Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 5

A newsletter dedicated to speech & language in school-age children

Social-Emotional Outcomes for


Children With SLI IN THIS IS S UE
Mok, P. L. H., Pickles, A., Durkin, K., & Conti-Ramsden, G.
REVIEWS
(2014). Longitudinal trajectories of peer relations in children with
specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 55, 516–527 Social-Emotional Outcomes
Summarized by Carol Westby for Children With SLI
page 1
Specific language impairment (SLI) is not so specific. SLI
R E V I E W significantly affects academic attainments and vocational
options (Conti-Ramsden, Durkin, Simkin, & Knox, 2009). SLI
Empathy in Students Who Are
Deaf or Hard of Hearing
puts students at risk not only for academic difficulties but also page 5
for social-emotional difficulties. Difficulties in expression and/
or comprehension of language make participation in conversation Pragmatic Knowledge Versus
challenging. As a consequence, children with SLI engage less in active Pragmatic Use
interactions than do those with typical language, enter less frequently into page 8
positive social interactions, are less sensitive to the initiations offered by
others, and manifest situationally inappropriate verbal responses (e.g., RESOURCE REVIEW
Fujiki, Brinton, Isaacson, & Summers, 2001; McCormack, Harrison,
McLeod, & McAllister, 2011). Experiencing difficulties with peer Observational Pragmatics Scales
relations during childhood means that many children with SLI enter page 12
adolescence less equipped in the skills needed for this area of life.
Numerous studies have shown the link between early SLI and I D E A S WA P
literacy skills. Consequently, speech-language pathologists (SLP)
incorporate reading and writing activities in their language intervention. Assessing Personal Event
Less attention has been given to the social-emotional implications of Narratives
SLI and the roles that SLPs might play in this area. In a recent study, page 13
Conti-Ramsden and colleagues investigated the developmental
trajectories of behavioral, emotional, and social difficulties in individuals
with a history of SLI from childhood to adolescence (St. Clair, Pickles,
Durkin, & Conti-Ramsden, 2011). They observed an increase in peer
problems and in the proportion of individuals functioning in the
impaired range from childhood to adolescence. By 16 years of age,
against national norms, nearly 40% of adolescents with SLI appeared
impaired in their interactions with peers.
SLI is a very heterogeneous disorder. In general, children with SLI
are sociable (Wadman, Durkin, & Conti-Ramsden, 2008), but there is
evidence that they are more likely to be withdrawn (Brinton & Fujiki,
2002) and less likely to exhibit skilled social behaviors (Fujiki, Brinton,

Volume 28 • Issue 4 http://wom.sagepub.com March/April 2017


Word of Mouth 28:4 March/April 2017

Morgan, & Hart, 1999). The authors of this study their concurrent language and Performance Intelligence
sought to identify the developmental patterns that lead Quotient (PIQ) scores:
to specific outcomes. They examined factors that have
the potential to influence developmental trajectories of •• SLI-PIQ standard score 85 or above (i.e., in the
children with SLI, including expressive, receptive, and normal range) and concurrent receptive or expressive
expressive language skills, as well as emotional language standard scores below 85.
symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity, and •• Non-specific language impaired–PIQ standard
prosocial behavior. scores below 85 and receptive or expressive language
standard scores below 85.
•• Low cognition, resolved language–PIQ standard
Participants scores below 85 and receptive and expressive
language standard scores 85 or above.
Participants had a history of SLI and were originally •• Resolved language–PIQ, receptive and expressive
part of a wider study: the Manchester Language Study language standard scores all 85 or above.
(Conti-Ramsden & Botting, 1999; Conti-Ramsden,
Crutchley, & Botting, 1997). The majority, 70.5%, of the children had impaired
The initial cohort of 242 children (6;6 to 7;9 years) language ability at age 7. There was little change in the
was a random sample of 50% of all 7-year-olds language status at age 8. At ages 11 and 16, the
attending 118 language units across England. Children percentages of children with SLI fell to 39.6% and
were excluded from the study if they were reported by 36.1%, respectively, while the percentages with non-
their teachers as having frank neurological difficulties, specific language impairment rose to 43.8% and 48.9%,
hearing impairment, a diagnosis of autism, or a general respectively. The changing SLI profiles of some of the
learning disability. Participants were contacted again at participants were thus due to their PIQ scores having
ages 8, 11, 14, and 16. The average standard scores for fallen since they were recruited to the study. There is
receptive language at both ages and for expressive evidence suggesting that children with low PIQ and
language at age 7 years were around 1 SD below the language skills perform in important ways much like
population mean, while average expressive language children with a history of SLI who have PIQ within the
score at age 11 years was close to 2 SD below. normal range (Leonard, 2003). At ages 11 and 16,
Participants were classified into four groups based on around 85% of the children showed language ability in

Teller Road, Thousand Oaks, CA 91320; phone:


800.818.SAGE (7243) and 805.499.0721; fax:
805.375.1700; e-mail: journals@sagepub.com;
Publisher: Donald D. Hammill Web site: journals.sagepub.com. Subscription
Periodicals Director: Peggy Kipping Price: Institutions: $77; Individuals: $43. For
Editor: Carol Westby, PhD Managing Editor, Prod.: Lisa Tippett all customers outside the Americas, please visit
For SAGE Publications: Bob Grantz http://www.sagepub.co.uk/customercare.nav for
Bilingual Multicultural Services Word of Mouth (ISSN #1048-3950) (J615) is information. Claims: Claims for undelivered
Board Members published five times annually—in September/ copies must be made no later than six months
Jennifer Enote-Becker, MA, October, November/December, January/February, following month of publication. Sage will
Zuni Public Schools March/April, and May/June—by the Hammill supply missing copies when losses have been
Marjory Handy-Reynolds, MA Institute on Disabilities and SAGE Publishing. sustained in transit and when the reserve stock
Belleville Public School District Send address changes to Word of Mouth, c/o SAGE will permit.
Publishing, 2455 Teller Road, Thousand Oaks, CA Copyright Permission: To request permission for
Deborah Hwa-Froelich, PhD
91320. republishing, reproducing, or distributing material
Saint Louis University Copyright © 2017 by Hammill Institute on from this journal, please visit the desired article on
Judy Johnson, MA, Wichita Public Schools Disabilities. All rights reserved. No portion of the the SAGE Journals website (journals.sagepub.
Karen Kuhn, MA, Wichita Public Schools contents may be reproduced in any form without com) and click "Permissions." For additional
Donna Reid, MA, Wichita Public Schools written permission from Sage Publishing. Change of information, please see https://us.sagepub.com/
Rosario Roman, MS, Bilingual Multicultural Address: Six weeks advance notice must be given en-us/journals-permissions. Author Guidelines
when notifying of change of address. Please send old are available from the editor: Carol Westby, 1808
Services
address label along with the new address to ensure Princeton NE, Albuquerque, NM 87106; e-mail:
Judy Sawyer, MA, Wichita State University proper identification. Please specify name of journal. carol_westby@att.net. Word of Mouth is indexed
Peggy Sawyer, EdS, Boise School District Subscription Information: All subscription and the Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied
Julie Scherz, PhD, Wichita State University inquiries, orders, back issues, claims, and renewals Health Literature.
Kathy Strattman, PhD, Wichita State University should be addressed to Sage Publishing, 2455
Becci Werner, MA, Wichita Public Schools

2
Word of Mouth 28:4 March/April 2017

the impaired range. None of the children in the original version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–
cohort had a diagnosis of autism. Third Edition (Wechsler, 1992) was administered.

Receptive and Expressive Language


Measures
At ages 7and 11, receptive language was assessed
Peer Relations
using the Test for Reception of Grammar (Bishop,
Two measures were used to assess peer relations: the 1982). Expressive language at age 7 was assessed using
peer problem subscale of the teacher-report version of the Bus Story Test (Renfrew, 1991), and at age 11, it
the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; was measured by the Recalling Sentences subtest of the
Goodman, 1997), and teachers’ responses to three Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals–
items in the Rutter Children’s Behaviour Questionnaire Revised (Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 1987).
(Rutter, 1967). The peer problem subscale of the SDQ
Reading Accuracy and Comprehension
was administered at ages 11 and 16. Items of the peer
problem subscale include Rather solitary, tends to play The Word Reading subtest of the British Abilities
alone, Has at least one good friend, Generally liked by Scale (Elliot, 1983) was used to assess reading accuracy
other children, Picked on or bullied by other children, at age 7. At age 11, the Basic Reading and the Reading
and Gets on better with adults than with other children. Comprehension subtests of the Wechsler Objective
The Rutter Children’s Behaviour Questionnaire was Reading Dimensions (Wechsler, 1993) were used to
completed by the participants’ teachers at ages 7, 8, and measure reading accuracy and reading comprehension,
11 years. This consists of 26 statements, and the teacher respectively.
is asked to score each item as doesn’t apply, applies
somewhat, or certainly applies. Scores derived from Pragmatic Language
the Rutter questionnaire and from the SDQ have been
found to be highly correlated and have equivalent Of the 171 children in this study, 54 (31.6%) were
predictive validity (Goodman, 1997). considered to have semantic/pragmatic difficulties at
age 7. Pragmatic language skills were formally assessed
Prosocial Behaviour and Behavioral Difficulties at age 11 using the Children’s Communication Checklist
(CCC; Bishop, 1998). The checklist has been shown to
The SDQ behavioral measures (teacher-reported differentiate between children with pragmatic language
version; Goodman, 1997): prosocial behavior, emotional impairment and those with more typical SLI.
symptoms, conduct problems, and hyperactivity were
obtained at age 11 years. The prosocial subscale consists
of five positive items: considerate of other people’s Results
feelings, shares readily with other children, helpful if
•• Peer problems increased significantly between 7 and
someone is hurt, upset or feeling ill, kind to younger
11, and continued to increase beyond 11, but not
children, and often volunteers to help others. Examples
significantly.
of items constituting three other behavioral difficulty
•• A four-group model best fit the data: low-level/no
subscales include: Many worries, often seems worried
problems in peer relations (22.2%), childhood-
(emotional symptoms); Often has temper tantrums or hot
limited problems (12.3%), childhood-onset persistent
tempers (conduct problems); Constantly fidgeting or
problems (39.2%), and adolescent-onset problems
squirming (hyperactivity). Thresholds for identifying
(26.3%).
normal, borderline, and abnormal behavior are available
•• The group with low/no problems exhibited few
for the subscales.
problems from childhood to adolescence. For the
group with childhood-limited problems, by age 16,
the SDQ mean peer problem score had fallen, and
Performance IQ (PIQ) none of these children were classified as having
Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices was used to borderline or abnormal levels of difficulties. The
assess participants’ PIQ at age 7 (Raven, 1986). At age childhood-onset persistent problems group showed
11, Block Design and Picture Completion of the U.K. an increase in the Rutter mean scores between ages

3
Word of Mouth 28:4 March/April 2017

7 and 11. Overall, 74.1% of children in this group at pragmatic difficulties was informative at age 7 as was
age 11 and 62.2% at age 16 were classified as having the CCC and the SDQ emotional scale in middle
borderline or abnormal levels of peer problems. For childhood, at age 11. Results of randomized controlled
the group with adolescent-onset problems, the trials show that pragmatic difficulties (Adams et al.,
percentages scoring above the SDQ threshold for 2012) and emotional problems (Sanders, Baker, &
borderline or abnormal levels of peer problems Turner, 2012) can be addressed effectively, in at least
increased from 10% to 40.6% during this period. some children. The authors conclude that clinically, it is
•• Five predictors for group membership were measures advisable not only to identify deficits but to evaluate
of pragmatic language skills at age 11, prosocial the extent to which potential protective and/or positive
behavior, hyperactivity levels, and levels of factors are present. The SDQ prosocial scale in middle
emotional and conduct disorders. childhood was found to be a particularly strong predictor
•• For children with persistent peer problems, the odds of differences in the trajectories of peer relations of
of being rated by their teachers as having pragmatic children with a history of SLI.
language difficulties was 2.5 times higher than for Just as SLPs now typically consider literacy skills when
children with low/no problems. working with students with SLI, they should also consider
the early signs of social difficulties and incorporate goals
in these areas on students’ individualized educational
Discussion plans.
Somewhat surprising was the finding that there was not
a significant relationship between expressive and receptive References
language abilities and the different trajectories. The
authors suggest that this was because the language Adams, C., Lockton, E., Freed, J., Gaile, J., Earl, G., McBean,
K., . . . Law, J. (2012). The Social Communication
assessments tapped only some structural language skills
Intervention Project: A randomized controlled trial of the
and not higher order skills such as making inferences and effectiveness of speech and language therapy for school-age
organizing discourse or texts. This finding could be children who have pragmatic and social communicative
viewed as a caution for SLPs. The majority of popular problems with or without autism spectrum disorders.
assessments for SLI focus on structural aspects of language International Journal of Language and Communication
development. As students get older, they may master these Disorders, 47, 233–244.
Bishop, D.V.M. (1982). Test for reception of grammar.
skills, but continue to have difficulty with the language Manchester, England: Author.
demands in social and academic settings. Scores on these Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the Children’s
formal tests may be used, inappropriately, to justify Communication Checklist (CCC): A method for assessing
discharging them from services. The findings offer positive qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children.
messages regarding peer relations without problems for Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 879–893.
Brinton, B., & Fujiki, M. (2002). Social development in children
some children and of amelioration of difficulties across
with specific language impairment and profound hearing
time in others. The groups with low/no problems or with loss. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), Blackwell handbook
childhood-limited difficulties comprised approximately of childhood social development (pp. 588–603). Malden,
one third of the sample. In contrast, children who had MA: Blackwell.
persistent, or adolescent-onset, difficulties in peer relations Conti-Ramsden, G., & Botting, N. (1999). Characteristics of
from childhood to adolescence represented two thirds of children attending language units in England: A national
study of 7-year-olds. International Journal of Language &
children with a history of SLI. Communication Disorders, 34, 359–366.
Differences in peer relations were observed from Conti-Ramsden, G., Crutchley, A., & Botting, N. (1997). The
early childhood (7 years) and were evident in extent to which psychometric tests differentiate subgroups of
adolescence, with trajectories diverging more widely as children with specific language impairment. Journal of
children entered middle childhood. Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 765–777.
Conti-Ramsden, G., Durkin, K., Simkin, Z., & Knox, E. (2009).
Based on these findings, the authors suggest that the
Specific language impairment and school outcomes. I:
identification of pragmatic difficulties and emotional Identifying and explaining variability at the end of compulsory
problems could be critical to the amelioration of potential education. International Journal of Language and
difficulties in peer relations. The use of teacher report of Communication Disorders, 44, 15–35.

4
Word of Mouth 28:4 March/April 2017

Elliot, C. D. (1983). British Ability Scales. Windsor, UK: Rutter, M. (1967). A children’s behaviour questionnaire for
NFER-Nelson. completion by teachers: Preliminary findings. Journal of
Fujiki, M., Brinton, B., Isaacson, T., & Summers, C. (2001). Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 8, 1–11.
Social behaviors of children with language impairment on Sanders, M. R., Baker, S., & Turner, K. M. (2012). A
the playground: A pilot study. Language, Speech, and randomized controlled trial evaluating the efficacy of
Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 101–113. Triple P Online with parents of children with early-onset
Fujiki, M., Brinton, B., Morgan, M., & Hart, C.H. (1999). conduct problems. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50,
Withdrawn and sociable behavior of children with language 675–684.
impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (1987). Clinical evaluation of
Schools, 30, 183–195. language fundamentals–revised. San Antonio: Psychological
Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Corporation.
Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology St. Clair, M.C., Pickles, A., Durkin, K., & Conti-Ramsden, G.
and Psychiatry, 38, 581–586. (2011). A longitudinal study of behavioral, emotional and
Leonard, L. (2003). Specific language impairment: social difficulties in individuals with a history of specific
Characterizing the deficit. In Y. Levy & J. Schaeffer (Eds.), language impairment (SLI). Journal of Communication
Towards a definition of SLI (pp. 209–231). Mahwah, NJ: Disorders, 44, 186–199.
Lawrence Erlbaum. Wadman, R., Durkin, K., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2008). Self-
McCormack, J., Harrison, L. J., McLeod, S., & McAllister, L. esteem, shyness, and sociability in adolescents with specific
(2011). A nationally representative study of the association language impairment (SLI). Journal of Speech, Language,
between communication impairment at 4–5 years and and Hearing Research, 51, 938–952.
children’s life activities at 7–9 years. Journal of Speech, Wechsler, D. (1992). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children–
Language, and Hearing Research, 54, 1328–1348. Third Edition–Revised, UK. Sidcup, UK: The Psychological
Raven, J. C. (1986). Coloured progressive matrices. London, Corporation.
England: H. K. Lewis. Wechsler, D. (1993). Wechsler objective reading dimensions.
Renfrew, C. (1991). The bus story: A test of continuous speech. London, England: The Psychological Corporation.
Oxford, UK: Author.

Empathy in Students Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing


Netten, A. P., Rieffe, C., Theunissen, S. C. P. M., Soede, W., Dirks, E., Briaire, J. J., &
Frijns, J. H. M. (2015). Low empathy in deaf and hard of hearing (pre)adolescents compared
to normal hearing controls. PLoS ONE, 10(4), Article e0124102. doi:10.1371/journal
Summarized by Carol Westby

Empathy is defined as the ability to perceive and empathy between DHH and NH children. To identify
understand another person’s emotional state and the those factors that may be most influential for the levels
competence to appropriately respond to others’ of empathy in DHH children, the researchers also
emotions. It is needed to induce prosocial behavior and investigated the influence of several audiological
is often referred to as the social glue in relationships. factors on empathic abilities, such as language
Little is known about the development of empathy in development, intelligence, degree of hearing loss, age
deaf and hard-of-hearing (DHH) children, but at intervention of hearing loss, type of device, mode of
prerequisites for empathy, such as emotion recognition communication, and educational setting.
and regulation and development of a Theory of Mind
(ToM), have been shown to be delayed in DHH
children. Although, initially, children with cochlear The Study
implants (CI) were found to perform as well as normal
hearing (NH) children, at an older age, they fell behind Participants
on more advanced ToM abilities such as false belief
tasks (Bachara, Raphael, & Phelan, 1980; Ketelaar, The researchers recruited 122 DHH children and a
Rieffe, Wiefferink, & Frijns, 2012; Rieffe, 2012) control group consisting of 162 NH children from the
The purpose of this study was to determine the Netherlands and the Dutch-speaking part of Belgium to
differences in the levels of self-reported and observed participate in this study. All children were between 9

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi