Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 15

International Perspectives in Psychology:

Research, Practice, Consultation


Causality Orientations and Psychological Well-Being in
Young European and Eurasian Adults
Michael J. Stevens, Petru-Madalin Constantinescu, Hasan Ugur, and Iuliana Constantinescu
Online First Publication, December 8, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ipp0000028

CITATION
Stevens, M. J., Constantinescu, P.-M., Ugur, H., & Constantinescu, I. (2014, December 8).
Causality Orientations and Psychological Well-Being in Young European and Eurasian Adults.
International Perspectives in Psychology: Research, Practice, Consultation. Advance online
publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ipp0000028
International Perspectives in Psychology: Research, Practice, Consultation © 2014 American Psychological Association
2014, Vol. 4, No. 1, 000 2157-3883/14/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ipp0000028

Causality Orientations and Psychological Well-Being in Young


European and Eurasian Adults

Michael J. Stevens Petru-Madalin Constantinescu


The Chicago School of Professional Psychology University of Bucharest

Hasan Ugur Iuliana Constantinescu


Fatih University The Lucian Blaga University
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

We sought to bridge the gap in the literature between general causality orientations and
psychological well-being by examining the relationship between these variables in a
sample of 76 (18 men and 58 women) currently enrolled or recently graduated
European and Eurasian university students between 18 and 35 years old. Participants
were administered the General Causality Orientations Scale, Satisfaction With Life
Scale, and Subjective Vitality Scale. General causality orientations differed in magni-
tude, with autonomy orientation being most prominent and impersonal orientation the
least pervasive. Women were more inclined toward autonomy orientation than were
men. Measures of hedonic (life satisfaction) and eudaimonic (vitality) well-being had
a moderate, positive correlation. Although general causality orientations did not predict
life satisfaction, income along with impersonal and controlled causality orientations
predicted vitality. We situate these findings within self-determination theory and
research, the demographics of the sample, and the larger social and cultural contexts
from which the sample was drawn.

Keywords: causality orientations, eudaimonia, psychological well-being, self-determination theory

Self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci, outcomes, including psychological well-being


2000) has long focused on determinants of psy- (Deci & Ryan, 1991, 2000; Ryan & Deci, 2000,
chological well-being. Early research under- 2001; Ryan et al., 2008; Sheldon & Kasser,
scored the role of intrinsic and self-concordant 2001). These needs are autonomy, competence,
forms of extrinsic motivation in the autonomous and relatedness. The relationship between these
pursuit of goals and activities (Deci & Ryan, needs and psychological well-being was then
1985, 1991; Ryan, Huta, & Deci, 2008; Ryan & examined in terms of the self-enhancing or self-
Deci, 2001). Subsequent research on self- limiting impact on well-being of intrinsic and
determination theory identified three universal extrinsic aspirations, respectively (Ryan et al.,
human needs whose gratification yields optimal 2008). In the current study, we were interested
in investigating the relationship general causal-
ity orientations, defined as enduring personal
characteristics that express individual differ-
ences the perceptions of what directs and sus-
Michael J. Stevens, International Psychology Program, tains behavior (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 1991,
The Chicago School of Professional Psychology; Petru- 2000), and psychological well-being.
Madalin Constantinescu, School of Psychological and Ed-
ucational Sciences, University of Bucharest; Hasan Ugur,
Department of Educational Sciences, Fatih University; Iu- Causality Orientations
liana Constantinescu, School of Social and Human Sci-
ences, The Lucian Blaga University. A key element of self-determination is the
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Michael J. Stevens, The Chicago School of
implicit or explicit perceptions held by individ-
Professional Psychology, 325 North Wells, Chicago, IL uals about the sources of their (a)motivation to
60654. E-mail: michaelstevens@thechicagoschool.edu initiate and regulate action. Self-determination
1
2 STEVENS, CONSTANTINESCU, UGUR, AND CONSTANTINESCU

theory construes these perceptions as enduring Few studies have examined gender differ-
and pervasive causality orientations (Deci & ences in general causality orientations. Admin-
Ryan, 1985, 1991, 2000). Causality orientations istering the General Causality Orientations
reflect either a self-determined internal locus of Scale to undergraduates, Deci and Ryan (1985)
causality or an environmentally determined or found that women were more autonomous than
introjected (e.g., I should do this) external locus men, whereas men leaned more than women
of causality, respectively (Deci & Ryan, 1985, toward controlled orientation. These findings
1991, 2000; Kasser & Ryan, 1996). Actions that were duplicated in a sample of French-Canadian
have an internal locus of causality, or a high undergraduates (Vallerand, Blais, Lacouture, &
level of experienced volition (i.e., self- Deci, 1987), and partially replicated with U.S.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

determination), are called autonomous, and tend high schoolers (Wong, 2000) and Belgian un-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

to be associated with the pursuit of intrinsic dergraduates (Soenens, Berzonsky, Vansteenk-


aspirations and freely chosen and valued extrin- iste, Beyers, & Goossens, 2005), with females
sic goals. Actions having an external locus of reporting higher autonomy orientation than
causality, or are typically experienced as im- males. The sparse research on gender differ-
posed by environmental and/or introjected de- ences in general causality orientations suggests
mands, are said to be controlled, and are likely that women tend to display a more self-
to manifest in compliance with real or imagined determined motivational profile than do men.
expectations (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 1991). There The findings suggest that traditional gender role
is also a third causality orientation characterized distinctions in the organizing principles that
by a lack of motivation (i.e., passive disengage- guide men and women in negotiating everyday
ment) that has been labeled impersonal, and tasks may be changing. Our second research
reflects the self-perception that actions are nei- question, then, concerned whether young Euro-
ther under volitional nor environmental control pean and Eurasian adults would report a simi-
larly gendered pattern in causality orientations
because they are seen as exceeding personal
given the global impact of feminism and mod-
competencies or as having no effect on the
ernization (Inglehart & Baker, 2000). Widening
outcomes.
the effort to identify a gendered pattern of cau-
According to Deci and Ryan (1985, 1991),
sality orientations would also lay the ground-
each individual possesses broad and stable au- work for follow-up research on whether and
tonomous, controlled, and impersonal causality how causality orientations manifest differently
orientations to varying degrees. This perspec- and have different consequences and implica-
tive on the perceived causal determinants of tions for young European and Eurasian men and
(in)action emerged from research which chal- women.
lenged the widely held view that each person
can be assigned to a single, mutually exclusive
Psychological Well-Being
causality orientation (Ryan & Deci, 2001; Wa-
terman, 2008). The General Causality Orienta- As mentioned at the outset, the current study
tions Scale (Deci & Ryan, 1985) measures auton- examined the relationship of general causality
omy, controlled, and impersonal orientations, the orientations to psychological well-being. The
magnitude of which is hypothesized to produce conceptual and empirical literature distin-
substantively distinct forms of affective, cogni- guishes two forms of psychological well-being:
tive, and behavioral responding that yield dif- hedonic and eudaimonic (Huta, 2013a, 2013b;
ferent adaptational outcomes, including psycho- Huta & Ryan, 2010; Ryan & Deci, 2001). He-
logical well-being. Based on the premise that donia and eudaimonia have been variously de-
most individuals are inclined to freely pursue fined as motivated behavior, a subjective feel-
intrinsic and internalized and valued extrinsic ing, or accomplishments and other outcomes
aspirations, our first research question examined (Huta, 2013a, 2013b). Detailing the breadth of
whether autonomy, controlled, and impersonal these definitions, however, is beyond the scope
causality orientations would differ in magnitude of this article. Instead, we highlight how self-
for young European and Eurasian adults, a pop- determination theory, particularly the perspec-
ulation that research on self-determination the- tive of Ryan and Deci (2000, 2001), views these
ory has not studied extensively. constructs. As a trait construct, hedonia empha-
CAUSALITY ORIENTATIONS AND WELL-BEING 3

sizes the comfort and pleasure that individuals suggest that life satisfaction and vitality are
are motivated to seek in their lives (Huta, overlapping constructs, but also that they mea-
2013b; Ryan & Deci, 2001; Ryan et al., 2008). sure benefits common to hedonia and eudaimo-
Unlike eudiamonia, self-determination theory nia (Huta, 2013a). For example, Huta (2013b)
posits that hedonia is linked to a relatively nar- conjectured that hedonia enhances vitality be-
row and time-limited set of well-being out- cause pleasure-seeking may replenish energy
comes (Ryan et al., 2008). Hedonic experience reserves, whereas eudaimonia may promote vi-
has typically been measured in terms of subjec- tality because living well requires active en-
tive well-being, whose constituent elements in- gagement. Replications with diverse samples of
clude the global evaluation of life satisfaction the relationship between life satisfaction and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and the balance between positive and negative vitality as outcomes in research on self-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

affect (Ryan & Deci, 2001). determination theory would support calls for a
Eudaimonia emerged from prior empirical more integrative approach to understanding
studies as a central construct in self-determina- psychological well-being (Henderson & Knight,
tion theory (Ryan et al., 2008). It refers to “a 2012; Huta & Ryan, 2010; Wong, 2011). Our
way of living that is focused on what is intrin- third research question inquired about the rela-
sically worthwhile to human beings” (Ryan et tionship between life satisfaction and vitality in
al., 2008, p. 147), that is, the ingredients and young European and Eurasian adults.
process of living well. Eudaimonia incorporates
the awareness and valuing of intrinsic and in- Causality Orientations in Relation to
ternalized extrinsic goals, volitional and self- Psychological Well-Being
concordant choices, and affect, cognition, and
behavior directed at fulfilling the universal hu- To recap, eudaimonia, or living well, is a key
man needs for autonomy competence, and re- construct of self-determination theory. Psycho-
latedness (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Ryan & Deci, logical well-being is one outcome of eudaimo-
2000, 2001). When defined as living well, eu- nia, and is tied to the satisfaction of fundamen-
daimonia provides a framework for examining tal human needs for autonomy, competence,
the relationship of general causality orientations and relatedness through the pursuit and fulfill-
to psychological well-being. Both eudaimonia ment of consciously chosen intrinsic and/or in-
and general causality orientations address estab- ternalized and valued (i.e., self-concordant) ex-
lished action tendencies that can produce differ- trinsic aspirations (Deci & Ryan, 1991, 2000;
ent adaptational outcomes. Self-determination Ryan & Deci, 2000, 2001; Ryan et al., 2008).
theory specifically posits that autonomy orien- Within self-determination theory, general cau-
tation is a key determinant of psychological sality orientations and eudaimonia share the
well-being (Huta, 2013a, 2013b; Ryan & Deci, view that certain broad and enduring action
2000, 2001). tendencies contribute to psychological well-
As mentioned, the outcomes of hedonic ful- being (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 1991, 2000; Huta,
fillment include conceptually consistent indi- 2013a, 2013b; Ryan & Deci, 2000, 2001). For
cants, such as positive affect and life satisfac- example, Deci and Ryan (1985) found that au-
tion. Outcomes of eudaimonia have also been tonomy orientation was linked to self-actualiz-
measured by life satisfaction along with other ing tendencies and maturity in personality.
indices of living well, including vitality (Ryan However, the relationship of general causality
& Deci, 2001), an important indicant of psycho- orientations to psychological well-being has re-
logical well-being in research on self-determi- ceived relatively little attention outside of the
nation theory. Vitality taps the experience of United States, although variants of autonomy
energy needed to engage fully in life pursuits orientation have consistently predicted psycho-
(Ryan et al., 1999; Ryan & Frederick, 1997). A logical well-being in cross-national research
number of studies have found a positive rela- (Chirkov, 2009; Deci et al., 2001; Ryan,
tionship between life satisfaction and vitality Chirkov, Little, Sheldon, Timoshina, & Deci,
when used to measure well-being in research on 1999; Ryan & Deci, 2001). In a related study
hedonia and eudaimonia (Huta & Ryan, 2010; that extended earlier research (e.g., Schmuck,
Ryan & Deci, 2001; Ryan & Frederick, 1997; Kasser, & Ryan, 2000), Stevens, Constanti-
Waterman, 2008). Not only do these findings nescu, and Butucescu (2011) found that intrin-
4 STEVENS, CONSTANTINESCU, UGUR, AND CONSTANTINESCU

sic aspirations for personal growth, often an education, employment, and social equality as
expression of autonomy orientation (Deci & central to well-being and underscored wide-
Ryan, 1985, 1991), were related to psycholog- spread uncertainty about the future (TNS
ical well-being in U.S. and Romanian university Qual⫹, 2011).
students. Stevens et al. also found that extrinsic We specifically hypothesized the following:
aspirations predicted well-being for Romanian
students when they function as a pathway to life 1. Autonomy (self-determined choice and in-
satisfaction. However, Stevens et al. did not ternally regulated functioning) would be
assess the degree to which extrinsic aspirations the strongest causality orientation, imper-
were experienced as self-concordant. Therefore, sonal (passive disengagement) the weak-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

it remains unclear whether extrinsic aspirations est, and controlled (pressured choice and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

per se and, more importantly, general causality externally regulated functioning) moder-
orientations other than autonomy can produce ately prominent;
psychological well-being in certain social and 2. women would show stronger autonomy
cultural contexts (see Ryan & Deci, 2001 on the orientation than would men;
antecedents of well-being). In a bibliographic 3. life satisfaction and vitality, the psycho-
review, Eiroa Orosa (2013) reported that locus logical well-being outcomes in our study,
of control, perceived control, and self-efficacy would be moderately and positively cor-
beliefs, which are conceptually related to auton- related; and
omy orientation, only partially explained the 4. higher autonomy, lower impersonal, and
impact of macrosocial changes and psycholog- lower controlled causality orientations
ical well-being in Central and Eastern European would predict life satisfaction and vitality.
countries. Our fourth research question centered
on the relationship of autonomy, controlled, and Method
impersonal causality orientations to psycholog-
ical well-being in an understudied group: young This research was conducted in keeping with
European and Eurasian adults. the Deontological Code of the Romanian Col-
lege of Psychologists and Law 213/2004, both
of which delineate principles and standards for
The Current Study the ethical conduct of psychological research.
The main purpose of our study was to bridge Consent was implicitly obtained based on
the conceptual and empirical gap in the litera- whether a person voluntarily chose to complete
ture between general causality orientations and the measures online.
psychological well-being. We also wanted to Participants
examine the relationship of general causality
orientations and psychological well-being in a Participants were a convenience sample of
sample of young adults from Europe and Eur- undergraduate, postgraduate, and recently grad-
asia where there have been few studies of these uated university students from seven countries
and other constructs germane to self-determina- in Europe and Eurasia. Although not represen-
tion theory (e.g., Chirkov, 2009; Deci et al., tative of the region’s population, we believed
2001; Ryan et al., 1999; Stevens et al., 2011). that well-educated, cosmopolitan young adults
Because “it is impossible to understand the would be interested in and capable of respond-
good life apart from various contextual factors” ing insightfully to measures of general causality
(Wong, 2011, p. 77), we were interested in the orientations and psychological well-being.
sources of psychological well-being in young Given their age, we also believed that this group
European and Eurasian adults (Ryan & Deci, would be keenly aware of personal challenges
2001). Although gratification of core human related to career goals, family plans, and life-
needs appears to promote psychological well- style values (Ryan & Deci, 2001; Ryan & La
being throughout life, the manifestation and ful- Guardia, 2000) in the context of prevailing eco-
fillment of these needs depend on age-related nomic, political, and social conditions (TNS
life challenges and affordances (Ryan & La Qual⫹, 2011).
Guardia, 2000). A recent study of the hopes and Once collected, data were checked for miss-
fears of this cohort from EU countries identified ing values, univariate and multivariate outliers,
CAUSALITY ORIENTATIONS AND WELL-BEING 5

and normality of distributions. Missing values Measures


were spread randomly across two scales (Lit-
tle’s MACR: ␹2 ⫽ ⫺0.759, p ⬎ .05) within 12 We measured general causality orientations,
cases and replaced using expectation maximi- life satisfaction, and vitality with the General
zation (Schlomer, Bauman, & Card, 2010). We Causality Orientations Scale (Deci & Ryan,
then cleaned the data of outlier responses.1 We 1985), Satisfaction With Life Scale (Diener,
inspected all scale and subscale scores using a Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985), and Subjec-
normal probability (Q-Q) plot comparison func- tive Vitality Scale (Ryan & Frederick, 1997),
tion and identified 19 outliers whose values respectively. We also devised a background
appeared extreme. From this pool, we removed questionnaire. All measures were in English.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

three multivariate outliers (p ⱕ .001 for Ma- General Causality Orientations Scale
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

halanobis’ D2). We then performed Shapiro- (GCOS). The GCOS (Deci & Ryan, 1985)
Wilk tests which showed that the data met the measures three enduring and pervasive ways in
assumptions of normality. For General Causal- which individuals explain the source of their
ity Orientation–Autonomy, W ⫽ 0.972, p ⬎ affective, cognitive, and behavioral tendencies
.05; for General Causality Orientation–Imper- that in turn guide and regulate their responses.
sonal, W ⫽ 0.985, p ⬎ .05; for General Cau- Autonomy orientation (GCOSA) is related to
sality Orientation–Controlled, W ⫽ 0.985, p ⬎ intrinsic motivation, and reflects the respon-
.05; and for the Subjective Vitality Scale, W ⫽ dent’s receptiveness to conditions and experi-
0.973, p ⬎ .05). No adjustments were made ences that offer opportunities to exert personal
based on responses to the Satisfaction With Life agency. High autonomy includes self-initiation,
Scale. We also eliminated data from partici- enjoyment of challenges, and taking personal
pants whose self-reported proficiency in written responsibility. Impersonal orientation (GCOSI)
English was poor or fair, and who identified refers to the absence of motivation stemming
their nationality as from outside Europe and from the belief that goal attainment exceeds the
Eurasia. respondent’s capability and/or expectations that
The resulting sample (N ⫽ 76) consisted of actions do not reliably lead to outcomes. High
18 men (23.7%) and 58 women (76.3%), be- impersonal orientation is linked to low self-
tween 18 and 35 years old (M ⫽ 25.09, SD ⫽ efficacy, passive disengagement, and a prefer-
5.22), with a monthly income between $0 and ence for sameness. Controlled orientation
$2,927 USD (Mdn ⫽ $320 USD). Seventeen (GCOSC) is tied to extrinsic motivation, and
were married or engaged (22.4%), 58 were reflects responsiveness to environmental and/or
never married or engaged (76.3%), and 1 was introjected imperatives, tangible rewards, and
divorced or widowed (1.3%). Twelve had chil- structure. High controlled orientation entails be-
dren (15.8%) and 64 were childless (84.2%). ing attentive to expectations and demands and
The sample was 69.7% Romanian (n ⫽ 53), sensitive to incentives like money and status.
18.4% Turkish (n ⫽ 14), 5.3% German (n ⫽ 4), GCOSA, GCOSI, and GCOSC subscale
3.9% Albanian (n ⫽ 3), 1.3% Estonian (n ⫽ 1), scores in our study were not correlated, al-
and 1.3% Polish (n ⫽ 1). Seventy-three were though the relationship between autonomy and
postgraduate students (96.1%), with 1 (1.3%) impersonal orientations approached statistical
undergraduate and 2 (2.6%) recently graduated significance (p ⫽ .54; see Table 2). The absence
students. The range of self-reported proficiency of statistically significant correlations between
in written English was as follows: moderate GCOS subscale scores comports with the as-
7.9% (n ⫽ 6), good 28.9% (n ⫽ 22), and very sumption of self-determination theory that cau-
good 63.2% (n ⫽ 48). Given the large number sality orientations are distinct (Olesen, Thom-
of Romanian participants, we were concerned
about the possibility that our sample was cul- 1
We recognize objections to removing potentially legit-
turally biased. t tests comparing responses of imate outliers, particularly in studies that administer mea-
Romanian participants with those from other sures developed in the United States to participants in other
countries. We would respond by noting that only three
nationalities on measures of general causality multivariate outliers, whose legitimacy could not be disam-
orientation and psychological well-being were biguated, were removed before the data analyses to obtain a
not statistically significant (see Table 1). more accurate estimate of population parameters.
6 STEVENS, CONSTANTINESCU, UGUR, AND CONSTANTINESCU

Table 1
Descriptive Data for Romanian and All Other Participants
GCOSA M (SD) GCOSI M (SD) GCOSC M (SD) SWLS M (SD) SVS M (SD)
Romanians (n ⫽ 53) 69.19 (6.651) 43.43 (10.500) 51.38 (9.322) 21.94 (6.614) 32.55 (8.579)
Other nationalities (n ⫽ 23) 68.00 (8.453) 48.26 (11.375) 55.39 (10.183) 23.87 (5.234) 33.91 (9.453)
Total (N ⫽ 76) 68.83 (7.208) 44.89 (10.926) 52.59 (9.701) 22.53 (6.258) 32.96 (8.811)
Note. Degrees of freedom ⫽ 74; GCOSA ⫽ General Causality Orientations Scale–Autonomy; GCOSI ⫽ General
Causality Orientations Scale–Impersonal; GCOSC ⫽ General Causality Orientations Scale–Controlled; SWLS ⫽ Satis-
faction With Life Scale; SVS ⫽ Subjective Vitality Scale.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sen, Schnieber, & Tonnesvang, 2010) and, are comparable with those obtained by Deci and
therefore, their measurement with instruments Ryan (1985).
such as the GCOS should be largely indepen- Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS).
dent (Koestner & Zuckerman, 1994). The SWLS (Diener et al., 1985) is a measure of
The GCOS consists of 12 vignettes which psychological well-being. The SWLS was de-
respondents rate on three 7-point Likert scales veloped out of a need to measure the cognitive
(1 ⫽ very unlikely, 7 ⫽ very likely) that measure evaluation of a person’s satisfaction with life, as
autonomy, impersonal, and controlled orienta- opposed to an ideal standard. The SWLS con-
tions. The 12 ratings given for each causality sists of five items that are rated on a 7-point
orientation are summed to yield a total score, Likert scale (1 ⫽ strongly disagree, 7 ⫽
and indicate how typical each orientation is for strongly agree), with scores above 20 indicating
the respondent, with higher scores denoting the life satisfaction. A sample item reads, The con-
magnitude of each orientation. A sample vi- ditions of my life are excellent. The scale has
gnette reads, You are embarking on a new ca- been used in cross-national studies with univer-
reer. The most important consideration is likely sity students (Pavot & Diener, 1993), with most
to be . . ., with respondents asked how they reporting good to excellent psychometric prop-
would likely react in that situation. The GCOS erties. The internal consistency the SWLS in
has good internal consistency and stability as our study was good (␣ ⫽ .874).
well as empirical ties to related constructs, in- Subjective Vitality Scale (SVS). The SVS
cluding self-actualization, self-esteem, and per- (Ryan & Frederick, 1997) measures functional
sonality development (Deci & Ryan, 1985), and elements of actualizing individuals, including
has been used cross-nationally with university the experience of agency, awareness, and alive-
students (e.g., Olesen et al., 2010). In our study, ness, all of which have been construed as well-
the internal consistency of GCOS subscales being outcomes of eudaimonia, or living well
ranged from acceptable to fair (GCOSA ␣ ⫽ (Ryan & Deci, 2000, 2001). Based on the in-
.661, GCOSI ␣ ⫽ .754, GCOSC ␣ ⫽ .760), and structions, the SVS can serve as an index of a

Table 2
Pearson Correlations for Scale Scores, Subscale Scores, and Demographic Data
GCOSA GCOSI GCOSC SWLS SVS Age Income
GCOSA .220 .054 .197 .134 ⫺.222 .041
GCOSI .163 ⫺.019 ⫺.275ⴱ .093 ⫺.020
GCOSC .000 .210 .061 ⫺.030
SWLS .469ⴱⴱ .112 .260ⴱ
SVS .239 .276ⴱ
Age .474ⴱⴱ
Note. Degrees of freedom ⫽ 74; GCOSA ⫽ General Causality Orientations Scale–Autonomy; GCOSI ⫽ General
Causality Orientations Scale–Impersonal; GCOSC ⫽ General Causality Orientations Scale–Controlled; SWLS ⫽ Satis-
faction With Life Scale; SVS ⫽ Subjective Vitality Scale.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
CAUSALITY ORIENTATIONS AND WELL-BEING 7

stable individual difference variable or a tran- concerns about the study or their experiences,
sient state confined to a specified situation. We and were assured of receiving an interpretation
had participants complete the SVS in terms of of their SWLS scores had they requested it (two
how well each item reflected their general self- participants requested feedback). The measures
perception. The SVS has seven items that are required 20 to 30 minutes to complete, and the
rated with a 7-point Likert scale (1 ⫽ not at all data collection took approximately 2 months.
true, 7 ⫽ very true), with higher scores denoting
greater subjective vitality. A sample item reads Design and Analyses
I am true to myself in most situations. The SVS
appears to be unidimensional (Bostic, Rubio, & The design of our study was correlational.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Hood, 2000), has positive associations with in- We performed a one-way repeated-measures
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

dicators of organismic wellness (Huta, 2013a, ANOVA to determine whether GCOS subscale
2013b; Nix, Ryan, Manly, & Deci, 1999; Ryan means differed in magnitude as predicted by
& Deci, 2001), is positively related to self- self-determination theory. We ran separate one-
actualization and self-esteem and negatively re- way ANOVAs to test for gender differences in
lated to anxiety and depression (Ryan & Fred- GCOSA, GCOSI, and GSOSC subscale scores
erick, 1997; Ryan & Deci, 2001), and has been to discover whether the results of previous re-
used cross-nationally and with university stu- search would hold for our European and Eur-
dents (e.g., Solberg, Halvari, & Ommundsen, asian sample. We calculated Pearson product–
2013). The internal consistency of the SVS in moment correlation coefficients to ascertain the
our study was acceptable (␣ ⫽ .682). association between SWLS and SVS scores, the
Background questionnaire. The back- degree of independence among CGOS subscale
ground questionnaire consisted of multiple- scores, and the zero-order relationships of pred-
choice and open-ended items that inquired icator and criterion scores with selected demo-
about gender, age, monthly income, marital sta- graphic data. Finally, we performed two step-
tus, number of children, nationality, education wise multiple regression analyses with the three
level, and proficiency in written English. GCOS subscale scores as predictors of SWLS
scores or SVS scores. In each analysis, income
Procedure was entered first as a control variable, followed
by the three CGOS subscale scores, whose or-
We recruited participants through e-mail der of entry was determined by the greatest R2
lists, listservs, and social network sites that were increase given the previously entered variables.
student-oriented (e.g., Academia.edu, Face- Our aim was to establish the most parsimonious
book, Google Groups, Yahoo Groups), inviting combination as well as the relative contribution
only undergraduate, postgraduate, and recent of the three general causality orientations in
university graduates to respond. Participants predicting a particular type of psychological
were informed that the study would examine the well-being. We adopted a .05 significance level
relationship of motivation to well-being, that or our analyses based on statistical power of .80
participation would involve competing ques- or above.
tionnaires, that participation was voluntary, that
they could withdraw from the study at will, and Results
that efforts were in place to maintain the confi-
dentiality of their identity and responses. We Inspection of the means for GCOS subscales
administered measures online using Google scores showed differences in magnitude. The
Forms. Participants completed the measures in means ranged from autonomy orientation as
invariant order (i.e., SWLS, GCOS, SVS, back- most pervasive (MGCOSA ⫽ 68.83, SDGCOSA ⫽
ground questionnaire), with instructions to be 7.208), to controlled orientation as moderately
forthright. Participants were offered the oppor- prevalent (M GCOSC ⫽ 52.59, SD GCOSC ⫽
tunity receive feedback on their SWLS scores 9.701), and to impersonal orientation as least
via secure e-mail after all data had been col- prominent (MGCOSI ⫽ 44.89, SDGCOSI ⫽ 10.
lected. After completing the measures, partici- 926). A one-way repeated measures ANOVA of
pants were given the e-mail address of the sec- GCOS subscale scores was statistically signifi-
ond author to contact if they had questions or cant, F(1, 75) ⫽ 318.476, p ⬍ .001, ␩2 ⫽ 0.809,
8 STEVENS, CONSTANTINESCU, UGUR, AND CONSTANTINESCU

which is a large effect. A Least Significant erately correlated, r(74) ⫽ 0.469, p ⬍ .001,
Difference follow-up test revealed that each 95% CI [.282 – .632], just shy of a large
GCOS subscale mean differed from every other effect. This association supports our third hy-
GCOS subscale mean at p ⬍ .001, supporting pothesis and is consistent with consistent with
our first hypothesis. a growing literature (Henderson & Knight,
We compared the autonomy, controlled, and 2012; Huta, 2013a, 2013b; Huta & Ryan,
impersonal causality orientations of men and 2010; Ryan & Deci, 2001; Ryan & Frederick,
women. A one-way ANOVA of gender on 1997; Waterman, 2008).
GCOSA subscale scores was statistically signif- Because income was positive correlated with
icant, F(1, 74) ⫽ 7.438, p ⫽ .008, d ⫽ 0.736, SWLS scores, r(74) ⫽ .260, p ⫽ .023, 95% CI
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

which is a large effect, with women reporting [.089 – .412], and SVS scores, r(74) ⫽ .276,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

higher GCOSA subscale scores (M ⫽ 70.03, p ⫽ .016, 95% CI [.066 – .466] (see Table 2),
SD ⫽ 7.162) than men (M ⫽ 64.94, SD ⫽ we entered income as a control variable in both
6.024). A statistically significant gender differ- regression analyses. The stepwise multiple re-
ence did not emerge on GCOSC subscale gression analysis of GCOS subscale scores on
scores, F(1, 74) ⫽ 2.598, p ⫽ .111; Mwomen ⫽ SWLS scores was not statistically significant,
51.60, SDwomen ⫽ 9.878; Mmen ⫽ 55.78, SDmen ⫽ with no GCOS subscale scores entered into the
8.599). Together, these findings partially con- regression equation; furthermore, there were no
firmed our second hypothesis. Finally, a gender statistically significant zero-order correlations
difference did not emerge on GCOCI subscale between GCOS subscale scores and SWLS
scores, F(1, 74) ⫽ 0.345, p ⫽ .559; Mwomen ⫽ scores (see Table 2).
44.48, SDwomen ⫽ 10.562; Mmen ⫽ 46.22, SVS scores had a moderate negative correlation
SDmen ⫽ 12.255). Nonsignificant Levene’s tests with GCOSI subscale scores, r(74) ⫽ ⫺.275, p ⫽
allayed concern about unequal variances in the .016, 95% CI [⫺.483 – ⫺.031], and a positive
three sets of GCOS subscale scores given un- association with GCOSC subscale scores that fell
equal numbers of men and women. Because close to statistical significance, r(74) ⫽ .210, p ⫽
unequal samples can distort p values, we com- .069, 95% CI [⫺.011 – ⫺.401]; the association
pared the GCOS subscale scores of a random between SVS and GCOSA scores was not statis-
sample of 18 women from the total of 58 to the tically significant (see Table 2). With income en-
scores of the 18 men. The three ANOVAs tered as a control variable, the stepwise multiple
yielded similar results to those reported above, regression analysis of GCOS subscale scores on
with a slightly higher p value, F(1, 34) ⫽ 4.761, SVS scores was statistically significant (see Table
p ⫽ .036, d ⫽ 0.727; women produced higher 3), with income, GCOSI subscale scores, and
GCOSA subscale scores (M ⫽ 69.44, SD ⫽ GCOSC subscale scores together accounting for a
6.345) than did men (M ⫽ 64.94, SD ⫽ 6.024). 18.7% of the variance of SVS scores, R2 ⫽ 0.187,
We correlated SWLS and SVS scores, F(3, 72) ⫽ 6.733, p ⫽ .001, f2 ⫽ 0.230, a mod-
which served as indicants of psychological erate overall effect. Income was a positive predic-
well-being. SWLS and SVS scores were mod- tor of SVS scores, ⌬R2 ⫽ .076, F(1, 74) ⫽ 6.101,

Table 3
Stepwise Multiple Regression Analysis Predicting SVS Scores From GCOS
Subscale Score
SVS
Model B SE (B) ␤ t 95% CI
Income .004 .002 .278 2.665ⴱⴱ .001 – .008
GCOSI ⫺.252 .085 ⫺.313 ⫺2.965ⴱⴱ ⫺.422 – ⫺.083
GCOSC .244 .096 .269 2.547ⴱ .053 – .435
Note. GCOS ⫽ General Causality Orientations Scale; GCOSA ⫽ General Causality Orienta-
tions Scale–Autonomy; GCOSI ⫽ General Causality Orientations Scale–Impersonal; GCOSC ⫽
General Causality Orientations Scale–Controlled; SVS ⫽ Subjective Vitality Scale.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.
CAUSALITY ORIENTATIONS AND WELL-BEING 9

p ⫽ .016, f2 ⫽ .082. GCOSI scores negatively orientation (Soenens et al., 2005; Wong, 2000)
predicted SVS scores, ⌬R2 ⫽ .070, F(1, 73) ⫽ or impersonal orientation. Our findings are
6.222, p ⫽ .015, f2 ⫽ 0.078. And, GCOSC scores noteworthy in that this is the only study to test
positively predicted SVS scores, ⌬R2 ⫽ .071, F(1, for gender differences in general causality ori-
72) ⫽ 6.486, p ⫽ .013, f2 ⫽ 0.076. These findings entations in an adult European and Eurasian
failed to support most of our fourth hypothesis sample. Cross-national evidence for a gendered
about the relationship of general causality orien- pattern in autonomy orientation suggests that
tations to psychological well-being. We also did women have acquired a stronger perception of
not anticipate the direction of the controlled cau- their own capacity for self-determination,
sality orientation that explained vitality. whereby they intentionally choose and freely
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

pursue intrinsic goals and/or self-concordant


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Discussion extrinsic goals. Notwithstanding evidence of


consistency in autonomy orientation across age
We endeavored to narrow the gap in the (Ryan & La Guardia, 2000), we believe it is
literature between general causality orientations premature to conclude that autonomy orienta-
and psychological well-being by examining the tion is not malleable within limits defined by
relationship of these variables in a sample of biological predisposition. Rather, we submit
young European and Eurasian adults. that this consistent gender difference in auton-
omy orientation may also reflect women’s de-
Causality Orientations velopmental history and current social and cul-
tural affordances (Ryan & Deci, 2001; Ryan et
We confirmed our first hypothesis that gen- al., 2008). The gendered pattern of results we
eral causality orientations would differ in mag- obtained, then, could reveal the effects of laws,
nitude, with autonomy orientation emerging as policies, education, and parenting (Joussemet,
predominant, controlled orientation as moder- Landry, & Koestner, 2008) that have supported
ately strong, and impersonal orientation as least the development of autonomy orientation in
pervasive. This pattern of means parallels those women the world over, including young, edu-
obtained by Olesen et al. (2010) and Deci and cated European and Eurasian women. Global-
Ryan (1985) for university students and engi- ization has likely accelerated the impact of fem-
neers. The large effect size for the repeated inism and raised women’s awareness of and
measures ANOVA bolsters the case for the willingness to express their inherent freedom to
cross-national generalizability of a major as- determine their own destiny (Inglehart & Baker,
sumption undergirding self-determination the- 2000). This interpretation comports with evi-
ory that self-initiated choices and internally reg- dence of high awareness among young Euro-
ulated functioning may be a stronger causality pean women about social inequality generally
orientation than pressured and imposed choices (TNS Qual⫹, 2011) and a lessening of gender
and externally regulated functioning (Chirkov, inequality in parts of Europe (e.g., Marginean,
2009; Deci et al., 2001; Ryan et al., 1999; Ryan 2012).
& Deci, 2001). This claim awaits substantiation
in research conducted with larger and more Psychological Well-Being
representative European and Eurasian samples.
Notwithstanding the reported strengths of these As in prior studies (Huta & Ryan, 2010; Ryan
causality orientations, they neither predicted life & Deci, 2001; Ryan & Frederick, 1997; Water-
satisfaction nor contributed in a similar order of man, 2008), we confirmed our third hypothesis
magnitude to the explanation of variance in that life satisfaction and vitality, both elements
reported vitality (see Table 3). of psychological well-being, would be moder-
Consistent with our second hypothesis, ately correlated in our sample of young Euro-
women were more inclined toward an auton- pean and Eurasian adults. Life satisfaction and
omy orientation than were men, a difference vitality have historically been used as outcome
found in studies involving U.S. and non-U.S. indices of hedonic fulfillment or eudaimonic
samples (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Soenens et al., living, respectively (Ryan & Deci, 2001; Ryan
2005; Vallerand et al., 1987; Wong, 2000). et al., 2008). However, the cognitive evaluation
Women did not differ from men in controlled of life satisfaction is a conceptually plausible
10 STEVENS, CONSTANTINESCU, UGUR, AND CONSTANTINESCU

outcome of eudaimonic living, and vitality chological well-being in cross-national research


could signal subjectively felt rejuvenation due (Chirkov, 2009; Deci et al., 2001; Ryan et al.,
to the pursuit hedonic pleasure (Huta 2013a, 1999; Ryan & Deci, 2001). Contrary to the
2013b). Hence, the moderate association be- self-determination theory and empirical evi-
tween life satisfaction and vitality was not sur- dence, we did not find that vitality was pre-
prising. Because life satisfaction and vitality dicted by autonomy orientation (i.e., self-
can operate as indicators of both hedonic and determined choice and internally regulated
eudaimonic well-being, we support calls to functioning), but rather was positively related to
more fully integrate the knowledge and research controlled orientation (i.e., pressured and/or im-
foci of these perspectives (Henderson & Knight, posed choice and externally regulated function-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

2012; Wong, 2011). Innovative approaches ing) and negatively tied to impersonal orienta-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(e.g., idiographic methods, and multilevel mod- tion (i.e., passive disengagement).
eling) may prove useful in establishing the com- Whereas controlled causality orientation was
bined benefits of hedonia and eudaimonia in the linked to higher vitality in our sample of young
contexts in which young European and Eurasian European and Eurasian adults and impersonal
adults lead their lives (Cheng, Cheung, Chio, & orientation appeared to undermine vitality, au-
Chan, 2013; Huta 2013a, 2013b). tonomy causality orientation was unrelated to
vitality, despite being a stronger orientation in
Causality Orientations in Relation to our sample. Notwithstanding some evidence for
Psychological Well-Being the consistency of autonomy orientation and its
contribution to psychological well-being over
Our fourth hypothesis, that autonomy, con- the life span (Ryan & La Guardia, 2000), we
trolled, and impersonal causality orientations wonder whether the developmental history and
would be related to well-being outcomes of life current social and cultural contexts in which our
satisfaction and vitality, was largely discon- young European and Eurasian adult sample is
firmed. None of the causality orientations pre- situated (e.g., family, school, work environ-
dicted life satisfaction, whereas only impersonal ment) adequately valued and supported the ac-
and controlled orientations predicted vitality. quisition and exercise of competencies needed
These inconsistent findings offer additional ev- to fully express their inherent autonomy orien-
idence to suggest that psychological well-being tation (Ryan & Deci, 2001; Joussemet et al.,
is a complex phenomenon with convergent and 2008; Ryan et al., 2008), at least in certain life
divergent elements (Henderson & Knight, 2012; domains (e.g., work). It is for future research to
Huta, 2013b; Wong, 2011). They also suggest determine whether the lack of environmental
that the cognitive evaluation of life satisfaction affordances that nurture the emergence of au-
may constitute a form of psychological well- tonomy orientation impedes the pursuit of in-
being not easily attained via the causality ori- trinsic aspirations and experience of vitality.
entations identified by self-determination theory We also found that income, which was a
(Ryan & Deci, 2000, 2001; Ryan et al., 2008), control variable in the regression analyses, ex-
measured by the GCOS (Deci & Ryan, 1985), plained 7.6% of the variance of SVS scores, our
or experienced by young European and Eur- outcome measure of vitality. In related research
asian adults. on Romanians undergraduates, Stevens et al.
Unlike life satisfaction, general causality ori- (2011) observed that extrinsic aspirations may
entations predicted vitality in our European and offer an indirect pathway to psychological well-
Eurasian sample. We should note that SVS being. Contrary to the view that the pursuit of
mean for our sample (M ⫽ 32.96) did not depart extrinsic goals that are not self-concordant is
from that for a U.S. sample of 18 –24 year olds unlikely to fulfill core human needs and have
tested by Ryan and Frederick (1997) (M ⫽ negative psychological consequences (Kasser &
35.72). Self-determination theory suggests that Ryan, 1996; Ryan et al., 1999; Schmuck et al.,
attaining psychological well-being depends on 2000; Sheldon & Kasser, 2001), our research
the presence and magnitude of certain broad and suggests a more complex relationship between
stable action tendencies (Deci & Ryan, 2000; goal content and well-being. When extrinsic
Ryan et al., 2008; Schmuck et al., 2000). Au- goals, irrespective of being internalized, pro-
tonomy orientation has reliably predicted psy- vide a route through which to fulfill intrinsic,
CAUSALITY ORIENTATIONS AND WELL-BEING 11

self-concordant goals, they may enhance the Sheldon & Kasser, 2001). Alternatively, as de-
experience of vitality. Henry Murray (1938) scribed above, efforts directed at securing reli-
labeled this dynamic as subsidization, a process able access to basic resources may provide the
whereby one goal is instrumental in the satis- functional freedom needed to pursue the fulfill-
faction of another. Within self-determination ment of individual potentialities (Ryan & Deci,
theory, Ryan et al. (2008) explained that “[ex- 2001; Ryan et al., 2008). In fact, the perceived
ternal] aspirations will often be instrumental, economic status of Romanians, and perhaps
having their salience because there is something other Europeans and Eurasians, has been tied to
more basic that they serve” (p. 149). their psychological well-being by securing a
Our sample of young European and Eurasian standard of living that in allows for personal
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

adults not only appeared to gain a sense of growth (Cernat, 2010; Eiroa Orosa, 2013; Ste-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

vitality from being receptive to material rewards vens et al., 2011; Tay & Kuykendall, 2013).
(i.e., income), but also from complying with Simply put, “ . . . material supports can enhance
real or imagined directives and structure (Deci access to resources and are important for hap-
& Ryan, 1985, 1991). The predication of vital- piness and self-realization” (Ryan & Deci,
ity by impersonal orientation further suggests 2001, p. 154). Future studies are needed to
that young European and Eurasian adults are determine whether and how controlled orienta-
less likely to experience a sense of energy and tion and extrinsic aspirations that are not self-
aliveness through passive disengagement, in concordant can produce psychological well-
which goal attainment is perceived as a exceed- being in certain social and cultural contexts.
ing personal capacities and resources or is un- From a cultural standpoint, although global-
related to volition. ization may have precipitated a decline in tra-
The relationship of impersonal and controlled ditional authority and rise in secular-rational
causality orientations to vitality may reveal values (Inglehart & Baker, 2000), Romania and
something else about the contexts in which our Turkey remain countries with strong patriarchal
sample is situated. Our sample was mainly Ro- and religious traditions, and have witnessed
manian and Turkish. Life in the Balkans can be centuries of behavioral and social control. Ac-
demanding and uncertain, even for young, edu- cording to self-determination theory and re-
cated, and globally connected citizens, particu- search, the effect on children of socialization
larly where the effects of massive social transi- practices within families and schools character-
tions linger and concerns about economic, ized by intrusive authority and psychological
political, and social stability are ongoing (Eiroa techniques to coerce the pursuit of material
Orosa, 2013). For young Romanian and Turkish goals may have encouraged the development
adults, the experience of vitality appears to rest and expression of a controlled, as opposed to
on two enduring and pervasive perspectives of autonomy, orientation in our sample of young
how they see themselves navigating the vicissi- European and Eurasian adults (Deci & Ryan,
tudes of their everyday world: (a) remaining 1985, 1991, 2000; Joussemet et al., 2008; Ryan
actively engaged despite ambient conditions & Deci, 2001).
that seem beyond the their personal control, and Studies are needed to clarify the types and
(b) being responsive to uncertain or changing magnitude of causality orientations that contrib-
external demands and self-imposed imperatives ute to different forms of psychological well-
as well as opportunities to earn money. Young being, particularly in large representative sam-
Romanian and Turkish adults may experience ples drawn from understudied areas, such as
energy and aliveness as a consequence of their Europe and Eurasia (Eiroa Orosa, 2013). We
tangible success in the face of adversity and encourage scholars to identify moderators and
competition. However, self-determination the- mediators of the relationship between causal-
ory and research would argue that, with the ity orientations and psychological well-being,
exception of dire material conditions, over time perhaps measuring promising cognitive (e.g.,
a controlled causality orientation will fail to control beliefs, generalized expectancies),
adequately satisfy core human needs and even- personality (e.g., the Big Five) variables, and
tuate in diminished psychological well-being environmental conditions (e.g., material and
(Kasser & Ryan, 1996; Ryan et al., 1999; Ryan social deprivation or support) at the individ-
& Deci, 2000, 2001; Schmuck et al., 2000; ual and national levels (Eiroa Orosa, 2013;
12 STEVENS, CONSTANTINESCU, UGUR, AND CONSTANTINESCU

Tay & Kuykendall, 2013). Critical to such comprehend instructions, items, and response
research is the need to understand the social options for the measures.
and cultural contexts that give general causal- Finally, the cross-sectional nature of this
ity orientations and psychological well-being study limited the conclusions that can be drawn
their situated meaning and that dynamically from the relationships that we found. Research-
influence the development and expression of ers should study the ties between general cau-
these constructs (Cheng et al., 2013; Inglehart sality orientations and psychological well-being
& Baker, 2000; Joussemet et al., 2008; Ryan in terms of how young European and Eurasian
& Deci, 2001). adults approach the situated demands of their
daily lives and the experiential consequences of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Limitations their actions as they unfold over time. We trust


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

that these limitations will be remedied in future


We would be remiss in overlooking the meth- research on causality orientations and psycho-
odological weaknesses of our study. Foremost logical well-being.
among these is the sample. Because young Despite its limitations, our study lent support
adults from EU countries have expressed con- to several hypotheses based on self-determina-
cerns and uncertainty about personal challenges tion theory by applying the theory to an under-
and the societal conditions germane to their studied sample of young European and Eurasian
future well-being (TNS Qual⫹, 2011), they adults. The most intriguing result, one that con-
seemed well-suited for investigating the link tradicted self-determination theory, was that in-
between general causality orientations and psy- come and controlled causality orientation pre-
chological well-being. However, although the dicted vitality, an indicant of psychological
sample was large enough to ensure adequate well-being, even though autonomy orientation
statistical power, it was nonetheless small and was more pervasive. Such a finding invites fur-
restricted in age, education, and nationality. Be- ther empirical tests of the cross-national gener-
cause of likely sampling bias, it is questionable alizability of a core proposition of self-
whether the obtained responses were typical, determination theory, namely that autonomy
and thus whether our findings are generalizable orientation yields optimal outcomes. Such re-
to the populations of young adults in Romania search would further articulate self-determina-
and Turkey, let alone in Europe and Eurasia. tion theory’s nomological network. Moreover
Second, although we administered psycho- situating this research in diverse social and cul-
metrically sound measures, we have no evi- tural contexts would serve to identify universal
dence that the constructs we measured provided versus particularistic pathways to well-being.
a cultural “fit” for our sample. However, it is not
unreasonable to infer from our findings that the
constructs as measured were not culturally in- References
appropriate. In addition, we did not measure
Bostic, T. J., Rubio, D. M., & Hood, M. (2000). A
dimensions of culture or level of cultural iden-
validation of the subjective vitality scale using
tity which would have allowed us to frame our structural equation modeling. Social Indicators
study with greater rigor and meaning from the Research, 52, 313–324. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/
perspective of culture. A:1007136110218
Third, our study suffered from several meth- Cernat, V. (2010). Socio-economic status and polit-
odological shortcomings common to Internet- ical support in post-communist Romania. Commu-
based research, many of which can be avoided nist and Post-Communist Studies, 43, 43–50.
through fastidious research design. Such issues http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.postcomstud.2010.01
as self-selection bias, authentication of partici- .001
pants, and participant misbehavior may have Cheng, C., Cheung, S. F., Chio, J. H., & Chan, M. P.
(2013). Cultural meaning of perceived control: A
compromised the trustworthiness of our data
meta-analysis of locus of control and psychologi-
(see Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002 for cal symptoms across 18 cultural regions. Psycho-
guidelines for online research). logical Bulletin, 139, 152–188. http://dx.doi.org/
Fourth, we relied on self-reported proficiency 10.1037/a0028596
in written English, with the result that some Chirkov, V. I. (2009). A cross-cultural analysis of
participants might have been ill-prepared to autonomy in education: A self-determination the-
CAUSALITY ORIENTATIONS AND WELL-BEING 13

ory perspective. Theory and Research in Educa- Inglehart, R., & Baker, W. E. (2000). Modernization,
tion, 7, 253–262. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ cultural change and the persistence of traditional
1477878509104330 values. American Sociological Review, 65, 19 –51.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The general http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2657288
causality orientations scale: Self-determination in Joussemet, M., Landry, R., & Koestner, R. (2008). A
personality. Journal of Research in Personality, self-determination theory perspective on parent-
19, 109 –134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0092- ing. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie cana-
6566(85)90023-6 dienne, 49, 194 –200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). A motivational a0012754
approach to self: Integration in personality. In R. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining
Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motiva- the American dream: Well-being correlates of in-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tion: Perspectives on motivation (Vol. 38, pp. trinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

237–288). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Psychology Bulletin, 22, 280 –288. http://dx.doi
Press. .org/10.1177/0146167296223006
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The ‘what’ and Koestner, R., & Zuckerman, M. (1994). Causality
‘why’ of goal pursuits: Human needs and the orientations, failure, and achievement. Journal of
self-determination of behavior. Psychological Personality, 62, 321–346. http://dx.doi.org/
Inquiry, 11, 227–268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/ 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1994.tb00300.x
S15327965PLI1104_01 Marginean, I. (2012). Performanta Romaniei in do-
Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Gagne, M., Leone, D. R., meniul dezvoltarii umane [Performance of Roma-
Usunov, J., & Kornazheva, B. P. (2001). Need nia in the realm human development]. Calitatea
satisfaction, motivation, and well-being in the Vieţii, 23, 277–298.
work organizations of a former Eastern Bloc Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality: A
country: A cross-cultural study of self-determi- clinical and experimental study of fifty men of
nation. Personality and Social Psychology Bul- college age. New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
letin, 27, 930 –942. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Nix, G. A., Ryan, R. M., Manly, J. B., & Deci, E. L.
0146167201278002
(1999). Revitalization through self-regulation: The
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin,
effects of autonomous and controlled motivation
S. (1985). The Satisfaction With Life Scale. Jour-
on happiness and vitality. Journal of Experimental
nal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. http://
Social Psychology, 35, 266 –284. http://dx.doi.org/
dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
10.1006/jesp.1999.1382
Eiroá Orosa, F. J. (2013). Psychosocial wellbeing in
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G.
the Central and Eastern European transition: An (2002). E-research: Ethics, security, design, and
overview and systematic bibliographic review. In- control in psychological research in the Internet.
ternational Journal of Psychology, 48, 481– 491. Journal of Social Issues, 58, 161–176. http://dx.doi
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2012.669481 .org/10.1111/1540-4560.00254
Henderson, L. W., & Knight, T. (2012). Integrating Olesen, M. H., Thomsen, D. K., Schnieber, A., &
the hedonic and eudaimonic perspectives to more Tonnesvang, J. (2010). Distinguishing general
comprehensively understand wellbeing and path- causality orientations from personality traits. Per-
ways to wellbeing. International Journal of Well- sonality and Individual Differences, 48, 538 –543.
being, 2, 196 –221. http://dx.doi.org/10.5502/ijw http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.032
.v2i3.3 Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the Sat-
Huta, V. (2013a). Eudaimonia. In S. A. David, I. isfaction With Life Scale. Psychological Assess-
Boniwell, & A. C. Ayers (Eds.), The Oxford hand- ment, 5, 164 –172.
book of happiness (pp. 201–213). Oxford, UK: Ryan, R. M., Chirkov, V., Little, T., Sheldon, K. M.,
Oxford University Press. Timoshina, E., & Deci, E. L. (1999). The Ameri-
Huta, V. (2013b). Pursuing eudaimonia versus hedo- can dream in Russia: Extrinsic aspirations in two
nia: Distinctions, similarities, and relationships. In cultures. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
A. S. Waterman (Ed.), The best within us: Positive letin, 25, 1509 –1524. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
psychology perspectives on eudaimonia (pp. 139 – 01461672992510007
158). Washington, DC: American Psychological Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determina-
Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14092-008 tion theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motiva-
Huta, V., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). Pursuing pleasure or tion, social development, and well-being. Ameri-
virtue: The differential and overlapping well-being can Psychologist, 55, 68 –78. http://dx.doi.org/
benefits of hedonic and eudaimonic motives. Jour- 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68
nal of Happiness Studies, 11, 735–762. http://dx Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and
.doi.org/10.1007/s10902-009-9171-4 human potentials: A review of research on hedonic
14 STEVENS, CONSTANTINESCU, UGUR, AND CONSTANTINESCU

and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psy- change in motivational variables play a role? Jour-
chology, 52, 141–166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/ nal of Applied Social Psychology, 43, 1259 –1272.
annurev.psych.52.1.141 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12088
Ryan, R. M., & Frederick, C. (1997). On energy, Stevens, M. J., Constantinescu, P-M., & Butucescu,
personality, and health: Subjective vitality as a A. (2011). Aspirations and wellbeing in Romanian
dynamic reflection of well-being. Journal of Per- and US undergraduates. International Journal of
sonality, 65, 529 –565. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j Psychology, 46, 436 – 445. http://dx.doi.org/
.1467-6494.1997.tb00326.x 10.1080/00207594.2011.565344
Ryan, R. M., Huta, V., & Deci, E. L. (2008). Living Tay, L., & Kuykendall, L. (2013). Promoting happi-
well: A self-determination theory perspective on ness: The malleability of individual and societal
eudaimonia. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, subjective wellbeing. International Journal of Psy-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

139 –170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10902-006- chology, 48, 159 –176. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

9023-4 00207594.2013.779379
Ryan, R. M., & La Guardia, J. G. (2000). What is TNS Qual⫹. (2011). Well-being in 2030: Aggregate
being optimized?: Self-determination theory and report. Retrieved from http://ec.europa.eu/public_
basic psychological needs. In S. H. Qualls & N. opinion/archives/quali/wellbeing_aggregate_
Abeles (Eds.), Psychology and the aging revolu- en.pdf
tion: How we adapt to longer life (pp. 145–172). Vallerand, R. J., Blais, M. R., Lacouture, Y., & Deci,
Washington, DC: American Psychological Associ- E. L. (1987). L’echelle des orientations generales a
ation. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10363-008 la causalite: Validation canadienne francaise du
Schlomer, G. L., Bauman, S., & Card, N. A. (2010). General Causality Orientations Scale [The General
Best practices for missing data management in Causality Orientations Scale: The Canadian
counseling psychology. Journal of Counseling French version of the General Causality Orienta-
Psychology, 57, 1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ tions Scale]. Canadian Journal of Behavioural
a0018082 Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du com-
Schmuck, P., Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). portement, 19, 1–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Intrinsic and extrinsic goals: Their structure and h0079872
relationship to well-being in German and U.S. Waterman, A. S. (2008). Reconsidering happiness: A
college students. Social Indicators Research, eudaimonist’s perspective. The Journal of Positive
50, 225–241. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A: Psychology, 3, 234 –252. http://dx.doi.org/
1007084005278 10.1080/17439760802303002
Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (2001). Goals, congru- Wong, M. M. (2000). The relations among causality
ence, and positive well-being: New empirical sup- orientations, academic experience, academic per-
port for humanistic theories. Journal of Humanis- formance, and academic commitment. Personality
tic Psychology, 41, 30 –50. http://dx.doi.org/ and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 315–326.
10.1177/0022167801411004 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167200265005
Soenens, B., Berzonsky, M. D., Vansteenkiste, M., Wong, P. T. P. (2011). Positive psychology 2.0:
Beyers, W., & Goossens, L. (2005). Identity style Toward a balanced interactive model of the good
and causality orientations: In search of the moti- life. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie cana-
vational underpinnings of the identity exploration dienne, 52, 69 – 81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
process. European Journal of Personality, 19, a0022511
427– 442.
Solberg, P. A., Halvari, H., & Ommundsen, Y. Received December 17, 2013
(2013). Linking exercise and causality orientations Revision received November 4, 2014
to change in well-being among older adults: Does Accepted November 5, 2014 䡲

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi