin to offer an appraisal of how p = .54, which was not close to significant. This is particu-
much they like it. One possibility is that during the build larly surprising in the high restructured condition where
up to a change in song section the individual anticipates songs were severely restructured.
the change and prepares to make an adjustment to the Consistent with our analysis of liking ratings, there
slider. When their expectations about the upcoming was a significant effect of song, F(2, 108) = 5.02, p = .008.
change in section are consistent with the structure of the Follow-up ANOVAs confirmed that while participants
song, as when the song is intact, they react to the new rated Song 1 and Song 2 as equally predictable, F(1, 54) =
section. When the sections of the song are rearranged, 1.09, p = .30, they rated Song 1 as more predictable than
however, the listener can no longer anticipate which section Song 3, F(1, 54) = 12.11, p = .001, and Song 2 as margin-
will follow. This would appear to suggest that at some ally more predictable than Song 3, F(1, 54) = 3.70,
perceptual level people are responsive to restructured music p = .06. This pattern of results is broadly consistent with
when this violates their expectations about song structure. our analysis of participants’ liking for the songs. It
It also supports the view that while they are sensitive to appears that participants generally liked more predict-
some degree to the overall structure, they are focused on able songs. However, there was also an effect of hearing,
the moment-by-moment exposition of the song. F(1, 54) = 13.95, p <.001, and an interaction between
condition and hearing, F(2, 54) = 3.64, p = .03. Follow-up
Overall Predictability Ratings ANOVAs indicated that while participants rated the
As well as rating their overall liking for the songs, partici- songs as more predictable after the 6th hearing (5.30)
pants also rated whether they followed a predictable than the 1st hearing (4.51) in the medium restructured
structure on a 7-point scale after the 1st (Session 1) and 6th condition, F(1, 18) = 21.22, p < .001, this was not the case
hearing (Session 2). Mean group predictability ratings are for the intact, F(1, 18) = 0.51, p = .48, and high restruc-
displayed in Table 2. In our analysis of participants’ tured, F(1, 18) = 1.78, p = .20, conditions.
ratings, we performed a three-way mixed ANOVA, this This effect can be seen in Figure 5, and appears to
time replacing participants’ overall liking ratings with provide some support for our hypothesis that restructured
their predictability ratings as the dependent variable. music would initially be perceived as less predictable.
Rearranging song sections should have had a strong effect However, two-way mixed ANOVAs comparing

5.4

5.2
Overall Predictability Rating

5.0

4.8
Intact

4.6 Medium Restructured

High Restructured
4.4

4.2

4.0
Session 1 Session 2

FIGURE 5. Interaction effect between condition and hearing on predictability ratings in Experiment 2.

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
The Role of Form in Popular Music 281

participants’ ratings between the three groups in Session evidence we obtained from the analysis of the
1 indicated that differences in predictability between the boundaries between changes in sections (the transi-
intact (mean rating = 4.81) and medium restructured tions) also indicated that at some level the overall form
(4.51), F(1, 37) = 1.30, p = .26, and between intact and of the song, at least at the structural boundaries,
high (4.48), F(1, 36) = 0.99, p = .33, restructured condi- influences their responses. Overall, however, listeners
tions were small and insignificant. Participants did, showed no preference for intact rather than restruc-
however, rate medium restructured songs as more tured songs. Taken as a whole the results point towards
predictable (5.30) than high restructured songs (4.75), a model of listening to music of this sort that is largely
F(1, 37) = 4.67, p = .04, but not intact songs (4.93), local and in the moment, but which can be influenced
F(1, 37) = 1.63, p = .21, in Session 2. Recall, however, that by overall form and structure. Whether the influence of
participants’ overall liking ratings did not appear to the overall structure is due to the listener maintaining a
follow a similar pattern. Hence, any effects of restructur- mental representation of what that form should be, or
ing popular music on perceptions of liking and is a result of the vagaries of taking songs composed
predictability appear to be small and inconsistent at best. according to a particular form and subsequently
The results demonstrate that violating the structure of violating them, is a topic for research in the future.
songs with clearly discernable sections has no substantial Structure appears to be fundamental to music
effects on people’s perceptual experiences of those songs. composition (Cohn & Dempster, 1992; Epstein, 1979;
This finding is perhaps more surprising when we Nadeau & Tesson, 1968). Despite this, people appear
consider the results of Experiment 1. We found that par- largely insensitive to rearranging segments within a
ticipants’ expectations about the order of sections in composition (e.g., Tillman & Bigand, 1996). One would
songs were broadly consistent with their true structure. expect that rearranging the order of musical segments
However, restructuring the songs and thus violating should affect people’s perceptions of liking and predict-
people’s expectations about the songs did not appear to ability if form is important for composition. However,
influence people’s perceptions of either predictability or previous studies have often used excerpts from composi-
liking for them. tions, which are then divided into arbitrary segments. It
Our results also demonstrate that immediate responses seemed likely to us that such methods may have only
to transitions across sections are made very quickly when subtle effects on the overall coherence and structure of a
songs are intact, but not when they are restructured. composition, which are perhaps not easily perceived by
Listeners are able very quickly to adjust their rating of a the listener. In Experiment 2, by contrast, our
new section as it begins in the intact version, but in the rearrangements were based on coherent formal units. To
restructured version they appear to not be able to make our surprise, but in a similar vein to established findings
such an immediate response. This is somewhat counter- in classical and romantic music, this restructuring
intuitive as we would expect a transition to an unex- appeared to have no effect on people’s liking for
pected section to be met with almost immediate compositions and their ratings of structural predictability.
disapproval. This appears not to be the case. Transitions The present findings suggest that untrained listeners
to unexpected sections seems to lead to momentary are less sensitive to disruptions in musical form than one
indifference and/or inability to judge that new section. might expect. One possibility is that people are simply
This suggests both that listeners are attending to the insensitive to form in music, or at the very least are less
moment-by-moment structure of the music (because concerned with overall form than they are with the
they offer an opinion so quickly in the intact version) moment-by-moment progress of the song. Our findings
but are also attentive, in some way, to the anticipated suggest that while listeners do have some sensitivity to
structure of the songs. overall form, this seems not to affect either their liking
for a song or their judgments of a song’s predictability. In
General Discussion Experiment 2 we found that participants generally
preferred songs that they rated as more structurally
These studies demonstrate that while in principle lis- predictable. This would seem to suggest that form does
teners are sensitive to the form of the songs used, and influence people’s perceptions. The results of
understand how they should proceed, they are relatively Experiment 2 provide some suggestion that people are
impervious to the effects of alterations in that form sensitive to restructuring when this violates their
when they listen to a song in its entirety. Their liking expectations about transitions between sections. One
and predictability ratings of those songs are much the possibility is that by disrupting the sequence of sections
same whether the song is intact or restructured. The in a song people’s expectations about song form are

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
282 Jonathan J. Rolison & Judy Edworthy

reduced. Hence, rather than influencing their perceptions of text comprehension have found that we are strikingly
of form, violating people’s expectations simply causes tolerant of such disruptions in coherence (Lehman &
them to think less about the form of the song. A further Schraw, 2002).
possibility is that listeners are not sensitive to the form of The use of concurrent rating tasks has become a popular
a song per se, but are responsive to the vagaries of form method for studying people’s moment-by-moment
that restructuring a song will have on its moment- emotional experiences while listening to music (Gregory,
by-moment progress. For example, a song in which an 1995; Krumhansl, 1997). For example, Sloboda and
intro is followed by a late chorus (due to restructuring) L e h m a n n ( 2 0 0 1 ) f o u n d t h a t p a r t i c i p a n t s’
is likely to produce a large change in loudness and change moment-by-moment ratings of emotionality reflected
in instrumentation, or result in some other important changes in loudness and timing of a piano composition.
and unexpected musical contrast. A song that has been In Experiment 2, we instead measured people’s concurrent
conceived as an intro followed by a chorus is likely to flow liking for compositions. While participants’ ratings
more smoothly from one section to another and therefore appeared to be generally independent of the sections
be less disturbing for the listener. Thus our findings may within a composition, their concurrent ratings did corre-
not be so much about the form of popular music per se spond with their overall ratings made retrospectively.
but more about disruption to the form intended by the Participants were told to rate their liking for each part of
songwriter. Our experiments do not allow this important the compositions as they heard them. However, it is
distinction to be made, but is a topic that seems worthy possible that their concurrent ratings instead reflected their
of future research and would necessitate the rerecording overall liking for each song, though at a finer level. We
of songs in new structural formats. More drastic restruc- expect that if people are exposed to individual sections of
turing of songs should also be investigated. In the studies a composition in isolation, rather than as part of a
presented here, we did not investigate restructured composition, their ratings may differ more between
versions where, for example, two verses or two choruses sections. This is a topic worthy of future investigation.
were heard in succession. Studies using more drastically We also investigated whether people’s liking for music
restructured versions would therefore be worthy of changed over time, but our findings are not in line with
investigation. those typically found in repeated exposure listening stud-
Interpreting our findings in terms of restructuring ies. They do however inform some potentially important
forcing listeners to focus on local rather than structural methodological issues. In Experiment 2, the overall liking
features seems compatible with studies of restructuring ratings did not indicate any change in liking with repeated
using nonmusical stimuli. For example, when the parts exposure but the data obtained from the slider ratings
of a face (such as the nose and mouth) are rearranged, indicated that there was a change in response with
recognition for individual parts is better when they are repeated exposure. Participants liked the songs less with
presented in isolation than when they are displayed as repeated hearings within each session, but rated the songs
part of the whole face (Collishaw & Hole, 2000; Tanaka similarly across the two sessions, so that they liked the
& Farah, 1993). The reverse is true for intact faces, where songs more on the first than the third hearing in each
parts are instead more easily recognized in the whole listening session. An obvious explanation is that
face. What this suggests is that by scrambling the features participants in Experiment 2 were frustrated or bored by
of a face people attend more to their individual parts hearing the same compositions repeatedly in a single
than the face as a whole. In terms of our findings, when sitting. It is well acknowledged that the “mere exposure”
song sections are rearranged people may attend more to effect, whereby a complex stimulus appears more pleas-
the individual sections than the overall structure of the ing with repeated exposure, is enhanced when a delay
song as a whole. Viewed in this way, disrupting the struc- separates exposure and ratings of a stimulus (Bornstein,
ture of a composition would have little effect on people’s 1989). Exposure effects are likely to be a function of
perceptions of predictability and liking for music, which people’s explicit memory for the stimulus, whereby
our findings seem to have shown. People may not be recognition of a stimulus can actually abate the effects of
insensitive to structure, but may instead simply attend exposure on liking (Bornstein & D’Agostino, 1992;
less to overall coherence when song sections are rear- Szpunar et al., 2004). Songs heard more than once within
ranged. Moreover, people seem quite resilient to breaks a session may appear too familiar, causing participants’
in continuity, such as those imposed by reordering song liking ratings to drop. Hence, overexposure to a musical
sections. For instance, when sentences within a written composition may satiate individuals’ interest. Other data
story are rearranged, one may expect that this would (Edworthy & Rolison, 2011) demonstrates that liking of
disrupt our understanding of the text. However, studies a popular song can increase with repeated exposure when

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
The Role of Form in Popular Music 283

the repetitions of the song are spaced out over weeks, were exposed to a fixed listening schedule. If one chooses
even when the song is restructured as in the studies when to hear a song again, or the order in which a set of
reported here. Disentangling the effects of repeated songs are heard, this may moderate the effects of over-
exposure from aesthetic preference, listening regime, and exposure. This corresponds to the distinction between
restructuring is a sizeable topic for future research. listening to the radio or listening to one’s stereo. If
Our results also demonstrate an effect of repeated ex- overexposure to music is moderated by choice in
posure on liking only for the moment-by-moment listening schedule, the relation between enjoyment and
tracking data, suggesting that this methodology might familiarity becomes a dynamic process that is controlled
tap into the processes at work more readily than overall by the listener.
liking ratings. These moment-by-moment ratings also
give us more detail on the aesthetic judgement of Author Note
sections; for example, our data show that one particular
section of one song (the extro from Song 1) was disliked Jonathan Rolison and Judy Edworthy contributed equally
more than any other section of the song. Thus, using this to this work. The work reported in this article was sup-
methodology should give more insight into the detailed ported by the UK Economic and Social Research Council
aesthetic responses that might be at the heart of broader (ESRC) grant RES-000-22-3783. Jonathan J. Rolison
aesthetic judgements. For example, we do not know how would like to thank Swiss&Global-Ca’ Foscari Foundation
people’s liking for a song is distributed across the song, for its support.
and our methodology can be used to tap into this. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed
An intriguing question is whether overexposure can to Jonathan J Rolison, School of Psychology, University
satiate our interest in a song when we instead choose our of Plymouth, Plymouth PL4 8AA, UK. e-mail: jonathan.
own listening regime. In the present studies participants rolison@plymouth.ac.uk

References

Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics in psychobiology. New York: Deliège, I., Melen, M., Stammers, D., & Cross, I. (1996).
Appleton-Century-Crofts. Musical schemata in real-time listening to music. Music
Bonds, M. E. (1991) Wordless rhetoric: Musical form and the meta- Perception, 14, 117-160.
phor of the oration. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press Edworthy, J., & Rolison, J (2011). The effect of repeated exposure
Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and on liking for popular song. Manuscript in preparation.
meta-analysis of research, 1968-1987. Psychological Review, Eitan, Z., & Granot, R.Y. (2008). Growing oranges on Mozart’s
106, 265-289. apple tree: ‘Inner form’ and aesthetic judgement. Music
Bornstein, R. F., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1992). Stimulus recogni- Perception, 25, 397-417.
tion and the mere exposure effect. Journal of Personality and Epstein, D. (1979). Beyond Orpheus: Studies in musical structure.
Social Psychology, 63, 545-522. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
BPI Statistical Handbook. (2009). British Phonographic Everett, W. (2009). The foundations of rock. Oxford, UK:
Industry Music Theory Spectrum, 5, 15-38. Oxford University Press
Carpenter, P. (1983) Grundgestalt as tonal function. Music Gotlieb, H., & Koneçni, V. J. (1985). The effects of instrumenta-
Theory Spectrum, 5, 15-38. tion, playing style, and structure in the Goldberg Variations
Cohn, R., & Dempster, D. (1992). Hierarchical unity, plural of Johann Sebastian Bach. Music Perception, 3, 87-102.
unities. In K. Bergeron, & P. Bohlman (Eds.), Disciplining Gregory, D. (1995). The continuous response digital interface: An
music: Musicology and its canons (pp. 156-181). Chicago, IL: analysis of reliability measures. Psychomusicology, 14, 197-208.
University of Chicago Press. Gurney, E. (1966). The power of sound. New York: Basic Books.
Collishaw, S. M., & Hole, G. J. (2000). Featural and configura- (Original work published 1966)
tional processes in the recognition of faces of different Hargreaves, D. J. (1984). The effects of repetition on liking for
familiarity. Perception, 29, 893-909. music. Journal of Research in Music Education, 32, 35-47.
Covach, J. (2005). Form in rock music. In D. Stein (Ed.), Heyduk, R. G. (1975). Rated preference for musical composi-
Engaging music (pp. 65-76). Oxford, UK: Oxford University tions as it relates to complexity and exposure frequency.
Press Perception and Psychophysics, 17, 84-90.
Deliège, I (1987). Grouping conditions in listening to music: Karno, M., & Koneçni, V. J. (1992). The effects of structural
An approach to Lerdahl & Jackendoff ’s grouping preference interventions in the first movement of Mozart’s Symphony in G
rules. Music Perception, 4, 325-359. Minor K. 550 on aesthetic preference. Music Perception, 10, 63-72.

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
284 Jonathan J. Rolison & Judy Edworthy

Koneçni, V. J. (1984). Elusive effects of artists’ “messages.” In Smith, K. C., & Cuddy, L. L. (1986). The pleasingness of
W. R. Crozier & A. J. Chapman (Eds.), Cognitive processes in melodic sequences: Contrasting effects of repetition and
the perception of art (pp. 71-93). Amsterdam: North-Holland. rule-familiarity. Psychology of Music, 14, 17-32.
Krumhansl, C. L. (1997). An exploratory study of musical emo- Stephenson, K. (2002). What to listen for in rock. New Haven,
tions and psychophysiology. Canadian Journal of Psychology, CT: Yale University Press.
5, 336-352 Szpunar, K. K., Schellenberg, E. G., & Pliner, P. (2004). Liking
Lehman, S., & Schraw, G. (2002). Effects of coherence and and memory for musical stimuli as a function of exposure.
relevance on shallow and deep text processing. Journal of Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Educational Psychology, 94, 738-750. Cognition, 30, 370-381.
Levinson, J. (1997). Music in the moment. Ithaca, NY: Cornell Tan, S. L., & Spackman, M. P. (2005). Listeners’ judgments of
University Press. the musical unity of structurally altered and intact musical
Nadeau, R., & Tesson, W. (1968). Music for the listener. Boston, compositions. Psychology of Music, 33, 133-153.
MA: Allyn & Bacon. Tan, S. L., Spackman, M. P., & Peaslee, C. L. (2006). The effects
North, A. C., & Hargreaves, D. J. (1995). Subjective complexity, of repeated exposure on liking and judgments of musical
familiarity, and liking for popular music. Psychomusicology, unity of intact and patchwork compositions. Music
14, 77-93. Perception, 23, 407-421.
Peretz, I., Gaudreau, D., & Bonnel, A-M. (1998). Exposure Tanaka, J. W., & Farah, M. J. (1993). Parts and wholes in face recog-
effects on music preferences and recognition. Memory and nition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 225-245.
Cognition, 26, 884-902. Tillmann, B., & Bigand, E. (1996). Does formal structure affect
Pollard-Gott, L. (1983). Emergence of thematic concepts in perception of musical expressiveness? Psychology of Music, 24, 3-16.
repeated listening to music. Cognitive Psychology, 15, Tillmann, B., Bharucha, J. J., & Bigand, E. (2000). Implicit
66-94. learning of tonality: A self-organizing approach. Psychological
Reti, R. (1951). The thematic process in music. New York: Da Review, 107, 885-913.
Capo. Tillmann, B., & Lebrun-Guillaud, G. (2006). Influence of tonal and
Sloboda, J. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (2001). Tracking performance temporal expectations on chord processing and on completion
correlates of changes in perceived intensity of emotion during judgments of chord sequences. Psychological Research, 70, 345-358.
different interpretations of a Chopin piano prelude. Music Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal
Perception, 19, 87-120. of Personality and Social Psychology Monographs, 9, 1-27.

This content downloaded from 146.96.29.49 on Tue, 05 Jun 2018 18:08:37 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi