Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 15

T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

The Creative Personality


Edwin C. Selby, Emily J. Shaw, and John C. Houtz
Fordham University

understanding of how all persons, regardless of the level


A B S T R A C T of their creativity, solve problems of all kinds and, hope-
fully, can learn to be better problem solvers.
The study of the creative personality has established
itself as a major avenue of research on creativity and
creative problem solving, other areas being creative P U T T I N G T H E
process, product, and environment (or press). With R E S E A R C H T O U S E
respect to personality research, over the past 50-plus
years, many studies have examined characteristics, Studies of personality characteristics of highly cre-
attitudes, preferences, styles, and other personal ative individuals have resulted in lists of hundreds of
qualities that appear to distinguish highly creative descriptors, which contain items that overlap and, at
individuals. The purposes of this article are to review times, contradict one another. This has made efforts
the accumulated body of creative personality to identify students with potential for creative pro-
research; describe the works of a few major ductivity difficult. The concept of style promises to
researchers and their methods; brief ly review theo- help our understanding of these apparent contradic-
ries that have been offered to explain why these per- tions while improving our ability to identify and
sonal qualities are causes, correlates, and/or develop creative talent.
outcomes of the creative process; and examine the Instead of asking, “How creative is this stu-
relatively new construct of creative and problem- dent?,” a focus on style leads us to ask, “How is this
solving styles. Style assessment builds upon tradi- student creative?” This lends itself to the assumption
tional personality research but holds substantial that all students have creative potential that can be
promise for talent identification and development identified and nurtured. Helping students appreciate
for all individuals, not just those recognized as cre- their creative style can enable them to be more effec-
atively gifted. tive when employing their problem-solving skills in
specific domains.
A student’s level of creative problem-solving skill
Creativity and creative problem solving have might be identified as “not yet evident,” “emerging,”
“expressing,” or “excelling.” Each level calls for a dif-
been argued to be essential to humanity’s progress, even
ferent instructional approach. Guiding students
its very survival (Taylor, 1964; Taylor & Barron, 1963).
through learning experiences appropriate for each
Decades of research focused on the creative person have
level and offering them opportunities to produce in a
produced a substantial literature and long lists of charac- particular domain of interest enables them to realize
teristics associated with individuals who have produced their innate creativity. Understanding style helps stu-
many creative achievements. The skills and dispositions dents to more effectively use their strengths and mit-
of creative problem solving among average, nonfamous, igate risks associated with their style when
everyday individuals have received much less attention responding to the environment. Also, when instruc-
(Nicholls, 1972). Yet, if we are to maximize the talents in tors understand their own creative style, they broaden
all of us, for progress large and small, we must know more the lens through which they evaluate products and
about the “average” person engaged in the creative prob- identify the creative spark glowing beneath the sur-
lem-solving process. A relatively new construct, creativ- face of the student’s personality.
ity or problem-solving style, holds promise for greater

3 0 0 G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4
T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

This article surveys research and theory on the char- motivation “. . . and needs, interests, and attitudes that
acteristics of personality, including style, associated with help the individuals to be productive creatively”
creativity. We review various descriptions of the charac- (Guilford, 1967, p. 12). Bloom (1963) concluded that
teristics that may define the creative personality, a few of “personality and motivational factors are at least as impor-
the major researchers, their methodologies and theories, tant as aptitude in determining [creative] performance”
a sample of the available creativity assessments, and the (p. 252). Williams’ (1972) model for developing creative
developing understanding of style as an important factor talent placed equal emphasis on cognitive and affective
in determining how different individuals approach situa- traits. Besides the cognitive abilities of ideational f luency,
tions that require a creative response. The article con- f lexibility, originality, and elaborative thinking, Williams
cludes with a discussion linking our current regarded the affective qualities of curiosity, courage,
understanding of creative style and other characteristics complexity, and imagination as critical to creativity.
associated with creativity to talent identification and Renzulli, who has devoted decades to the discovery and
development in education. encouragement of exceptional talent, has consistently
At times in this review, the terms creativity and prob- pressed for wider views and broader definitions of gifted-
lem solving are used interchangeably. The intention is not ness that include far more than cognitive abilities. His
to blur distinctions that others may feel are necessary; recent “Houndstooth Model” (Renzulli, Sytsma, &
rather it is to reinforce the commonality between the Berman, 2000) recognizes the interaction of such affec-
two. Their respective literatures have historically been tive qualities or traits as optimism, courage, absorption or
linked to both theory and findings. Undoubtedly, both passion for a topic, empathy, charisma, and vision or a
terms share a common focus on the creation of new sense of destiny.
responses—new solutions—to problems and questions
that heretofore had not been effectively answered.
Creative Personality
Theories
Guiding Principles
In order to frame a context for our understanding of
Two essential principles to the study of the creative the creative aspects of personality, it may be useful to
personality are Field Theory (Lewin, 1936) and the impor- refresh our memories about the major theoretical
tance of the affective domain. Field Theory suggests that approaches to the subject. Even a brief description of
human behavior is a function of the interaction of person- dominant creativity theories reinforces the role of affect
ality and the environment. Any study of the person must and the interaction of person and environment (press) in
1
consider the environment (i.e., other people, organizations the creative process.
both effective and ineffective, the presence or lack of stim- Table 1 lists some of the major Freudian-based theo-
ulation, rules, beliefs, attitudes, and expectations) in which ries beginning with the work of Freud himself, followed
the person functions. Secondly, we consider that the affec- by theories focusing on self-growth and the positivist
tive domain is as important to creativity as is the cognitive views of creativity beginning with Rank, and later devel-
domain. While the literature contains numerous refer- opmental theories. Freud never directly formulated a
ences to insight and serendipitous events, most creativity theory of creativity. However, in one paper (Freud,
scholars regard the work of creative thinking and creative 1908/1959), he did describe the artist’s creative process as
problem solving as difficult but energizing, often exciting, one of conf lict resolution or sublimation. This was
but usually requiring sustained engagement, dedication, enough to move the consideration of the creative person-
and commitment (Amabile, 1989; Gruber, 1989; Russ, ality forward. At first, the focus was on the unconscious
1993; Torrance, 1967). mind, with creativity being tied to the id-instincts.
Because problems are often complex, creativity is not Dynamicist thinking moved the focus to the precon-
easy work. We must not assume that to be creative one scious and onto creativity’s transcendent qualities.
need only “think,” or use certain “tools” or cognitive The classic psychoanalytic view of a struggle between
skills, to generate creative solutions. Logic, as well as neu- fantasy and reality led many to think of creativity as involv-
roscience and brain research, offers strong evidence that ing the darker side of human nature. However, Rank
emotional processes and cognition must interact if cre- (1932/1960) and other psychoanalytic psychologists
ativity is to occur. Consideration must be given to the regarded this struggle as the ideal state of human nature.

G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4 3 0 1
T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

Ta b l e 1
Theories of Creativity

Theory Theorist(s) Description

Psychoanalytic/ Freud (1908/1959) Creative process as conf lict resolution, or as “sublimation,” the production of something
Psychodynamic new or original, resulting from the modification of id-impulses into something more accept-
able.

Lee (1940) Creative productivity as an effort to overcome destructive, disabling emotions manifested
from the id-instinct.

Kris (1952) “Regression in service to the ego” permits individuals to access the dynamic energy of the
unconscious while seeking the unique positive qualities of fantasies.

Jung (1923) Individuals access the “collective unconscious” or “archetypes,” representing the sum of all
human existence when creating, perceiving some universal quality or truth from human his-
tory and translating that perception into some real creative product.

Arieti (1970) The “seat” of creativity is the preconscious mind, but the process that occurs “there” is a
unique combination of both primary (fantasy, wish-fulfillment) and secondary (logical,
structured) processes. Tertiary process thinking, directed by the ego, accesses and unites both
realms, producing a unique outcome.

Csikszentmihalyi The creative enterprise represents a point where all elements are in harmony, working
(1996), Gowan symbiotically and directed to a natural and inexorable conclusion rising above the normal
(1975) experience.

Self-growth, Rank (1932/1960) The conf lict between fantasy and reality is viewed as the ideal state of human nature,
Developmental, resulting in cognitive growth.
Positivistic
Sinnott (1959) Creativity is a natural, developmental life force. The nature of the conscious mind is inher-
ently creative and directs the unconscious mind to action and ultimately forms and shapes
our final products.

Maslow (1968), The drive to create is the drive to actualization. Creativity is a “transcendent” or “peak”
White (1959) experience, representing the highest levels of achievement, leading to novel, original, or new
ideas.

Rogers (1954) Creativity is the “emergence in action of a novel, relational product, growing out of the
uniqueness of the individual . . . and the materials, events, people, or circumstances of his life
. . .” (p. 71). Creative expression is enhanced by psychological safety and freedom.

Abra (1997) The need or impetus for self-expression is what unites creativity in all aspects of life (art or
science, sports, religion, etc.). What consistently sets individuals engaged in the creative and
problem-solving processes apart is their dedication, commitment, steadfastness, vigor, and
intensity—or their motivation for creative work.

Eysenck (1983, Creativity is not an ability, but rather a personality variable.


1993, 1997)

They theorized that our species has progressed as a result of The rise of self-growth or developmental theories of
our collective creative imagination. The nature of the con- creativity paralleled theories of child development, such as
scious mind is inherently creative. It directs the uncon- that of Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget (Flavell, 1963) who
scious mind to action and ultimately forms and shapes our described cognitive growth as the result of a constant
final products. This is equally true for paradigm-shifting struggle between oppositional and natural processes,
creativity, as well as everyday small creative acts that incre- accommodation and assimilation. We are driven to seek
mentally and imperceptibly advance humankind, or one’s out stimulation, expand our awareness, develop additional
satisfaction with life. skills, and gain mastery of the environment. Maslow

3 0 2 G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4
T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

(1968) described a hierarchy of human motives to action, Ta b l e 2


from low-level, biological needs to higher level motives,
such as knowledge and self-actualization. The drive to cre- Personality Characteristics Associated With Two
ate is the drive to actualization. It leads us to novel, origi-
Patterns of Creative Individuals
nal, new ideas. But, it can be risky and dangerous behavior.
Characteristics Associated Characteristics Associated
Hence, our affective qualities become more important to
With Openness With Listening to One’s
creative productivity than intellective abilities, sustaining
and Courage Inner Voice
us in our quest despite possible physical, social, and/or to Explore Ideas
emotional dangers.
Rogers (1954) defined creativity as “. . . the emer- • Sensitivity to problems • Self-awareness of cre-
gence in action of a novel, relational product, growing • Aesthetic sensibilities ativeness
out of the uniqueness of the individual . . . and the mate- • Curiosity • Persistence
rials, events, people, or circumstances of his life . . .” (p. • Sense of humor • Independence of
71). Creativity involves openness, an internal locus of • Playfulness thought
evaluation, and the self-confidence or courage to pursue • Fantasy thinking • Self-disciplined
• Risk-taking • Self-directed
ideas that one considers important, despite external dis-
• Tolerance for ambiguity • Autonomous
couragements. When acting creatively, individuals attend • Tenacity • Self-confident
to their “inner voices” (see Table 2; Treffinger, Young, • Openness to experience • Ref lective
Selby & Shepardson, 2002), their personal beliefs about • Adaptability • Introspective
what is right or worthwhile, rather than being inf luenced • Intuition • Internal locus of control
by contrary views. • Willingness to grow • Rejecting of stereotypes
The importance of an internal locus of evaluation, or • Openness to feelings • Energetic
intrinsic motivation, has long been recognized in the cre- • Unwillingness to accept • Hard-working
ative process (Deci, 1975, 1980), as has the principle of authoritarian assertions • Absorption in work
withholding judgment when ideas are being generated without critical exami- • Unsociable
nation
(Osborn, 1963; Parnes, 1967). Premature external evalua-
• Integration of
tion stifles one’s willingness to express new ideas and may dichotomies
destroy intrinsic motivation and self-confidence. Amabile
and her colleagues (Amabile, 1983, 1990, 1996; Hennessy ages one to put one’s all into the work. It also permits and
& Amabile, 1998; Hennessy & Zbikowski, 1993) demon- promotes the freedom of symbolic expression (Rogers,
strated the inhibitory effects of external evaluation on cre- 1954), experimentation, playfulness, and exploration.
ativity and the beneficial effects of classroom and personal The environment must be responsive to the creative
motivational structures that stress the intrinsic value of task process, resources must be present, rewards for new
performance. thinking offered, and challenges and questions encour-
Creativity not only results from the interaction of aged.
cognition and personality, but also from interaction with Abra (1997) argues that what unites creativity in art,
the situation or environment. Recall Lewin’s (1936) science, or any area of human endeavor is motivation—
Field Theory. As Rogers (1954) interpreted this princi- the need or impetus for self-expression. True, there are
ple, creative expression is enhanced by two main envi- positive and negative aspects to motivation, just as there
ronmental conditions: psychological safety and freedom. are variable reactions from the external world in response
Creative expression requires the courage to risk destroy- to individuals’ efforts. But, what consistently sets individ-
ing well-established and favored ideas. When risking uals who successfully engage in the creative process apart
everything, the individual needs to know (or feel) that, from those who are less successful is their dedication,
even in failure, he or she will still be valued. commitment, steadfastness, vigor, and intensity—their
The absence of typically negative or irrelevant feed- motivation for creative work. Eysenck (1983, 1993, 1997)
back permits individuals to follow their own instincts and reinforces this view by proposing that creativity is a per-
notions of what is best. A psychologically safe environ- sonality variable, not an ability. His research and theory
ment reduces inappropriate external evaluation while added to the foundation for the study of creative prob-
communicating empathic understanding. This allows the lem-solving style reported on by Selby, Treffinger,
true self to emerge in the creative enterprise and encour- Isaksen, and Lauer (2004).

G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4 3 0 3
T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

Developing Our experienced a great deal of encouragement from parents


Contemporary or mentors to pursue their interests or talents.
Understanding of Creativity Many authors continue to examine the lives of cre-
atively productive individuals for insights to their think-
Typically, creativity researchers begin their reviews ing and working processes and characteristics of their
with reference to Guilford’s (1950) Presidential Address to creative works (Gardner, 1993; Oremland, 1997; Smith
the American Psychological Association, calling attention & Carlsson, 1990). While the bulk of the attention has
to psychology’s neglect of creativity and the importance of focused on artistic creativity, there are also many biogra-
developing creative talent. Another popular reference is to phical studies of scientists and others (Gedo & Gedo,
the classification by Rhodes (1961) of four major areas of 1992; Mansfield & Busse, 1981; Phillips, 1957; Wallace &
creativity research that have grown since Guilford’s call, Gruber, 1989).
known as the four “P’s”: creative process, product, press
(the environment), and personality.
As for creative personality research, there is perhaps Creative Personality
no greater amount of empirical research comparing cre- Characteristics
ative and “less creative” individuals than that comparing
their respective personality test scores. Several creativity Lists of creative personality characteristics abound
researchers, whose respective and collective contribu- (Barron, 1955; Dellas & Gaier, 1970; Feist, 1999;
tions have largely created the knowledge base, stand MacKinnon, 1962; Stein, 1974; Vervalin, 1962). Many of
out. For more than a quarter century, beginning in the these lists overlap, while others offer unique examples.
late 1940s, the Institute for Personality Assessment and Some listed characteristics are even contradictory. No
Research (IPAR) at the University of California at one person can be expected to exhibit all of the charac-
Berkeley was a major center for creative personality teristics that appear in the literature, nor will an individ-
research. MacKinnon (1962, 1970, 1978), the founder ual who exhibits one or more of these characteristics
and long-time director of IPAR, with his students and necessarily exhibit that one or those characteristics all of
colleagues, collected signif icant amounts of data the time. Among those suggested by Barron were prefer-
through interviews and objective and projective test ence for complexity, independence in judgment, self-
scores with samples of architects, writers, mathemati- assertion, less use of suppression as a defense mechanism,
cians, scientists, inventors, engineers, and individuals and greater tendency to express impulses. Vervalin listed
from other professions and occupations. Among his col- a high level of broadly defined intelligence, openness to
leagues and collaborators at IPAR were Barron (1955, experience and emotion, freedom from inhibitions and
1969, 1990, 1995), Helson (1965, 1966, 1967), and stereotyped thinking, aesthetic sensitivity, f lexibility,
Gough (1979). independence in thought and action, love of creation for
However, IPAR’s “psychometric” approach was not creation’s sake, and endlessly questing new challenges
the only methodology used to study creative personality. and solutions. MacKinnon’s “more creative” compared
A substantial body of literature exists from biographical to “less creative” architects scored higher on social pres-
research as well (Abra, 1997; Gedo & Gedo, 1992; ence, self-acceptance, dominance, self-confidence, free-
Gruber, 1989; Taylor & Ellison, 1967; Wallace & Gruber, dom from conventional restraints and inhibitions, and
1989). Biographical case-study methods often provided willingness to admit unusual and unconventional self-
richer, deeper insights through detailed analysis of indi- views. They were lower on sense of well-being, respon-
vidual life histories. Self-reports by some creatively pro- sibility, socialization, self-control, interest in achievement
ductive adults indicated that their childhoods were not in conforming situations, or preoccupation with impress-
especially happy. Their home situations involved chal- ing others. Stein described the creative person as a curi-
lenges and difficulties, including parental death or ous, self-assertive, aggressive achiever, motivated by a
absences due to divorce or separation (Roe, 1952), that need for order, who while being self-critical, conven-
were greater than what one might consider the norm. As tional, self-sufficient, intuitive, and empathic, is also less
a consequence, these individuals may have learned to inhibited. His creative person, while emotionally unsta-
adapt by seeking ways to overcome adversity, and eventu- ble, is capable of using instability effectively.
ally succeeded in widely recognized creative achieve- One recognized limitation of creative personality
ments. There are indications that these individuals also research has been that the majority of studies involved

3 0 4 G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4
T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

only adult males. Dellas and Gaier’s (1970) review was openness and sensitivity exposes them to a great deal of
notable for its inclusion of research on creative women suffering, as well as enjoyment.
and children. Their results noted the expressed feminin- After reviewing 120 definitions of creativity and
ity of interests for males and expressed masculinity of conducting a wide search for characteristics associated
interests for females. Additionally, research on female sci- with creative productivity drawing from over 100 articles,
entists (Helson, 1967) and curiosity and nonconformity Treffinger et al. (2002) described four patterns of abili-
in children (Starkweather, 1964, 1976) suggests that the ties, dispositions, styles, and personal characteristics. The
“creative personality” crosses both gender and age. creative individual generates ideas (using divergent and
Amabile (1989) added self-discipline about work, per- metaphorical thinking); “digs more deeply” into ideas
severance even when frustrated, the ability to wait for (using convergent and critical thinking); is open to and
rewards, self-motivation, and willingness to take risks. has the courage to explore ideas; and, to a greater degree,
Dacey’s (1989) list is constructed around eight qualities of is willing to listen to his or her inner voice. These last two
the creative mind, including tolerance for ambiguity, flex- categories are lengthy and are summarized in Table 2.
ibility, androgyny (uninhibited by gender stereotypes), and These two patterns gather many of the personality char-
delay of gratification. Feist (1999) categorized more than acteristics listed over the years. The authors note that the
100 references comparing artists and non-artists, scientists characteristics that they found in the literature include
and non-scientists. His list, distinguishing “creatives” from not only cognitive abilities and personality traits, but also
“non-creatives,” included imagination, impulsivity, lack of past experience.
conscientiousness, anxiety, emotional sensitivity, ambi-
tion, norm-doubting, hostility, aloofness, unfriendliness,
lack of warmth, dominance, arrogance, and autonomy. Measuring the Creative
McMullen’s (1976) list illustrated “synergistic Personality
swings,” or the combining of ideas in ways that at first
glance seem impossible. Rothenberg (1971, 1990) To develop the long lists of creative personality char-
referred to this synergistic process as Janusian thinking. acteristics, researchers and theorists have constructed and
Bruner (1973) called it “connectedness,” a blending of used an array of instruments designed to assess aspects of
apparent opposites and contradictions. Creative individ- human personality. Many of these personality profiles,
uals present an array of “paradoxes.” They are relaxed but inventories, questionnaires, or checklists are self-report
attentive, confident but humble, disinterested but selfish, measures. Some are “objectively scored” and others are
detached but involved, constructive but discontented, projective measures requiring considerable training and
mindless but perceptive, convergent and divergent, and experience to score and interpret. Still other measures are
able to delay closure but able to stay with a decision once more like experimental tasks than tests. Starkweather
made (McMullen, 1976). (1964, 1976), for example, created several creativity meas-
Csikszentmihalyi (1996) described these “polarities” ures for preschool children to assess conformity/noncon-
even more explicitly. Creative individuals have a great formity by having them match (or not match) shapes that
deal of physical energy, yet they are often quiet and at they were told were selected for their parents. In her Target
rest. They are “smart,” but can be naïve. They might Game, Starkweather assessed children’s risk-taking by
appear playful and undisciplined but also exceptionally their choice of distance from a target in a bowling-type
hard-working and responsible. They might alternate game.
between “f lights of fantasy” and a very “down-to-earth” Houtz and Krug (1995) and Treffinger et al. (2002)
sense of reality. Creative people seem to harbor opposite provide extensive reviews of creativity assessment instru-
tendencies for introversion and extraversion, are at the ments and methods. Several well-known instruments
same time humble and proud of their achievements, and include the California Personality Inventory, the Sixteen
are thought to be rebellious and independent, yet cannot Factor Personality Questionnaire, and the Minnesota
create in the absence of the knowledge, rules, or conven- Multiphasic Personality Inventory. These instruments
tions of their cultures. They have internalized the values were designed to assess a broad array of needs and/or per-
of their domains while maintaining their instinct for sonality traits and are administered to individuals who
questioning the “givens” and assumptions of those generally would be characterized as exhibiting “normal”
domains. Creative individuals are simultaneously pas- behavior. Instruments such as the Rorschach Test and the
sionate and objective about their work. Finally, their Thematic Apperception Test are more subjective, pre-

G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4 3 0 5
T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

senting subjects with rather ambiguous stimuli and rely- The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI; Briggs &
ing upon individuals’ “projections” of unconscious moti- Myers, 1976; Kroeger & Thueson, 1988; Myers &
vations or feelings. McCaulley, 1985; Myers & Myers, 1980) is a self-report
A number of self-report inventories are more specific measure designed to assess individuals’ preference for dif-
to the assessment of creativity or creative potential. For ferent types of information processing along four affective
example, the Something About Myself (SAM) and What dimensions. From a series of forced-choice items, individ-
Kind of Person Are You (WKOPAY; Khatena, 1971) uals are rated on introversion–extraversion, intuitive–sens-
together make up the Creative Perception Inventory ing, thinking–feeling, and perceiving–judging. There is a
(Khatena & Morse, 1994; Khatena & Torrance, 1976). The body of literature suggesting a pattern among the four
SAM asks individuals to check-off activities that they have dimensions most closely associated with creativity: intro-
engaged in that might be indicative of creative potential. version, intuitive, thinking, perceiving (Houtz, LeBlanc, &
Items include hobbies, taking trips, writing poems or Butera, 1994; Houtz et al., 2003; Houtz, Tetenbaum, &
plays, and inventing. Other items on the SAM ask individ- Phillips, 1981).
uals to agree or disagree with certain self-descriptors, such
as “I am talented in many different ways” or “I am
resourceful.” The WKOPAY asks individuals to check Creative Style
personality traits or characteristics that they feel typify their
This review of the creative personality is not the first,
behavior.
nor will it be the last. But, what is new since a number of
Another well-known checklist is Gough’s (1952,
prior reviews is the emergence of the construct of creative
1979) Adjective Checklist (ACL) of 300 descriptors, of
or problem-solving style. This and other creative personal-
which Domino (1970) identified 59 that formed a
ity reviews have clearly established the important role of
Creativity Scale. Some of the 59 included absentminded,
affective traits or characteristics in the creative process. To
disorderly, logical, artistic, idealistic, restless, curious,
establish this principle, data from numerous studies
insightful, sensitive, demanding, spontaneous, egotistical,
employing a variety of personality measures have been col-
sarcastic, assertive, energetic, and clever. A useful scale for
lected. However, the reader is reminded again of the first
children that can be completed by teachers was developed
principle of the current review—that human behavior
by Renzulli, Hartman, and Callahan (1975). Pupils’
results from the interaction of personality and the environ-
behavioral traits are rated in such areas as curiosity, f lu-
ment. A significant limitation of decades of creative per-
ency, risk-taking, intellectual playfulness, humor, sensi-
sonality assessment research has been that environmental
tivity to beauty, conformity, individualism, and tenacity.
factors have not been a part of the data gathering. Despite
Davis and Rimm (1982; Davis, 1998) collaborated the arguments of the developmental, positivist creativity
on the development of a number of inventories for chil- theories, affective characteristics are still presented as static
dren, adolescents, and college students. Davis (1975) entities, described in terms of amounts (scores) possessed
described six major trait clusters in the creative personal- or not possessed by individuals, which may or may not
ity that appear from the items on these scales: energetic come into play during creative problem solving.
originality; creative interests and activities; creative writ- The construct of creative style has begun to change
ing and attraction to the complex; self-confidence and this view of the role of affect in the creative process.
sense of humor; freedom and f lexibility together with a Rather than focusing on the level of creativity exhibited
belief in psychical phenomenon; and arousal seeking, by a child or an adult, creative style research identifies dif-
risk-taking, and playfulness. ferences in the ways people approach problems they
The Barron-Welsh Art Scale (Welsh & Barron, encounter in their environments. Attention is directed
1963), a portion of Welsh’s Figure Preference Test, pres- less at how much creativity an individual displays, but on
ents individuals with pairs of line drawings. One drawing how individuals use their creative skills in response to the
of each pair is more “balanced or symmetrical,” using conditions confronting them. By shifting the focus from
straighter, more regular lines. The second is more asym- “how much” to “in what ways,” the focus of research also
metrical, irregular, ambiguous, using more curved lines shifts. We see that many of the characteristics listed above
or “ill-defined” boundaries. Individuals with more artis- are manifestations of style that might promote or prohibit
tic talents or aspirations and individuals with greater creative productivity, depending on the environment. It
demonstrated creativity prefer the complex drawings. is fair to say that creative style research has been a concur-

3 0 6 G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4
T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

Figure 1. The influence of style on complex behavior

rent development along with the growing literature of Style research is a promising area for educators for at
the past 20 years on learning and thinking styles (Jonassen least two reasons. First, the construct of style allows for an
& Grabowski, 1993; Sternberg, 1997). explanation of the personality polarities described earlier
A theory of the inf luence of style on complex behav- and often cited by creativity researchers. During the
iors such as creative problem solving is diagrammed in interactive, back and forth, give and take problem-solv-
Figure 1. Both genetics and experience affect the devel- ing process, where both divergent and convergent think-
opment of styles of information processing, which in turn ing and their affective correlates or concomitants are
inf luence how the individual responds to the environ- required (Treffinger, Isaksen, & Dorval, 2000), it could
be expected that successful individuals would learn to
ment and selects new experiences. Experiences then lead
modify their styles to the demands of the situation. Thus,
to the development of new skills and reinforce styles
“synergistic swings,” as Williams (1973) noted, or even
again. This process is interactive; each step is inf luenced
contradictory traits or characteristics (Csikszentmihalyi,
by the prior steps. Of course, experiences can lead to
1996), would be exhibited. Individuals who, through the
reinforcement, successful problem solving (i.e., a positive
instructional process, come to understand their style can
change in one’s environment), or failure (i.e., no change learn how to stretch beyond their preferences and to acti-
or a change for the worse). With respect to creativity vate appropriate responses to the environment that they
styles, this theory suggests that when confronted with a would otherwise leave unexplored.
problem, individuals may act differently according to the The second reason is that creative or problem-solv-
style they have developed and currently prefer to follow. ing style applies to all persons, not just the creatively
These actions will result in different types of experiences gifted. Everyone solves problems—simple, mundane,
that, in turn, will lead individuals to different choices yet everyday problems; therefore, the assumption is that
again. everyone can be a better problem solver. Educators need

G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4 3 0 7
T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

not think of creativity only as a “gift.” Rather, it is a nat- structures and are very interested in details and proceed-
ural survival trait like the ability to run. Focusing on indi- ing at a careful, deliberate, gradual pace. Solving prob-
viduals who are creatively productive at a high level leads lems within a system may make the system work better,
to the perception that only people who in some way enhance its value, and lead to many benefits. These indi-
match the characteristics of highly creative individuals viduals are termed Developers (Selby et al.). Other indi-
have creative potential. We do not make the same viduals may feel constrained and uncomfortable with the
assumption about running ability. We do not expect all current organization and its structures and may want to
children to become Olympic runners, yet we expect all approach problems by ignoring the rules completely
normally healthy children to be able to run. We also rather than simply bending them. They prefer breaking
assume that, with training and practice, any child can new ground or going off in new directions. If successful,
improve his or her running time. It is well established their efforts actually may change an old system or create a
(Niu & Sternberg, 2003; Sternberg, 2000; Treffinger et completely new system. These individuals are referred to
al., 2002) that an individual’s creative productivity can be as Explorers.
improved through instruction. The Explorer-Developer designation refers to an
Style research focuses on identifying the ways individ- individual’s orientation to change. Individuals who have a
uals prefer to process information, generate new ideas, test well-defined Explorer style often generate ideas and pos-
them, and put them into practice. With knowledge of sible solutions. They find imposed structure, authority,
styles, teachers can better individualize instruction. and rules confining. They are comfortable working on
Researchers and curriculum builders can look for meth- many open-ended tasks at once and often show little con-
ods, techniques, and activities that complement student cern with closure. To them, deadlines are f luid and f lexi-
preferences. Strong preferences can guide learning activi- ble. On the other hand, individuals who have a
ties and weaker, less developed styles can be strengthened. well-defined Developer style prefer to generate “just
Several researchers have developed measures of cre- enough” workable ideas that will serve to make things
ativity styles. Kirton (1976) proposed that some individ- better. They are enabled by structure, authority and rules,
uals prefer to adapt to external conditions and solve and maintain energy by persisting until a task is com-
problems within existing rules, while others prefer to pleted, working out the details of follow-through and
bend, ignore, or break rules to generate new ideas. The implementation. They seek, accept, and meet given dead-
Kirton Adaption-Innovation Inventory (Kirton, 1976, lines.
1994) has been used to identify two problem-solving Two other VIEW dimensions are manner of process-
styles. Adaptors are individuals who define and approach ing information and one’s decision-making focus during
problems within existing frameworks and structures. problem solving. One’s manner of processing can be char-
Innovators “solve problems by creating a new frame- acterized as Internal (“I need to think about this.”) or
work. . . . They are original, energetic, individualistic, External (“I need to talk to other people about this.”). This
spontaneous, and insightful” (Selby, Treffinger, Isaksen, dimension addresses how individuals use their inner
& Powers, 1993, p. 224). energy and resources, how they manage information, and
Basadur (1994) described individuals as generators, how and when in the problem-solving process they share
conceptualizers, optimizers, or implementers, resulting their thoughts.
in four distinct styles of approaching problems. In the Those with an External style prefer processing
Creative Problem Solving Profile Inventory (CPSP; information in a social setting that allows engagement
Basadur, Graen, & Wakabayashi, 1990), individuals are with the outer environment. They learn and work best
presented with 18 sets of four adjectives. They rank when interacting, listening, and talking with others.
order the adjectives in each set as to their appropriate- Externals share options freely with a broad range of peo-
ness as descriptive of themselves. The idea is that the ple. They seek a great deal of input before reaching or
entire creative problem-solving process requires a vari- agreeing to closure. They tend to press for immediate
ety of preferences associated with all four styles, but action, at times without thought or taking any time for
individuals may exhibit clear preferences of one style or ref lection. Internals prefer private processing and often
another. become engrossed with inner events and ideas. They
VIEW, a recent measure of problem-solving style work and learn best alone, in a quiet environment, and
(Selby, Treffinger, Isaksen & Lauer, 2002), identifies will take advantage of opportunities for quiet concen-
individuals who prefer working within existing rules or tration. This quiet concentration and ref lection is a

3 0 8 G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4
T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

source of energy for them. After taking time to think style more than the other, but that preference is often not
their ideas and suggestions through, they are ready to strongly developed or sharply differentiated (Selby et al.,
share them with others, usually beginning with those 2004). Such individuals may find it easier to understand
with whom they have established trust and confidence. and empathize with the other style in that dimension
They prefer to think before they act. This may, at times, than a person whose style was more strongly developed.
result in inaction. There is also a social aspect to style. For instance, an
An individual’s focus when making decisions falls on individual with a moderate Developer style on VIEW’s
a continuum from Person (“How will these ideas affect Orientation to Change Dimension, when working with a
others?”) to Task (“What is necessary to get the job group with pronounced Developer preferences, may be
done?”). This dimension addresses where an individual seen by the group as more of an Explorer. The same indi-
prefers to start in arriving at a decision, what is given first vidual working with strong Explorers might be seen by
priority, and what trade-offs might be made when that group as a pronounced Developer. The individual has
weighing the demands of the task or those people not changed, but the social environment has and therefore,
involved in or affected by the situation. It should not be so has the way that individual’s style is seen and reacted to.
construed that those whose focus is on people do not What also might change is the amount of stress the individ-
think about the task, or that those who focus on the task ual experiences as a result of the disconnect between the
do not think about people. Rather, those with a Person social environment and his or her style.
Focus work along the lines that if people and their needs Each style preference represents certain strengths
are fully addressed, the task will get taken care of, while that, when employed appropriately, can help to move the
those with a Task Focus work along the lines that if the problem-solving process forward. However, there are
task is addressed completely and logically, people will be also risks associated with each style that, if not mitigated
taken care of. or attended to, could disrupt the process. When students
When deciding, those with a Person Focus set prior- (Selby, 1997) and adults (Esposito et al., 2004) under-
ities based on their judgments on personal and emotional stand their style they are able to enter and move through
criteria. They tend to consider the personal impact or the process of problem solving more efficiently and effec-
consequences of a decision. They attend to relationships tively.
and seek harmony and consensus. They try to avoid con- No style is “better” than another, and each has a role
f licts or tense situations, sometimes at the expense of in any component or stage of creative problem solving.
their own needs, and may skim over facts or information Understanding style enables one to build on strengths
in order to maintain harmony. They often become the and become a more effective problem solver.
mediators or peace-makers between those with strong Understanding each other’s character strengths and limi-
but opposing positions. When considering an option tations can help teams work together successfully and
they tend to first consider what’s good, attractive, or productively. Lack of understanding may allow minor
pleasing about it. At times they might put people’s feel- differences to become insurmountable obstacles. People
ings over the quality of the outcome. tend to see the world through the lens of their own style.
Those whose focus is on the Task when making Talking about style and how each group member experi-
decisions prefer well-reasoned conclusions and imper- ences change, processes information, and decides on a
sonal judgments. They choose criteria that are authorita- course of action will help build smooth relationships,
tive, verifiable, and objective. They may address conf licts allowing differences to become assets.
or tense situations at the expense of others’ feelings,
focusing solely on facts and information while ignoring
emotions. They prefer rigor and/or quality over feelings I m p l i c a t i o n s f o r Ta l e n t
and emotion. When considering an option they tend to Development in Education
first consider what is wrong, what is lacking, or what
improvements are needed. The construct of creative style provides researchers
The preferences described for the six styles on and educators with another tool to use to help optimize
VIEW’s three dimensions, and most other style measures, creative expression. Because the creative style construct
become more pronounced as an individual’s score or rat- rests on the principle that all persons solve problems, it is
ing moves away from the mean. Those whose preferences well suited to the needs and requirements of designers
are closer to the mean, or more moderate, may prefer one and planners of instruction. As mentioned earlier, despite

G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4 3 0 9
T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

significant and important contributions, a great deal of stand and work to include that understanding into their
creative personality research has ignored the concept of planning increase their opportunities to notice growing
“average creativity” by focusing only on the very creative competence on the part of their students by trying to
individuals who have helped to change the world through look at student products on the basis of criteria driven by
their great discoveries, productions, or ideas. Creativity differing styles.
style research holds great promise for affecting everyone’s This in turn may aid instructors in identifying those
creativity. learners who are developing the knowledge, skills, and
Torrance (1987) long argued that through skilled passion needed in a particular domain that will enable
instruction, students can learn to be more effective creative them to successfully express themselves through higher-
problem solvers. With knowledge of their students’ cre- level creative works. Learners identified as “expressing”
ative problem solving styles, teachers are in a better posi- might need help applying the tools and skills to problems
tion to foresee potential student difficulties in response to and challenges that are realistic and manageable. While
new learning situations and develop alternative learning these opportunities might offer some real-life challenges,
experiences that will match particular styles. Treffinger et they would carry a low level of risk.
al. (2002) advocate first identifying a learner’s present level Finally, those who are identified as “excelling” may
of performance with multiple data sources. Possible per- benefit from opportunities to work with real, self-initi-
formance levels are described as “not yet evident,” ated and self-directed challenges, identifying and apply-
“emerging,” “expressing,” or “excelling.” The level at ing the skills, process, and tools they have studied to a
which one is functioning provides a starting point for variety of tasks both individually and in groups. Again,
determining the educational programming that help style comes into play in terms of how learners approach
develop the student’s natural creative abilities (Treffinger, and deal with the levels of risk and responsibility required
Young, Nassab, & Wittig, 2004). for the successful real-world application of their talents in
An individual whose skills are “not yet evident” their domain, and how they manage problems and
might receive instruction designed to build foundational change within that domain.
skills in a domain and a foundational understanding of
creative tools, techniques, and process. By attending to
this student’s learning style, we might expect that such Conclusion
instruction will be more effective (Dunn, Beaudry, &
Klavas, 1989). By attending to students’ problem-solving Creative personality research continues to be an
style and helping them understand their own preferences active and useful endeavor. Two principles are likely
and the implications of their style, we might expect stu- guides for future research. The first is that personality
dents to be better able to efficiently and effectively navi- research may yield its most productive results when con-
gate the problem-solving process and employ ducted together with the study of cognitive abilities and
idea-generating and -focusing tools (Schoonover, 1996; environmental conditions within which individuals func-
Selby, 2000). As a result, the teacher might have more tion. Time and again, researchers focused on intellectual
opportunities to note special areas of interest that would abilities and steeped in “cognitive traditions,” have come
call for the creative applications of these skills. Also, by to recognize the importance of personality traits or char-
using knowledge of their own style, instructors might acteristics in describing and explaining creativity.
widen the lens through which they evaluate the creative Similarly, divergent creativity personality theories
level of student products and thereby identify products appear to converge on the importance of environmental
whose creative spark might have gone unnoticed. interaction with individual characteristics. “What is hon-
A learner whose creative ability is “emerging” might ored in a culture will be cultivated there,” a dictum
receive help in practicing domain skills and the use of attributed to Plato, appears now to have much evidence
certain tools and creativity skills. Interest might be fur- to support it. Whether we refer to large blocks of time in
ther pursued in independent study, small group assign- the history of a country or a culture (see Simonton, 1987,
ments, clubs, and other domain-specific activities. While 1988), or we refer to an individual’s family history, edu-
practicing problem-solving skills, the student has an cational background, or workplace conditions, it seems
opportunity to further understand the implications of his clear that behavior is inf luenced by the “match” or “mis-
or her style when working with a team or preparing a match” of personality and environment.
product for a specific audience. Instructors who under- As creative personality research advances, however,

3 1 0 G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4
T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

the “level-style” distinction offers new opportunities to Briggs, K., & Myers, I. (1976). The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator.
understand and encourage the creative process. We can- Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
not deny that all types of individuals, with different skills, Bruner, J. S. (1973). The conditions of creativity. In J. M.
Anglin (Ed.), Jerome S. Bruner. Beyond the information given:
working in different environments, solve problems every
Studies in the psychology of knowing (pp. 208–217). New
day. Creativity, or problem-solving, styles offer an inte-
York: W. W. Norton.
gration of traditional personality theory, environmental Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity, flow and the psychology of
inf luences, and attention to the creative and problem- discovery and invention. New York: Harper Collins.
solving performance of all individuals, not just those rec- Dacey, J. S. (1989). Discriminating characteristics of the fami-
ognized as exceptionally talented. Creativity or lies of highly creative adolescents. Journal of Creative
problem-solving styles may very well prove key in the Behavior, 23, 263–271.
search for effective instructional approaches designed to Davis, G. A. (1975). In frumious pursuit of the creative person.
develop creativity skills and modify environments to Journal of Creative Behavior, 9, 75–87.
Davis, G. A. (1998). Creativity is forever (4th ed.). Dubuque, IA:
enhance creative problem solving. It may also be an aid in
Kendall/Hunt.
the identification and development of talent that might
Davis, G. A., & Rimm, S. (1982). Group Inventory for Finding
otherwise have gone unnoticed. Interests (GIFFI) I and II: Instruments for identifying cre-
ative potential in the junior and senior high school. Journal
of Creative Behavior, 16, 50–57.
References Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.
Deci, E. L. (1980). The psychology of self-determination. Lexington,
Abra, J. (1997). The motives for creative work. Cresskill, NJ: MA: D. C. Heath/Lexington Books.
Hampton Press. Dellas, M., & Gaier, E. L. (1970). Identification of creativity:
Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity. New The individual. Psychological Bulletin, 73, 55–73.
York: Springer-Verlag. Domino, G. (1970). Identification of potentially creative per-
Amabile, T. M. (1989). Growing up creative: Nurturing a lifetime of sons from the Adjective Check List. Journal of Consulting
creativity. New York: Crown. and Clinical Psychology, 35, 48–51.
Amabile, T. M. (1990). Within you, without you: The social Dunn, R., Beaudry, J., and Klavas, A. (1989). Survey of
psychology of creativity, and beyond. In M. Runco & R. S. research on learning styles. Educational Leadership, 46(6),
Albert (Eds.), Theories of creativity (pp. 61–91). Newbury 50–58.
Park, CA: Sage. Esposito, B., Roehm, S., Treffinger, D., Selby, E., Isaksen, S., &
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context: Update to the social Lauer, K. (2004). Innovative leadership in today’s demand-
ing marketplace: A new tool for understanding our problem
psychology of creativity. Boulder, CO: Westview.
solving style to better leverage human assets. ABI Insights.
Arieti, S. (1970). Creativity: The magic synthesis. New York: Basic
Retrieved March 17, 2004, from http://www-
Books.
03.ibm.com/ibm/palisades/abinsight/issues/2004-Mar/
Barron, F. (1955). The disposition towards originality. Journal of
article-1-print.pdf
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 478–485.
Eysenck, H. J. (1983). Roots of creativity: Cognitive ability or
Barron, F. (1969). Creative person and creative process. New York:
personality trait? Roeper Review, 3, 10–12.
Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Eysenck, H. J. (1993). Creativity and personality: Suggestions
Barron, F. (1990). Creativity and mental health: Origins of personal
for a theory. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 147–178.
vitality and creative freedom. Buffalo, NY: Creative Education
Esyenck, H. J. (1997). Creativity and personality. In M. A.
Foundation. Runco (Ed.), The creativity research handbook: Volume 1 (pp.
Barron, F. (1995). No rootless flower: An ecology of creativity. 41–66). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.
Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. Feist, G. J. (1999). The inf luence of personality on artistic and
Basadur, M. (1994). Managing the creative process in organiza- scientific creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of
tions. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, creativity (pp. 273–296). New York: Cambridge University
and creativity (pp. 237–268). Norwood, NJ: Alblex. Press.
Basadur, M., Graen, G., & Wakabayashi, M. (1990). Identifying Flavell, J. H. (1963). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget.
individual differences in creative problem solving style. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.
Journal of Creative Behavior, 24, 111–131. Freud, S. (1908/1959). Creative writers and daydreaming. In J.
Bloom, B. S. (1963). Report on creativity research by the Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological
examiner’s office of the University of Chicago. In C. W. works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 9, pp. 143–144, 146–153).
Taylor & F. Barron (Eds.), Scientific creativity: Its recognition London: Hogarth Press.
and development (pp. 251–264). New York: Wiley. Gardner, H. (1993). Creating minds. New York: Basic Books.

G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4 3 1 1
T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

Gedo, J. E., & Gedo, M. M. (1992). Perspectives on creativity: The Khatena, J. (1971). Something About Myself: A brief screening
biographical method. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. device for identifying creatively gifted children and adults.
Gough, H. G. (1952). Adjective Check List. Palo Alto, CA: Gifted Child Quarterly, 15, 262–266.
Consulting Psychologists Press. Khatena, J., & Morse, D. (1994). Khatena-Morse Multitalent
Gough, H. G. (1979). A creative personality scale for the Perception Inventory. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Test Service.
Adjective Check List. Journal of Personality and Social Khatena, J., & Torrance, E. P. (1976). Khatena-Torrance Creative
Psychology, 37, 1398–1405. Perception Inventory. Chicago, IL: Stoelting Company.
Gowan, J. C. (1975). Trance, art, and creativity. Northridge, CA: Kirton, M. (1976). Adaptors and innovators: A description and
Author. measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 622–629.
Gruber, H. (1989). The evolving systems approach to creative Kirton, M. (Ed.). (1994). Adaptors and innovators: Styles of creativ-
work. In D. Wallace & H. Gruber (Eds.), Creative people at ity and problem-solving (Rev. ed.). New York: Routledge.
work: Twelve case studies (pp. 3–43). New York: Oxford. Kris, E. (1952). Psychoanalytic explorations in art. New York:
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 50, International Universities Press.
444–454. Kroeger, O., & Thueson, J. (1988). Type talk. New York: Dell.
Guilford, J. P. (1967). Creativity: Yesterday, today, and tomor- Lee, H. B. (1940). A theory concerning free creation in the
row. Journal of Creative Behavior, 1, 3–14. inventive arts. Psychiatry, 40, 283–286, 288–292.
Helson, R. (1965). Childhood interest clusters related to cre- Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of typological psychology. New York:
ativity in women. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 29, McGraw-Hill.
352–361. MacKinnon, D. W. (1962). The nature and nurture of creative
Helson, R. (1966). Personality of women with imaginative and talent. American Psychologist, 17, 484–495.
artistic interests: The role of masculinity, originality, and MacKinnon, D. W. (1970). The personality correlates of cre-
other characteristics in their creativity. Journal of Personality, ativity: A student of American architects. In P. E. Vernon
35, 214–233. (Ed.), Creativity (pp. 289–310). New York: Penguin.
Helson, R. (1967). Personality characteristics and developmen- MacKinnon, D. W. (1978). In search of human effectiveness:
tal history of creative college women. Genetic Psychology Identifying and developing creativity. Buffalo, NY: Creative
Monographs, 76, 205–256. Education Foundation.
Hennessey, B. A., & Amabile, T. M. (1998). Reward, intrinsic Mansfield, R. S., & Busse, T. V. (1981). The psychology of creativ-
motivation, and creativity. American Psychologist, 53, ity and discovery: Scientists and their work. Chicago: Nelson-
674–675. Hall.
Hennessey, B. A., & Zbikowski, S. M. (1993). Immunizing Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a psychology of being (2nd ed.). New
children against the negative effects of reward: A further York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
examination of intrinsic motivation training techniques. McMullen, W. E. (1976). Creative individuals: Paradoxical per-
Creativity Research Journal, 6, 297–307. sonages. Journal of Creative Behavior, 10, 265–275.
Houtz, J. C. (Ed.). (2003). The educational psychology of creativity. Myers, I., & McCaulley, M. (1985). Manual: A guide to the devel-
Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. opment and use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto,
Houtz, J. C., & Krug, D. (1995). Assessment of creativity: CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Resolving a mid-life crisis. Educational Psychology Review, 7, Myers, I., & Myers, P. (1980). Gifts differing. Palo Alto, CA:
269–300. Consulting Psychologists Press.
Houtz, J., LeBlanc, E., & Butera, T. (1994). Personality type, Nicholls, J. (1972). Creativity in the person who will never pro-
creativity, and classroom teaching style. Journal of Classroom duce anything original and useful: The concept of creativ-
Interaction, 29, 19–24. ity as a normally distributed trait. American Psychologist, 27,
Houtz, J. C., Selby, E., Esquivel, G. E., Okoye, R. A., Peters, 717–722.
K., & Treffinger, D. J. (2003). Creativity style and person- Niu, W., & Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Societal and school inf lu-
ality type. Creativity Research Journal, 15, 321–330. ences on student creativity: The case of China. Psychology
Houtz, J., Tetenbaum, T., & Phillips, R. H. (1981). Affective in the Schools, 40, 103–114.
correlates in the problem solving process. Journal of Oremland, J. D. (1997). The origins and psychodynamics of creativ-
Psychology, 109, 265–269. ity: A psychoanalytic perspective. Madison, CT: International
Jonassen, D. H., & Grabowski, B. L. (1993). Handbook of individ- Universities Press.
ual differences, learning, and instruction. Hillsdale, NJ: Osborn, A. F. (1963). Applied imagination: Principles and procedures
Erlbaum. of creative problem solving (3rd Rev. ed.). New York: Charles
Jung, C. (1923). On the relation of analytic psychology to Scribner’s Sons.
poetic art. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 3, 219, 225, Parnes, S. J. (1967). The literature on creativity. Part II. Journal
226, 227–231. of Creative Behavior, 1, 191–240.

3 1 2 G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4
T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

Phillips, W. (Ed.). (1957). Art and psychoanalysis: Studies in the Sinnott, E. W. (1959). The creativeness of life. In H. H.
application of psychoanalytic theory to the creative process. Anderson (Ed.), Creativity and its cultivation (pp. 21–29).
Cleveland, OH: Meridian. New York: Harper & Row.
Rank, O. (1932/1960). Art and artist (C. F. Atkinson, Trans.). Smith, G. J. W., & Carlsson, I. M. (1990). The creative process: A
New York: Alfred A. Knopf. functional model based on empirical studies from early childhood to
Renzulli, J. S., Hartman, R. K., & Callahan, C. M. (1975). middle age. Madison, CT: International Universities Press.
Scale for Rating the Behavioral Characteristics of Superior Starkweather, E. K. (1964). Problems in the measurement of
Students. In W. S. Barbe & J. S. Renzulli (Eds.), Psychology creativity in preschool children. Journal of Educational
and education of the gifted (2nd ed., pp. 264–272). New York: Measurement, 1, 109–113.
Irvington/Halstead/Wiley. Starkweather, E. K. (1976). Creativity research instruments
Renzulli, J. S., Sytsma, R. E., & Berman, K. B. (2000). designed for use with preschool children. In A. M. Biondi
Operation houndstooth. Storrs, CT: The National Research & S. J. Parnes (Eds.), Assessing creative growth: The tests—
Center on the Gifted and Talented, University of Book one (pp. 79–90). Buffalo, NY: Creative Education
Connecticut. Foundation.
Rhodes, M. (1961). An analysis of creativity. Phi Delta Kappan, Stein, M. I. (1974). Stimulating creativity. Volume 1: Individual pro-
42, 305–310. cedures. New York: Academic Press.
Roe, A. (1952). The making of a scientist. New York: Dodd, Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Thinking styles. New York: Cambridge
Mead. University Press.
Rogers, C. (1954). Toward a theory of creativity. ETC: A Sternberg, R. J. (2000). Identifying and developing creative
Review of General Semantics, 11, 250–258. giftedness. Roeper Review, 23, 60–65.
Rothenberg, A. (1971). The process of Janusian thinking in Taylor, C. W. (Ed.). (1964) Creativity: Progress and potential. New
creativity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 24, 195–205. York: McGraw-Hill.
Rothenberg, A. (1990). Creativity and madness: New findings and Taylor, C. W., & Barron, F. (Eds.). (1963). Scientific creativity: Its
old stereotypes. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University recognition and development. New York: John Wiley.
Press. Taylor, C. W., & Ellison, R. (1967, March). Biographical pre-
Runco, M. A., & Albert, R. S. (Eds.). (1990). Theories of creativ- dictors of scientific performance. Science, 155, 1075–1080.
ity. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Torrance, E. P. (1967). Nature of creative talents. Theory Into
Russ, S. (1993). Affect and creativity: The role of affect and play in the Practice, 5, 168–173, 201–202.
creative process. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Torrance, E. P. (1987). Teaching for creativity. In S. G. Isaksen
Schoonover, P. (1996). The preference for and use of creative (Ed.), Frontiers of creativity research: Beyond the basics (pp.
problem solving tools among adaptors and innovators. 189–215). Buffalo, NY: Bearly Limited.
Creative Learning Today: Center for Creative Learning Treffinger, D. J., Isaksen, S. G., & Dorval, K. B. (2000).
Newsletter, 6(3), 10–11. Creative problem solving: An introduction (4th ed.). Sarasota,
Selby, E. C. (1997). Lucy and Michael: Case studies of creative FL: Center for Creative Learning.
styles in teenagers. Creative Learning Today: Center for Treffinger, D. J., Young, G. C., Nassab, C. A., & Wittig, C. V.
Creative Learning Newsletter, 7(2), 4–6. (2004). Enhancing and expanding gifted programs: The levels of
Selby, E. C. (2000, September). Helping gifted children appre- service approach. Waco, TX: Prufrock Press.
ciate their problem solving style. Parenting for High Potential, Treffinger, D., Young, G., Selby, E., & Shepardson C. (2002).
18–22. Assessing creativity: A guide for educators. Storrs, CT: The
Selby, E. C., Treffinger, D. J., Isaksen, S. G., & Lauer, K. J. National Research Center on the Gifted and Talented.
(2002). VIEW: An assessment of problem solving style. Technical Vervalin, C. (1962). Just what is creativity? Hydrocarbon
manual and user’s guide. Sarasota, FL: Center for Creative Processing, 41(10), 109–111.
Learning. Wallace, D. B., & Gruber, H. E. (Eds.). (1989). Creative people at
Selby, E. C., Treffinger, D. J., Isaksen, S. G., & Lauer, K. J. work: Twelve case studies. New York: Oxford.
(2004). VIEW: An assessment of problem solving style technical Welsh, G. S., & Barron, F. (1963). Barron-Welsh Art Scale: A por-
manual. Sarasota, FL: Center for Creative Learning. tion of the Welsh Figure Preference Test. Palo Alto, CA:
Selby, E., Treffinger, D., Isaksen, S., & Powers, S. (1993). Use Consulting Psychologists Press.
of the Kirton Adaption-Innovation Inventory with middle White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of
school students. Journal of Creative Behavior, 27, 223–235. competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.
Simonton, D. K. (1987). Genius: The lessons of historiometry. Williams, F. E. (1972). Total program for individualizing and
In S. G. Isaksen (Ed.), Frontiers of creativity research: Beyond humanizing the learning process. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
the basics (pp. 66–87). Buffalo, NY: Bearly Limited. Educational Technology Publications.
Simonton, D. K. (1988). Creativity, leadership, and chance. In Williams, F. E. (1973). Stabilizing the swings: A synergistic
R. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity (pp. 386–426). approach toward more creativity in education. Journal of
New York: Cambridge University Press. Creative Behavior, 7, 187–195.

G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4 3 1 3
T H E C R E AT I V E P E R S O N A L I T Y

End Note
1. For greater detail concerning creativity theories,
the reader is referred to Houtz (2003) and Runco and
Albert (1990).

3 1 4 G I F T E D C H I L D Q U A R T E R LY • F A L L 2 0 0 5 • V O L 4 9 N O 4

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi