Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 13

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3668-9

ORIGINAL PAPER

Training Physical Therapists in Early ASD Screening


Ayelet Ben‑Sasson1 · Osnat Atun‑Einy2 · Gal Yahav‑Jonas3 · Shimona Lev‑On4 · Tali Gev5

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
Physical therapists (PTs) are often one of the first professionals to evaluate children at risk. To examine the effect of an early
screening training on pediatric PTs’: (1) knowledge of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), (2) clinical self-efficacy, and (3)
identification of markers. Twenty-six PTs participated in a 2-day “Early ASD Screening” workshop. The ASD Knowledge
and Self-Efficacy Questionnaire, and video case study analysis were completed pre- and post-training. Changes following
training were significant for ASD knowledge related to etiology and learning performance, early signs, risk factors, and
clinical self-efficacy. Rating the videoed case study after the training, was significantly more accurate than it was before.
Training PTs is important for enhancing early identification of ASD.

Keywords  Early screening · ASD · Physical therapy · Training · Knowledge · Self-efficacy · Healthcare providers

Introduction of ASD are highly dependent on the knowledge and skills of


early childhood professionals. However, unique challenges
There is consensus regarding the high priority for early exist in implementing current diagnostic guidelines for
identification of children with autism spectrum disorder ASD for very young children because there are no biologi-
(ASD; Baghdadli et al. 2003; Barbaro et al. 2011; Corsello cal markers for ASD and experienced clinicians are lacking
2005; Pierce et al. 2016; Yeargin-Allsopp et al. 2003). Early (Zwaigenbaum et al. 2009).
screening and diagnosis are critical for obtaining adequate, Pediatric healthcare professionals, such as pediat-
early, specialized interventions that promote better outcomes ric physical therapists (PTs), are in a position to closely
for children with ASD (Dawson et al. 2010; Harris and Han- observe a child’s development, offering an opportunity to
dleman 2000; Sallows and Graupner 2005; Szatmari et al. detect young children at-risk for ASD. It is well-recog-
2003; Rogers and Vismara 2008). Early screening may also nized that early screening and diagnosis of children with
shorten the duration of uncertainty families often encounter ASD pose specific challenges. Among these are the reli-
in the process of obtaining a diagnosis for their child (Sip- ance on behavioral observations, typical variations in com-
erstein and Volkmar 2004). Early screening and diagnosis munication development, the heterogeneous early mani-
festations and developmental trajectories of ASD, and the
instability of early diagnosis (Johnson and Myers 2007;
Electronic supplementary material  The online version of this Zwaigenbaum et al. 2009). The regressive type of ASD
article (https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​3-018-3668-9) contains
supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

1
* Ayelet Ben‑Sasson Department of Occupational Therapy, University of Haifa,
asasson@univ.haifa.ac.il 3498838 Haifa, Israel
2
Osnat Atun‑Einy Department of Physical Therapy, University of Haifa,
osnat.atuneiny@gmail.com 3498838 Haifa, Israel
3
Gal Yahav‑Jonas Association for Children at Risk, 9 Hazvi St.,
gal.yahav@gmail.com Tel Aviv 67197, Israel
4
Shimona Lev‑On Weinberg Child Development Center, Sheba Tel-Hashomer
shimonalev@gmail.com Hospital, Ramat Gan 52621, Israel
5
Tali Gev Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University,
gevtali@gmail.com Ramat Gan 5290002, Israel

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

(Siperstein and Volkmar 2004) is an example of the hetero- and communication would allow PTs to identify children
geneous trajectory which can confound screening. There- at-risk for ASD.
fore, ASD screening is a complex process which requires
professionals to conduct ongoing observations for early ASD Knowledge Among Healthcare Professionals
markers and to prompt parental involvement (Johnson
and Myers 2007). Pediatric professionals should undergo One of the barriers to screening is lack of knowledge in the
training in order to become familiar with the signs and area of ASD among healthcare providers (Self et al. 2010).
symptoms of ASD (Dillenburger et al. 2016; Filipek et al. Studies from around the world indicate a lack of ASD-spe-
1999; Janvier et al. 2016). cific knowledge among pediatric allied healthcare profes-
Pediatric PTs are uniquely positioned to conduct ASD sionals including SLPs, OTs and psychologists (Dillenburger
screening for young children. They are often one of the first et al. 2016; Imran et al. 2011; Schwartz and Drager 2008).
healthcare professionals to treat children at developmental Healthcare professionals often have outdated beliefs regard-
risk and may be the first professional to whom parents raise ing the etiology of the disorder, as well as its social, emo-
concerns regarding social-communication development. In tional and cognitive aspects (Heidgerken et al. 2005; Imran
addition, PTs treat children who have an increased likeli- et al. 2011).
hood for ASD, as they specialize in assessing and treating Moreover, knowledge regarding early signs of ASD in
motor delays and young children at developmental risk, toddlers is lacking among pediatricians (Crais et al. 2014;
including those born preterm. PTs are likely to encounter Heidgerken et al. 2005), with only 8% using an ASD-specific
children with ASD because motor delays are characteristic measure, mostly relying upon informal assessments (Dosreis
of some infants and toddlers who are eventually diagnosed et al. 2006; Sices et al. 2003). A recent survey of providers,
with ASD (Brisson et al. 2012; Bhat et al. 2011, 2012; Landa including PTs OTs and SLPs, indicated a need to increase
and Garrett-Mayer 2006; Ozonoff et al. 2008; Provost et al. knowledge of early signs of ASD in order to aid early recog-
2007; West 2018). Research suggests that developmental nition and diagnosis (Dillenburger et al. 2016). Knowledge
motor delays during early childhood may represent a poten- of early signs was low among OTs, SLPs and PTs based on
tial early marker for ASD before more diagnostically specific the same questionnaire implemented in the current study,
signs develop (Harris 2017; Lemcke et al. 2013; Sacrey et al. regardless of profession and experience level (Atun-Einy and
2015). Studies that examined motor development of infant Ben-Sasson 2018). This underscores the need for training
siblings of children with ASD, found that a significantly in this area.
greater proportion of siblings showed delayed motor per-
formance (Bhat et al. 2012; LeBarton and Iverson 2016). Existing ASD Training Programs for Healthcare
Very low birth weight infants later diagnosed with ASD Professionals
showed less midline head position than typically develop-
ing children (Gima et al. 2018). Preterm infants, who fre- The importance of early ASD diagnosis has led to programs
quently receive PT are at increased risk for ASD (Goyen and for imparting ASD knowledge to doctors and medical resi-
Lui 2002; Johnson et al. 2010; Limperopoulos et al. 2008). dents (e.g., Elmensdorp 2011) and raising awareness of ASD
Although motor developmental delays and premature birth in the general population (Peacock and Lin 2012). The pro-
are not specific to ASD (Harris 2017; Ozonoff et al. 2008; grams focus on various topics and target different audiences,
Provost et al. 2007), their co-occurrence suggests increased including using specific ASD screening tools (e.g., Honig-
ASD risk among children undergoing PT. feld et al. 2012; Kobak et al. 2011), integrating general ASD
The ongoing therapeutic interaction also enables the PT screening at the age of 1 year (Pierce et al. 2011), raising
to gather and analyze a representative sample of the child’s public awareness of ASD (Daniel et al. 2009; Peacock and
social behavior over time. This is of particular value given Lin 2012), and changing misconceptions about the condition
the challenge of screening infants who have an inhibited (e.g., Clark et al. 2016; Major et al. 2013‫)‏‬.
temperament and for discerning communication problems of Training programs that focused on the use of specific
varying etiologies. However, basic training for PTs does not ASD screening tools resulted in an increased use of these
focus on ASD screening as a core competency. In a USA- tools (Kobak et al. 2011), as well as high satisfaction rates
based study, PTs reported receiving less academic training among participants (Honigfeld et al. 2012; Kobak et al.
in ASD than occupational therapists (OTs) or speech and 2011; Pierce et al. 2011). For example, physicians partici-
language pathologists (SLPs) do (Self et al. 2010). There pating in a workshop related to the Modified Checklist for
is a call for PTs to expand their clinical knowledge of early Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT; Robins et al. 2001) versus
interventions to cognition, communication, social interac- those who did not, reported greater use of ASD-specific
tions and object interactions (Lobo et al. 2013). Expanding screening tools, identifying ASD at younger ages and rec-
the scope of screening to include the child’s pattern of play ognizing the need for more primary care visits (Kairys

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

and Petrova 2016). Evidence from a 2-day workshop for In light of these needs, the current study was framed as a
pediatricians that included ASD screening tools, indicated pilot designed to develop and to test the efficacy of an ASD-
a rise in the number of cases referred for an ASD diagnostic specific training program for PTs. We focused on changes in:
evaluation, alongside high concurrence rates with diagnostic (1) knowledge and attitudes regarding ASD in general, and
outcomes, and a boost in self-reported clinical confidence early signs of ASD in particular, (2) self-efficacy pertaining
(Swanson et al. 2014). Given the recognized need to com- to ASD detection, and (3) clinical skills in identifying risk
bine the use of ASD screening tools with clinical assess- level, urgency of referral, and ASD markers based on a video
ment (Barbaro et al. 2011), it is important to provide training case study shown at the beginning and end of the training
about early signs of ASD to healthcare professionals. workshop.
Two workshops that addressed early ASD recognition
among healthcare providers had mixed results. One 2.5 h
workshop was delivered to pediatric nurses in Australia. Methods
At its conclusion, the nurses reported a positive change in
their ability to focus on communicative behaviors, and they Procedure
identified a large number of at-risk children (Barbaro et al.
2011). Findings from another workshop were ambiguous. Participants completed a 16-h workshop over 2 days, titled
This workshop targeted physicians, pediatricians and medi- The Early ASD Screening Workshop. The workshop focused
cal residents directly involved in early childhood develop- on early screening of ASD and was designed to support and
mental assessment and taught them about ASD and its early advance PTs’ ability to detect early signs of ASD in pediat-
signs. The training included two sessions, 60 and 30 min ric patients. The training curriculum was developed based
long, and involved presentations and filmed case studies. on current research in the field (see Appendix A for details
Following the workshop there was no difference in the gen- regarding the topics and their evidence base).
eral knowledge and attitudes scores between the trained The workshop dealt with the process of ASD screening,
group and control group, but workshop participants listed early markers, the importance and challenges of screening,
more symptoms of ASD and were better able to identify a as well as specific screening tools and opportunities in PT
lack of social smiling and abnormal interest in game pieces for such screening. It expanded on the following specific
in the filmed case studies. The physicians in the workshop early signs: joint attention, imitation, response to name,
group reported greater confidence in identifying children eye contact, and object exploration (see Appendix A). The
with ASD and in discussing the disorder with parents, as rationale for focusing on these signs related to their per-
well as satisfaction with the amount of training they received sistence in early screening research and the potential to
in the field (Elmensdorp 2011). Allied healthcare profession- identity them at an early age and readily elicit them in the
als have rarely been included in such programs. context of a PT session. The teaching methods in the work-
shop included frontal lectures, group discussions, analyzing
Clinical Self‑Efficacy filmed and written case studies, and role playing. Workshop
format was based on active learning, which combines varied
In addition to knowledge and skills, an important outcome forms of delivery, including “real-life examples” in training
measure of these training programs is clinical self-efficacy. health care professionals (Dillenburger et al. 2016; Menon
Clinical self-efficacy refers to an individual’s beliefs about et al. 2009; Oxman et al. 1995). Lectures were delivered by
their ability to successfully perform a particular behavior pediatric OTs, developmental psychologist who was a spe-
(Bandura 1986). Healthcare professionals’ self-efficacy cialist in ASD and early childhood, while lessons describ-
beliefs have been shown to influence their clinical behavior ing motor development and PT in ASD were delivered by a
(e.g., Godin et al. 2008; Greenhalgh et al. 2004). Evidence specialist in pediatric PT. During the workshop participants
shows that lack of clinical self-efficacy hinders implementa- received handouts, a reference list and links to relevant edu-
tion of clinical knowledge (Godin et al. 2008). cational resources (e.g., https​://www.wellc​hildl​ens.com/).
In our previous work using the questionnaire included The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Uni-
in this study, four types of clinical self-efficacy emerged: in versity of Haifa. Participants were recruited through local
diagnosis and intervention delivery, in communicating with child development centers, pediatric PT clinics, PT gradu-
parents, need for additional training in confidence, and avail- ate programs, professional meetings, professional platforms
able resources for caring for children with ASD. We found a on Facebook, through the Ministry of Health’s Director of
significant correlation between knowledge and self-efficacy Physiotherapy, and personal acquaintance with pediatric
among allied health professionals. Increasing knowledge in PTs. Among 50 therapists who were interested in participat-
ASD may increase their clinical confidence. ing, 39 met the inclusion criteria, 27 of whom registered for

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

the workshop. One participant was excluded from analyses Table 1  Background Information
due to attending only the second day. Variables N (%)
Participants signed a consent form prior to the workshop.
Preceding the workshop, participants were sent the ASD Number of patients with ASD
Knowledge and Self-Efficacy Questionnaire to complete  None 2 (7.7)
and return via mail or e-mail. The same questionnaire was  1–10 Cases 22 (84.6)
completed on site, at the end of the training. The participants  10–20 Cases 2 (7.7)
were asked not to use external sources of information while Referral of children under 3 with an ASD concern in the past year
completing the questionnaire. At the beginning and end of  None 7 (26.9)
the workshop, participants completed a video case study  1–3 11 (42.3)
analysis, and a workshop satisfaction report. After train-  4–6 Children 4 (15.38)
ing was completed, participants were mailed a certificate  Unknown 4 (15.38)
of attendance signed by the National “Child Development Work ­settinga
and Rehabilitation Department” of the Ministry of Health.  Child development center 23 (88.5)
 School 2 (7.7)
Participants  Rehabilitation daycare 4 (15.38)
 Private clinic/other 2 (7.4)
The study included 26 female pediatric PTs. Inclusion Education
criteria were: (1) licensed in PT, (2) experience working  BA 22 (84.6)
with children under the age of 24 months, (3) no previous  MA 3 (11.5)
specialized education in ASD, (4) no experience working  Unknown 1 (3.8)
in a specialized setting for children with ASD, in order to Years of experience with young children
target a group of trainees with similar ASD knowledge and  Up to 1 3 (11.5)
experience. The final sample included 26 participants at an  1–5 3 (11.5)
average age of 43.42 (SD = 10.84, Min–Max = 30–65), with  5–10 7 (26.9)
an average of 14.98 (SD = 9.60, Min–Max = 3–35) years  > 10–15 13 (50)
of experience, who work an average of 25.76 (SD = 9.62, Years of experience in early screening and diagnosis
Min–Max = 4–40) hours per week. Table 1 presents demo-  Up to 5 7 (26.9)
graphics and information on the clinical experience of the  5–10 7 (26.9)
sample.  > 10 12 (46.2)
Family history of ASD 4 (15.4)
Measures a
 Not mutually exclusive categories

ASD Knowledge and Self-Efficacy Questionnaire (Atun-Einy


and Ben-Sasson 2018). This is a self-report survey aimed to (2) Knowledge of ASD early markers (9 items). Rated as
assess health care providers’ knowledge and self-efficacy in in Part 1 and yields an average knowledge score and
the area of ASD in general and in early ASD signs and risk percentage of no knowledge rating. In addition, this
factors in particular. The questionnaire includes 79 items, part includes two open-ended questions asking for a
divided into four sections: description of early ASD markers, and ASD risk fac-
tors.
(1) Knowledge and beliefs about ASD (30 items). Items are (3) Knowledge of diagnostic criteria for ASD (26 items).
rated on a six-point Likert scale or “I have no specific Each item was designed to match one of three catego-
knowledge”. Five mean factor scores are computed: ries: core diagnostic features (10), helpful diagnostic
Misconceptions, Social Affective Symptoms, Interven- features (12), or features irrelevant to the diagnosis (4).
tion, Etiology and Learning Performance, and Related Items are rated as matching one of the categories. Per-
Conditions and Features. In addition, two total scores cent of correct answers is computed for this part.
are calculated for this part—an average score (some (4) Self-efficacy in ASD (14 items). Items are rated from
items are reversed so that higher scores reflect greater ‘1’ highly disagree to ‘4’ highly agree. Four factor
knowledge), and the percentage of items marked “I scores are computed: Self-Efficacy in ASD Clinical
have no specific knowledge”. For calculating mean Skills, Resources, Training, Parent Communication
scores, “I have no knowledge” ratings are transformed (some items are reversed so higher scores reflect higher
into ‘0’ values. self-efficacy).

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

In a previous paper, the psychometric properties of this However, they were included, as the results did not differ
measure were tested with 234 allied healthcare profession- with or without their data.
als (82 PTs, 85 OTs, and 67 SLPs). Findings indicated high Non-parametric, within subject tests were conducted
internal and test–retest reliability, and factor analysis demon- due to the non-normal distribution of most of the variables
strated good construct validity (Atun-Einy and Ben-Sasson and the small sample size. Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were
2018). computed to examine changes in ASD Knowledge and Self-
Efficacy Questionnaire scores from pre- to post-training. To
Case Study Analysis estimate change in scores that exceed test–retest effects,
the reliable change index (RCI; Jacobson and Truax 1991;
The workshop began and ended with participants analyzing Jacobson et al. 1984) was computed for the group and for
the same case study. The case study analysis included view- each participant. The RCI is compared to a Z threshold of
ing a 5-min video of a baby, at ages 10–12 months, during 1.96 to determine its significance. The RCI implies that the
interactions with family members at home and play time. larger the inter-rater reliability, the smaller the change that
The child was later diagnosed with ASD Participants were is needed following intervention. Individual RCI scores
blinded to his later diagnosis. They were asked to rate the enables viewing the direction of individual changes as
baby’s level of risk on three scales: global developmental increased, decreased or stable. RCI could only be computed
delay, ASD, and sensory processing. Risk was rated on a for variables for which test–retest coefficients were available
10-point scale from 1 (no risk) to 10 (very high risk). Par- from prior research with this questionnaire (Atun-Einy and
ticipants were also asked to rate urgency of referral to an Ben-Sasson 2018).
ASD expert from 1 (no need to refer) to 10 (highly urgent The open-ended questions of Part 2 of the questionnaire
to refer). Two open-ended questions asked about early ASD were scored for the number of early signs and risk factors
signs observed, and for additional information they would specific to ASD mentioned, based on categories that were
obtain to verify the diagnosis. pre-defined in a content analysis procedure. The first and
The case study analysis was validated by an expert panel third authors analyzed 63 responses of healthcare providers
of eight clinicians (pediatric PTs, OTs, and a developmen- (outside of this study) to these two questions. Each author
tal psychologist), each with at least 5 years of experience in extracted the number and types of distinct ASD signs and
early ASD identification and intervention. The panel’s rat- risk factors mentioned in answers. The authors conducted
ing of the case study served as a standard to determine the five rounds of discussions regarding disagreements until an
correct answers. A high degree of inter-rater reliability was 85% agreement was reached. In this study, the number of
found among the experts’ scoring (ICC = 0.79, 0.31–0.97 ASD signs and risk factors mentioned pre- and post-training
across the three risk scales). The three types of risk and was based on the presence of the aforementioned catego-
urgency for referral were scored as the difference between ries coded in previous research. Wilcoxon signed-rank tests
the therapist’s answer and the average answer of the experts. compared the number of signs and risk factors mentioned
The two open-ended questions were coded for the number of before and after training. McNemar tests were conducted to
signs and types of information listed relative to the experts’ compare the types of risk factors and signs mentioned before
answers. versus after training.
The case study analysis was scored by creating an abso-
lute difference score between the participants’ and the
Satisfaction Report experts’ rating for the three risk scales and urgency for infor-
mation. Wilcoxon signed-rank tests compared pre- and post-
This survey was designed to measure satisfaction with the training differences. Spearman correlations were conducted
training. Four mean scores were computed based on 23 items to examine relation between change in knowledge, self-
corresponding to the following categories rated on a six- efficacy and skills and therapist’s background information.
point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly disagree, 6 = strongly
agree): (1) novelty of the knowledge gained, (2) clarification
of pre-existing knowledge, (3) clarity of materials, and (4) Results
quality of teaching methods.
Changes in the ASD Knowledge Questionnaire
Data Analysis Scores

Three participants were excluded from analyses of Part 3 of Comparison of the before and after scores in the five fac-
the ASD Knowledge and Self-Efficacy Questionnaire as they tors of Part 1 showed a statistically significant change and
did not complete it. Two participants missed one lecture. a significant mean group RCI score in factor 4: Etiology

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

and Performance, for which 58% participants increased in Changes in Self‑Efficacy


knowledge (see Fig. 1). Table 2 presents the rate of increase
at the individual level for all questionnaire scores. A Wil- Wilcoxon signed-rank tests indicated statistically and RCI
coxon signed-rank test showed a significant increase in the significant increases in the total self-efficacy score. Look-
participants’ mean score for Part 1 ASD Knowledge and a ing at the factor scores, for the Self-efficacy in ASD clinical
decrease in the number of items answered with “I have no skills factor, the individual RCI scores of 62% of participants
specific knowledge” after the training; however, mean RCI increased and for the Need for ASD training factor score 50%
did not reach significance (see Table 2). increased (see Table 2).
For Part 2, Knowledge of ASD Early Markers, a Wil-
coxon signed-rank test showed a significant increase,
Changes in Case Study Analysis
Z = − 4.38, p < 0.001, effect size r = 0.60. Mean group RCI
was significant, with 54% showing an increase in knowl-
A Wilcoxon signed-rank test demonstrated that participants’
edge and 46% showing stability (see Table 2; Fig. 2). For
ratings of degree of three types of risk in a filmed case study
the open-ended question listing early ASD signs in Part 2,
were significantly closer to that of experts after the work-
Wilcoxon signed-rank tests indicated a significant increase
shop (see Table 3). Urgency of obtaining additional infor-
in the number of early signs the participants listed after
mation was also significantly increased between pre- and
the workshop as compared to before, Z = − 3.28, p = 0.001,
post-training. For the open ended answers coded, Wilcoxon
effect size r = 0.46 (Mpre = 4.56, SDpre = 2.53, Medpre = 4 vs.
signed-rank test found significant increase in the number of
Mpost = 7.12, SDpost = 3.53, Medpost = 7). Similarly, there was
ASD symptoms identified (Mpre = 3.04, SDpre = 1.59, Med-
a significant increase in the mean number of risk factors
listed by participants before the workshop [0.60 (SD = 1.08, pre = 3, Mpost = 4.73, SDpost = 2.15, Medpost = 5, Z = − 3.05,
p = 0.002) and in the relevant additional information
Med = 0)]; relative to after [2.56 (SD = 1.55, Med = 2.5),
requested (Mpre = 2.27, SDpre = 2.27, Medpre = 2, Mpost = 4.69,
Z = − 3.73, p < 0.001, effect size r = 0.52]. Next, changes in
SDpost = 2.20, Medpost = 5), Z = − 3.96, p < 0.001.
the types of risk factors mentioned were examined using
McNemar’s test (Fig. 3). This test showed premature birth
(p < 0.001) and genetics (p < 0.001) were mentioned as risk Satisfaction with the Training Program
factors significantly more often. None of the participants
listed parental concerns as a risk factor before the workshop; Among the participants, 25/26 completed the satisfaction
however, 34% listed it after. questionnaire at the end of the second training day. Mean
As for Part 3, Advanced ASD Diagnostic Criteria, a Wil- satisfaction scores in all four categories of the questionnaire
coxon signed-rank test showed a significant increase after were high: (1) novelty of the knowledge gained (M = 5.44,
the workshop in the percentage of correct answers, but the SD = 0.44), (2) refinement of pre-existing knowledge
RCI was not significant (Table 2). (M = 5.72, SD = 0.46), (3) clarity of materials (M = 5.41,

Fig. 1  Part 1. Etiology and


learning performance knowl-
edge factor score pre- and
post-training

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Table 2  Change in ASD Knowledge and Self-Efficacy


Variables Pearsona Pre-test M, Med Post-test M, Med Z Effect size RCI Type of change N (%)
(SD) (SD)
Increase Decrease Same

Part 1: knowledge and beliefs about ASD


 Total mean 0.78 4.08, 4.25 (0.81) 4.71, 4.87 (0.53) − 4.28*** 0.59 1.65 8 (30.77) 0 (0) 18 (69.23)
knowledge
 % of “No specific 0.75 11.12, 5.00 (14.14%) 5.00, 3.33 (7.13%) − 2.26* 0.31 − 0.72 3 (11.50) 0 (0) 23 (88.46)
knowledge”
­ratingb
 Factor 1: miscon- 0.84 5.03, 5.17 (0.81) 5.63, 5.67 (0.27) − 3.55*** 0.49 1.87 9 (34.62) 0 (0) 17 (65.38)
ceptions
 Factor 2: social 0.73 4.64, 4.80 (0.84) 4.89, 5.30 (0.86) − 1.85 0.25 0.55 3 (11.54) 1 (3.84) 22 (84.62)
affective symp-
toms
 Factor 3: inter- 0.85 3.83, 4.20 (1.42) 4.41, 4.80 (1.27) − 1.45 0.23 0.82 7 (26.92) 2 (7.69) 15 (57.69)
vention
 Factor 4: etiology 0.86 3.64, 3.93 (1.08) 4.70, 4.71 (0.68) − 4.26*** 0.59 2.62* 15 (57.69) 0 (0) 11 (42.31)
and perfor-
mance
 Factor 5: related 0.65 3.25, 3.50 (1.24) 3.90, 4 (0.82) − 2.64** 0.36 0.89 4 (15.38) 1 (3.84) 21 (80.77)
conditions and
features
Part 2: ASD early markers
 ASD early mark- 0.76 3.46, 3.83 (1.19) 4.91, 4.94 (0.51) − 4.38*** 0.60 2.50* 14 (53.84) 0 (0) 12 (46.15)
ers
 % of “No specific 0.83 18.38, 11.11 (22.65) 2.14, 0 (6.3) − 3.47** 0.48 − 2.81* 0 (0) 15 (57.70) 11 (42.30)
knowledge”
­ratingb
Part 3: advanced knowledge of ASD diagnostic criteria
 % of correct 0.58 46.60, 50 (19.80) 64.20, 65.38 (12.53) − 3.49*** 0.49 1.37 9 (34.62) 1 (3.84) 16 (61.54)
answers
Part 4: self-efficacy
 Factor 1: self- 0.85 1.95, 2.17 (0.57) 2.39, 2.42 (0.57) − 3.31** 0.48 2.51* 16 (69.57) 2 (8.70) 5 (21.74)
efficacy in  ASD
clinical skills
 Factor 2: 0.77 2.97, 3 (0.63) 3.09, 3 (0.64) − 1.59 0.22 0.49 2 (8.70) 1 (4.35) 19 (82.61)
resources
 Factor 3: training 0.76 1.41, 1.5 (0.44) 1.86, 2 (0.45) − 2.78** 0.40 2.20* 13 (50.00) 1 (4.35) 8 (34.78)
 Factor 4: parent 0.60 2.41, 2.5 (0.69) 2.34, 2.50 (0.72) − 2.87 0.40 − 0.10 3 (13.04) 2 (8.70) 17 (73.91)
communication

*RCI exceeds 1.96


a
 Pearson moment correlations are based on test–retest results reported in Atun-Einy and Ben-Sasson (2018)
b
 This is the only variable for which an increase indicates less knowledge

SD = 0.54), (4) quality of teaching methods (M = 5.67, Discussion


SD = 0.38).
This is the first study to test the efficacy of a workshop aimed
Change and Professional Background at promoting early detection of ASD among PTs. This train-
ing focused on social-communication markers (e.g., joint
Spearman Rho correlations indicated that there were no attention, response to name and social gaze) and sensory-
significant (p > .05) correlations between ASD Knowledge motor markers (e.g., visual disengagement, atypical visual
and Self-Efficacy Questionnaire change scores and years of exploration and gaze, repetitive object play, sensory sensi-
clinical experience, years of experience in the pediatric field, tivity) because they might manifest in a PT session, but are
and number of young children under care. not part of routine PT training. The rationale for this training

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Fig. 2  Part 2. Early signs of 6


ASD knowledge pre- and post-
training
5

Mean
3

2 Pre
Post

0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26

Participant Number

Fig. 3  Percentage reporting in 90
Part 2: each type of risk factor
pre- and post-training 80
70
60
50
40
Pre 30
Post 20

10
0

Table 3  Change in case study rating relative to expert ratings


Variables Experts Pre-training Post-training Statistics Effect size (r)
M (SD) M difference (SD) M difference (SD)

ASD risk 8.87 (1.55) 2.92 (2.03) 1.05 (0.98) − 3.82*** 0.53
Sensory processing risk 7.62 (1.4) 2.65 (2) 1.58 (1.04) − 2.51** 0.34
Global developmental risk 7.57 (1.81) 2.23 (1.71) 1.22 (0.93) − 2.45* 0.34
Urgency of referral 9 (1.92) 3 (2.47) 0.96 (0.82) − 3.61*** 0.50

Rating is out of 10. Mean differences pre and post-training were calculated relative to the mean answer of the experts
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

was to enable PTs to make use of their ongoing clinical Post-training, participants were able to identify additional
encounters with young children who may be at risk for ASD, early ASD signs presented by the child and could list more
for screening. Findings showed that following training, PTs information they would request in a clinical intake for this
significantly increased their knowledge of the etiology of child. ASD risk rating and urgency of referral had a stronger
ASD, its signs and risk factors, and learning performance effect with a decrease in variance than the changes in their
features. Importantly, their ability to identify early markers rating of global developmental risk and sensory risk. As
of ASD and to seek specific information for verifying risk expected, the participants’ answers before the workshop
was increased, based on the analysis of a video case study. regarding global delay and sensory risk were close to those
Furthermore, their perceived clinical self-efficacy increased. of the experts. This demonstrates the skills of PTs in devel-
Nonetheless, at the group level, changes in knowledge opmental risk assessment, and the possibility of extending
regarding diagnostic features, basic social-affective symp- these skills through training to include ASD risk assessment.
toms, interventions, comorbid features, misconceptions and The changes we found in analyzing a case study were not
self-efficacy related to communicating with parents, were observed in Elmensdorp’s (2011) training study. We used
inconsistent. Based on the findings from this pilot study, a more detailed rating scale (10-point scale) as opposed to
we are cautiously optimistic of the potential impact of this the yes/no answer format used in that study. Also, we relied
training and recommend its further investigation. on active learning methods and our participants were pedi-
atric PTs as opposed to pediatricians and medical residents.
Changes in Knowledge and Skills in Early Detection These findings stress the recommendation for incorporat-
ing filmed case studies to bridge the gap between classroom
Increases in knowledge regarding early signs of ASD were and clinical practice (Richardson 1999; Scott et al. 2012).
observed in Part 2 of the questionnaire. The percent of items The case study analysis was the only measure in the current
for which participants reported lack of knowledge and the study which approximated the clinical application of the
variance within the group decreased. Participants demon- knowledge gained. Therefore, these findings are clinically
strated greater awareness of the developmental regression valuable.
type of ASD and risk factors after the workshop. Changes
in clinical reasoning and skills were apparent in the partici- Changes in ASD Knowledge
pants’ analysis of the case study. Studies have shown that
increasing awareness and expanding clinical reasoning can Of the four factors from Part 1 of the questionnaire assess-
promote early detection of ASD (Barbaro et al. 2011; Daniel ing knowledge and attitudes regarding ASD, the greatest
et al. 2009). improvement was evident in the etiology and learning per-
The effect of the training was also evident in the increased formance factor, which was statistically significant, had a
number of early ASD markers and ASD-specific risk factors high effect size, and a high mean RCI. This factor includes
listed by the participants, that would raise their suspicion for seven items, four of which relate to the genetic basis of ASD
ASD. This is similar to previous findings regarding medi- and three to learning performance. The training may have
cal personnel following training (Elmensdorp 2011). Since impacted these types of knowledge by teaching about the
screening is highly dependent upon observation, clinical genetic risk factors and epidemiology, as well as addressing
skills in eliciting and recognizing early markers needs to be performance factors that improve testability of children with
improved, and not only by introducing standardized screen- ASD. In contrast, changes in knowledge related to the other
ing tools. Fewer participants listed social–environmental four factors: interventions, social affective symptoms and
factors (e.g., parental neglect, abuse) after the training, and secondary characteristics were inconsistent. The curriculum
at the same time, more participants listed genetic factors, did not cover interventions applied to children with ASD;
premature birth, and parental concerns as risk-factors. This hence, the lack of change in the intervention factor is not sur-
is consistent with the observed change in knowledge regard- prising. However, we expected improvements in knowledge
ing etiology in Part 1 of the questionnaire, which included regarding social-affective symptoms, as this was part of the
items referring to the genetic basis of ASD such as “ASD has workshop’s core curriculum. It is possible that the question-
a strong genetic basis in its etiology” and “ASD tends to run naire was not sensitive enough to assess knowledge change
in families”. These results suggest that training can increase in this domain, as the workshop dealt with qualities of early
one’s awareness of risk factors coupled with knowledge of social-effective signs as opposed to the basic information
signs in early childhood. In turn, this can promote faster sampled in Part 1 (e.g., children with ASD do not show
and more accurate identification and referral for diagnostic social attachments, even to parents/primary caregivers).
assessment. As for secondary characteristics (e.g., “Most children with
Analysis of a filmed case study before and after train- ASD have eating problems”), the change was significant,
ing indicated an increase in screening competencies. but not high enough to differentiate it from spontaneous

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

change resulting from repeated testing (i.e., the RCI was is needed, specifically in probing about concerns regarding
not significant). the communicative development of their child during diag-
The core diagnostic features of ASD in the DSM-V were nostic testing and treatment.
reviewed briefly in the workshop, to maintain the goal of Another significant result in the self-efficacy part of the
enhancing screening ability rather than diagnostic skills. questionnaire was that 50% of the participants reported a
The average RCI did not show a meaningful change in the need for additional training. Increased knowledge regarding
rate of correctly classified ASD features in Part 3 of the the heterogeneous nature of ASD and the complexities of
questionnaire. However, at the individual level, the rate of screening for ASD may have led to the realization that iden-
correct responses increased among 30% of the participants. tifying and diagnosing ASD requires specific, advanced clin-
In addition, Part 3 may be less sensitive to small differences ical skills and raised the need for additional training. These
in knowledge, similar to previous research indicating that findings agree with research showing that most SLPs, those
Part 3 was not sensitive to differences between experienced with moderate knowledge and those with high self-efficacy,
and non-experienced providers (Atun-Einy and Ben-Sasson expressed a need for additional training (Adam 2014).
2018). The participants reported a high level of satisfaction
with the training program in terms of relevance to daily PT
Changes in Clinical ASD Self‑Efficacy services and in terms of training methods. They reported
acquiring new knowledge and refining prior knowledge.
The self-efficacy part of the questionnaire included four These findings are valuable as this was an initial examina-
factors: clinical self-efficacy, need for additional training, tion of a new training program.
confidence in communication with parents, and available
resources for referring at-risk children. Results showed a Training Change and Participant Professional
significant increase after the workshop in clinical self- Background
efficacy and in the need for additional training. The RCI
in self-confidence regarding diagnosing and treating ASD Changes in knowledge were not associated with clinical or
increased among 69.6% of the participants. Previous stud- diagnostic experience. This indicates that the workshop was
ies also showed increased confidence in identifying children relevant to professionals at different points in their career.
with ASD following ASD training programs geared towards Given the low variance of these professional background
doctors (Elmensdorp 2011) and nurses (Barbaro et al. 2011). variables in our sample and the similarities in their training,
Increasing confidence, although not a sufficient dimension this explanation should be taken with caution. The effect of
for promoting early ASD screening by PTs, is critical for years of experience may have been obscured by the relatively
increasing the likelihood of translating knowledge change recent increase in PTs involvement in ASD treatment (Atun-
into practice. Einy et al. 2013). Thus, PTs in the field for a shorter period
Training did not improve confidence in communicating might have received more exposure to ASD-related materi-
with parents. Overall, the participants reported insufficient als during their education and clinical training. In contrast,
confidence in communicating with parents about ASD. At previous studies found an association between knowledge
the same time, in the open-ended questions, they reported and years of experienced among allied healthcare clinicians,
an increased recognition of parental concerns as an early including those answering the same questionnaire used in
risk factor. Following the workshop, in Part 4, 32.0% of the current study (Adam 2014; Atun-Einy and Ben-Sasson
the participants agreed or strongly agreed that they were 2018). The fact that clinical experience was not related to
worried about hurting parents if they asked them whether PTs knowledge of ASD indicates their low involvement in
they had concerns about ASD, and 62.5% agreed or strongly the treatment of children with ASD and supports the choice
agreed that it was not their role to probe for concerns. After of PTs as the target population for the workshop.
the workshop, 50% of the participants agreed or strongly
agreed that they would feel comfortable or confident discuss- Limitations
ing early warning signs of ASD with parents. A possible
explanation for these results is that the workshop focused on This pilot study had some methodological limitations. The
detecting early signs of ASD, not communication with par- relatively small convenience sample reflects the overall
ents. Furthermore, in our previous research with this ques- small population from which to recruit pediatric PTs. This
tionnaire, we found that the factor of communicating with small sample may have impeded the possibility of observ-
parents was not associated with level of ASD experience and ing change across measures. In terms of study design,
PTs reported much less confidence in communicating with there was no control group or follow-up phase. Changes
parents as compared with SLPs and OTs (Atun-Einy and in clinical recognition of early markers was tested by
Ben-Sasson 2018). Training in parent communication skills having participants rate one videotape, rather than rating

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

multiple cases or actual children. Assessing behavioral Compliance with Ethical Standards 
changes among healthcare clinicians is a complex process,
and the effect of knowledge on behavior can be limited Conflict of interest  All authors declare that they have no conflict of
(Scott et al. 2012). Although some studies found changes interest.
in clinical behavior following training, they focused on Ethical Approval  All procedures performed in studies involving human
the use of specific measures (Kairys and Petrova 2016; participants were in accordance with the Ethical Standards of the
Swanson et al. 2014). The participants in Barbaro et al.’s Institutional and/or National Research Committee and with the 1964
study (2011) reported a change in clinical behavior and Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical
standards.
improved focus while evaluating social-communicative
skills. As opposed to our study, they taught the partici- Informed Consent  Informed consent was obtained from all individual
pants a specific evaluation measure to guide them. We rec- participants included in the study.
ommend that future studies examine short- and long-term
changes in the application of the knowledge in a clinical
setting, to support the carryover of new knowledge into
References
practice.
Adam, Y. (2014). Diagnosis and treatment of ASD: Knowledge, self-
efficacy and attitudes of speech–language pathologists in Israel.
Unpublished Master’s Thesis. University of Tel-Aviv, Israel (in
Hebrew).
Conclusions Atun-Einy, O., & Ben-Sasson, A. (2018). Pediatric allied health-
care professionals’ knowledge and self-efficacy regarding ASD.
This pilot study provides preliminary evidence to the effi- Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 47, 1–13. https​://doi.
cacy of the “Early ASD Screening” workshop for training org/10.1016/j.rasd.2017.12.001.
Atun-Einy, O., Lotan, M., Harel, Y., Shavit, E., Burstein, S., & Kemp-
PTs. This intensive, 2-day, problem-based training was
ner, G. (2013). Physical therapy for young children diagnosed
effective in increasing knowledge regarding early signs, with autism spectrum disorders—Clinical frameworks model
risk factors, and etiology of ASD, as well as improving in an Israeli setting. Frontiers in Pediatrics, 1, 19. https​://doi.
performance and clinical self-efficacy of a group of pediat- org/10.3389/fped.2013.00019​.
Baghdadli, A., Pascal, C., Grisi, S., & Aussilloux, C. (2003). Risk fac-
ric PTs in recognizing ASD-specific behaviors in a young
tors for self-injurious behaviors among 222 young children with
child. Our study points to the possibility of involving PTs, autistic disorders. Journal of Intellectual Disabilities Research,
who are routinely engaged with young children, in the 47, 622–627. https​://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2788.2003.00507​.x.
detection of ASD. These changes were independent of pre- Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
vious experience with diagnosing young children or years
Barbaro, J., Ridgway, L., & Dissanayake, C. (2011). Developmental
of pediatric experience. As for changes in clinical skills, surveillance of infants and toddlers by maternal and child health
when watching a filmed case study, participants showed nurses in an Australian community-based setting: Promoting
greater accuracy in evaluating ASD risk, recognizing ASD the early identification of autism spectrum disorders. Journal
of Pediatric Nursing, 26(4), 334–347. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.
markers, and recognizing additional information needed
pedn.2010.04.007.
to decide whether to refer a child for further evaluation. Bhat, A., Galloway, J., & Landa, R. (2012). Relation between early
Evidence from this study suggests the need for workshops motor delay and later communication delay in infants at risk for
with modules teaching communication with parents and autism. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(4), 838–846. https​
://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbe​h.2012.07.019.
early intervention techniques. Future research, studying
‫‏‬Bhat, A. N., Landa, R. J., & Galloway, J. C. (2011). Current perspec-
the efficacy of this training for other professions and its tives on motor functioning in infants, children, and adults with
carryover to practice would be beneficial. autism spectrum disorders. Physical Therapy, 91(7), 1116–1129.
https​://doi.org/10.2522/ptj.20100​294.
Acknowledgments  We wish to thank Mrs. Malka Stoller, Head of Pedi- Brisson, J., Warreyn, P., Serres, J., Foussier, S., & Adrien-Louis, J.
atric Physical Therapy Services, the Ministry of Health, Israel and Mrs. (2012). Motor anticipation failure in infants with autism: A retro-
Danya Hofi head of the Physical Therapy School in Tel-Hashomer, Tel- spective analysis of feeding situations. Autism: The International
Aviv University. This study was part of the third author’s master thesis. Journal of Research and Practice, 16(4), 420–429. https​://doi.
org/10.1177/13623​61311​42338​5.
Clark, A., Browne, S., Boardman, L., Hewitt, L., & Light, S. (2016).
Author Contributions  AB and OA have made substantial contributions
Implementing UK Autism Policy and National Institute for Health
to conception, design, acquisition of data, analysis, and manuscript
and Care Excellence guidance—Assessing the impact of Autism
writing. GY was involved in the design and coordination of the work-
training for frontline staff in community learning disabilities
shop, data management, analysis and helped draft the manuscript. SL
teams. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44(2), 103–110.
and TG participated in the acquisition of the data by delivering parts
https​://doi.org/10.1111/bld.12116​.
of the workshop and in revising the manuscript. All authors read and
Corsello, C. M. (2005). Early intervention in autism. Infants and Young
approved the final manuscript.
Children, 18(2), 74–85. https​://doi.org/10.1097/00001​163-20050​
4000-00002​.

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Crais, E. R., McComish, C. S., Humphreys, B. P., Watson, L. R., Imran, N., Chaudry, M. R., Azeem, M. W., Bhatti, M. R., Choudhary,
Baranek, G. T., Reznick, J. S., Christian, R. B., & Earls, M. Z. I., & Cheema, M. A. (2011). A survey of Autism knowledge
(2014). Pediatric healthcare professionals’ views on autism and attitudes among the healthcare professionals in Lahore, Paki-
spectrum disorder screening at 12–18 months. Journal of stan. BMC Pediatrics, 11(1), 107. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​
Autism and Developmental Disorders. https​://doi.org/10.1007/ 3-011-1344-4.
s1080​3-014-2101-2. Jacobson, N. S., Follette, W. C., & Revenstorf, D. (1984). Psycho-
Daniel, K., Prue, C., Taylor, M., Thomas, J., & Scales, M. (2009). therapy outcome research: Methods for reporting variability and
‘Learn the signs. Act early’: A campaign to help every child evaluating clinical significance. Behavior Therapy, 15(4), 336–
reach his or her full potential. Public Health, 123, e11–e16. 352. https​://doi.org/10.1016/S0005​-7894)84)80002​-7.
https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.puje.2009.06.002. Jacobson, N. S., & Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: A statis-
Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J., Greenson, tical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy
J., Donaldson, J., & Varley, J. (2010). Randomized, controlled research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59(1),
trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: The Early 12. https​://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.59.1.12.
Start Denver Model. Pediatrics, 125(1), e17–e23. https​://doi. Janvier, Y. M., Harris, J. F., Coffield, C. N., Louis, B., Xie, M., Cidav,
org/10.1542/peds/2009-0958. Z., & Mandell, D. S. (2016). Screening for autism spectrum dis-
Dillenburger, K., McKerr, L., Jordan, J. A., & Keenan, M. (2016). order in underserved communities: Early childcare providers as
Staff training in autism: The one-eyed wo/man…. International reporters. Autism, 20, 364–373. https​://doi.org/10.1177/13623​
Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 13(7), 61315​58505​5.
716. https​://doi.org/10.3390/ijerp​h1307​0716. Johnson, C. P., & Myers, S. M. (2007). Identification and evaluation
Dosreis, S., Weiner, C. L., Johnson, L., & Newschaffer, C. J. (2006). of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 120(5),
Autism spectrum disorder screening and management practices 1183–1215.
among general pediatric providers. Journal of Developmental Johnson, S., Hollis, C., Kochhar, P., Hennessy, E., Wolke, D., & Mar-
and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2), S88–S94. low, N. (2010). Autism spectrum disorders in extremely preterm
Elmensdorp, S. T. (2011). Training physicians on the early behavio- children. The Journal of Pediatrics, 156(4), 525–531. https​://doi.
ral characteristics of autism: The use of a brief, group didactic org/10.1016/j.peds.2009.10.041.
training module. Doctoral Dissertation. University of Califor- Kairys, S. W., & Petrova, A. (2016). Role of participation of pedia-
nia, Santa Barbara, CA. tricians in the “Activated Autism Practice” program in prac-
Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Baranek, G. T., Cook, E. H., Dawson, ticing children with Autism Spectrum Disorders at the pri-
G., Gordon, B., Gravel, J. S., Johnson, C. P., Kallen, R. L., mary pare setting. Global Pediatric Health, 3, 1–6. https​://doi.
Levy, S. E., Minshew, N. J., Ozonoff, S., Prizant, B. M., Rapin, org/10.1177/23337​94X16​66354​4&%23x20​0F;.
I., Rogers, S. J., Stone, W. L., Teplin, S., Tuchman, R. F., & Kobak, K. A., Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., & Swanson, A. (2011).
Volkmar, F. R. (1999). The screening and diagnosis of autistic Web-based training in early autism screening: Results from a pilot
spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis- study. Telemedicine and E-Health, 17(8), 640–644. https​://doi.
orders, 29(6), 439–484. org/10.1089/tmj.2011.0029.
Gima, H., Kihara, H., Watanabe, H., Nakano, H., Nakano, J., Koni- Landa, R. J., & Garrett-Mayer, E. (2006). Development in infants with
shi, Y., et al. (2018). Early motor signs of autism spectrum dis- autism spectrum disorders: A prospective study. Journal of Child
order in spontaneous position of the head. Experimental Brain Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(6), 629–638. https​://doi.org/10.1
Research, 236, 1139–1148. 111/j.1469-7610.2006.01531​.x.
Godin, G., Bélanger-Gravel, A., Eccles, M., & Grimshaw, J. LeBarton, E. S., & Iverson, J. M. (2016). Associations between gross
(2008). Healthcare professionals’ intentions and behav- motor and communicative development in at-risk infants. Infant
iours: A systematic review of studies based on social cogni- Behavior and Development, 44, 59–67. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j/
tive theories. Implementation Science, 3(1), 36. https​: //doi. infbe​h.2016.05.003.
org/10.1186/1748-5908-3-36. Lemcke, S., Juul, S., Parner, E. T., Lauritsen, M. B., & Thorsen, P.
Goyen, T., & Lui, K. (2002). Longitudinal motor development of (2013). Early signs of autism in toddlers: A follow-up study in
“apparently normal” high-risk infants at 18 months, 3 and 5 the Danish National Birth Cohort. Journal of Autism and Devel-
years. Early Human Development, 70(1), 103–115. https​://doi. opmental Disorders, 43(10), 2366–2375‫‏‬. https​://doi.org/10.1007/
org/10.1016/S0378​-3782(02)00094​-4. s1080​3-013-1785-z.
Greenhalgh, T., Robert, G., Macfarlane, F., Bate, P., & Kyriakidou, O. Limperopoulos, C., Bassan, H., Sullivan, N. R., Soul, J. S., Robertson,
(2004). Diffusion of innovations in service organizations: System- R. L. Jr., Moore, M., Ringer, S. A., Volpe, J. J., & du Plessis,
atic review and recommendations. The Milbank Quarterly, 82(4), A. J. (2008). Positive screening for autism in ex-preterm infants:
581–629. https​://doi.org/10.1111/j,0887-378X.2004.00325​.x. Prevalence and risk factors. Pediatrics, 121(4), 758–765. https​://
Harris, S. L., & Handleman, J. S. (2000). Age and IQ at intake as pre- doi.org/10.1542/peds.2007-2158.
dictors of placement for young children with autism: A four- to Lobo, M. A., Harbourne, R. T., Dusing, S. C., & McCoy, S. W. (2013).
six-year follow-up. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- Grounding early intervention: Physical therapy cannot just be
ders, 30(2), 137–142. https​://doi.org/10.1023/A:10054​59606​120. about motor skills anymore. Physical Therapy, 93(1), 94–103.
Harris, S. R. (2017). Early motor delays as diagnostic clues in autism https​://doi.org/10.2522/ptj.20120​158.
spectrum disorder. European Journal of Pediatrics, 176(9), 1259– Major, N. E., Peacock, G., Ruben, W., Thomas, J., & Weitzman, C.
1262. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s0043​1-017-2951-7. C. (2013). Autism training in pediatric residency: Evaluation of
Heidgerken, A. D., Geffken, G., Modi, A., & Frakey, L. (2005). A a case-based curriculum. Journal of Autism and Developmen-
survey of autism knowledge in a health care setting. Journal of tal Disorders, 43(5), 1171–1177. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(3), 323–330. https:​ //doi. 3-012-1662-1.
org/10.1007/s1080​3-005-3298-x. Menon, A., Korner-Bitensky, N., Kastner, M., McKibbon, K., & Straus,
Honigfeld, L., Chandhok, L., & Spiegelman, K. (2012). Engaging pedi- S. (2009). Strategies for rehabilitation professionals to move evi-
atricians in developmental screening: The effectiveness of aca- dence-based knowledge into practice: A systematic review. Jour-
demic detailing. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, nal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 41(13), 1024–1032. https​://doi.
42(6), 1175–1182. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​3-011-1344-4. org/10.2340/16501​977-0451.

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Oxman, A. D., Thomson, M. A., Davis, D. A., & Haynes, R. B. (1995). American Journal on Mental Retardation, 110(6), 417–438. https​
No magic bullets: A systematic review of 102 trials of interven- ://doi.org/10.1352/0895.
tions to improve professional practice. Canadian Medical Asso- Schwartz, H., & Drager, K. D. (2008). Training and knowledge in
ciation Journal, 153(10), 1423–1431. autism among speech–language pathologists: A survey. Language,
Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Goldring, S., Greiss-Hess, L., Herrera, A. Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 39(1), 66–77. https​://
M., Steele, J., et al. (2008). Gross motor development, move- doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2008/007).
ment abnormalities, and early identification of Autism. Journal Scott, S. D., Albrecht, L., O’Leary, K., Ball, G. D., Hartling, L.,
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(4), 644–656. https​:// Hofmeyer, A., Jones, C. A., Klassen, T. P., Burns, K., New-
doi.org/10.1007/s1080​3-007-0430-0. ton, A. S., Thompson, D., & Dryden, D. M. (2012). Systematic
Peacock, G., & Lin, S. C. (2012). Enhancing early identification and review of knowledge translation strategies in the allied health
coordination of intervention services for young children with professions. Implementation Science, 7(1), 70. https ​ : //doi.
autism spectrum disorders: Report from the act early regional org/10.1186/1748-5908-7-70.
summit project. Disability and Health Journal, 5(1), 55–59. https​ Self, T. L., Coufal, K., & Parham, D. F. (2010). Allied healthcare pro-
://doi.org/10.1016/j.dhjo.2011.10.001. viders’ role in screening for autism spectrum disorders. Journal
Pierce, K., Carter, C., Weinfeld, M., Desmond, J., Hazin, R., Bjork, R., of Allied Health, 39(3), 165–174‫‏‬.
& Gallagher, N. (2011). Detecting, studying, and treating autism Sices, L., Feudtner, C., McLaughlin, J., Drotar, D., & Williams, M.
early: The one-year well-baby check-up approach. The Journal (2003). How do primary care physicians identify young children
of Pediatrics, 159(3), 458–465. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds​ with developmental delays? A national survey. Journal of Devel-
.2011.02.036. opmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 24(6), 409–417. https​://doi.
Pierce, K., Courchesne, E., & Bacon, E. (2016). To screen or not to org/10.1097/00004​703-20031​2000-00002​.
screen universally for autism is not the question: Why the task Siperstein, R., & Volkmar, F. (2004). Brief report: Parental reporting
force got it wrong. The Journal of Pediatrics, 176, 182–194. https​ of regression in children with pervasive developmental disorders.
://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds​.2016.06.004. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(6), 731–734.
Provost, B., Lopez, B. R., & Heimerl, S. (2007). A comparison of https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​3-004-5294-y.
motor delays in young children: Autism spectrum disorder, Swanson, A. R., Warren, Z. E., Stone, W. L., Vehorn, A. C., Dohrmann,
developmental delay, and developmental concerns. Journal of E., & Humberd, Q. (2014). The diagnosis of autism in commu-
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(2), 321–328. https​:// nity pediatric settings: Does advanced training facilitate practice
doi.org/10.1007/s1080​3-006-0170-6. change? Autism, 18(5), 555–561. https​://doi.org/10.1177/13623​
Richardson, B. (1999). Professional development: 2. Professional 61313​48150​7.
knowledge and situated learning in the workplace. Physiotherapy, Szatmari, P., Bryson, S. E., Boyle, M. H., Streiner, D. L., & Duku, E.
85(9), 467–474. https:​ //doi.org/10.1016/S0031-​ 9406(05)65471-​ 5. (2003). Predictors of outcome among high functioning children
Robins, D. L., Fein, D., Barton, M. L., & Green, J. A. (2001). The with autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology
Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers: An initial study inves- and Psychiatry, 44(4), 520–528. https​://doi.org/10.1111/1469-
tigating the early detection of autism and pervasive developmental 7610.00141​.
disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(2), West, K. L. (2018). Infant motor development in autism spectrum dis-
131–144. https​://doi.org/10.1023/A:10107​38829​569. order: A synthesis and meta-analysis. Child Development. https​
Rogers, S. J., & Vismara, L. A. (2008). Evidence-based comprehensive ://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13086​.
treatments for early autism. Journal of Clinical Child and Adoles- Yeargin-Allsopp, M., Rice, C., Karapurkar, T., Doernberg, N., Boyle,
cent Psychology, 37(1), 8–38. C., & Murphy, C. (2003). Prevalence of autism in a US metro-
Sacrey, L. A. R., Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Brian, J., Smith, I. M., politan area. JAMA, 289(1), 49–55. https​://doi.org/10.1001/
Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Roncadin, C., Garon, N., Novakm, C., jama.289.1.49.
Vaillancourt, T., McCormick, T., MacKinnon, B., Sanne, J., & Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Lord, C., Rogers, S., Carter, A., Carver,
Armstrong, V. (2015). Can parents’ concerns predict autism spec- L., Chawarska, K., Constantino, J., Dawson, G., Dobkins, K.,
trum disorder? A prospective study of high-risk siblings from 6 to Fein, D., Iverson, J., Klin, A., Landa, R., Messinger, D., Ozo-
36 months of age. Journal of the American Academy of Child and noff, S., Sigman, M., Stone, W., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Yirmiya,
Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(6), 470–478. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j. N. (2009). Clinical assessment and management of toddlers
jaac.2015.03.014. with suspected autism spectrum disorder: Insights from studies
Sallows, G. O., & Graupner, T. D. (2005). Intensive behavioral treat- of high-risk infants. Pediatrics, 123(5), 1383–1391. https​://doi.
ment for children with autism: Four-year outcome and predictors. org/10.1542/peds/2008-1606.

13

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi