Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 10

European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 23, pp. 1860–1868, 2006 doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04703.

Estrogen receptor-b gene disruption potentiates estrogen-


inducible aggression but not sexual behaviour in male mice

Masayoshi Nomura,1 Sandra Andersson,2 Kenneth S. Korach,3 Jan-Åke Gustafsson,2 Donald W. Pfaff1 and Sonoko
Ogawa1
1
Laboratory of Neurobiology and Behaviour, The Rockefeller University, 1230 York Avenue, New York, NY 10021, USA
2
Department of Medical Nutrition, Karolinska Institute, S-141–86 Hudding, Sweden
3
The Laboratory Reproductive and Developmental Toxicology, National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences, Research
Triangle Park, North Carolina 27709, USA

Keywords: androgen receptor, knockout mouse, estrogen receptor a, sex difference, testosterone

Abstract
Aggressive behaviour of gonadally intact male mice is increased by estrogen receptor (ER)-b gene disruption, whereas sexual
behaviour remains unchanged. The elevated aggression levels following ER-b gene disruption is pronounced during repeated
aggression tests in young animals and the first aggression test in adults. In the present study, the roles of ER-b activation in the
regulation of aggressive and sexual behaviour were investigated in gonadectomized ER-b knockout (bERKO) and wild-type (WT)
male mice treated with various doses of estrogen. Overall, estradiol benzoate (EB) treatment induced higher levels of aggression in
bERKO mice than in WT mice. In WT mice, the levels of aggression induced by EB were highest in the lowest-dose (2.5 lg ⁄ day)
group and gradually decreased in higher-dosage groups. On the other hand, equally high levels of aggressive behaviour were
induced by all three doses of EB in bERKO mice. A marked genotype difference in dose responses is inferred, such that the ER-a-
mediated facilitatory action of estrogen is more pronounced at lower and physiological doses and the ER-b-mediated inhibitory action
is only unveiled at higher doses of estrogen. In contrast to aggression, the levels of sexual behaviour induced by EB were not different
between bERKO and WT at either dose of EB (2.5 and 12.5 lg ⁄ day) examined. These findings support the notion that ER-b
activation may exert an attenuating action on male aggression induced by estrogen through ER-a-mediated brain mechanisms,
whereas its effect on male sexual behaviour is relatively small.

Introduction
It is well established that testosterone is a major hormone facilitating In a series of behavioural studies using single (aERKO and
aggressive and sexual behaviour in male mice. Testosterone exerts its bERKO) or double (abERKO) ER-knockout mice, we confirmed the
neural function not only by acting through androgen receptors (AR), in involvement of ERs in male aggressive behaviour and found that the
its original form or as the 5a-reduced form (dihydrotestosterone), but two types of ERs differentially regulate them. Aggressive behaviour
also through estrogen receptors (ER) after aromatization to estradiol was greatly reduced in both aERKO and abERKO male mice, whereas
(E2). There is accumulating evidence that not only AR-dependent they were increased in bERKO male mice (Ogawa et al., 1997; Ogawa
mechanisms but also ER-dependent mechanisms play crucial roles in the et al., 1998; Ogawa et al., 1999; Ogawa et al., 2000; Scordalakes &
regulation of aggressive and sexual behaviour in male mice directly in Rissman, 2003), particularly in young (5–12 weeks) age groups
adulthood as well as through organizational action during perinatal (Nomura et al., 2002a) compared to their respective wild-type (WT)
development (Simon & Whalen, 1986; Simon & Whalen, 1987; Ogawa control mice. The opposite effects ER-a and ER-b gene disruption on
et al., 1997; Wersinger et al., 1997). Two types of ERs, ER-a and the aggressive behaviour led us to hypothesize that activation of ER-a by
more recently identified ER-b, have been identified in the central E2 may be necessary for the induction of aggressive behaviour whereas
nervous system (Shughrue et al., 1997; Laflamme et al., 1998; Shughrue activation of ER-b by E2 may inhibit aggression in male mice
& Merchenthaler, 2001; Mitra et al., 2003). ER-a and -b are similar depending on age and social situation. In gonadally intact mice,
estrogen-binding proteins which act as ligand-dependent transcription however, we could not rule out the possibility that elevated levels of
factors. Neuroanatomical, transfection and behavioural studies collec- aggression in bERKO mice were simply because AR-dependent neural
tively suggest that ER-a and ER-b play different roles in the regulation systems of aggression were compensatory in a potentiated fashion.
of neuroendocrine functions and behaviour (Laflamme et al., 1998; Furthermore, it is not known whether ER-b activation directly inhibits
Couse & Korach, 1999; Shapiro et al., 2000; Shughrue et al., 2000; aggression induced by activation of ER-a by E2, as an aromatization
Vasudevan et al., 2001; Ogawa et al., 2002; Gustafsson, 2003). product of testosterone, or ER-b activation inhibits aggression induced
by activation of AR by androgens. In the present study therefore we
tested whether ER-b activation may actively participate in modulating
Correspondence: Dr Sonoko Ogawa, as above. aggressive behaviour induced by ER-a-mediated actions of E2 by
E-mail: ogawa@mail.rockefeller.edu
examining aggressive behaviour in response to various doses of
Received 2 October 2005, revised 6 January 2006, accepted 18 January 2006 estrogen in gonadectomized (GDX) male mice.

ª The Authors (2006). Journal Compilation ª Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
ER-b and male aggressive and sexual behaviour 1861

In male sexual behaviour, on the other hand, simple mounting was they can elicit aggressive behaviour from resident mice. By testing
altered greatly only in the mice lacking both ER genes, but not in against olfactory bulbectomized intruder mice, aggressive behaviour
single-knockout mice (Ogawa et al., 1997; Ogawa et al., 1998; Ogawa of resident animals, which were not influenced by any experience of
et al., 1999; Ogawa et al., 2000). These findings suggest that, for the defeat, were therefore measured (Denenberg et al., 1973). Each
induction of male sexual behaviour, androgenic stimulation of AR resident mouse was tested against a different intruder mouse during six
alone may not be sufficient and at least one ER needs to be intact. aggression tests. All aggressive behaviour tests were performed under
Although in bERKO mice all components of sexual behaviour were red light during the dark phase of the light–dark cycle starting 2 h after
intact (Ogawa et al., 1999; Ogawa et al., 2000), it is not determined lights were turned off. The aggressive behaviour tests were videotaped
yet whether the levels of sexual behaviour may still remain intact in for further analysis by an observer who did not know the genotypes or
bERKO mice even when only ER-a, but not AR, are activated. the treatment groups of the mice.
Therefore, in the present study we also examined the levels of male
sexual behaviour in estrogen-treated gonadectomized bERKO and WT
male mice. Definition and measurements of aggressive behaviour
Chasing, boxing, tail rattling, biting, lunging and offensive lateral
attack (often accompanied by biting and wrestling), previously shown
Materials and methods to be typical for intermale (male vs. male) aggression, were defined as
Mice aggressive behavioural acts (Ogawa et al., 1996). An aggression bout
One hundred and forty-five male bERKO mice and their WT was defined as a continuous series of behavioural interactions that
littermates were used. They were obtained from the breeding colony included at least one aggressive behavioural act. Three seconds was
maintained at the Rockefeller University by mating heterozygous the maximum amount of time that could elapse between aggressive
mice. Original breeding pairs (mixed background of C57BL ⁄ 6J and behavioural acts to be considered part of the same aggressive bout; if
129) were obtained from the National Institute of Environmental intervals between the occurrences of two aggressive behavioural acts
Health Sciences. Genotyping of tail DNA was accomplished by exceeded 3 s, the two behavioural acts were scored as two separate
using PCR with primers from intron 2 (5¢-GTGATGAGCT- aggressive bouts.
GAGGTGGTGCTT-3¢), the 3¢ end of the Neo (5¢-GCAGCCTCTG All aggressive bouts observed during the 15-min aggression tests
TTCCACATACA-3¢) and exon 3 (5¢-CATCCTTCACAGGACC- were combined and defined as total aggressive bouts. In addition,
AGACAC-3¢); a 1435-bp band (intron 2 and exon 3 primers) is aggressive behaviour bouts with offensive lateral attacks (the most
amplified for homozygous wild-type (+ ⁄ +) mice, a 1479-bp band vigorous aggressive behaviour acts) were counted separately from
(intron 2 and Neo primers) for homozygous mutant mice, and both those without offensive attacks. For each experimental male, cumu-
bands for heterozygous (+ ⁄ –) mice. All mice were gonadectomized lative durations of total aggressive bouts, offensive attack bouts and
under isoflurane inhalation anaesthesia at the age of 9–15 weeks and nonattack bouts were recorded.
individually housed in plastic cages (30 · 20 · 12 cm) throughout
the study. They were maintained on a 12–12 h light–dark cycle (light
off at noon) at constant temperature (22 C) throughout the period of Sexual behaviour tests
the studies. Food and water were available ad libitum. All An additional 63 mice were tested for sexual behaviour on the 14th
procedures were approved by the IACUC at the Rockefeller and 21st day (first and second preimplant tests, respectively) after
University. GDX and assigned to one of three different treatment groups (see
below) matched in the levels of total sexual behavioural acts during
the preimplant tests. Three days after the second preimplant test,
Aggressive behaviour tests animals were implanted with an EB pellet at a dose of 2.5 (WT,
Eighty-two mice were tested for aggression once on the 9th day after n ¼ 11; bERKO, n ¼ 9) or 12.5 lg ⁄ day (WT, n ¼ 9; bERKO,
GDX (preimplant test) and assigned to one of four different treatment n ¼ 13), or a placebo pellet (WT, n ¼ 11; bERKO, n ¼ 10). All
groups (see below) matched in the levels of total aggression during the pellets were on a 21-day release schedule and the average amount
preimplant test. Eight days after the preimplant test, animals were released per day was calculated based on the total amount in the
implanted under isoflurane inhalation anaesthesia with an estradiol pellets. Mice were then tested for sexual behaviour four more times
benzoate (EB) pellet (Innovative Research of America, USA) between (first to fourth postimplant tests), one test every 5 days. Male sexual
subcutaneous and muscle layers around the shoulder at doses of 2.5 behaviour was measured during a 30-min behavioural test with a
(WT, n ¼ 9; bERKO, n ¼ 13), 5.0 (WT, n ¼ 9; bERKO, n ¼ 12) or female mouse in the male’s home cage during the dark phase (2–6 h
12.5 lg ⁄ day (WT, n ¼ 10; bERKO, n ¼ 11), or a placebo pellet after lights off) of the light–dark cycle under red light. All stimulus
(WT, n ¼ 7; bERKO, n ¼ 11). All pellets were on a 21-day release females (heterozygous mice from the bERKO breeding colony) were
schedule and the average amount released per day was calculated ovariectomized and primed with 10 lg of EB (48 h prior to the tests)
based on the total amount in the pellets. Mice were then tested for and 500 lg of progesterone (4–7 h prior to the tests) to ensure high
aggressive behaviour five more times (first to fifth postimplant tests) sexual receptivity.
every 3–4 days.
Aggressive behaviour was assessed in a resident–intruder paradigm.
Each mouse was tested in his home cage (as a resident) against a Definition and measurements of sexual behaviour
group-housed (four or five mice per cage) weight-matched olfactory Male sexual behaviour, i.e. mount, intromission and ejaculation,
bulbectomized male mouse (heterozygous mouse from the bERKO were defined as previously described (McGill, 1970). For each male,
breeding colony) for 15 min. Expression of aggression in mice is the number and latency (1800 s was used for mice that did not show
mainly regulated by olfactory cues; therefore, olfactory bulbectomized the behaviour) of mounts, intromissions and ejaculations were
intruders rarely show aggression. However, as their gonads are intact, recorded.

ª The Authors (2006). Journal Compilation ª Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 1860–1868
1862 M. Nomura et al.

Measurement of serum estradiol fourth postimplant tests, when EB had the maximum effects on
After decapitation trunk blood was collected for measurements of aggression in bERKO mice. The levels of aggression were examined
serum estradiol one day after the completion of behaviour tests (19th in terms of (i) the number of tests with the behaviour (ranging from 0
and 21st day after pellet implant for mice tested for aggression and to 3) and (ii) the mean cumulative duration during the three
sexual behaviour, respectively). Serum levels of estradiol were postimplant tests, for each of three aggressive behavioural bouts, i.e.
determined using Coat-A-Count Total Estradiol Kit (DPC, USA). total aggressive bouts, offensive attack bouts and nonattack bouts.

Total aggressive bouts


Data analysis and statistics
During the second to fourth postimplant tests, there were overall
All data are presented as mean ± SEM. Data on cumulative duration of significant genotype differences in EB-treated groups in both the
total aggressive bouts, offensive attack bouts and nonattack bouts were number of tests with aggression (F1,58 ¼ 5.28, P < 0.05) and the
log-transformed before further statistical analyses to ensure homogen- cumulative duration of total aggressive bouts (F1,58 ¼ 4.04, P < 0.05;
eity of the variance. Behavioural data were first analysed using a two- Fig. 2A), whereas the interaction between genotype and treatment in
way anova for repeated measurements for the main effects of treatment the number of tests with aggression did not reach statistical
(for aggressive behavioural data) or genotype (for sexual behavioural significance (F1,78 ¼ 2.80, P ¼ 0.102). Overall, EB-treated bERKO
data), test day as a repeated measure, and their interactions to examine mice were more aggressive than WT mice. These genotype differences
the time course of behavioural changes before and after hormonal were most pronounced in the group treated with 5.0 lg ⁄ day of EB. At
treatment. Behavioural data combined for multiple test days and blood this dose, bERKO mice were significantly more aggressive than WT
estradiol data were analysed using a two-way factorial anova for the mice in terms of both the number of tests with the behaviour
main effects of genotype, treatment and their interaction followed by (F1,19 ¼ 5.48, P < 0.05) and cumulative duration of bouts
post hoc pair-wise comparisons. Fisher’s exact probability test was used (F1,19 ¼ 4.53, P < 0.05). WT mice showed relatively high levels of
for post hoc pair-wise comparisons at a ¼ 0.05. aggression only in the group treated with 2.5 lg ⁄ day of EB. At the
higher doses, EB failed to induce aggressive behaviour beyond the
Results placebo levels. In contrast to WT mice, in bERKO mice all three doses
of EB induced aggression. Particularly, bERKO mice from groups
Aggressive behaviour
treated with 2.5 and 5.0 lg ⁄ day of EB showed significantly higher
Changes in aggressive behaviour during pre- and postimplant tests levels of aggression than the placebo group (Fig. 2A).
Changes in cumulative duration of total aggressive bouts during six
aggression tests were first examined in each genotype by combining Offensive attack bouts
data from three EB treatment groups and comparing them with those EB-treated bERKO mice showed a significantly higher number of
of the placebo control group. tests with offensive attack bouts than WT mice (F1,58 ¼ 4.31,
In the preimplant tests, which were performed 9 days after GDX, P < 0.05). EB-treated bERKO mice also tended to show longer
some mice of both genotypes were still aggressive (Fig. 1A and B). duration of offensive attack bouts than WT mice although this
Mean ± SEM cumulative durations of total aggression in WT and difference did not reach statistically significant levels (F1,58 ¼ 3.47,
bERKO mice were 7.9 ± 4.3 and 15.1 ± 7.4 s, respectively, and there P ¼ 0.067; Fig. 2B). WT and bERKO mice showed similar levels of
was no genotype difference. In bERKO mice, as seen in the placebo offensive attacks in the group treated with 2.5 lg ⁄ day of EB, whereas
group, aggressive behaviour had subsided by the first postimplant test bERKO mice showed higher levels of aggression than WT mice in the
(21 days after GDX and 4 days after pellet implant) and completely rest of the groups treated with EB (Fig. 2B). It should be noted that
disappeared by the second postimplant test. On the other hand, WT WT mice from the placebo group, although showing nonattack
mice of the placebo group were aggressive up to the third postimplant aggression, did not show any offensive attacks (Fig. 2B).
test (28 days after GDX and 11 days after pellet implant) and became
completely nonaggressive thereafter. However, no significant interac-
tion between genotype and test was observed in the placebo-treated Non-attack bouts
group. WT mice from the EB-treated group showed steady levels of Genotype differences and EB dose effects in nonattack bouts were
aggression during five postimplant tests. In WT mice, there were no similar to those of total aggression, although there were no overall
overall significant differences between placebo- and EB-treated groups statistically significant genotype differences in either the number of
in the cumulative duration of total aggression (Fig. 1A). In marked tests or the cumulative duration (number of tests: F1,58 ¼ 3.20,
contrast, there were overall significant effects of EB treatment on the P ¼ 0.079; duration: F1,58 ¼ 3.82, P ¼ 0.055; Fig. 2C). EB treatment
induction of aggression in terms of cumulative duration of total tended to be more effective in bERKO mice than in WT mice at all
aggressive bouts (F1,45 ¼ 7.60, P < 0.01) in bERKO mice (Fig. 1B). doses except 2.5 lg ⁄ day of EB.
bERKO mice from EB-treated group were highly aggressive
throughout the postimplant tests. Particularly, they were significantly
more aggressive than the placebo control mice during the second, third Sexual behaviour
and fourth postimplant tests (F1,45 ¼ 4.49, P < 0.05; F1,45 ¼ 7.12, Changes in sexual behaviour during pre- and postimplant test
P < 0.05; and F1,45 ¼ 5.58, P < 0.05, respectively; Fig. 1B). A small
interaction between genotype and test was also observed in EB Unlike aggressive behaviour, regardless of genotype GDX eliminated
treatment group (F1,62 ¼ 2.11, P ¼ 0.079). sexual behaviour and the number of mounts with intromission in the
placebo groups remained low throughout the four postimplant tests.
Therefore, we examined genotype differences in the time course of
Effects of different doses of EB on the levels of aggression restoration of sexual behaviour by EB treatment. Data from two EB-
Genotype differences in the effects of different doses of EB on treatment groups (2.5 and 12.5 lg ⁄ day) were combined and compared
aggressive behaviour were analysed for the data from the second to between WT and bERKO mice.

ª The Authors (2006). Journal Compilation ª Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 1860–1868
ER-b and male aggressive and sexual behaviour 1863

Fig. 1. Effect of EB treatment on aggression in terms of the cumulative duration of total aggressive behaviour bouts in (A) WT and (B) bERKO mice.
Gonadectomized mice were tested once before (Pre) and five times after (Post 1–Post 5) pellet implant with the intervals indicated at the bottom of the figure. Pre-
implant test data include all mice, which were assigned to one of four treatment groups (three different doses of EB and placebo). For the postimplant tests, data from
all three EB-treatment groups were combined and compared with those of the placebo control group in each genotype. The data were log-transformed and presented
as mean ± SEM. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01 vs. respective placebo group. WT n ¼ 7, bERKO n ¼ 11 for the placebo-treated groups and WT n ¼ 28, bERKO n ¼ 36
for the EB-treated groups.

During two preimplant tests, there were no overall significant test in the number of mounts with intromission (F3,171 ¼ 3.42,
genotype differences in the number of mounts with intromission P < 0.05; Fig. 3A) but not in the latency to the first mount
(Fig. 3A) or the latency to first mount (Fig. 3B). Both WT and bERKO (F3,171 ¼ 0.48, P ¼ 0.483; Fig. 3B). In WT mice, the levels of
mice showed low levels of sexual behaviour in the first preimplant sexual behaviour were initially low but increased gradually in
tests and even less in the second preimplant tests. EB treatment subsequent tests. bERKO mice, on the other hand, showed steadily
restored sexual behaviour in both genotypes of mice, but with a high levels of sexual behaviour throughout all four postimplant tests,
different time course after EB pellet implant. During postimplant tests, including the first two tests. No mice showed ejaculation throughout
there were statistically significant interactions between genotype and pre- and postimplant sexual behavioural tests.

ª The Authors (2006). Journal Compilation ª Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 1860–1868
1864 M. Nomura et al.

Fig. 2. Effects of different doses of EB on aggression in terms of (A) total aggression, (B) offensive attack and (C) nonattack aggression during the second to
fourth postimplant tests. In each mouse, the number of tests with the behaviour and the mean cumulative duration during these three tests were calculated for each of
three aggressive behavioural bouts. In EB-treated mice, there were overall significant genotype differences in total aggression (both the number of tests and the mean
cumulative duration) and offensive attack (the number of tests) but not in the mean cumulative duration of offensive attack and nonattack aggression (both the
number of tests and the mean cumulative duration). The data for the cumulative duration were log-transformed. All data are presented as mean ± SEM. *P < 0.05 vs.
WT, aP < 0.05 vs. respective placebo group.

Effects of different doses of EB on the levels of sexual behaviour mounts with intromission (Fig. 4A) and (ii) the mean latency to the
The effects of different doses of EB on the levels of sexual behaviour first mount (Fig. 4B) during four tests. There were significant effects
during four postimplant tests were further analysed. The levels of of EB treatment on the number of mounts with intromission
sexual behaviour were examined in terms of (i) the mean number of (F2,57 ¼ 8.22, P < 0.01) and the latency to the first mount

ª The Authors (2006). Journal Compilation ª Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 1860–1868
ER-b and male aggressive and sexual behaviour 1865

Fig. 3. Genotype differences in the time course of restoration of sexual behaviour by EB treatment, in terms of (A) the number of mounts with intromission and
(B) the latency to the first mount. Gonadectomized mice were tested twice before (Pre 1 and Pre 2) and four times after (Post 1–Post 4) pellet implant with the
intervals indicated at the bottom of the figure. Data from two EB treatment groups (2.5 and 12.5 lg ⁄ day) were combined and compared between WT and bERKO
mice. During the postimplant tests, there were significant interactions between genotype and test in the number of mounts with intromission. The levels of sexual
behaviour in WT mice were initially low but increased gradually in subsequent tests, whereas those of bERKO mice were steady throughout the postimplant tests.
The data are presented as mean ± SEM. WT n ¼ 20, bERKO n ¼ 22. *P < 0.05 vs. WT,  P < 0.1 vs. WT.

(F2,57 ¼ 24.36, P < 0.01), but not those of genotype or genotype · groups, there were no significant differences between these two groups
treatment. Two doses of EB, 2.5 and 12.5 lg ⁄ day, equally restored of mice in any treatment groups. Therefore, the data from these two
sexual behaviour. behavioural testing groups were combined. There were no significant
genotype differences in the levels of serum E2 in each EB-treated
group. The serum levels were nondetectable in the group treated with
Serum estradiol levels placebo, 54.0 ± 11.9 (WT, n ¼ 20) and 58.7 ± 12.6 pg ⁄ mL (bERKO,
Serum E2 levels were measured in 145 samples. Although blood n ¼ 22) in the group treated with 2.5 lg ⁄ day of EB, 428.4 ± 116.4
samples were collected on different days after pellet implant, i.e. 19th (WT, n ¼ 9) and 247 ± 77.0 pg ⁄ mL (bERKO, n ¼ 12) in the group
day for aggression test groups and 21st day for sexual behavioural test treated with 5.0 lg ⁄ day of EB, and 762.4 ± 76.9 (WT, n ¼ 19) and

ª The Authors (2006). Journal Compilation ª Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 1860–1868
1866 M. Nomura et al.

Fig. 4. Effects of different doses of EB (2.5 and 12.5 lg ⁄ day) on sexual behaviour in terms of (A) the number of mounts with intromission and (B) the latency to
the first mount. Two doses of EB, 2.5 and 12.5 lg ⁄ day, equally restored sexual behaviour in both genotypes of mice. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.
**P < 0.01 vs. respective placebo group.

919.7 ± 223.8 pg ⁄ mL (bERKO, n ¼ 24) in the group treated with with our previous studies in gonadally intact mice. In our earlier
12.5 lg ⁄ day of EB. studies in gonadally intact adult males, we found that aggressive
behaviour was higher in bERKO than WT mice in the first aggression
tests but not in subsequent tests, in which the levels of aggression in
Discussion WT mice steadily increased whereas those in bERKO mice were
unchanged (Ogawa et al., 1999). Potentiation of aggressive behaviour
Aggressive behaviour by ER-b gene disruption was more obvious in younger age bERKO
The present study revealed a clear genotype difference between WT mice (5–12 weeks), which showed significantly higher levels of
and bERKO male mice in estrogen-inducible aggression in terms of aggression than WT mice during all three repeated tests (Nomura
both time course and dose dependency. Overall, EB-treated bERKO et al., 2002a). In these gonadally intact mice, however, elevated levels
mice showed significantly higher levels of aggressive behaviour of aggression were not simply attributed to the lack of ER-b-mediated
during the five postimplant tests, particularly second to fourth tests, inhibitory action in bERKO mice. Rather, the AR-mediated neural
than did the placebo-treated mice. On the other hand, in WT mice system of aggression might be altered in bERKO mice due to the lack
relatively low levels of aggression were seen in EB-treated mice of ER-b. Although the possibility that this may additionally contribute
throughout the postimplant tests and difference from the placebo to the elevated levels of aggression in bERKO mice is not completely
group was only apparent during the last two tests. In addition, there excluded, the present study supports the idea that the very lack of
was a small interaction between genotype and treatment in the levels ER-b can increase the levels of aggression in male mice. As the levels
of aggression during postimplanted tests. These genotype differences of intermale aggression are greatly reduced or even completely
in the effects of EB treatment reflect the difference in responses to abolished in terms of the most vigorous offensive attacks in aERKO
different doses of EB. Detailed analyses during second to fourth mice (Ogawa et al., 1997; Ogawa et al., 1998; Ogawa et al., 2000;
postimplant tests revealed that, in WT mice, only the treatment with Scordalakes et al., 2003), ER-a is necessary for the induction of male
2.5 lg ⁄ day of EB was effective for the induction of aggressive aggression. Taken together, we can conclude that activation of ER-a
behaviour beyond the placebo levels. In contrast, the treatments with facilitates and activation of ER-b inhibits aggression. It should be
all doses of EB were effective in bERKO mice although the levels of noted that this is the first demonstration that two types of estrogen
aggression tended to be higher in the groups treated with two lower receptors regulate one specific behavioural endopoint in opposite
doses, 2.5 and 5 lg ⁄ day, than the higher 12.5 lg ⁄ day dose group. directions, although it has been shown that ER-a and ER-b seem to
These results indicate that estrogenic stimulation by itself is sufficient operate in such an antagonistic ‘yin–yang’ mode in other physiolo-
to induce aggression in gonadectomized male mice. Our findings that gical contexts (Gustafsson, 2003; Lindberg et al., 2003; Matthews &
the overall effects of EB were more pronounced in bERKO mice than Gustafsson, 2003; Weihua et al., 2003). It still remains to be
in WT mice suggest that facilitatory behavioural effects of estrogen are determined whether activation of ER-b may also have opposite effects
mediated by ER-a and that additional activation of ER-b plays an to AR-activation on aggression: whether aggressive behaviour induced
inhibitory role in estrogen-inducible aggression in male mice. by activation of AR can be reduced by activation of ER-b.
Furthermore, a marked genotype difference in dose responses suggests Even though the effects may not be completely opposite to those of
that the ER-a-mediated facilitatory action of estrogen may be more ER-a, there are other cases in which the final outcome of behaviour
pronounced at lower and physiological doses and the ER-b-mediated may be modulated by ER-b activation (Ogawa et al., 2002). For
inhibitory action may only be unveiled at higher doses of estrogen. It instance, expression of female sexual behaviour measured as lordosis
should be noted that the possibility that activation of ER-b by quotient was not affected by ER-b gene disruption on the day of the
moderate to high doses of E2 causes lethargy in WT mice cannot be behavioural estrus, when females are most receptive (Ogawa et al.,
completely ruled out, although we did not notice any genotype 1999). However, bERKO females showed significantly higher recep-
differences in general activity during aggression tests in the mice tivity than WT females on the day after behavioural estrus. These
treated with these doses of EB. findings suggest that ER-b may be responsible for fine-tuning of
The notion that ER-b activation mediates inhibitory effects of expression of receptivity, particularly the turning-off phase during the
gonadal steroids on aggressive behaviour in male mice is consistent estrous cycle. As some ER-a- and ER-b-specific agonists are currently

ª The Authors (2006). Journal Compilation ª Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 1860–1868
ER-b and male aggressive and sexual behaviour 1867

available, a complementary study using these agonists would solidify activated. This does not necessarily mean, however, that ER-b is not
behavioural alterations observed in genetically modified mice. involved in the control of male sexual behaviour. A recent study in
There are a number of hypotheses of potential brain mechanisms pubertal age bERKO male mice reported that they showed reduced
underlying modulatory roles of ER-b activation on aggressive levels of sexual behaviour (Temple et al., 2003). In addition, our
behaviour. It is known that ER-b protein and mRNA are expressed previous studies in gonadally intact adult male mice revealed that male
more abundantly than ER-a in brain areas such as the paraventricular sexual behavioural components such as ultrasonic vocalization and
nucleus, bed nucleus of stria terminalis, medial amygdala and mounting behaviour were intact not only in bERKO (Ogawa et al.,
midbrain dorsal raphe nuclei (Shughrue et al., 1997; Shughrue & 1999) but also in aERKO mice, although aERKO mice showed
Merchenthaler, 2001; Mitra et al., 2003; Nomura et al., 2003). reduced levels of intromission and complete abolition of ejaculation
Neuropeptides, e.g. oxytocin and vasopressin, as well as neurotrans- (Ogawa et al., 1997, 1998, 2000; Wersinger et al., 1997). On the other
mitters, e.g. serotonin, known to be synthesized in these brain areas, hand, all components of male sexual behaviour were completely
are also implicated in the regulation of aggressive behaviour. Both abolished in double knockout mice (Ogawa et al., 2000). These
oxytocin (Richard & Zingg, 1990; Mohr & Schmitz, 1991) and findings suggest that ER-b plays a role, at least partially, in the
arginine vasopressin (Shapiro et al., 2000) genes contain an ER- expression of sexual behaviour in male mice.
responsive element and recent in vitro studies in ER-b transfected cells
have demonstrated that ER-b activation by estradiol regulates the
oxytocin and arginine vasopressin promoter activity (Shapiro et al., Acknowledgements
2000; Loven et al., 2001). It has been shown that both oxytocin- and The authors are thankful to Ms L. Durbak and Ms M. Markey for their technical
vasopressin-synthesizing neurons in the paraventricular nucleus assistance. Support for this work was provided by NIMH 62147 to S.O. M.N.
coexpress ER-b (Simonian & Herbison, 1997; Alves et al., 1998; was a recipient of postdoctoral fellowships for research abroad from the Japan
Society for Promotion of Science.
Hrabovszky et al., 1998; Laflamme et al., 1998) and estrogenic up-
regulation of oxytocin and down-regulation of vasopressin in this
brain area are either abolished or attenuated in bERKO male mice
(Nomura et al., 2002b). A number of behavioural studies have Abbreviations
collectively demonstrated that oxytocin facilitates affiliative behaviour AR, androgen receptor; E2, estradiol; EB, estradiol benzoate; ER, estrogen
(Insel, 1992; Nelson & Panksepp, 1998; Young et al., 2001) and receptors; ERKO, ER-knockout; GDX, gonadectomized ⁄ gonadectomy; WT,
inhibits aggression (Harmon et al., 2002; Lubin et al., 2003), and wild-type.
vasopressin facilitates aggression (Ferris & Potegal, 1988; Wersinger
et al., 2002). Therefore, it is possible that the elevated levels of
aggression in bERKO male mice may be partially due to an alteration References
of estrogenic regulation on syntheses of these neuropeptides. Alves, S.E., Lopez, V., McEwen, B.S. & Weiland, N.G. (1998) Differential
Another possible mechanism is ER-b-mediated regulation of brain colocalization of estrogen receptor beta (ERbeta) with oxytocin and
vasopressin in the paraventricular and supraoptic nuclei of the female rat
serotonergic systems. It is now well established that ER-b is the brain: an immunocytochemical study. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA, 95, 3281–
predominant form of ER localized in the dorsal raphe nuclei (Lu et al., 3286.
1999; Gundlah et al., 2001; Lu et al., 2001). Our preliminary studies Couse, J.F. & Korach, K.S. (1999) Estrogen receptor null mice: what have we
revealed that > 90% of ER-b-immunopositive cells in the dorsal raphe learned and where will they lead us? Endocrine Rev., 20, 358–417.
Denenberg, V.H., Gaulin-Kremer, E., Gandelman, R. & Zarrow, M.X. (1973)
nuclei coexpress tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH), the rate-limiting
The development of standard stimulus animals for mouse aggression testing.
enzyme of serotonin synthesis, whereas only 20% of ER-a positive Anim. Behav., 21, 590–598.
cells express TPH. Although the exact outcome of colocalization of Ferris, C.F. & Potegal, M. (1988) Vasopressin receptor blockade in the anterior
ER-b and TPH still needs to be determined, it is assumed that a lack of hypothalamus suppresses aggression in hamsters. Physiol. Behav., 44, 235–
ER-b in TPH-positive cells may profoundly affect estrogenic regu- 239.
Gundlah, C., Lu, N.Z., Mirkes, S.J. & Bethea, C.L. (2001) Estrogen receptor
lation of the brain serotonin system. As serotonin is the most well beta (ERbeta) mRNA and protein in serotonin neurons of macaques. Mol.
characterized neurotransmitter for its inhibitory effect on aggression Brain Res., 91, 14–22.
(Miczek et al., 2001; Nelson & Chiavegatto, 2001), it is hypothesized Gustafsson, J.-Å. (2003) What pharmacologists can learn from recent advances
that alteration of ER-b-mediated regulation of serotonin systems may in estrogen signalling. Trends Pharmacol. Sci., 24, 479–485.
Harmon, A.C., Huhman, K.L., Moore, T.O. & Albers, H.E. (2002) Oxytocin
also be involved in the modulatory role of ER-b on aggression.
inhibits aggression in female Syrian hamsters. J. Neuroendocrinol., 14, 963–
969.
Hrabovszky, E., Kallo, I., Hajszan, T., Shughrue, P.J., Merchenthaler, I. &
Liposits, Z. (1998) Expression of estrogen receptor-beta messenger
Sexual behaviour ribonucleic acid in oxytocin and vasopressin neurons of the rat supraoptic
and paraventricular nuclei. Endocrinology, 139, 2600–2604.
Unlike its effects on aggression, the effects of estrogen treatment on Insel, T.R. (1992) Oxytocin – a neuropeptide for affiliation: evidence from
the restoration of sexual behaviour were not different between WT and behavioral, receptor autoradiographic, and comparative studies. Psychoneu-
bERKO mice, except a small, but statistically significant, difference in roendocrinology, 17, 3–35.
the time course of behavioural recovery after EB pellet implant. Also, Laflamme, N., Nappi, R.E., Drolet, G., Labrie, C. & Rivest, S. (1998)
Expression and neuropeptidergic characterization of estrogen receptors
two doses of EB, the lower (2.5 lg ⁄ day) and the higher
(ERalpha and ERbeta) throughout the rat brain: anatomical evidence of
(12.5 lg ⁄ day), induced equal amounts of male sexual behaviour in distinct roles of each subtype. J. Neurobiol., 36, 357–378.
both WT and bERKO mice. This is also in striking contrast to possible Lindberg, M.K., Moverare, S., Skrtic, S., Gao, H., Dahlman-Wright, K.,
EB dose-dependent differential roles of the two types of ER in Gustafsson, J.-Å. & Ohlsson, C. (2003) Estrogen receptor (ER)-beta reduces
aggressive behaviour. These results collectively demonstrate that the ERalpha-regulated gene transcription, supporting a ‘ying yang’ relationship
between ERalpha and ERbeta in mice. Mol. Endocrinol., 17, 203–208.
lack of ER-b does not affect the expression of male sexual behaviour. Loven, M.A., Wood, J.R. & Nardulli, A.M. (2001) Interaction of estrogen
Furthermore, these results suggest that the levels of sexual behaviour receptors alpha and beta with estrogen response elements. Mol. Cell
remain intact in bERKO mice even when only ER-a, but not AR, is Endocrinol., 181, 151–163.

ª The Authors (2006). Journal Compilation ª Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 1860–1868
1868 M. Nomura et al.

Lu, H., Ozawa, H., Nishi, M., Ito, T. & Kawata, M. (2001) Serotonergic Ogawa, S., Lubahn, D.B., Korach, K.S. & Pfaff, D.W. (1997) Behavioral
neurones in the dorsal raphe nucleus that project into the medial preoptic area effects of estrogen receptor gene disruption in male mice. Proc. Natl. Acad.
contain oestrogen receptor beta. J. Neuroendocrinol., 13, 839–845. Sci. USA, 94, 1476–1481.
Lu, N.Z., Shlaes, T.A., Gundlah, C., Dziennis, S.E., Lyle, R.E. & Bethea, C.L. Ogawa, S., Robbins, A., Kumar, N., Pfaff, D.W., Sundaram, K. & Bardin, C.W.
(1999) Ovarian steroid action on tryptophan hydroxylase protein and (1996) Effects of testosterone and 7a-methyl-19-nortestosterone (MENT) on
serotonin compared to localization of ovarian steroid receptors in midbrain of sexual and aggressive behaviors in two inbred strains of male mice. Horm.
guinea pigs. Endocrine, 11, 257–267. Behav., 30, 74–84.
Lubin, D.A., Elliott, J.C., Black, M.C. & Johns, J.M. (2003) An oxytocin Ogawa, S., Washburn, T.F., Taylor, J., Lubahn, D.B., Korach, K.S. & Pfaff, D.W.
antagonist infused into the central nucleus of the amygdala increases (1998) Modifications of testosterone-dependent behaviors by estrogen
maternal aggressive behavior. Behav. Neurosci., 117, 195–201. receptor-alpha gene disruption in male mice. Endocrinology, 139, 5058–5069.
Matthews, J. & Gustafsson, J.-Å. (2003) Estrogen signaling: a subtle balance Richard, S. & Zingg, H.H. (1990) The human oxytocin gene promoter is
between ERa and ERb. Mol. Intervent., 3, 281–292. regulated by estrogens. J. Biol. Chem., 265, 6098–6103.
McGill, T.E. (1970) Genetic analysis of male sexual behavior. In Lindzey, G. & Scordalakes, E.M. & Rissman, E.F. (2003) Aggression in male mice lacking
Thiessen, D.D. (eds) Contributions to Behavior-Genetic Analysis: the Mouse functional estrogen receptor a. Behav. Neurosci., 117, 38–45.
as a Prototype. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, pp. 57–88. Shapiro, R.A., Xu, C. & Dorsa, D.M. (2000) Differential transcriptional
Miczek, K.A., Maxson, S.C., Fish, E.W. & Faccidomo, S. (2001) Aggressive regulation of rat vasopressin gene expression by estrogen receptor alpha and
behavioral phenotypes in mice. Behav. Brain Res., 125, 167–181. beta. Endocrinology, 141, 4056–4064.
Mitra, S.W., Hoskin, E., Yudkovitz, J., Pear, L., Wilkinson, H.A., Hayashi, S., Shughrue, P.J., Lane, M.V. & Merchenthaler, I. (1997) Comparative
Pfaff, D.W., Ogawa, S., Rohrer, S.P., Schaeffer, J.M., McEwen, B.S. & distribution of estrogen receptor-alpha and -beta mRNA in the rat central
Alves, S.E. (2003) Immunolocalization of estrogen receptor beta in the nervous system. J. Comp. Neurol., 388, 507–525.
mouse brain: comparison with estrogen receptor alpha. Endocrinology, 144, Shughrue, P.J. & Merchenthaler, I. (2001) Distribution of estrogen receptor beta
2055–2067. immunoreactivity in the rat central nervous system. J. Comp. Neurol., 436,
Mohr, E. & Schmitz, E. (1991) Functional characterization of estrogen and 64–81.
glucocorticoid responsive elements in the rat oxytocin gene. Mol. Brain Res., Shughrue, P.J., Scrimo, P.J. & Merchenthaler, I. (2000) Estrogen binding and
9, 293–298. estrogen receptor characterization (ERalpha and ERbeta) in the cholinergic
Nelson, R.J. & Chiavegatto, S. (2001) Molecular basis of aggression. Trends neurons of the rat basal forebrain. Neuroscience, 96, 41–49.
Neurosci., 24, 713–719. Simon, N.G. & Whalen, R.E. (1986) Hormonal regulation of aggression:
Nelson, E.E. & Panksepp, J. (1998) Brain substrates of infant-mother Evidence for a relationship among genotype, receptor binding, and
attachment: contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine. behavioral sensitivity to androgen and estrogen. Aggr. Behav., 12, 255–266.
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 22, 437–452. Simon, N.G. & Whalen, R.E. (1987) Sexual differentiation of androgen-
Nomura, M., Durbak, L., Chan, J., Smithies, O., Gustafsson, J.-Å., Korach, sensitive and estrogen-sensitive regulatory systems for aggressive behavior.
K.S., Pfaff, D.W. & Ogawa, S. (2002a) Genotype ⁄ age interactions on Horm. Behav., 21, 493–500.
aggressive behavior in gonadally intact estrogen receptor beta knockout Simonian, S.X. & Herbison, A.E. (1997) Differential expression of estrogen
(betaERKO) male mice. Horm. Behav., 41, 288–296. receptor alpha and beta immunoreactivity by oxytocin neurons of rat
Nomura, M., Korach, K.S., Pfaff, D.W. & Ogawa, S. (2003) Estrogen receptor paraventricular nucleus. J. Neuroendocrinol., 9, 803–806.
beta (ERbeta) protein levels in neurons depend on estrogen receptor alpha Temple, J.L., Scordalakes, E.M., Bodo, C., Gustafsson, J.-Å. & Rissman, E.F.
(ERalpha) gene expression and on its ligand in a brain region-specific (2003) Lack of functional estrogen receptor beta gene disrupts pubertal male
manner. Mol. Brain Res., 110, 7–14. sexual behavior. Horm. Behav., 44, 427–434.
Nomura, M., McKenna, E., Korach, K.S., Pfaff, D.W. & Ogawa, S. (2002b) Vasudevan, N., Koibuchi, N., Chin, W.W. & Pfaff, D.W. (2001) Differential
Estrogen receptor-beta regulates transcript levels for oxytocin and arginine crosstalk between estrogen receptor (ER) alpha and ERbeta and the thyroid
vasopressin in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus of male mice. Mol. hormone receptor isoforms results in flexible regulation of the consensus
Brain Res., 109, 84–94. ERE. Mol. Brain Res., 95, 9–17.
Ogawa, S., Chan, J., Chester, A.E., Gustafsson, J.-Å., Korach, K.S. & Pfaff, Weihua, Z., Andersson, S., Cheng, G., Simpson, E.R., Warner, M. &
D.W. (1999) Survival of reproductive behaviors in estrogen receptor beta Gustafsson, J.-Å. (2003) Update on estrogen signaling. FEBS Lett., 546,
gene-deficient (betaERKO) male and female mice. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 17–24.
USA, 96, 12887–12892. Wersinger, S.R., Ginns, E.I., O’Carroll, A.M., Lolait, S.J., Young, W.S. & 3rd
Ogawa, S., Chester, A.E., Hewitt, S.C., Walker, V.R., Gustafsson, J.-Å., (2002) Vasopressin V1b receptor knockout reduces aggressive behavior in
Smithies, O., Korach, K.S. & Pfaff, D.W. (2000) Abolition of male sexual male mice. Mol. Psychiatry, 7, 975–984.
behaviors in mice lacking estrogen receptors alpha and beta (alpha beta Wersinger, S.R., Sannen, K., Villalba, C., Lubahn, D.B., Rissman, E.F. & De Vries,
ERKO). Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA, 97, 14737–14741. G.J. (1997) Masculine sexual behavior is disrupted in male and female mice
Ogawa, S., Korach, K.S. & Pfaff, D.W. (2002) Differential roles of two types of lacking a functional estrogen receptor a gene. Horm. Behav., 32, 176–183.
estrogen receptors in reproductive behavior. Curr. Opin. Endocrinol. Young, L.J., Lim, M.M., Gingrich, B. & Insel, T.R. (2001) Cellular
Diabetes, 9, 224–229. mechanisms of social attachment. Horm. Behav., 40, 133–138.

ª The Authors (2006). Journal Compilation ª Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 1860–1868

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi