Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 24

Are Consumers Really Willing to Pay

More for a Favorable Country Image?


A Study of Country-of-Origin Effects
on Willingness to Pay
Nicole Koschate-Fischer, Adamantios Diamantopoulos, and Katharina Oldenkotte

ABSTRACT
Price-related consequences of the country-of-origin (COO) cue have received limited attention in extant literature. In
this study, the authors draw from equity theory and cue utilization theory and investigate (1) whether a brand’s COO
affects a consumer’s willingness to pay and (2) the extent to which the consumer’s familiarity with the brand moderates
this relationship. The results of three complementary experimental studies reveal that COO indeed has a positive
impact on willingness to pay. Furthermore, the authors find a negative moderating influence of brand familiarity on the
COO effect in a high-involvement setting but not in a low-involvement setting. The authors discuss the theoretical and
managerial implications of the findings, and they identify directions for further research.

Keywords: country of origin, willingness to pay, brand familiarity, experimental research

ne of the oldest and most persistent concerns in reviews of COO research, see Jaffe and Nebenzahl

O international marketing is whether the origin of a


product makes it more or less preferable to con-
sumers. Country of origin (COO) serves as an extrinsic
2006; Pharr 2005; Phau and Chao 2008; Wilcox 2005).

Various articles in the business press also underscore the


informational cue for consumers’ perceptions and relevance of the COO effect. For example, after several
evaluations of a product (Verlegh and Steenkamp 1999), recalls and disasters associated with products made
and research on COO effects—that is, the impact that overseas (e.g., lead in toys), U.S. consumers became sen-
cognitive, affective, and normative associations with a sitive to product origins, and many now actively search
particular country have on consumer attitudes—shows for products “Made in the USA” (Martin 2007).
that a product’s COO acts as a signal of product quality,
influences consumers’ perceptions of risk and value, and Yet despite the acknowledged importance of COO
directly affects the likelihood of purchase (for recent effects, their price-related consequences remain neg-
lected, such that “very little is known regarding the influ-
ence of COO on pricing decisions” (Agrawal and
Nicole Koschate-Fischer is Professor of Marketing (e-mail: Kamakura 1999, p. 257). Extant COO research has
nicole.koschate-fischer@wiso.uni-erlangen.de), and Katharina Old- mainly focused on consumers’ quality evaluations and
enkotte is a postdoctoral researcher (e-mail: katharinaoldenkotte@ intentions to purchase a product. However, several meta-
gmx.de), GfK-Chair of Marketing Intelligence, University of analyses and recent empirical studies indicate that the
Erlangen–Nuremberg. Adamantios Diamantopoulos is Professor of COO effect loses its strength when purchase intentions,
International Marketing, Chair of International Marketing, Univer-
sity of Vienna (e-mail: adamantios.diamantopoulos@univie.ac.at). The
authors thank the Dr. Theo and Friedl Schöller Research Center for Journal of International Marketing
Business and Society and the Hans Frisch-Foundation for supporting ©2012, American Marketing Association
this research study. They also thank the four anonymous JIM review- Vol. 20, No. 1, 2012, pp. 19–41
ers for useful comments over several review rounds. ISSN 1069-0031X (print) 1547-7215 (electronic)

Country-of-Origin Effects and Willingness to Pay 19


and not quality perceptions, serve as a dependent “made-in,” which may or may not coincide with the
variable (Josiassen, Lukas, and Whitwell 2008; Verlegh home country of the brand, that is, the brand origin
and Steenkamp 1999); indeed, “COO has significantly (Jaffe and Nebenzahl 2006). Willingness to pay refers to
lesser impact as consumers move closer to the actual the maximum amount of money a consumer is willing to
purchase situation from belief formation regarding the spend for a product (Homburg, Koschate, and Hoyer
relative quality of brands” (Agrawal and Kamakura 2005). To the best of our knowledge, with the exception
1999, p. 256). of Hu and Wang’s (2010) study, which focuses on the
origin of (online) retailers and used real transaction
Focusing on price rather than quality evaluations or pur- data, no previous COO investigations have examined
chase intentions as a dependent variable offers a (much) WTP as a dependent variable.
stricter test of COO effects because price represents “the
amount of money we must sacrifice to acquire something From a managerial point of view, knowledge of individual
we desire” (Monroe 2003, p. 5); thus, it is possible for consumers’ WTP is crucial for the construction of price
consumers to evaluate a product from country X more response functions (Völckner 2008), as well as for the
positively than a product from country Y but, at the same implementation of popular pricing instruments, such as
time, be unwilling to pay a price premium for it. Similarly, price differentiation, value-based pricing, or price bundling.
consumers may indicate a greater willingness to buy a Furthermore, consumers retrieve and use their WTP as an
product from country X, though they may not actually internal reference price that influences their purchase deci-
purchase the product because they find its price unac- sions (Miyuri and Bettman 2005). Thus, WTP represents a
ceptable. Thus, product evaluations and purchase inten- highly relevant yet neglected outcome variable worthy of
tions are “softer” outcome variables than price and there- investigation in a COO research setting.
fore are more likely to reveal a COO effect than if price
serves as the dependent variable. We apply equity theory (Adams 1965) to conceptually
underpin our investigation and conduct a series of
A second reason for focusing on price is that it allows experiments designed to reveal the extent to which con-
for the “monetization” of the COO effect (Nebenzahl sumers are willing to spend more money for a branded
and Jaffe 1993) because it reveals the extent to which product that originates from a COO with a favorable
consumers’ perceptions of different COOs are reflected image than for a branded product from a COO with a
in the differences in the amount that those consumers less favorable image. We also apply cue utilization theory
are prepared to pay for products associated with each to investigate the role of brand familiarity—“the extent
COO. In this context, “intuitively, it should be self evi- of a consumer’s direct and indirect experience with a
dent that, ceteris paribus, a country having a better brand” (Campbell and Keller 2003, p. 293)—as a poten-
image than others, especially as a source for a product, tial moderator of the COO–WTP relationship. We
has a comparative advantage that should translate to hypothesize that the strength of the COO effect may
economic value” (Jaffe and Nebenzahl 2006, p. 59). depend on the consumer’s familiarity with the brand
From a practitioner perspective, being able to “mone- involved (Josiassen, Lukas, and Whitwell 2008) and
tize” the COO effect is of great significance because examine this relationship in both a low- and high-
price is one of the most powerful marketing-mix ele- involvement setting. In the former setting, we employ
ments due to its direct and disproportional impact on Becker, DeGroot, and Marschak’s (1964) procedure
profitability (Han, Gupta, and Lehmann 2001). Marn (hereinafter, BDM) to measure consumers’ WTP. The
and Rosiello (1992), for example, report that a price BDM procedure is a highly accurate approach for reveal-
increase of 1% leads to an average 11.1% improvement ing respondents’ “true” WTP because of its incentive-
in operating income,1 whereas a 1% increase in sales vol- compatible character and realistic point-of-purchase set-
ume enhances operating income by only 3.3%. For given ting (Wertenbroch and Skiera 2002).
operating costs, higher prices due to a more favorable
country image will lead to an increase in operating Our study’s contribution is threefold. First, on the theo-
income and, thus, higher profitability. retical front, we apply equity theory as a theoretical
framework to the relationship between COO percep-
Against this background, the current study investigates tions and consumers’ WTP and provide empirical evi-
the link between the COO of a branded product and dence as to whether this effect exists. Furthermore, we
consumers’ willingness to pay (WTP) for it. In this con- investigate whether brand familiarity moderates the
text, COO designates the country where the product is strength of the COO effect on WTP in both low- and

20 Journal of International Marketing


high-involvement situations. To the best of our knowl- countries may cause demand effects (Johansson 1989).
edge, our study is the first to analyze in a comparatively Conversely, studies focusing on the link between COO
realistic point-of-purchase setting the extent to which a and actual sales prices indicate the prices consumers are
branded product’s origin directly influences the price actually willing to accept for a product but do not reveal
that the consumer is willing to pay for the brand. the maximum amount of money consumers are willing
to pay (i.e., their WTP) (Garbarino and Slonim 2003).
Second, on the methodological front, we illustrate the
application of the BDM approach and thus introduce Only a single, recently published study (Hu and Wang
researchers to a promising procedure, which, though 2010) has examined, in an online auction setting
used in other fields (e.g., Homburg, Koschate, and (eBay), whether consumers are willing to pay different
Hoyer 2005), has not yet been applied in COO-related prices for a given product depending on the origin of
research. Although the BDM method can realistically be the retailer. The results reveal that U.S. retailers are able
applied only when low-priced products are involved— to command a premium, which “appears to stem from
because participants are actually obliged to buy the country-of-origin equity instead of trading risk or prod-
product—it nevertheless enables a researcher to study uct quality” (Hu and Wang 2010, p. 200). Using actual
actual buying behavior rather than simply purchase transaction data, the study thus provides support for
intentions. This is important because “there exists a gap price-related consequences of the COO in an inter-
between what consumers say they are going to do and national trading context.
what they actually do at the point of purchase” (Car-
rington, Neville, and Whitwell 2010, p. 141). Our study is similar to Hu and Wang’s (2010) in the sense
that it is also based on an auction mechanism; however, it
Third, on the managerial front, we provide empirically differs from the latter study in several important ways.
based insights into the extent to which consumers will Specifically, Hu and Wang use eBay data in which partici-
pay price premiums or expect price discounts, depend- pants have the opportunity to adjust their offer (WTP)
ing on the COO of the product. These insights are of and thus react to steps undertaken by the other partici-
substantial value to international marketing managers pants of the auction. In contrast, with the BDM proce-
designing a pricing strategy, particularly when the con- dure, respondents do not compete with other bidders, and
sumer is confronted with product offerings from multi- there is no interaction between the various players
ple COOs differing in their image. Thus, our study’s involved because the final price is determined on the basis
findings provide managerial support for decisions of a lottery. Moreover, in Hu and Wang’s investigation,
related to customer value-oriented pricing, price differ- the origin of the retailer, and not that of the product, was
entiation, and market segmentation (Monroe 2003). the focus of analysis. The image that respondents associ-
ate with a retailer of a certain country is not necessarily
the same as the one they associate with a product of a cer-
PRIOR RESEARCH tain country and is likely to be based on different criteria
(e.g., sales skills, delivery times). Finally, in contrast to Hu
COO and Price and Wang, we explicitly investigate the influence of a
Price-related consequences of COO have been widely moderating variable (brand familiarity) on the COO
neglected in extant literature because “few researchers effect. In summary, our study has a different focus and a
have attempted to study the country image effect on different methodology and thus provides complementary
product price” (Nebenzahl and Jaffe 1993, p. 161). The insights to those Hu and Wang provide.
handful of studies that address price-related issues in a
COO context focus either on consumers’ price tolerance WTP as a Dependent Variable
(Drozdenko and Jensen 2009; Nebenzahl and Jaffe
1993) or on actual sales prices (Agrawal and Kamakura Considering the COO of the product as an influence on
1999; Hulland, Todiño, and Lecraw 1996). Price toler- consumers’ WTP, an obvious question is whether using
ance studies reveal a base price for a product associated the latter as a dependent variable is likely to generate
with a given COO and then ask consumers to indicate insights beyond those generated by product evaluations
how much more or less they would be willing to pay for or buying intentions. After all, WTP would be expected
the same product if it were made in another country. to correlate positively with outcome variables (particu-
However, the base price may serve as an anchor (Simon- larly purchase intentions) for which a COO effect has
son and Drolet 2004), and direct comparison of the already been established in prior research.

Country-of-Origin Effects and Willingness to Pay 21


We believe that such additional insights can be expected research (Usunier 2006). Indeed, our focus on WTP is
for at least three reasons. First, if two variables are corre- consistent with recent calls in the literature to “focus on
lated, this does not automatically mean (1) that they must the importance of the COO at the time of purchase
share the same antecedents or (2) even if they do, that the rather than assessing this at any other time” (Josiassen
impact of these antecedents can be assumed to be the and Harzing 2008, p. 266, emphasis in original).
same for both variables. As an example, consider product
evaluations and purchase intentions, which are positively
correlated and have been widely used as outcome THEORETICAL BACKGROUND AND
variables in COO research. Do they have the same HYPOTHESES
antecedents? Not necessarily—consumer animosity, for
example, has been found to affect purchase intentions but Equity Theory and COO
not product evaluations (see, e.g., Klein, Ettenson, and We employ equity theory (Adams 1965) to underpin the
Morris 1998). Is the impact of their common antecedents hypothesized relationship between COO and WTP.
the same? Again, the answer is no—COO, for example, Equity theory involves exchange relationships in which
has been found to explain, on average, approximately people compare the outcome they receive from an
30% in product evaluations but less than 20% in pur- exchange to the input they provide into the exchange.
chase intentions (Peterson and Jolibert 1995). The outcome describes expected positive and negative
consequences of the exchange, whereas the input reflects
Second, a basic premise underlying COO research is the contribution the person must make within the
that consumer preferences will be greater for products exchange to earn rewards (Walster, Berscheid, and Wal-
from a favorably rated COO because their product ster 1973).
evaluations will be more positive. However, this premise
focuses only on the “benefit” side of the COO influence According to equity theory, distributive justice among
and fails to consider the “sacrifice” side. Consumers exchange partners is achieved when the benefits of each
may indeed assess products coming from certain COOs are proportional to their investments (Adams 1965;
more favorably than from other COOs, but this does Homans 1961). Thus, a person involved in an exchange
not automatically imply that they are also willing to relationship perceives equitable treatment if the ratio of
make the necessary (monetary) sacrifice to actually buy his or her outcome to input is, in some sense, “fair” rela-
them. Focusing on WTP “reminds” consumers that tive to the outcome-to-input ratio of the referent other
there is also a cost associated with acquiring products, (Homans 1961; Homburg, Koschate, and Hoyer 2005).
thus encouraging them to be more careful when indicat-
ing their preferences. In an economic exchange situation between a buyer (i.e.,
consumer) and a seller, a product or service becomes the
Third, while the use of intentions data to predict behav- focus of the exchange. The consumer offers a certain
ior is widespread among academics and practitioners input (e.g., money, shopping effort) to receive a certain
alike, it is not without problems. Specifically, “inten- outcome (i.e., the benefit of the product or service)
tions data often contain systematic biases; intentions (Oliver and Swan 1989). For an equitable deal, con-
change over time and may not accurately predict actual sumers seek to adjust their input according to the
purchases” (Sun and Morwitz 2010, p. 356). Indeed, expected outcome (Huppertz, Arenson, and Evans
“economists, observing the frequent divergence between 1978; Walster, Berscheid, and Walster 1973). That is,
stated intentions and subsequent behavior, generally when they receive a relatively high outcome (e.g., a
ignore intentions data” (Manski 1990, p. 936). Ignoring greater benefit from the product), in response, they pro-
the discrepancies between intentions and purchase vide a relatively higher input (in terms of money and/or
behavior can cause biased results, and thus the use of effort), whereas a relatively lower benefit from the prod-
intention ratings alone could be misleading (Sun and uct prompts a relatively lower input. If the outcome-to-
Morwitz 2010). By focusing on consumers’ WTP, as input ratio represents, from the consumers’ point of
measured with the BDM approach (as we do in two of view, an equitable deal, they consider the exchange rela-
our experiments), we are able to study how COO affects tionship fair, such as a “fair product for a fair price”
actual behavior. This is important both because it pro- (Huppertz, Arenson, and Evans 1978, p. 251).
vides a much stricter test of the COO effect than either
product evaluations or purchase intentions and because Equity theory is particularly appealing for our purposes
actual purchase behavior is hardly ever studied in COO because it explicitly considers both the benefit a con-

22 Journal of International Marketing


sumer is likely to obtain for a branded product associ- direct experience with the brand, extrinsic cues provide
ated with a particular COO and the sacrifice that he or information consumers can use to form expectations
she must make to actually receive this benefit. Thus, the and evaluations (Tse and Gorn 1993). Therefore, under
outcome-to-input ratio refers to the comparison of the conditions of low brand familiarity, a product’s origin
expected benefit a consumer obtains from a branded becomes a more determinant cue (Josiassen, Lukas, and
product that originates from a certain COO and his or Whitwell 2008).
her WTP for that particular product. Because consumers
have different images for different COOs, their evalua- However, the expected influence of brand familiarity is
tions of products originating from the various COOs likely to vary depending on whether a purchase situa-
also differ (Pappu, Quester, and Cooksey 2007). Bearing tion is low or high involvement. Involvement refers to
in mind that country image refers to “the overall percep- “the subjective perception of the personal relevance of
tion consumers form of products from a particular an object, activity, or situation” (Van Trijp, Hoyer, and
country, based on their prior perception of the country’s Inman 1996, p. 283). In a low-involvement situation,
production and marketing strengths and weaknesses” consumers are likely to consider an extrinsic cue such as
(Roth and Romeo 1992, p. 480), from a consumer’s per- COO important in their WTP determination, regardless
spective, a product from a COO with a favorable coun- of whether they are highly familiar with the brand. This
try image is likely to be associated with a higher benefit corresponds with heuristic behavior in low-involvement
than a product from a COO with a less favorable coun- contexts that supports decision making (Merlo, Lukas,
try image. and Whitwell 2008). The use of an extrinsic cue as a
heuristic helps consumers simplify a decision because it
According to equity theory, consumers seek to establish reduces decision complexity and minimizes cognitive
an equitable deal, so they adjust the input (WTP) they effort (Verlegh, Steenkamp, and Meulenberg 2005).
provide in the exchange to the outcome they expect to Because in a low-involvement situation consumers are
receive (benefit associated with the product from a given often not motivated to integrate all relevant product
COO). Therefore, consumers should display a higher information in their information processing, they tend to
WTP when they receive in exchange a product from a rely on extrinsic cues (e.g., the COO) that can be easily
COO with a favorable country image rather than one processed (Chen and Chaiken 1999); the influence of
from a COO with a less favorable country image. Thus: intrinsic cues is likely to be negligible because their use
requires more time and effort than consumers perceive
H1: Consumers’ WTP is higher for a given as being worthwhile (Zeithaml 1988). Thus:
branded product that originates from a COO
with a more favorable country image than H2: In a low-involvement setting, brand famili-
one that originates from a COO with a less arity does not substantially influence the effect
favorable country image. of COO on consumers’ WTP.

Moderating Role of Brand Familiarity In a high-involvement setting, both extrinsic and intrin-
sic cues should become important in product evaluation.
According to cue utilization theory, consumers use an Consumers’ use of cues as indicators to assess a product’s
array of cues to make product-related inferences in an quality may depend on the level of prior knowledge the
attempt to assess a product’s quality (Olson 1972). consumer disposes of regarding the product (Rao and
Extrinsic cues constitute attributes that are product- Monroe 1988). If the consumer is relatively unfamiliar
related but external to the physical product, such as the with the focal brand, which also means that he or she
COO, the brand name, or the price. In contrast, intrinsic cannot revert to intrinsic cues, the use of extrinsic cues
cues refer to the product’s inherent attributes that cannot such as the COO should play an important role in the
be modified without changing the physical features of monetary product evaluation. If, however, the consumer
the product, such as its ingredients. Country of origin is highly familiar with the brand, it becomes more likely
can be considered an extrinsic cue that consumers use to that he or she retrieves prior experiences and will use
judge the product offered (Peterson and Jolibert 1995). intrinsic cues along with COO information to assess the
product (Rao and Monroe 1988). Thus, in a high-
Consumers are more inclined to use extrinsic cues when involvement situation, the reliance on extrinsic cues such
the intrinsic cues (e.g., performance) are not available or as the COO should decline in case of high familiarity
quality is difficult to judge (Zeithaml 1988). Without with the brand. This leads to the following hypothesis:

Country-of-Origin Effects and Willingness to Pay 23


H3: In a high-involvement setting, the higher the EXPERIMENT 1
consumers’ brand familiarity, the weaker is Experimental Design and Stimuli
the effect of COO on consumers’ WTP.
Experiment 1 uses a 2 (COO: favorable vs. unfavora-
ble) ¥ 2 (brand familiarity: high vs. low) between-
METHOD OVERVIEW subjects factorial design. The participants, postgradu-
We conduct three complementary experimental stud- ate business and social sciences students from a well-
ies, using different countries and actual brands as known German university, were randomly assigned to
stimuli. Our first experiment focuses on the investigation the four experimental groups and received an opportu-
of COO effects for highly familiar and less familiar nity to actually buy the branded products in the study.
brands under conditions of congruency—that is, when They also received extra course credit for their partici-
the COO coincides with the home country of the pation. The sample consisted of 127 participants;
brand (Häubl and Elrod 1999). Specifically, we employ their average age was 23.6 years, and 70.1% were
a 2 (COO) ¥ 2 (brand familiarity) between-subjects female.
design and use brands that actually originate from the
respective countries. Our second experiment extends We chose mineral water as a product category for two
the first experiment and examines the focal relation- main reasons. First, it is an everyday consumer good
ships in an incongruent condition—that is, when the used by students and nonstudents alike, so the use of a
COO and home country of the brand (i.e., the brand student sample should not pose a major threat to the
origin) are different. Such incongruence is common in study’s external validity (Lynch 1982). Second, the study
today’s marketplace because the globalization of mar- participants might be obliged to actually buy the
kets drives numerous companies to consider produc- branded product with their own money (see our descrip-
tion shifts to other countries (Funk et al. 2010) in an tion of the BDM procedure), and mineral water is suit-
effort to stay competitive (Jo, Nakamoto, and Nelson able because, in general, it is affordable to anyone. The
2003). The new production places are usually countries choice of a low-value product category follows previous
consumers would not associate with the brand, and applications of the BDM approach (e.g., Wertenbroch
notably, they often have less positive country images and Skiera 2002).
than the brand’s home country. While the first two
experiments are both conducted in a low-involvement Regarding the choice of specific countries and brands,
setting (mineral water), the third experiment investigates we used actual branded products rather than fictitious
the impact of COO and brand familiarity on WTP in a alternatives because participants might actually have to
high-involvement setting (sport shoes).2 Finally, we buy the product. For the experimental manipulations,
undertake a follow-up study to Experiment 3, also in a we needed two countries that, on the one hand, differed
high-involvement setting (DVD players), to check the in their country images and, on the other hand, offered
stability of the results.3 mineral water brands varying in familiarity. Because
German consumers are mainly familiar with mineral
All our experiments are separate and self-standing water brands from neighboring European countries, we
studies, and we carried them out in the second half of selected France as the country with a more favorable
2008. Each experiment is based on a separate sample country image and Austria as the country with a less
of university students. While this inevitably places lim- favorable country image. Using Roth and Romeo’s
its on the generalizability of the observed effects, the (1992) well-established scale to measure country image
use of student samples is justified because our focus is (see the Appendix), a pretest (n = 30) confirmed that
on testing theoretical hypotheses rather than generat- France’s image (MFrance = 5.13) is significantly more
ing representative estimates of consumers’ absolute favorable than Austria’s (MAustria = 4.43; t(58) = 2.46,
WTP values. In this context, “when the researcher is p < .05).
interested in theoretical explanation, a homogeneous
sample is the preferred option…. Lowering inter- For brand familiarity, we manipulated the experimental
subject variance in this way enhances the likelihood of conditions by choosing mineral water brands that actu-
finding support for the theory is true. In such ally originate from the two countries. For France, we
instances, student samples or other homogeneous selected Evian as the well-known brand and Thonon as
groups are preferred” (Sternthal, Tybout, and Calder the lesser-known brand. Pretest results (n = 30) revealed
1994, p. 208). a significant difference in terms of familiarity between

24 Journal of International Marketing


the two brands (Mfamiliar France = 4.46 vs. Munfamiliar France = than or equal to (lower than) the selling price drawn
1.09; t(58) = 12.72, p < .001). For the brands from Aus- from the urn, he or she had to (could not) buy the prod-
tria, we chose Römerquelle (high familiarity) and Vös- uct at that drawn price.
lauer (low familiarity). Pretest results also revealed signifi-
cant differences in brand familiarity (Mfamiliar Austria = All participants in our study agreed to take part after
2.32 vs. Munfamiliar Austria = 1.61; t(58) = 1.99, p < .05). they understood the BDM procedure. Furthermore,
Our experimental manipulation is in line with previous none of the participants refused to fulfill their purchase
studies that differentiated between varying levels of obligations (in case WTP ≥ selling price). These condi-
brand familiarity by choosing a highly familiar and an tions confirm the high criterion validity of the BDM
unfamiliar brand name (e.g., Cordell 1992). approach (Wertenbroch and Skiera 2002).

Procedure In addition to indicating their WTP, study participants


subsequently completed a questionnaire with measures
For the WTP elicitation, we used the BDM approach of country image (Roth and Romeo 1992) and brand
(Becker, DeGroot, and Marschak 1964), which offers familiarity (Diamantopoulos, Smith, and Grime 2005),
several benefits over other methods of WTP elicitation, which enabled us to perform manipulation checks on
including incentive compatibility and proximity to an the experimental conditions. The questionnaire also
actual act of purchase (Wertenbroch and Skiera 2002). elicited information about potentially relevant covari-
The incentive-compatible character of this method ates identified in previous literature: price consciousness
prompts consumers to reveal their true WTP and (Lichtenstein, Ridgway, and Netemeyer 1993), purchase
thereby solves the problem of respondents’ tendency to involvement (Lichtenstein, Bloch, and Black 1988),
indicate a higher or lower price than their true WTP, as brand commitment (Chaudhuri and Holbrook 2001),
is the case in hypothetical WTP elicitation formats. and product design (Bloch, Brunel, and Arnold 2003).
As additional control variables, we included measures of
Participants consider a point-of-purchase scenario, in experience with the COO (measured as visits to the
which they might have to buy and pay for the product, noted countries) and sociodemographic variables (age,
depending on their stated WTP and a randomly deter- sex, and income). Participants also responded to ques-
mined selling price. The actual price for the product is tions about the realism of the experimental treatments.
drawn, lottery style, from an urn. The lottery tickets Their answers revealed no threats to the study’s internal
represent price tags that indicate a range of prices; how- validity. We provide a description of all the variables
ever, the distribution of prices remains secret to prevent and associated psychometric information based on con-
anchoring effects (Wertenbroch and Skiera 2002). Each firmatory factor analysis (CFA) in the Appendix.
bid (WTP) above or equal to the drawn price forces the
bidder to purchase the product at the price determined Results
by the lottery. Participants with lower bids are not
allowed to purchase the product, nor are they allowed Manipulation Checks. To investigate the validity of the
to rebid. COO manipulation, we conducted an analysis of vari-
ance (ANOVA) with the manipulated COO (France vs.
To implement the BDM procedure, we first showed the Austria) as the independent variable and the measured
actual product to the participants in each experimental country image (Roth and Romeo 1992) as the depend-
group, along with a short product description that ent variable. The COO manipulation check showed that
reflected the brand and its associated COO. We next the country image of France is significantly more favor-
informed participants of their opportunity to actually able than that of Austria (MFrance = 4.71, MAustria =
purchase the product for the amount of money they 4.09; F(1, 125) = 16.34, p < .001). We used a similar
would be willing to spend to obtain it. We indicated that manipulation check for brand familiarity. As we
the selling price had not yet been fixed and would be expected, the group of well-known brands (French
determined randomly at the end of the study, through a brand Evian, Austrian brand Römerquelle; Mfamiliar =
drawing of lots from the urn. Participants also under- 2.88) is much more familiar to consumers than the com-
stood that they could not influence the distribution of bination of lesser-known brands (French brand Thonon,
prices in the urn. Finally, we asked them for the maxi- Austrian brand Vöslauer; Munfamiliar = 1.22; F(1, 125) =
mum amount of money they would be willing to pay for 59.70, p < .001). Thus, the manipulation of both inde-
the product. If a participant’s stated WTP was higher pendent variables was successful.

Country-of-Origin Effects and Willingness to Pay 25


We also tested if the aforementioned control variables In Figure 2, we display the price response functions per
affect consumers’ WTP. The results showed that only experimental treatment. Consistent with H1, the price
brand commitment had a (marginally) significant response function for the favorable country image
impact on WTP, and therefore we retained it as a covari- (France) is associated with higher WTP values than the
ate in the subsequent analysis. function with the less favorable country image (Austria).

Test of Hypotheses. We ran an analysis of covariance


(ANCOVA) with WTP (derived from the BDM procedure) EXPERIMENT 2
as the dependent variable, COO and brand familiarity as
factors, and brand commitment as a covariate. Country of
Experimental Design and Stimuli
origin had a significant main effect (F(1, 122) = 7.52, p < In Experiment 2, we extend and validate the findings of
.01), in support of H1. As Figure 1 shows, respondents Experiment 1 under conditions of incongruity between
are willing to pay a higher price for a brand originating the COO and the home country of the brand. We again
from France (COO with a more favorable country use a 2 (COO: favorable vs. unfavorable) ¥ 2 (brand
image) than for a brand originating from Austria (COO familiarity: high vs. low) between-subjects factorial
with a less favorable country image); the mean WTP for design and apply it to the same product category as in
a brand from France is 1.02 euros, whereas that for a Experiment 1, that is, mineral water. In the current con-
brand from Austria is only .77 euros. The strength of the text, a situation of incongruity exists when the company
main effect reveals that COO has a notable influence on decides to use sources (i.e., water springs) and bottling
WTP (partial h2COO = .06), which corresponds to a plants in another country than the brand’s home country.
medium effect size (Cohen 1988).
To manipulate the COO and brand familiarity, we
Regarding H2, we found no evidence that brand famili- retained France as the COO with a more favorable coun-
arity moderates the effect of COO on WTP, as indicated try image with Evian as the familiar and Thonon as the
by the nonsignificant interaction term (F(1, 122) = .15, unfamiliar brands. For the COO with a less favorable
p = .70). That is, the COO influences a consumer’s WTP, country image, we chose Turkey and kept the same two
regardless of familiarity with the brand in a low- brands that were used for France. Therefore, Turkey rep-
involvement setting. Finally, the covariate brand com- resents the incongruent setting because the COO is not
mitment revealed a marginally positive impact on WTP.4 the same as the home country of the focal brands.

Figure 1. Experiment 1: Means of WTP by Experimental Condition


1.15
1.10 1.09 (.09)
1.05
WTP (in Euros)

1.00
.95 .95 (.09)
High brand
.90 familiarity
.85 Low brand
.80 (.09) familiarity
.80
.75
.73 (.09)
.70
Favorable Country Image Less Favorable Country
(France) Image (Austria)
COO
Notes: Standard deviations are in parentheses.

26 Journal of International Marketing


Figure 2. Experiment 1: Price Response Functions per Experimental Treatment
A: COO

100%
90%
Relative Number of Consumers

80%
70%
(Cumulative)

60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
.00 .40 .80 1.20 1.60 2.00 2.40 2.80 3.20
WTP (in Euros)
France
Austria

B: Brand Familiarity

100%
90%
Relative Number of Consumers

80%
70%
(Cumulative)

60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
.00 .40 .80 1.20 1.60 2.00 2.40 2.80 3.20
WTP (in Euros)
High brand familiarity
Low brand familiarity

A pretest (n = 30) using Roth and Romeo’s (1992) ment, we used a new sample of 129 postgraduate
scale verified the different perceptions of the two business and social sciences students who were ran-
countries in terms of country image, showing that the domly allocated to the four experimental groups. The
country image of France (MFrance = 5.13) is signifi- average age of participants was 23.5 years, and 55%
cantly more favorable than that of Turkey (MTurkey = were female. We also used the same procedure (i.e.,
2.77; t(58) = 9.47, p < .001). To conduct the experi- BDM method) to elicit WTP values and the same

Country-of-Origin Effects and Willingness to Pay 27


multi-item scales as in Experiment 1 to measure our (F(1, 124) = 11.30, p < .01). Specifically, as Figure 3
variables (see the Appendix). shows, respondents display a higher WTP for a product
from a COO with a more favorable country image
Results (MFrance = .99 euros) than for the same product from a
COO with a less favorable country image (MTurkey = .76
Manipulation Checks. To investigate the validity of the euros). This provides further support for H1. The
COO manipulation, we conducted an ANOVA with the strength of the main effect reveals that the COO has a
measured country image (Roth and Romeo 1992) as the considerable influence on WTP (partial h2COO = .08),
dependent variable. Respondents evaluated France’s which corresponds to a medium to large effect size
country image much more favorably (MFrance = 4.70) (Cohen 1988).
than Turkey’s (MTurkey = 2.82; F(1, 127) = 164.01, p <
.001). We used a similar manipulation check for brand Regarding our second hypothesis, the interaction effect
familiarity, running an ANOVA between the two brands between COO and brand familiarity was not significant
(Evian vs. Thonon). As we expected, Evian enjoys much (F(1, 124) = .62, p = .43), again indicating no moderat-
higher brand familiarity among respondents (Mfamiliar = ing role of brand familiarity in a low-involvement set-
4.14) than Thonon (Munfamiliar = 1.16; F(1, 127) = ting. Regarding the covariate, brand commitment
265.33, p < .001). Thus, the manipulation of the two revealed a positive impact on WTP (F(1, 124) = 10.44,
independent variables of interest was successful. p < .01).5 Figure 4 shows the relevant price response
functions.
As in Experiment 1, an analysis of the control variables
revealed that, apart from brand commitment, none of Unlike in Experiment 1, in which the COO and the home
them had any significant impact on WTP. Therefore, we country of the brand are the same and thus interpretation
retained brand commitment, whereas we dropped the of the COO effect on WTP is straightforward, here the sig-
other control variables from further analysis. nificant effect of COO on the outcome variable might be
explained by the differences in country image and/or the
Test of Hypotheses. We again used an ANCOVA with differences in congruity (Häubl and Elrod 1999). In an
WTP as the dependent variable, COO and brand famili- attempt to “isolate” a potential incongruity effect, we first
arity as factors, and brand commitment as a covariate. run a regression analysis with WTP as the dependent
We found a significant main effect of COO on WTP variable and the measured country image as the predictor.

Figure 3. Experiment 2: Means of WTP by Experimental Condition


1.15
1.10
1.05 1.02 (.07)
WTP (in Euros)

1.00
.97 (.07)
.95
High brand
.90 familiarity

.85 Low brand


familiarity
.80 .79 (.07)
.75 .74 (.07)
.70
Favorable Country Image Less Favorable Country
(France) Image (T
(Turkey)
Turkey)
urkey)
COO
Notes: Standard deviations are in parentheses.

28 Journal of International Marketing


Figure 4. Experiment 2: Price Response Functions per Experimental Treatment

A: COO

100%
Relative Number of Consumers

90%
80%
70%
(Cumulative)

60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
.00 .40 .80 1.20 1.60 2.00 2.40 2.80 3.20
WTP (in Euros)
France Turkey

B: Brand Familiarity

100%
Relative Number of Consumers

90%
80%
70%
(Cumulative)

60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
.00 .40 .80 1.20 1.60 2.00 2.40 2.80 3.20
WTP (in Euros)
High brand familiarity Low brand familiarity

In a second step, we extracted the (unstandardized) regres- EXPERIMENT 3


sion residuals and used them in a t-test analysis of the con- Experimental Design and Stimuli
gruent and incongruent conditions. Because the t-test
revealed no significant difference between the congruent Experiment 3 examines the impact of COO on WTP in a
COO–country-of-brand condition (France) and the incon- high-involvement setting and further tests the moderating
gruent COO–country-of-brand condition (Turkey) (t(127) influence of brand familiarity, as described in H3. Because
= .78, p = .44), no incongruity effect can be identified. the issue of a production shift to another country is par-
Thus, the observed COO effect on WTP is solely attribut- ticularly relevant in the case of high-involvement prod-
able to the difference in country images, whereas con- ucts, we conduct Experiment 3 under conditions of incon-
gruity does not seem to play a role. gruity between the COO and the brand’s home country.

Country-of-Origin Effects and Willingness to Pay 29


We chose sports shoes as a high-involvement product ning to buy new sports shoes. The shoes were presented
category for several reasons. First, consumers usually in a picture, and the brand and COO were included in
take interest when buying such a product because the the product’s description. Participants then indicated
performance of the shoes during sports activities is a their WTP for the product.
critical issue. Second, sports shoes are a widely used
product bought by many types of consumers (both male Unlike in Experiments 1 and 2, we could not apply the
and female consumers, both students and nonstudents). BDM method in Experiment 3 because it is only suitable
Third, the product’s value should be in line with most for inexpensive products (Wertenbroch and Skiera
people’s budgets, and a reasonable price range can be 2002). In the case of more expensive products (e.g.,
expected in response behavior. Fourth, several previous sport shoes), it is not realistic to expect consumers to
studies have identified sport shoes as high-involvement actually purchase the product “on the spot” because of
products (e.g., Quester and Lim 2003). the financial outlay involved. Thus, we turned to contin-
gent valuation (Mitchell and Carson 1989) and asked
Using Zaichkowsky’s (1994) scale, we measured prod- respondents directly to state their WTP. Compared with
uct involvement with four items that refer to the per- BDM, contingent valuation is a less satisfactory
ceived importance, relevance, and valuation of the approach for measuring WTP (Miller et al. 2011;
product. A pretest subsequently revealed that partici- Wertenbroch and Skiera 2002). We address this issue
pants are significantly more involved with sports shoes when we discuss the testing of the hypotheses.
(M sports shoes = 5.08) than with mineral water
(Mmineral water = 4.17; t(505) = 6.77, p < .001). In addition to revealing their WTP, participants com-
pleted the same questions as in Experiments 1 and 2; we
Similar to Experiment 2, we employed a 2 (COO: favor- also included acquired product knowledge through
able vs. unfavorable) ¥ 2 (brand familiarity: high vs. product experience (Roehm and Sternthal 2001) as an
low) between-subjects factorial design. A total of 251 additional control variable to account for possible dif-
participants, undergraduate students from a well-known ferences due to varying expertise (for measurement
German university, participated in the study and were details, see the Appendix).
randomly allocated to one of the four experimental
groups. Participants were business and social sciences Results
students; their average age was 23.4 years, and 55.8%
were female. We used lottery winnings as incentives. Manipulation Checks. To perform checks on the COO
manipulation, we conducted an ANOVA with the meas-
For the manipulation of the COO, we selected the United ured country image as the dependent variable. The
States as the COO with a favorable country image and results revealed that the country image of the United
South Korea as the COO with a less favorable country States (MUSA = 4.76) is significantly more favorable
image. Several previous studies provide evidence that the than the country image of South Korea (MSouth Korea =
images of the two countries differ significantly from each 3.56; F(1, 249) = 87.82, p < .001).
other (e.g., Ahmed and d’Astous 2008). For the manipu-
lation of brand familiarity, we distinguished between We conducted a similar manipulation check on brand
Nike as the well-known brand of sports shoes and familiarity. Consistent with our expectations, the results
Brooks as the lesser known brand. The Unites States is showed that Nike is a highly familiar brand (Mfamiliar =
the home country of both brands in terms of headquar- 5.15) among respondents compared with Brooks
ters location and therefore represents the congruent con- (Munfamiliar = 1.52; F(1, 249) = 726.80, p < .001). Thus,
dition; we used the same brands, but made in South as in Experiments 1 and 2, the manipulation of both
Korea, to create the incongruent condition. independent variables was successful.

Procedure Test of Hypotheses. Empirical evidence shows that con-


tingent valuation is afflicted by overestimations in
Participants received a questionnaire that presented a response behavior (Neill et al. 1994; Völckner 2006).
short scenario that varied over the experimental treat- Hypothetical settings that offer no consequences for
ment conditions. They were told to imagine that they participants’ decisions do not sufficiently motivate
made up their mind to do more sports, such as running, respondents to reveal their “true” preferences: What
going to the gym, or the like, and therefore were plan- participants say they would do in hypothetical situa-

30 Journal of International Marketing


tions might not necessarily correspond to what they difference between the experimental treatment groups
actually do (Ding, Grewal, and Liechty 2005). If there is (H(3) = 13.81, p < .01).
no purchase obligation, WTP estimates exceed the
actual WTP (Völckner 2006); thus, consumers’ WTP is To discover which of the experimental treatment groups
significantly higher in a hypothetical purchase situation differ significantly from one another, we subsequently
than in a real one (Neill et al. 1994). conducted Mann-Whitney tests. To avoid inflation of a
Type I error, we applied a Bonferroni correction, result-
Strategic response behavior provides a further explana- ing in a significance level of .008. The results show that
tion because respondents might overstate (or under- only one group (less favorable COO/low brand famili-
state) their true WTP when they believe that their arity) significantly differs from the rest (for comparison
response has an influence on the actual product offering with favorable COO/high brand familiarity, U = 1392.5,
(or on the product’s price) (Wang, Venkatesh, and Chat- p < .008, z = –2.90, r = –.26; for comparison with less
terjee 2007). Unfortunately, extant research does not favorable COO/high brand familiarity, U = 1318.5, p <
provide an answer on how to handle the problem that .008, z = –3.15, r = –.28; for comparison with favorable
WTP values from a hypothetical payment context are COO/low brand familiarity, U = 1313.5, p < .008,
likely to differ from WTP estimates elicited with an z = –3.05, r = –.27). The effect sizes r indicate small to
actual purchase commitment (Völckner 2006). Never- medium effect sizes for all three comparisons. The other
theless, even though an open-ended question format three experimental treatment groups (favorable
generates a hypothetical bias when asking respondents COO/high brand familiarity, less favorable COO/high
directly for their WTP, such an approach still has the brand familiarity, favorable COO/low brand famili-
potential to forecast an accurate demand curve (Miller arity) do not differ significantly from one another.
et al. 2011).
Thus, our analysis reveals a significant interaction effect
Bearing the preceding discussion in mind and given that of COO and brand familiarity. Specifically, the results
we directly asked respondents for their WTP in Experi- indicate a COO effect in case of low brand familiarity
ment 3, we are aware of the overestimation in WTP that (because there is a significant difference between the less
is likely to occur. To take this critical point into consid- favorable COO/low brand familiarity and favorable
eration, we treated the reported WTP estimates as hav- COO/low brand familiarity conditions) and an effect of
ing only rank (ordinal) data quality and used a nonpara- brand familiarity in case of a COO with a less favorable
metric statistical method (Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA) to image.6 Thus, H3 is supported. Figure 5 shows the mean
test our hypotheses. Nonparametric statistical tests are WTP values for the different experimental conditions.
less restrictive in their requirements toward the data. Note that care needs to be exercised when interpreting
Our data show skewed and tailed distributions within the absolute WTP values depicted in the Figure because,
groups that result from extreme scores caused from the as we already noted, the contingent valuation approach
open-ended character of the WTP measurement. In this applied in Experiment 3 tends to overestimate respon-
case, the Kruskal-Wallis test is actually more powerful dents’ WTP.
than the F-test of the AN(C)OVA.
Following the same procedure as in Experiment 2 to
With regard to the COO main effect, the Kruskal- identify a potential congruity effect, we found no differ-
Wallis test results revealed that consumers’ WTP is signifi- ence between the congruent COO–country-of-brand
cantly affected by the COO (H(1) = 4.03, p < .05). This condition (United States) and the incongruent
supports H1. Furthermore, Jonckheere’s test shows a sig- COO–country-of-brand condition (South Korea)
nificant trend in the data, such that the less favorably the (t(249) = .44, p = .66). In line with Experiment 2, we can
country image of a COO is evaluated, the more the conclude that the COO effect on WTP is attributable
median WTP decreases (J = 6725.5, z = –2.01, r = –.13). only to the difference in country images and that con-
gruity does not play a role.
Regarding the interaction effect, H3 postulated that in a
high-involvement setting, the COO effect on WTP Finally, common method variance should not cause a
declines with increasing brand familiarity. To investigate problem in this experiment, despite the self-reported
the COO ¥ brand familiarity interaction, we simultane- nature of the dependent variable (WTP). We measured
ously compared all four experimental conditions; the WTP before the assessment of the covariates to avoid
corresponding Kruskal-Wallis test revealed a significant the possibility of biasing the measurement of the

Country-of-Origin Effects and Willingness to Pay 31


Figure 5. Experiment 3: Means of WTP by Experimental Condition
85
83 81.51 (4.04)
81 81.02 (4.04)
4.04)
79
78.21 (4.05)
WTP (in Euros)

77
75 High brand
familiarity
73
Low brand
71
familiarity
69
67 66.59 (4.17)
4.17)
65
Favorable Country Image Less Favorable Country
(United States) Image (South Korea)
COO

Notes: Standard deviations are in parentheses.

dependent variable. In addition, we used different meas- confirmed that Samsung is more familiar (Mfamiliar =
urement formats for our variables (Podsakoff et al. 4.80) to respondents than LG (Munfamiliar = 3.52; F(1,
2003). Finally, we employed Lindell and Whitney’s 217) = 54.52, p < .001).
(2001) approach using “the smallest observed correla-
tion among the manifest variables as a proxy for com- Consistent with Experiment 3, the results show that
mon method variance” (Podsakoff et al. 2003, p. 893). brand familiarity moderates the COO effect on WTP.
All significant correlations between the covariates and Specifically, a Kruskal-Wallis test reveals that the exper-
WTP remained significant after this adjustment, provid- imental treatment groups differed significantly from
ing no evidence for common method variance. each other (H(3) = 12.49, p < .01). Subsequently, Mann-
Whitney post hoc tests confirm the existence of a COO
Follow-Up to Experiment 3 effect for the less familiar brand (U = 739, p < .008, z =
–2.89, r = –.30) but not for the more familiar brand.7
One issue with Experiment 3 is that the more familiar
brand also comes from the country with the stronger
country image. Thus, the question arises whether we DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS
would obtain a similar pattern of results if we were to
study a strong, familiar brand but from a weak COO. With equity theory as a theoretical foundation, our
To address this issue, we conducted an additional fol- experimental studies reveal that consumers are willing
low-up study on a high-involvement product category to pay higher prices for branded products from a COO
(DVD players), in which we focused on a strong and with a favorable country image than for products from
well-known brand (Samsung) that originates from a a COO with a less favorable image (e.g., for products
country with a relatively poor country image (South from France vs. Austria or products from the United
Korea). For the lesser-known brand, we selected another States vs. South Korea). In addition, we controlled for
South Korean brand, namely, LG Electronics. As in potential incongruity effects, which did not show any
Experiment 3, we chose the United States as the COO impact on consumers’ WTP. Thus, the driving force that
with the more favorable country image to compare it explains the COO effect is the country image, not
with South Korea. The sample consisted of 219 business whether the COO coincides with the brand’s home
and social sciences students; their average age was 21.6 country. These findings extend the field’s knowledge of
years, and 58.4% were female. Manipulation checks the impact of COO in several ways.

32 Journal of International Marketing


First, our research adds to existing literature that Third, we offer important insights into the price-related
focuses mainly on the influence of COO on product consequences of COO for different levels of consumer
evaluations and purchase intentions. However, the theo- brand familiarity. In previous studies, brand familiarity
retical rationale underpinning COO effects is often not has emerged as a relevant factor in the use of extrinsic
elaborated, and indeed, “the absence of a theory or cues, such as COO (Cordell 1992; Jo, Nakamoto, and
sound conceptual frameworks have been the hallmark Nelson 2003). Our study tests explicitly whether a con-
of a large number of CO studies” (Samiee 2011, p. 476). sumer’s familiarity with a brand dilutes the strength of
With the application of equity theory, we introduce a the COO’s impact on his or her WTP. While, as
new conceptual basis that further develops our theoreti- expected, we do not observe a moderating role of brand
cal understanding of COO effects by explicitly consider- familiarity in a low-involvement setting (Experiments 1
ing both the benefits and the sacrifices associated with and 2), we do in a high-involvement setting (Experiment
obtaining a branded product from a particular COO. 3). In the latter case, in which consumers have a greater
Our findings show that consumers not only prefer and interest in and thus are more involved with the product
assign a higher value to branded products from a COO category, they rely on extrinsic cues such as the COO
with a favorable country image but also, consistent with only if they are relatively unfamiliar with the brand. As
equity theory, are willing to spend more money to consumers become more familiar, they become more
obtain them. Therefore, one of the main theoretical con- likely to use intrinsic cues rather than extrinsic cues and
tributions of this study stems from “monetizing” the base their WTP on previous experiences with the brand.
COO effect (Nebenzahl and Jaffe 1993) through the Indeed, as Experiment 3 and its follow-up study show,
application of equity theory. in a high-involvement setting, WTP for highly familiar
brands is not materially affected by the COO.
Second, in a low-involvement setting, our findings are
based on the application of a WTP elicitation method Fourth, of the several control variables we included in our
(BDM) that is notable for its incentive-compatible analysis, brand commitment exhibited a positive influ-
approach. This approach ensures that respondents ence on consumers’ WTP determination in all three
will reveal their true WTP and not overestimate or experiments. This finding supports previous research sug-
understate the maximum price they would be willing gesting that consumers with a strong and favorable brand
to pay. Surprisingly, the BDM method had not been attitude are willing to pay a price premium (Chaudhuri
used previously in a COO context; this study indicates and Holbrook 2001). From a company’s perspective, it is
that it offers a promising procedure for empirical therefore worthwhile to take the extent of a consumer’s
investigations seeking to link COO to price-related brand commitment into account and improve it, because
outcomes. consumers reveal higher WTP as their attachment to the
brand increases. Regarding consumer price conscious-
Although in the high-involvement setting (Experiment ness, we find no influence on WTP in Experiments 1 and
3) we had to rely on the less reliable contingent evalua- 2, likely because mineral water is a product of low outlay
tion method for WTP elicitation purposes, we do not and low involvement. However, price consciousness
consider this a major shortcoming of our study. This is exhibited a negative impact on WTP in Experiment 3
because our focus was on a comparison of WTP across (sport shoes). This finding corresponds with the results of
countries and brand familiarity levels (to enable a test Lichtenstein, Bloch, and Black (1988), who show, using
of H3) rather than on an estimation of absolute WTP the same product category we used in Experiment 3, that
levels for the brands concerned. Any overestimation of price consciousness negatively affects the price accepta-
WTP generated by contingent valuation would apply bility level and the width of the acceptable price latitude.
across all experimental conditions and, therefore, not
greatly influence comparisons between them. In this Fifth, regarding the ongoing debate in the literature
context, recent evidence indicates that price response about the relevance of COO as an influence on consumer
functions obtained using the BDM method have a decision making (e.g., Josiassen and Harzing 2008;
comparable shape to those derived from the contingent Samiee 2011; Usunier 2006), our results emphasize its
valuation approach (Miller et al. 2011). The fact that importance as a consumer behavior cue. We empirically
we adopted a conservative approach and treated the support the view that the COO cue influences not only
WTP data as ordinal further helped overcome the lim- consumers’ perceptions but also their actions: In general,
itations of contingent valuation in the current study. consumers will spend more money for a branded prod-
uct from a COO with a more favorable country image.

Country-of-Origin Effects and Willingness to Pay 33


In two of our studies, this result emerges from a rela- account when they make a decision about the maxi-
tively realistic buying situation in which participants mum amount of money they are willing to spend on a
actually fulfilled the purchase act. This shows that the branded product, managers can use this information in
COO effect does not disappear when actual purchase their pricing decisions. If their brand originates and is
behavior (in terms of real WTP) is the dependent produced in a country with a good reputation and
variable, rather than “softer” measures such as reported image, the implementation of a premium pricing
attitudes and buying intentions. strategy should be easier because consumers’ WTP is
also likely to be higher. Although this suggestion might
Further research should try to replicate our findings appear intuitive, attitudes and behavior often differ,
using other COOs and product categories as stimuli, as and it is only through empirical support (such as that
well as respondents from different countries. For exam- offered by the current study) that one can determine
ple, the strength of the COO impact as an informational whether this is likely to be the case.
cue on consumers’ WTP may depend on the respondents’
country or culture. Comparative research seeking to Second, communication activities about the COO can
identify such cross-national or cross-cultural differences facilitate pricing policies such as price differentiation. If
would be most welcome. Similarly, the extent to which the firm finds itself in the fortunate position of benefit-
the COO impact on WTP is moderated by the product ing from a favorable COO image, it should emphasize
type involved (e.g., utilitarian vs. hedonic) or the per- the notion of COO in its communication strategy—for
ceived globalness/localness of the brand (Steenkamp and example, by calling attention to the COO during
De Jong 2010) is also open to investigation. Replications advertising activities or on the package design. With
and/or extensions of our study using nonstudent samples such measures, consumers should notice and process
would also enhance the external validity and generaliz- the COO as an informational cue, which in turn
ability of our findings. Although Experiments 1 and 2 should enhance their WTP. For example, in an inter-
have been conducted under relatively realistic conditions view with the Financial Times, the chief executive offi-
in which respondents actually had to pay for the prod- cer of the luxury group Tod’s stated the following
uct, they were still carried out in a laboratory setting. when being asked whether the “Made in Italy” label
Field study replications to validate our findings would will retain its luster: “Yes, because it is still the maxi-
therefore be highly desirable. In such field studies, the mum guarantee of high quality products such as ours.
impact of environmental conditions (e.g., the availability Like the French for perfume, the Swiss for watches.…
of substitute products) could also be studied when We have the hundreds, the thousands, of family firms,
assessing consumers’ WTP. Moreover, it would be worth- micro-enterprises, almost a Renaissance model, that
while examining whether and how the COO effect on guarantee that quality. That’s not easy to copy” (Asp-
WTP changes over time as a result of changes in country den 2011).
image perceptions. Economic (e.g., economic crisis in
Greece), political (e.g., uprising in Egypt), and even natu- Conversely, if the brand is linked to a less favorable
ral events (e.g., earthquakes in Japan) can all be expected COO image, the firm should highlight other product
to affect consumers’ images of countries and, in turn, attributes, rather than the COO, in its communication
those of their products; as a result, consumer response strategy (Verlegh, Steenkamp, and Meulenberg 2005).
variables (e.g., WTP) may also be affected. Finally, atten- However, in deciding whether to emphasize the COO,
tion to price-related outcomes other than WTP would managers should also consider the product category. In
provide insights complementary to those generated in the a low-involvement setting, our results indicate that an
current study. Price sensitivity, perceived price fairness, accentuation of the COO seems to be promising. For a
and price search behavior just are some examples of high-involvement setting, however, it is advisable to
variables that could be studied in conjunction with COO “prime” the COO within marketing activities, especially
and for which extant knowledge is limited. if consumers are relatively unfamiliar with the brand.

Third, at a broader strategic level, the effect of COO on


MANAGERIAL IMPLICATIONS consumers’ WTP, as shown in Experiments 2 and 3, indi-
cates the need for caution in decisions about a change in
From a managerial point of view, our findings hold COO. In this context, the inability to identify an incon-
interest for international marketers in several respects. gruity effect suggests that a discrepancy between the home
First, if COO is a major factor that consumers take into country of the brand and the country of production is not

34 Journal of International Marketing


necessarily problematic. However, relocating plants or NOTES
production facilities to a COO with a less favorable coun-
try image for the sake of cost savings should be weighed 1. Operating income is defined as “total revenues from
against the possible margin losses that may result from the operations minus cost of goods sold and operating
lower WTP that also may result from the move. For exam- costs (excluding interest expense and income taxes)”
ple, Steiff, the German producer of plush toys, restored (Horngren, Datar, and Rajan 2012, p. 64). Operating
production in Germany after outsourcing it to China for a income differs from net income in that the latter also
few years. Bringing the brand back to its home country takes nonoperating benefits (e.g., interest revenue)
(Wiesmann 2008), the company is likely to benefit from a into account from which nonoperating costs (e.g.,
higher WTP among potential customers given the much interest) and income taxes are deducted (Horngren,
better country image of Germany in relation to China. In Datar, and Rajan 2012, p. 92).
a similar vein, if a company is envisaging a foreign part-
nership, it should explicitly consider the extent to which 2. Common method bias, which often arises when the
this cooperation may have repercussions (positive or nega- same respondent offers assessments of both the inde-
tive) on its customers’ WTP. This issue is particularly pendent and the dependent variables (Podsakoff et al.
important for low-involvement products and for high- 2003), does not pose a problem in Experiments 1 and
involvement products that do not enjoy high familiarity 2. Unlike survey-based designs, we manipulate both
among customers. independent variables (i.e., COO and brand famili-
arity) experimentally instead of measuring them, and
Finally, consumers increasingly demand and are more respondents only provide data on the dependent
aware of sustainable production, which includes envi- variable (WTP).
ronmental as well as ethical issues. Sustainability is a
major topic for internationally operating companies and 3. We conducted this follow-up study on the suggestion
is often anchored in their strategy; the provenance of the of an anonymous reviewer, whom we would like to
product can play a role with respect to the logistical out- thank.
lay, conditions of employment or eco-friendly produc-
tion. In Germany, for example, consumers increasingly 4. Our analysis of the statistical power of the applied
tend to buy local and regional products (positive image ANCOVA model reveals a power value of .86 (a =
of local/regional area that consumers can trust, avoid- .05), which exceeds the recommended value of .80
ance of long-distance shipments, and so on play a role (Cohen 1988).
in this regard). Similarly, with respect to ethical and eco-
friendly concerns, some countries have better images 5. Our analysis of the statistical power of the applied
related to stricter laws and conditions than others. Some ANCOVA model reveals a power value of .99 (a =
Asian countries, for example, allow a much cheaper but .05), which well exceeds the recommended value of
also less ethical and eco-friendly production and have .80 (Cohen 1988).
the respective (negative) reputation for it. Thus, the
provenance of the product has the potential to reassure 6. We found the interaction effect of COO and brand
consumers’ decisions to make a greater investment to familiarity to be significant when examined with
obtain a product they can trust. In other words, the ANCOVA (F(1, 243) = 5.04, p < .05). Regarding the
COO can represent a unique selling proposition for covariates, price consciousness (negative) (F(1, 243) =
which consumers are willing to pay. 6.99, p < .01), brand commitment (positive) (F(1,
243) = 4.38, p < .05), and product design (positive)
In conclusion, our findings demonstrate that COO not (F(1, 243) = 11.14, p < .01) revealed significant
only affects what consumers are likely to buy (as repeat- effects on WTP.
edly shown in prior research) but also how much they
are willing to pay. As we outlined, practitioners will 7. We obtained the same results when we applied an
consider this finding highly relevant, while researchers ANCOVA (F(1, 214) = 8.20, p < .01). In terms of
should view our study as a springboard into further covariates, the latter revealed a significant negative
investigations of the price-related consequences of COO influence of price consciousness (F(1, 214) = 11.69,
effects. p < .01).

Country-of-Origin Effects and Willingness to Pay 35


Appendix. Constructs and Items

Variable
(Assumed Average
Influence on Construct Variance
WTP) Conceptualization Measurement Cronbach’s a Reliability Extracted

Country “Country image is the overall Four items on a seven-point bipolar semantic differential .81/.86/.86 .81/.87/.86 .52/.63/.61
image (+) perception consumers form of scale:
products from a particular 1. How would you rate innovativeness of products from
country, based on their prior [COO]? Innovativeness designates the use of new tech-
perception of the country’s nology and engineering advances. (1 = “not innovative,”
production and marketing and 7 = “innovative”)
strengths and weaknesses.” 2. How would you rate the attractiveness of the design of

36 Journal of International Marketing


(Roth and Romeo 1992, products from [COO], regarding appearance, style, colors,
p. 480) and variety? (1 = “no attractive design,” and 7 = “attrac-
tive design”)
3. How would you rate the prestige of products from [COO],
including their exclusivity, status, and brand name reputa-
tion? (1 = “low prestige,” and 7 = “high prestige”)
4. How would you rate the workmanship of products
from [COO], which comprises reliability, durability, crafts-
manship, and manufacturing quality? (1 = “bad workman-
ship,” and 7= “good workmanship”)
Source: Roth and Romeo (1992)
Brand “Brand familiarity reflects the Five items on a seven-point semantic differential scale: .92/.94/.97 .95/.96/.97 .80/.84/.87
familiarity (+) extent of a consumer’s direct 1. 1 = “unfamiliar,” and 7 = “familiar”
and indirect experience with a 2. 1 = “inexperienced,” and 7 = “experienced”
brand.” (Campbell and Keller 3. 1 = “not knowledgeable,” and 7 = “knowledgeable”
2003, p. 293) 4. 1 = “uninformed,” and 7 = “informed”
5. 1 = “novice buyer,” and 7 = “expert buyer”
Source: Diamantopoulos, Smith, and Grime (2005)
Appendix. Continued

Variable
(Assumed Average
Influence on Construct Variance
WTP) Conceptualization Measurement Cronbach’s a Reliability Extracted

Covariates

Price Price consciousness refers to Four items on a seven-point Likert-type scale (1 = “strongly .85/.87/.77 .87/.88/.79 .64/.65/.50
consciousness “the degree to which the con- disagree,” and 7 = “strongly agree”)
sumer focuses exclusively on 1. I usually buy products when they are on sale.
paying a low price.” (Lichten- 2. I buy the lowest priced product that will suit my needs.
stein, Ridgway, and Nete- 3. When it comes to choosing a product for me, I rely heavily
meyer 1993, p. 235) on price.
4. Price is the most important factor when I am choosing a
brand.
Source: Lichtenstein, Bloch, and Black (1988)
Purchase The level of perceived per- Three items on a seven-point Likert-type scale (1 = “strongly .90/.91/.89 .91/.91/.89 .76/.78/.73
involvement sonal relevance or importance disagree,” and 7 = “strongly agree”)
a consumer attaches to a 1. I choose my [product] very carefully.
product or brand during 2. Which [product] I use matters to me a lot.
choice processes. (Mittal and 3. Choosing a [product] is an important decision to me.
Lee 1988) Source: Mittal and Lee (1988)
Brand The construct refers to the Three items on a seven-point Likert-type scale (1 = “strongly .79/.80/.78 .80/.81/.79 .57/.59/.56
commitment pledging and binding of a disagree,” and 7 = strongly agree”)
consumer to a brand choice 1. I consider myself to be highly loyal to [brand name].
within a product class; thus, it 2. I would generally purchase [brand name] even when
describes an emotional or psy- another brand is on sale.
chological attachment to a 3. I would recommend [brand name] to others.
brand. (Ahluwalia, Unnava, Source: Ahluwalia, Unnava, and Burnkrant (2001), Chaudhuri
and Burnkrant 2001) (1999)

Country-of-Origin Effects and Willingness to Pay 37


Appendix. Continued

Variable
(Assumed Average
Influence on Construct Variance
WTP) Conceptualization Measurement Cronbach’s a Reliability Extracted

Product design The visual product esthetics Four items on a seven-point semantic differential scale .89/.89/.89 .90/.88/.87 .70/.65/.65
that create a product’s appear- 1. 1 = “unappealing,” and 7 = “appealing”
ance in terms of color, size, 2. 1 = “not likeable,” and 7 = “likeable”
shape, proportion, pattern, 3. 1 = “ordinary,” and 7 = “unique”
and so forth. (Bloch, Brunel, 4. 1 = “commonplace,” and 7 = “original”
and Arnold 2003) Source: Dahl, Chattopadhyay, and Gorn (1999)
Product The construct refers to the Five items on a seven-point semantic differential scale N.A./N.A./.94 N.A./N.A./.94 N.A./N.A./75

38 Journal of International Marketing


experience knowledge, experience and 1. How often do you use [product]? (1 = “never,” and 7 =
expertise a consumer disposes “permanently”)
with respect to a particular 2. How familiar are you with [product]? (1 = “unfamiliar,”
product. (Alba and Hutchin- and 7 = “familiar”)
son 1987) 3. How well-acquainted do you consider yourself with
[product]? (1 = “not at all acquainted,” and 7 = “very well
acquainted”)
4. How much of an expert would you call yourself regarding
[product]? (1 = “novice,” and 7 = “expert”
5. How regularly do you use [product]? (1 = “intermittently,”
and 7 = “regularly”)
Source: Roehm and Sternthal (2001)
Experience Visit to the country Have you ever been to [COO]? (yes/no)
with COO

Notes: Cronbach’s a, construct reliability, and average variance extracted values relate to Experiments 1, 2, and 3 respectively. N.A. = not applicable. CFA model fit was as follows: Experiment 1: c2(215) = 384.55,
RMSEA = .079, and CFI = .936. Experiment 2: c2(215) = 407.62, RMSEA = .084, and CFI = .930; Experiment 3: c2(329) = 976.46, RMSEA = .089, and CFI = .926.
REFERENCES Cohen, Jacob (1988), Statistical Power Analysis for the Behav-
Adams, Stacy J. (1965), “Inequity in Social Exchange,” in ioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Leonard
Berkowitz, ed. New York: Academic Press, 267–99. Cordell, Victor V. (1992), “Effects of Consumer Preferences for
Foreign Sourced Products,” Journal of International Business
Agrawal, Jagdish and Wagner A. Kamakura (1999), “Country Studies, 23 (2), 251–69.
of Origin: A Competitive Advantage?” International Journal
of Research in Marketing, 16 (4), 255–67. Dahl, Darren W., Amitava Chattopadhyay, and Gerald J. Gorn
(1999), “The Use of Visual Mental Imagery in New Product
Design,” Journal of Marketing Research, 36 (February),
Ahluwalia, Rohini, H. Rao Unnava, and Robert E. Burnkrant
18–28.
(2001), “The Moderating Role of Commitment on the
Spillover Effect of Marketing Communications,” Journal of
Diamantopoulos, Adamantios, Gareth Smith, and Ian Grime
Marketing Research, 38 (November), 458–70.
(2005), “The Impact of Brand Extensions on Brand Personal-
ity: Experimental Evidence,” European Journal of Marketing,
Ahmed, Sadrudin A. and Alain d’Astous (2008), “Antecedents,
39 (1/2), 129–49.
Moderators and Dimensions of Country-of-Origin Evalua-
tions,” International Marketing Review, 25 (1), 75–106.
Ding, Min, Rajdeep Grewal, and John Liechty (2005), “Incen-
tive-Aligned Conjoint Analysis,” Journal of Marketing
Alba, Joseph W. and J. Wesley Hutchinson (1987), “Dimensions
Research, 42 (February), 67–82.
of Consumer Expertise,” Journal of Consumer Research, 13
(4), 411–54.
Drozdenko, Ronald and Marlene Jensen (2009), “Translating
Country-of-Origin Effects into Prices,” Journal of Product &
Aspden, Peter (2011), “Lunch with the Ft: Diego Della Valle,”
Brand Management, 18 (5), 371–78.
Financial Times, (August 11), [available at http://www.ft.com].
Funk, Charles A., Jonathan D. Arthurs, Len J. Treviño, and Jeff
Becker, Gordon M., Morris H. DeGroot, and Jacob Marschak Joireman (2010), “Consumer Animosity in the Global Value
(1964), “Measuring Utility by a Single-Response Sequential Chain: The Effect of International Production Shifts on Will-
Method,” Behavioral Science, 9 (3), 226–32. ingness to Purchase Hybrid Products,” Journal of Inter-
national Business Studies, 41 (4), 639–51.
Bloch, Peter H., Frédéric F. Brunel, and Todd J. Arnold (2003),
“Individual Differences in the Centrality of Visual Product Garbarino, Ellen and Robert Slonim (2003), “Interrelationships
Aesthetics: Concept and Measurement,” Journal of Consumer and Distinct Effects of Internal Reference Prices on Perceived
Research, 29 (4), 551–65. Expensiveness and Demand,” Psychology & Marketing, 20
(3), 227–48.
Campbell, Margaret C. and Kevin L. Keller (2003), “Brand
Familiarity and Advertising Repetition Effects,” Journal of Han, Sangman, Sunil Gupta, and Donald R. Lehmann (2001),
Consumer Research, 30 (2), 292–304. “Consumer Price Sensitivity and Price Thresholds,” Journal of
Retailing, 77 (4), 435–56.
Carrington, Michal, Benjamin Neville, and Gregory Whitwell
(2010), “Why Ethical Consumers Don’t Walk Their Talk: Häubl, Gerald and Terry Elrod (1999), “The Impact of Con-
Towards a Framework for Understanding the Gap Between gruity Between Brand Name and Country of Production on
the Ethical Purchase Intentions and Actual Buying Behaviour Consumers’ Product Quality Judgements,” International
of Ethically Minded Consumers,” Journal of Business Ethics, Journal of Research in Marketing, 16 (3), 199–215.
97 (1), 139–58.
Homans, George C. (1961), Social Behavior: Its Elementary
Chaudhuri, Arjun (1999), “Does Brand Loyalty Mediate Brand Forms. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.
Equity Outcomes?” Journal of Marketing Theory & Practice,
7 (2), 136–46. Homburg, Christian, Nicole Koschate, and Wayne D. Hoyer
(2005), “Do Satisfied Customers Really Pay More? A Study of
——— and Morris B. Holbrook (2001), “The Chain of Effects the Relationship Between Customer Satisfaction and Willing-
from Brand Trust and Brand Affect to Brand Performance: ness to Pay,” Journal of Marketing, 69 (April), 84–96.
The Role of Brand Loyalty,” Journal of Marketing, 65 (April),
81–93. Horngren, Charles, Skrikant M. Datar, and Madhav V. Rajan
(2012), Cost Accounting: A Managerial Emphasis, 14th
Chen, Serena and Shelly Chaiken (1999), “The Heuristic-Sys- (international) ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
tematic Model in Its Broader Context,” in Dual-Process
Theories in Social Psychology, Shelly Chaiken and Yaacov Hu, Ye and Xin Wang (2010), “Country-of-Origin Premiums
Trope, eds. New York: The Guildford Press, 73–96. for Retailers in International Trades: Evidence from eBay’s

Country-of-Origin Effects and Willingness to Pay 39


International Markets,” Journal of Retailing, 86 (2), Marn, Michael V. and Robert L. Rosiello (1992), “Managing
200–207. Price, Gaining Profit,” Harvard Business Review, 70 (5),
84–94.
Hulland, John, Honorio S. Todiño, and Donald J. Lecraw
(1996), “Country-of-Origin Effects on Sellers’ Price Premiums Martin, Andrew (2007), “U.S.-Made Toys Benefit from China’s
in Competitive Philippine Markets,” Journal of International Troubles,” The New York Times, (August 15), [available at
Marketing, 4 (1), 57–79. http://www.nytimes.com].

Huppertz, John W., Sidney J. Arenson, and Richard H. Evans Merlo, Omar, Bryan A. Lukas, and Gregory J. Whitwell (2008),
(1978), “An Application of Equity Theory to Buyer–Seller “Heuristics Revisited: Implications for Marketing Research
Exchange Situations,” Journal of Marketing Research, 15 and Practice,” Marketing Theory, 8 (2), 189–204.
(May), 250–60.
Miller, Klaus M., Reto Hofstetter, Harley Krohmer, and Z. John
Jaffe, Eugene D. and Israel D. Nebenzahl (2006), National Zhang (2011), “How Should Consumers’ Willingness to Pay
Image and Competitive Advantage. Copenhagen: Copen- Be Measured? An Empirical Comparison of State-of-the-Art
hagen Business School Press. Approaches,” Journal of Marketing Research, 48 (February),
172–84.
Jo, Myung-Soo, Kent Nakamoto, and James E. Nelson (2003),
“The Shielding Effects of Brand Image Against Lower Quality Mitchell, Robert C. and Richard T. Carson (1989), Using Sur-
Countries-of-Origin in Global Manufacturing,” Journal of veys to Value Public Goods: The Contingent Valuation
Business Research, 56 (8), 637–46. Method. Washington, DC: Resources for the Future.

Johansson, Johny K. (1989), “Determinants and Effects of the Mittal, Banwari and Myung-Soo Lee (1988), “Separating
Use of ‘Made in’ Labels,” International Marketing Review, 6 Brand-Choice Involvement from Product Involvement via
(1), 47–58. Consumer Involvement Profiles,” in Advances in Consumer
Research, Vol. 15, Michael J. Houston. ed. Provo, UT: Asso-
Josiassen, Alexander and Anne-Wil Harzing (2008), “Descend- ciation for Consumer Research, 43–49.
ing from the Ivory Tower: Reflections on the Relevance and
Future of Country-of-Origin Research,” European Manage- Miyuri, Shirai and James R. Bettman (2005), “Consumer
ment Review, 5 (4), 264–70. Expectations Concerning Timing and Depth of the Next
Deal,” Psychology & Marketing, 22 (6), 457–72.
———, Bryan A. Lukas, and Gregory J. Whitwell (2008),
“Country-of-Origin Contingencies,” International Marketing Monroe, Kent B. (2003), Pricing: Making Profitable Decisions.
Review, 25 (4), 423–40. Boston: McGraw-Hill/Richard D. Irwin.

Klein, Jill Gabrielle, Richard Ettenson, and Marlene D. Morris Nebenzahl, Israel D. and Eugene D. Jaffe (1993), “Estimating
(1998), “The Animosity Model of Foreign Product Purchase: Demand Functions from the Country-of-Origin Effect,” in
An Empirical Test in the People’s Republic of China,” Journal Product-Country Images: Impact and Role in International
of Marketing, 62 (January), 89–100. Marketing, Nicolas Papadopoulos and Louise A. Heslop, eds.
New York: International Business Press, 159–78.
Lichtenstein, Donald R., Peter H. Bloch, and William C. Black
(1988), “Correlates of Price Acceptability,” Journal of Con- Neill, Helen R., Ronald G. Cummings, Philip T. Ganderton,
sumer Research, 15 (2), 243–52. Glenn W. Harrison, and Thomas McGuckin (1994), “Hypo-
thetical Surveys and Real Economic Commitments,” Land
———, Nancy M. Ridgway, and Richard G. Netemeyer (1993), Economics, 70 (2), 145–54.
“Price Perceptions and Consumer Shopping Behavior: A Field
Study,” Journal of Marketing Research, 30 (May), 234–45. Oliver, Richard L. and John E. Swan (1989), “Equity and Dis-
confirmation Perceptions as Influences on Merchant and
Lindell, Michael K. and David J. Whitney (2001), “Accounting Product Satisfaction,” Journal of Consumer Research, 16 (3),
for Common Method Variance in Cross-Sectional Research 372–83.
Designs,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 86 (1), 114–21.
Olson, Jerry C. (1972), “Cue Utilization in the Quality Percep-
Lynch, John G. (1982), “On the External Validity of Experi- tion Process: A Cognitive Model and an Empirical Test,” doc-
ments in Consumer Research,” Journal of Consumer toral dissertation, Purdue University.
Research, 9 (3), 225–39.
Pappu, Ravi, Pascale G. Quester, and Ray W. Cooksey (2007),
Manski, Charles F. (1990), “The Use of Intentions Data to Pre- “Country Image and Consumer-Based Brand Equity: Rela-
dict Behavior: A Best-Case Analysis,” Journal of the American tionships and Implications for International Marketing,”
Statistical Association, 85 (412), 934–40. Journal of International Business Studies, 38 (5), 726–45.

40 Journal of International Marketing


Peterson, Robert A. and Alain J.P. Jolibert (1995), “A Meta- Tse, David K. and Gerald J. Gorn (1993), “An Experiment on
Analysis of Country-of-Origin Effects,” Journal of Inter- the Salience of Country-of-Origin in the Era of Global
national Business Studies, 26 (4), 883–900. Brands,” Journal of International Marketing, 1 (1), 57–76.

Pharr, Julie M. (2005), “Synthesizing Country-of-Origin Usunier, Jean-Claude (2006), “Relevance in Business Research:
Research from the Last Decade: Is the Concept Still Salient in The Case of Country-of-Origin Research in Marketing,”
an Era of Global Brands?” Journal of Marketing Theory & European Management Review, 3 (1), 60–73.
Practice, 13 (4), 34–45.
Van Trijp, Hans C.M., Wayne D. Hoyer, and J. Jeffrey Inman
Phau, Ian and Paul Chao (2008), “Country-of-Origin: State of (1996), “Why Switch? Product Category-Level Explanations
the Art Review for International Marketing Strategy and Prac- for True Variety-Seeking Behavior,” Journal of Marketing
tice,” International Marketing Review, 25 (4), 349–53. Research, 33 (August), 281–92.

Podsakoff, Philip M., Scott B. MacKenzie, Lee Jeong-Yeon, and Verlegh, Peeter W.J. and Jan-Benedict E.M. Steenkamp (1999),
Nathan P. Podsakoff (2003), “Common Method Biases in “A Review and Meta-Analysis of Country-of-Origin
Behavioral Research: A Critical Review of the Literature and Research,” Journal of Economic Psychology, 20 (5), 521–46.
Recommended Remedies,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 88
(5), 879–903. ———, ———, and Matthew T.G. Meulenberg (2005), “Coun-
try-of-Origin Effects in Consumer Processing of Advertising
Quester, Pascale and Ai Lin Lim (2003), “Product Involve- Claims,” International Journal of Research in Marketing, 22
ment/Brand Loyalty: Is There a Link?” Journal of Product & (2), 127–39.
Brand Management, 12 (1), 22–38.
Völckner, Franziska (2006), “An Empirical Comparison of
Rao, Akshay R. and Kent B. Monroe (1988), “The Moderating Methods for Measuring Consumers’ Willingness to Pay,”
Effect of Prior Knowledge on Cue Utilization in Product Marketing Letters, 17 (2), 137–49.
Evaluations,” Journal of Consumer Research, 15 (2), 253–64.
——— (2008), “The Dual Role of Price: Decomposing Con-
Roehm, Michelle L. and Brian Sternthal (2001), “The Moderat- sumers’ Reactions to Price,” Journal of the Academy of Mar-
ing Effect of Knowledge and Resources on the Persuasive keting Science, 36 (3), 359–77.
Impact of Analogies,” Journal of Consumer Research, 28 (2),
257–72. Walster, Elaine, Ellen Berscheid, and G. William Walster (1973),
“New Directions in Equity Research,” Journal of Personality
Roth, Martin S. and Jean B. Romeo (1992), “Matching Product and Social Psychology, 25 (2), 151–76.
Category and Country Image Perceptions: A Framework for
Managing Country-of-Origin Effects,” Journal of Inter- Wang, Tuo, R. Venkatesh, and Rabikar Chatterjee (2007),
national Business Studies, 23 (3), 477–97. “Reservation Price as a Range: An Incentive-Compatible
Measurement Approach,” Journal of Marketing Research, 44
Samiee, Saeed (2011), “Resolving the Impasse Regarding (May), 200–213.
Research on the Origins of Products and Brands,” Inter-
national Marketing Review, 28 (5), 473–85. Wertenbroch, Klaus and Bernd Skiera (2002), “Measuring Con-
sumers’ Willingness to Pay at the Point of Purchase,” Journal
Simonson, Itamar and Aimee Drolet (2004), “Anchoring Effects of Marketing Research, 39 (May), 228–41.
on Consumers’ Willingness-to-Pay and Willingness-to-
Accept,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (3), 681–90. Wiesmann, Gerrit (2008), “Steiff Teddies Head Home as Out-
sourcing Is Too Much to Bear,” Financial Times, (July 5),
Steenkamp, Jan-Benedict E.M. and Martijn G. de Jong (2010), [available at http://www.ft.com].
“A Global Investigation into the Constellation of Consumer
Attitudes Toward Global and Local Products,” Journal of Wilcox, D. (2005), “Country-of-Origin Bias: A Literature
Marketing, 74 (November), 18–40. Review and Prescription for the Global World,” Develop-
ments in Marketing Science, 28 (3), 87–97.
Sternthal, Brian, Alice M. Tybout, and Bobby J. Calder (1994),
“Experimental Design: Generalization and Theoretical Expla- Zaichkowsky, Judith L. (1994), “Research Notes: The Personal
nation,” in Principles of Marketing Research, Richard P. Involvement Inventory: Reduction, Revision, and Application
Bagozzi, ed. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Publishers, 195–223. to Advertising,” Journal of Advertising, 23 (4), 59–70.

Sun, Baohong and Vicki G. Morwitz (2010), “Stated Intentions Zeithaml, Valarie A. (1988), “Consumer Perceptions of Price,
and Purchase Behavior: A Unified Model,” International Jour- Quality, and Value: A Means-End Model and Synthesis of Evi-
nal of Research in Marketing, 27 (4), 356–66. dence,” Journal of Marketing, 52 (July), 2–22.

Country-of-Origin Effects and Willingness to Pay 41


Copyright of Journal of International Marketing is the property of American Marketing Association and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi