Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 9

Journal of Adolescent Health 40 (2007) 266 –274

Original article

The Effectiveness of the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program


in Public Middle Schools: A Controlled Trial
Nerissa S. Bauer, M.D., M.P.H.a,*, Paula Lozano, M.D., M.P.H.a,
and Frederick P. Rivara, M.D., M.P.H.a,b
a
Department of Pediatrics, Child Health Institute, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington
b
Department of Epidemiology, Child Health Institute, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington
Manuscript received June 22, 2006; manuscript accepted October 2, 2006

Abstract Purpose: To examine the effectiveness of a widely disseminated bullying prevention program.
Methods: A nonrandomized controlled trial with 10 public middle schools (7 intervention and 3
control) was conducted. Student-reported relational (e.g., spreading rumors, social exclusion) and
physical victimization, and whether the program improved student attitudes and perceptions toward
bullying were assessed pre- and post-implementation using available school survey data.
Results: Regression analyses controlling for baseline prevalence and school characteristics showed
no overall effect on student victimization. However, when stratified by ethnicity/race, reports of
relational and physical victimization decreased by 28% (RR ⫽ .72, 95% CI: .53–.98) and 37% (RR
⫽ .63, 95% CI: .42–.97), respectively, among white students relative to those in comparison schools.
No similar effect was found for students of other races/ethnicities; there were no differences by
gender or by grade. Students in intervention schools were more likely to perceive other students as
actively intervening in bullying incidents, and 6th graders were more likely to feel sorry and want
to help victims.
Conclusions: The program had some mixed positive effects varying by gender, ethnicity/race, and
grade but no overall effect. Schools implementing the program, especially with a heterogeneous
student body, should monitor outcomes and pay particular attention to the impact of culture, race and
family influences on student behavior. Future studies of large-scale bullying prevention programs in
the community must be rigorously evaluated to ensure they are effective. © 2007 Society for
Adolescent Medicine. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Bullying; School-based intervention; Prevention; Victimization; Middle schools

Bullying is aggressive behavior marked by an imbalance lies, victims, and bully-victims are at risk for negative
of power occurring repetitively with intent to harm [1,2] and mental health and social outcomes that may persist into
can be either physical (e.g., fighting, pushing) or relational adulthood [5–7].
(e.g., social exclusion, spreading rumors). Bullying is a Without appropriate intervention, bullying behaviors tend
social phenomenon, with each child’s role— bully, victim, to increase and contribute to a negative school environment
bully-victim, or bystander [1,3]— dependent on the situa- [8 –10]. The Olweus Bullying Prevention Program (OBPP)
tion. Children who either join in bullying or observe without was designed to improve peer relations and promote a safe
trying to stop it reinforce the bully’s behavior [4]. Children and positive school environment by fostering school-wide
who refrain from intervening are termed bystanders. Bul- awareness of bullying [8]. Program core components target
school-, classroom-, individual-, and community-level in-
terventions and include regular discussions about anti-
*Address correspondence to: Nerissa S. Bauer, M.D., M.P.H., Indiana
University, Department of General & Community Pediatrics, 1001 West
bullying rules and other activities designed to engage stu-
10th Street, Wishard-Bryce Building, B2007, Indianapolis, IN 46202. dents, with the long-term goal of changing student attitudes
E-mail address: nsbauer@iupui.edu and perceptions surrounding bullying. The original study of
1054-139X/07/$ – see front matter © 2007 Society for Adolescent Medicine. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.10.005
N.S. Bauer et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 40 (2007) 266 –274 267

Figure 1. Study design and implementation timeline of the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program. *One of seven intervention schools implemented program
in 2004 –2005 academic year.

OBPP involving 2500 students in 42 schools in Norway requiring all middle schools to implement anti-bullying pol-
reported a 50% reduction in student bullying behavior 2 icies and measures. We chose to limit our sample to middle
years after implementation [8]. Since then, other studies, schools because the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program
including one in U.S. rural schools [11], using interventions was originally targeted for this age range. School leaders
modeled after or replicating the OBPP, have found mixed decided how they would satisfy the mandate, thus random-
results [12–14]. Nonetheless, the OBPP has been dissemi- ization was not possible. Seven schools elected to imple-
nated widely in the United States and abroad. ment the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program and the
In 2003–2004, as part of a system-wide focus on high- remainder pursued less formal activities (Figure 1).
risk youth [15], leaders from 7 Seattle middle schools
(grades 6 – 8) implemented the OBPP to address bullying Procedures
and victimization. Three remaining middle schools chose We assessed the type and extent of bullying prevention
less formal activities for bullying prevention in the same efforts in OBPP intervention and comparison schools. With
district. Concurrently, in the fall of 2002, a state-level man- the help of district trainers overseeing the OBPP, we invited
date required all schools to implement anti-bullying school- key informants from each middle school to participate in
level policies by August 2003. In 2004, the school district in-depth interviews. In 2002, the district proactively inte-
requested a program evaluation to assess its effectiveness grated specific questions from the Olweus survey verbatim
and identify areas for improvement for future adaptation of into existing school climate surveys to learn more about the
the program in other schools. This afforded us the unique prevalence of bullying. As this survey was administered to
opportunity to evaluate the OBPP in a nonrandomized trial all schools, regardless of participation in the OBPP, it pro-
1–2 years after implementation. The objective of our study vided us the ability to determine the effectiveness of the
was two-fold: (1) to characterize the implementation of the OBPP. We used all available annual school climate survey
OBPP in the seven schools, and (2) to compare schools with data from the Olweus indicators and select others felt to
(N ⫽ 7) and without (N ⫽ 3) the OBPP to determine if the measure key program target outcomes at two different time
program was effective with regard to: (a) reducing student- points, pre- and post-implementation. Study procedures
reported victimization (primary outcome), (b) improving were approved by the University of Washington Human
student attitudes toward bullying and perceptions of others’ Subjects Protection Committee.
readiness to intervene (key program targets), and (c) im-
proving the general school experience beyond bullying. Measures
Primary outcome (victimization experiences)
Methods Students were asked to respond to four questions regard-
ing relational and physical bullying victimization. Ques-
Sample
tions were from the Revised Olweus Bully/Victim Ques-
We conducted a controlled trial with a cohort of 10 tionnaire [8,16] as shown in Figure 2. The section was
middle schools (grades 6 – 8) following a statewide mandate prefaced by the following general question, “Have you been
268 N.S. Bauer et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 40 (2007) 266 –274

Figure 2. Survey questions.

bullied at school in the past couple of months in one or more General school experiences
of the following ways?” Students responded on the fre- Additional questions related to the perception of safety,
quency of each of these events using a five-point scale. support, and school engagement were selected to measure
Responses were dichotomized using the cut-off of “2–3 the general school experience beyond bullying. In the
times a month” or greater to capture the repetitive nature present study, one indicator (“I feel safe in my classroom”)
specific to the definition of bullying, consistent with prior was chosen to measure the perception of safety. There were
literature [8,16 –18]. The two relational and two physical seven variables that related to the students’ perception of
indicators were collapsed into separate composite measures. school support. A factor analysis was conducted and re-
If a student responded positively to at least one of the pair vealed all seven variables loaded on one factor, “being
of indicators, the student was coded as experiencing rela- supported.” These 7 variables explained 42% of the vari-
tional or physical victimization, respectively. ance. A new composite variable was created so that a
positive response to any of the seven indicators resulted in
Secondary outcomes the student being coded as feeling supported. School en-
Selected questions were chosen from the student climate gagement was measured by one indicator.
survey to measure student attitudes, perceptions of others’
readiness to intervene, and school experience (Figure 2). Intervention
The 10 schools were assigned to 1 of 2 groups based on
Program targets whether they elected to implement the OBPP (intervention
As the program objective was to reduce student bullying group) or pursued less formal prevention efforts (compari-
by increasing awareness, we wanted to measure students’ son group). Each intervention school underwent consulta-
pro-victim attitudes. To examine the desired program effect tion by district trainers prior to implementation.
of shifting more students’ attitudes towards wanting to help
victims, we dichotomized this indicator at “feel sorry and Covariates
want to help.” Perceptions of others’ readiness to intervene Variables correlated within a particular school were used
were measured by two separate questions, one for the actions in the statistical analyses to account for interschool varia-
of other students and the other for actions of teachers or other tions. In this study, school size, percent of students eligible
adults. Responses were dichotomized so that “sometimes” or for free/reduced lunch, and percent of students meeting state
greater was considered to be a positive response. standards for a reading achievement test were obtained from
N.S. Bauer et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 40 (2007) 266 –274 269

Table 1
Definitions of “Olweus Standards” for core component implementation*
Component Olweus Standard definition

School-wide
1. OBPP survey 1. Administer survey & results shared with school
2. School assembly 2. Official implementation start date. Show commitment to bullying prevention, introduce
concepts & school rules, raise enthusiasm
3. Student supervision 3. Schools should revise problem spots as identified, intermittently revisit protocol to
ensure adequacy
4. Staff discussions 4. Hold regular meetings to discuss problematic issues, with goal of fostering collaboration
in implementation efforts
5. Coordinating committee 5. Identify core group of people responsible for initial planning & oversight of
implementation
6. School rules 6. Set common language & expectation for student behavior
7. Engaging parents 7. If engage parents, expectations & follow-through of consequences of student behavior at
school can occur
8. Engaging students 8. In effort to change student attitudes and perceptions, involve students in activities to
raise awareness. Target students who are otherwise known as bystanders
9. Tracking & identification of “hot spots” 9. Not by one-on-one experience with individual students per se, but by examining patterns
of problem areas with intent to shift supervision as needed
10. Staff training 10. After coordinating committee trained, committee members train remaining adult staff
Classroom
1. Class discussions 1. Regular & consistent discussions of schoolwide rules; teach skills to deal with bullying,
foster empathy for others
2. Reinforcement of school rules 2. Teachers should feel comfortable in intervening in bullying incidents, either by actively
stopping it themselves or at least identifying and reporting to administration/counselors
Community
1. Raise community awareness 1. Public relations and foster OBPP-inspired program development based in the community

* Schools rated on 4-point scale: 0-no implementation, 1-attempted but not to Olweus standard, 2-meets Olweus standard, 3-exceeds Olweus standard.

each school’s annual reports and entered into the database lying prevention) and a study investigator (NB) indepen-
(Table 1). These covariates were predictive of victimization dently coded the 10 schools’ activities across the 3 levels, as
in a previous study [19]. Clustering by schools was not well as, implementation fidelity of individual core compo-
possible because the results became unstable due to the nents based on a 4-point scale (0 ⫽ no effort/activity, 1 ⫽
small number of clusters. attempted but not to Olweus standards, 2 ⫽ meets Olweus
standard, or 3 ⫽ exceeds Olweus standard). Inter-rater re-
Data analysis liability was 87% (113 of the 130 coded observations were
Key informant interviews and analysis in 100% agreement). Sixteen of the 17 discordant observa-
Key informant interviews were conducted by the primary tions were coded within a 1-point difference. Conflicts in
investigator (NB) at each of the schools. Informed consent coding were resolved by consensus. Mean school scores
was obtained prior to the interview. Information concerning were aggregated by group (intervention or comparison) and
the timeline of implementation of any prevention efforts calculated. To test for statistically significant differences
(OBPP for intervention or other methods for comparison between implementation fidelity, t-tests with equal vari-
schools) was ascertained. As many of the OBPP core com- ances were performed to examine differences between im-
ponents are not unique to the program, both intervention and plementation efforts by school-, classroom-, and community-
comparison schools were asked further questions about each level scores.
core component. We were not able to collect information Quantitative analysis
regarding individual-level components, as records of stu-
dent incidents and meetings with parents were not available Because surveys lacked identifying information, individ-
at the time of the interview and not consistently docu- ual students could not be linked to survey responses. Stu-
mented. Each interview lasted approximately 60 minutes. dent data were aggregated by school, and school-level data
School efforts were evaluated with respect to an “Olweus were used in the analyses.
Standard,” to adequately capture teachers’ use of core com- Poisson regression was performed, controlling for the
ponents with the program’s intended regularity and consis- school-level covariates. Baseline frequencies of victimiza-
tency to remain involved with students (Table 1). The Ol- tion were included in the models to adjust for differences at
weus standard was developed from a review of the literature baseline. Separate models were examined for each of the
[8,11]. A blinded coder (an outside expert in the field of secondary outcomes using the same procedures. As bullying
developmental psychology and particular expertise in bul- trends for both gender and age have been documented
270 N.S. Bauer et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 40 (2007) 266 –274

previously [8,12,17,20], we stratified each outcome by gen- forms and marking where the incident took place on a
der and grade level. Given the ethnicity/race diversity in school map. Reporting forms were dropped in designated
these schools, we elected to examine the effect of ethnicity/ locked boxes or specific lockers and reviewed by counselors
race on our outcomes of interest. and/or administration. Schools differed in their tracking of
incidents, with the majority of schools having an informal
Results tracking system in place. One school started an electronic
system by year two. Holding regular staff discussions and
Qualitative data class meetings was difficult for most intervention schools.
Some schools designated specific class periods to discuss
On average, intervention schools implemented more core
components at the school-wide and classroom levels to the school rules against bullying, for role playing, and other
Olweus Standard than comparison schools (school-wide curricular activities. Among intervention schools, two con-
score: mean, 9.1 intervention vs. 2.7 comparison, p ⬍ .05 sistently maintained high cooperation and enthusiasm for
[10 points max]; classroom score: 1.3 intervention vs. 0 the program, whereas the others faced obstacles related to
comparison, p ⬍ .05 [2 points max]). Only one of the funding, effort, and administrative support. One of the com-
intervention schools implemented a community-level com- parison schools elected to start the OBPP in fall of 2004.
ponent by the Olweus Standard by offering bullying pre- This school underwent consultation with district trainers
vention training to businesses surrounding the school to and formed a school coordinating committee but did not get
raise awareness of bullying that may occur to and from the started during the time of the present study and therefore
school. Comparison schools tried implementing some ac- remained classified as a comparison school. The other two
tivities, but without regularity or within the context of a comparison schools were involved with less formal mea-
whole school approach. The greatest sources of variation sures such as peer mediation and curriculum dealing with
within the intervention schools were activities to engage racist attitudes.
parents, methods to keep abreast of bullying incidents, hold-
ing regular staff discussions, and classroom meetings. Most Quantitative analysis–student characteristics at baseline
of the OBPP schools, at a minimum, notified parents about
the start of the OBPP, but only a few held additional events Comparison schools had a higher proportion of African
to actively educate parents. All intervention schools used American students (28% vs. 12%), while the intervention
the OBPP survey data to identify “hot spots” (problematic schools had more white students (40% vs. 23%). Other
areas prone to bullying), but some actively monitored these demographic characteristics were similar between the two
hot spots by having students complete bullying reporting groups (Table 2).

Table 2
Baseline student characteristics and school-level covariates
Characteristic Intervention students Comparison students
N ⫽ 4959 N (%) N ⫽ 1559 N (%)

Female gender 2522 (51) 782 (50)


Ethnicity/Race*
Black/African American 610 (12) 422 (28)*
Hispanic/Latino 362 (7) 102 (7)
Asian 1148 (24) 384 (25)
White 1941 (40) 348 (23)*
Native American 95 (2) 22 (1)
Other 434 (9) 159 (10)
Grade level
6th 1672 (34) 570 (37)
7th 1629 (33) 515 (33)
8th 1588 (32) 449 (29)
Achievement
Mostly As 2241 (47) 661 (44)
Mostly Bs/Cs 2262 (47) 724 (48)
Mostly D/F 274 (6) 116 (8)
School-level covariates Intervention group N ⫽ 7 Comparison group N ⫽ 3
School size 877 (617–1247) 714 (467–994)
% Free lunch 45 (16–69) 56 (36–71)
% Meeting state standards reading 53.8 (38.8–81) 44 (31.9–66.7)
WASL (55.0)

* Test of proportions, p-value ⬍ .001.


N.S. Bauer et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 40 (2007) 266 –274 271

Table 3
Intervention and comparison schools’ student-reported prevalence of student victimization, attitudes and perceptions, baseline and follow-up
Student-reported 2003: Baseline 2005: Follow-up

Comparison Intervention Comparison Intervention


N/total N (%) N/total N (%) N/total N (%) N/total N (%)

Relational victimization* 428/1408 (30.4) 1144/4607 (24.8) 439/1456 (30.2) 1105/4480 (24.7)
Physical victimization* 224/1373 (16.3) 627/4531 (13.8) 254/1448 (17.5) 643/4419 (14.6)
Student attitude N ⫽ 1559 N ⫽ 4959 N ⫽ 1580 N ⫽ 4843
Probably what student deserved 131 (8.4) 513 (10.3) 162 (10.3) 516 (10.7)
Don’t feel much 214 (13.7) 705 (14.2) 217 (13.7) 655 (13.5)
Feel sorry for student 511 (32.8) 1838 (37.1) 513 (32.5) 1556 (32.1)
Feel sorry & want to help 544 (34.9) 1594 (32.1) 575 (36.4) 1773 (36.6)
Students help other students# 44/731 (6) 140/2518 (5.6) 219/1075 (20.4) 769/3193 (24.1)
Teachers help other students# 670/1100 (60.9) 2324/3664 (63.4) 764/1167 (65.5) 2545/3730 (68.2)

* At least 2–3 times per month.


#
Sometimes or more often.

Victimization vene, almost two thirds of the students or greater felt teach-
Middle school students of both groups at baseline re- ers were already actively intervening at baseline among all
ported being victims of relational bullying more frequently schools. Only 6% of students in both intervention and com-
than physical bullying (Table 3). For both types of victim- parison groups felt other students were actively intervening
ization, baseline frequencies in the intervention group were on behalf of student victims at baseline.
lower than in the comparison group. Males consistently
reported more relational victimization at baseline than fe- Multivariable analyses
males (29.5% vs. 20.4% intervention, 34.6% vs. 26.2% Primary outcome: victimization
comparison, p ⱕ .001). This was also true for physical Overall, there was no difference in relational (RR ⫽ .96,
victimization at baseline (18.4% vs. 9.5% intervention, 95% CI: .86 –1.08) or physical (RR ⫽ 1.00, 95% CI: .87–
19.8% and 12.9% comparison, p ⱕ .001). 1.17) victimization reports for the intervention schools ver-
sus comparison schools over the two-year period (Table 4).
Attitudes and perceptions When stratified by ethnicity/race, white students in inter-
Fewer students in intervention schools held pro-victim vention schools were 27.5% less likely to report relational
attitudes and wanted to help others at baseline when com- (RR ⫽ .72, 95% CI: .53–.98) and 36.6% less likely to report
pared to students in comparison schools (34% vs. 39%, p ⱕ physical victimization (RR ⫽ .63, 95% CI: .42–.97) com-
.001). With regard to perceiving others’ readiness to inter- pared to white students in comparison schools. There were

Table 4
Multivariable analyses of primary outcomes and program targets
Student-reported Relational Physical Attitude RR Students help other Teachers help
victimization victimization (95% CI) students RR (95% CI) other students
RR (95% CI) RR (95% CI) RR (95% CI)

Overall 0.96 (0.86–1.08) 1.01 (0.87–1.17) 1.04 (0.94–1.14) 1.21 (1.05–1.40) 0.98 (0.93–1.03)
Gender
Female 1.01 (0.84–1.21) 0.91 (0.71–1.16) 1.07 (0.95–1.20) 1.18 (0.97–1.43) 1.02 (0.95–1.09)
Male 0.94 (0.80–1.09) 1.05 (0.87–1.26) 0.97 (0.82–1.15) 1.30 (1.04–1.62) 0.94 (0.87–1.02)
Ethnicity/Race
Black 0.89 (0.66–1.19) 1.24 (0.88–1.75) 1.13 (0.89–1.44) 1.01 (0.75–1.36) 0.96 (0.85–1.08)
White 0.72 (0.53–0.98) 0.63 (0.42–0.97) 1.20 (0.98–1.48) 1.33 (0.83–2.13) 1.14 (0.93–1.40)
Asian 1.00 (0.77–1.31) 1.07 (0.81–1.43) 0.91 (0.75–1.10) 1.50 (1.13–1.99) 0.95 (0.87–1.03)
Other 0.90 (0.70–1.16) 0.84 (0.61–1.16) 1.00 (0.80–1.24) 1.04 (0.76–1.41) 0.97 (0.86–1.10)
Grade
6th 1.10 (0.90–1.34) 0.88 (0.68–1.14) 1.21 (1.05–1.40) 1.30 (1.05–1.60) 1.02 (0.94–1.10)
7th 0.87 (0.71–1.07) 1.11 (0.85–1.44) 0.82 (0.68–0.99) 1.06 (0.81–1.40) 0.95 (0.87–1.04)
8th 0.89 (0.71–1.11) 0.98 (0.76–1.28) 0.95 (0.79–1.15) 1.16 (0.86–1.57) 0.95 (0.86–1.06)

ⴱAdjusted for school size; % students eligible free lunch, % students passing achievement test and baseline frequencies for each outcome.
Bold: p ⱕ .05.
272 N.S. Bauer et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 40 (2007) 266 –274

no effects for students of other ethnicities/races. No effects effect of OBPP on student attitudes add to scant literature on
were seen when examined by gender or grade. these intermediate outcomes, and contrast with a report
finding no effect of a bullying prevention program on 10- to
Secondary outcomes (program targets): attitudes
12-year-olds’ attitudes [24]. Because questions were gener-
and perceptions
ally phrased and not linked to individual students, we were
There was no difference between the intervention and
not able to assess contextual variables associated with stu-
comparison schools in student attitudes to intervene overall
dent attitudes toward bullying. In general, students possess
(RR ⫽ 1.04, 95% CI: .94 –1.14). When stratified by grade,
pro-victim attitudes. Yet, we cannot conclude the OBPP
6th grade students in the intervention schools were more
was responsible for the difference in perception because
likely to feel sorry for students being bullied and want to
these feelings may have been influenced by gender [25],
help. No effect was found based on gender, ethnicity/race,
ethnicity/race [23,24,26,27], and age of students.
or in the other grades.
Several possibilities exist for our findings. The OBPP
Students in intervention schools were more likely to
was developed in Norway for a relatively homogenous
perceive other students as actively intervening in bullying
population; the program may not translate easily into a
incidents (RR ⫽ 1.21, 95% CI: 1.05–1.40) compared to
multi-ethnic society. Secondly, the major developmental
those in comparison schools. Similar analyses for student
task of adolescence is the formation of one’s identity. In
perception of teachers’ or other adults’ readiness to inter-
schools with a diverse student body, ethnic identity and
vene did not show intervention effects.
attitudes toward others could be influenced by experiences
Secondary outcomes: general school experience with peer groups. One study examining middle school stu-
Perception of support, perception of safety, and school dents’ perception of discrimination and attitudes toward
engagement were not affected by the intervention (data not others found European-Americans held more positive atti-
shown). tudes toward other ethnic groups and reported less discrim-
ination when compared to the attitudes of African Ameri-
Discussion cans, Vietnamese Americans, and Mexican Americans [28].
Therefore, it is quite possible that white students in our
Bullying is a common experience as reported by middle sample held similar beliefs and thus, schools with a higher
school students in our sample. Almost one third of our proportion of white students did not require as intensive or
sample reported being a victim of frequent relational bul- tailored approaches to bullying prevention. It is also possi-
lying in the past couple of months, and a smaller proportion ble that direct consultation to the schools by Olweus was
reported being physically bullied. In this controlled trial, responsible for the treatment effect seen in the original
there was no overall effect of the Olweus Bullying Preven- evaluation, although another study using dedicated consult-
tion Program on student-reported victimization. However, ants did not show an intervention effect [24]. In addition, a
when stratified by ethnicity/race, white students were less variety of factors affect students’ perception and report-
likely to report relational and physical victimization over ing of victimization, including cultural biases, making the
the two-year period. Among intervention schools, we found transfer of a program developed for one culture to another
students were 21% more likely to perceive other students difficult. Our study found post-implementation student-
actively intervening on behalf of student victims. With reported victimization rates remained stable or increased. A
regard to student attitude, 6th graders were 21% more likely recent study found student perception of experienced vic-
to feel sorry for victims and want to help. timization influenced responses to the Olweus Bully/Victim
The prevalence of bullying victimization in our sample, Questionnaire items [29]. Therefore, post-implementation
and the higher prevalence in boys, fall within the range prevalence could be made up of previously unlabeled vic-
reported in the literature [3,14,17,21,22], although there are tims and may be a direct consequence of the district’s effort
differences among these studies in the time period and to raise awareness through bullying prevention policies as
whether overall victimization was reported or separated by increases occurred for both intervention and comparison
type experienced. The lack of overall effect of OBPP in schools. Lastly, we were not able to control for additional
these Seattle middle schools suggests that the OBPP may home and family factors previously linked to student ag-
not be as effective as hoped. Evaluations of OBPP (or gression, such as exposure to intimate partner violence in
OBPP-inspired programs) in Europe, Australia, and Canada the home [30], history of sexual or physical abuse [31], or
have found mixed results [11–13,23,24] but are difficult to harsh parenting [32].
compare due to differences in sampling, design, and anal- As we found a substantial decrease in victimization only
yses. The Norwegian evaluation found a 50% reduction in among white students in our sample, it raises the question of
both types of bullying over 2.5 years [5], but had no com- the overlap between racism and bullying. There are few
parison schools and used pre-post comparisons between studies that examine the issue of racism and children spe-
age-equivalent groups, a design that is subject to historical cifically [28,33]. Racist remarks do not need to be experi-
threats to internal validity. Our findings of the apparent enced repeatedly to result in harm [33]; whereas, the current
N.S. Bauer et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 40 (2007) 266 –274 273

accepted definition of bullying emphasizes the repetitive mixed findings on the program’s effectiveness. Given the
nature of an aggressive act to be considered bullying. More- link between students’ personal experiences with victim-
over, the Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire has only one ization and school avoidance [35], schools should continue
indicator to capture racism (“I was called nasty names about planning specific, structured curriculum and interventions in
my color or race”). Eslea et al found Asian students expe- the classroom. Peer groups are usually aware of the bullying
rience a high prevalence of racism regarding student skin that occurs [36] and are desirable targets for intervention.
color, language and dress, and religious practice [33]. Be- Teaching victim assertiveness and bolstering friendship
cause our sample had a higher proportion of heterogeneity quality among both bullies and victims [37] can serve as
than previous studies, racism could explain some of the two important strategies.
observed variability of effect. A cross-sectional survey con- We acknowledge certain limitations of our study. This
ducted by Romero and Roberts is the only study we are study was a natural experiment and there were measurement
aware of that examined the association between ethnic iden-
issues that we had to account for (establishing the “Olweus
tity, perceived discrimination, and attitudes toward other
standard” and qualitative interviews). Key informant inter-
racial groups among an ethnically diverse sample consisting
views are subject to recall bias, as well as sampling error.
of 3071 middle school students (grades 6 – 8) [28]. In this
One investigator (NB) conducted all the interviews and
study, the authors explored two distinct but related factors
confirmed obtained data through follow-up contact with key
within adolescent ethnic identity— ethnic exploration and
ethnic affiliation. Students who engaged in more ethnic informants to ensure data accuracy and help filling in miss-
exploration or possessed more negative attitudes toward ing timeline information. Reliance on student reports may
others were more likely to perceive more discrimination have led to under-reporting of victimization, although our
[28]. Conversely, students who reported a higher sense of rates are similar or higher than previously published studies
belonging to one’s ethnic group (ethnic affirmation—the [17,22,38]. In addition, from year to year, we had varying
social identity aspect of ethnic identity) were more likely to rates of missing data, due to students not responding to all
hold more positive attitudes toward other groups [28]. More questions. The school climate survey used in the present
research is needed to further the work done by Romero and study did not have an operational definition and may have
Roberts by examining the interplay between ethnic identity, caused students to report on incidents involving another
bullying, and racism among this segment of children. student of equal strength, which does not meet the criteria
In light of the various roles of the family and ethnicity/ for bullying behavior. Self-reported bullying behavior and
race on student behavior and attitudes, we encourage student knowledge about what constitutes bullying was not
schools not to stop implementing the Olweus Bullying Pre- measured, nor did we have observational or cross-informant
vention Program. One reason is that this program is the only reports of bullying victimization. Lastly, our results may not
available bullying prevention program that is comprehen- be generalizable to other schools wishing to implement the
sive and encompasses a whole school approach. Addition- program, given the inherent complexity required for pro-
ally, it is a vehicle for schools to bring about change because gram implementation and historical factors in our study
it establishes a common language and provides the neces- setting.
sary framework for schools to tackle bullying. The fact that In summary, the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program
we found positive effects among white students is encour- had some positive effects varying by gender, ethnicity/race,
aging but, given the likelihood that increased heterogeneity and grade, but no overall effect in these Seattle middle
exists in schools across North America as compared to
schools. Implementation of the OBPP program may lead to
Norway, schools should be aware that the role of ethnicity/
variable differences in effectiveness based on factors related
race and culture must be considered for reasons already
to culture, race, and the influence of the family/home
stated. Moreover, students come from varied family/home
environment. Therefore, schools should be aware of the
environments and schools should work to engage parents to
effectively bring about change in student behavior. influence that home, culture, and society have on student
Implementation efforts of intervention schools were behavior, and tailor preventive measures accordingly. A
broad and encompassed a significant regularity and consis- major implication of our study is for schools with an eth-
tency when compared to efforts of comparison schools. This nically-diverse student body not to stop implementing the
“whole school” approach is the distinguishing feature of the program, but rather encourage the process of ethnic explo-
OBPP from less formal bullying prevention activities. ration to nurture adolescents’ emerging ethnic identity,
Teacher/staff communication and school attention to bully- while advocating the development of tolerance and sensi-
ing problems have been predictive of implementation on the tivity to other ethnic groups. Identification of resources,
school level [34]. These elements are essential to contin- administrative support, and commitment to implementation
ued education and commitment of the staff, parents, and fidelity is an ongoing process. Future studies of large-scale
administration and should remain a priority for schools violence prevention programs in the community need to be
implementing or sustaining the program despite our rigorously evaluated to ensure they are working as intended.
274 N.S. Bauer et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 40 (2007) 266 –274

Acknowledgments [17] Nansel TR, Overpeck M, Pilla RS, et al. Bullying behaviors among
US youth: Prevalence and association with psychosocial adjustment.
This study was supported by a grant from Health Re- JAMA 2001;285:2094 –100.
sources and Service Administration (HRSA) and a National [18] Glew GM, Fan MY, Katon W, et al. Bullying, psychosocial adjust-
Research Service Award (NRSA) Primary Care Research ment, and academic performance in elementary school. Arch Pediatr
Adolesc Med 2005;159:1026 –31.
Fellowship. The authors thank the following people for their
[19] Wilson D. The interface of school climate and school connectedness
contributions to this project: James F. Lymp, Ph.D., with the and relationships with aggression and victimization. J School Health
Child Health Institute for consultation on statistical analy- 2004;74:293–9.
ses; Susan Haws, M.P.H., and Michael Arthur, Ph.D., from [20] Eslea M, Rees J. At what age are children most likely to be bullied at
the Social Development Research Group (SDRG), for their school? Aggressive Behav 2001;27:419 – 429.
input in the development phase of the project; the district [21] Nansel TR, Craig W, Overpeck MD, et al. Cross-national consistency
in the relationship between bullying behaviors and psychosocial ad-
trainers from the Seattle school district and key informants
justment. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2004;158:730 – 6.
from the participating middle schools for their time, effort, [22] DeVoe JF. Student Reports of Bullying: Results from the 2001
and their continued support; and Susan P. Limber, Ph.D., School Crime Supplement to the National Crime Victimization Sur-
from the Institute on Family & Neighborhood Life at Clem- vey, Statistical Analysis Report (NCES 2005-310). In: National De-
son University, for her consultation and for coding the partment for Education Statistics (ed). U.S. Department of Education
qualitative data. IoES.Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office, 2005.
[23] Rigby K, Slee PT. Bullying among Australian school children: Re-
ported behavior and attitudes toward victims. J Soc Psychol 1991;
References 131:615–27.
[24] Stevens V, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Van Oost P. Bullying in Flemish
[1] Olweus D. Bullying at School: Blackwell Publishing, 1993. schools: An evaluation of anti-bullying intervention in primary and
[2] Smith PK, Morita, Y, Junger-Tas J, et al (eds). The Nature of School secondary schools. Br J Educ Psychol 2000;70(Pt 2):195–210.
Bullying: A Cross-National Perspective. New York: Routledge, 1999. [25] Baldry AC. “What about bullying?” An experimental field study to
[3] Haynie DL, Nansel T, Eitel P, et al. Bullies, victims, and bully/victims: understand students attitudes towards bullying and victimization in
Distinct groups of at-risk youth. J Early Adolesc 2001;21:29 – 49. Italian middle schools. Br J Educ Psychol 2004;74(Pt 4):583–98.
[4] Schwartz D, Dodge KA, Coie JD. The emergence of chronic peer [26] Smith PK, Ananiadou K, Cowie H. Interventions to reduce school
victimization in boys’ play groups. Child Dev 1993;64:1755–72. bullying. Can J Psychiatry 2003;48:591–9.
[5] Olweus D. Bully/victim problems in school: Facts and intervention. [27] Stevens V, Van Oost P, De Bourdeaudhuij I. The effects of an
Eur J Psychol Educ 1997;12:495–510. anti-bullying intervention programme on peers’ attitudes and behav-
[6] Juvonen J, Graham S, Schuster MA. Bullying among young adoles- iour. J Adolesc 2000;23:21–34.
cents: The strong, the weak, and the troubled. Pediatrics 2003;112(6 [28] Romero AJ, Roberts RE. Perception of discrimination and ethnocul-
Pt 1):1231–7. tural variables in a diverse group of adolescents. J Adolesc 1998;21:
[7] Kim YS, Koh YJ, Leventhal B. School bullying and suicidal risk in 641–56.
Korean middle school students. Pediatrics 2005;115:357– 63. [29] Theriot MT, Dulmus CN, Sowers KM, et al. Factors relating to
[8] Olweus D. Bullying at school: Basic facts and effects of a school self-identification among bullying victims. Child Youth Serv Rev
based intervention program. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1994;35: 2005;27:979 –94.
1171–90. [30] Bauer NS, Herrenkohl TI, Lozano P, et al. Childhood bullying in-
[9] Patterson GR. Families: Applications of Social Learning to Family volvement and exposure to intimate partner violence. Pediatrics 2006;
Life. Revised. Champaign, IL: Research Press, 1975. 118:e235– 42.
[10] Webster-Stratton C, Mihalic S, Fagan A, et al. Blueprints for Vio- [31] Shields A, Cicchetti D. Parental maltreatment and emotion dysregu-
lence Prevention, Book Eleven: The Incredible Years—Parent, lation as risk factors for bullying and victimization in middle child-
Teacher, and Child Training Series. Boulder, CO: Center for the
hood. J Clin Child Psychol 2001;30:349 – 63.
Study and Prevention of Violence, 2001.
[32] Carney AG, Merrell KW. Bullying in schools: Perspectives on un-
[11] Limber SP, Nation M, Tracy AJ, et al. Implementation of the Olweus
derstanding and preventing an international problem. School Psychol
Bullying Prevention Programme in the Southeastern United States.
Int 2001;22:364 – 82.
In: Smith PK, Pepler D, Rigby K (eds). Bullying in Schools: How
[33] Eslea M, Mukhtar K. Bullying and racism among Asian schoolchil-
Successful can Interventions Be? Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Uni-
dren in Britain. Educ Res 2000;42:207–17.
versity Press, 2004:334.
[12] Eslea M, Smith, PK. The long-term effectiveness of anti-bullying [34] Kallestad JH, Olweus D. Predicting teachers’ and schools’ implemen-
work in primary schools. Educ Res 1998;40:203–18. tation of the Olweus Bullying Prevention Program: A multilevel
[13] Salmivalli C, Kaukiainen A, Voeten M. Anti-bullying intervention: Im- study. Prev Treat 2003;6:23.
plementation and outcome. Br J Educ Psychol 2005;75(Pt 3):465– 87. [35] Astor RA, Benbenishty R, Zeira A, et al. School climate, observed
[14] Limber SP, Nation M, Tracy AJ, et al. Implementation of the Olweus risky behaviors, and victimization as predictors of high school stu-
Bullying Prevention programme in the Southeastern United States. In: dents’ fear and judgments of school violence as a problem. Health
Smith PK, Pepler D, Rigby K (Eds.) Bullying in Schools: How Educ Behav 2002;29:716 –36.
Successful Can Interventions Be? Cambridge: Cambridge University [36] Craig WM, Pepler DJ. Peer processes in bullying and victimization:
Press, 2004, 55–79. An observational study. Except Educ Canada 1995;5:81–95.
[15] Hawkins JD, Catalano, RF, Arthur MW. Promoting science-based [37] Bollmer JM, Milich R, Harris MJ, et al. A friend in need: The role of
prevention in communities. Addict Behav 2002;27:951–76. friendship quality as a protective factor in peer victimization and
[16] Solberg M, Olweus D. Prevalence estimation of school bullying with bullying. J Interpers Violence 2005;20:701–12.
the Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire. Aggressive Behav 2003;29: [38] Genta ML, Menesini E, Fonzi A, et al. Bullies and victims in schools
239 – 68. in central and southern Italy. Eur J Psychol Educ 1996;11:97–110.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi