Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 17

BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 1 7 9 ( 2 00 7 ) 8 9 –1 05

a v a i l a b l e a t w w w. s c i e n c e d i r e c t . c o m

w w w. e l s e v i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / b r a i n r e s

Research Report

Neural mechanisms for learning actions in context

Phan Luu a,b,⁎, Don M. Tucker a,b , Roy Stripling c


a
Electrical Geodesics, Inc., 1600 Millrace Dr. Suite 307, Eugene, OR 97403, USA
b
Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403, USA
c
US Naval Research Laboratory, 4555 Overlook Ave. SW, Washington, DC 20375, USA

A R T I C LE I N FO AB S T R A C T

Article history: The transition from actions that require effortful attention to those that are exercised
Accepted 23 March 2007 automatically reflects the progression of learning. Full automaticity marks the performance
Available online 5 June 2007 of the expert. Research on changes in brain activity from novice to skilled performance has
been consistent with this behavioral characterization, showing that a highly practiced skill
Keywords: often requires less brain activation than before practice. Moreover, the decrease in brain
Event-related potential activity with practice is most pronounced in the general or executive control processes
ERP mediated by frontal lobe networks. Consistent with these human cognitive neuroscience
Learning findings, animal neurophysiological evidence suggests that two elementary learning
Medial frontal negativity systems support different stages of skill acquisition. One system supports rapid and
MFN focused acquisition of new skills in relation to threats and violations of expectancies. The
P300 other involves a gradual process of updating a configural model of the environmental
Expertise context. We collected dense array electroencephalography as participants performed an
Action arbitrary associative (“code learning”) task. We predicted that frontal lobe activity would
decrease, whereas posterior cortical activity would increase, as the person gains the
knowledge required for appropriate action. Both predictions were confirmed. In addition, we
found that learning resulted in an unexpected increase in activity in the medial frontal lobe
(the medial frontal negativity or MFN). Although preliminary, these findings suggest that the
specific mechanisms of learning in animal neurophysiology studies may prove informative
for understanding the neural basis of human learning and executive cognitive control.
© 2007 Published by Elsevier B.V.

1. Introduction tomatic processes, which result from learning and are not
strongly limited by the capacity of working memory, and
When experts become skilled in a given task domain, they which, when engaged in the appropriate context, may be
perform routine tasks automatically, thereby freeing up carried out unconsciously with minimal distraction of con-
cognitive resources for more executive tasks, such as organiz- scious attention (Chein and Schneider, 2005).
ing situational awareness (Fitts, 1964). Psychological studies of NeuroImaging studies have suggested there may be direct
expert cognition have emphasized the importance of controlled neural correlates of the reduced demands for controlled pro-
processes, which are limited by the capacity of working me- cesses, as evidenced by decreased demands on brain activity
mory, require active attention, and can be directed con- resulting from increasing practice with task performance.
sciously in new task situations (Schneider and Shiffrin, 1977; Given that frontal lobe activity is thought to be particularly
Shiffrin and Schneider, 1977). These are contrasted with au- important to goal representations and providing control-

⁎ Corresponding author. Electrical Geodesics, Inc., 1600 Millrace Dr. Suite 307, Eugene, OR 97403, USA. Fax: +1 541 687 7963.
E-mail address: pluu@egi.com (P. Luu).

0006-8993/$ – see front matter © 2007 Published by Elsevier B.V.


doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2007.03.092
90 BR A IN RE S E A RCH 1 1 79 ( 20 0 7 ) 8 9 –1 05

related outputs (Miller and Cohen, 2001), it is theoretically Although we have emphasized the importance of the ACC
important that both meta-analysis of fMRI studies and new in action monitoring (Luu and Tucker, 2003), many of the
experiments have suggested a specific decrease in frontal lobe human findings of error-related negativities (response-locked
activity (bilateral dorsal frontal, left ventral prefrontal, anterior ERNs) or medial frontal negativities (stimulus-locked MFN or
cingulate cortex, left insular regions) as participants become N2) have shown strong responses in medial frontal cortex
more practiced in task performance (Chein and Schneider, 2005). when the subject's expectancies are violated. These results
In attempting to link the more elementary neurophysiology may be consistent with more elementary learning processes;
of action regulation to the development of expert control of discrepancy or conflict detection is a requirement for beha-
human cognition, we looked to the literature on neurophysio- vioral adjustments. Indeed, it is through the detection of
logical models of animal learning. In research on discriminant discrepancies between what is expected, given a particular
learning in mammals, two cortico-limbic-thalamic circuits action, and what actually happened that new learning occurs
have been identified, each providing a unique strategic control (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972). According to an influential
on the learning process (Gabriel et al., 2002). The first system is theory of medial frontal control of cognitive conflict, the ERN
responsible for rapid discriminant learning (both avoidance reflects the activity of a learning system that relies on
and conditional). This system includes the anterior cingulate prediction errors (Holroyd and Coles, 2002).
cortex (ACC), amygdala, and mediodorsal nucleus of the In the present study, we presented participants with an
thalamus. The unique properties of the fast learning system, arbitrary associative (“code learning”) task, in which they
e.g., its contribution to overcoming habitual responses, led needed to discover an arbitrary mapping of stimuli (digits or
Gabriel and colleagues (2002) to suggest this circuit is integral spatial location) with key presses of the correct hand and
to what has been called the executive control of cognition finger, or with no response. We hypothesized, broadly, that
(Posner and DiGirolamo, 1998). Bussey and colleagues (2001) the learning process would engage frontal lobe activity
have shown that the ventral and orbital prefrontal cortices implicated in controlled cognitive processes. More specifically,
should also be included as part of this fast learning system. in line with the neurophysiologic model of action regulation,
A second circuit, centered on the posterior cingulate cortex we hypothesized that the demands on rapid learning early in
(PCC) and anterior thalamic nucleus, is involved in the late the task would engage activity of the anterior ventral network.
stages of learning (Bussey et al., 1996; Keng and Gabriel, 1998). In contrast, we hypothesized that as participants learned this
In these late stages, a contextual model is formed, and minor task, the consolidation of a cognitive context for performance
changes that are consistent with the contextual model can be would be indexed by an increasingly robust P300 or Late
made with minimal attentional demands. This late stage Positive Complex (LPC), generated in medial temporal, poster-
learning process can be described as forming the basis of ior cingulate, and parietal cortices, thus reflecting the more
habits (Bussey et al., 1996). automatic process typical of expert performance. For both the
If this neurophysiological model is correct, learning is frontal, ventral contribution to early learning, and the poster-
achieved by circuits with qualitative strategic biases, toward ior, dorsal contribution to late learning, we hypothesized that
either (1) gradual updating of a valued context model or (2) the digit code task would result in greater activity in left
rapid, focused changes of associations under conditions of hemisphere networks and the spatial code task would result
threat or context violation (Tucker and Luu, 2006). In extend- in greater activity in right hemisphere networks.
ing this model to expertise in human learning, the rapid,
focused learning system would be required as participants
learn new arbitrary associations, such as in visuomotor 2. Results
mapping of an arbitrary visual stimulus to a particular
response (Bussey et al., 2001; Wise and Murray, 2000). 2.1. Behavioral data
Researchers studying arbitrary visuomotor mapping (also
referred to as conditional visuomotor mapping) regard the The following factors were considered in repeated measures
underlying processes as indicative of the remarkable cap- ANOVA models: task (digit, spatial), accuracy (error, correct),
ability of mammals to associate any arbitrary stimulus with a target (go, nogo), and learning (pre, post). Although learning is
motor response, and thus to behave appropriately in any given a likely to be a dynamic and continuous phenomenon, for
context. analytic purposes, it is convenient to determine a cutoff that
Functional neuroimaging studies of hemodynamic res- marks when participants have fully acquired the target–
ponses during arbitrary visuomotor association learning have response mapping. There are several methods for determining
identified the involvement of the anterior cingulate sulcus, when learning has occurred (Smith et al., 2004). We use the
parahippocampal gyrus, caudate nucleus (i.e., dorsal stria- simplest method, the fixed-number of consecutive correct
tum), inferior frontal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, dorsal responses method1 (FCCR). In this method, a learning
premotor cortex, and parietal cortex (Toni and Passingham,
1
1999; Toni et al., 2001; Wise and Murray, 2000). These regions For several subjects, we compared the fixed-number of con-
appear to overlap substantially with the neural networks secutive correct responses method against a dynamic state–space
method for determination of a learning thershold (Smith et al.,
implicated in controlled cognitive processes by Chein and
2004). On average, the state–space method identified the occur-
Schneider (2005), and certain of these structures are closely
rence of learning to be two trials earlier, consistent with what
related to the fast learning cortico-thalamic-limbic circuit Smith et al. (2004) reported. Because analyses of the behavioral and
implicated by Gabriel and colleagues in the early stages of ERP data are based on subject averages, it is unlikely that the
discriminant learning. reported results were affected by this small difference.
BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 1 7 9 ( 2 00 7 ) 8 9 –1 05 91

threshold was defined as the moment that participants were revealed that participants' responses were significantly
able to make four consecutive correct responses (or with- longer for error responses than correct responses and that
holding of a response) for a particular target. As noted in the this difference was greater for the post-learned state than the
Experimental procedures section, the same target could not pre learned state.
occur on consecutive trials. Because the task permitted participants to regulate the
Based on the fixed-number of consecutive correct response duration of the feedback stimulus, we can use duration of
method, analysis of the number of trials that participants took feedback processing to assess whether participants took
to learn the target–response mappings, with Target and Task more time to view the feedback prior to and after learning
as factors, revealed a significant main effect for target, F(1,10) = of the mappings. The assumption is that prior to knowing the
23.7, p b 0.001. Participants required more trials to learn the go target–response mappings, participants will take more time
target–response mapping (M = 16.3, SD = 8.0) than withholding viewing the feedback. For this analysis, task and learning
a response for nogo targets (M = 8.2, SD = 2.9). This makes factors were included. The results only showed a main effect
intuitive sense because nogo targets do not require response for Learning, F(1,10) = 11.1, p b 0.01. Participants viewed the
mappings. All participants reported, during task debriefing feedback longer prior (M = 2215 ms, SD = 1977 ms) to learning
sessions, that they focused primarily on go targets. Fig. 1 shows than after learning (M = 531 ms, SD = 164 ms). Feedback
accuracy (for digit and spatial tasks) as a function of trials processing as a function of trials, averaged across all stimuli,
averaged across all stimuli. is presented Fig. 1.
Analysis of reaction time to targets included task, The behavioral analyses demonstrate that task did not
accuracy, and learning. The analysis revealed significant impact the various dependent measures, which illustrate the
main effects for Learning, F(1,10) = 11.22, p b 0.01, and Accu- important fact that the tasks were equated for difficulty.
racy, F(1,10) = 11.6, p b 0.02, that were qualified by a Learning × Therefore, significant differences in brain activity between
Accuracy interaction, F(1,10) = 8.3, p b 0.02. The interaction tasks cannot be attributed to task difficulty.

Fig. 1 – Accuracy and Feedback processing as a function of trials for digit and spatial tasks. Vertical line marks the average trial
at which subjects reached the learning criterion.
92 BR A IN RE S E A RCH 1 1 79 ( 20 0 7 ) 8 9 –1 05

2.2. ERP data hemisphere (see Fig. 7). For the left hemisphere, this included
channels F9, FT9, and three channels inferior to both these
2.2.1. Spatiotemporal dynamics of target effects locations, and for the right hemisphere channels F10 and FT10
Approximately 120 ms after target onset a clear P1 component and three inferior channels (homologous to the left hemi-
is maximal over occipital and occipitoparietal recording sites sphere) were included. For each hemisphere, the data were
(see Figs. 2 and 3). The P1 is much larger and more focal for the averaged over the five channels.
digit targets compared to the spatial targets. At approximately The MFN was quantified at four medial frontal channels,
170 ms after target onset, the N1 reached it maximum. Like the including FCz and Fz (see Fig. 7), by identifying the negative
P1, the N1 is much larger for the digit targets. The N1 showed a peak between 260 and 350 ms after target onset (see Fig. 5)
strong right lateralization for spatial targets. The amplitude and averaging over a 44-ms interval centered on the peak
and laterality differences of these components by target type (inclusive). Next, we identified the positivity preceding the
reflect the properties of the target themselves; the digits were MFN and obtained an average over a 44-ms interval
presented foveally and the spatial targets are dots presented centered on the positive peak. This served as the reference
parafoveally to demarcate spatial locations. for the MFN. The MFN was then averaged over the four
Interestingly, the maximum of the N1 has a different channels.
spatial distribution than the negativity that appears to initiate The P300 was quantified by first identifying the positive
its onset. For the digit targets, the onset of the N1 began over peak between 350 and 700 ms after target onset (see Fig. 6) and
the left temporal recording sites, which can be seen in Fig. 2 at averaging over a 44-ms interval centered on the peak
the time of the P1. For the spatial targets, the onset of the N1 (inclusive). The data were referenced to a baseline extending
occurred over right temporoparietal sites and spreads caud- from − 400 ms to target onset. This was done for individual
ally to occipitotemporal regions. channels in six regions (see Fig. 7). For each region, the average
Soon after the maximum of the N1 clear laterality of the channels was obtained.
differences emerged for digit and spatial targets, starting For analysis of the LIAN, a repeated measures ANOVA was
from occipitotemporal recording sites and spreading to employed, with learning, task, and laterality (left, right) as
inferior temporal and frontal locations (see Fig. 4). This factors. There was a main effect for Learning, F(1,10) = 5.8,
negativity peaked approximately 390 ms after target onset at p b 0.04, that was qualified by a significant Learning × Task
frontal recording sites. For the digit targets, the negativity is interaction, F(1,10) = 9.2, p b 0.02. This interaction indicated
left lateralized whereas it is right lateralized for the spatial that after participants learned the stimulus–response map-
targets. We refer to this as the lateral inferior anterior pings, the LIAN was reduced in the spatial task whereas it
negativity (LIAN). increased for the digit task. Mean comparisons revealed that
this difference was only significant for the spatial task,
2.2.2. Electrophysiological signs of learning F(1,10) = 10.6, p b 0.01. A Task × Laterality interaction was also
At approximately 327 ms after target onset a negativity can be significant, F(1,10) = 7.6, p b 0.03. Mean comparisons confirmed
observed over the medial frontal channels, in particular FCz the observation that the LIAN was larger over the right
(see Fig. 5). Based on the latency and scalp distribution of this hemisphere, compared to the left hemisphere, for the spatial
component, we refer to it as the MFN (see Gehring and task, F(1,10) = 6.4, p b 0.03. Although not significant in the
Willoughby, 2002). The latency and distribution of the MFN is analysis, for the digit task the LIAN was larger over the left
similar to a component often referred to as the N2 in previous hemisphere than the right hemisphere.
ERP studies; given the improved topography of the dense The factors learning and task were used in a repeated
array EEG, we prefer to emphasize the medial frontal dis- measures ANOVA for the MFN component. This analysis
tribution of this effect, rather than the wave deflection alone. revealed significant Learning, F(1,10) = 15.2, p b 0.01, and Task,
The MFN is most prominent for the post-learning trials for F(1,10) = 18.6, p b 0.01, effects. The MFN was substantially
both types of targets. At approximately the same time as the more negative after participants learned the task, and this
MFN, a P300 component began to develop and peaked ap- component was more negative for the verbal compared to the
proximately 498 ms after target onset. The P300 is larger for spatial task.
both target types after participants learned the visuomotor Analysis of P300 amplitude included learning, task, lobe
association (see Fig. 6). Interestingly, the distribution of the (parietal, occipitoparietal), and laterality (left, midline, and
P300 differed according to target type. For the digit targets right) as factors in a repeated measures ANOVA model. The
(which were foveally presented), the P300 has a centroparietal analysis revealed a significant two-way interaction involving
distribution whereas for spatial targets it had an occipitopar- task and lobe, F(1,10) = 11.0, p b 0.01, and a significant three-
ietal distribution. way interaction involving learning, task, and laterality,
F(2,20) = 5.6, p b 0.02 (Geisser–Greenhouse corrected). For the
2.2.3. Statistical analysis two-way interaction, mean comparisons revealed that P300
We focus on three components for statistical analysis: LIAN, amplitude was largest at parietal sites, compared to occipi-
MFN, and P300. We quantified the LIAN by first identifying the toparietal sites, for the digit task, F(1,10) = 47.1, p b 0.01, and
negative peak between 350 and 450 ms after target onset (see that this same difference was marginally significant for the
Fig. 4). Next the average amplitude was obtained over a 44-ms spatial task, F(1,10) = 4.7, p b 0.06.
interval centered on the peak (inclusive). The data were The three-way interaction was examined by analyzing the
referenced to a baseline extending from − 400 ms to target Learning × Laterality interaction for each task. For the spatial
onset (time 0). This was done for five channels on each task, the two-way interaction was not significant, although
BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 1 7 9 ( 2 00 7 ) 8 9 –1 05
Fig. 2 – Topographic waveform plots for pre- and post-learning averages for the digit and spatial tasks. Orientation of map is top looking down with nose at top of page.

93
94 BR A IN RE S E A RCH 1 1 79 ( 20 0 7 ) 8 9 –1 05

Fig. 3 – Topographic maps and waveform plots for pre learned correct go targets. Orientation of topographic maps is top
looking down with nose at top of page. Topographic maps are presented for the peaks of the P1 and N1. Dots on topographic
maps represent channel locations of the waveform plots.

Fig. 4 – 3D topographic maps and waveform plots for pre learned correct go targets. Topographic maps are presented for the
peak of the LIAN. White circles represent channel locations of the waveform plots. Large and small boxes in waveform plots
mark the time window used to quantify the P300 and LIAN, respectively.
BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 1 7 9 ( 2 00 7 ) 8 9 –1 05 95

Fig. 5 – Topographic maps and waveform plots for correct go targets. Topographic maps are presented for the peak of the MFN.
Dot on topographic maps represent channel location of the waveform plots. Box in waveform plot mark the time window used
to quantify the MFN.

the direction of the P300 amplitude is such that it is learning and that this difference was largest over the left
consistently larger across the left, midline, and right recording hemisphere recording sites.
sites post-learning. For the digit task, this same interaction Using the Wisconsin Card Sorting task, Barceló and
was significant, F(2,20) = 4.5, p b 0.03 (Geisser–Greenhouse cor- colleagues (2000) found that P300 amplitude increased as
rected), confirming that the P300 amplitude was largest post- participants learned the sorting rule. To examine similar

Fig. 6 – Topographic maps and waveform plots for correct go targets. Topographic maps are presented for the peak of the P300.
Dots on topographic maps represent channel locations of the waveform plots. Large and small boxes in waveform plots mark
the time window used to quantify the P300 and LIAN, respectively.
96 BR A IN RE S E A RCH 1 1 79 ( 20 0 7 ) 8 9 –1 05

Fig. 7 – Channel groups used for quantification of the LIAN (black), MFN (red), and P300 (blue).

learning effects, we analyzed the progressive changes in each rected). This effect illustrates that MFN amplitude continued
component during the learning process. For these analyses, to increase as learning progressed. Similar to the MFN re-
we divided post-learning trials into four quartiles and sults, analysis of the P300 revealed a significant main effect
obtained an average ERP for each quartile. The components for Learning, F(4,40) = 10.2, p b 0.01, that did not significantly
were as previously described, and ANOVAs for each compo- interact with any other factor. This effect showed that P300
nent included the same factors, with the exception that the amplitude increased as participants learned the task (see
learning factor now contained five levels (pre, post1, post2, Fig. 8).
post3, post4). It should be noted that although there is temporal overlap
The post-learning stages were defined using FCCR cri- between the LIAN, MFN, and P300, our quantification of these
teria: post1 = FCCR ≥4 and ≤8; post2 = FCCR ≥9 and ≤14; components rely on temporal integration around a specific
post3 = FCCR ≥ 15 and ≤22; and post4 = FCCR ≥23. This resulted peak to minimize the overlap. It is likely that the component
in relatively equal number of trials for each of the post-learning quantification is imprecise with regards to component over-
stages (digit task: post1 = 45.5, post2 = 56.8, post3 = 55.2, lap, particularly for the LIAN and P300. To have a better sense
post4 = 60.3, spatial task: post1 = 67.3, post2 = 65.8, post3 = 56.8, of the uniqueness of each component in relation to learning,
post4 = 74.2). For each component, we focus on the learning we supplemented the analysis by employing principal com-
effects that were found to be significant in the preceding ponents analysis (PCA).
analyses in order to clarify the nature of the changes during the
learning process. 2.2.4. Spatial–temporal principal components analysis
For the LIAN, the Learning × Task interaction was signifi- We submitted the averaged data to PCA using ERP PCA Toolbox
cant, F(4,40) = 4.57, p b 0.02 (Geisser–Greenhouse corrected). (http://people.ku.edu/~jdien/downloads.html, see Dien et al.,
Analysis of the main effects revealed that the learning effect 2003). For this analysis, a covariance matrix of the channels
was only significant for the spatial task, F(4,40) = 3.75, p b 0.03 was employed. The results were rotated into simple structure
(Geisser–Greenhouse corrected), which is similar to the using the Promax rotation procedure. From this analysis, we
analyses reported above. This main effect for the spatial task kept four components that accounted for 96% of the variance.
revealed that LIAN amplitude continued to decrease as The components are shown in Fig. 9. Components 1 and 4
learning progressed (see Fig. 8). reflect the LIAN, component 2 resembles the P300, and
Analysis of the MFN revealed a significant learning effect component 3 clearly represents the MFN. Fig. 9 also shows
(see Fig. 8), F(4,40) = 8.16, p b 0.01 (Geisser–Greenhouse cor- the respective factor scores for each component.
BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 1 7 9 ( 2 00 7 ) 8 9 –1 05 97

Fig. 8 – (A) LIAN amplitude as a function of learning. (B) MFN and P300 amplitudes as a function of learning.

The factor scores of the four components were then interaction, a means comparison confirmed that the LIAN
submitted to a temporal PCA using a covariance matrix of decreased as subjects learned the spatial task, F(1,10) = 10.5,
time points. The results were rotated into simple structure p b 0.01. The difference between pre and post-learning for
using the Promax rotation procedure. For each spatial the verbal task was not significant. A means comparison of
component, we retained 15 temporal components and identi- the Task × Component interaction reveal that component 4
fied the relevant component(s) for each spatial component was larger for the spatial than the digit task, F(1,10) = 10.5,
based on the time window and condition differences specified p b 0.01, see Fig. 10. Although component 1 was larger for
from the scalp data. Fig. 9 shows the relevant temporal the digit task than the spatial task, this difference did not
components. For spatial components 1, 2, 3, and 4, the reach statistical significance.
corresponding identified temporal components accounted Analysis of component 2 included learning and task as
for 12%, 21%, 2%, and 6% of the variance. repeated measures factor. There was a significant learning
Repeated measures ANOVAs were performed on the effect, F(1,10) = 30.3, p b 0.01, showing that the component was
identified temporal components. For statistical analysis, we largest after learning. For component 3, learning and task also
employed the same time windows used for analysis of the served as repeated measures factors. The analysis revealed
scalp data. For example, the time window of the LIAN was significant main effect for Learning, F(1,10) = 12.71, p b 0.01, and
employed for the temporal components of components 1 Learning × Task interaction, F(1,10) = 7.08, p b 0.03. This interac-
and 4. For analysis of the LIAN equivalent, learning, task, tion revealed that the amplitude increase in the post-learning
and component (1 and 4) served as repeated measures state was largest for the digit task.
factors. The results are identical to results obtained with the These analyses are not complicated by potential compo-
scalp data; there were significant interactions involving nent overlap, and the results confirm the results obtained
Learning and Task, F(1,10) = 6.19, p b 0.04, and Task and from the scalp data. Therefore, it is significant that learning
Component, F(1,10) = 11.66, p b 0.01. For the Learning × Task effects were observed for the LIAN, MFN, and P300.
98 BR A IN RE S E A RCH 1 1 79 ( 20 0 7 ) 8 9 –1 05

Fig. 9 – Topographic maps of the spatial PCA component loadings, spatial factor scores, and temporal factor loadings. Box in
waveform plot mark the time window used to quantify each component. Boxes correspond to those presented in Figs. 4–6.

2.2.5. Source estimates Eugene, OR). GeoSource uses a finite difference model (FDM)
Source estimates of the scalp potential were accomplished for accurate computation of the lead field in relation to cranial
using the GeoSource electrical source imaging software (EGI, orifices (primarily optical canals and foramen magnum).
Tissue compartments of the model were constructed from
whole-head MRI and CT scans of a single subject.2 The MRI
and CT images were coregistered prior to segmentation of the
brain and cerebral spinal fluid, derived from MRI data, and the
skull and scalp, identified from CT images. The tissue volumes
were parceled using 2-mm voxels to form the computational
elements of the FDM.
Conductivity values used in the FDM model are as follows:
0.25 S/m (Siemens/meter) for brain, 1.8 S/m for cerebral spinal

2
This subject is the same subject whose average MRI is used in
SPM99 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Because the average
MRI of this subject does not contain data from the inferior surface
of the head and the skull cannot be easily extracted from MRI
data, EGI, in collaboration with this subject, acquired whole head
MRI and CT scans of this subject to construct a whole-head
Fig. 10 – Task × component interaction. model.
BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 1 7 9 ( 2 00 7 ) 8 9 –1 05 99

fluid, 0.018 S/m for skull, and 0.44 S/m for scalp (see Ferree et estimates of the sources, a minimum norm solution with the
al., 2000). Source locations were derived from the Montreal LAURA (local autoregressive average) constraint (Grave de
Neurological Institute probabilistic MRI (to which the typical Peralta Menendez et al., 2004) was employed.
subject matches closely). Based on the probabilistic map, gray The estimated source regions contributing to the LIAN, at
matter volume was parceled into 7 mm voxels, each voxel its peak, are illustrated in Fig. 11A. For the digit task, the LIAN
served as a source location with three orthogonal orientations. is associated with left lateralized activity in the medial
This resulted in a total of 2,394 sources. The source locations temporal lobe (including the parahippocampal gyrus), lateral
were registered to the MRI and CT volumes. Once the head temporal lobe, and inferior frontal gyrus (BA 47). For the
model was constructed, an average of the 128-channel spatial task, the sources for the LIAN are in the right medial
positions was registered to the scalp surface. To compute temporal lobe (including the parahippocampal gyrus), anterior

Fig. 11 – (A) Source estimates for LIAN component for digit and spatial targets. Lines at each voxel represent orientation vectors
(pointing in the positive direction). The vectors indicate the scalp topography features that are accounted by the source voxels.
Shown are sources in the temporal and ventrolateral (top row) and parahippocampal regions (bottom row). (B) Source estimates
for MFN component for digit and spatial targets in the post-learned state. Note that other sources not included in the clusters,
but illustrated, account for other aspects of the scalp topography that are not the MFN (see Fig. 3). Brighter colors denote stronger
current sources.
100 BR A IN RE S E A RCH 1 1 79 ( 20 0 7 ) 8 9 –1 05

Fig. 12 – Source estimates for P300 component for digit and spatial targets. Top row shows sources in medial and lateral
parietal cortex, BA 7, and BA 7/BA 40, respectively, and parahippocampal gyri. Note laterality differences in the
parahippocampal sources as a function of target type. Brighter colors denote stronger current sources.

temporal cortex (including superior and inferior temporal learning stages, when participants are still trying to associate
gyri), and inferior frontal gyrus (BA 47). visual information with appropriate responses, lateralized
For the MFN associated with the digit task in the post- negativities originated with the onset of the N1. These
learned state, sources (see Fig. 11B) were identified in three lateralized negativities (left for digit code, right for spatial
clusters: (1) medial frontal gyrus (BA 6 and 8), (2) dorsal ACC (BA code) spread from lateral temporal recording sites to inferior
24 and 32) and paracentral lobule (BA 31), and (3) ventral medial anterior sites, peaking at approximately 390 ms post-target.
frontal gyrus (BA 10), orbital gyrus (BA 11), and ventral ACC (BA We measured the peak of this effect as the LIAN.
32). Similar sources of the MFN were identified for the spatial After participants learned the task, there were three
task in the post-learned state, with the exception of cluster 1. prominent changes identified in the ERPs. The first was a
The P300 sources for the digit task (estimated from the post- reduction in LIAN amplitude, particularly for spatial targets.
learning data, see Fig. 12) are localizable to the medial (BA 7) This seems consistent with the hypothesis of a decrease in
and lateral aspects of the left parietal lobe (BA 7, 40), right frontal contributions to controlled processes as learning
superior temporal gyrus (BA 39), posterior cingulate cortex (BA progresses. The other two changes involved increases in
30, 31), medial occipital cortex (BA 17, 18, and 19), and bilateral MFN and P300 amplitude in the post-learned state. Whereas
parahippocampal gyrus (BA 36 and 37). The pattern was similar the increase in P300 was predicted and is consistent with
for the P300 for the spatial task. In addition, however, there previous research, the increase in the MFN with learning was
were sources in the right parietal lobe (BA 7 and 40) and medial unexpected. We consider each of these components in turn.
frontal gyrus (BA 6). It is noteworthy that the major sources for
the P300, in parahippocampal regions, demonstrated greater 3.1. LIAN
activity in the right compared to the left hemisphere for the
spatial task, and this was reversed in the digit task. LIAN amplitude showed some signs of tracking the learning
process, but differently for the two tasks. For spatial targets,
after participants learned the stimulus–response mappings,
3. Discussion LIAN amplitude was significantly reduced as was hypothe-
sized. However, although the effect was not significant, LIAN
Results of this study provided clues to progressive engage- amplitude increased after learning the correct responses for
ment of brain networks in the process of learning. In early the digit targets. This increase could be due to component
BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 1 7 9 ( 2 00 7 ) 8 9 –1 05 101

overlap because the increase of the LIAN may actually reflect is that the right ventral frontal activity was particularly
P300 amplitude increase (the P300 is positive at parietal, but important for attentional orienting to the configural features
negative at inferior frontal and temporal regions). However, of the spatial task, and thus reflected hemisphere-specific
analysis of the PCA components (1 and 4), which addresses the processing that declined as learning increased within the right
overlap problem, revealed similar results as analysis of the hemisphere specifically.
scalp ERPs, confirming the differential decrease of the LIAN by
task. There are two issues related to the functional interpreta- 3.2. MFN
tion of these findings: the functional significance of the LIAN
and the differential rate of learning-related reduction. In the present study, MFN amplitude associated with the
Source estimates for digit targets revealed that during the target increased as participants learned the task. This effect
time of the LIAN, activity was lateralized to left temporal lobe was opposite to the hypothesis that frontal contributions
and left inferior frontal gyrus. For spatial targets, similar would decrease with increasing expertise and appears to be a
sources were identified in the right hemisphere. Studies using novel finding. For the response-locked medial frontal compo-
fMRI methods have shown activations in these same cortical nent, the error-related negativity (ERN), previous research has
regions during arbitrary visuomotor (Grol et al., 2006; Toni and shown this component did not appear until subjects had
Passingham, 1999; Toni et al., 2001) and sequence learning learned the task (Dehaene et al., 1994). The increase in MFN
(Poldrack et al., 2005; Van der Graaf et al., 2006). In primates, amplitude with increasing learning in the present study may
ventral and orbital regions of the prefrontal cortex have been therefore reflect an improving model of the performance
shown to be involved in the early learning stages of arbitrary context that is used to guide performance (Luu and Tucker,
visuomotor mapping, perhaps through the acquisition of 2003). Previous research with the medial frontal negativity has
response strategies (Bussey et al., 2001). In humans, the role shown it to be sensitive to evaluation of negative feedback
of frontal and temporal regions in learning has been attributed (Luu and Tucker, 2003; Tucker et al., 1999) and to perception of
to memory operations, in which targets take on associative reward reduction or loss (Gehring and Willoughby, 2002;
meaning (Bunge et al., 2003; Toni et al., 2001). Results from Yeung et al., 2005). These studies have examined the MFN in
several studies have suggested that ventrolateral prefrontal response to feedback, not the target stimulus. In these cases,
cortex is involved in memory retrieval through its interaction the feedback-locked MFN decreases with learning (see Holroyd
with temporal lobe structures, including the hippocampus and Coles, 2002).
(Badre et al., 2005; Simons and Spiers, 2003). To examine the similarity between the MFN of the present
In a meta-analytic analysis of 29 studies, Chein and study with previous dense array MFN findings, using the
Schneider (2005) reported that 8 of 12 brain regions that improved source localization with the FDM head model in
showed practice related decreases were in the right hemi- GeoSource, we compared the present source distribution of
sphere. Of particular relevance to the present LIAN results is the MFN with that from our previously published data (Luu et
the finding of right lateralization of practice-related decreases al., 2003). The source estimate, using the same parameters for
in the insula and surrounding cortical regions (including the GeoSource analysis described previously, showed a source in
ventral aspects of the frontal lobe and anterior temporal lobe). dorsomedial frontal cortex for the Luu et al. (2003) results that
Similarly, a recent study using target locations to guide is almost identical to the dorsomedial frontal source for the
sequence learning demonstrated right lateralized involve- present MFN (cluster 2 in Fig. 11). Notably different for the
ment of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex early in learning present data, however, were additional sources, particularly in
(Van der Graaf et al., 2006). the rostroventral prefrontal cortex (cluster 3), that were also
A relevant recent finding is that visual orienting (used for identified as contributing to the MFN. With the appearance of
selecting stimuli for action) is anatomically and functionally activity in this rostroventral midline region, the source model
separable into two sub-systems: a dorsal system (bilateral for the present MFN more closely resembles that for our
intraparietal sulcus and bilateral superior frontal sulcus) and a previous ERN results (Luu et al., 2003) rather than the previous
ventral system (right temporal-parietal junction and right MFN results.
ventral frontal cortex; Fox et al., 2006). The ventral system is The MFN response in the present experiment can be
believed to be involved in detection of salient stimuli. integrated with previous results by adopting a learning
Functionally, early in learning it is likely that to-be-learned model of regulating actions in context (Luu et al., 2003). This
stimuli are particularly salient because participants must map model is derived from Gabriel's theoretical analysis of cortico-
each stimulus to an appropriate response. Thus, based on thalamic-limbic circuits in discriminant avoidance learning
previous research and the characteristics of the present tasks, (Gabriel, 1993). In this analysis, the amygdala and ACC form a
we might believe that activity of the right hemisphere system involved in the early stages of learning to discriminate
(particularly the ventrolateral prefrontal structures) could between conditioned stimuli that predict delivery of a shock
decrease at a faster rate as a function of learning than the versus those that do not (Gabriel et al., 2002). Cells within this
activity in homologous networks the left hemisphere. ACC circuit become activated in a discriminant manner as the
It is important to note that the right ventrofrontal and animal acquires the differentiating behavior for a new context.
anterior temporal activation indexed by the LIAN is present Consistent with this analysis, a recent visuomotor association
during learning of both the digit and spatial tasks. Therefore, learning study found cells within the medial prefrontal cortex
this system must be critical for learning generally, although that differentiated between novel and familiar cues, with
the decrease in right ventral frontal activity was only observed neurons sensitive to familiar cues more active when new
for the spatial task. Our speculative interpretation at this point learning was required (Inase et al., 2006). Single cell recordings
102 BR A IN RE S E A RCH 1 1 79 ( 20 0 7 ) 8 9 –1 05

in the human ACC also show cells that are sensitive to reward using ERP measures have consistently shown increases in
reductions that signal requirement for behavior change P300 amplitude after learning. Using the Wisconsin card
(Williams et al., 2004). These several findings suggest that sorting task these, Barceló and colleagues (2000) discovered
the ACC supports rapid motivated learning, particularly when that P300 amplitude was reduced whenever participants
actions must be regulated strongly and quickly to adapt to new were required to learn a new rule, and it gradually increased
contextual contingencies. Therefore, one way to interpret the as a new rule was acquired. This led to the hypothesis that
increasing MFN amplitude with learning is to consider this the P300 reflects cortical activity associated with stable
component as reflecting monitoring functions that are representation of rule-coding (or attentional) sets. We think
required as subjects learn appropriate actions for a given this is consistent with the classical notion of context-
context. Once the context (i.e., stimulus–response mappings) updating (Donchin and Coles, 1988), and with our interpreta-
is acquired in the present task, potential for response conflict tion that context-updating is supported by the dorsal limbic
becomes established more strongly. In fact, the behavioral (Papez circuit) late stage learning system (Luu and Tucker,
data show that errors after learning are associated with the 2003).
longest reaction times, consistent with a view of increased
conflict after learning because the correct response is now 3.4. Source estimation issues
established. In this sense, therefore, the MFN may reflect
mechanisms related to conflict monitoring functions of the The analysis of neural mechanisms of scalp EEG can be greatly
ACC (Botvinick et al., 2004). Based on the interpretation that facilitated by source analysis. In describing the present results,
the MFN reflects response and conflict monitoring that we have called out the specific gyri and regions implied by the
increase as the correct actions and context are learned, we atlas based cortically constrained source model of the
predict that with additional practice, where stimulus– GeoSource software. However, with only 128 scalp channels,
response mappings are further automated, involvement of without exact registration of sensor positions, and with the
the ACC (as indexed by the MFN) would decrease, consistent limitation of the atlas reconstruction rather than the subject's
with prior studies of ACC and automaticity (Raichle et al., own cortex, we estimate that a 1-cm3 error is the minimum to
1994). be expected. Therefore, we can be fairly confident that the
P300, for example, is localized to medial temporal regions, but
3.3. P300 any further precision is probably spurious. It should be
emphasized that the present 128 channel sensor array (the
In studies using both fMRI and ERP methods, sources of the HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net) was constructed to provide
scalp recorded EEG have been identified in the parietal cortex improved coverage of the inferior head regions (face and neck)
(BA 40; Menon et al., 1997), bilateral posterior parietal cortex, compared to the previous 128 array (GSN 200). The present
inferior temporal cortex, temporoparietal junction, and ven- results show that, regardless of source localization, without
trolateral prefrontal cortex (Bledowski et al., 2006). In the adequate coverage of inferior head regions, components such
present study, we found roughly similar sources in the parietal as the LIAN or P300 cannot be measured in the full extent of
cortex (both medial and lateral locations, BA 7). Additionally, their volume conduction through head tissues.
we found sources in posterior cingulate cortex, medial
occipital cortex, superior temporal cortex, with the prominent 3.5. Conclusion
source moments in bilateral parahippocampal regions. These
sources were common to the P300 for both digit and spatial Learning is a dynamic and continuous process, wherein
target types. For spatial targets, there were additional sources expertise and automaticity may be the end state. The present
in right parietal and medial frontal cortices (BA 6). results do not address potential changes associated with
Research using fMRI methods have identified increases in extended practice. Rather, the results reveal that the transi-
lingual gyrus (BA 19) and parahippocampal gyrus during tion between not knowing what to do and performing
acquisition of arbitrary visuomotor mappings (Toni and appropriate actions in context (learning) is marked by
Passingham, 1999). Recently, Grol and colleagues (2006) differential decreases in right frontal lobe activation and
found increases in activity in three regions when participants increased activation in the ACC and posterior networks of
overlearn visuomotor mappings: intraparietal sulcus, medial the temporal lobe, PCC, and parietal cortex.
BA 6, and BA 45/46. These brain regions are similar to the P300 Unlike fMRI studies, EEG and ERP methodologies show the
sources we identified in the present study. Grol et al. noted time course of brain activity. The results revealed substantial
that activity in parietal cortex correlated with the degree of temporal overlap between processes indexed by three ERP
automaticity that participants exhibited. These authors sug- components. The LIAN is initiated quite early and appears to
gested that learning related changes observed in the parietal be a continuation of the neural activity that extends along the
cortex might reflect consolidation of procedural memories, extent of the temporal lobe, perhaps reflecting memory
perhaps through slow Hebbian-like plasticity changes. These operations in occipitotemporal networks. The strong activa-
findings may be consistent with the notion that automaticity tion of inferior frontal and anterior temporal regions (at the
in learning continues to engage activity in parietal regions peak of the LIAN) overlaps temporally with ACC (MFN), PCC,
(Chein and Schneider, 2005). medial temporal, and parietal activations. The data also
In the present study, as participants gained increasing suggest that the course of learning related to the processes
skill with the code learning tasks, they showed a correspond- regulated by these structures is quite different. Activity
ing increase in the amplitude of the P300. Learning studies recorded from frontal and temporal structures, at least the
BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 1 7 9 ( 2 00 7 ) 8 9 –1 05 103

right-hemisphere response associated with spatial targets, 4.2. Tasks


decrease at a faster rate than activity in the ACC. It remains to
be seen whether left-hemisphere activity (indexed by the Each participant completed two studies, the order of which
LIAN) and ACC activity associated with conflict monitoring was counterbalanced. The time lapse between studies was not
would decrease, as expected, with extended practice. greater than seven days. Both studies were identical with
These electrophysiological signs of early learning pro- regards to event timeline and stimulus duration (see Fig. 13).
cesses are followed by a gradual development of the P300, Each trial began with a presentation of a target (1500 ms max
reflecting activation within medial and posterior networks of duration). Targets were randomly presented, with the con-
the temporal lobe, as well as associated networks of the straint that the same target could not occur on consecutive
posterior cingulate and parietal cortices. The involvement of trials. The target was terminated when participants made a
these posterior cortical structures mark the transition to the response or 1500 ms elapsed. After target termination, a
later stage of learning and perhaps the onset of automaticity. feedback stimulus was presented to inform participants of the
status of their response. There were six possible forms of
feedback: (1) ErrorGo (error of omission in response to go
4. Experimental procedures target), (2) ErrorNG (error of commission in response to nogo
target), (3) Correct (correct response to go target but response
4.1. Participants committed with wrong hand), (4) CorrectH (correct response
with correct hand to go target but response committed with
Eleven undergraduates students were recruited from the wrong finger), (5) CorrectNG (correct withholding of response
University of Oregon Psychology Department subject pool. to nogo target), and (6) CorrectF (correct response with correct
Prior to participation in the study, all participants gave inform hand and finger to go target). The feedback was presented for a
consent. The study was approved by EGI and the University of maximum duration of 10 s, unless terminated by the subject
Oregon's institutional review boards. Participants were paid with a button press. The inter-trial interval varied between
$15 for their participation and an additional amount, ranging 1500 and 2500 ms. There was a total of 800 trials for each task
between $25 and $45, depending on task performance. On and the trails were grouped into 100 trial blocks.
average participants earned $40. All participants had normal Participants were informed that correct feedback (Cor-
or corrected-to-normal vision and none reported taking rectNG and CorrectF) results in 8 points earned, Error feedback
medications or drugs. The participants were all right handed (ErrorGo and ErrorNG) results in 8 points lost, and partial
and five were men. Their age ranged between 19 and 32 correct feedback, Correct and CorrectH, result in loss of 4 and 2
(M = 24.54, SD = 4.5). points, respectively. Participants started the study with zero
Prior to application of the 128-channel HydroCel Geodesic points. To motivate participants to learn the task, participants
Sensor Net (Electrical Geodesics, Inc., Eugene, OR), participants were informed that each feedback also denote points earned
completed several brief mood questionnaires. After prepara- or lost and that their goal should be to earn as many points as
tion for EEG recording, participants were seated 55 cm in front possible because they will be paid a monetary bonus according
of a computer monitor. Distance from the monitor and to the amount of points they accumulate by the end of the
alignment were controlled by a fixed chin rest. The chin rest study. In between each block, participants took a brief break
also helped to minimize head movements. and recorded their accumulated points on a form.

Fig. 13 – Diagram of events within a single trial for the digit and spatial tasks.
104 BR A IN RE S E A RCH 1 1 79 ( 20 0 7 ) 8 9 –1 05

4.2.1. Digit task at 250 s/s. Blink-contaminated EEG intervals were corrected in
In the digit task, target stimuli were two-digit numbers the continuous record prior to segmentation of the data (Ille
(courier new bold font type, 18 font size). A target replaced et al., 2002).
the fixation mark at the start of each trial. Participants were The data were then segmented relative to target onset
informed that during the course of the study they will be (400 ms before and 1000 ms after) and sorted according to pre
presented with a total of 16 two-digit numbers and that their and post-learning (see Results section). Each segment of the
task is to learn, through trial and error, which numbers EEG was excluded from signal averaging if 10 or more
required a response and which numbers required no response. channels contain data that exceed a voltage threshold of
For those that required a response, they were instructed to 200 μV (absolute) or a transition threshold of 100 μV (sample
learn which hand and finger they should use to press a button. to sample). After averaging, the data were re-referenced to
Eight of the two-digit numbers were go targets. Participants the average reference. Only go targets are examined because
responded with the middle and index finger of each hand (two we are specifically interested in elucidating the neural
go targets were mapped onto each finger). mechanisms responsible for visuomotor mapping. Errors
Prior to the actual task, participants were provided with 32 were not analyzed because not all subjects had enough pre-
practice trials (16 go trials). In the practice trials, only four (two and post-learning trials for ERP averages. Overall, all error
go and two nogo targets) two-digit numbers were presented types made up 41% of trials for the digit task and 33% for the
and these were mapped onto left or right hand only (i.e., spatial task.
participants did not have to determine the finger mapping).
The practice targets are unique in that they are not repeated
during the actual study. The practice trial was to orient Acknowledgments
participants to the trial-and-error learning nature of the task.
All participants were able to learn the response mapping by This project was supported by the Office of Naval Research,
the end of the practice session. “Cognitive Performance Enhancement” and VIRTE pro-
grams, DARPA contract W31P4Q-04-C-R250, and NIMH grant
4.2.2. Spatial task MH070911.
Spatial locations were created by spacing a dot every 22.5°
around a fixation mark (radius of ∼ 4 cm) for a total of 16
locations. Based on participants distance from the monitor, REFERENCES
the dots were presented approximately 4.2° from fixation.
Participants were informed that during the course of the
Badre, D., Poldrack, R.A., Paré-Blagoev, J., Insler, R.Z., Wagner, A.D.,
study, a dot will be presented in eight different locations3 (four
2005. Dissociable controlled retrieval and generalized selection
go and four nogo locations randomly selected from 16 mechanisms in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuron 47,
locations) around fixation and that their task is to learn, 907–918.
through trial and error, which location required a response Barceló, F., Muñoz-Céspedes, J.M., Pozo, M.A., Rubia, F.J., 2000.
and which location required no response. For those that Attentional set shifting modulates the target p3b response in
required a go response, they were instructed to learn which the Wisconsin card sorting test. Neuropsychologia 38,
1342–1355.
hand and finger they should use to press a button. Participants
Bledowski, C., Kadosh, K.C., Wibral, M., Rahm, B., Bittner, R.A.,
responded with the middle and index finger of each hand (one
Hoechstetter, K., et al., 2006. Mental chronometry of working
go target was mapped onto each finger). memory retrieval: a combined functional magnetic resonance
Prior to the actual task, participants were provided with 32 imaging and event-related potentials approach. J. Neurosci. 26,
practice trials (16 go trials). In the practice trials, four (two go 821–829.
and two nogo targets) target locations were presented and Botvinick, M.W., Cohen, J.D., Carter, C.S., 2004. Conflict monitoring
these were mapped onto left or right hand only (i.e., and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8,
539–546.
participants did not have to determine the finger mapping).
Bunge, S.A., Kahn, I., Wallis, J.D., Miller, E.K., Wagner, A.D., 2003.
The practice targets are unique in that they are not repeated Neural circuits subserving the retrieval and maintenance of
during the actual study. All participants were able to learn the abstract rules. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 3419–3428.
response mapping by the end of the practice session. Bussey, T.J., Muir, J.L., Everitt, B.J., Robbins, T.W., 1996. Dissociable
effects of anterior and posterior cingulate cortex lesions on the
4.3. EEG recording acquisition of a conditional visual discrimination: facilitation
of early learning vs. Impairment of late learning. Behav. Brain
The EEG was acquired using a 128-channel HydroCel Geodesic Res. 8245–8256.
Bussey, T.J., Wise, S.P., Murray, E.A., 2001. The role of ventral and
Sensor Net (Electrical Geodesics, Inc., Eugene, OR). All electro-
orbital prefrontal cortex in conditional visuomotor learning
des were kept below 70 KΩ (Ferree et al., 2001). All recordings and strategy use in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behav.
were referenced to Cz. The EEG was bandpass filtered (0.1– Neurosci. 115, 971–982.
100 Hz) and sampled with a 16-bit analog-to-digital converter Chein, J.M., Schneider, W., 2005. Neuroimaging studies of
practice-related change: fMRI and meta-analytic evidence of a
domain general control network for learning. Cogn. Brain Res.
3
In pilot studies, it was determined that participants found the 25, 607–623.
task to be very difficult when 16 target positions (all positions) Dehaene, S., Posner, M.I., Tucker, D.M., 1994. Localization of a
were employed. To equate task difficulty with the digit task, we neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychol.
had to reduce the target locations to eight. Sci. 5, 303–305.
BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 1 7 9 ( 2 00 7 ) 8 9 –1 05 105

Dien, J., Spencer, K.M., Donchin, E., 2003. Localization of the temporal–parietal cortex activation during target detection.
event-related potential novelty response as defined by NeuroReport 8, 3029–3037.
principal components analysis. Cogn. Brain Res. 17, 637–650. Miller, E.K., Cohen, J.D., 2001. An integrative theory of prefrontal
Donchin, E., Coles, M.G.H., 1988. Is the p300 component a cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202.
manifestation of context updating? Behav. Brain Sci. 11, Poldrack, R.A., Sabb, F.W., Foerde, K., Tom, S.M., Asarnow, R.F.,
357–374. Bookheimer, S.Y., et al., 2005. The neural correlates of motor
Ferree, T., Eriksen, K.J., Tucker, D.M., 2000. Region head tissue skill automaticity. J. Neurosci. 25, 536–5364.
conductivity estimation for improved EEG analysis. IEEE Trans. Posner, M.I., DiGirolamo, G., 1998. Executive attention: conflict,
Biomed. Eng. 47, 1584–1592. target detection and cognitive control. In: Parasuraman, R.
Ferree, T., Luu, P., Russel, J.S., Tucker, D.M., 2001. Scalp electrode (Ed.), The Attentive Brain. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp. 401–423.
impedance, infection risk, and EEG data quality. Clin. Raichle, M.E., Fiez, J.A., Videen, T.O., MacLeod, A.-M.K., Pardo, J.V.,
Neurophysiol. 112, 536–544. Fox, P.T., et al., 1994. Practice-related changes in human brain
Fitts, P.M., 1964. Perceptual–motor skill learning. In: Melton, A.W. functional anatomy during nonmotor learning. Cereb. Cortex 4,
(Ed.), Categories of Human Learning. Academic Press, 8–26.
New York, pp. 243–285. Rescorla, R.A., Wagner, A.R., 1972. A theory of pavlovian
Fox, M.D., Corbetta, M., Snyder, A.Z., Vincent, J.L., Raichle, M.E., conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement
2006. Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human and nonreinforcement. In: Black, A.H., Prokasy, W.F. (Eds.),
dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 103, Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory.
10046–10051. Appleton-Century-Corfts, New York, pp. 64–99.
Gabriel, M., 1993. Discriminative avoidance learning: a model Schneider, W., Shiffrin, R.M., 1977. Controlled and automatic
system. In: Vogt, B.A., Gabriel, M. (Eds.), Neurobiology of the human information processing: I. Detection, search, and
Cingulate Cortex and Limbic Thalamus. Birkhäuser, Boston, attention. Psychol. Rev. 84, 1–66.
pp. 478–523. Shiffrin, R.M., Schneider, W., 1977. Controlled and automatic
Gabriel, M., Burhans, L., Talk, A., Scalf, P., 2002. Cingulate cortex. human information processing: II. Perceptual learning,
In: Ramachandran, V.S. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the Human Brain. automatic attending and a general theory. Psychol. Rev. 84,
Elsevier Science, pp. 775–791. 127–190.
Gehring, W.J., Willoughby, A.R., 2002. The medial frontal cortex Simons, J.S., Spiers, H.J., 2003. Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe
and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science interactions in long-term memory. Nat. Rev., Neurosci. 4,
295, 2279–2282. 637–648.
Grave de Peralta Menendez, R., Murray, M.M., Michel, C.M., Smith, A.C., Frank, L.M., Wirth, S., Yanike, M., Hu, D., Kubota, Y.,
Martuzzi, R., Gonzalez Andino, S.L., 2004. Electrical et al., 2004. Dynamic analysis of learning in behavioral
neuroimaging based on biophysical constraints. NeuroImage experiments. J. Neurosci. 24, 447–461.
21, 527–539. Toni, I., Passingham, R.E., 1999. Prefrontal-basal ganglia pathways
Grol, M.J., de Lange, F.P., Verstraten, F.A.J., Passingham, R.E., Toni, are involved in the learning of arbitrary visuomotor
I., 2006. Cerebral changes during performance of overlearned associations: a pet study. Exp. Brain Res. 127, 19–32.
arbitrary visuomotor associations. J. Neurosci. 26, 117–125. Toni, I., Ramnani, N., Josephs, O., Ashburner, J., Passingham, R.E.,
Holroyd, C.B., Coles, M.G.H., 2002. The basis of human error 2001. Learning arbitrary visuomotor associations: temporal
processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the dynamic of brain activity. NeuroImage 14, 1048–1057.
error-related negativity. Psychol. Rev. 109, 679–709. Tucker, D.M., Luu, P., 2006. Adaptive binding. In: Zimmer, H.,
Ille, N., Berg, P., Scherg, M., 2002. Artifact correction of the ongoing Mecklinger, A., Lindenberger, U. (Eds.), Binding in Human
EEG using spatial filters based on artifact and brain signal Memory: A Neurocognitive Approach. Oxford University Press,
topographies. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 19, 113–124. New York, pp. 85–113.
Inase, M., Li, B.-M., Takashima, I., Iijima, T., 2006. Cue familiarity is Tucker, D.M., Hartry-Speiser, A., McDougal, L., Luu, P., deGrandpre,
represented in monkey medial prefrontal cortex during D., 1999. Mood and spatial memory: emotion and the right
visuomotor association learning. Exp. Brain Res. 168, hemisphere contribution to spatial cognition. Biol. Psychol. 50,
281–286. 103–125.
Keng, E., Gabriel, M., 1998. Hippocampal modulation of van der Graaf, F.H.C.E., Maguire, R.P., Leenders, K.L., de Jong, B.M.,
cingulo-thalamic neuronal activity and discriminative 2006. Cerebral activation related to implicit sequence learning
avoidance learning in rabbits. Hippocampus 8, 491–510. in a double serial reaction time task. Brain Res. 1081, 179–190.
Luu, P., Tucker, D.M., 2003. Self-regulation and the executive Williams, Z.M., Bush, G., Rauch, S.L., Cosgrove, G.R., Eskander, E.N.,
functions: electrophysiological clues. In: Zani, A., Preverbio, 2004. Human anterior cingulate neurons and the integration of
A.M. (Eds.), The Cognitive Electrophysiology of Mind and monetary reward with motor responses. Nat. Neurosci. 7,
Brain. Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 199–223. 1370–1375.
Luu, P., Tucker, D.M., Derryberry, D., Reed, M., Poulsen, C., 2003. Wise, S.P., Murray, E.A., 2000. Arbitrary associations between
Electrophysiological responses to errors and feedback in the antecedents and actions. Trends Neurosci. 23, 271–276.
process of action regulation. Psychol. Sci. 14, 47–53. Yeung, N., Holroyd, C.B., Cohen, J.D., 2005. ERP correlates of
Menon, V., Ford, J.M., Lim, K.O., Glover, G.H., Pfefferbaum, A., 1997. feedback and reward processing in the presence and absence
Combined event-related fMRI and EEG evidence for of response choice. Cereb. Cortex 15, 535–544.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi