Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 268

D

Dance cultural influences, dance can be conceived of


much more than a performing art, it can be a
Roger J. R. Levesque liberal art, and it could also be an applied art.
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA Dance can be deemed as an important part of
education (see Hanna 2008).
During adolescence, dance can be an impor-
Dance refers to sequences of body movements tant part of physical activity that is promoted
(and lack of them) that are purposeful, intention- through extracurricular activities. As such,
ally rhythmical, and culturally influenced expres- dance can be added to the long list of extracur-
sions that typically are accompanied by music and ricular activities that have been demonstrated to
sometimes by props and costumes. Dance plays result in positive benefits for students, both in the
an important role in adolescence, with some way that it promotes healthy development by
aspects of dance being studied considerably and encouraging positive behaviors and that it pro-
others essentially ignored. Somewhat surpris- tects adolescents from negative outcomes by
ingly, for example, researchers essentially have deterring negative or risky behaviors (for
ignored how the dance that may come to mind reviews, see Eccles et al. 2003). Dance activities
when thinking of dancing, a sort of play and during adolescence provide a context for youth
spontaneous response to music, actually has not to develop a sense of competence, build social
received much attention. Instead, research has skills, and develop a healthy sense of self. Impor-
focused on formal dance, dance as an after-school tantly, though, the process and effects of vying
group activity, dance as a form of therapy, and for entry to extracurricular activities that are
dance as a social event that can bring hazards to competitive and selective can result in negative
youth. outcomes. Although positive effects result for
Dance can be formal, as in actions that are those who are successful, those who are not can
purposeful presentations, such as a performing suffer devastating and extended effects of not
art (e.g., ballet). This aspect of dance is one that, being selected on their personal, social, and
for example, tells stories and emotionally moves school identity (Barnett 2006). Although dance
audiences. In this manner, dance is a mode of can relate to positive physical activity promo-
communication that seeks to share knowledge tion, it also continues to be linked to negative
between dancers and audiences. As expected, an outcomes, especially during adolescence. For
individual’s culture influences their dance mak- example, students who study ballet and some
ing, performing, and viewing. Cultural beliefs and other types of more professional dance are
attitudes help give meaning to dance. Given regarded as being at a high risk for developing
# Springer International Publishing AG 2018
R.J.R. Levesque (ed.), Encyclopedia of Adolescence,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-33228-4
854 Dangerousness

eating disorders (for a review, see Toro et al. References


2009). The risks relate to personality traits asso-
ciated with dancers, such as drive for thinness Barnett, L. A. (2006). Flying high or crashing down: Girls’
accounts of trying out for cheerleading and dance.
and perfectionism (Thomas et al. 2005). Thus, it
Journal of Adolescent Research, 21, 514–541.
may not be the activity itself that contributes to Bogt, T. T., Engels, R., Hibbel, B., Van Wel, F., & Verhagen,
positive or negative outcomes; the effects S. (2002). “Dancestasy”: Dance and MDMA use in
depend on the individuals involved in the activ- Dutch youth culture. Contemporary Drug Problems,
29, 157–181. An Interdisciplinary Quarterly.
ities and the contexts in which dance activities
Chaiklin, S., & Wengrower, H. (Eds.). (2009). The art and
take place. science of dance/movement therapy: Life is dance.
Dance therapy has been developed as a form of New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
art therapy for children, youth (as well as adults) Eccles, J. S., Barber, B. L., Stone, M., & Hunt, J. (2003).
Extracurricular activities and adolescent development.
as it combines music, light exercise, and sensory
Journal of Social Issues, 59, 865–889.
stimulation (see Chaiklin and Wengrower 2009). Hanna, J. L. (2008). A nonverbal language for imagining
Importantly, despite the focus on physical aspects, and learning: Dance education in K-12 curriculum.
there is a clear focus on relating it to emotional Educational Researcher, 37, 491–506.
Hunt, G., Evans, K., & Kares, F. (2007). Drug use and
issues in that dance therapy is viewed as the use of
meanings of risk and pleasure. Journal of Youth Stud-
movement as a process that furthers the emo- ies, 10, 73–96.
tional, cognitive, and physical integration of Jeong, Y., Hong, S., Lee, M., & Park, M. (2005). Dance
individuals. This form of treatment, although movement therapy improves emotional responses and
modulates neurohormonesinadole scents with mild
sometimes criticized for lacking in empirical sup-
depression. International Journal of Neuroscience,
port, increasingly has been the subject of research 115, 1711–1720.
demonstrating effectiveness. For example, dance Thomas, J. J., Keel, P. K., & Heatherton, T. F. (2005).
movement therapy has been shown to be effective Disordered eating attitudes and behaviours in ballet
students: Examination of environmental and individual
in beneficially modulating concentrations of sero-
risk factors. The International Journal of Eating Dis-
tonin and dopamine, and in improving psycholog- orders, 38, 263–268.
ical distress in adolescents with mild depression Toro, J., Guerrero, M., Sentis, J., Castro, J., & Puértolas,
(Jeong et al. 2005). C. (2009). Eating disorders in ballet dancing students:
Problems and risk factors. European Eating Disorders
Going to clubs and dancing, of course, can
Review, 17, 40–49.
have both positive and negative effects. Dance
events have long been associated with risk,
especially with drug and alcohol use (see Hunt
et al. 2007). For example, for some youth,
“house” is a combination of going to parties Dangerousness
and dancing all night while being high on
MDMA, a drug better known as XTC/Ecstasy. Roger J. R. Levesque
Studies have shown that the drugs can be physi- Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
cally and cognitively harmful, but the “dancestasy”
experience may contribute positively to the
development of personal and social identity dur- Dangerousness is a state of being a peril or hazard;
ing adolescence. The dancestasy experience can it also is the ability to inflict harm or injury. The
have temporary negative effects on functioning concept of dangerousness has gained significance
in school or at work, but the effects tend to be in social responses to adolescents. For example, it
short term (see Bogt et al. 2002). Although some was perceptions that some youth were dangerous,
studies do put risks into this perspective, they hard criminals that led to important reforms in the
also do highlight the extent to which dance can juvenile justice system, such as the move toward
be of significance and how it can relate to other being more punitive. Illustrative of that move is
aspects of adolescent development and develop- the transfer to adult courts of those deemed dan-
mental outcomes. gerous. Dangerousness to the community is one of
Date Rape 855

the key factors that courts focus on when deter-


mining whether to transfer youth to adult courts Date Rape
(the other factors are the adolescents’ level of
maturity and level of amenability to treatment) Roger J. R. Levesque
(see Salekin et al. 2002). Dangerousness to one- Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
self (as well as the community) also figures prom-
inently in commitment proceedings to hospitals or
other institutions (see Weithorn 1988). Date rape is the popular term used to describe the
Despite being an important concept, what con- nonconsensual sexual (vaginal, anal, oral) inter- D
stitutes dangerousness remains vague. One of the course that is forced, either by physical force or
most important studies in this regard was psychological coercion, on a person by an
conducted by Salekin and colleagues (see Salekin acquaintance. Estimates reveal that sexual
et al. 2001, 2002). His research team examined assault is most prevalent among adolescents in
how clinical psychologists and, equally impor- contrast to any other age group; 33% of all vic-
tantly, judges make decisions about dangerous- tims fall within the ages of 13–17 (see Young
ness. Most notably, they found that both juvenile et al. 2009). The majority of sexual assault cases
court judges and clinical child psychologists involve acquaintances (Rennison 2002). Regret-
believed that assessments of dangerousness tably, the statistics are not very precise, as com-
should include current and past violent offenses, mentaries often conflate acquaintance sexual
and the extent to which that violence might have assault with date rape. Sexual assault could
been unprovoked; that a focus should be placed on involve many different types of assaults than
the juvenile’s history of the juvenile; and they also would date rape, and that may explain why
placed on whether juveniles exhibited psycho- there is so much variation in estimates reporting
pathic personality traits. The main difference the lifetime prevalence of date rape. Estimates
was that juvenile court judges placed greater range from 13% to 27% among college-age
importance on psychopathic traits when determin- women and from 20% to 68% among adolescents
ing dangerousness while clinicians focused more (Rickert and Wiemann 1998).
on indicated violence, aggression, and reckless Researchers have identified many demo-
and sensation-seeking behavior, in conjunction graphic characteristics that increase vulnerabil-
with psychopathic traits. Studies such as these ity to date rape (which, again, often are studied
are of significance as they seek to guide more in the context of the more general term of sexual
effective ways to determine how to respond to assault) (see Rickert and Wiemann 1998; Vicary
adolescents deemed troubling or troubled. et al. 1995). Those factors include younger age
at first date, early sexual activity, earlier age of
menarche, a past history of sexual abuse or prior
sexual victimization, and being more accepting
References of rape myths and violence toward women.
Salekin, R. T., Rogers, R., & Ustad, K. L. (2001). Juvenile
Other risk factors include date-specific behav-
waiver to adult criminal courts: Prototypes for dangerous- iors such as who initiated, who paid expenses,
ness, sophistication-maturity, and amenability to treat- who drove, date location and activity, as well as
ment. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 381–408. the use of illicit alcohol. These types of findings
Salekin, R. T., Yff, R. M. A., Neumann, C. S., Leistico,
A. R., & Zalot, A. A. (2002). Juvenile transfer to adult
have contributed to a wide variety of efforts
courts: A look at the prototypes for dangerousness, aimed at preventing date rape and other assaults
sophistication-maturity, and amenability to treatment (for a review see Weisz and Black 2009).
through a legal lens. Psychology, Public Policy, and Legally, date rape involves a complex and
Law, 8, 373–410.
Weithorn, L. (1988). Mental hospitalization of troublesome
challenging area of criminal law (see Anderson
youth: An analysis of skyrocketing admission rates. 2005). Jurisdictions tend not to make legal dis-
Stanford Law Review, 40, 773–838. tinctions between date rape and rape that is
856 Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors

committed by strangers. But, the reality is that


date rape is difficult to prosecute (Duncan 2007). Dating Abuse: Prevalence,
The difficulties emerge from the nature of the Consequences, and Predictors
relationship between the victim and the defendant
and those difficulties of proving a lack of consent. May S. Chen1, Vangie A. Foshee2 and
Consent typically is viewed as positive coopera- Heathe H. Luz McNaughton Reyes2
1
tion in an act freely and voluntarily, and with Department of Health Behavior, University
knowledge of the nature of the act. In date rape of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill,
situations, consent may be a challenge to deter- NC, USA
2
mine due to such factors as perceived consent, Gillings School of Global Public Health,
poor communication, diminished capacity, and University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill,
intent. The complex legal issues relating to con- Chapel Hill, NC, USA
sent mirror the same issues that are involved in the
perpetration of many date rape situations, namely,
when and how did someone consent to sexual Overview
activity.
Dating relationships can play a central part in
facilitating healthy adolescent development by
Cross-References providing opportunities for youth to acquire skills
for interacting with others, establish autonomy,
and develop identity and intimacy. However, dat-
▶ Rape
▶ Statutory Rape ing relationships that are violent and abusive can
have harmful effects on psychosocial develop-
ment and adjustment across the life-course
References (Exner-Cortens et al. 2013). Recognizing this,
numerous organizations have declared preventing
Anderson, M. J. (2005). Negotiating sex. Southern Cali- adolescent dating abuse a public health and
fornia Law Review, 78, 1401–1438. human rights imperative (Graffunder et al. 2004;
Duncan, M. J. (2007). Sex crimes and sexual miscues: The Garcia-Moreno and Watts 2011), and research that
need for a clearer line between forcible rape and non-
aims to understand the etiology of dating abuse
consensual sex. Wake Forest Law Review, 42,
1087–1139. has soared over the past 25 years. As shown in
Rennison, C. M. (2002). Rape and sexual assault: Fig. 1, the number of papers on adolescent dating
Reporting to police and medical attention, 1992–2000 abuse published in peer-reviewed journals has
(Bureau of justice report, NCJ 194530). Washington:
grown every year since 1990 to a peak of 77 arti-
Bureau of Justice Statistics, Office of Justice Programs,
U.S. Department of Justice. cles published in 2014. The current entry synthe-
Rickert, V. I., & Wiemann, C. M. (1998). Date rape among sizes results from this growing body of research.
adolescents and young adults. Journal of Pediatric and First, we provide a brief overview of how dating
Adolescent Gynecology, 11, 167–175.
abuse is defined and measured. Then, we describe
Vicary, J., Klingman, L., & Harkness, W. (1995). Risk
factors associated with date rape and sexual assault of the prevalence of dating abuse perpetration and
adolescent girls. Journal of Adolescence, 18, victimization in the USA and how the prevalence
289–306. varies over time and by demographic characteris-
Weisz, A., & Black, B. (2009). Programs to reduce
teen dating violence and sexual assault: Perspec-
tics. Next we summarize the results from longitu-
tives on what works. New York: Columbia Univer- dinal studies that examined the consequences and
sity Press. predictors of dating abuse perpetration and vic-
Young, A. M., Grey, M., & Boyd, C. J. (2009). Adoles- timization. Findings reported are from studies of
cents’ experiences of sexual assault by peers: Preva-
youth ages 12–21, excluding college and adult
lence and nature of victimization occurring within and
outside of school. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, samples. Longitudinal studies that control for
38, 1072–1083. temporality of associations are emphasized since
Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors 857

Published items in each year


90
80
70
60
50
40 D
30
20
10
0

Years

Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors, Fig. 1 Number of articles published on adolescent
dating abuse from 1991 to 2015

that method allows the closest test of causation 1986). Sexual abuse includes physically forcing
available in the adolescent dating abuse literature. someone to engage in a sexual act against his or
The entry ends by highlighting future directions her will, regardless of whether the act is com-
for research. pleted or not, and attempting or completing sexual
acts against a person who is unable to consent to
the sexual act (Saltzman et al. 2002).
What Is Adolescent Dating Abuse These different types of abuse have been found
and How Is It Measured? to commonly co-occur (Hamby et al. 2012; Ozer
et al. 2004; Raiford et al. 2007; Thompson
Dating abuse refers to the use of violence against a et al. 2006; Sears et al. 2007), and psychological
current or former dating partner and includes psy- abuse against partners has been found to predict
chological, physical, and sexual abuse (CDC later use of physical abuse by adolescents (Cano
2016; Saltzman et al. 2002). Psychological abuse et al. 1998; O’Leary and Slep 2003). Further,
can occur in person or electronically (i.e., among adolescents, being a victim of dating
technology-assisted or cyber dating abuse) and abuse is correlated with being a perpetrator of
includes nonphysical acts intended to produce abuse (Gray and Foshee 1997; Miller et al. 2013;
emotional harm or fear, such as those that humil- Orpinas et al. 2013), with the majority of adoles-
iate, diminish, embarrass, and control the victim, cents involved in dating abuse reporting both vic-
isolate the victim from friends and family, deny timization and perpetration (Gray and Foshee
the victim access to money or other basic 1997; Haynie et al. 2013; O’Leary et al. 2008).
resources, produce anxiety or insecurity about Adolescents who perpetrate violence against a
the relationship, and threaten the victim with dating partner once are also likely to perpetrate
harm (Murphy and Hoover 2001; Saltzman again (Cano et al. 1998; O’Leary and Slep 2003;
et al. 2002). Physical abuse includes actual use Williams et al. 2008; White and Smith 2009).
of physical force, such as slapping, kicking, hit- Almost all studies of adolescent dating abuse
ting, punching, and attacking with a weapon, with measure abuse with act scales on which respon-
the intention or perceived intention of causing dents indicate whether and how often they have
physical harm or injury (Straus and Gelles used (or experienced) a specific abusive act
858 Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors

against (or from) a partner (Smith et al. 2015). drawn from five studies that used nationally rep-
Respondents are categorized as perpetrators resentative samples (see Table 1 for summary of
and/or as victims based on whether they have these studies; Halpern et al. 2001; Wolitzky-
done or experienced any of the acts listed, or they Taylor et al. 2008; Hamby et al. 2012; Haynie
are given a numerical score based on the number et al. 2013; Kann et al. 2014). In the National
and/or frequency of acts committed (or experi- Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add
enced). The Conflict Tactics Scale-2 (Straus et al. Health), which assessed both psychological and
1996), the Safe Dates Scale (Foshee 1996), and the physical victimization among students in grades
Conflict in Adolescent Dating Relationships Inven- 7 through 12, 29% and 12% of youth who had
tory (CADRI; Wolfe et al. 1998, 2001) are the most been in heterosexual relationships reported
commonly used act scales, but there are others as experiencing psychological and physical dating
well (see Exner-Cortens et al. 2016 for a review). abuse, respectively, in the prior 18 months. For
Act scales have been criticized for being too sim- those reporting same-sex relationships, the preva-
plistic to capture the complexity of dating abuse lences were 20% and 11% (Halpern et al. 2001,
because they do not assess intent, motives, circum- 2004). The NEXT Generation Health Study also
stances surrounding the abuse, acts perpetrated or examined psychological and physical dating
received in self-defense, or severity of acts (Foshee abuse victimization but, in the past 12 months,
and Matthew 2007). Even so, act scales continue to reported 24.2% and 10.7% of youth experiencing
be widely used because they are easy to administer psychological and physical dating abuse, respec-
and code and they lend themselves well to creating tively. Neither of these studies made distinctions
abuse variables that can be used in quantitative as to whether psychological abuse was experi-
analyses. enced in person or through technology, and no
national studies to date have uniquely examined
the prevalence of technology-assisted psycholog-
How Prevalent Is Adolescent Dating ical dating abuse. However, local studies have
Abuse? reported prevalences of technology-assisted
dating abuse victimization that range from 26%
Prevalence estimates of adolescent dating abuse (Zweig et al. 2013) to 56% (Cutbush et al. 2010).
vary widely, making it difficult to determine the The remaining three national studies measured
true extent of the problem. The variability is due only physical dating abuse. The Youth Risk
to inconsistencies in the way prevalence estimates Behavior Survey (YRBS), which assessed past
are calculated (e.g., among daters only vs. among 12-month physical victimization among 9th
daters and non-daters), the time frame (e.g., past through 12th graders, reported rates of physical
year vs. lifetime) and types of abuse (e.g., physi- victimization that remained fairly stable from
cal, psychological, sexual) assessed, the specific 1999 to 2013, ranging from 8.8% in 1999 to
behaviors of each type measured (e.g., mild, 10.3% in 2013 (Centers for Disease Control and
moderate, and/or severe acts), ages studied, Prevention [CDC] 2000, 2002, 2004, 2006, 2008,
mode of data collection (e.g., telephone vs. self- 2010, 2012, 2014). The National Survey of Ado-
administered), and characteristics of the sample lescents (NSA; Wolitzky-Taylor et al. 2008) and
(e.g., national vs. local; Foshee and Matthew the National Survey of Children’s Exposure to
2007). Here, prevalence data for dating abuse Violence (NatSCEV; Hamby et al. 2012) both
perpetration and victimization are presented for assessed lifetime physical dating abuse victimiza-
each type of dating abuse (psychological, physi- tion among 12–17-year-old adolescents. The for-
cal, and sexual). mer study found that 1.6% of adolescents
experienced serious physical and/or sexual abuse
Victimization Prevalence Rates during their lifetime, whereas the latter study
Data on the prevalence of psychological and phys- reported that 6.4% of adolescents experienced
ical dating abuse victimization in the USA are physical abuse in their lifetime.
Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors 859

Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors, Table 1 Nationally representative estimates of the
prevalence of adolescent dating abuse victimization and perpetration
DV prevalence estimate
Year % Victimization % Perpetration
Study assessed Sample Type of abuse Overall Boys Girls Overall Boys Girls
National 1994–1995 7–12 grades Past 29.0 28.0 29.0 – – –
Longitudinal (N = 7,493) 18 months’
Study of psychological
Adolescent abuse
D
Health (Halpern Past 12.0 12.0 12.0 – – –
et al. 2001) 18 months’
physical abuse
National Survey 2005 12–17 years Lifetime 1.6 0.6 2.7 – – –
of Adolescents (N = 3,614) serious
(Wolitzy-Taylor, physical or
et al. 2008) sexual abuse
National Survey 2008 12–17 years Lifetime 6.4 8.3 4.5 – – –
of Children’s (N = 1,680) physical abuse
Exposure to
Violence
(Hamby
et al. 2012)
NEXT 2009–2010 Tenth grade Past 24.2 16.5 30.5 21.4 13.0 28.2
Generation (N = 2,203) 12 months’
Health Study psychological
(Haynie abuse
et al. 2013) Past 10.7 11.7 9.8 9.1 6.3 11.4
12 months’
physical abuse
Youth Risk 2013 Ninth–twelfth Past 10.3 7.4 13.0 – – –
Behavior Survey grade 12 months’
(Kann (N = 9,900) physical abuse
et al. 2014) Past 10.4 6.2 14.4 – – –
12 months’
sexual abuse

The YRBS is the only national study that has forced sexual activity that range from 9%
assessed sexual dating abuse victimization, which (Foshee 1996) to 15% (Jezl et al. 1996).
was defined as any sexual act that the adolescent
did not want to do including kissing, touching, or Perpetration Prevalence Rates
being physically forced to have sexual inter- Only one national study to date has examined the
course. YRBS assessment of past 12-month sex- prevalence of adolescent dating abuse perpetra-
ual abuse victimization began in 2013 and the tion (Haynie et al. 2013). In the NEXT Generation
reported prevalence for that year was 10.4% Health Study, 21.4% and 11.4% of youth reported
(Centers for Disease Control and Prevention past 12-month perpetration of psychological and
2014). This estimate falls within the range of physical dating abuse, respectively. These preva-
local studies that reported prevalence rates for lence rates are within the range reported by local
date rape or attempted rape between 1% (Ackard studies (Niolon et al. 2015; O’leary et al. 2008;
and Neumark-Sztainer 2002) and 14% (Poitras Foshee and Matthew 2007); however, prevalence
and Lavoie 1995) and with local studies that rates from local studies vary broadly, likely due to
have found prevalence rates for other types of the methodological issues noted above. For
860 Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors

example, a 2007 review of local studies found that interpersonal communication (Furman and
psychological abuse perpetration rates range Shomaker 2008), conflict resolution (Furman
between 14% and 82% and physical abuse rates and Shomaker 2008; Larson et al. 1999), and
range between 11% and 41% (Foshee and Mat- emotional control (Shulman 2003) needed to nav-
thew 2007). When only severe forms of violence igate the complex challenges of initiating and
likely to result in injury (such as hitting a partner maintaining romantic relationships (Moffitt
with an object, beating up a partner, and using a 1993). During late adolescence, these behaviors
knife or gun against a partner) are considered, may decline as teens gain social, intellectual, and
prevalence rates are generally lower ranging emotional maturity (Reyes 2009; Reyes Foshee
between 8% (Coker et al. 2000; Foshee 1996) 2013; White and Smith 2009). For a select group
and 14% (Gorman-Smith et al. 2001). of teens, however, patterns of aggressive behavior
No national studies have examined the preva- toward others may begin in early childhood, gen-
lence of sexual dating abuse perpetration. How- eralize to romantic partners during adolescence,
ever, estimates from local studies suggest that and then persist into adulthood, reflecting a life-
sexual dating abuse is generally less prevalent course persistent versus an adolescent-limited tra-
than other types of perpetration, with between jectory of interpersonal aggression (Reyes and
3% (Foshee 1996) and 15% (Niolon et al. 2015) Foshee 2013; Moffitt 1993). Consistent with the
of teens reporting having perpetrated some form notion that there may be different types of trajec-
of sexual abuse against a date. tories of aggression over time, some recent studies
suggest there may be subgroup heterogeneity in
the development of dating abuse during adoles-
The Initiation and Development cence, although the patterns identified in these
of Dating Abuse Over Time studies are not consistent (Brooks-Russell
et al. 2013; Orpinas et al. 2012, 2013). For exam-
Teens generally begin to date during middle ple, using data that spanned grades 6 through
school (Connolly and McIsaac 2009; Connolly 12, Orpinas et al. (2012) identified two trajectory
et al. 2004), and research suggests that dating classes for physical dating abuse victimization for
abuse occurs even in these early adolescent rela- females: consistently low across all grades and
tionships (Miller et al. 2009; Simon et al. 2010; increasing linearly over time. In contrast, using
Taylor et al. 2010). Longitudinal studies indicate data that spanned grades 8 through 12, Brooks-
that the typical pattern of physical and sexual Russell et al. (2013) found three physical dating
dating abuse perpetration is curvilinear, with abuse trajectory classes for females: a first class
levels of perpetration increasing up until around characterized by consistently low levels of phys-
age 16 or the end of the tenth grade and then ical dating abuse victimization, a second class that
dropping as adolescents’ transition into young started with low levels of victimization that
adulthood (Foshee et al. 2009a; Giordano increased to moderate levels until grade 10 and
et al. 2015; Reyes et al. 2011; Nocentini decreased thereafter, and a third class that higher
et al. 2010; White and Smith 2009). This pattern initial levels of victimization that increased until
is similar to that for other types of antisocial grade 10 and decreased thereafter. In a longitudi-
behavior (e.g., peer aggression, delinquency), nal study focused on dating abuse (perpetration
which also tend to peak during late adolescence and/or victimization) within relationships over
and decline during young adulthood (Farrell time, Johnson et al. (2015b) found that dating
et al. 2005; Windle 2000). These findings are abuse, although prevalent, displays a high degree
also consistent with theoretical perspectives of variability across adolescence and young adult-
suggesting that the peak in antisocial behaviors hood (ages 13–29); across five waves of data,
such as dating abuse during adolescence can be 7.9% of respondents reported always having dat-
attributed to a “maturation gap” during which ing abuse (perpetration or victimization), 39%
teens may not yet have developed the skills in reported never having dating abuse, and 53%
Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors 861

experienced some discontinuity in dating abuse. NatSCEV study found that rates were signifi-
Extant literature parsing apart these developmen- cantly higher for males than for females, although
tal processes is limited, however, and more work rates of fear and injury due to dating abuse were
is needed to understand whether there are different found to be higher for females than for males in
types of trajectories of dating aggression and, if that study. Findings from the 1991 to 2011 YRBS
so, identify the factors that increase risk of chronic suggest that rates of physical victimization did not
involvement across the life-course. differ for boys and girls; however, the 2013 YRBS
found higher rates among girls than boys (Vagi
et al. 2015). Vagi et al. (2015) speculate that this D
Demographic Correlates finding may be attributed to changes in the word-
ing of the 2013 YRBS physical dating abuse
A large body of research suggests that adolescent victimization measure that aimed to minimize
health risk behaviors are not equally distributed reporting of unintentional violence (i.e., violence
across subgroups of adolescents defined by demo- perpetrated in play) and capture more severe
graphic characteristics such as sex, race/ethnicity, forms of physical abuse.
and socioeconomic status (SES). These demo- The only national study to examine sex differ-
graphic characteristics may influence and/or be ences in dating abuse perpetration rates found that
markers for adolescent exposure to social contexts girls were more likely than boys to perpetrate both
(e.g., the family environment) and socialization psychological and physical dating abuse (Haynie
processes (e.g., gender role socialization, cultural et al. 2013). This finding is consistent with local
socialization) that can increase and/or decrease studies that have generally found that female ado-
risk of involvement in dating abuse (Foshee lescents are as or more likely than males to report
et al. 2008). Below we summarize findings from these types of dating abuse perpetration (O’Leary
research that has examined whether and how ado- et al. 2008; Niolon et al. 2015; for a review see
lescent sex, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic sta- Foshee and Mathew 2007). In contrast, local stud-
tus are associated with dating abuse victimization ies have consistently found that sexual dating
and perpetration. Such research is critical to abuse is more likely to be reported by males than
informing our understanding of who is at risk for females (Niolon et al. 2015; Foshee 1996; Bennett
dating abuse and thus should be targeted with and Finneran; Poitras and Lavoie 1995).
selective prevention interventions. We note that studies that have found that girls
are as or more likely than boys to engage in and/or
Sex Differences experience dating abuse have caused considerable
Both the NSA and the YRBS suggest that girls are controversy, primarily because they have not
more likely than boys to be victims of sexual matched normative public perceptions, the expe-
dating abuse (Hamby et al. 2012; Wolitzky-Taylor riences of service providers interacting with those
et al. 2008; CDC 2014). In contrast, findings of involved in intimate partner violence, or the fact
national studies with respect to sex differences in that domestic violence homicides are overwhelm-
risk for psychological and physical dating abuse ingly male perpetrated. It has been suggested that
victimization are inconsistent (see Table 1). Rates sex differences in physical dating abuse are more
of psychological dating abuse victimization were likely to be detected when severe forms of dating
not found to differ by sex in the Add Health study, abuse are considered, with female adolescents
but rates were higher for girls than for boys in the experiencing and male adolescents using more
NEXT Generation Study. In the NSA, which severe forms of abuse. The findings of the NSA
assessed lifetime serious physical abuse victimi- study and the 2013 YRBS are consistent with this
zation, rates of victimization were higher for girls assertion; both studies assessed severe forms of
than for boys. In contrast, the Add Health and dating abuse victimization and found higher rates
NEXT Generation studies found no sex differ- among females than among males. In addition,
ences in rates of physical victimization, and the some local studies with adolescents have found
862 Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors

that females are more likely than males to be more evident in those contexts. This finding is
victims of severe physical dating abuse (Bennett consistent with findings of research examining
and Fineran 1998; Coker et al. 2000; Foshee intimate partner violence (IPV) among adult
et al. 2009a). However, we note that other local women that has found that the association
studies report no gender differences in victimiza- between education (a marker of SES) and victim-
tion from severe physical dating abuse (e.g., Jezl ization risk depends on macro-level factors (e.g.,
et al. 1996; Lichter and McCloskey 2004; community-level tolerance of violence; Heise and
O’Keefe and Treister 1998) or that boys are Kotsadam 2015).
more likely than girls to be victims of severe Findings from studies assessing the link
dating abuse (Munoz-Rivas et al. 2007). The between SES and dating abuse perpetration are
NatSCEV study as well as some local studies more consistent, with most studies reporting no
(e.g., Foshee 1996; Munoz-Rivas et al. 2007) association (Chapple 2003; Foshee et al. 2001;
found that girls were more likely than boys to be Lavoie et al. 2002; Malik et al. 1997; O’Keefe
injured by a date, but O’Leary et al. (2008) found 1997; Simons et al. 1998). However, a study by
no gender differences in injuries from dating Foshee et al. (2009a) found that parent education,
abuse. These inconsistencies point to the need a proxy for SES (Goodman 1999), was negatively
for more nuanced measurement and analytic tech- associated with levels of psychological and phys-
niques to better understand sex differences and ical perpetration across ages 13–19 years.
similarities in adolescent dating abuse (Hamby
2015). Racial and Ethnic Differences
Overall, findings from studies assessing associa-
Socioeconomic Status Differences tions between race/ethnicity and dating abuse are
Studies assessing the association between socio- mixed and may vary depending on sex, type of
economic status (SES) and adolescent dating abuse assessed (i.e., psychological, physical, or
abuse victimization have been inconsistent, with sexual; victimization or perpetration), and
no clear pattern emerging. For example, higher whether the study controlled for potential con-
SES has been associated with both reduced founders such as SES. Findings from the 2013
(Foster et al. 2004; Raiford et al. 2007; Sanderson YRBS, which did not control for SES, found no
et al. 2004; Spriggs et al. 2009) and increased differences by race/ethnicity in risk of physical
(Malik et al. 1997) risk of dating abuse victimiza- dating abuse victimization for either boys or
tion among girls. Among boys, some studies have girls, but there were racial/ethnic differences in
found no association between SES and victimiza- risk for sexual dating abuse, with different pat-
tion risk (Malik et al. 1997; Spriggs et al. 2009), terns for girls as compared to boys (CDC 2014).
whereas others have found that SES is associated Specifically, sexual dating abuse victimization
with reduced risk (Halpern et al. 2001). These was found to be higher among White and His-
inconsistencies may be explained by differences panic girls than among Black girls, whereas,
in sample characteristics and in the variables among boys, sexual dating abuse victimization
included in analyses. For example, Spriggs was higher among Black than among White and
et al. (2009) found that SES was negatively Hispanic boys. In studies using data from Add
related to dating abuse victimization among both Health, race/ethnicity was more strongly related
boys and girls, but when age and race/ethnicity to physical dating abuse victimization for boys
were included in the model, the association than for girls, with minority boys, particularly
became nonsignificant for boys. This study also Black (Halpern et al. 2001; Spriggs et al. 2009)
found that the effects of SES on victimization and Hispanic (Halpern et al. 2009; Spriggs
were context dependent; SES was more strongly et al. 2009) boys at greater risk than White boys.
related to girls’ victimization in more advantaged However, other local studies have found that race/
schools, perhaps because their status differential is ethnicity was not associated with physical
Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors 863

victimization among boys either when controlling abuse in adolescence significantly predicted IPV
(Foshee et al. 2004) or not controlling for SES victimization among young adults into their
(O’Leary et al. 2008; Silverman et al. 2001). mid-20s to mid-30s. In addition, existing retro-
The only nationally representative study that spective studies indicate that approximately 51%
examined dating abuse perpetration did not report of the abused women in a shelter sample (Roscoe
differences by race/ethnicity (Haynie et al. 2013). and Benaske 1985) and 25% of abused women in
However, some local studies suggest that Black another sample indicated that they had been a
and Latino adolescents may be at increased risk victim of violence while dating (Gayford 1975).
for physical dating abuse perpetration than White Several studies, primarily with girls, have D
adolescents even when controlling for SES examined health-related correlates of adolescent
(Connolly et al. 2010; Foshee et al. 2009a, dating abuse victimization. These studies report
2010). Findings from studies that stratify by gen- that victimization is correlated with physical
der have been mixed, with some studies reporting injury, unhealthy weight-control methods, sub-
stronger associations between race/ethnicity and stance use, early sexual onset, multiple sexual
dating abuse perpetration for boys than for girls partners, nonuse of condoms, and pregnancy
(Malik et al. 1997; O’Leary et al. 2008), and (Howard and Wang 2003; Kreiter et al. 1999;
others finding stronger associations for girls O’Leary et al. 2008; Roberts et al. 2005;
(Foshee et al. 2010). Silverman et al. 2001). Because of the cross-
sectional nature of these studies, it is not clear
whether these are causes or consequences of dat-
What Are the Consequences ing abuse victimization. However, the use of lon-
of Adolescent Dating Abuse? gitudinal designs to examine psychological and
behavioral consequences of adolescent dating
Understanding the consequences of adolescent abuse victimization is becoming more common.
dating abuse is essential for accurately estimating These studies suggest that consequences of dating
the extent of the public health problem. In addi- abuse victimization for boys include increased
tion, understanding the consequences can help suicidal ideation, antisocial behavior, cigarette
researchers and practitioners set priorities for the smoking, depressive symptoms, anxiety, and
field and develop appropriate primary and second- alcohol and marijuana use (Ackard et al. 2007;
ary prevention programs for youth. It is often Brown et al. 2009; Ellis et al. 2009; Exner-Cortens
presumed that being a victim of dating abuse et al. 2013; Foshee et al. 2013a; Roberts
during adolescence increases the risk of becoming et al. 2003). The consequences for girls include
a victim of intimate partner violence (IPV) as an depression, anxiety, cigarette smoking, suicide
adult, continuing the cycle of family violence. attempts, marijuana use, illicit substance abuse,
Consistent with this perspective, a number of lon- binge eating, and antisocial behavior. An impor-
gitudinal studies suggest that being victimized by tant limitation is that only two of these longitudi-
romantic partners in adolescence increases risk for nal studies distinguished consequences of dating
IPV victimization in young adulthood (Halpern abuse victimization separately by abuse type
et al. 2009; Exner-Cortens et al. 2013; Cui (Exner-Cortens et al. 2013; Foshee et al. 2013a).
et al. 2013; Gomez 2010). Using data from Add The remaining studies combined different types of
Health, Halpern et al. (2009) found that, although violence into a single composite score, making it
the most common pattern of IPV victimization impossible to delineate consequences of different
was young adult onset, a significant proportion types of dating abuse. Future studies should
of the population showed persistent victimization examine consequences of each type of dating
from adolescence to young adulthood (age abuse victimization separately.
18–25). Expanding on these findings, Cui To date, only four longitudinal studies have
et al. (2013) found that experiencing dating examined the consequences of being a perpetrator
864 Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors

of dating abuse (Calvete et al. 2014; Foshee peers, direct bullying) have been shown to con-
et al. 2016; Johnson et al. 2014; Nahapetyan sistently predict dating abuse perpetration among
et al. 2014). These studies indicate that being a both boys (Cleveland et al. 2003; Ferguson
perpetrator of adolescent dating abuse predicts et al. 2012; Lavoie et al. 2002; Foshee
negative changes in cognitive-emotional pro- et al. 2010, 2014; Morris et al. 2015; Reyes and
cessing of dating conflicts, lower college aspira- Foshee 2013) and girls (Brendgen et al. 2001;
tions, higher family conflict, greater likelihood of Foshee et al. 2010, 2014; Morris et al. 2015).
marijuana use, higher depressive symptoms, and For example, Foshee et al. (2014) found that direct
greater suicide ideation (Calvete et al. 2014; bullying (i.e., hitting, slapping, or picking on
Foshee et al. 2016; Johnson et al. 2014; another kid) in the sixth grade predicted the
Nahapetyan et al. 2014). onset of physical dating abuse perpetration by
the eighth grade among both boys and girls.
Importantly, the analyses were conducted in a
What Are the Predictors of Adolescent sample of adolescents who had not perpetrated
Dating Abuse? dating abuse at the sixth grade assessment,
strengthening the case for temporality of
Knowing the “causes” of a behavior is a prereq- associations.
uisite to developing approaches to prevent that Acceptance of dating abuse and traditional
behavior (Gordon 1987). Below we provide a gender norms. A small body of longitudinal
narrative summary of predictors for dating abuse research has examined whether beliefs about the
perpetration and victimization. Longitudinal acceptability of violence (Connolly et al. 2010;
designs provide better evidence of causation Foshee et al. 2001; Mueller et al. 2013; Wolfe
than cross-sectional designs; therefore, only lon- et al. 2004; Reyes et al. 2016) and/or traditional
gitudinal studies of adolescent dating abuse are gender roles (Foshee et al. 2001; Reyes
presented below. In addition, only studies that et al. 2016) predict dating abuse, with inconsistent
measure dating abuse perpetration and/or victim- findings. In particular, while some studies have
ization during adolescence were included. Our found a longitudinal association between beliefs
description of the predictors identified in longitu- that are tolerant of dating abuse and abuse perpe-
dinal studies is organized by levels of the social- tration (Connolly et al. 2010; Foshee et al. 2001;
ecological model, beginning with individual-level Jouriles et al. 2013; Reyes et al. 2016), others have
factors (i.e., cognitions), interpersonal factors found no such association (Wolfe et al. 2004;
(i.e., characteristics of the romantic relationship, Mueller et al. 2013). Mueller et al. (2013)
peers, or the family), and then attributes of the conducted the only study that has explicitly exam-
school and neighborhood context. Figure 2 sum- ined the potential for reciprocal relations between
marizes the list of predictors discussed below and attitudes toward violence and abusive behavior.
whether they are associated with perpetration, Using a sample of teens with a history of
victimization, or both. All factors included in the delinquency-related behavior, that study found
figure were found to be statistically significant evidence for a prospective relationship from dat-
predictors of dating abuse at the 0.05 level in at ing aggression to beliefs among boys and girls,
least one longitudinal study. but not vice versa (Mueller et al. 2013). Foshee
et al. (2001) similarly found no evidence that
Predictors of Dating Abuse Perpetration traditional gender role beliefs predict dating
abuse among boys or girls. However, findings
Individual-Level Predictors of Perpetration from a study by Reyes et al. (2016) suggests that
Aggressive and antisocial behaviors. Aggressive this lack of association may have been due to a
and antisocial behaviors (e.g., disobedience failure to examine synergistic effects between
toward parents, damaging of property, delinquent beliefs about traditional gender roles and beliefs
behavior, conduct problems, aggression toward about the acceptability of violence. Using a
Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors 865

Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors, Fig. 2 Longitudinal predictors of dating abuse perpe-
tration (p) and victimization (v)

general sample of adolescent boys, that study abuse perpetration (e.g., Epstein-Ngo et al. 2013;
found that traditional gender role attitudes pre- Foshee et al. 2010; Rothman et al. 2011; Reyes
dicted dating abuse among boys who reported et al. 2014; Temple et al. 2013), although findings
high, but not low, levels of acceptance of dating are difficult to synthesize and compare due to dif-
abuse (Reyes et al. 2016). Additional work is ferences in the types of substance use behaviors
needed to better understand whether and how examined as well as differences in analytic
various norms, attitudes, and beliefs about gender approach. Foshee et al. (2010) found that mari-
and acceptability of violence may work synergis- juana use predicted the onset of dating abuse
tically to predict dating abuse. (DA) among girls, but neither cigarette nor alcohol
Substance use. Several longitudinal studies use predicted DA onset among either boys or girls.
suggest that substance use behavior (alcohol, mar- Temple et al. (2013) found that both hard drug and
ijuana, and/or other drug uses) predicts dating alcohol use uniquely predicted change in DA
866 Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors

(i.e., levels of DA at follow-up controlling for partner violence has been studied extensively,
baseline) among boys and girls, but marijuana use but only a few longitudinal studies have examined
did not. Longitudinal research that has examined the influence of those characteristics on adoles-
within-person associations between substance cent dating abuse. In a longitudinal study span-
use and DA across more than two time points has ning adolescence and young adulthood, Johnson
also found evidence that alcohol (Epstein-Ngo et al. (2015b) found that infidelity, disagreements,
et al. 2013; Rothman et al. 2011; Reyes et al. and partner continuity were associated with an
2014) and hard drug use (boys only; Reyes et al. increase in the proportion of relationships with
2014), but not marijuana (Epstein-Ngo et al. 2013; dating abuse (victimization and/or perpetration),
Reyes et al. 2014) or cigarette use (Reyes and increased trust and commitment were associ-
et al. 2014), predicted dating abuse perpetration. ated with a decrease in the proportion of relation-
In sum, these findings suggest a consistent within- ships with dating abuse. Connolly et al. (2010)
person link between alcohol use and dating abuse found that adolescent relationships characterized
perpetration, although more work is needed to by high levels of hostility predicted physical dat-
understand associations with other substance use ing abuse 1 year later for both boys and girls,
behaviors. Future longitudinal research should dis- although these findings again did not differentiate
tinguish between- from within-person effects, elab- between perpetration and victimization. Williams
orate on the potential biopsychosocial mechanisms et al. (2008) studied adolescent dating abuse per-
that explain effects, and determine whether and petrators to identify predictors of the recurrence of
how mechanisms differ for boys and girls. dating abuse with other partners a year later. They
Internalizing symptoms. Internalizing symp- found that boys and girls who were perpetrators in
toms, including depression and anxiety, have relationships that involved frequent quarrels, argu-
been found to predict dating abuse perpetration, ments, and hostility at the first assessment were
with some indication that the effect is stronger for more likely than perpetrators whose relationships
girls than for boys. For example, McCloskey and did not contain those characteristics to perpetrate
Lichter (2003) found that depression was a stron- dating abuse a year later with a different partner;
ger predictor of dating abuse perpetration for girls contrary to their expectations, the association was
than for boys 1 year later. However, dating abuse significant only among those who reported low
was not measured at baseline in this study so the acceptance of aggression in dating relationships.
temporality of the association was not completely Engaging in sexual intercourse may increase the
controlled for. Foshee et al. (2010) also found that emotional intensity of the romantic relationship,
depression predicted dating abuse perpetration which in turn might increase the risk for dating
among girls but not boys, but in a different sam- abuse perpetration (Brooks-Russell et al. 2015).
ple, Foshee et al. (2001) found that depression did Consistent with this hypothesis, having higher
not predict dating abuse perpetration by girls or number of sex partners (Cleveland et al. 2003)
boys. In addition, several studies suggest that and early sexual initiation (O’Donnell et al. 2006)
trauma symptoms (Wolfe et al. 2004), suicide have been found to significantly predict dating
attempt (Kerr and Capaldi 2011), and anxiety abuse perpetration by boys.
(Foshee et al. 2010; Ulloa et al. 2014) may also Peer context. A limited number of studies have
be predictors of dating abuse perpetration. In sum, examined whether and how the peer context influ-
these findings suggest that internalizing symp- ences risk for dating abuse. This is an important
toms predict dating abuse perpetration, although area for research given that social learning per-
more work is needed to explain subgroup differ- spectives suggest that peers may play an impor-
ences by demographic characteristics. tant role in modeling dating behaviors and
shaping normative beliefs about the acceptability
Interpersonal-Level Predictors of Perpetration of dating abuse. Consistent with this perspective,
Romantic partners. The influence of romantic existing longitudinal studies have found that ado-
relationship characteristics on adult intimate lescents with friends who engage in dating abuse
Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors 867

are more likely to perpetrate dating abuse them- longitudinal study (O’Donnell et al. 2006) found
selves (Arriaga and Foshee 2004; Foshee that experiencing abuse from parents predicted
et al. 2010, 2013b). Because these longitudinal perpetration of dating abuse by boys, but others
studies controlled for the temporality of associa- have found that corporal punishment and child
tions, it can be concluded that the adolescents maltreatment predicted perpetration of dating
perpetrated dating abuse after rather than before abuse only in subgroups of boys. For example,
their friend had been a perpetrator. Similarly, Foshee et al. (2005) found that corporal punish-
peers may model and reinforce general antisocial ment predicted dating abuse perpetration for
behaviors, which in turn may increase dating Black boys whose mothers had not graduated D
abuse perpetration. Several longitudinal studies from high school, but not for those whose mothers
have found positive associations between affilia- had graduated from high school or for White
tion with deviant peers and psychological and adolescents regardless of mother education.
physical dating abuse, with some studies finding Exposure to domestic violence in the home is
a stronger association for boys than for girls commonly thought to be a risk factor for adoles-
(Morris et al. 2015), and others finding stronger cent dating abuse, but empirical evidence for this
associations for girls than for boy (Schnurr and is mixed. O’Donnell et al. (2006) found that
Lohman 2008). In addition, having lower quality witnessing violence between parents predicted
and unreciprocated friendships have been found physical dating abuse perpetration by boys, and
to predict dating abuse perpetration among both Tschann et al. (2009) found longitudinal evidence
boys and girls (Foshee et al. 2010; Linder and of an association for both genders. Likewise,
Collins 2005), and girls who are typically high Latzman et al. (2015) found that exposure to
in social status are at increased risk for using IPV was associated with higher levels of dating
dating abuse throughout adolescence (Foshee abuse perpetration during a 5-month follow-up
et al. 2010). assessment among a sample of adolescents
Family-related factors. Family characteristics living in high-risk, urban communities. However,
are among the most commonly studied predictors several other longitudinal studies that examined
of adolescent dating abuse. Low levels of parental the effects of exposure to domestic violence on
monitoring and supervision have consistently adolescent dating abuse perpetration found
been found to predict dating abuse perpetration no association (Arriaga and Foshee 2004; Capaldi
(Brendgen et al. 2001; Capaldi and Clark 1998; and Clark 1998; Foshee et al. 2001; Lavoie
Foshee et al. 2001; Lavoie et al. 2002; Simons et al. 2002; Lichter and McCloskey 2004;
et al. 1998), and several studies suggest that the McCloskey and Lichter 2003; Simons
association between parental lack of supervision et al. 1998). This lack of association may be due
and dating abuse perpetration is mediated by to unexamined moderated associations effects: for
increased antisocial and delinquent behaviors example, Foshee et al. (2005) found that exposure
(Capaldi and Clark 1998; Lavoie et al. 2002; to domestic violence predicted adolescent dating
Simons et al. 1998). Exposure to harsh parenting abuse perpetration by Black boys living in single-
practices (Jouriles et al. 2012; Lavoie et al. 2002; parent households, but not by Black boys living in
Simons et al. 1998), inconsistent discipline (Bank two-parent households or White boys living in
and Burraston 2001; Simons et al. 1998), and low either situation. Differences in when the exposure
levels of parental warmth (Magdol et al. 1998; occurred and the frequency or level of severity of
Simons et al. 1998) have also been found to pre- the exposure may also explain the lack of consis-
dict dating abuse perpetration. In a longitudinal tent findings.
analysis of eighth and ninth grade adolescents,
Reyes et al. (2015) found that being hit by an School and Neighborhood Predictors
adult increased normative beliefs about dating of Perpetration
aggression and anger dysregulation, which in Factors at the upper levels of the social ecology
turn increased dating abuse perpetration. Another such as policy, media, neighborhoods, and
868 Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors

schools may influence dating abuse perpetration, better understanding of how these characteristics
but only those at the school and neighborhood might influence dating abuse.
levels have been examined with longitudinal stud-
ies, with some mixed findings. For example, Factors Not Predictive of Perpetration by
Foshee et al. (2011) found that school bonding Boys or Girls
was associated with lower levels of dating abuse Several factors have not predicted dating abuse
perpetration among boys but not girls, but Cleve- perpetration for boys or girls. Although self-
land et al. (2003) found that school attachment is esteem is considered an important factor in ado-
associated with lower dating abuse perpetration lescent behavior, the two longitudinal studies that
for girls but not boys. Another recent study of examine it as a predictor of dating abuse perpetra-
class norms (i.e., students in the same school and tion found no associations for boys or girls
grade level) found that, among both boys and (Foshee et al. 2001; Manning et al. 2014). Few
girls, class-level norms that are more supportive longitudinal studies have examined whether per-
of dating abuse predicted higher levels of physical sonal interaction skills such as skills for
dating abuse perpetration (Taylor et al. 2015). responding to anger and settling disagreements,
Only two studies to date examine longitudinal ability to empathize with dating partners, and/or
associations between neighborhood-level factors self-efficacy in coping with relationship issues
and adolescent dating abuse perpetration. Jain predict dating abuse perpetration; the few studies
et al. (2010) used longitudinal data to explore that have examined interaction skills as longitudi-
the role of neighborhood collective efficacy nal predictors of dating abuse perpetration did not
(defined as a community’s cohesiveness and will- find significant associations (Foshee et al. 2001;
ingness to intervene for the common good) on Wolfe et al. 2004), yet changing interpersonal
dating abuse perpetration, but found no significant skills are primary foci for many dating abuse
effects for either boys or girls. Chang et al. (2015) prevention programs (De La Rue et al. 2016).
examined the effect of structural (economic
disadvantage, residential instability, ethnic hetero- Predictors of Dating Abuse Victimization
geneity) and physical (physical disorder) charac-
teristics of the neighborhood on trajectories of Individual-Level Predictors of Victimization
dating abuse perpetration, and found that girls Aggressive and antisocial behaviors. Many of the
but not boys living in economically disadvan- same factors that predicted dating abuse perpetra-
taged neighborhoods were at increased risk of tion also predict victimization. Early antisocial
perpetrating dating abuse at all grades, even behaviors, aggression against peers, and conduct
when controlling for individual-level socioeco- problems have all predicted the onset of dating
nomic status. The authors also found that neigh- abuse victimization among boys (Magdol
borhood collective efficacy was protective against et al. 1998; Miller et al. 2013; O’Donnell
dating abuse in bivariate analyses, but the effect et al. 2006) and girls (Foster et al. 2004; Magdol
was no longer significant after controlling for et al. 1998; O’Donnell et al. 2006; Williams
individual- and neighborhood-level covariates. et al. 2008). Morris et al. (2015) found that early
More research examining neighborhood-level adolescent involvement in aggression predicted
predictors of dating abuse perpetration is currently subsequent psychological and physical dating
needed, especially studies that utilize longitudinal abuse victimization more strongly for females
designs and assess potential mediators of the than males, but the magnitude of the gender dif-
effect of these macro-level factors. In addition, ference was small.
consistent measures of neighborhood-level fac- Acceptance of dating abuse and traditional
tors are needed to facilitate comparison of find- gender norms. With respect to attitudes and
ings across studies (Johnson et al. 2015a). More norms, Foshee et al. (2004) found that holding
nuanced ways of conceptualizing and measuring traditional gender stereotypes predicted chronic
neighborhood-level factors are needed to obtain a victimization from sexual dating abuse for girls
Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors 869

but not boys. Raiford et al. (2007) found that increased risk of dating abuse victimization
among African American girls, knowledge and (Kaestle and Halpern 2005).
attitudes about healthy and unhealthy relation- Peer context. With respect to peers, having
ships, measured primarily through items tapping friends who were victims of dating abuse predicted
unhealthy gender-based expectations of partners, dating abuse victimization among both boys and
predicted female victimization. Together, these girls (Foshee et al. 2004). Having more deviant
findings suggest that girls who hold traditional friends has also been found to predict victimization
views of women may be at increased risk of dating in longitudinal studies (East and Hokoda 2015).
abuse victimization. Sexual harassment by peers may also be a risk D
Substance use. Early alcohol use has been factor for dating abuse victimization: Chiodo
associated with later dating abuse victimization et al. (2009) found that boys and girls who were
among both boys and girls (Cleveland sexually harassed in the ninth grade were signifi-
et al. 2003; East and Hokoda 2015; Millett cantly more likely to be a victim of dating abuse
et al. 2013; Swahn et al. 2008), whereas cocaine 2.5 years later than those who were not sexually
use and sedative/opiate use were associated with harassed. The forms of sexual harassment that pre-
severe dating victimization among girls only dicted victimization were similar for boys and girls
(Epstein-Ngo et al. 2013). except that girls received fewer homosexual slurs
Internalizing symptoms. Similar to perpetration, and are more likely to experience comments about
depression has been found to predict both the onset or be rated on the sexual parts of their bodies.
of dating abuse victimization of girls (Cleveland Family-related factors. Experiencing violence
et al. 2003; Foshee et al. 2004; Lehrer et al. 2006; from parents (Foshee et al. 2004; O’Donnell
Tyler et al. 2011) and chronic victimization of girls et al. 2006), family conflict (Magdol et al. 1998),
from sexual dating abuse (Foshee et al. 2004). and lack of parental warmth (Magdol et al. 1998)
Using longitudinal data that spanned from grades have been found to predict dating abuse victimi-
8 through 12, Brooks-Russell et al. (2013) found zation among both boys and girls. Witnessing
that anxiety increased the odds of experiencing violence between parents has been found to pre-
physical dating violence victimization throughout dict male dating abuse victimization (O’Donnell
adolescence among girls but not boys. et al. 2006), but Lichter and McCloskey (2004)
found that exposure to marital violence did not
Interpersonal-Level Predictors of Victimization predict abuse among boys or girls. Finally, higher
Romantic partners. Among both boys and girls, levels of bonding with parents (Maas et al. 2010)
being a victim in a relationship containing many and mothers’ strictness, monitoring, and conser-
quarrels, arguments, and hostility predicted vic- vative sexual attitudes (East and Hokoda 2015)
timization in a later relationship by a different have been found to be protective against dating
partner, but only among those who reported low abuse victimization although the latter study
acceptance of aggression in dating relationships found stronger associations for girls than boys.
(Williams et al. 2008). Likewise, Johnson A recent longitudinal study found that low paren-
et al. (2015b) found that infidelity, disagreements, tal closeness was associated with greater risk of
and partner continuity were associated with an dating abuse victimization in residentially stable
increase in the proportion of relationships with neighborhoods but in not unstable neighbor-
dating abuse (victimization and/or perpetration). hoods, pointing to the need for examining
Increased trust and commitment were associated potential synergistic effects of family and
with a decrease in the proportion of relationships neighborhood-level factors (Foshee et al. 2015).
with dating abuse. Among boys only, early sexual
initiation has been associated with greater risk for School- and Neighborhood-Level Predictors
physical dating abuse victimization (O’Donnell of Victimization
et al. 2006). Among both boys and girls, being Like perpetration, factors at the upper level of the
in a relationship that includes sexual intercourse social ecology likely predict dating abuse
870 Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors

victimization, but few studies have examined harsh parenting, and lack of parental warmth; and
these effects longitudinally. Two studies to date witnessing of violence between parents. Some
examine neighborhood-level predictors of victim- school- and neighborhood-level factors have
ization. Using data from Add Health, Spriggs been found to predict dating abuse perpetration
et al. (2009) found that witnessing a violent and victimization, though research examining
crime (someone getting stabbed or shot) in the these associations is still nascent.
past 12 months predicted dating abuse victimiza- Despite the increased attention to adolescent
tion among adolescents. The other study to dating abuse in recent years, far less research has
explore neighborhood-level factors found that been conducted in this area than on other adoles-
higher levels of neighborhood collective efficacy cent health risk behaviors. For example, com-
were protective against dating abuse victimization pared to the 77 studies of dating abuse published
among boys but not among girls (Jain et al. 2010). in 2014, 1,276 peer-reviewed articles were
One study found that watching X-rated movies in published on adolescent alcohol use and
the past 3 months predicted dating violence vic- 492 were published on adolescent tobacco use.
timization 1 year later among girls (Raiford In addition, as noted above, to date, only one
et al. 2007). Additional longitudinal work is nationally representative study of adolescent dat-
needed to examine whether and how policy, ing abuse perpetration has been conducted. Future
media, neighborhood, and school-level factors research is needed to develop measures of dating
effect dating abuse victimization. abuse that capture the complexity of the phenom-
enon while still lending themselves to quantitative
analysis. In addition, while longitudinal research
Conclusions and Future Directions examining the etiology of dating abuse is grow-
ing, more work is needed to identify both individ-
Dating abuse during adolescence is a prevalent ual and, in particular, social contextual risk and
public health problem that has serious emotional, protective factors that predict victimization and/or
developmental, and physical consequences and perpetration. Such research should seek to deter-
can be predicted by a number of factors that mine whether and how associations between risk
could be targeted for change in prevention pro- and protective factors vary by sex, race, SES, age,
grams. Despite the broad range of prevalence and sexual orientation so as to contribute to a
estimates, it is clear that a substantial number of better understanding of who is at risk and when
adolescent boys and girls become involved in intervening on these factors would be most effec-
dating abuse and that most are both victims and tive. In particular, more research is needed to
perpetrators. Even the most conservative esti- assess the prevalence and predictors of dating
mates suggest that at least one in ten US teens abuse perpetration by gay and lesbian adolescents
has experienced some form of physical dating since all of the studies reviewed here assumed
abuse and one-third have experienced psycholog- heterosexual relationships. Finally, emerging
ical abuse. Boys and girls who experience dating gene by environment studies suggest that genes
abuse, either as a victim or as a perpetrator, face a may play an important role in the etiology of
range of serious consequences, including physical adolescent dating abuse (Foshee et al. 2015) and
injury, depression, substance use, binge drinking, should be explored further in future studies.
antisocial behavior, and suicide ideation and
attempts. Factors predicting dating abuse perpe-
tration and victimization for both boys and girls References
include early antisocial and delinquent behaviors
and aggression against peers; having friends who Ackard, D. M., Eisenberg, M. E., & Neumark-Sztainer, D.
(2007). Long-term impact of adolescent dating vio-
are involved in dating abuse; relationships that
lence on the behavioral and psychological health of
involve quarrels, arguments, and hostility; family male and female youth. The Journal of Pediatrics,
conflict; the experience of violence from parents, 151(5), 476–481.
Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors 871

Ackard, D. M., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2002). Date Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2008). Sur-
violence and date rape among adolescents: Associa- veillance summaries. MMWR Morbidity Mortal Weekly
tions with disordered eating behaviors and psycholog- Report, 57(SS04), 1–131.
ical health. Child Abuse & Neglect, 26, 455–473. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2010). Youth
Arriaga, X. B., & Foshee, V. A. (2004). Adolescent dating risk behavior surveillance-United States, 2009. Mor-
violence: Do adolescents follow their friends’ or their bidity and mortality weekly report. Surveillance Sum-
parents’ footsteps? Journal of Interpersonal Violence, maries (Washington, DC: 2002), 59(5), 1–142.
19, 162–184. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2012). Youth
Bank, L., & Burraston, B. (2001). Abusive home environ- risk behavior surveillance: United States, 2011. Mor-
ments as predictors of poor adjustment during adoles- bidity and Mortality Weekly Report. Surveillance Sum-
cence and early childhood. Journal of Community maries, 61(4), 1–162. D
Psychology, 29, 195–217. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2014). Youth
Bennett, L., & Fineran, S. (1998). Sexual and severe phys- risk behavior surveillance: United States, 2013.
ical violence among high school students-power MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 63(Suppl 4), 1–168.
beliefs, gender, and relationship. American Journal of Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2016).
Orthopsychiatry, 68, 645–652. Understanding Teen Dating Violence. Atlanta: Divi-
Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., & Lavoie, F. sion of Violence Prevention, National Center for Injury
(2001). Reactive and proactive aggression: Predictions Prevention and Control, Centers for Disease Control
to physical violence in different contexts and moderat- and Prevention.
ing effects of parental monitoring and caregiving Chang, L. Y., Foshee, V. A., Reyes, H. L. M., Ennett, S. T.,
behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, & Halpern, C. T. (2015). Direct and indirect effects of
293–304. neighborhood characteristics on the perpetration of dat-
Brooks-Russell, A., Foshee, V. A., & Ennett, S. T. (2013). ing violence across adolescence. Journal of Youth and
Predictors of latent trajectory classes of physical dating Adolescence, 44(3), 727–744.
violence victimization. Journal of Youth and Adoles- Chapple, C. (2003). Examining intergenerational violence:
cence, 42(4), 566–580. Violent role modeling or weak parental controls? Vio-
Brooks-Russell, A., Foshee, V. A., & Reyes, H. L. M. lence and Victims, 18, 143–162.
(2015). Dating violence. In T. P. Gullotta, R. W. Plant, Chiodo, D., Wolfe, D. A., Crooks, C., Hughes, R., & Jaffe,
& M. Evans (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent behavioral P. (2009). Impact of sexual harassment victimization by
problems (pp. 559–576). New York: Springer. peers on subsequent adolescent victimization and
Brown, A., Cosgrave, E., Killackey, E., Purcell, R., adjustment: A longitudinal study. The Journal of Ado-
Buckby, J., & Yung, A. R. (2009). The longitudinal lescent Health, 45, 246–252.
association of adolescent dating violence with psychi- Cleveland, H. H., Herrera, V. M., & Stuewig, J. (2003).
atric disorders and functioning. Journal of Interper- Abusive males and abused females in adolescent rela-
sonal Violence, 24, 1964–1979. tionships: Risk factor similarity and dissimilarity and
Calvete, E., Orue, I., Gamez-Guadix, M., & de Arroyabe, the role of relationship seriousness. Journal of Family
E. L. (2014). Social information processing in dating Violence, 18(6), 325–339.
conflicts reciprocal relationships with dating aggres- Coker, A. L., McKeown, R. E., Sanderson, M., Davis,
sion in a one-year prospective study. Journal of Inter- K. E., Valois, R. F., & Huebner, E. S. (2000). Severe
personal Violence, 55, 1–25. dating violence and quality of life among South Caro-
Cano, A., Avery-Leaf, S., Cascardi, M., & O’Leary, K. D. lina high school students. American Journal of Preven-
(1998). Dating violence in two high school samples: tive Medicine, 19, 220–227.
Discriminating variables. The Journal of Primary Pre- Connolly, J. A., & McIsaac, C. (2009). Romantic relation-
vention, 18, 431–446. ships in adolescence. In R. M. Lerner & L. Steinberg
Capaldi, D. M., & Clark, S. (1998). Prospective family (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology, Vol. 2:
predictors of aggression toward female partners for Contextual influences on adolescent development (3rd
young at-risk males. Developmental Psychology, 34, ed., pp. 104–151). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
1175–1188. Connolly, J., Craig, W., Goldberg, A., & Pepler, D. (2004).
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2000). Sur- Mixed-gender groups, dating, and romantic relation-
veillance summaries. MMWR Morbidity Mortal Weekly ships in early adolescence. Journal of Research on
Report, 49(SSO5), 1–96. Adolescence, 14(2), 185–207.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2002). Sur- Connolly, J., Friedlander, L., Pepler, D., Craig, W., &
veillance summaries. MMWR Morbidity Mortal Weekly Laporte, L. (2010). The ecology of adolescent dating
Report, 51(SSO4), 1–64. aggression: Attitudes, relationships, media use, and
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2004). Sur- socio-demographic risk factors. Journal of Aggression,
veillance summaries. MMWR Morbidity Mortal Weekly Maltreatment & Trauma, 19(5), 469–491.
Report, 53(SS-2), 1–100. Cui, M., Ueno, K., Gordon, M., & Fincham, F. D. (2013).
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2006). Sur- The continuation of intimate partner violence from
veillance summaries. MMWR Morbidity Mortal Weekly adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of Marriage
Report, 55(SS05), 1–108. and Family, 75(2), 300–313.
872 Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors

Cutbush, S., Ashley, O. S., Kan, M. L., Hamptom, J., & predictors of serious physical and sexual dating vio-
Hall, D. M. (2010). Electronic aggression among ado- lence victimization during adolescence. Preventive
lescent dating partners: Demographic correlates and Medicine, 39, 1007–1016.
associations with other types of violence. Poster pre- Foshee, V. A., Ennett, S. T., Bauman, K. E., Benefield, T.,
sented at the American Public Health Association & Suchindran, C. (2005). The association between
Annual Meeting. family violence and adolescent dating violence onset:
De La Rue, L., Polanin, J. R., Espelage, D. L., & Pigott, Does it vary by race, socioeconomic status, and family
T. D. (2016). A meta-analysis of school-based interven- structure? Journal of Early Adolescence, 25, 317–344.
tions aimed to prevent or reduce violence in teen dating Foshee, V. A., Karriker-Jaffe, K. J., Reyes, H. L. M.,
relationships. Review of Educational Research. https:// Ennett, S. T., Suchindran, C., Bauman, K. E., &
doi.org/10.3102/0034654316632061. Benefield, T. S. (2008). What accounts for demo-
East, P. L., & Hokoda, A. (2015). Risk and protective graphic differences in trajectories of adolescent dating
factors for sexual and dating violence victimization: violence? An examination of intrapersonal and contex-
A longitudinal, prospective study of latino and african tual mediators. Journal of Adolescent Health, 42(6),
american adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adoles- 596–604.
cence, 44(6), 1288–1300. Foshee, V. A., Benefield, T., Suchindran, C., Ennett, S. T.,
Ellis, W. E., Crooks, C. V., & Wolfe, D. A. (2009). Rela- Bauman, K. E., Karriker-Jaffe, K. J., McNaughton
tional aggression in peer and dating relationships: Reyes, K. J., & Mathias, H. L. (2009a). The develop-
Links to psychological and behavioral adjustment. ment of four types of adolescent dating abuse and
Social Development, 18(2), 253–269. selected demographic correlates. Journal of Research
Epstein-Ngo, Q. M., Cunningham, R. M., Whiteside, L. K., on Adolescence, 19, 380–400.
Chermack, S. T., Booth, B. M., Zimmerman, M. A., & Foshee, V. A., Reyes, H. L. M., & Ennett, S. T. (2010).
Walton, M. A. (2013). A daily calendar analysis of Examination of sex and race differences in longitudinal
substance use and dating violence among high risk predictors of the initiation of adolescent dating violence
urban youth. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 130 perpetration. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment, and
(1–3), 194–200. Trauma, 19(5), 492–516.
Exner-Cortens, D., Eckenrode, J., & Rothman, E. (2013). Foshee, V. A., Reyes, H. L. M., Ennett, S. T., Suchindran,
Longitudinal associations between teen dating violence C., Mathias, J. P., Karriker-Jaffe, K. J., . . . Benefield,
victimization and adverse health outcomes. Pediatrics, T. S. (2011). Risk and protective factors distinguishing
131(1), 71–78. profiles of adolescent peer and dating violence perpe-
Exner-Cortens, D., Gill, L., & Eckenrode, J. (2016). tration. Journal of Adolescent Health, 48(4), 344–350.
Measurement of adolescent dating violence: Foshee, V. A., Reyes, H. L. M., Gottfredson, N. C., Chang,
A comprehensive review (Part 1, behaviors). Aggres- L. Y., & Ennett, S. T. (2013a). A longitudinal exami-
sion and Violent Behavior, 27, 64–78. nation of psychological, behavioral, academic, and
Farrell, A. D., Sullivan, T. N., Esposito, L. E., Meyer, A. L., relationship consequences of dating abuse victimiza-
& Valois, R. F. (2005). A latent growth curve analysis tion among a primarily rural sample of adolescents.
of the structure of aggression, drug use, and delinquent Journal of Adolescent Health, 53(6), 723–729.
behaviors and their interrelations over time in urban Foshee, V. A., Benefield, T. S., Reyes, H. L. M., Ennett,
and rural adolescents. Journal of Research on Adoles- S. T., Faris, R., Chang, L. Y., . . . Suchindran, C. M.
cence, 15(2), 179–204. (2013b). The peer context and the development of the
Ferguson, C. J., Miguel, C. S., Garza, A., & Jerabeck, J. M. perpetration of adolescent dating violence. Journal of
(2012). A longitudinal test of video game violence Youth and Adolescence, 42(4), 471–486.
influences on dating and aggression: A 3-year longitu- Foshee, V. A., Reyes, H. L. M., Vivolo-Kantor, A. M.,
dinal study of adolescents. Journal of Psychiatric Basile, K. C., Chang, L., Faris, R., & Ennett, S. T.
Research, 46(2), 141–146. (2014). Bullying as a longitudinal predictor of adoles-
Foshee, V. A. (1996). Gender differences in adolescent cent dating violence. Journal of Adolescent Health,
dating abuse prevalence, types, and injuries. Health 55(3), 439–444.
Education Research, 11, 275–286. Foshee, V. A., Benefield, T. S., Puvanesarajah, S., Reyes,
Foshee, V. A., & Matthew, R. A. (2007). Adolescent dating H. L. M., Haberstick, B. C., Smolen, A., . . .
abuse perpetration: A review of findings, methodological Suchindran, C. (2015). Self-regulatory failure and the
limitations and suggestions for future research. In perpetration of adolescent dating violence: Examining
D. Flannery, A. Vazonsyi, & I. Waldman (Eds.), The an alcohol use by gene explanation. Aggressive Behav-
Cambridge handbook of violent behavior and aggression ior, 41(2), 189–203.
(pp. 431–449). New York: Cambridge University Press. Foshee, V. A., Chang, L. Y., Reyes, H. L. M., Chen, M. S.,
Foshee, V. A., Linder, F., MacDougall, J. E., & & Ennett, S. T. (2015b). The synergy of family and
Bangdiwala, S. (2001). Gender differences in the lon- neighborhood on rural dating violence victimization.
gitudinal predictors of dating violence. Preventive American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 49(3),
Medicine, 32, 128–141. 483–491.
Foshee, V. A., Benefield, T. S., Ennett, S. T., Bauman, Foshee, V. A., Gottfredson, N. C., Reyes, H. L. M., Chen,
K. E., & Suchindran, C. (2004). Longitudinal M. S., David-Ferdon, C., Latzman, N. E., ... & Ennett,
Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors 873

S. T. (2016). Developmental outcomes of using physi- nationally representative sample. The Journal of Ado-
cal violence against dates and peers. Journal of Ado- lescent Health, 45(5), 508–516.
lescent Health, 58(6), 665–671. Hamby, S. (2015). A scientific answer to a scientific ques-
Foster, H., Hagan, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2004). Age, tion the gender debate on intimate partner violence.
puberty, and exposure to intimate partner violence in Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 1524838015596963.
adolescence. Annals of the New York Academy of Sci- Hamby, S., Finkelhor, D., & Turner, H. (2012). Teen dating
ences, 1036, 151–166. violence: Co-occurrence with other victimizations in
Furman, W., & Shomaker, L. B. (2008). Patterns of inter- the National Survey of Children’s Exposure to Violence
action in adolescent romantic relationships: Distinct (NatSCEV). Psychology of Violence, 2(2), 111–124.
features and links to other close relationships. Journal Haynie, D. L., Farhat, T., Brooks-Russell, A., Wang, J.,
of Adolescence, 31(6), 771–788. Barbieri, B., & Iannotti, R. J. (2013). Dating violence D
Garcia-Moreno, C., & Watts, C. (2011). Violence against perpetration and victimization among US adolescents:
women: An urgent public health priority. Bulletin of the Prevalence, patterns, and associations with health com-
World Health Organization, 89(1), 2–2. plaints and substance use. Journal of Adolescent
Gayford, J. (1975). Wife battering: A preliminary survey of Health, 53(2), 194–201.
100 cases. British Medical Journal, 1(5951), 194–197. Heise, L. L., & Kotsadam, A. (2015). Cross-national and
Giordano, P. C., Johnson, W. L., Manning, W. D., multilevel correlates of partner violence: An analysis of
Longmore, M. A., & Minter, M. D. (2015). Intimate data from population-based surveys. The Lancet
partner violence in young adulthood: Narratives of Global Health, 3(6), e332–e340.
persistence and desistance. Criminology, 53(3), Howard, D. E., & Wang, M. Q. (2003). Psychosocial
330–365. factors associated with adolescent boy’s reports of dat-
Gómez, A. M. (2010). Testing the cycle of violence ing violence. Adolescence, 38, 519–533.
hypothesis: Child abuse and adolescent dating violence Jain, S., Buka, S. L., Subramanian, S. V., & Molnar, B. E.
as predictors of intimate partner violence in young (2010). Neighborhood predictors of dating violence
adulthood. Youth & Society, 43, 171–192. victimization and perpetration in young adulthood:
Goodman, E. (1999). The role of socioeconomic status A multilevel study. American Journal of Public Health,
gradients in explaining differences in U.S. teens’ health. 100(9), 1737–1744.
American Journal of Public Health, 89, 1522–1528. Jezl, D. R., Molidor, C., & Wright, R. L. (1996). Physical,
Gordon, R. (1987). An operational classification of disease sexual and psychological abuse in high school dating
prevention. In J. A. Steinberg & M. M. Silverman relationships: Prevalence rates and self-esteem issues.
(Eds.), Preventing mental disorders. Rockville: Depart- Child & Adolescent Social Work Journal, 13(1), 69–87.
ment of Health and Human Services. Johnson, W. L., Giordano, P. C., Longmore, M. A., &
Gorman-Smith, D., Tolan, P. H., Shiedow, A. J., & Henry, Manning, W. D. (2014). Intimate partner violence and
D. B. (2001). Partner violence and street violence depressive symptoms during adolescence and young
among urban adolescents: Do the same family factors adulthood. Journal of Health and Social Behavior,
relate? Journal of Research on Adolescence, 11, 55(1), 39–55.
273–295. Johnson, R. M., Parker, E. M., Rinehart, J., Nail, J., &
Graffunder, C. M., Noonan, R. K., Cox, P., & Wheaton, Rothman, E. F. (2015a). Neighborhood factors and
J. (2004). Through a public health lens. Preventing dating violence among youth: A systematic review.
violence against women: An update from the US Cen- American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 49(3),
ters for Disease Control and Prevention. Journal of 458–466.
Women’s Health, 13(1), 5–16. Johnson, W. L., Manning, W. D., Giordano, P. C., &
Gray, H. M., & Foshee, V. A. (1997). Adolescent dating Longmore, M. A. (2015b). Relationship context and
violence: Differences between one-sided and mutually intimate partner violence from adolescence to young
violent profiles. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 12, adulthood. Journal of Adolescent Health, 57(6),
126–141. 631–636.
Halpern, C. T., Oslak, S. G., Young, M. L., Martin, S. L., & Jouriles, E. N., Mueller, V., Rosenfield, D., McDonald, R.,
Kupper, L. L. (2001). Partner violence among adoles- & Dodson, M. C. (2012). Teens’ experiences of harsh
cents in opposite-sex romantic relationships: Findings parenting and exposure to severe intimate partner vio-
from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent lence: Adding insult to injury in predicting teen dating
Health. American Journal of Public Health, 91, violence. Psychology of Violence, 2(2), 125–138.
1679–1685. Jouriles, E. N., Rosenfield, D., McDonald, R., Kleinsasser,
Halpern, C. T., Young, M. L., Waller, M. W., Martin, S. L., A. L., & Dodson, M. C. (2013). Explicit beliefs about
& Kupper, L. L. (2004). Prevalence of partner violence aggression, implicit knowledge structures, and teen
in same-sex romantic and sexual relationships in a dating violence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol-
national sample of adolescents. Journal of Adolescent ogy, 41(5), 789–799.
Health, 35(2), 124–131. Kann, L., Kinchen, S., Shanklin, S. L., Flint, K. H.,
Halpern, C. T., Spriggs, A. L., Martin, S. L., & Kupper, Kawkins, J., Harris, W. A., ... & Whittle, L. (2014).
L. L. (2009). Patterns of intimate partner violence vic- Youth risk behavior surveillance—United States, 2013.
timization from adolescence to young adulthood in a MMWR Surveill Summ, 63(Suppl 4), 1–168.
874 Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors

Kaestle, C. E., & Halpern, C. T. (2005). Sexual intercourse McCloskey, L. A., & Lichter, E. L. (2003). The contribu-
precedes partner violence in adolescent romantic rela- tion of marital violence to adolescent aggression across
tionships. The Journal of Adolescent Health, 36, different relationships. Journal of Interpersonal Vio-
386–392. lence, 18, 390–412.
Kerr, D. C., & Capaldi, D. M. (2011). Young men’s inti- Miller, S., Gorman-Smith, D., Sullivant, T., Orpinas, P., &
mate partner violence and relationship functioning: Simon, T. R. (2009). Parent and peer predictors of
Long-term outcomes associated with suicide attempt physical dating violence perpetration in early adoles-
and aggression in adolescence. Psychological Medi- cence: Tests of moderation and gender differences.
cine, 41(04), 759–769. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,
Kreiter, S. R., Krowchuk, D. P., Woods, C. R., Sinal, 38(4), 538–550.
S. H., Lawless, M. R., & DuRant, R. H. (1999). Miller, S., Williams, J., Cutbush, S., Gibbs, D., Clinton-
Gender differences in risk behaviors among adoles- Sherrod, M., & Jones, S. (2013). Dating violence, bul-
cents who experience date fighting. Pediatrics, 104, lying, and sexual harassment: Longitudinal profiles and
1286–1292. transitions over time. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
Larson, R. W., Clore, G. L., & Wood, G. A. (1999). The cence, 42(4), 607–618.
emotions of romantic relationships: Do they wreak Millett, L. S., Kohl, P. L., Jonson-Reid, M., Drake, B., &
havoc on adolescents? In W. Furman, B. B. Brown, & Petra, M. (2013). Child maltreatment victimization and
C. Feiring (Eds.), The development of romantic rela- subsequent perpetration of young adult intimate partner
tionships in adolescence (pp. 19–49). New York: Cam- violence an exploration of mediating factors. Child
bridge University Press. Maltreatment, 18(2), 71–84.
Latzman, N. E., Vivolo-Kantor, A. M., Niolon, P. H., & Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-
Ghazarian, S. R. (2015). Predicting adolescent dating persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxon-
violence perpetration role of exposure to intimate part- omy. Psychological Review, 100(4), 674.
ner violence and parenting practices. American Journal Morris, A. M., Mrug, S., & Windle, M. (2015). From
of Preventive Medicine, 49(3), 476–482. family violence to dating violence: Testing a dual path-
Lavoie, F., Hebert, M., Tremblay, R. E., Vitaro, F., Vezina, way model. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44(9),
L., & McDuff, P. (2002). History of family dysfunction 1819–1835.
and perpetration of dating violence by adolescent boys: Mueller, V., Jouriles, E. N., McDonald, R., & Rosenfield,
A longitudinal study. The Journal of Adolescent D. (2013). Adolescent beliefs about the acceptability of
Health, 30, 375–383. dating violence: Does violent behavior change them?
Lehrer, J. A., Buka, S., Gortmaker, S., & Shrier, L. A. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 28(2), 436–450.
(2006). Depressive symptomatology as a predictor of Munoz-Rivas, M. J., Grana, J. L., O’leary, K. D., &
exposure to intimate partner violence among U.- Gonzalez, M. P. (2007). Aggression in adolescent dat-
S. female adolescents and young adults. Archives of ing relationships: Prevalence, justification, and health
Pediatric Adolescent Medicine, 160, 270–276. consequences. The Journal of Adolescent Health,
Lichter, E. L., & McCloskey, L. A. (2004). The effects of 40(4), 298–304.
childhood exposure to marital violence on adolescent Murphy, M., & Hoover, S. A. (2001). Measuring emotional
gender-role beliefs and dating violence. Psychology of abuse in dating relationships as a multifactorial con-
Women Quarterly, 28, 344–357. struct. In K. D. O’Leary & R. D. Maiuro (Eds.), Psy-
Linder, J. R., & Collins, W. A. (2005). Parent and peer chological abuse in violent domestic relations
predictors of physical aggression and conflict manage- (pp. 29–46). New York: Springer.
ment in romantic relationships in early adulthood. Jour- Nahapetyan, L., Orpinas, P., Song, X., & Holland,
nal of Family Psychology, 19(2), 252–262. K. (2014). Longitudinal association of suicidal ideation
Maas, C. D., Fleming, C. B., Herrenkohl, T. I., & Catalano, and physical dating violence among high school stu-
R. F. (2010). Childhood predictors of teen dating vio- dents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43(4),
lence victimization. Violence and Victims, 25(2), 629–640.
131–149. Niolon, P. H., Vivolo-Kantor, A. M., Latzman, N. E., Valle,
Magdol, L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1998). L. A., Kuoh, H., Burton, T., . . . Tharp, A. T. (2015).
Developmental antecedents of partner abuse: A - Prevalence of teen dating violence and co-occurring
prospective-longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal risk factors among middle school youth in high-risk
Psychology, 107, 375–389. urban communities. Journal of Adolescent Health,
Malik, S., Sorenson, S. B., & Aneshensel, C. S. (1997). 56(2), S5–S13.
Community and dating violence among adolescents: Nocentini, A., Menesini, E., & Pastorelli, C. (2010). Phys-
Perpetration and victimization. The Journal of Adoles- ical dating aggression growth during adolescence. Jour-
cent Health, 21, 291–302. nal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(3), 353–365.
Manning, W. D., Longmore, M. A., Copp, J., & Giordano, O’Donnell, L., Stueve, A., Myint-U, A., Duran, R.,
P. C. (2014). The complexities of adolescent dating and Agronick, G., & Wilson-Simmons, R. (2006). Middle
sexual relationships: Fluidity, meaning(s), and implica- school aggression and subsequent intimate partner
tions for young adults’ well-being. Positive and Nega- physical violence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
tive Outcomes of Sexual Behaviors, 144, 53–69. 25, 693–703.
Dating Abuse: Prevalence, Consequences, and Predictors 875

O’Keefe, M. (1997). Predictors of dating violence among adolescent dating violence perpetration: Normative
high school students. Journal of Interpersonal Vio- beliefs as moderators. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
lence, 12, 546–568. cence, 45(2), 350–360.
O’Keefe, M., & Treister, L. (1998). Victims of dating Roberts, T., Klein, J. D., & Fisher, S. (2003). Longitudinal
violence among high school students: Are predictors effect of intimate partner abuse on high-risk behavior
different for males and females? Violence Against among adolescents. Archives of Pediatric and Adoles-
Women, 4, 195–223. cent Medicine, 157, 875–881.
O’Leary, K. D., & Slep, A. S. (2003). A dyadic longitudi- Roberts, T. A., Auinger, P., & Klein, J. D. (2005). Intimate
nal model of adolescent dating aggression. Journal of partner abuse and the reproductive health and sexually
Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 32, active female adolescents. The Journal of Adolescent
314–327. Health, 36(5), 380–385. D
O’Leary, K. D., Slep, A. M. S., Avery-Leaf, S., & Cascardi, Roscoe, B., & Benaske, N. (1985). Courtship violence
M. (2008). Gender differences in dating aggression experienced by abused wives: Similarities in patterns
among multiethnic high school students. Journal of of abuse. Family Relations, 34, 419–424.
Adolescent Health, 42(5), 473–479. Rothman E.F., Reyes L.M., Johnson R.M., LaValley M.
Orpinas, P., Nahapetyan, L., Song, X., McNicholas, C., & (2011) Does the alcohol make them do it? Dating
Reeves, P. M. (2012). Psychological dating violence violence perpetration and drinking among youth. Epi-
perpetration and victimization: Trajectories from mid- demiologic Reviews, 34, 103–119.
dle to high school. Aggressive Behavior, 38(6), Saltzman, L., Fanslow, J., McMahon, P., & Shelley,
510–520. G. (2002). Intimate partner violence surveillance: Uni-
Orpinas, P., Hsieh, H. L., Song, X., Holland, K., & form definitions and recommended data elements, ver-
Nahapetyan, L. (2013). Trajectories of physical dating sion 1.0. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,
violence from middle to high school: Association with National Center for Injury Prevention and Control.
relationship quality and acceptability of aggression. Sanderson, M., Coker, A. L., Roberts, R. E., Tortolero,
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42(4), 551–565. S. R., & Reininger, B. M. (2004). Acculturation, ethnic
Ozer, E. J., Tschann, J. M., Pasch, L. A., & Flores, identity, and dating violence among Latino ninth-grade
E. (2004). Violence perpetration across peer and part- students. Preventive Medicine, 39(2), 373–383.
ner relationships: Co-occurrence and longitudinal pat- Schnurr, M. P., & Lohman, B. J. (2008). How much does
terns among adolescents. The Journal of Adolescent school matter? An examination of adolescent dating
Health, 34, 64–71. violence perpetration. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
Poitras, M., & Lavoie, F. (1995). A study of the prevalence cence, 37(3), 266–283.
of sexual coercion in adolescent heterosexual dating Sears, H. A., Byers, E. S., & Price, E. L. (2007). The
relationships in a Quebec sample. Violence and Victims, co-occurrence of adolescent boys’ and girls’ use of
10, 299–313. psychologically, physically, and sexually abusive
Raiford, J. L., Wingwood, G. M., & Diclemente, R. J. behaviours in their dating relationships. Journal of
(2007). Prevalence, incidence, and predictors of dating Adolescence, 30(3), 487–504.
violence: A longitudinal study of African American Shulman, S. (2003). Conflict and negotiation in adolescent
female adolescents. Journal of Womens Health, 16(6), romantic relationships. In P. Florsheim (Ed.), Adoles-
822–832. cent romantic relations and sexual behavior: Theory,
Reyes, H. L. M. (2009). Adolescent alcohol use and dating research, and practical implications (pp. 109–135).
violence perpetration: Three studies examining con- Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.
current and longitudinal relations across grades Silverman, J., Raj, A., Mucci, L., & Hathaway, J. (2001).
8 through 12. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Uni- Dating violence against adolescent girls and associated
versity of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. substance use, unhealthy weight control, sexual risk
Reyes, H. L. M., & Foshee, V. A. (2013). Sexual dating behavior, pregnancy, and suicidality. Journal of the
aggression across grades 8 through 12: Timing and American Medical Association, 286, 572–579.
predictors of onset. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, Simon, V. A., Feiring, C., & McElroy, S. K. (2010). Mak-
42(4), 581–595. ing meaning of traumatic events: Youths’ strategies for
Reyes, H. L. M., Foshee, V. A., Bauer, D. J., & Ennett, S. T. processing childhood sexual abuse are associated with
(2011). The role of heavy alcohol use in the develop- psychosocial adjustment. Child Maltreatment, 15(3),
mental process of desistance in dating aggression dur- 229–241.
ing adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Simons, R. L., Lin, K., & Gordon, L. C. (1998). Socializa-
Psychology, 39(2), 239–250. tion in the family of origin and male dating violence:
Reyes, H. L. M., Foshee, V. A., Bauer, D. J., & Ennett, S. T. A prospective study. Journal of Marriage and the
(2014). Proximal and time-varying effects of cigarette, Family, 60, 467–478.
alcohol, marijuana and other hard drug use on adoles- Smith, J., Mulford, C., Latzman, N. E., Tharp, A. T.,
cent dating aggression. Journal of Adolescence, 37(3), Niolon, P. H., & Blachman-Demner, D. (2015). Taking
281–289. stock of behavioral measures of adolescent dating vio-
Reyes, H. L. M., Foshee, V. A., Niolon, P. H., Reidy, D. E., lence. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma,
& Hall, J. E. (2016). Gender role attitudes and male 24(6), 674–692.
876 Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts

Spriggs, A. L., Halpern, C. T., Herring, A. H., & across different adolescent relationships:
Schoenbach, V. J. (2009). Family and school socioeco- A developmental psychopathology approach. Journal
nomic disadvantage: Interactive influences on adoles- of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76(40),
cent dating violence victimization. Social Science & 622–632.
Medicine, 68(11), 1956–1965. Windle, M. (2000). A latent growth curve model of delin-
Straus, M. A., & Gelles, R. J. (1986). Societal change and quent activity among adolescents. Applied Develop-
change in family violence from 1975 to 1985 as mental Science, 4(4), 193–207.
revealed by two national surveys. Journal of Marriage Wolfe, D., Wekerle, C., Reitzel-Jaffe, D., & Lefebvre,
and the Family, 48, 465–479. L. (1998). Factors associated with abusive relationships
Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Boney-McCoy, S., & among maltreated and nonmaltreated youth. Develop-
Sugarman, D. B. (1996). The revised Conflict Tactics ment and Psychopathology, 10, 61–85.
Scales (CTS2): Development and preliminary psycho- Wolfe, D. A., Scott, K., Reitzel-Jaffe, D., Wekerle, C.,
metric data. Journal of Family Issues, 17, 283–316. Grasley, C., & Straatman, A. L. (2001). Development
Swahn, M. H., Bossarte, R. M., & Sullivent, E. E. (2008). and validation of the Conflict in Adolescent Dating
Age of alcohol use initiation, suicidal behavior, and Relationships Inventory. Psychological Assessment,
peer and dating violence victimization and perpetration 13, 277–293.
among high-risk, seventh-grade adolescents. Pediat- Wolfe, D., Wekerle, C., Scott, K., Straatman, A., &
rics, 121(2), 297–305. Grasley, C. (2004). Predicting abuse in adolescent dat-
Taylor, B., Stein, N., & Burden, F. (2010). The effects of ing relationships over 1 year: The role of child mal-
gender violence/harassment prevention programming treatment and trauma. Journal of Abnormal
in middle schools: A randomized experimental evalua- Psychology, 113, 406–415.
tion. Violence and Victims, 25(2), 202–223. Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B., Ruggiero, K. J., Danielson, C. K.,
Taylor, K. A., Sullivan, T. N., & Farrell, A. D. (2015). Resnick, H. S., Hanson, R. F., Smith, D. W., . . .
Longitudinal relationships between individual and Kilpatrick, D. G. (2008). Prevalence and correlates of
class norms supporting dating violence and perpetra- dating violence in a national sample of adolescents.
tion of dating violence. Journal of Youth and Adoles- Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adoles-
cence, 44(3), 745–760. cent Psychiatry, 47(7), 755–762.
Temple, J. R., Shorey, R. C., Fite, P., Stuart, G. L., & Le, Zweig, J. M., Dank, M., Yahner, J., & Lachman, P. (2013).
V. D. (2013). Substance use as a longitudinal predictor The rate of cyber dating abuse among teens and how it
of the perpetration of teen dating violence. Journal of relates to other forms of teen dating violence. Journal
Youth and Adolescence, 42(4), 596–606. of Youth and Adolescence, 42, 1063–1077.
Thompson, R. S., Bonomi, A. E., & Anderson, M. (2006).
Intimate partner violence-prevalence, types and chro-
nicity in adult women. American Journal of Preventive
Medicine, 30(6), 447–457.
Tschann J.M., Pasch L.A., Flores E., VanOss Marin B., Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention
Baisch E.M., Wibbelsman C.J. (2009) Nonviolent
aspects of interparental conflict and dating violence
Efforts
among adolescents. Journal of Family Issues, 30(3),
295–319. Heathe H. Luz McNaughton Reyes1,
Tyler, K. A., Brownridge, D. A., & Melander, L. A. (2011). Vangie A. Foshee1 and May S. Chen2
The effect of poor parenting on male and female dating 1
Gillings School of Global Public Health,
violence perpetration and victimization. Violence and
Victims, 26(2), 218–230. University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill,
Ulloa, E. C., Martinez-Arango, N., & Hokoda, A. (2014). Chapel Hill, NC, USA
Attachment anxiety, depressive symptoms, and adoles- 2
Department of Health Behavior, University of
cent dating violence perpetration: A longitudinal medi-
North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill,
ation analysis. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment &
Trauma, 23(6), 652–669. NC, USA
Vagi, K. J., Olsen, E. O. M., Basile, K. C., & Vivolo-
Kantor, A. M. (2015). Teen dating violence (physical
and sexual) among US high school students: Findings
from the 2013 National Youth Risk Behavior Survey.
Introduction
JAMA Pediatrics, 169(5), 474–482.
White, J. W., & Smith, P. H. (2009). Covariation in the use of Preventing dating abuse (DA) among adolescents
physical and sexual intimate partner aggression among has been recognized as a public health problem
adolescent and college-age men: A longitudinal analysis.
and human rights imperative (Spivak et al. 2014;
Violence Against Women, 15(1), 24–43.
Williams, T. S., Connolly, J., Pepler, D., Craig, W., & Lundgren and Amin 2015). Nationally represen-
Laporte, L. (2008). Risk models of dating aggression tative studies of US high school students indicate
Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts 877

that approximately 20% of students who have PubMed, Google Scholar, and Web of Science.
dated report having experienced psychological Previous DV literature reviews were analyzed, as
DV in the past year and 10% have experienced were the reference lists for all reports and articles
physical and/or sexual DV (Kann et al. 2014; identified. Search terms included “adolescents” or
Haynie et al. 2013). DV victimization can have “youth” and “dating abuse prevention” or “sexual
serious negative health consequences for both violence prevention” or “partner violence preven-
boys and girls, including physical injury; tion”. Studies that evaluated the effects of pro-
unhealthy behaviors, such as drug and alcohol grams on DV using measures that did not
use and binge eating; and depression and suicide specifically assess violence perpetrated or experi- D
ideation and attempts (Ackard et al. 2007; Brown enced in the context of a dating (or intimate part-
et al. 2009; Exner-Cortens et al. 2013; Foshee ner) relationship were excluded (e.g., studies
et al. 2013; O’Leary et al. 2008; Roberts using measures of sexual violence that did not
et al. 2003). Further, patterns of relationship con- distinguish between non-partner- vs. partner-
flict established during adolescence may carry perpetrated violence). In addition, programs
over into young adulthood, contributing to adult targeting university students were excluded
intimate partner violence (Bouchey and Furman because they were considered to focus on emerg-
2006; Magdol et al. 1998). Thus, prevention of ing adulthood rather than adolescence.
DV during adolescence is of particular importance We identified 17 randomized control trials
because doing so may contribute to reduce part- (RCTs) of 18 prevention programs that evaluated
ner violence across the life course. This chapter effects on at least one type of DV victimization
provides a review of efforts to prevent DV and/or perpetration during adolescence. All
victimization and perpetration during adoles- 17 RCTs included a baseline and at least one
cence. While numerous programs have been post-intervention assessment with time frames
implemented that aim to prevent DV, relatively for follow-up assessments ranging from 1–2
few of these programs have used rigorous weeks to 4 years post-intervention. All DV out-
methods to evaluate effects on DV outcomes, comes were assessed via self-report. Consistent
which is critical to establishing a causal effect with literature that has found that both boys and
of the intervention on DV victimization and/or girls experience DV victimization and engage in
perpetration behavior (Lundgren and Amin DV perpetration (Foshee and Reyes 2012), most
2015). This structured narrative review thus of the RCTs evaluated program effects on both
focuses on describing DV prevention programs boys and girls (n = 16) and assessed effects on
that have been evaluated using randomized both victimization and perpetration outcomes
controlled trials (RCTs) and that have assessed (n = 13). Fifteen studies assessed program effects
effects on at least one DV outcome (psycholo- on one or more types of DV victimization: eight
gical, physical, and/or sexual victimization assessed psychological (including cyber) DV,
and/or perpetration) during adolescence (ages nine assessed physical DV, five assessed sexual
10–19 years). Findings from the evaluations of DV, and three assessed effects on a composite
these programs are described and synthesized, victimization outcome (i.e., psychological and/or
and directions for future DV prevention and eval- physical and/or sexual DV). Fifteen studies
uation efforts are discussed. assessed program effects on one or more types
of DV perpetration: seven assessed psychological
(including cyber) DV, ten assessed physical DV,
Evaluations of Programs That Aim five assessed sexual DV, and four assessed effects
to Prevent Adolescent Dating Abuse on a composite DV perpetration outcome. All of
the programs were evaluated with adolescents in
To identify programs and studies eligible for the the USA with the exception of the Stepping Stones
review, electronic searches for published and evaluation, which took place in South Africa
peer-reviewed literature were conducted using (Jewkes et al. 2008).
878 Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts

Organizations interested in selecting and et al. 2013), which was designed to make changes
implementing DV prevention programs often to the school contextual environment. The two
look for programs shown to be effective with the universal programs that are not school based are
particular population they wish to target. For this Families for Safe Dates (FSD; Foshee et al. 2012),
reason, we have organized our description of the which is a family-based program designed to be
programs using Gordon’s (1983) threefold classi- completed by caregivers and their adolescents in
fication system, which categorizes programs as their homes, and Stepping Stones (Jewkes
universal, selected, or indicated, according to the et al. 2008), which is a community-based program.
population that is targeted. We classified half A summary of program and study characteristics
(9) of the 18 programs as universal because they and findings for each of the universal programs is
were developed to be implemented with general presented in Tables 1 and 2, respectively.
populations of adolescents. Six (33%) programs Safe Dates. The Safe Dates program targets
were classified as selective, because they were adolescents in the eighth and ninth grades and
developed for populations determined to be at includes a ten-session curriculum (45-min per
risk for DV based primarily on demographic char- session), a theater production, and a poster contest
acteristics. Three (17%) programs were classified (Foshee et al. 1996, 1998). The program is
as indicated because they were developed for designed to prevent and reduce DV perpetration
populations with identifiable markers (e.g., his- and victimization. Activities to prevent the initia-
tory of child maltreatment) indicating high risk tion of DV are designed to decrease acceptance of
for involvement in DV. In the following we pro- DV, increase perceived sanctions for DV, reduce
vide a brief description of each program and find- gender stereotyping, and improve conflict man-
ings from research assessing the effects of the agement skills and communication skills. Activi-
program on DV outcomes. Several studies ties to promote cessation of DV target change in
assessed program effects on DV outcomes imme- these same constructs, along with cognitive fac-
diately after the program had ended as well as at tors found to influence decisions to take preven-
follow-up assessments that occurred at later time tive action, like leaving an abusive dating partner,
points. The description of findings focuses on belief in the need for help, and awareness of
summarizing analyses of program effects on DV community resources for help. The RCT of Safe
outcomes assessed at later time points rather than Dates involved random allocation of 14 schools to
immediately post-intervention. We refer to the treatment and control conditions. Follow-up
relationship between treatment exposure and a assessments were conducted yearly up until
particular DV outcome as statistically significant 4 years post-intervention (Foshee et al. 1998,
when the reported p-value associated with the 2000, 2004, 2005). Adolescents exposed to SD
effect of treatment is less than 0.05; when the in the eighth or ninth grade reported less psycho-
reported p-value is less than 0.10, we refer to the logical, moderate physical, and sexual DV
association as marginally significant. perpetration and less moderate physical DV vic-
timization at all four follow-up periods, and there
Universal Programs were also marginal (p = 0.07) program effects on
Of the nine universal programs that met our selec- sexual DV victimization across follow-up (Foshee
tion criteria, seven are school based. With two et al. 2005). Additionally, the program was effec-
exceptions, these school-based programs include tive in preventing severe physical DV perpetra-
a classroom-based curriculum as the primary com- tion across follow-up among those who reported
ponent. The two exceptions are the School Health no or average involvement in severe physical DV
Center Healthy Adolescent Relationships Program at baseline. The program did not have any effects
(SHARP; Miller et al. 2015), which was designed on psychological or severe physical DV victimi-
to be delivered by healthcare providers in school zation. The program was equally effective for
health centers, and the building-only version of boys and girls and for white and minority
the Shifting Boundaries (SB) program (Taylor adolescents.
Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts 879

Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts, Table 1 Universal dating abuse (DA) prevention program characteristics
Target
Type; audience; Theoretical base; Components
Program name setting deliverer mediators (dose) Description
Safe Dates Universal; Audience: Theories: social Classroom-based Safe Dates promotes
school eighth and control, cognitive, curriculum (ten primary and
ninth graders and precaution 45-min sessions), secondary
Deliverer: adoption role-play, poster prevention of DV
teachers Mediators: norms, contest through activities
conflict designed to address D
management knowledge of DV,
skills, belief in DV norms, and
need for help, improvement of
awareness of prosocial skills (e.g.,
services conflict management
skills). Secondary
prevention of DV is
promoted through
encouragement of
help-seeking
Fourth R Universal; Audience: Theory: none Classroom-based Fourth R addresses
school ninth graders specified curriculum DV in the context of
Deliverer: Mediators: peer (21 75-min overlapping areas of
teachers resistance skills sessions), teacher risk behaviors (sexual
training, four activity, substance
parent newsletters, use, and peer
“safe school violence). Sessions
committees” divided into three
units addressing
safety and injury
prevention, healthy
growth and sexuality,
and substance use:
emphasis is on core
relationship issues
and pressures and
teaching skills to
promote safer
relationship decision-
making
Interaction- Universal; Audience: Theory: theory of Classroom-based IB curriculum
Based school sixth and reasoned action curriculum (five addresses
(IB) curriculum seventh Mediators: beliefs, 40-min sessions) boundaries in
graders attitudes relationships,
Deliverer: formation of
rape crisis deliberate
center relationships,
educator determination of
wanted/unwanted
behaviors, and the
role of the bystander
as intervener.
Lessons were
designed to make
students struggle
with “subjectivity
and ambiguity”
(continued)
880 Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts

Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts, Table 1 (continued)


Target
Type; audience; Theoretical base; Components
Program name setting deliverer mediators (dose) Description
Law and Universal; Audience: Theory: theory of Classroom-based LJ curriculum
Justice-Based school sixth and reasoned action curriculum (five focused on laws,
(LJ) curriculum seventh Mediator: 40-min sessions) definitions,
graders knowledge information, and
Deliverer: data about penalties
rape crisis for sexual assault/
center harassment and
educator research about
consequences for
perpetrators of
gender-based
violence/sexual
harassment
Shifting Universal; Audience: Theory: theory of Classroom-based Classroom sessions
Boundaries school sixth and reasoned action curriculum (six address DV
(SB) seventh Mediators: 40-min sessions), consequences, DV
graders knowledge, posters in school laws, relationship
Deliverer: attitudes, buildings, staff boundaries, and
school intentions surveillance of bystander
personnel unsafe areas, intervention.
“respecting Building-based
boundary intervention
agreements” includes hallway
posters, “respecting
boundary
agreements” that
students complete
when they want to
resolve a “boundary
dispute” with
another student, and
increased staff
surveillance of areas
of schools
(“hotspots”)
identified as unsafe
Respect in Universal; Audience: Theory: none Two 45-min RISE presentations
Schools school Seventh specified classroom-based focus on bullying
Everywhere and eighth Mediators: presentations by and gender-based
(RISE) graders knowledge, youth leaders aggression,
Deliverer: attitudes, including sexual
high school emotional school harassment and
age peers adjustment dating aggression.
Presentations are
manualized but were
allowed to be
personalized by
youth leaders
(continued)
Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts 881

Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts, Table 1 (continued)


Target
Type; audience; Theoretical base; Components
Program name setting deliverer mediators (dose) Description
School Health Universal; Audience: Theory: none Provider SHARP aims to
Center Healthy school High school specified discussion and facilitate clinic-based
Adolescent health students age Mediators: assessment of DV, assessment of DV
Relationships centers 14–19 years awareness, provision of a with the premise that
Program Deliverer: intentions to palm-sized discussion of DV
(SHARP) healthcare intervene to brochure, referral during a clinical D
providers prevent to victim service encounter may
relationship abuse, advocate (if DV enhance adolescents’
knowledge of was disclosed). ability to address DV.
resources, and Typical Providers reviewed a
self-efficacy to use discussions brochure that
harm reduction lasted <1 min discusses healthy
behaviors (longer if DV was relationships, how to
disclosed) help a friend, and DV
resources. During the
discussion, providers
screened for DV and
provided “warm”
referral (connecting
the patient to an
advocate via phone/in
person) to services if
DV was disclosed
Families for Universal; Audience: Theory: protection Six self- FSD aims to motivate
Safe Dates family Caregivers motivation administered and facilitate caregiver
(FSD) and their Mediators: booklets delivered engagement in dating
13–15-year- caregiver by mail to families. abuse prevention
old teens protection Each booklet is activities with their
Deliverer: motivation and designed to take teens that can lead to
families (self- date rule setting/ approximately 1 h decreases in risk
delivered) monitoring; teen to complete factors for DV
attitudes, norms, including attitudes,
and conflict norms,
resolution skills communication, and
conflict resolution
skills
Stepping Universal; Audience: Theory: adult 13 3-h sessions, Stepping Stones
Stones community 16–23-year- education theory three peer group aims to improve
old youth Mediators: not meetings, one sexual health
Deliverer: specified community through building
trained meeting gender-equitable
facilitators (approximately partnerships with
(sex matched) 50 h total). better
Delivered to communication
single-sex groups between partners.
Sessions address
sexual risk and
protective behaviors
and outcomes,
gender-based
violence, dealing
with grief and loss,
and communication
skills
882 Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts

Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts, Table 2 Universal dating abuse prevention programs: study character-
istics and findings
Study design; baseline sample Data collection mode;
Program size (n) assessment schedule Findings for DV outcomes
Safe Dates 14 schools randomized to Paper/pencil survey; Across follow-ups effects on
intervention and control immediate post and four psychological, moderate
groups; n = 1885 follow-ups 1.5, 2.5, 3.5, and physical, and sexual
4.5 years post-baseline (1, 2, DVP. Effects on moderate
3, and 4 years post- physical DVV, marginal
intervention) effects on sexual
DVV. Effects on severe
DVV among those
reporting no or average
baseline involvement.
No effects on
psychological or severe
physical DVV
Fourth R 20 schools randomized to Online survey; one follow-up At follow-up, effects on
intervention and control; 2.5 years post-baseline (2 years physical DVP at follow-up
n = 1722 post-intervention) for boys, but not girls
Interaction- 123 sixth- and seventh-grade Paper/pencil survey; At follow-up, no effects of
Based (IB) and classes (in seven schools) immediate post and one follow- the IB program (vs. control)
Law and Justice- randomized to IB, LJ, or up 6 m post-baseline (4–5 m on sexual or physical
Based control; n = 1639 post- intervention) DVP or sexual or physical
(LJ) programs DVV. No effects of the LJ
program (vs. control) on
sexual or physical DVV or
physical DVP. Iatrogenic
effects of LJ program
(vs. control) on frequency of
sexual DVP
Shifting 30 schools randomized to Paper/pencil survey; At follow-up, no effects of
Boundaries combined (building and immediate post and one follow- combined or classroom-only
classroom), building only, up 7–9 m post-baseline (6 m programs (vs. control) on
classroom only, or control; post-intervention) sexual DVP or sexual
n = 2655 DVV. Effects of building-
only (vs. control) program on
sexual DVV, but not sexual
DVP
Respect in 4 schools randomized to Paper/pencil survey; one At follow-up, no effects on
Schools intervention and control; follow-up approximately 7 m composite (psychological,
Everywhere n = 509 post-baseline (4–5 m post- physical, and sexual) DVV
intervention) outcome
School Health 11 school health centers Audio computer-assisted self- At follow-up, effects on
Center Healthy (SHCs) randomized to interview; one follow-up 3 m composite (physical and
Adolescent treatment and control; post-baseline (3 m post- sexual) DVV. Effects on
Relationships n = 1011 intervention) cyber DVV among those
Program reporting any DVV at the
baseline
Families for Families recruited nationwide Computer-assisted telephone At follow-up, effects on
Safe Dates and randomized to treatment interviews; one follow-up 6 m physical DVV. No effects on
and control; n = 464 post-baseline (3 m post- psychological DVV. No
intervention) effects on physical
DVP. Marginal
effects on psychological
DVP
(continued)
Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts 883

Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts, Table 2 (continued)


Study design; baseline sample Data collection mode;
Program size (n) assessment schedule Findings for DV outcomes
Stepping Stones 64 villages and 6 townships Individual face-to-face At both follow-ups marginal
randomized to treatment and interviews, two follow-ups effects on composite
control; n = 2776 1 and 2 years post-baseline (physical and sexual) DVP
(10 and 22 m post-intervention) among boys (not assessed for
girls). No effects on DVV
among girls (not assessed for
boys) at either follow-up D
Note: DAV dating abuse victimization, DAP dating abuse perpetration

Fourth R: Skills for Youth Relationships focused on gender socialization and interaction
(Fourth R). The Fourth R program targets adoles- skills, and the Law and Justice-Based
cents in the ninth grade and includes 21 75-min (LJ) program focused on the laws and conse-
sessions that are designed to be integrated into quences associated with gender violence and sex-
health education classes (Wolfe et al. 2009). Ses- ual harassment. Each curriculum is comprised of
sions are divided into three seven-session units five 40-min sessions. The RCT of the IB and LJ
focused on (1) personal safety and injury preven- programs involved allocation of 123 classrooms
tion, (2) healthy growth and sexuality, and (3) sub- to one of the two programs or to a control group
stance use and abuse. A focus on developing (Taylor et al. 2010a). One follow-up assessment
healthy relationship skills, including assertive was conducted approximately 6 months post-
communication and conflict resolution skills, is intervention. At follow-up, neither the IB nor the
integrated throughout the units. Techniques used LJ program (vs. control) was effective in reducing
to promote skills development include scenario DV perpetration or victimization. In fact, contrary
analysis and role-play opportunities to model to expectations, participants who received the LJ
and practice skills. Unlike other school-based program reported significantly more involvement
DV prevention programs, this program was in sexual dating abuse perpetration than controls.
implemented in sex-segregated classrooms, and The authors suggested that this may have been
activities for boys and girls differed slightly to due to treatment group participants’ increased
maximize relevance and minimize defensiveness recognition of certain behaviors as acts of
on the part of participants. School-level compo- SV. Effects on DV outcomes were not moderated
nents included teacher training on DV and healthy by sex (Taylor et al. 2010b).
relationships, parent newsletters, and student-led Shifting Boundaries (SB). Taylor et al. (2013)
safe school committees. The RCT of Fourth R built on the work they did assessing the IB and LJ
involved random allocation of 20 schools to treat- curriculum described above to assess the effects of
ment and control conditions with one follow-up a modified combined version of the two programs
assessment conducted 2 years post-intervention. with the addition of a school building-wide inter-
Analyses suggest that exposure to the program vention component. Three versions of the inter-
was significantly associated with reduced physi- vention, which was called Shifting Boundaries
cal DV perpetration at follow-up for boys, but not (SB), were developed that target sixth- and
for girls (Wolfe et al. 2009). Effects on other types seventh-grade students. The first was a
of DV perpetration or victimization were not classroom-only version (SB-classroom), which
assessed. synthesized lessons from the IB and LJ programs
Interaction-Based and Law and Justice-Based and comprised six 40-min sessions designed to
Programs. Taylor et al. (2010a) assessed the help students to determine and set personal
effects of two universal programs on DV in a boundaries. Sessions emphasize consequences
sample of early adolescents (sixth and seventh for perpetrators of DV and sexual harassment,
graders). The Interaction-Based (IB) program state and federal laws related to DV and sexual
884 Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts

harassment, the setting and communicating of SH) and the other focuses on bullying. The work-
one’s boundaries in interpersonal relationships, shop presentations are manualized although youth
and the role of bystanders as interveners. The leaders have flexibility to adapt the presentation
second was a building-based intervention format. The workshops are intended to increase
(SB-building) which also aimed to increase knowledge about aggressive behaviors, improve
respect for personal boundaries and comprised attitudes toward aggression (i.e., decrease toler-
the following components: placement of posters ance), and increase emotional school adjustment.
in school buildings to increase awareness and The authors proposed that a violence prevention
reporting of DV and sexual harassment; hotspot program led by youth, as compared to the usual
mapping in which students identified unsafe areas practice of having programs led by teachers or
of schools which were then targeted for greater adults, might be more engaging and influential in
presence of school personnel in those areas; and terms of modifying participants’ attitudes and
temporary school-based restraining orders school adjustment. The RCT of RISE involved
(respecting boundary agreements), which were random assignment of four schools to intervention
forms distributed to school personnel that students and control conditions (Connolly et al. 2015). Stu-
could fill out to resolve a “boundary issue” with dents attending schools in the control condition
another student. The third was a combined inter- received board-mandated “usual practice” anti-
vention (SB-combined) that included both the violence programming led by teachers or other
classroom- and building-based components. adults. One follow-up assessment was conducted
The RCT of SB involved random assignment approximately 4–5 months post-intervention.
of 30 schools to SB-classroom, SB-building, RISE was not associated with DV victimization
SB-combined, and control conditions. A follow- (composite of psychological, physical, and sex-
up assessment was conducted approximately ual) at follow-up; results were consistent for boys
6 months post-intervention. At follow-up there and girls and across racial/ethnic subgroups. DV
were no effects of the SB-combined or perpetration was not assessed.
SB-classroom interventions (vs. control) on School Health Center Healthy Adolescent
sexual DV (perpetration or victimization). Relationships Program (SHARP). SHARP is a
SB-building (vs. control) was associated with brief education and counseling intervention
reduced sexual DV victimization, but was not designed to be delivered by healthcare providers
associated with sexual DV perpetration. Effects in school health centers (SHCs, comprehensive
on these DV outcomes were not moderated by sex clinics located in high schools) during routine
or history of DV (Taylor et al. 2015). Effects on healthcare visits (Miller et al. 2015). Delivery of
other types of DV (i.e., psychological, physical) the SHARP requires that healthcare providers
were not assessed. engage all patients, regardless of their reason for
Respect in Schools Everywhere (RISE). seeking care, in a discussion about healthy and
Connolly et al. (2015) developed and evaluated unhealthy relationships guided by a palm-sized
RISE, which is a brief youth-led program for brochure. During this discussion SHARP pro-
preventing dating abuse as well as other youth viders screen for DV and, if DV is disclosed,
violence outcomes (bullying, sexual harassment) conduct a warm referral (i.e., connecting a patient
among middle school youth. The youth leaders of to an advocate by phone or in person) to a victim
the program are selected from local high schools service advocate. SHARP providers reported that
and are required to participate in 16 after-school this discussion typically lasted <1 min but could
sessions during which they are provided with last longer if DV was disclosed. In addition to the
education in leadership skills and aggression pre- provider-delivered intervention, SHARP SHCs
vention. The youth leaders then prepare and pre- engage with youth advisory boards to organize
sent two 45-min classroom workshops (the RISE school-wide outreach events to raise awareness
program) to seventh and eighth graders; one about DV and encourage student use of SHCs.
workshop focuses on gender violence (DV and The RCT of SHARP involved random assignment
Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts 885

of 11 SHCs to treatment and control conditions note that the study was not powered to detect
(Miller et al. 2015). One follow-up assessment effects on DV behaviors. Effects were consistent
was conducted approximately 3 months post- for boys and girls.
intervention. Analyses of effects on DV at Stepping Stones. Jewkes et al. (2008) evaluated
follow-up were stratified by baseline DV status. the effects of Stepping Stones, a community-based
Among those who reported no past 3-month DV program, on dating abuse behavior in a sample of
victimization at baseline, exposure to SHARP was 16- to 23-year-old South African youth. Stepping
associated with reduced onset of physical or sex- Stones is a prevention program that aims to reduce
ual (composite measure) DV victimization; no sexual risk behavior and partner violence through D
association was found with onset of cyber DV building gender-equitable relationships with bet-
victimization. Among those who reported any ter communication between partners. The pro-
past 3-month victimization at baseline, SHARP gram is delivered to single-sex groups, is run in
was associated with reduced risk of both physical parallel, and includes 13 core 3-h-long sessions as
or sexual (composite measure) and cyber DV well as three meetings where the peer groups
victimization. Intervention effects were the same come together that include activities designed to
for boys and girls. Effects on DV perpetration promote dialogue about session topics. The ses-
were not assessed. sions are delivered by trained sex-matched facili-
Families for Safe Dates (FSD). FSD is a tators and use participatory learning approaches
family-based program that is comprised of six (role-play, drama, critical reflection) to engage
booklets mailed to caregivers of 13–15-year-old participants in critical reflection about motiva-
adolescents (Foshee et al. 2012). Booklets include tions for behavior. Session topics include sexual
interactive activities for caregivers and adoles- behavior and motivations, sex and love, concep-
cents to do together that are designed to increase tion and contraception, negative consequences of
caregivers’ motivation for talking to their teens sexual risk behavior, safer sex and condoms,
about DV, improve their knowledge of DV, and gender-based violence, dealing with grief and
increase their date rule setting; alter adolescent loss, and communication skills. The RCT of
norms related to DV and improve their conflict Stepping Stones involved randomization of 64 vil-
management skills; and improve communication lages and 6 townships to intervention and control
between adolescents and caregivers, so that there conditions (Jewkes et al. 2008). Two follow-up
would be less family conflict and easier discussion interviews were conducted approximately 10 and
of dating and DV issues. Two weeks after being 22 months post-intervention (1 and 2 years post-
mailed a booklet, the caregiver was contacted via baseline). Analyses of program effects on DV
telephone by a health educator who assessed outcomes were stratified by sex. DV victimiza-
booklet completion, answered questions, encour- tion, but not DV perpetration, was assessed
aged participation, and mailed the family the next among women. DV perpetration, but not DV vic-
booklet if indicated. The RCT of FSD involved timization, was assessed among men. The pro-
random assignment of 464 caregiver–teen dyads gram was marginally associated with reduced
to intervention and control conditions (Foshee physical or sexual (composite measure) DV per-
et al. 2012). Families were recruited across the petration among men (at 1-year follow-up,
nation via targeted telephone lists; one follow-up p = 0.09; at 2-year follow-up, p = 0.054); how-
telephone interview was conducted with each ever, the program was not associated with physi-
member of the dyad approximately 3 months cal or sexual (composite measure) DV
post-intervention. FSD was significantly associ- victimization among women at either follow-up.
ated with reduced physical DV victimization and Summary of Findings of Studies Evaluating
marginally (p = 0.09) associated with reduced Universal Programs. Five of the nine universal
psychological DV perpetration. No effects were programs had at least one significant effect on a
found for psychological DV victimization or DV outcome (Safe Dates, Fourth R, Shifting
physical DV perpetration, although the authors Boundaries, SHARP, and Families for Safe
886 Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts

Dates), and one program (Stepping Stone) had a population. Tracks within schools were randomly
marginally significant effect. Only one program, allocated to the treatment or control condition, and
Safe Dates, had effects on both victimization and one follow-up was conducted 6 months post-
perpetration outcomes. intervention. At follow-up, no significant effects
were found on DV perpetration (composite of
Selective Programs psychological, physical, and sexual); these results
The six selective programs selected for review were consistent for boys and girls. Effects on DV
targeted the following subgroups: male athletes victimization were not assessed.
(one program), low-income minority adolescents Coaching Boys into Men (CBIM). The CBIM
(one program), Hispanic adolescents (two pro- program was developed for male student ath-
grams), pregnant adolescent girls (one program), letes based on research suggesting that male
and pregnant adolescent girls and their partners athletes are more likely to engage in DV perpe-
(one program). However, two of these programs, tration and endorse attitudes accepting of vio-
Ending Violence: A Curriculum for Educating lence against women than males who are not
Teens on Domestic Violence and the Law (EV; athletes (Miller et al. 2012). The program aims
Jaycox et al. 2006) and It’s Your Game: Keep It to engage athletic coaches as positive influential
Real (IYG; Peskin et al. 2014), while evaluated, role models to deliver violence prevention mes-
respectively, with populations comprised of sages to their athletes. The program comprises
Latino teens and low-income minority teens, do 11 “training cards” that guide coaches to lead
not appear to have been developed explicitly for 10–15 min weekly discussions with their ath-
selective (i.e., exclusive) implementation with letes throughout the sports season. Through
those populations (i.e., these two interventions these discussions the program aims to increase
could also potentially be classified as universal). awareness and recognition of DV, increase
Four of the selective interventions were school- gender-equitable attitudes, and increase inten-
based and three were community-based. tions to intervene as bystanders with peers.
A summary of program and study characteristics Changes in these mediating factors were, in
and findings for each of the selective programs is turn, hypothesized to lead to decreased DV per-
presented in Tables 3 and 4, respectively. petration and increased bystander intervention
Ending Violence: A Curriculum for Educating in response to peer perpetration of abuse. The
Teens on Domestic Violence and the Law (EV). RCT of CBIM involved randomization of
The EV program is comprised of three 1-h ses- 16 high schools to intervention and control con-
sions focusing on legal aspects of dating abuse ditions (Miller et al. 2012, 2013). Athletic direc-
that were taught by attorneys (Jaycox et al. 2006). tors in each school approached all head coaches
The program aims to increase knowledge of DV, of male and coeducational sports to participate.
decrease acceptance of DV, promote help seeking, One follow-up assessment was conducted
and reduce perpetration and victimization. Topics approximately 9 months post-intervention
addressed in the session include ways to recognize (1 year post-baseline). Findings suggest that
DV; barriers to help seeking; domestic violence CBIM was effective in reducing DV perpetra-
laws, including rights and responsibilities of per- tion (composite of psychological, physical, and
petrators and victims; ways to obtain protective sexual DV) at follow-up. Effects on DV victim-
orders; and ways for DV victims to stay safe. The ization were not assessed.
RCT of EV was conducted with ninth graders in It’s Your Game: Keep It Real (IYG). The IYG
11 high schools with greater than 80% Latino program is a sexually transmitted infection (STI)
students (Jaycox et al. 2006). Although this pro- and pregnancy prevention program, but effects of
gram was not developed specifically to address the program on DV outcomes were also assessed
Latino culture, the approach of emphasizing legal (Tortolero et al. 2010; Peskin et al. 2014). The
aspects of dating abuse delivered by lawyers was program, which was developed for implementa-
justified based on a knowledge gap in the target tion with ethnic-minority middle school youth,
Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts 887

Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts, Table 3 Selective dating abuse (DA) prevention program characteristics
Program Type; Target audience; Theoretical base;
name setting deliverer mediators Components (dose) Description
Ending Universal; Audience: ninth Theory: social Classroom-based Ending Violence
Violence school graders learning theory curriculum (three 1-h teaches youth
Deliverer: Mediators: sessions) about laws that
attorneys knowledge, norms, protect victims/
help seeking punish perpetrators
of DV. Emphasizes
legal dimensions D
of DV and
increasing comfort
seeking legal
services
Coaching Selective; Audience: male Theory: social 11 training cards CBIM aims to alter
Boys into school high school norms that guide coaches DV norms and
Men athletes Mediators: gender- to lead brief promote bystander
(CBIM) Deliverer: equitable attitudes, (10–15 min) weekly intervention
coaches awareness of group discussions among male
abusive behaviors, with athletes about athletes. The
intention to respect and DV program engages
intervene as prevention athletic coaches as
bystanders role models. Cards
guide coaches to
lead discussions to
raise awareness of
DV, promote
nonviolent
definitions of
masculinity, and
model bystander
intervention
It’s Your Selective; Audience: Theory: social 24 45-min lessons IYG aims to teach
Game: school low-income, cognitive, social (12 in seventh students to select
Keep It minority seventh influence grade, 12 in eighth personal limits,
Real (IYG) and eighth Mediators: grade), detect signs or
graders knowledge, refusal 12 parent–child situations that
Deliverer: and condom use homework challenge those
trained facilitator self-efficacy, activities. Includes limits, and use
normative beliefs group-based and refusal skills and
related to sexual individual other tactics to
behavior, risk computer-based protect those
exposure activities limits. Session
topics include
relationship
norms; setting
personal limits
and practicing
refusal skills;
information
about HIV, STIs,
and pregnancy;
and contraceptive
use skills
training
(continued)
888 Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts

Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts, Table 3 (continued)


Program Type; Target audience; Theoretical base;
name setting deliverer mediators Components (dose) Description
Juntos Selective: Audience: Theory: Six group sessions JOVEN was
Opuestos a community Hispanic ninth ecodevelopmental, for adolescents, two designed for
la Violencia graders social cognitive sessions for parents, Hispanic youth.
(JOVEN) Deliverer: Mediators: not two sessions for Session topics
research staff specified school personnel include relationship
(session length norms, Hispanic
unspecified) traditions, risky
substance use; safer
sexual; legal rights
and services;
conflict
management and
assertive
communication.
Parent sessions
addressed on DV
and effective
monitoring and
communication
practices
Young Selective; Audience: Theory: family Ten 75-min couple YPP was designed
Parenthood community 14–18-year-old systems and counseling sessions for pregnant
Program pregnant adolescent delivered during adolescent couples
(YPP) adolescents and developmental pregnancy and aims to promote
their partners theory. Mediators: (10 weeks +/ coparenting and
Deliverer: positive 2 weeks) paternal
master’s level relationship skills engagement. YPP
clinicians related to phases are
coparenting, designed to educate
positive paternal couples about
engagement coparenting, child
development, and
the parenting
transition; set
relationship goals
and identify skills
needed to reach
goals; and develop
communication and
self-regulation
skills
Building a Selective; Audience: Theory: social Four 90-min BALL was designed
Lasting community pregnant/ learning sessions for pregnant and
Love parenting Mediators: parenting African-
(BALL) adolescents communication American
Deliverer: skills, emotion adolescent girls.
research staff regulation Sessions address
strategies DV awareness,
safety planning,
healthy
relationships,
communication
skills, emotion
regulation, and
coping
Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts 889

Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts, Table 4 Selective dating abuse prevention programs: study character-
istics and findings
Study design; baseline sample Data collection mode;
Program size (n) assessment schedule Findings for DV outcomes
Ending 47 school tracks (in ten schools) Paper/pencil survey; immediate At follow-up, no effects on
Violence randomized to intervention and post and one follow-up 6 m composite (psychological,
control; n = 2540 post-baseline (6 m post- physical, and sexual) DVV
intervention) outcome
Coaching 16 schools randomized to Online survey; baseline, At follow-up, effects on
Boys into treatment and control; n = 2006 immediate post and one follow- composite (psychological, D
Men up 1 y post-baseline (9 m post- physical, and sexual) DVP
intervention) outcome
It’s Your 10 schools randomized to Audio computer-assisted self- At follow-up, effects on
Game: treatment and control; n = 981 interview; two follow-ups 18 m psychological (H only) and
Keep It (3 m post-intervention) and 2 y physical DVV (AA only) and
Real post-baseline (9 m post- psychological DVP (boys only,
intervention) H only). No effects on physical
DVP
Juntos Adolescents from within one Self-administered paper and Across follow-ups no effects on
Opuestos a school were randomized to pencil surveys; immediate post composite (physical and
la Violencia intervention and waitlist control (1 w post-intervention) and two sexual) DVV or DVP; no
Entre conditions; n = 82 follow-ups 3 and 12 m post- effects on psychological DVV
Novios intervention (how long after or psychological DVP
baseline not specified)
YPP Adolescents receiving teen Semi-structured face-to-face At 3-m follow-up, marginal
pregnancy services from clinics individual interviews with each effects (p = 0.06) on levels of
and schools and their partners member of their couple; physical DVP among couples
randomized to intervention and baseline (second trimester of (DVP score was computed as
usual care; n = 105 pregnancy) and two follow-ups average of both partners’
3 m post-birth (4–6 m post- reports). No effects on DVP at
intervention, 7–9 m post- 18-m follow-up
baseline) and 18 m post-birth
(19–21 m post-intervention,
22–24 m post-baseline)
BALL Adolescents receiving teen Self-administered paper and At post, marginal effects on
pregnancy services randomized pencil surveys; baseline and psychological DVP and severe
to intervention or waitlist immediate post 6 w post- physical DVV. No effects on
control; n = 72 baseline (1–2 w post- mild or severe physical DVP,
intervention) psychological DVV, or mild
physical DVV
Note: y year, m months, w weeks, DAV dating abuse victimization, DAP dating abuse perpetration

includes 24 45-min classroom sessions; half of the and the importance of HIV, STI, and pregnancy
sessions are delivered in the seventh grade and the testing; and skills training on condom and contra-
other half are delivered in the eighth grade. Class- ceptive use. The program also includes six
room sessions integrate group-based activities parent–child homework activities in each grade
with individual activities delivered on laptop that are designed to promote communication
computers that include quizzes, animations, peer about friendship, dating, and sexual behavior.
video, fact sheets, “real-world” style teen serials, The RCT of IYG involved randomization of ten
and journaling. Session topics include healthy schools to intervention and control conditions
relationships; setting personal limits; practicing (Tortolero et al. 2010; Peskin et al. 2014). Partic-
refusal skills in both general (e.g., regarding sub- ipating schools had student populations that were
stance use) and sexual behavior-specific contexts; predominantly low income and ethnic minority
information on puberty, reproduction, and STIs (39% African-American, 45% Hispanic). Trained
890 Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts

facilitators delivered the intervention. One follow- intervention and a time-delayed control condition
up assessment was conducted 1 year post- (Gonzalez-Guarda et al. 2015). Research staff
intervention. Analyses of effects on DV outcomes delivered the intervention which was not
assessed the main effects of the intervention on implemented during school hours (the specific
DV behaviors as well as moderation by sex and location(s) where the program was implemented
race/ethnicity (Peskin et al. 2014). Moderation was not specified). Two follow-up assessments
analyses suggest that the effects of IYG on phys- were administered 3 and 12 months post-
ical and emotional DV victimization were consis- intervention. Across follow-ups, no effects were
tent for boys and girls but differed by found on DV (composite of physical, sexual, and
race–ethnicity. In particular, IYG was effective psychological) victimization or perpetration.
in preventing physical victimization among Effects were consistent for boys and girls.
African-American, but not Hispanic youth. In Young Parenthood Program (YPP). The YPP is
contrast, IYG was effective in preventing emo- a 10-week counseling intervention designed for
tional victimization among Hispanic, but not pregnant adolescent couples that aims to support
African-American youth. Effects on emotional the development of relationship skills needed for
DV perpetration varied by sex and by positive coparenting and parenting (Florsheim
race–ethnicity. IYG was effective in preventing et al. 2011, 2012). The intervention is adminis-
emotional abuse perpetration among boys, but tered by master’s level counselors who follow an
not girls, and among Hispanic but not African- intervention manual. Intervention sessions lasted
American youth (note three-way interactions an average of 75 min and are typically provided
between intervention condition, sex, and race once a week. The program is divided into five
were not examined). No main or moderated phases focused on the following: (1) establishing
effects of the program were found for physical a therapeutic alliance and educating couples on
DV perpetration (Peskin et al. 2014). the link between coparenting and child develop-
Juntos Opuestos a la Violencia (JOVEN). The ment; (2) setting relationship goals and determin-
JOVEN program was designed for implementa- ing what skills are needed to reach those goals;
tion with Hispanic 13- to 16-year-old adolescents (3) communication and self-regulation skills
(Gonzalez-Guarda et al. 2015). The program is development; (4) negotiating the transition to par-
comprised of six group sessions for adolescents, enthood; and (5) other coparenting issues, includ-
two group sessions for parents, and two sessions ing family planning. The RCT of YPP involved
for school personnel (session length unspecified). randomization of 105 pregnant 14–18-year-old
Sessions include educational and skill-building adolescents and their coparenting partner to inter-
activities delivered using videos, music, group vention and control conditions (Florsheim
discussion, and other modalities. Topics of the et al. 2011). Two follow-up assessments were
sessions for adolescents include Hispanic tradi- conducted 4–6 months post-intervention
tions, pop culture, and dating; healthy and (3 months post-birth) and 19–21 months post-
unhealthy relationships; sex and drugs; legal intervention (12 months post-birth). For analyses
rights and accessing services; and bystander and of intervention effects on DV outcomes, partners’
assertive communication skills. Sessions for par- reports of physical DV perpetration and victimi-
ents were designed to raise awareness about DV zation were averaged at each assessment point.
and community resources and increase effective That is, reciprocal, male-to-female, and female-
parenting and communication practices. Sessions to-male violence were collapsed into a single IPV
for school personnel were designed to increase score denoting whether any violence had been
effective mentoring of youth on relationships. experienced or perpetrated by either member of
The RCT of JOVEN involved randomization of the couple. Findings suggest the intervention was
82 ninth-grade Hispanic adolescents, all of whom marginally effective in preventing DV at the first
were recruited from one public school, and their follow-up (3 months post-birth, p = 0.06), but
participating parent or legal guardian to an these marginal effects were not sustained at the
Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts 891

second follow-up (18 months post-birth, domestic violence (one program). Two of the
p = 0.13). interventions were community-based and one
Building a Lasting Love (BALL). The BALL was family-based. A summary of program and
program was developed for African-American study characteristics and findings for each of the
girls receiving teen pregnancy services indicated programs is presented in Tables 5 and 6,
(Langhinrichsen-Rohling and Turner 2012). The respectively.
program is comprised of four 90-min stand-alone Youth Relationship Project (YRP). The YRP
group sessions. Sessions were designed to program targets 14–16-year-old adolescents with
improve awareness of DV, communication skills, a history of maltreatment and is comprised of D
emotion regulation, and coping skills. Session 18 2-h sessions taught by social workers and
topics included healthy relationships and safety other community professionals, with male and
planning; coping with anger and disrespect; asser- female co-facilitators leading each session
tive communication and conflict management; (Wolfe et al. 1996). The program is designed to
and stress, coping, and time management skills. increase knowledge about DV, alter attitudes
Sessions, which were specified in a facilitator’s toward DV, increase understanding of power
manual and participant workbooks, included dynamics in relationships, improve communica-
material to be taught didactically as well as facil- tion and problem-solving skills, increase aware-
itated group discussions and other content-related ness of gender stereotypes that contribute to DV,
activities. The RCT of BALL involved randomi- and increase participants’ help-seeking abilities.
zation of 72 adolescent girls receiving teen preg- Teaching techniques include role modeling and
nancy services to intervention and control behavioral rehearsal, value clarification and
conditions (Langhinrichsen-Rohling and Turner problem-solving activities, and field trips to com-
2012). One post-program assessment was munity agencies. The RCT of YRP was conducted
conducted immediately after the last program ses- with 191 maltreated teens who were randomized
sion (6 weeks post-baseline); no other follow-up to intervention and control conditions (Wolfe
assessments were conducted. BALL was margin- et al. 2003). Follow-up assessments were
ally associated with decreased psychological DV conducted every 2 months for up to 20 months
perpetration (p = 0.08) and decreased severe post-intervention. Exposure to YRB was associ-
physical violence victimization (p = 0.09). No ated with reduced physical DV perpetration and
effects were found for psychological DV victim- reduced physical and psychological DV victimi-
ization, moderate physical DV victimization, or zation. There were no significant effects on psy-
moderate or severe physical DV violence chological DV perpetration. The effects on
perpetration. physical abuse victimization were greater for
Summary of Findings for Selective Programs. boys than for girls; all other effects did not differ
Two of the five selective programs had at least one by sex.
significant effect on a DV outcome (CBIM and SafERteens. The SafERteens program targets
IYG) and two programs (YPP and BALL) had 14–18-year-old adolescents seeking care at an
marginally significant effects. Only one program, emergency department who screened positive
IYG, had effects on both victimization and perpe- for past-year violence and alcohol use
tration outcomes. (Cunningham et al. 2009, 2013). The program
comprises a brief (30-min) counseling and edu-
Indicated Programs cation intervention based on principles of moti-
The three indicated programs selected for review vational interviewing (MI), which focuses on
targeted the following populations: adolescents “. . ..enhancing motivation to change in a respect-
who had experienced child maltreatment (one ful, non-confrontational, and non-judgmental man-
program), adolescents who screened positive for ner” (Cunningham et al. 2009, p. 6). Activities are
youth violence and alcohol use (one program), designed to develop a discrepancy between current
and adolescents who had been exposed to behavior and future goals/values, reset norms,
892 Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts

Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts, Table 5 Indicated dating abuse (DA) prevention program characteristics
Target
Program Type; audience; Theoretical Components
name setting deliverer base; mediators (dose) Description
Youth Indicated; Audience: Theory: 18 sessions YRP was designed for youth
Relationships community 14–16-year- cognitive (15 2-h who had experienced
Project (YRP) old developmental; education maltreatment and aims to
maltreated feminist sessions, promote the development of
youth Mediators: 2 sessions adaptive nonviolent
Deliverer: healthy related to communication and problem-
trained relationship visiting solving skills. Sessions focus
facilitators skills, community on education and awareness,
emotional agencies, skills development, and social
distress 1 closing action activities to increase
session) help seeking
SafERteens Indicated; Audience: Theory: One 30-min SafERteens was designed for
community 14–18-year- cognitive session with high-risk inner-city youth and
old youth dissonance; two delivery uses adaptive motivational
reporting social learning modes: interviewing to enhance
past-year Mediators: computer- motivation and self-efficacy to
violence and norms, self- guided change, reset norms, and
alcohol use efficacy, intervention develop refusal, conflict
Deliverer: readiness to (CBI) and resolution, and emotion
self- change therapist- regulation skills
administered guided
(computer- intervention
guided (TBI)
mode)/
therapists
(therapist-
guided
mode)
Moms and Indicated; Audience: Theory: Six self- MTSD was designed for teens
Teens for Safe family Mothers who protection administered exposed to domestic violence
Dates have motivation booklets and was adapted from the
(MTSD) experienced Mediators: delivered by Families for Safe Dates (FSD)
domestic factors related mail to program (described above
violence and to mothers’ mothers who under universal programs).
their 12–16- program have MTSD aims to engage
year-old engagement; experienced mothers’ in DV prevention
teens mothers’ rule domestic and change teen risk factors
Deliverer: setting and violence and associated with
families monitoring; no longer live DV. Adaptations to the
(self- teen DV norms, with their program that were changes
delivered) gender abuser. from the original FSD
stereotyping, Booklets take program included increased
and conflict approximately emphasis on increasing
resolution skills 1 h to complete mothers’ self-efficacy to
address DV and beliefs that
their behaviors can influence
DV. In addition new content
addressed gender stereotyping

increase motivation and self-efficacy for change, organizational formal but differ in their mode of
and develop refusal skills. Two different versions delivery and presentation. The first version was
(or modes) of SafERteens were developed that designed to be delivered by research therapists
were designed to have the same content and trained in MI (therapist-based intervention, TBI)
Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts 893

Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts, Table 6 Indicated dating abuse (DA) prevention programs: study
characteristics and findings
Study design; baseline sample Data collection mode;
Program size (n) assessment schedule Findings for DV outcomes
Youth Adolescents with a history of Structured face-to-face Across follow-ups effects on
Relationships maltreatment randomized to interviews; immediate post and physical DVP, psychological
Project intervention or control; then follow-ups every 2 m until DVV, and physical DVV;
n = 191 final assessment at 24 m post- stronger effects on physical
baseline (20 m post- DVV for boys than for girls.
intervention) No effects on psychological D
DVP
SafERteens Adolescents seeking care at an Self-administered CBI (vs. control) had effects on
emergency department who computerized assessments; moderate physical DVV at
reported past-year violence and three follow-ups 3 m, 6 m, and 3 and 6 m, but not at 12-m
alcohol use randomized to 1 y post-baseline/-intervention follow-up; effects were
computer-based intervention stronger for those reporting
(CBI), therapist-based higher levels of the outcome at
intervention (TBI), or control baseline. CBI was not
conditions. DV analysis associated with severe physical
restricted to those reporting any DVV any follow-up. TBI
past-year DVV or DVP; (vs. control) had effects on
n = 397 moderate physical DVVamong
those reporting higher baseline
levels at 6 m and 12 m. TBI
(vs. control) had effects on
severe physical DVV among
those reporting higher baseline
levels at 3 m, but not at 6- or
12-m follow-up. Neither CBI
nor TBI was associated with
moderate or severe physical
DVP at any follow-up
Moms and Mother–adolescent dyads Telephone interviews; one At follow-up, effects on
Teens for Safe randomized to treatment and follow-up 9 m post-baseline psychological and physical
Dates control; n = 409 (6 m post-intervention) DVV and psychological and
cyber DVP for adolescents
with high, but not average or
low exposure to domestic
violence. No effects on cyber
or sexual DVV or physical or
sexual DVP
Note: DAV dating abuse victimization, DAP dating abuse perpetration

who use tablet laptop computers to provide person- who guides them through the intervention
alized feedback from screening and baseline sur- elements.
veys. Therapists guide participants through “Safer The RCT of SafERteens involved randomiza-
Choices” role-play scenarios selected by the com- tion of 533 eligible adolescents to computer-based
puter using preprogrammed algorithms. For exam- (CBI), therapist-based (TBI), or control condi-
ple, when participants reported they had been tions (Cunningham et al. 2013). Follow-up assess-
involved in dating abuse, role-play scenarios were ments were conducted 3, 6, and 12 months post-
presented on this topic. The second version was intervention. Analysis of the effects of SafERteens
designed as an interactive multimedia computer on DV outcomes was restricted to those reporting
program auto-administered via tablet laptops any past-year DV at baseline because these partic-
(computer-based intervention, CBI). The CBI ver- ipants received tailored DV intervention messages
sion of SafERteens is narrated by a virtual “buddy” in addition to the peer violence and alcohol use
894 Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts

messages. Exposure to the CBI (vs. control) was 6 months post-intervention. Analysis of interven-
associated with reduced moderate physical DV tion effects first tested whether effects varied by
victimization at the 3- and 6-month follow-ups, four a priori identified moderators (mother’s psy-
but not at 12-month follow-up. These short-term chological health, amount of adolescent exposure
effects were stronger for those reporting higher to DV, adolescent sex, and race/ethnicity). Pro-
levels of victimization at baseline. The CBI was gram effects were found on psychological and
not associated with severe physical DV victimi- physical DV victimization and psychological
zation across any of the follow-ups. Exposure to and cyber DV perpetration for adolescents with
the TBI (vs. control) was associated with reduc- high, but not average or low, exposure to domestic
tions in moderate DV victimization at both the 6- violence. There were no main or moderated
and 12-month follow-ups among those reporting effects of MTSD on cyber or sexual DV victimi-
higher levels of victimization at baseline. The TBI zation or physical or sexual DV perpetration.
(vs. control) was also associated with reduced Summary of Findings for Indicated Programs.
severe physical DV victimization at the 3-month All of indicated programs had at least one signif-
but not at the 6- or 12-month follow-ups, among icant effect on a DVoutcome. Two programs, YRP
those reporting higher levels of baseline victimi- and MTSD, had effects on both victimization and
zation. Neither the CBI nor the TBI (vs. control) perpetration outcomes.
was associated with moderate or severe physical
DV perpetration across any of the follow-ups.
Moms and Teens for Safe Dates (MTSD). The Global Summary of Study Findings
MTSD program targets 12–16-year-old adoles-
cents who were exposed to domestic violence Main Program Effects
against their mothers (Foshee et al. 2015c). The Just over half (n = 10, 56%) of the 18 programs
program was adapted from the FSD program had at least one statistically significant (p < 0.05)
(reviewed above under universal programs) and effect on a DV outcome in the expected direction
is comprised of six self-administered booklets (i.e., preventing or reducing DV). Six programs
which are delivered by mail and take approxi- (Safe Dates, Fourth R, CBIM, IYG, YRP, and
mately 1 h each to complete. Each booklet con- SafERteens) were shown to have sustained effects
tains interactive activities designed to engage more than 6 months post-intervention; however
mothers in DV prevention; increase mother mon- only three programs (Safe Dates, IYG, and YRP)
itoring of the adolescent; increase family close- were found to have sustained effects on both
ness and cohesion; and change teen risk factors victimization and perpetration outcomes and on
associated with DV including conflict manage- multiple types of DV (i.e., psychological, physi-
ment skills, DV norms, and gender stereotyping. cal, and/or sexual).
A systematic process was followed to adapt FSD Programs varied widely in terms of their length
for the target population (mothers exposed to and intensity (i.e., dose) ranging from a
domestic violence and their teens); example adap- two-session workshop (RISE) to a curriculum
tations included increased emphasis on increasing comprised of 21 75-min sessions (Fourth R).
mothers’ self-efficacy to address DV and beliefs Overall findings suggest that the more intensive
that their behaviors can influence DV as well as school-based programs (e.g., Safe Dates, Fourth
new content addressing gender stereotyping R, IYG) may have been more effective than shorter
(Foshee et al. 2015c). The RCT of MTSD (i.e.,  6 sessions) school-based programs (IB, LJ,
involved randomization of mother–teen dyads to RISE, SB-classroom only, EV, JOVEN), all of
intervention and control conditions (Foshee which were found to not have any favorable
et al. 2015b). To be eligible for the study, mothers effects on DV victimization or perpetration. How-
had to have been in an abusive relationship but ever, it is possible that differences in program
currently living apart from the abusive partner. content rather than duration account for differ-
One follow-up assessment was completed ences in these effects. Further, findings suggest
Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts 895

that some of the “brief” programs that did not on severe physical DV victimization were stron-
involve implementation of a classroom-based cur- ger for those whose baseline risk was lower. In
riculum (e.g., SB-building only, SHARP, CBIM, contrast the effects of SHARP (Miller et al. 2015)
and SafERteens) may have promise for and SafERteens (Cunningham et al. 2013) were
preventing DV. found to be stronger for those with higher baseline
Overall, more programs had statistically sig- risk. Baseline history of DV was not found to
nificant effects on psychological and physical DV moderate the effects of the Shifting Boundaries
than on sexual DV. Specifically, in terms of vic- program on DV outcomes (Taylor et al. 2013).
timization outcomes, three programs were shown Only one of the four studies that examined D
to have significant effects on psychological DV race/ethnicity as a potential moderator found dif-
victimization (IYG, YRP, and MTSD), five pro- ferential program effects, and the direction of
grams were shown to have effects on physical these findings differed depending on the type of
DV victimization (Safe Dates, FSD, IYG, DV being considered (Peskin et al. 2014). In
SafERteens, and MTSD), and only one program particular, IYG was found to reduce psychological
was shown to have effects on sexual DV victim- DV (victimization and perpetration) among His-
ization (Shifting Boundaries-building only). In panic but not African-American youth. In con-
terms of perpetration outcomes, three programs trast, the effects of IYG on physical DV
were shown to have significant effects on psycho- victimization were significant for African-
logical DV perpetration (Safe Dates, IYG, and American but not Hispanic youth (Peskin
MTSD), three programs were shown to have et al. 2014). All but two of the 12 studies that
effects on physical DV perpetration (Safe Dates, examined sex differences found no sex differ-
Fourth R, and YRP), and only one program was ences in program effects on DV behaviors. The
shown have effects on sexual DV perpetration two that found sex differences (Fourth R and IYG)
(Safe Dates). The effects of two programs both found that the programs prevented DV per-
(SHARP and CBIM) on specific types of victimi- petration among boys but not girls (Wolfe
zation and perpetration could not be determined et al. 2009; Peskin et al. 2014).
because composite measures were used.
Mediation of Program Effects
Moderation of Program Effects The assessment of the processes through which a
Most of the studies selected for review (n = 14, program works to prevent or reduce DV can
82%) examined at least one potential moderator of inform scientific understanding of why the inter-
program effects. Such analyses are important for vention worked. Although several of the studies
determining the generalizability of program (n = 12, 70%) assessed the effects of the pro-
effects across different subpopulations participat- grams on risk factors for DV that were targeted
ing in the study. Commonly examined moderators for change by program activities, only two studies
included baseline levels of the DV outcome conducted formal mediation analysis to identify
(4 studies), race/ethnicity (4 studies), and sex the mechanisms explaining the effects of the pro-
(12 studies). Studies that examined baseline DV gram on DV (Foshee et al. 2005, 2015b). Ana-
as a moderator sought to determine whether pro- lyses of the Safe Dates program suggest that
gram effects varied depending on whether or the effects on DV were mediated primarily by
extent to which an adolescent had engaged in changes in acceptance of DV, gender stereo-
and/or experienced DV prior to program partici- typing, and awareness of community services,
pation. Three of the four studies that examined not through improvements in conflict manage-
baseline DV history as a potential moderator ment skills or belief in need for help (Foshee
found differential program effects on at least one et al. 2005). Analyses of MTSD found that ado-
DV outcome (Foshee et al. 2005; Miller lescent acceptance of DV, conflict management
et al. 2015; Cunningham et al. 2013). Foshee skills, and feelings of family closeness and cohe-
et al. (2005) found that the effects of Safe Dates sion mediated the effects of MTSD on DV
896 Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts

outcomes among teens with higher exposure to to detect any effects on DV outcomes, particularly
domestic violence; there were no significant indi- for less prevalent behaviors such as severe phys-
rect effects of the program on DV outcomes ical and sexual DV. Some studies appeared to
through any of the targeted mediators for teens have been powered to detect program effects in
with lower exposure to domestic violence the full sample, but analyses of program effects
(Foshee et al. 2015b). We note, however, that were conducted in samples that were stratified by
the analytic approaches used to assess mediation sex, race, or baseline DV status (e.g., SHARP,
in both the Safe Dates and MTSD studies did not IYG), and those analyses may thus have been
address temporality between the mediators and underpowered. None of the studies conducted a
the outcomes; as such it is unclear whether cost-effectiveness analysis which can provide
changes in the mediators were, in fact, prospec- information that is very useful for agencies when
tively associated with changes in DV. Despite making decisions about choosing which programs
this limitation, these mediation analyses are use- to implement in their communities.
ful in that they suggest that program components In addition to these limitations, as noted above,
targeting these particular risk and protective fac- almost no studies went beyond establishing a
tors are important for achieving effects on DV main effect of the program to examine why the
behavior and thus should not be cut in program program was or was not effective in producing
adaptations. change in risk for DV. In some cases, we believe
this may be due to a lack of specification of the
program. For example, in several studies, the par-
Recommendations for Future Directions ticular behavioral determinants (i.e., risk and pro-
tective factors for DV) being targeted by the
Overall the number of programs that met the program and/or their theoretical or empirical link-
selection criteria for this review was small (n = age to DV victimization and/or perpetration were
18). Clearly more research is needed to expand the not described; only five of the 18 studies provided
number of programs being developed and tested a conceptual or logic model for the program being
using rigorous designs. In the following, we pre- evaluated that described the constructs targeted by
sent recommendations for future directions for program content and/or their relationship with DV
this research based on this review. outcomes. In other cases, research reports did
Future studies should address methodological identify behavioral determinants targeted by pro-
limitations of previous program evaluations. All gram activities, but evaluation was not conducted
of the studies reviewed were randomized trials to determine whether the program was, in fact,
that controlled for most threats to internal validity. effective in changing the targeted determinants
However, nearly half (n = 8) of the studies and/or whether change in these determinants led
assessed only short-term program effects (final to change in DV outcomes. We thus recommend
follow-up conducted 6 months post- that designers of future programs to prevent DV
intervention); as such, it is unclear whether these clearly describe the types of DV behaviors the
programs had lasting effects on DV behavior. program is intended to prevent, the behavioral
Assessment of DV outcomes varied substantially determinants being targeted by the program and
across studies; some studies conducted compre- their linkages to the DV outcomes and their theo-
hensive assessments of multiple types of DV vic- retical or empirical base, and the behavior change
timization and perpetration outcomes (e.g., Safe techniques (e.g., modeling, feedback on behavior;
Dates), whereas others assessed only one type of providing information about health conse-
DV victimization and/or perpetration behavior quences) being used to produce change in the
(e.g., Fourth R). The evaluations of several of targeted determinants. Clear specification of the
the programs (e.g., JOVEN, BALL, YPP, program should, in turn, facilitate more nuanced
SafERteens) had relatively small sample sizes evaluation research that identifies the content and
that may have limited the power of those studies mechanisms (through mediation analyses) that
Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts 897

produce change in DV thus informing scientific factors hold particular promise because they have
understanding and future development of more the potential to influence large numbers of indi-
effective programs. viduals and thus influence population-level
Also, although many of the studies examined change. Bystander intervention programs repre-
moderators of program effects, which is critical sent one potential social contextual prevention
for answering questions about for whom and/or in approach that may be particularly powerful when
what contexts the intervention is and is not effec- used with adolescent populations given the
tive, some potentially important moderators were saliency of peer context during this developmen-
not examined. For example, an important poten- tal period. Programs that take a bystander D
tial effect modifier that should be considered in approach aim to provide individuals with the
evaluation research is the sexual orientation of the skills necessary to effectively intervene to change
adolescent. Heterosexuality is typically assumed social norms that are accepting of violence and
in current programs, and, because sexual orienta- thus reduce the likelihood of future violence
tion has not been examined as an effect modifier, it (Banyard 2011). These types of interventions are
is unclear whether the programs reviewed here unique in that they engage individuals “. . ..as
would or would not have had differential effects potential witnesses to violence rather than as pos-
depending on sexual orientation. It is also notable sible victims or perpetrators” (Coker et al. 2015,
that the types of moderators examined by studies p. 295). As such, they are posited to have a broad
in the current review were limited to individual social impact because increased positive
personal or demographic characteristics (e.g., sex, bystander behaviors among program participants
race, history of DV). Future studies should also may diffuse through social networks, resulting in
examine other types of social contextual variables community-level changes in violent behavior
(e.g., school context, family context) that could (Coker et al. 2015). Bystander-focused interven-
moderate program effectiveness on DVoutcomes. tions have been found to be effective in reducing
Additionally, although many studies examined interpersonal violence among university students
potential moderators of program effects, none (e.g., the Green Dot program; Coker et al. 2015),
empirically examined why there were differential and two of the DV prevention programs we
program effects for subgroups when subgroup reviewed (SHARP and CBIM) included
differences were found. For example, although bystander components, though, due to the meth-
most studies found that program effects were con- odological limitations described above, it is
sistent for boys and girls, two found sex differ- unclear whether these programs worked to effect
ences in program effects; three of four studies change in DV through changes in bystander
found that program effects differed depending on intervention.
reports of DV at baseline; and two found race/ Also, more research is needed to develop,
ethnicity differences. More research is needed to implement, and evaluate programs that leverage
explain why these programs were effective for the use of technology to prevent or reduce DV
some groups but not others. Such research could behavior. Technology access and use among ado-
contribute to identify specific avenues for pro- lescents have increased tremendously over the
gram adaptation and development. past decade, sparking the development of numer-
Most of the programs reviewed aimed to ous eHealth programs that target youth. However,
change individual-level determinants of DV only two of the programs selected for this review
(e.g., knowledge, attitudes). Yet social–ecological included a technology-based component, IYG and
models of partner violence (Fulu and Miedema SafERteens. Emerging research suggests that
2015; Heise 1998) suggest that social contextual- behavior change programs for youth that can be
level risk factors for relationship violence (e.g., accessed through the Internet can be effective and
laws, social norms, neighborhood characteristics) have the potential to provide easy non-school-
may be important targets for DV prevention pro- based access to program content that is engaging
grams. Interventions that target social contextual to adolescents, minimizes cost, and circumvents
898 Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts

organizational barriers present in many settings one other type of risk behavior (Safe Dates,
such as pressure on schools to devote class time Fourth R, Shifting Boundaries-building only,
to academics (Shegog et al. 2014). IYG, and SafERteens). Specifically, Safe Dates,
All of the programs selected for review were Shifting Boundaries-building only, and
implemented with adolescent populations, yet SafERteens were found to have effects on other
longitudinal empirical research (for a review youth violence outcomes (Foshee et al. 2014; Tay-
see Vagi et al. 2013) has identified several child- lor et al. 2013; Peskin et al. 2014; Cunningham
hood risk factors for intimate partner abuse (e.g., et al. 2013), and the Fourth R and IYG were found
child maltreatment, negative family relations, to reduce sexual risk behaviors (Peskin
early problem behavior) that could be potent et al. 2014; Wolfe et al. 2009). Taken together
targets for early life-course intervention efforts these findings suggest adolescent DV prevention
(Magdol et al. 1998; Ehrensaft et al. 2003). More programs can be effective in targeting multiple
research is needed to evaluate whether programs behavioral outcomes. More research is needed to
targeting childhood risk factors may be effective understanding the optimal number and types of
in preventing DV perpetration and/or victimiza- behaviors to intervene on, the shared determinants
tion during adolescence. There are a number of that should be targeted, and whether to intervene
extant evidence-based programs that target chil- on behaviors simultaneously or sequentially
dren that have been shown to alter risk factors for (Spring et al. 2012). This is a critical line of
DV (e.g., Nurse–Family Partnership; Miller scientific inquiry given the potential for crosscut-
2015); an important line of future research ting programs to be more efficient and have
could be to evaluate whether these programs influ- broader benefits than programs that target a single
ence DV victimization and/or perpetration during risk behavior.
adolescence.
Finally, in order to maximize public health
impact, future research is needed to determine Conclusion
whether DV prevention programs can also be
effective at preventing other adolescent risk In summary, studies have found evidence to sug-
behaviors. Both theory (Jessor et al. 1995) and gest that a number of different universal, selective,
empirical research (e.g., Niolon et al. 2015; and indicated prevention programs may be effec-
Foshee et al. 2015a; Reyes et al. 2014) suggest tive in preventing DV victimization and/or perpe-
that DV is strongly connected to other forms of tration during adolescence. Evidence of sustained
peer violence (e.g., bullying, sexual harassment) (>6 months post-intervention) effects on multiple
as well as to other risk behaviors (e.g., substance types of victimization and perpetration outcomes
use, sexual risk behavior). These findings suggest was found for one universal (Safe Dates), one
that these behaviors and DV may share risk and indicated (IYG), and one selective (YRP)
protective factors that could be targeted for program. We recommend that future evaluation
change in single programs to prevent multiple studies use designs that allow for long-term
risk behaviors (i.e., crosscutting prevention pro- assessment; evaluate multiple types of DV victim-
grams; Centers for Disease Control and Preven- ization and perpetration outcomes; are well
tion [CDC] 2016). Although the current review powered to detect program effects; allow for
focused on summarizing the effects of programs assessment of cost-effectiveness; and provide
on DV outcomes, it is noteworthy that ten of the nuanced information about what program compo-
18 programs reviewed evaluated effects on risk nents do and do not work, how they work, and for
behaviors or outcomes other than DV. Findings whom they work. Future areas of research should
for program effects on these other risk behaviors seek to identify effective approaches that target
were mixed overall; however, five programs that distal and social contextual risk factors, use infor-
were found to have significant effects on DV were mation technology to engage youth, and influence
also found to have significant effects on at least multiple behavioral outcomes.
Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts 899

References Exner-Cortens, D., Eckenrode, J., & Rothman, E. (2013).


Longitudinal associations between teen dating violence
Ackard, D. M., Eisenberg, M. E., & Neumark-Sztainer, victimization and adverse health outcomes. Pediatrics,
D. (2007). Long-term impact of adolescent dating vio- 131(1), 71–78. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2012-1029.
lence on the behavioral and psychological health of Florsheim, P., McArthur, L., Hudak, C., Heavin, S., &
male and female youth. Journal of Pediatrics, 151(5), Burrow-Sanchez, J. (2011). The young parenthood pro-
476–481. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.04.034. gram: Preventing intimate partner violence between
Banyard, V. L. (2011). Who will help prevent sexual vio- adolescent mothers and young fathers. Journal of Cou-
lence: Creating an ecological model of bystander inter- ple & Relationship Therapy, 10(2), 117–134.
Florsheim, P., Burrow-Sanchez, J. J., Minami, T.,
vention. Psychology of Violence, 1(3), 216–229. https://
McArthur, L., Heavin, S., & Hudak, C. (2012). Young D
doi.org/10.1037/a0023739.
Bouchey, H. A., & Furman, W. (2006; 2008). Dating and parenthood program: Supporting positive paternal
romantic experiences in adolescence. In Blackwell engagement through coparenting counseling. Ameri-
handbook of adolescence (pp. 312–329) Blackwell Pub- can Journal of Public Health, 102(10), 1886–1892.
lishing. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470756607.ch15 https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2012.300902.
Brown, A., Cosgrave, E., Killackey, E., Purcell, R., Foshee, V. A., & Reyes, H. L. M. (2012). Dating abuse:
Buckby, J., & Yung, A. R. (2009). The longitudinal Prevalence, consequences, and predictors. In
association of adolescent dating violence with psychi- R. J. R. Levesque (Ed.), Encyclopedia of adolescence.
atric disorders and functioning. Journal of Interper- Springer, New York (pp. 602–615).
sonal Violence, 24(12), 1964–1979. https://doi.org/ Foshee, V., Linder, G., Bauman, K., Langwick, S., Arriaga,
10.1177/0886260508327700. X., Heath, J.,. . . Bangdiwala, S. (1996). The safe dates
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2016). Pre- project: Theoretical basis, evaluation design, and
venting multiple forms of violence: A strategic vision selected baseline findings. American Journal of Pre-
for connecting the dots. (). Atlanta: Division of Vio- ventive Medicine, 12(5), 39–47.
lence Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention Foshee, V., Bauman, K., Arriaga, X., Helms, R., Koch, G.,
and Control, Centers for Disease Control and & Linder, G. (1998). An evaluation of safe dates, an
Prevention. adolescent dating violence prevention program. Amer-
Coker, A. L., Fisher, B. S., Bush, H. M., Swan, S. C., ican Journal of Public Health, 88(1), 45–50. https://
Williams, C. M., Clear, E. R., & DeGue, S. (2015). doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.88.1.45.
Evaluation of the green dot bystander intervention to Foshee, V. A., Bauman, K. E., Greene, W. F., Koch, G. G.,
reduce interpersonal violence among college students Linder, G. F., & MacDougall, J. E. (2000). The Safe
across three campuses. Violence Against Women, Dates program: 1-year follow-up results. American
21(12), 1507–1527. https://doi.org/10.1177/107780 Journal of Public Health, 90(10), 1619.
1214545284. Foshee, V. A., Bauman, K. E., Ennett, S. T., Linder, G. F.,
Connolly, J., Josephson, W., Schnoll, J., Simkins-Strong, Benefield, T., & Suchindran, C. (2004). Assessing the
E., Pepler, D., MacPherson, A.,. . . Jiang, D. (2015). long-term effects of the Safe Dates program and a
Evaluation of a youth-led program for preventing bul- booster in preventing and reducing adolescent dating
lying, sexual harassment, and dating aggression in violence victimization and perpetration. American
middle schools. Journal of Early Adolescence, 35(3), Journal of Public Health, 94(4), 619–624.
403–434. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431614535090. Foshee, V., Bauman, K., Ennett, S., Suchindran, C.,
Cunningham, R. M., Walton, M. A., Goldstein, A., Benefield, T., & Linder, G. (2005). Assessing the
Chermack, S. T., Shope, J. T., Bingham, C. R.,. . . effects of the dating violence prevention program
Blow, F. C. (2009). Three-month follow-up of brief “safe dates” using random coefficient regression
computerized and therapist interventions for alcohol modeling. Prevention Science, 6(3), 245–258. https://
and violence among teens. Academic Emergency Med- doi.org/10.1007/s11121-005-0007-0.
icine, 16(11), 1193–1207. https://doi.org/10.1111/ Foshee, V. A., Reyes, H. L. M., Ennett, S. T., Cance, J. D.,
j.1553-2712.2009.00513.x. Bauman, K. E., & Bowling, J. M. (2012). Assessing the
Cunningham, R. M., Whiteside, L. K., Chermack, S. T., effects of families for safe dates, a family-based teen
Zimmerman, M. A., Shope, J. T., Bingham, C. R.,. . . dating abuse prevention program. Journal of Adoles-
Walton, M. A. (2013). Dating violence: Outcomes fol- cent Health, 51(4), 349–356. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
lowing a brief motivational interviewing intervention jadohealth.2011.12.029.
among at-risk adolescents in an urban emergency Foshee, V. A., Reyes, H. L. M., Gottfredson, N. C., Chang,
department. Academic Emergency Medicine, 20(6), L., & Ennett, S. T. (2013). A longitudinal examination
562–569. https://doi.org/10.1111/acem.12151. of psychological, behavioral, academic, and relation-
Ehrensaft, M., Cohen, P., Brown, J., Smailes, E., Chen, H., ship consequences of dating abuse victimization among
& Johnson, J. (2003). Intergenerational transmission of a primarily rural sample of adolescents. Journal of
partner violence: A 20-year prospective study. Journal Adolescent Health, 53(6), 723–729. https://doi.org/
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71(4), 741–753. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.06.016.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.71.4.741. Foshee, V. A., Reyes, L. M., Agnew-Brune, C. B., Simon,
T. R., Vagi, K. J., Lee, R. D., & Suchindran, C. (2014).
900 Dating Abuse: Primary Prevention Efforts

The effects of the evidence-based safe dates dating Jewkes, R., Nduna, M., Levin, J., Jama, N., Dunkle, K.,
abuse prevention program on other youth violence out- Puren, A., & Duvvury, N. (2008). Impact of stepping
comes. Prevention Science: The Official Journal of the stones on incidence of HIV and HSV-2 and sexual
Society for Prevention Research, 15(6), 907–916. behaviour in rural South Africa: Cluster randomised
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-014-0472-4. controlled trial. British Medical Journal, 337(7666),
Foshee, V. A., McNaughton Reyes, L., Tharp, A. T., a506. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.a506.
Chang, L. Y., Ennett, S. T., Simon, T. R.,. . . & Kann, L., Kinchen, S., Shanklin, S. L., Flint, K. H.,
Suchindran, C. (2015a). Shared longitudinal predictors Kawkins, J., Harris, W. A.,. . . & Chyen, D. (2014).
of physical peer and dating violence. The Journal of Youth risk behavior surveillance—United States,
Adolescent Health: Official Publication of the Society 2013. MMWR Surveill Summ, 63(Suppl 4), 1–168.
for Adolescent Medicine, 56(1), 106–112. https://doi. Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J., & Turner, L. A. (2012). The
org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.08.003. efficacy of an intimate partner violence prevention pro-
Foshee, V. A., Benefield, T., Dixon, K. S., Chang, L., gram with high-risk adolescent girls: A preliminary
Senkomago, V., Ennett, S. T.,. . . Bowling, J. M. test. Prevention Science, 13(4), 384–394. https://doi.
(2015b). The effects of moms and teens for safe dates: org/10.1007/s11121-011-0240-7.
A dating abuse prevention program for adolescents Lundgren, R., & Amin, A. (2015). Addressing intimate
exposed to domestic violence. Journal of Youth and partner violence and sexual violence among adoles-
Adolescence, 44(5), 995–1010. https://doi.org/ cents: Emerging evidence of effectiveness. Journal of
10.1007/s10964-015-0272-6. Adolescent Health, 56(1), S42–S50. https://doi.org/
Foshee, V. A., Dixon, K. S., Ennett, S. T., Moracco, K. E., 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.08.012.
Bowling, J. M., Chang, L., & Moss, J. L. (2015c). The Magdol, L., Moffitt, T., Caspi, A., & Silva, P. (1998).
process of adapting a universal dating abuse prevention Developmental antecedents of partner abuse: A -
program to adolescents exposed to domestic violence. prospective-longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 30(12), 2151–2173. Psychology, 107(3), 375–389. https://doi.org/10.1037/
https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260514552278 0021-843X.107.3.375.
Fulu, E., & Miedema, S. (2015). Violence against women: Miller, T. R. (2015). Projected outcomes of nurse-family
Globalizing the integrated ecological model. Violence partnership home visitation during 1996–2013, USA.
Against Women, 21(12), 1431–1455. https://doi.org/ Prevention Science, 16(6), 765–777. https://doi.org/
10.1177/1077801215596244. 10.1007/s11121-015-0572-9.
Gonzalez-Guarda, R. M., Guerra, J. E., Cummings, A. A., Miller, E., Tancredi, D. J., McCauley, H. L., Decker, M. R.
Pino, K., & Becerra, M. M. (2015). Examining the , Virata, M. C. D., Anderson, H. A.,. . . Silverman, J. G.
preliminary efficacy of a dating violence prevention (2012). “Coaching boys into men”: A cluster-
program for hispanic adolescents. Journal of School randomized controlled trial of a dating violence pre-
Nursing, 31(6), 411–421. https://doi.org/10.1177/ vention program. Journal of Adolescent Health, 51(5),
1059840515598843. 431–438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.
Gordon, R. (1983). An operational classification of disease 01.018
prevention. Public Health Reports, 98(2), 107–109. Miller, E., Tancredi, D. J., McCauley, H. L., Decker, M. R.,
Haynie, D. L., Farhat, T., Brooks-Russell, A., Wang, J., Virata, M. C. D., Anderson, H. A.,. . . Silverman, J. G.
Barbieri, B., & Iannotti, R. J. (2013). Dating violence (2013). One-year follow-up of a coach-delivered dating
perpetration and victimization among U.S. adolescents: violence prevention program A cluster randomized
Prevalence, patterns, and associations with health com- controlled trial. American Journal of Preventive Medi-
plaints and substance use. Journal of Adolescent cine, 45(1), 108–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.
Health, 53(2), 194–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 2013.03.007.
jadohealth.2013.02.008. Miller, E., Goldstein, S., McCauley, H. L., Jones, K. A.,
Heise, L. (1998). Violence against women: An integrated, Dick, R. N., Jetton, J.,. . . Tancredi, D. J. (2015).
ecological framework. Violence Against Women, 4, A school health center intervention for abusive adoles-
262–290. https://doi.org/10.1177/107780129800400 cent relationships: A cluster RCT. Pediatrics, 135(1),
3002. 76–85. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-2471.
Jaycox, L. H., McCaffrey, D., Eiseman, B., Aronoff, J., Niolon, P. H., Vivolo-Kantor, A. M., Latzman, N. E., Valle,
Shelley, G. A., Collins, R. L., & Marshall, G. N. (2006). L. A., Kuoh, H., Burton, T.,. . . Tharp, A. T. (2015).
Impact of a school-based dating violence prevention Prevalence of teen dating violence and co-occurring
program among latino teens: Randomized controlled risk factors among middle school youth in high-risk
effectiveness trial. Journal of Adolescent Health, urban communities. Journal of Adolescent Health,
39(5), 694–704. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth. 56(2), S5–S13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.
2006.05.002. 2014.07.019.
Jessor, R., Vandenbos, J., Vanderryn, J., Costa, F., & Turbin, O’Leary, K. D., Slep, A. M. S., Avery-Leaf, S., & Cascardi,
M. (1995). Protective factors in adolescent problem M. (2008). Gender differences in dating aggression
behavior – Moderator effects and developmental- among multiethnic high school students. Journal of
change. Developmental Psychology, 31(6), 923–933. Adolescent Health, 42(5), 473–479. https://doi.org/
https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.31.6.923. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.09.012.
Deafness 901

Peskin, M. F., Markham, C. M., Shegog, R., Baumler, correlates: A review of risk and protective factors for
E. R., Addy, R. C., & Tortolero, S. R. (2014). Effects adolescent dating violence perpetration. Journal of
of the it’s your game . . . keep it real program on dating Youth and Adolescence, 42(4), 633–649. https://doi.
violence in ethnic-minority middle school youths: org/10.1007/s10964-013-9907-7.
A group randomized trial. American Journal of Public Wolfe, D. A., Wekerle, C., Gough, B., Reitzel-Jaffe, D.,
Health, 104(8), 1471–1477. https://doi.org/10.2105/ Grasley, C., Pittman, A., et al. (1996). The youth rela-
AJPH.2014.301902. tionships manual: A group approach with adolescents
Reyes, H. L. M., Foshee, V. A., Bauer, D. J., & Ennett, S. T. for the prevention of woman abuse and the promotion
(2014). Proximal and time-varying effects of cigarette, of healthy relationships. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
alcohol, marijuana and other hard drug use on adoles- Wolfe, D., Wekerle, C., Scott, K., Straatman, A., Grasley,
cent dating aggression. Journal of Adolescence, 37(3), C., & Reitzel-Jaffe, D. (2003). Dating violence preven- D
281–289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2014. tion with at-risk youth: A controlled outcome evalua-
02.002. tion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Roberts, T., Klein, J., & Fisher, S. (2003). Longitudinal 71(2), 279–291. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.
effect of intimate partner abuse on high-risk behavior 71.2.279.
among adolescents. Archives of Pediatrics & Adoles- Wolfe, D. A., Crooks, C., Jaffe, P., Chiodo, D., Hughes, R.,
cent Medicine, 157(9), 875–881. https://doi.org/ Ellis, W.,. . . Donner, A. (2009). A school-based pro-
10.1001/archpedi.157.9.875. gram to prevent adolescent dating violence A cluster
Shegog, R., Peskin, M., & Markham, C. (2014). It’s your randomized trial. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent
game-tech: Toward sexual health in the digital age. Medicine, 163(8), 692–699.
Creative Education, 5, 1428–1447.
Spivak, H. R., Jenkins, E. L., VanAudenhove, K., Lee, D.,
Kelly, M., & Iskander, J. (2014). CDC grand rounds:
A public health approach to prevention of intimate
partner violence. MMWR-Morbidity and Mortality Deafness
Weekly Report, 63(2), 38–41.
Spring, B., Moller, A. C., & Coons, M. J. (2012). Multiple
Roger J. R. Levesque
health behaviours: Overview and implications. Journal
of Public Health, 34, I3–I10. https://doi.org/10.1093/ Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
pubmed/fdr111.
Taylor, B., Stein, N., & Burden, F. (2010a). The effects of
gender Violence/Harassment prevention programming
Deafness involves partial or complete hearing
in middle schools: A randomized experimental evalua-
tion. Violence and Victims, 25(2), 202–223. https://doi. loss. Although the range of loss can be wide, and
org/10.1891/0886-6708.25.2.202. quite specific terms could be used to describe a
Taylor, B. G., Stein, N., & Burden, F. F. (2010b). Exploring variety of levels of hearing impairment, there
gender differences in dating violence/harassment pre-
essentially are three broad categories of hearing
vention programming in middle schools: Results from a
randomized experiment. Journal of Experimental loss: profoundly deaf, moderately deaf, and hard
Criminology, 6(4), 419–445. https://doi.org/10.1007/ of hearing. The broad variation is of significance
s11292-010-9103-7. in that it inevitably relates to adolescents’ experi-
Taylor, B. G., Stein, N. D., Mumford, E. A., & Woods,
ences. The various levels of hearing loss also are
D. (2013). Shifting boundaries: An experimental eval-
uation of a dating violence prevention program in mid- important to consider in that they complicate
dle schools. Prevention Science, 14(1), 64–76. https:// research examining its effects and the nature of
doi.org/10.1007/s11121-012-0293-2. hearing loss. The different levels also challenge
Taylor, B. G., Mumford, E. A., & Stein, N. D. (2015).
efforts to understand the different interventions
Effectiveness of “shifting boundaries” teen dating vio-
lence prevention program for subgroups of middle that recently have emerged, as well as efforts to
school students. Journal of Adolescent Health, 56(2), understand their effectiveness. Despite these com-
S20–S26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014. plexities, research relating to deafness during ado-
07.004.
lescence and other periods across the life span still
Tortolero, S. R., Markham, C. M., Peskin, M. F., Shegog,
R., Addy, R. C., Escobar-Chaves, S. L., & Baumler, provides us with important findings and trends.
E. R. (2010). It’s your game: Keep it real: Delaying Estimates of the prevalence of deafness are
sexual behavior with an effective middle school pro- difficult to establish. Best estimates reveal that
gram. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46(2), 169–179.
permanent deafness affects roughly 1 in 750 chil-
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.06.008.
Vagi, K. J., Rothman, E. F., Latzman, N. E., Tharp, A. T., dren (Davis et al. 1997; Davis and Wood 1992).
Hall, D. M., & Breiding, M. J. (2013). Beyond The prevalence of other forms of hearing
902 Deafness

impairments during adolescence is not well schools versus special schools (Aplin 1987;
known given that they may be difficult to identify. Arnold and Atkins 1991; Van Eldik 2005).
National studies reveal that approximately 14.9% Despite the above encouraging findings,
of children in the United States have low- researchers still report differences between hear-
frequency or high-frequency hearing loss of at ing and non-hearing-impaired adolescents. For
least 16-dB hearing level in one or both ears example, a recent study using children as infor-
(Niskar et al. 1998). Importantly, those studies mants, in addition to parents and teachers, found
also reveal that youth may not even be aware of that deaf children have higher rates of emotional
the significant hearing loss; of those with mea- and behavioral problems; the most significant dif-
sured hearing loss, 10.8% were reported to have ferences in deaf children were in conduct prob-
current hearing loss during the interview (Niskar lems, emotional problems, and peer problems;
et al. 1998). Using more narrow definitions of yet, no significant differences in overall mental
hearing impaired leads to conclusions that as health were observed (Fellinger et al. 2008). Prev-
many as 738,000 individuals in the United States alence rates of emotional problems have been
have severe to profound hearing loss; of these, reported to be more than twice as high as in the
almost 8% are under the age of 18 (Blanchfield general population of hearing adolescents but
et al. 2001). Thus, those affected by hearing loss behavioral problems are found only to be slightly
tend to be adults. Not surprisingly, research tends higher than for hearing groups (van Gent et al.
to focus on adults (see Agrawal et al. 2008). 2007). Despite these findings, other research does
There is no doubt that hearing loss affects indicate that it may not be hearing impairment per
health and quality of life, but the extent to which se that causes emotional and behavioral problems
it does remains debatable to the extent that there in deaf children (Hindley and Van Gent 2002; van
are no simple direct links between hearing loss Gent et al. 2007). A recent study found that three
and health outcomes. Research does indicate that factors associated with psychopathology in deaf
deafness associates with cognitive, communica- youth: lower IQ, signing as mode of communica-
tion, and socio-emotional development problems tion, and multiple physical disorders; and that
(Fellinger et al. 2008). Indeed, research has long study concluded that “the only variable signifi-
reported increased rates of psychosocial problems cantly associated with a dossier diagnosis of a
in deaf children (Hindley et al. 1994), with the primary emotional disorder appeared to be a his-
prevalence of psychopathology reported over the tory of three or more physical disorders” (van
last three decades varying from 0% to 77% (van Gent et al. 2007, p. 955). Importantly, physical
Gent et al. 2007). Although some researchers have health factors also are risk factors for hearing
reported a link between psychopathology and children (Friedman and Chase-Lansdale 2002) as
degree of hearing loss, other more recent reports is low IQ (Friedman and Chase-Lansdale 2002).
actually do not (Hindley et al. 1994). It is believed These factors make it difficult to pinpoint the
that, at least in the United States and several other sources of difficulties. Recent research has found
countries, conditions for hearing-impaired and that the overall quality of life for deaf children was
deaf youth have significantly changed over the largely similar to that of the control group, but that
last decade. The change tends to be viewed as children reported dissatisfaction with interests and
improvement and greater integration with those recreational activities (isolation) and their physi-
who are not hearing impaired. The difference is cal health (Fellinger et al. 2008). Current research
due to technology, mainstreaming in schools, and clearly suggests, then, that actors such as commu-
earlier detection, as well as cochlear implants, that nication problems, physical health issues, and liv-
have altered and significantly improved the qual- ing conditions may account for the increase in the
ity of life for hearing-impaired youth. Some note, risk of psychiatric disorders in the deaf youth
for example, that mainstreaming in schools may population and that more research is needed.
account for fewer differences in levels of psycho- Although the above studies offer complex
pathology in deaf children attending mainstream results, they represent a new wave of research in
Deception 903

that they actually focus on the adolescents them- estimates and demographics. Journal of the American
selves. Studies that have deaf children as infor- Academy of Audiology, 12, 183–189.
Davis, A., & Wood, S. (1992). The epidemiology of child-
mants are lacking and most studies use hood hearing impairment: Factors relevant to planning
questionnaires directed at the adults observing of services. British Journal of Audiology, 26, 77–90.
deaf children’s lives (i.e., parents and teachers). Davis, A., Bamford, J., Wilson, I., Ramkalawan, T.,
These studies are important to consider in that Forshaw, M., & Wright, S. (1997). A critical review
of the role of neonatal hearing screening in the detec-
differences in reports pertaining to the same child tion of congenital hearing impairment. Health Technol-
have been found (Fellinger et al. 2008) and the ogy Assessment, 1, 1–176.
level of agreement between informants (child, Fellinger, J., Holzinger, D., Sattel, H., & Laucht, M. (2008). D
teacher, parent) is generally low (van Gent et al. Mental health and quality of life in deaf pupils.
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 17, 414–423.
2007). With the exception of cochlear implant Friedman, R. J., & Chase-Lansdale, P. L. (2002). Chronic
studies, quality of life studies do not focus on the adversities. In E. T. M. Rutter (Ed.), Child and adoles-
deaf child as the informant. The choice of infor- cent psychiatry (4th ed., pp. 261–276). London:
mants may explain much of the variation in reports Blackwell.
Hindley, P., & Van Gent, T. (2002). Psychiatric aspects of
of psychopathology in deaf youth and more specific sensory impairment. In M. Rutter & E. Taylor
research is needed using deaf youth as informants. (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychiatry (4th ed.,
Despite progress in understanding the experi- pp. 842–857). London: Blackwell.
ence of hearing loss during adolescence, much Hindley, P., Hill, P. D., McGuigan, S., & Kitson, N. (1994).
Psychiatric disorder in deaf and hearing impaired chil-
remains to be investigated and learned. For exam- dren and young people: A prevalence study. Journal of
ple, the prevalence of hearing loss in the United Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 917–934.
States differs across racial/ethnic groups, and risk Niskar, A. S., Kieszak, S. M., Holmes, A., Esteban, E.,
factors are not well known (Agrawal et al. 2008). Rubin, C., & Brody, D. J. (1998). Prevalence of hearing
loss among children 6 to 19 years of age: The third
Similarly, given that hearing loss becomes more National Health and nutrition examination survey.
pronounced in later life, hearing loss prevention Journal of the American Medical Association, 279,
(through modifiable risk factor reduction) and 1071–1075.
screening should begin much earlier. These and Van Eldik, T. (2005). Mental health problems of Dutch
youth with hearing loss as shown on the youth self
other conclusions are likely to emerge as the study report. American Annals of the Deaf, 150(1), 11–16.
of adolescence focuses increasingly on the factors van Gent, T., Goedhart, A., Hindley, P., & Treffers,
that contribute to adolescents’ adjustment and the P. (2007). Prevalence and correlates of psychopathol-
types of environments that society should aim to ogy in a sample of deaf adolescents. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(9), 950–958.
provide youth to ensure their most healthy
adjustment.

References Deception
Agrawal, Y., Platz, E. A., & Niparko, J. K. (2008). Preva- Roger J. R. Levesque
lence of hearing loss and differences by demographic
characteristics among US adults: Data from the
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
National Health and nutrition examination survey,
1999–2004. Archives of Internal Medicine, 168,
1522–1530. Overview
Aplin, D. (1987). Social and emotional adjustment of
hearing-impaired children in ordinary and special
schools. Educational Research, 29, 56–64. Deception, the variety of ways that individuals
Arnold, P., & Atkins, J. (1991). The social and emotional can be dishonest to get what they want, appears
adjustment of hearing-impaired children integrated in commonplace. There are deceptions to others as
primary schools. Educational Research, 33, 223–228.
well as to ourselves. These deceptions serve
Blanchfield, B. B., Feldman, J. J., Dunbar, J. L., & Gard-
ner, E. N. (2001). The severely to profoundly hearing- important functions as they help maintain rela-
impaired population in the United States: Prevalence tionships. But, they can also be problematic,
904 Deception

such as when they relate to deviant behavior but information from parents, especially information
more commonly in the harm it can cause relation- that relates to personal or deep moral issues, and
ships as well as people’s healthy development. that withholding is what adolescents use to create
This essay examines trends in this area of research boundaries between themselves and others (see
as it relates to adolescents, including highlighting Darling et al. 2006; Smetana et al. 2006).
areas in need of more research. The reasons for creating boundaries and for
withholding information are numerous, but they
typically include a desire to foster privacy, con-
Deception cern that parents would not be understanding, and
effort to avoid sanctions (Darling et al. 2006; Yau
Deception, typically defined as deceiving and dis- et al. 2009). These motivations mean that the
honesty, comes in many forms. Deception can nature of parent–child relationships has a power-
involve pretense, trickery, concealment, feigning, ful effect on the extent to which adolescents
obfuscation, diversion, lying, misleading, and a deceive. Perceptions of parents’ roles and legiti-
variety of other ways that individuals actively macy have been deemed key factors that influence
provide misinformation. Whether misinformation adolescents’ deceptions. Adolescents are more
is provided in specific instances rests on how likely to disclose information accurately when
individuals create boundaries that determine the they believe that they were obliged to obey par-
amount of information that they will share with ents with regards to a particular issue (Darling
others as well as, importantly, with themselves et al. 2006) and when adolescents believe that
(in the form of self-deception). Research relating their parents have a legitimate claim to informa-
to adolescence has tended not to address decep- tion (Cumsille et al. 2010). Deception, then, may
tion directly, but much research relating to the be part of healthy individuation, but low levels of
adolescent period sheds light on the significance disclosure and high levels of concealment reflect
of deception as it relates to adolescence. That poor quality relationships and problem behaviors
research, however, tends to focus on adolescents’ (Finkenauer et al. 2002, 2005). Not surprisingly,
own deceptions rather than others’ deceptions of youth involved in problem behavior are more
adolescents. Overall, research does reveal that likely to be manipulative and more likely to tell
deceiving may be a necessary part of healthy self-serving lies as well as more lies (Kashy and
development but that relatively greater deception DePaulo 1996). Adolescents who have strong
likely reflects problematic circumstances and con- relationships with parents, and who view parents
tributes to negative outcomes. as more trustworthy, are less likely to keep infor-
Among other areas of research relating to mation from them (Yau et al. 2009; Kerr
deception by adolescents, one of the most et al. 1999), a finding that may explain why par-
illustrative areas involves deception in their ents’ more aggressive efforts to pursue greater
relationships with their parents. The adolescent disclosure runs the risk of fortifying boundaries
period involves developmental milestones in and increasing distress (Mazur and Hubbard
parent–child relationships that contribute to 2004; Masche 2010).
changes in the management of information that Importantly, little research has examined the
reflect, and shape, changing parenting relation- extent to which, and if so why, parents deceive
ship boundaries. Part of those boundary changes their children. A handful of studies have exam-
involves adolescents’ expanding their views of ined the phenomenon of “parenting by lying.”
what they deem personal and their resisting par- That phenomenon involves parents’ telling their
ents’ monitoring in efforts to change boundaries. children lies to influence their emotional states
That resistance has important implications for the and behaviors. Late adolescents report that their
parent–child relationship, parental socialization parents lied to them while maintaining a concur-
efforts, and the transfer of information. The vast rent emphasis on the importance of being honest,
majority of adolescents report withholding some and parents report lying to their children to
Deception 905

promote behavioral compliance or positive feel- relationships, relatively high deception can con-
ings (Heyman et al. 2009). It also appears that tribute to negative outcomes, as evidenced by
parents who promote honesty do not necessarily research on self-silencing in adolescent relation-
lie less to their children. This aspect of parenting ships, which indicates that self-silencing relates to
appears across cultures, although there appear to poor relationship satisfaction overall as well as
be variations, such as parents in China using frustration and depressive symptoms (Harper
deception to control behavior more often (or, at and Welsh 2007).
least admitting to it more) than do parents in the Although research on deception tends to focus
United States (Heyman et al. 2013). Although on relationships, it is important to consider the D
these findings are intriguing, it is important nature of self-deception. Self-deception figures
to note that there continues to be scant literature prominently in the study of mental health. For
on parents’ deceptions; what does exist lacks example, drug and alcohol addiction constitutes
the depth and nuances of other areas of research a disorder characterized by denial, dishonesty, and
relating to either deception or parent–child self-delusion, and substance abusing individuals
relationships. demonstrate more denial and self-deception than
The place of deception in parent–child rela- nondrug-using peers (Strom and Barone 1993;
tionships finds parallels in research on peer rela- DiClemente et al. 2004). This denial is made
tionships. For example, researchers who study quite obvious in approaches that aim to assist
deception in romantic relationships (as well as youth who have addictions. Treatments necessar-
other forms of everyday relationships) suggest ily involve confronting an addict’s denial and
that people communicate their love and support resistance to treatment, and recovery involves a
of others, in part, by lying to them, such as switch in focus from denial to readiness for
complimenting them, pretending to agree with change or from a restructuring of one’s self-
them, and claiming to understand them (see perception based more on realism and honesty
Ennis et al. 2008). As with parent child–rela- than deception. It is no mistake that leading pro-
tionships, deception in romantic relationships grams, such as 12-step programs, focus on having
has been linked to three factors (see Cole 2001). participants examine their personal weaknesses,
First, deception relates to the relative need for a admit their mistakes, and make amends to these
reciprocal exchange of information, such as the individuals; these activities aim to confront more
perception that another acts dishonestly leads one extreme forms of denial and self-delusion (see
to be dishonest toward them as may happen when Ferrari et al. 2008).
relationships lack of emotional closeness and Although self-deception plays a key role in
decline in commitment. Second, deception can addictions, it also plays a key role in other pathol-
also occur to avoid punishment, in such instances ogies and deviant behavior. Indeed, deception is a
in which telling the truth is not in one’s best necessary prerequisite to criminal conduct and
interests when the confidant may act in a judg- behavioral expressions of pscychopathy and a
mental manner and disapproval. Relatedly, decep- wide variety of complex mental health disorders.
tion may be important to nurturing positive The denial of illness in anorexia nervosa illus-
images of partners and may help avoid negative trates well the significance of self-deceptions (for
thoughts, including conflict. Third, deception fills a review, Vandereycken 2006). The current under-
individuals’ attachment beliefs, such as if they are standing of anorexia nervosa reveals that it can
uncomfortable with intimacy, they may use consist of least two forms of deception. The first
deception to keep partners at a safe distance. In form of deception is unintentional and can include
this sense, deception in romantic relationships can a variety of distorted information processing that
mirror those of parent–child relationships in that may be signs of neurobiologically impaired self-
they serve as a mechanism to preserve or elimi- awareness (anosognosia), distortions of reality
nate boundaries, such as their partners’ closeness, that are psychotic-like, dissociation, or efforts to
interest, and intrusiveness. As with parent–child cope with illness (such as would be expected
906 Deception

through wishful thinking or the normal filtering can become problematic. Researchers have
out of ideas that do not fit our preconceived started to take deception seriously and have
notions of circumstances). The second form is made important steps to understand its place in
more marked by intention and deliberate, such as adolescent development. But, it is clear that
refusing to self-disclose and avoiding disclosure research remains in its infancy and focused on
due to fears of consequences, and not necessarily adolescents’ own deception rather than decep-
engaging fully in efforts to get better (e.g., malin- tions aimed to adolescents.
gering). Deception, then, may be a negativistic
refusal to communicate or an expression of dis-
turbed perceptions and interpretations of Cross-References
experiences.
Efforts to understand the roots of deception ▶ Confabulation
continue to grow dramatically and focus on rea- ▶ Lying
sons for deceiving, the ability to resist it, and ▶ Malingering
developmental considerations. As we have seen,
individuals are dishonest for multiple reasons,
such as because they wish to avoid painful reali-
References
ties or they succumb to social influences (social Cole, T. (2001). Lying to the one you love: The use of
norms, expectations, pressures) (see, e.g., deception in romantic relationships. Journal of Social
DePaulo and Kashy 1998). Resisting these influ- and Personal Relationships, 18, 107–129.
ences and facing difficult realities require individ- Cumsille, P., Darling, N., & Martínez, M. L. (2010). Shad-
ing the truth: The patterning of adolescents’ decisions
uals to behave authentically (by remaining true to to avoid issues, disclose, or lie to parents. Journal of
themselves) or honestly (by refusing to deceive), Adolescence, 33, 285–296.
and such resistance requires personal strength, Darling, N., Cumsille, P., Caldwell, L. L., & Dowdy,
psychological stability, and an awareness of B. (2006). Predictors of adolescents’ disclosure strate-
gies and perceptions of parental knowledge. Journal of
one’s own mental processes. Attachment theory Youth and Adolescence, 35, 667–678.
has been the leading theory used to examine how DePaulo, B. M., & Kashy, D. A. (1998). Everyday lies in
personal strength and accurate self-perceptions close and casual relationships. Journal of Personality
develop, and that research suggests that those and Social Psychology, 74, 63–79.
DiClemente, C. C., Schlundt, D., & Gemmell, L. (2004).
strengths develop from supportive relationships Readiness and stages of change in addiction treat-
(see Gillath et al. 2010). Youth who engaged in ment. The American Journal on Addictions, 13,
greater “true-self” behaviors report higher self- 103–119.
esteem, positive affect, and unconditional support Ennis, E., Vrij, A., & Chance, C. (2008). Individual differ-
ences and lying in everyday life. Journal of Social and
from close others (Harter et al. 1996). In a real Personal Relationships, 25, 105–118.
sense, these findings are unsurprising in that Ferrari, J. R., Groh, D. R., Rulka, G., Jason, L. A., & Davis,
healthy relationships are central to mental health, M. I. (2008). Coming to terms with reality: Predictors
but they are surprising to the extent that healthy of self-deception within substance abuse recovery.
Addictive Disorders and Their Treatment, 7, 210–218.
relationships tend to be conceived as devoid of Finkenauer, C., Engels, R. C. M. E., & Meeus, W. (2002).
deception. Keeping secrets from parents: Advantages and disad-
vantages of secrecy in adolescence. Journal of Youth
and Adolescence, 31, 123–136.
Finkenauer, C., Frijns, T., Engels, R. C. M. E., & Kerkhof, P.
Conclusion (2005). Perceiving concealment in relationships
between parents and adolescents: Links with parental
Deception figures prominently in adolescents’ behavior. Personal Relationships, 12, 387–406.
lives. Although it tends to be viewed as problem- Gillath, O., Sesko, A. K., Shaver, P. R., & Chun, D. S.
(2010). Attachment, authenticity, and honesty: Dispo-
atic, it turns out that deception plays important sitional and experimentally induced security can reduce
roles in healthy development as well as healthy self- and other-deception. Journal of Personality and
relationships. Yet, it also is clear that deception Social Psychology, 98, 841–855.
Decriminalization 907

Harper, M. S., & Welsh, D. P. (2007). Keeping quiet: Self- adolescents, the term most likely connotes the
silencing and its association with relational and indi- removal of certain types of juvenile offenders
vidual functioning among adolescent romantic couples.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 24, from the juvenile court system and handling
99–116. them, instead, in the child welfare system or out-
Harter, S., Marold, D. B., Whitesell, N. R., & Cobbs, side of courts altogether. In the USA, this process
G. (1996). A model of the effects of perceived parent occurred largely in the late 1960s and 1970s,
and peer support on adolescent false self-behavior.
Child Development, 67, 360–374. although its reversal quickly occurred in the
Heyman, G. D., Luu, D. H., & Lee, K. (2009). Parenting by 1980s and has been largely ignored.
lying. Journal of Moral Education, 38, 353–369. The decriminalization of status offenses in the D
Heyman, G. D., Hsu, A. S., Fu, G., & Lee, K. (2013). USA was hastened by an enormously influential
Instrumental lying by parents in the US and China.
International Journal of Psychology, 48(6), federal legislation known as the Juvenile Justice
1176–1184. and Delinquency Prevention Act of 1974 (see, e.g.,
Kashy, D. A., & DePaulo, B. M. (1996). Who lies. Journal Blitzman 2007). Among other provisions, the act
of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1037–1051. established a federal Office of Juvenile Justice and
Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Trost, K. (1999). To know you is to
trust you: Parents’ trust is rooted in child disclosure of Delinquency Prevention to assist the states in bet-
information. Journal of Adolescence, 22, 737–752. ter accomplishing juvenile justice system objec-
Masche, J. G. (2010). Explanation of normative declines in tives and made grants to states contingent on
parents’ knowledge about their adolescent children. averting placement of status offenders in detention
Journal of Adolescence, 33, 271–284.
Mazur, M. A., & Hubbard, A. S. E. (2004). “Is there and correctional facilities. The legislation is also
something I should Know?”: Topic avoidant responses known for its focus on deinstitutionalizing status
in parent-adolescent communication. Communication offenses, which was an effort to not institutional-
Reports, 17(1), 27–37. ize youth who had committed status offenses. The
Smetana, J. G., Metzger, A., Gettman, D. C., & Campione-
Barr, N. (2006). Disclosure and secrecy in adolescent- federal mandates helped support significant
parent relationships. Child Development, 77, 201–217. reforms, including state statutes that were then
Strom, J., & Barone, D. F. (1993). Self-deception, self-esteem, tailored to federal requirements.
and control over drinking at different stages of alcohol A major result of legislative changes was the
involvement. Journal of Drug Issues, 23, 705–714.
Vandereycken, W. (2006). Denial of illness in anorexia removal of status offenses from the category of
nervosa – A conceptual review: Part 2 – Different delinquency and the creation of new a status-
forms and meanings. European Eating Disorder offense category or the use preexisting depen-
Review, 14, 352–368. dency/neglect category. It was this separation
Yau, J. P., Tasopoulos-Chan, M., & Smetana, J. G. (2009).
Disclosure to parents about everyday activities among that became known as decriminalization. Differ-
American adolescents from Mexican, Chinese, and Euro- ent states recategorized status offenses into differ-
pean backgrounds. Child Development, 80, 1481–1498. ent labels, such as person in need of supervision
(PINS), minor otherwise in need of supervision
(MINS), juvenile in need of supervision (JINS),
child in need of supervision (CHINS), youth in
Decriminalization need of supervision (YINS), and a child of a
family in need of services (FINS).
Roger J. R. Levesque Although decriminalization has long been seen
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA as the default rule, the legislation was amended in
1980, in response to family and juvenile court
judges’ frustration with their inability to enforce
Decriminalization describes the process by which orders by placing juveniles in secure facilities.
acts or deeds are removed from legal categories of Under the “valid court order exception,” status
criminal behavior. In popular culture, it most offenders may be incarcerated, without a state’s
likely refers to the legalization of drugs or certain jeopardizing federal funding, for violations of
behaviors (such as sexual behaviors) that were court orders entered in dependency or children in
previously subject to criminal penalties. For need of supervision proceedings that prohibit the
908 Defense Mechanisms

child from engaging in specified status offense (see Vaillant 1993). Defense mechanisms can
behaviors, such as running away from home or be adaptive and support normal functioning;
foster care. The valid court order exception indeed, it is difficult to argue that they are
substantially has undermined the Act’s original uncommon ways of approaching life. It is when
goal of reducing the use of secure detention to individuals excessively rely on these mecha-
punish status offenders. The use of valid court nisms that their use can reflect severe psycholog-
orders has been viewed as disproportionately ical problems.
used against certain groups of status offenders, Theorists and researchers have identified
especially girls (Humphrey 2004). This process many types of defense mechanisms. Among the
has become known as the recriminalizing sta- most well-recognized defenses are those that
tus offenses in that offenders could now be involve denial (refusal to admit something), pro-
reclassified as delinquents and placed in secure jection (ascribing one’s own unacceptable quali-
confinement. ties to others), rationalization (explaining away
unacceptable matters to make them acceptable),
intellectualization (reducing anxiety by thinking
about matters in a cold, calculated, and unemo-
Cross-References
tional manner), repression (keeping information
out of conscious awareness), sublimation
▶ Juvenile Court Rationales and Models
(converting unacceptable impulses to acceptable
ones), reaction formation (reducing anxiety by
References acting in the opposite manner of true feelings),
and regression (reverting to prior patterns of cop-
Blitzman, J. D. (2007). Gault’s promise. Barry Law ing). Although these are the most recognized
Review, 9, 67–98.
Humphrey, A. (2004). The criminalization of survival
types of defense mechanisms, it is important to
attempts: Locking up female runaways and other status recognize that there are many other forms that
offenders. Hastings Women’s Law Journal, 15, 165–184. often are not even construed as such, perhaps
because they are positive. Those forms of coping
include, for example, altruism (satisfying needs
by helping others), affiliation (turning to others
Defense Mechanisms for support), and humor (coping by making
light of situations or showing their ironic
Roger J. R. Levesque components). Together, these types of defense
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA mechanisms certainly support the claim that
defense mechanisms are common and serve
important ends.
Defense mechanisms are forms of thinking Vaillant (1993) arguably offered the best mod-
meant to preserve an individual’s self-esteem or ern analysis of defense mechanisms. Vaillant,
to protect them psychologically from feelings of who drew from psychoanalytic theory, argued
anxiety that may arise from particular situations. that defenses could be categorized along four
These mechanisms are often unconscious pro- levels. Level I involves pathological defenses,
cesses, rendering the individual unaware that which include extreme projection, denial, and
they are employing them as coping mechanisms. delusional projection. Level II refers to immature
Early theorists, such as Freud (1937, 1966), often defenses, which include fantasy, idealization,
equated defense mechanisms with psychopathol- passive aggression, and acting out. Level III
ogy since they involved a distortion of reality. involves neurotic defenses, which include
Today, however, psychologists and others are intellectualization, regression, and repression.
discovering that defense mechanisms also inevi- The last level, Level IV, refers to mature
tably play a role in normal, everyday functioning defenses, and these would include the use
Delay of Gratification 909

of humor, sublimation, and altruism. Although References


not adopting a full developmental approach,
Vaillant’s categorization clearly does highlight Freud, A. (1937/1966). The ego and the mechanisms of
defense. New York: International Universities Press.
how some defense mechanisms reveal a sense
Porcerelli, J. H., Thomas, S., Hibbard, S., & Cogan,
of maturity and emotional health as they help R. (1998). Defense mechanisms development in chil-
optimize development. dren, adolescents, and late adolescents. Journal of Per-
Other researchers have attempted to take more sonality Assessment, 71, 411–420.
Vaillant, G. E. (1993). The wisdom of the ego. Cambridge:
explicitly developmental approaches to the study
of defense mechanisms. Some, for example, have
Harvard University Press. D
placed different types of defense mechanisms in
a hierarchy (most notably, see Porcerelli et al.
1998). Researchers have done so based on the Degradation
age at which they emerge or on their relative
level of maturity or immaturity. For example, ▶ Humiliation
some defense mechanisms require less cogni-
tion, such as denial, whereas others require
more complex cognitive activity, such as intel-
lectualization, or simply more abilities, such as Delay of Gratification
sublimation. As a result of the need for different
cognitive or other abilities, denial tends to be Roger J. R. Levesque
seen more frequently in children aged 5–7, and Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
is infrequent in adolescents and adults. Con-
versely, projection is almost never seen among
the youngest children, a little more so in older
Overview
children, and frequently seen in adolescents
The ability to control oneself, often referred to as
and adults. When observed in adults, denial
self-regulation, is a developmental process that
and projection are considered immature, and
sublimation and altruism are considered mature has broad implications for individuals as well as
society at large. Yet, precisely how adolescents
defenses. Few doubt that the ability to use differ-
learn to be able to control their actions that they
ent mechanisms varies with a variety of individ-
ual differences. wish to commit but should resist for a better
outcome, referred to as delay of gratification, has
The above developments in the understanding
not been the subject of much empirical research.
of defense mechanisms are important. Still, a
more empirical understanding of them likely will This entry examines the concept and highlights
key findings as well as challenges that emerge
remain unsatisfactory given that the area is marred
from efforts to study the phenomenon. It also
by limitations in efforts to investigate them. Lim-
itations are not surprising. The use of defense concludes by highlighting findings relating to the
effects of the ability to delay gratification.
mechanisms is difficult to assess given that indi-
viduals need to be, by definition, unaware of their
use. Even despite these challenges, considerable Defining Delay of Gratification
progress has been made toward better understand-
ing how people address anxieties and potential The concept of delayed gratification refers to
stresses to their psychological health. This pro- the psychological orientation that individuals
gress confirms the need to factor developmental adopt when they decide to forego immediate grat-
considerations, not the least of which would be ification for the sake of reaching a more valuable
how the period of adolescence shapes the devel- but delayed outcome. In research, delayed gratifi-
opment of defense mechanisms, and how adoles- cation also refers to the ability to exercise self-
cents make use of them. control that one exhibits during the waiting
910 Delay of Gratification

process used to assess the ability to delay gratifi- participants during preschool and a decade later
cation. Delayed gratification has been studied pri- when the children were in adolescence. They
marily in children, and it has been studied for a found persistent correlations between self-imposed
considerably long time (see Mischel and Ayduk delay time during childhood and later ratings on a
2004). Although it is believed that the delay of variety of healthy characteristics. They noted that
gratification paradigm applies to adolescents, delaying behavior in childhood predicted a set of
researchers who study the delay of gratification cognitive and social competencies and stress toler-
during adolescence tend to refer to it or view it as a ance consistent with experimental analyses of the
part of the constructs of impulse control, self- process underlying effective delay when the chil-
control, or self-regulation. Regardless of the dren were in preschool. Children who are able to
term used, researchers tend to view the delay of wait longer during childhood, for example, were
gratification as the key component of children’s rated, by parents, as more rational, planful, atten-
ability to exercise self-control, as a disposition tive, tolerant of stress and frustration, as well as
that is quite stable over time, and a basic and academically and socially competent. The ability to
positive aspect of personality. delay gratification apparently stabilizes early and
its benefits continue.
Research demonstrating continuity in terms of
Delay of Gratification During related outcomes is important, but it remains a
Adolescence challenge to study delay of gratification during
adolescence. The study of self-regulation during
Given that the above constructs typically studied childhood typically uses Mischel’s (1974) delay
during adolescence relate to the phenomenon of of gratification assessment procedure. This
delayed gratification, there is value to examining method involves leaving young children alone
important research in this area in order to under- with two attractive reward objects, such as one
stand the roots of what appears during adoles- versus two marshmallows. The children’s ability
cence. The ability to self-regulate or otherwise to delay gratification is determined by evaluating
control one’s impulses is important for a variety what they do after they have been instructed that
of reasons. It is important for understanding ado- they will receive the larger reward if they wait for
lescents’ own behaviors, especially their risk tak- the experimenter to return. If they cannot wait and
ing. It is also important for greater society given call the experimenter to return, they receive the
that the difficulties in exerting self-control link to smaller reward. The ability to delay gratification is
numerous problem behaviors such as substance measured by the time that the child delays ringing
abuse, risky sexual activity, and violence (Carroll the bell. Studies of adolescents have tried to tap
et al. 2009). Indeed, the ability to control oneself into the same process as Mischel’s delay of grat-
and act in ways that consider the future certainly ification paradigm. Although those studies have
constitutes a key aspect of human development. used approaches that are subject to criticism, they
Individual differences in self-control are read- nevertheless identify trends similar to those found
ily apparent early in life and tend to be fairly stable in research conducted with younger participants.
across the life span. Children with self-regulatory Given how rewards work, it is understandably
deficits show more psychopathology, are more difficult to design appropriate experiments that
readily frustrated and aggressive, pay less atten- would meaningfully tap into mechanisms relating
tion in school, and have lower academic success to Mischel’s delay procedure. Incentives are diffi-
than children who exercise self-control. As ado- cult to find for adolescents, and it is difficult to
lescents, such children have deficits in their aca- determine what a meaningful delay would be for
demic, cognitive, and social competencies, and them. Still, some have used analogue measures such
these deficits persist into adulthood (Ayduk as choices between hypothetical money rewards
et al. 2000). In their study of delay of gratification, and financial incentives (Funder and Block 1989;
for example, Mischel et al. (1988) assessed Krueger et al. 1994; Wulfert et al. 2002). These
Delay of Gratification 911

studies, as well as some that use other methods, systems. Hot systems are those that are impul-
generally report consistent findings that support sive (the “go” systems) and the cold ones are
the positive view of the ability to delay gratification. those that are more cool and deliberate (the
A few studies provide strong examples of this “know” systems). The ability to delay gratifica-
area of research. For example, Funder and Block tion comes when an individual is able to exert
(1989) present their findings from experiments the willpower needed to “cool” or self-distance
that assessed 14-year-old subjects who chose oneself from negative arousal in frustrating or
between immediate monetary payment and otherwise difficult situations. These controls
larger, delayed payments on five occasions. prevent the use of “hot” impulsive responses D
They found that, compared to those who delayed, that would inhibit the ability to achieve longer-
adolescents who were unable to contain their term goals. This observation has led researchers
impulse for immediate gratification were rated to try to identify ways to help individuals cool
as less intelligent and more rebellious, self- their systems, such as by using purposeful self-
indulgent, irritable, restless, aggressive, and gen- distraction and cognitive reframing (Mischel
erally not self-controlled and hostile. In another and Ayduk 2004). The ability to work though
study (Funder et al. 1983), the same research these systems is seen as not only leading to
team reports that boys who delayed gratification positive life outcomes, as noted above, but also
tended to be described as deliberative, attentive, to protecting individuals from long-term conse-
reasonable, reserved, cooperative, and able to quences of dispositional vulnerabilities.
modulate motivational and emotional impulses.
Girls who delayed gratification were consistently
Conclusion
described in such positive terms as intelligent,
resourceful, and competent. Girls who did not
Studies have had difficulty studying the process
delay were viewed as tending to fall apart under
of delaying gratification during adolescence.
stress and characterized as easily offended,
Those that do exist tend to reveal patterns similar
sulky, and whiny. Girls who were less able to
to those found during early childhood. The stud-
delay gratification were also more likely to be
ies also find that a disposition conducive to
victimized by other children. Wulfert
delaying gratification tends to be quite stable
et al. (2002) found that adolescents who were
over time, even though it may manifest itself
deemed more impulsive had low self-esteem,
differently. Although researchers who study the
underperformed in school, and engaged in a vari-
adolescent period adopt different terminologies
ety of behaviors deemed problematic, such as
and focus on different aspects of the ability to
cigarette smoking, drinking, marijuana use, and
delay gratification, it is clear that the capacity to
fighting. Together, these studies indicate that
delay gratification is important during adoles-
adolescents who are unwilling to delay gratifica-
cence and that this actually is the very time
tion show a broad range of personality attributes,
when real-life difficulties arise from youth’s
attitudes, and behaviors that might be described
inability to control their impulses.
as signs of being undercontrolled.
The mechanisms that account for the ability to
delay gratification have been studied most exten- Cross-References
sively in children, although the mechanisms
appear to transfer to adolescents. The leading ▶ Self-Regulation
researchers in this area, Mischel and his colleagues
(see, e.g., Mischel and Ayduk 2002, 2004), frame
References
the ability to delay in terms of “willpower”
dynamics. They use a framework that casts Ayduk, O., Mendoza-Denton, R., Mischel, W., Downey, G.,
mechanisms in terms of the interaction between Peake, P. K., & Rodriguez, M. (2000). Regulating the
“hot” and “cold” cognitive decision-making interpersonal self: Strategic self-regulation for coping
912 Delinquency

with rejection sensitivity. Journal of Personality and offenders; it may be lower than 14 years of age
Social Psychology, 79, 776–792. to as high as 21 years (Levesque 2000). In recent
Carroll, A., Houghton, S., Durkin, K., & Hattie, J. (2009).
Adolescent reputations and risk: Developmental trajec- years, there has been a tendency to include viola-
tories to delinquency. New York: Springer. tions of regulations that would not ordinarily be
Funder, D. C., & Block, J. (1989). The role of ego-control, considered crimes, such as, truancy and running
ego-resiliency, and IQ in delay of gratification in ado- away from home. Equally making it difficult to
lescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 57, 1041–1050. maintain a general rule is the variety of ways that
Funder, D. C., Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1983). Delay of offenses that were once deemed delinquent acts
gratification: Some longitudinal personality correlates. could now be charged as crimes by transferring
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, juveniles to adult court and by having many
1198–1213.
Krueger, R. F., Caspi, A., Moffit, T. W., White, J., & offenses that confer dual jurisdiction with juvenile
Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1994). Delay of gratification, and adult courts (Polachek 2009).
psychopathy, and personality: Is low self-control spe-
cific to externalizing problems? Journal of Personality,
64, 107–129. Cross-References
Mischel, W. (1974). Processes in delay of gratification. In
L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 249–292). San Diego: Academic. ▶ Deviancy
Mischel, W., & Ayduk, O. (2002). Self-regulation in a ▶ Deviancy Training
cognitive-affective personality system: Attentional con- ▶ Gangs
trol in the service of the self. Self and Identity, 1, 113–120.
Mischel, W., & Ayduk, O. (2004). Willpower in a
cognitive-affective processing system: The dynamics References
of delay of gratification. In R. F. Baumeister &
K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation:
Research, theory, and applications (pp. 99–129). New Levesque, R. J. R. (2000). Adolescents, sex and the law:
York: Guilford. Preparing adolescents for responsible citizenship.
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Peak, P. (1988). The nature of Washington: American Psychological Association.
adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay Polachek, E. A. (2009). Juvenile transfer: From “Get Bet-
of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- ter” to “Get Tough” to where we go from here. William
chology, 54, 4–10. Mitchell Law Review, 35, 1162–1193.
Wulfert, E., Block, J. A., Ana, E. S., Rodriguez, M. L., &
Colsman, M. (2002). Delay of gratification: impulsive
choices and problem behaviors in early and late ado-
lescence. Journal of Personality, 70, 533–552.
Delinquency Trajectories

Frances P. Abderhalden1 and Sara Z. Evans2


1
Department of Criminology and Criminal
Delinquency Justice, University of West Florida, Pensacola,
FL, USA
2
Roger J. R. Levesque Department of Criminology and Criminal
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA Justice, University of West Florida, Pensacola,
FL, USA

Delinquency is a behavior that refers to the viola-


tion of law that, if occurring in an adult, would be Developmental Trajectories
considered a crime. That general definition, how-
ever, is fraught with so many exceptions that it is Overview/Introduction
now a challenge to view it as a general rule. In the During adolescence, delinquency is closer to
United States, the age at which an offense is being normative than at any other point in the
defined as crime rather than a delinquent act varies life course. It is also the case, however, that
by characteristics of crimes, victims, and adolescents demonstrate dramatically different
Delinquency Trajectories 913

patterns or trajectories of deviant behavior during and discontinuity in behavior. Their theory of
this period. These trajectories vary with regard to age-graded informal social control tries to explain
onset, seriousness, and desistance of delinquent trajectories of offending by identifying changing
behavior. Explaining these differing patterns of social influences over time. Their intent is to
externalizing behavior problems is important include both childhood as well as experiences
because there is strong evidence that some por- later in life to understand developmental
tend negative consequences during adulthood. outcomes.
The likelihood of criminal behavior persisting Moffitt (1993, 1997) developed a dual-
from childhood and teenaged years into adulthood taxonomy model in which she identified two tra- D
can be predicted in part by an individual’s trajec- jectories of delinquent behavior: adolescent-
tory of delinquent behavior. limited and life-course-persistent trajectories.
She argues that these two trajectories vary in
timing and length of antisocial behavior. For
Theoretical Perspectives on Delinquency most individuals the highest rate of delinquency
Trajectories is found during adolescence. Much past research
has shown that offending drops dramatically by
Life course theory is useful in examining patterns age 16–17 for most persons and remains very low
of offending from a developmental perspective. thereafter. For a small subset of offenders, how-
This approach emphasizes factors such as social ever, offending continues to increase or remains
ties to others, how the past shapes the future, and stable throughout life. These two offending pat-
the importance of timing (Elder 1998). With terns overlap in adolescence, which makes it dif-
regard to antisocial behavior, the theory posits ficult to identify differences during this time
that childhood behavioral characteristics and period; a prediction has been supported in recent
experiences determine whether an individual will research (Young 2014). Life-course-persistent
act out in deviant ways just as a teenager and then offenders engage in antisocial behavior at earlier
desist, as most do, or will become a persistent ages and at every stage of life. Factors such as
offender throughout their life. Laub and Sampson inept parenting, cognitive deficits, and psycholog-
(2003) integrated life course principals with their ical traits such as low self-control lead to early
social control framework and note that various interactions with teachers, law enforcement, and
sources of influence can affect an individual’s other authority figures. Most of these interactions
pattern of behavior in both positive and negative will lead to negative consequences, which in turn
ways. They conceptualize these events as “times limit options for a change in lifestyle, a process
of decision or opportunity when life trajectories whereby “life course persistents” become
may be directed on to more adaptive or maladap- ensnared by the consequences of their antisocial
tive paths” (Rutter and Rutter 1993, p. 244). They behavior (Moffitt et al. 2002; Woodward
extend the life course perspective by adding an et al. 2002). This process, known as “cumulative
emphasis on an age-graded theory of informal continuity,” is a gradual buildup of consequences
social control and argue that throughout an indi- associated with delinquent or antisocial behavior,
vidual’s life social institutions are influential to wherein each individual act snowballs upon the
differing degrees (Laub and Sampson 2003). For results of previous acts, making it more and more
example, children and preteens are influenced difficult for one to change the trajectory of their
more heavily by their parents than older adoles- own life (Moffitt 1997).
cents, but as youth reach adolescence, the influ- Adolescent-limited delinquency refers to anti-
ence of parental behaviors and attitudes begins to social behavior during adolescence, whether in
fade in favor of peers (Steinberg and Monahan the form of status offenses (e.g., truancy, drinking
2007; Hoffmann and Cerbone 1999). Unlike other underage) or other minor delinquent acts such as
theories of antisocial behavior, Sampson and shoplifting. These individuals have more norma-
Laub (2003) attempt to explain both continuity tive childhoods and do not begin to display
914 Delinquency Trajectories

antisocial or delinquent behaviors until a later age. issue show that with the exception of stepparents,
By middle adolescence these behaviors are nor- youth in households with two parents or care-
mative and expected but decline with age (Moffitt givers have the most positive outcomes while
1997). For adolescent-limited offenders, respon- those in households with only one parent or care-
sibilities of work and/or school begin to take giver tend to have more behavior problems in
precedence over delinquent activities and most comparison (Simons et al. 2006a).
begin to realize that these activities may threaten
their future success. In addition, some of the acts Family Factors
that were delinquent or taboo (i.e., drinking, sex- One of the best known predictors of antisocial
ual activity) become more acceptable as they behavior stems from parenting practices (Simons
grow older (Moffitt 1997). There is some support et al. 2004). Some have found that maltreatment
in the literature for the effect of this “maturity during adolescence is more predictive of
gap” on adolescent-limited offending (Young increased offending, as compared to during youn-
2014; Barnes and Beaver 2010). Finally, these ger years (Stewart et al. 2008). Similarly, Pires
adolescent-limited offenders also do not have the and Jenkins (2007) found that effects of parental
same psychological and familial deficits as most warmth and rejection on drug use differed based
life-course-persistent offenders and are not bat- on the age of the respondent. Rejection was more
tling against a history of antisocial behavior that influential for young adolescents (age 10) than
has accumulated for years. This results in better warmth, while the opposite was true later in ado-
opportunities for success in postsecondary educa- lescence. They suggest that positive attributes of
tion and the work force. There have been numer- parent–child relationships such as warmth may
ous studies that investigated the presence of these inadvertently be interpreted by the child as
two patterns through adolescence (for a review approval for their behaviors, even if those behav-
see Moffitt 2003). iors are antisocial in nature.
Besemer and Farrington (2012) investigated
Factors Influencing Trajectory Group the delinquency trajectories of children who
Membership have sporadic and chronic offending fathers.
Past research indicates that there are many factors They look at how intergenerational criminal
that distinguish between offenders who desist behavior is related to offspring criminality.
early in life and those that who do not, including Using data from the Cambridge Study in Delin-
contact with delinquent peers (Miller et al. 2009); quent Development with 411 males from London,
cognitive abilities (Piquero and White 2003); and as well as some with female siblings who were
victimization (Cullen et al. 2008). Further, some factored into the study as offspring, they found
events that are somewhat normative to adults, that the sons’ conviction trajectories resulted in
such as a residential move or the death of a friend, five groups: nonoffenders, low desisters, low
may have a more lasting negative impact on chil- chronic offenders, and high desisters, and chronic
dren or adolescents (Hoffmann and Cerbone offenders (Besemer and Farrington 2012). The
1999). Finally, family of origin factors are found daughters’ conviction trajectories were similar
to play an important role in adolescent problem but only resulted in three groups: nonoffenders,
behaviors. For example, parenting practices con- low desisting offenders, and chronic offenders.
sistent with authoritative parenting such as Overall, chronic offending fathers did not predict
warmth (Simons and Conger 2007) and harsh chronic offending children significantly different
discipline (Simons et al. 2013) are negatively than low desisting fathers. This means that con-
associated with delinquency for adolescents, viction of any kind was the biggest predictor of
regardless of SES or ethnicity (Simons delinquent behavior, regardless if the father
et al. 2013, 2014). Scholars have also investigated desisted or persisted (Besemer and Farrington
the influence of family structure on childhood 2012). Yonai et al. (2015) also examined how
behavioral outcomes. Studies investigating this parental convictions influenced the trajectories of
Delinquency Trajectories 915

delinquency. They found similar results that con- categories are similar to prior literature, with non-
viction, or incarceration, was a bigger predictor of delinquent, minor persisting, moderate desisting,
later chronic offending for the juveniles as com- serious persisting, and serious desisting trajecto-
pared to chronic relative offending. Parental ries found. They identified three parenting tech-
imprisonment matters more in the chronic trajec- niques, authoritative, authoritarian, and
tory of children, than persistent offending by par- neglectful, to compare against the development
ents (Besemer and Farrington 2012; Yonai of delinquency in the youth. They found that
et al. 2015). parenting style does matter, but is not the sole
The relationship between harsh discipline and factor, in regards to non- or minor delinquents D
behavioral outcomes is studied substantially in versus more serious offenders. Serious persisters
prior literature (For reviews see (Gershoff 2002; were significantly more likely to stem from fam-
Hicks-Pass 2009; Larzelere and Kuhn 2005; ilies using authoritarian parenting as compared to
Paolucci and Violato 2004). The relationship nondelinquents. The researchers do point out that
studied most prominently in this type of research parenting is not the only factor that should be
is that between corporal punishment and antiso- considered, as all of the sample prior offending
cial behavior. There is a significant amount of was the most predictive of continual offending.
empirical support that adolescents who are phys- Keijsers and colleagues (2012) investigated the
ically punished by their parents display increased transformation of parent–child relationships in
levels of antisocial behavior, when using scaled later childhood and adolescence to see if trajecto-
measures of antisocial behavior indicators ries change. They used the Pittsburgh Youth Study
(Grogan-Kaylor 2004, 2005; McKee et al. 2007; and a male only sample to look at the quality of the
Simons et al. 2000; Straus and Mouradian 1998; parent–child relationship, joint activities between
Straus et al. 1997). the parent and child, and the general delinquency
While disciplinary techniques are critical in of the children. Five offending trajectories were
understanding the effects of parenting on child- uncovered: nonoffenders, moderate childhood
hood outcomes, additional components of the offenders, adolescent-limited offenders, serious
parent–child relationship are consequential as childhood offenders, and serious persistent
well. Multiple scholars argue that “authoritative” offenders (Keijsers et al. 2012). The differences
parents foster the most prosocial children; author- in parent–child relationship differ between the
itative parenting is illustrated as support and nur- trajectory groups and provide support for the
turance but also a reasonable amount of control developmental changes mattering in delinquent
which includes setting rules and enforcing conse- behavior. Nonoffenders reported high and stable
quences for breaking those rules (Meteyer and quality of relationship between their parents,
Perry-Jenkins 2009; Simons et al. 2005). Author- while serious persistent offenders reported poorer
itative parenting is associated with numerous quality of relationships across their childhood and
positive outcomes for children, including psycho- adolescence. These results indicate that parenting
logical adjustment and school achievement factors matter when following offending trajecto-
(Simons et al. 2004, 2007). Researchers point ries of youth.
out, however, that it is not simply control in iso- Baglivio et al. (2015) investigated how adverse
lation that promotes these positive outcomes but childhood experiences impact delinquency trajec-
the control coupled with support and warmth tories of youth over time from 7 to 17. Using data
(Simons et al. 2000). from 64,000 adjudicated juveniles in Florida, they
Hoeve and colleagues (2008) examined how identified five trajectory groups and how the expo-
delinquent trajectories and parenting styles are sure to adverse childhood experiences impacted
related. Using the Pittsburgh Youth Study, they early onset and chronic offending risk factors.
wanted to see the extent that parenting styles Similarly, Murphy and colleagues (2012) exam-
predicted the different trajectory paths of adoles- ined how protective factors changed the trajectory
cence between 10 and 19. The five trajectory of delinquent offending versus the vulnerability
916 Delinquency Trajectories

factors (read adverse childhood experiences) increased interest in investigating the relative out-
impacted risk factors for youth. Murphy comes of cohabitating families, as this form of
et al. (2012) used the National Longitudinal Sur- family is increasing in popularity (Amato 2005).
vey of Youth with a sample size of 8,984 (49% In general, cohabitating couples have a negative
female) to see how these protective factors at impact on behavior, with results consistently the
home were predictive factors for delinquency tra- effect, although not as significantly as in single-
jectories. In both samples (Baglivio et al. 2015; parent households (Amato 2005; Apel and
Murphy et al. 2012), exposure to adverse child- Kaukinen 2008; Cavanagh and Huston 2006).
hood experiences, or low protective factors, was Family transitions into new forms often are dis-
predictive of high delinquency trajectory groups. ruptive and upsetting to an adolescent, so it is not
The adverse nature of parenting and environmen- surprising that these transitions are linked to
tal support resulted in trajectory groups of high increased behavior problems and outbursts
delinquency rates and more persistence in crimi- (Magnuson and Berger 2009; Petts 2009).
nal behavior than other trajectory groups com- Recently, two studies found that a shift into a
pared to higher protective factors. single-mother family structure was associated
Effectively monitoring and supervising chil- more significantly with antisocial behavior
dren is a key element in authoritative parenting. than a transition into a two-parent household
Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents is (Langenkamp and Frisco 2008; Magnuson and
shown to be related to parental monitoring/super- Berger 2009).
vision (Simons et al. 2004). Parents who are more Effects of transitions may also differ based on
aware of where their children are and how they are the type of family structure that occurs. For exam-
spending their time have less opportunity to ple, Juby and Farrington (2001) found that paren-
engage in delinquent behaviors. Multiple studies tal divorce was more important in predicting
have supported this connection, either in the form delinquency than parental death. While separation
of authoritative/supportive parenting or a unique of the family unit negatively impacted an adoles-
effect of limit setting or supervision (Juon cents’ behavior, regardless of the reason for sepa-
et al. 2006; Kirk 2009; Meteyer and Perry-Jenkins ration, the degree of negative behavior was
2009; Middleton et al. 2009). Additionally, the dependent on the reason. Murray and Farrington
components of the guardians own antisocial (2005) investigated how incarceration of parents
behavior or mental health may also impact the effected boys’ antisocial behavior. They argue
offending trajectories of adolescents. that imprisonment of a parent may be a particu-
Family structure is also investigated for having larly important risk factor for delinquency
an influence on the childhood behavioral out- because of the example it may set for appropriate
comes. It is quickly becoming the societal norm behavior. In line with this thinking, they found
to come from households without both biological parental imprisonment is a strong predictor of
parents (Amato 2005). This decrease in being delinquency through the life course for boys
raised with a family form that includes both bio- and that separation was the most important
logical parents leads to support research that chil- factor in predicting the antisocial behavior and
dren are more at risk for developing behavioral, delinquency.
emotional, and cognitive difficulties (Amato Krohn et al. (2009), in contrast, found that the
2005; Simons et al. 2004). In general, results effect of family transitions differed by gender.
show that children raised in families with both Family transitions impact on delinquency and
biological parents have the most positive out- drug use through direct and indirect effects on
comes. Households with only one biological par- the peer relationships, and school performance
ent, or with a stepparent, are more inclined to have of the youth was the primary investigation of the
behavior problems in comparison (Apel and authors. They found that there were no direct or
Kaukinen 2008; Cavanagh and Huston 2006; indirect effects of family transitions for females.
Simons et al. 2006a). Recently there has been an Nevertheless, number of family transitions
Delinquency Trajectories 917

directly influenced delinquency and indirectly steady (low level) group in relation to the incre-
through peer interactions for males. mental and high starter group (Park et al. 2010).
Bradshaw and colleagues (Bradshaw
Race/Ethnicity et al. 2010) identified trajectories of antisocial
Although there are a few notable exceptions (Park behavior using the same methodology as above
et al. 2008, 2010), most of the research on trajec- with the intent to predict experiences of later
tories of offending has focused on Caucasian sam- negative life outcomes. Using a sample of Balti-
ples. Piquero and White (2003) investigated the more African Americans, they hypothesized that
effect of cognitive abilities on possessing charac- individuals within two different types of “early D
teristics identified by Moffitt (1993) as more prev- starter” trajectories would be at a greater risk for
alent among life-course-persistent offenders using these negative life events (such as substance use,
an African American sample. They found that dropping out of school, teen pregnancy, etc.).
increased cognitive abilities negatively affected Their results supported this hypothesis for both
one’s likelihood of being a life-course-persistent boys and girls; membership in an early starter
offender. The identification of an individual as a trajectory predicted the experience of a larger
life-course-persistent offender in this study was number of negative outcomes, as compared to
based upon criteria specified by Moffitt (1993) later starter groups.
as hallmarks of this type of offender. Racial Higgins et al. (2012) examined the trajectories
discrimination is linked to a variety of negative of delinquency and victims of bullying with an
outcomes in regards to African American adoles- African American sample. Using the National
cents, including cognitive abilities like depression Longitudinal Survey of the Youth they included
(Dubois et al. 2002; Simons et al. 2002), physical a sample of Hispanic and African American youth
health problems (Williams and Mohammed to determine if there is an intersection of victimi-
2009), anger (Simons et al. 2003, 2006b), as zation from bullying and delinquency trajectories.
well as other forms of externalizing behaviors They found three delinquency trajectory groups: a
such as delinquency (Burt 2009; Simons low stable group, a desisting group, and a high
et al. 2006b). and changing group (Higgins et al. 2012). They
Park and colleagues (Park et al. 2008, 2010) also found that bullying was found between the
have conducted two studies identifying trajectory two highest groups of delinquency trajectories,
groups with growth mixture modeling (Muthen the desisting group and the high and changing
and Muthen 1997–2008) and using the Mobile group, with bullying victimization trajectories
Youth Survey, an inner-city African American being the lowest for the low and stable delin-
population. Their research uncovered three trajec- quency trajectory group (Higgins et al. 2012).
tory groups, including a “steady” (but low level) These findings support that bullying may be con-
group displaying little delinquency, an “incremen- sidered a “street credibility” influence on African
tal” group showing a stable increase in delin- American youth, as well as considered a sociali-
quency over time, and a “high starter” group that zation process to gain acceptance into delinquent
begin with a high level of delinquency but peer groups.
declined over time. Further analysis on these tra- Evans et al. (2016) looked specifically at the
jectory groupings indicated that the high starter trajectories of an African American sample,
and incremental group fared worse in terms of examining trajectories of offending from age
variables such as substance use, self-esteem, and 10 to 19 using longitudinal data from a sample
parental monitoring (Park et al. 2008). In a follow- of nearly 400 African American males. The
up study with the same sample, the same authors results indicated there were four groups of
predicted membership in trajectory group with offenders displaying distinct patterns of behavior:
multivariate analyses. They found that similar a low-level group, an early starter/declining
factors of parental control, school suspension, group, a late starter group, and an early starter/
and substance use predicted membership in the chronic group. Consistent with prior research
918 Delinquency Trajectories

(Park et al. 2008, 2010), there were multiple tra- look at long-term trajectories. They found that
jectory groups, including the presence of a late gang affiliation does predict future offending as
starter group. The results from this study indicated an adult and that delinquent peer association
that individuals who were exposed to early and results in less serious long-term offending. These
increasing levels of racial discrimination were findings show that deviant peers can vary from
more likely to be in the early starter groups as gang affiliation, which Dong and Krohn (2016)
compared to negligible delinquents. Those that argue also impacts family variables, to traditional
had more delinquent friends were also more likely misbehavior of delinquent peer groups, both
to be in the early onset and persistent delinquent resulting in trajectory paths of crime.
groups when compared to the lower level delin- Similarly, Brook and colleagues (2013) inves-
quents. Recent research (Burt et al. 2012) high- tigated whether membership in a delinquent tra-
lights the importance of racial discrimination as a jectory group can predict long-term offending.
correlate of crime and delinquency, and Evans Membership in trajectory groups between the
et al. (2016) add to this growing body of empirical ages of 14 and 24 were used to determine if a
support. Increases in racial discrimination also relationship was found to higher violence, deviant
significantly predicted group membership in the peers, substance abuse, and other risk factors later
expected direction. This study, unlike most of the in life. The sample consisted of African American
others using an African American population, and Puerto Rican individuals from New York
does not rely upon data from urban inner-city City. They found that those in desisting trajectory
populations. groups during adolescence did not persist to
engage in deviant behavior or with deviant peers
Deviant Peers in adulthood. They also found that membership in
Deviant peers are considered one of the strongest high persistence trajectory groups in adolescence
correlates for delinquent behavior. Deviant peer was a predictor of future violence and criminal
networks which foster negative behaviors are fre- behavior in adulthood, compared to moderate
quently studied in criminology (Warr 2002), with delinquent trajectories. Overall, Brook
most researchers taking one of two viewpoints. et al. (2013) found that membership in trajectory
The first, endorsed by Gottfredson and Hirschi groups as an adolescent did help predict future
(1990), suggests that deviant peers seek out trajectories in adulthood.
those who are similar to themselves, so they
“self-select” into deviant peer groups. In contrast, Age of Onset
the other argues that deviance is learned through Moffitt (1993, 1997) argues that an especially
peer networking, meaning that association with important predictor of childhood- versus
antisocial peers exacerbates or teaches deviance adolescent-onset delinquency is age of onset. This
to youth that would otherwise be prosocial (Akers was investigated by Dandreaux and Frick (2009)
and Sellers 2009; Dong and Krohn 2016). using two samples of pre-adjudicated boys, one
Regardless of the mechanism, peers influence identified as childhood onset and the other as ado-
each other to engage in both deviant and conven- lescent onset. They uncovered that more severe
tional behaviors. A multitude of research has aggression was found primarily in children from
focused on how involvement with delinquent the childhood-onset group. This group also dem-
peers contributes to an individual’s continued onstrated higher levels of family disorder and
and increased delinquent behavior, even after con- showed higher rates of callous-unemotional traits.
trolling for background characteristics including Furthermore, the adolescent-onset group displayed
self-control (Button et al. 2007; Matsueda and lower levels of traditionalism or support for tradi-
Anderson 1998; Miller et al. 2009). Dong and tional values. A recent study also found links
Krohn (2016) expand on this deviant peer and between age of onset and violence, although the
trajectory relationship by looking at gang affilia- effects only lasted through adolescence
tion, as well as the delinquent peer association, to (Tzoumakis et al. 2012). Results from this study
Delinquency Trajectories 919

indicated that although early onset was linked to low-rate chronic offenders, and high-rate chronic
violent behavior in adolescence, there were not offenders. Human capital indicators were found to
significant impacts on violence in adulthood. be most prevalent in the very low-rate chronic
offenders, as compared to the other two trajectory
Employment groups. This result indicates some evidence of
Employment often has no effect on delinquency education and occupational skills having an
after controlling for differences between workers impact on criminal trajectory paths.
and nonworkers (Apel et al. 2007); yet, these
authors did find one positive effect for employ- Cognitive Abilities D
ment for group of adolescents who are at risk for One characteristic identified by Moffitt (1993,
antisocial behavior at an early age. Skardhamar 1997) as a feature that can distinguish between
and Savolainen (2012) examined if employment life-course-persistent and adolescent-limited
could change the trajectory of an individual. offenders are cognitive abilities. Results are some-
They investigate if the trajectory of antisocial what mixed, with some indicating a link between
behavior is changed at the point of job entry and cognitive deficits and delinquency, including low
if this transition is the point of desistence for IQ (Wolfgang et al. 1972) as well as other neuro-
offenders. Using a sample of 1,063 males psychological measures (Donnellan et al. 2000;
between 2001 and 2006, from the administrative Moffitt and Silva 1988; Piquero 2001; Piquero
registers data in Norway, they were able to merge and White 2003). Excelling in school can be one
this data with governmental databases to deter- manifestation of greater cognitive abilities that can
mine the intersection of job entry and desistence serve to buffer adolescents from pursuing criminal
(or persistence) in criminal activity. They found activity past common teenage delinquency.
that the majority of the sample had desisted from McCuish et al. (2014) examined how cognitive
criminal behavior prior to gainful employment abilities in regards to psychopathic traits are
and that the employment did not reduce any fur- related to offending and the trajectories of adoles-
ther criminal behavior. This study serves to pro- cences on a life-course-persistent offender. Using
vide a limitation in the employment literature on the Incarcerated Serious and Violent Young
trajectories, as the turning point was looked at as Offender Study, McCuish et al. (2014) investi-
the gain of employment and not the search for gated 243 males and 64 females on their violent
employment. Skardhamar and Savolainen (2012) offending trajectories. Additionally, they used the
suggest the turning point and desistence from Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version to rate the
criminal trajectories actually occurs when one symptoms and severity of the psychopathic traits
begins seeking employment and not when the for the youth. They found that antisocial behavior
employment is actually obtained. in regards to psychopathy was the only indicator
Education is often thought of as a turning point of membership in a high-risk trajectory, as com-
in life course literature, and human capital is often pared to a moderate-risk trajectory of offending.
described as utilizing learned occupational skills The presence of psychopathic traits is cumula-
(Piquero et al. 2014). Piquero and colleagues tively related to the chronic offending trajectories
(2014) studied the trajectories of longitudinal of youth, but at the individual level, only antiso-
offending from adolescence through early adult- cial traits are significantly supported (McCuish
hood to determine if social or human capital was et al. 2014). Reiterated in Corrado et al. (2015),
more predictive of antisocial behavior. Using data the individual nature of psychopathic traits makes
from the Pathways to Desistance, Piquero determining how trajectories are influenced by
et al. (2014) examined males and females of pri- psychosis alone difficult, but rather the break-
marily nonwhite demography, from 14 to 17 at the down of individual traits such as antisocial behav-
beginning of the study and through 7 year’s initial ior, high impulsivity, and aggressive behavior are
collection. They found three trajectory groups to the true indicators of chronic offending trajecto-
be present: very low-rate chronic offenders, ries in youth.
920 Delinquency Trajectories

Victimization than males throughout; however, both genders


Victimization and offending clearly linked desisted in similar fashion from all trajectory
(Lauritsen and Laub 2007), but the theoretical groups. Only males were found in the chronic
reasons behind this link and the causal direction trajectory group, with self-reported mild to mod-
is less clear (Ousey et al. 2011). A number of erate antisocial behaviors. All groups had some
studies have found that experiencing victimiza- trace of antisocial tendencies, but females were
tion exacerbates future offending behaviors less violent and desisted from criminal behavior
(Cullen et al. 2008; Daigle et al. 2007; Hay and sooner than males. Walters (2012), using the
Evans 2006). Kim and Lo (2015) found that not National Longitudinal Survey of Youth, also
only does experiencing victimization increases found that males had slightly more serious trajec-
delinquency trajectories, but who the victimiza- tories than females but found that females did
tion was delivered by also increases the delin- exist in the high trajectory group.
quency. They found that youth who were
victimized by parents belonged to a trajectory Immigration
group with a much sharper decline in delinquency Jennings et al. (2013) investigated how immigra-
during adolescence but projected long-term tion status relates to offending. A trajectory anal-
offending following the decline. While non- ysis found four trajectories of Hispanic
parental victimization resulted in membership in immigrants: very low-rate offenders, high-rate
a stable nonchronic offending trajectory. late-onset escalators, initially high-rate desisters,
and high-rate chronic offenders. Results indicated
Gender that Hispanic immigrants were more likely to be
There is substantial support that the processes desisters or low-rate offenders than native-born
leading to offending may be different for males Hispanics in the United States. Interestingly,
versus females (Giordano et al. 2002; Heimer and they also found that Hispanic immigrants who
De Coster 1999). Moffitt (2003) posits that this is were not married were the most likely to be
not necessarily true and that even though the high-rate late-onset escalators, with offending
proportion of males and females in each type of beginning in late adulthood. These findings only
group may be different, the predictors of group examined a singular state and immigrant group,
membership are the same. Silverthorn and Frick nevertheless, the insight into the links between
(1999) argue that this may be true but needs revi- immigration and trajectories provides insight
sion. They hypothesized that females will display into a specific group and intersection.
a delayed-onset pattern that is developmentally
similar to early onset boys. This viewpoint was Number of Trajectory Groups
tested by White and Piquero (2004), who found While a taxonomic approach may be appropriate,
mixed results. Females in their sample who dividing offenders into only two groups is
displayed a late onset of delinquent behavior problematic. While studies have consistently
were very similar to males who showed early shown the presence of the two groups, other dis-
onset. However, they also found the presence of tinct trajectory groups have appeared as well.
an early onset group in both gender samples, and Most recent research investigating trajectory
these comparisons showed that females fared groups has identified between three and five dis-
worse on about half of the risk factors considered. tinct groups, depending upon the sample (Piquero
Zheng and Cleveland (2013) recently investi- and White 2003), including a group characterized
gated gender differences in trajectories of devel- by antisocial behavior in childhood but not later in
opment in antisocial behavior. Using the National adolescence (Park et al. 2008) and a group
Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health and a displaying little antisocial behavior at any point
sample size of 6244, they found four different during childhood or adolescence (Roisman
trajectory groups: low, desister, decliner, and et al. 2010). Piquero and colleagues (2010) used
chronic. Females were consistently less violent the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development
Delinquency Trajectories 921

data to identify trajectories of offending. They et al. 2012). They found that early onset did not
were interested to see if, while controlling for predict violent behavior continuing into adult-
early environmental and individual risk factors, hood and that adult offenders specializing in vio-
an individuals’ membership in these trajectory lent crimes were not always adolescent offenders.
groups affected their likelihood of “life failure” These results help in developmental life course
at later ages. They identified five trajectory groups with the suggestion to look into the subcategories
and found support for their hypothesis, the pat- of versatility and specializations as the stages of
terns of offending predicted life failure at later criminal behavior patterns (Tzoumakis
ages net of the effect of early risk factors. et al. 2012). Young (2014) used Add Health data D
Offenders in the chronic trajectory group were to look at the trajectories of popularity (perceived
more likely to be heavy drug users and have and sociometric) to identify predictors of life-
more convictions than lower rate, nonoffenders, course-persistent individuals and adolescence-
and adolescent-peak offender trajectory groups. limited individuals in early childhood. They
Using the same data set described above, uncovered that children who seek popularity
Farrington et al. (2009) also investigated multiple through marijuana use and property crime ended
categories of offenders, resulting in four groups: being classified as adolescent-limited, with the
nonoffenders, adolescence-limited offenders, antisocial behavior ending at the end of their
late-onset offenders, and persistent offenders. childhood. However, the children who seek pop-
Farrington et al. (2009) were interested in a wide ularity through violent and aggressive means are
variety of family, school, socioeconomic, individ- more likely to continue to offend past their ado-
ual, and behavioral factors. Overall, results lescence, resulting in the life-course-persistent
supported the view that chronic offenders fared classification (Young 2014).
the worst in terms of most risk factors compared to
all other groups.
Bersani et al. (2009) also investigated trajecto- Methodological Limitations
ries of offending over the life course using a
sample of convicted criminal offenders in the While the methodologies that produce trajectories
Netherlands. They found some significant differ- have provided insight into adolescent delin-
ences in individual characteristics across trajec- quency, the assignment of individuals to a group,
tory group, yet they found no consistent pattern of especially when the behavior of interest is an
risk factors to differentiate individual offending undesirable one, has risks that need to be
patterns. Overall they had weak predictive power acknowledged as well. Nagin says, “Because the
for grouping individuals into trajectory groups; groups are intended as an approximation of a more
however, their data for offending was based on complex reality, the objective is not to identify the
official conviction records, and the number of ‘true’ number of groups. Instead, the aim is to
adolescent risk factors was narrow. identify as simple a model as possible that dis-
Tzoumakis and colleagues (2012) investigated plays the distinctive features of the population
the link between age of onset and violent offenses distribution of trajectories (2005, p. 173).” There
from adolescence through adulthood. They used are two particular risks associated with this anal-
the Montreal Longitudinal Study using a sample ysis (Nagin 2005). First, both the number of
of adjudicated youth, with 210 individuals used groups and the pattern each follows should not
for the final sample. The data for the final sample be interpreted as permanently fixed. The stability
has collection taken from different points in time, of the groups identified and their patterns are
early to mid-adolescence, mid- to late adoles- highly sensitive to the length of time being
cence, and early 30s. They found that while age assessed (Nagin 2005; Sampson et al. 2004).
of onset may be predictive of committing a violent Researchers should be cognizant of this and
offense in adolescence, the age of onset is not acknowledge it as a limitation. Secondly, while
predictive of adult violence (Tzoumakis identifying risk factors for increased delinquency
922 Delinquency Trajectories

is an important task, one must remain mindful that health backgrounds. They suggest that only
just because an event or characteristic is a risk through the comparison of adolescence-limited
factor does not imply that everyone who experi- and life-course-persistent offenders can the risk
ences that event or possesses that characteristic will assessment and prevention and treatment be
actually display increased delinquency (Nagin discussed (Jennings and Reingle 2012; Assink
2005). Trajectory models provide an avenue to et al. 2015). Future work should continue to inves-
summarize complex patterns in a parsimonious tigate these processes to further understand how
way. They “improve a researcher’s ability to iden- they affect offending patterns through young
tify, summarize, and communicate complex pat- adulthood and beyond, as well as for their impli-
terns in longitudinal data (Nagin 2005, p. 184).” cations for prevention of delinquency during
adolescence.

Conclusion
References
It is important that we continue to investigate
factors affecting patterns of offending through Akers, R. L., & Sellers, C. S. (2009). Criminological
adolescence and into early adulthood. Sampson theories: Introduction, evaluation, and application.
New York: Oxford University Press.
and Laub (2003, 1993) theorize that both early Amato, P. R. (2005). The impact of family formation
indicators such as parental relationships, as well change on the cognitive, social, and emotional well-
as experiences throughout adolescence, are being of the next generation. The Future of Children,
important in determining patterns of antisocial 15(2), 75–96.
Apel, R., & Kaukinen, C. (2008). On the relationship
behavior over the life course. Jennings and between family structure and antisocial behavior:
Reingle (2012) conducted a meta-analysis on Parental cohabitation and blended households. Crimi-
delinquency trajectories and found that trajectory nology, 46(1), 35–70.
research is broken into 2–7 groups, separated by Apel, R., Bushway, S., Brame, R., Haviland, A. M., Nagin,
D. S., & Paternoster, R. (2007). Unpacking the rela-
severity of offending, as well as persisters and tionship between adolescent employment and antiso-
desisters. They also found that there is consider- cial behavior: A matched samples comparison.
able variation between trajectory groupings, Criminology, 45(1), 67–97.
based on what is being studied and who the data Assink, M., van der Put, C. E., Hoeve, M., de Vries, S. L.,
Stams, G. J. J., & Oort, F. J. (2015). Risk factors for
comes from. No surprise, the majority of the tra- persistent delinquent behavior among juveniles:
jectory research is based out of Moffitt’s taxon- A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review,
omy and shaped with adolescence limited and life 42, 47–61.
course persisters. Finally, Jennings and Reingle Baglivio, M. T., Wolff, K. T., Piquero, A. R., & Epps, N.
(2015). The relationship between adverse childhood
(2012) suggest that research moves beyond the experiences (ACE) and juvenile offending trajectories
categorizing of groups of trajectories and severity, in a juvenile offender sample. Journal of Criminal
but back to theoretical implication and answering Justice, 43(3), 229–241.
research questions using the trajectories, as well Barnes, J. C., & Beaver, K. M. (2010). An empirical
examination of adolescence-limited offending:
as looking to unpack the influences and preven- A direct test of Moffitt’s maturity gap thesis. Journal
tion of the trajectory groups as compared to each of Criminal Justice, 38(6), 1176–1185.
other. Assink and colleagues (2015) also agree Bersani, B. E., Nieuwbeerta, P., & Laub, J. H. (2009).
with this continued research being more focused Predicting trajectories of offending over the life course:
Findings from a Dutch conviction cohort. Journal of
not on the grouping itself but on the implication Research in Crime and Delinquency, 46, 468–494.
from the trajectories and the comparisons between Besemer, S., & Farrington, D. P. (2012). Intergenerational
adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent transmission of criminal behaviour: Conviction trajec-
tracks. Assink et al. (2015) suggests looking tories of fathers and their children. European Journal of
Criminology, 9(2), 120–141.
more into the family unit and trajectory paths of Bradshaw, C. P., Schaeffer, C. M., Petras, H., & Ialongo, N.
mother, sibling, father, and grandparent related (2010). Predicting negative life outcomes from early
risk factors, as well as the mental and physical aggressive–disruptive behavior trajectories: Gender
Delinquency Trajectories 923

differences in maladaptation across life domains. Jour- persistent offenders from age 8 to age 48. Aggressive
nal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(8), 953–966. Behavior, 35(2), 150–163.
Brook, J. S., Lee, J. Y., Finch, S. J., Brown, E. N., & Gershoff, E. T. (2002). Corporal punishment by parents
Brook, D. W. (2013). Long-term consequences of and associated child behaviors and experiences:
membership in trajectory groups of delinquent behav- A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological
ior in an urban sample: Violence, drug use, interper- Bulletin, 128(4), 539.
sonal, and neighborhood attributes. Aggressive Giordano, P. C., Cernkovich, S. A., & Rudolph, J. L.
Behavior, 39, 440–452. (2002). Gender, crime, and desistance: Toward a theory
Burt, C. H. (2009). Discrimination and deviance: Racial of cognitive transformation. American Journal of Soci-
socialization matters. Athens: Sociology, The Univer- ology, 107(4), 990–1064.
sity of Georgia. Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory D
Burt, C. H., Simons, R. L., & Gibbons, F. X. (2012). Racial of crime. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
discrimination, ethnic-racial socialization, and crime: Grogan-Kaylor, A. (2004). The effect of corporal punish-
A microsociological model of risk and resilience. ment on antisocial behavior in children. Social Work
American Sociological Review, 77(4), 648–677. Research, 28(3), 153–162.
Button, T. M. M., Corley, R. P., Rhee, S. H., Hewitt, J. K., Grogan-Kaylor, A. (2005). Corporal punishment and the
Young, S. E., & Stallings, M. C. (2007). Delinquent growth trajectory of children’s antisocial behavior.
peer affiliation and conduct problems: A twin study. Child Maltreatment, 10(3), 283–292.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(3), 554–564. Hay, C., & Evans, M. M. (2006). Violent victimization and
Cavanagh, S. E., & Huston, A. C. (2006). Family instabil- involvement in delinquency: Examining predictions
ity and children’s early problem behavior. Social from general strain theory. Journal of Criminal Justice,
Forces, 85(1), 551–581. 34(3), 261–274.
Corrado, R. R., DeLisi, M., Hart, S. D., & McCuish, E. C. Heimer, K., & Coster, S. D. (1999). The gendering of
(2015). Can the causal mechanisms underlying chronic, violent delinquency. Criminology, 37(2), 277–318.
serious, and violent offending trajectories be elucidated Hicks-Pass, S. (2009). Corporal punishment in America
using the psychopathy construct? Journal of Criminal today. Best Practices in Mental Health, 5(2), 71–88.
Justice, 43, 251–261. Higgins, G. E., Khey, D. N., Dawson-Edwards, B. C., &
Cullen, F. T., Unnever, J. D., Hartman, J. L., Turner, M. G., Marcum, C. D. (2012). Examining the link between
& Agnew, R. (2008). Gender, bullying victimization, being a victim of bullying and delinquency trajectories
and juvenile delinquency: A test of general strain the- among an African American sample. International
ory. Victims and Offenders, 3, 346–364. Criminal Justice Review. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Daigle, L. E., Cullen, F. T., & Wright, J. P. (2007). Gender 1057567712443965 (bullying).
differences in the predictors of juvenile delinquency Hoeve, M., Blokland, A., Dubas, J. S., Loeber, R., Gerris,
assessing the generality-specificity debate. Youth Vio- J. R., & Van der Laan, P. H. (2008). Trajectories of
lence and Juvenile Justice, 5(3), 254–286. delinquency and parenting styles. Journal of Abnormal
Dandreaux, D. M., & Frick, P. J. (2009). Developmental Child Psychology, 36(2), 223–235.
pathways to conduct problems: A further test of the Hoffmann, J. P., & Cerbone, F. G. (1999). Stressful life
childhood and adolescent-onset distinction. Journal of events and delinquency escalation in early adolescence.
Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(3), 375–385. Criminology, 37(2), 343–374.
Dong, B., & Krohn, M. D. (2016). Dual trajectories of gang Jennings, W. G., & Reingle, J. M. (2012). On the number
affiliation and delinquent peer association during ado- and shape of developmental/life-course violence,
lescence: An examination of long-term offending out- aggression, and delinquency trajectories: A state-of-
comes. Journal of youth and adolescence, 45(4), 1–17. the-art review. Journal of Criminal Justice, 40(6),
Donnellan, M. B., Ge, X., & Wenk, E. (2000). Cognitive 472–489.
abilities in adolescent-limited and life-course-persistent Jennings, W. G., Zgoba, K. M., Piquero, A. R., & Reingle,
criminal offenders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, J. M. (2013). Offending trajectories among native-born
109(3), 396. and foreign-born Hispanics to late middle age. Socio-
Dubois, D. L., Burk-Braxton, C., Swenson, L. P., logical Inquiry, 83(4), 622–647.
Tevendale, H. D., & Hardesty, J. L. (2002). Race and Juby, H., & Farrington, D. P. (2001). Disentangling the link
gender influences on adjustment in early adolescence: between disrupted families and delinquency socio-
Investigation of an integrative model. Child Develop- demography, ethnicity and risk behaviours. British
ment, 73(5), 1573–1592. Journal of Criminology, 41(1), 22–40.
Elder, G. H. (1998). The life course as developmental Juon, H. S., Doherty, E. E., & Ensminger, M. E. (2006).
theory. Child Development, 69, 1–12. Childhood behavior and adult criminality: Cluster anal-
Evans, S. Z., Simons, L. G., & Simons, R. L. (2016). ysis in a prospective study of African Americans. Jour-
Factors that influence trajectories of delinquency nal of Quantitative Criminology, 22(3), 193–214.
throughout adolescence. Journal of youth and adoles- Keijsers, L., Loeber, R., Branje, S., & Meeus, W. (2012).
cence, 45(1), 1–16. Parent-child relationships of boys in different offending
Farrington, D. P., Ttofi, M. M., & Coid, J. W. (2009). trajectories: A developmental perspective. Journal of
Development of adolescence-limited, late-onset, and Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(12), 1222–1232.
924 Delinquency Trajectories

Kim, Y. S., & Lo, C. C. (2015). Short-and mid-term effects Moffitt, T. E. (1997). Adolescence-limited and life-course-
of violent victimization on delinquency a multilevel persistent offending: A complementary pair of devel-
growth-curve modeling approach. Journal of Interper- opmental theories. In T. P. Thornberry (Ed.), Develop-
sonal Violence, 0886260515580368. mental theories of crime and delinquency (Vol. 7,
Kirk, D. S. (2009). Unraveling the contextual effects on pp. 11–54). New Brunswick: Transaction.
student suspension and juvenile arrest: The indepen- Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Life-course persistent and
dent and interdependent influences of school, neigh- adolescent-limited antisocial behaviour: A ten-year
borhood, and family social controls. Criminology, research review and research agenda. In B. B. Lahey,
47(2), 479–520. T. E. Moffitt, & A. Caspi (Eds.), Causes of conduct
Krohn, M. D., Hall, G. P., & Lizotte, A. J. (2009). Family disorder and juvenile delinquency (pp. 49–75). New
transitions and later delinquency and drug use. Journal York: The Guilford Press.
of Youth and Adolescence, 38(3), 466–480. Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1988). Neuropsychological
Langenkamp, A. G., & Frisco, M. L. (2008). Family deficit and self-reported delinquency in an unselected
transitions and adolescent severe emotional distress: birth cohort. Journal of the American Academy of Child
The salience of family context. Social Problems, & Adolescent Psychiatry, 27(2), 233–240.
55(2), 238. Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Harrington, H., & Milne, B. J. (2002).
Larzelere, R. E., & Kuhn, B. R. (2005). Comparing child Males on the life-course-persistent and adolescence-
outcomes of physical punishment and alternative disci- limited antisocial pathways: Follow-up at 26 years. Devel-
plinary tactics: A meta-analysis. Clinical Child and opment and Psychopathology, 14, 179–207.
Family Psychology Review, 8(1), 1–37. Murphy, D. A., Brecht, M. L., Huang, D., & Herbeck,
Laub, J. H., & Sampson, R. J. (2003). Shared beginnings, D. M. (2012). Trajectories of delinquency from age
divergent lives: Delinquent boys to age 70. Cambridge, 14 to 23 in the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth
MA: Harvard University Press. sample. International Journal of Adolescence and
Lauritsen, J. L., & Laub, J. H. (2007). Understanding the Youth, 17(1), 47–62.
link between victimization and offending: New reflec- Murray, J., & Farrington, D. P. (2005). Parental imprison-
tions on an old idea. Crime Prevention Studies, 22, 55. ment: Effects on boys’ antisocial behaviour and delin-
Magnuson, K., & Berger, L. M. (2009). Family structure quency through the life-course. Journal of Child
states and transitions: Associations with children's Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(12), 1269–1278.
well-being during middle childhood. Journal of Mar- Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (1997–2008). Mplus. Los
riage and Family, 71(3), 575–591. Angeles: Muthen and Muthen.
Matsueda, R. L., & Anderson, K. (1998). The dynamics of Nagin, D. (2005). Group-based modeling of development.
delinquent peers and delinquent behavior. Criminology, Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
36(2), 269–308. Ousey, G. C., Wilcox, P., & Fisher, B. S. (2011). Something
McCuish, E. C., Corrado, R., Lussier, P., & Hart, S. D. old, something new: Revisiting competing hypotheses
(2014). Psychopathic traits and offending trajectories of the victimization-offending relationship among ado-
from early adolescence to adulthood. Journal of Crim- lescents. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 27(1),
inal Justice, 42(1), 66–76. 53–84.
McKee, L., Roland, E., Coffelt, N., Olson, A. L., Forehand, Paolucci, E. O., & Violato, C. (2004). A meta-analysis of
R., Massari, C., . . . & Zens, M. S. (2007). Harsh disci- the published research on the affective, cognitive, and
pline and child problem behaviors: The roles of positive behavioral effects of corporal punishment. The Journal
parenting and gender. Journal of Family Violence, of Psychology, 138(3), 197–222.
22(4), 187–196. Park, N. S., Lee, B. S., Bolland, J. M., Vazsonyi, A. T., &
Meteyer, K. B., & Perry-Jenkins, M. (2009). Dyadic par- Sun, F. (2008). Early adolescent pathways of antisocial
enting and children’s externalizing symptoms. Family behaviors in poor, inner-city neighborhoods. The Jour-
Relations, 58(3), 289–302. nal of Early Adolescence, 28(2), 185–205.
Middleton, M., Scott, S. L., & Renk, K. (2009). Parental Park, N. S., Lee, B. S., Sun, F., Vazsonyi, A. T., & Bolland,
depression, parenting behaviours, and behaviour prob- J. M. (2010). Pathways and predictors of antisocial
lems in young children. Infant and Child Development, behaviors in African American adolescents from poor
18(4), 323–336. neighborhoods. Children and Youth Services Review,
Miller, S., Loeber, R., & Hipwell, A. (2009). Peer devi- 32, 409–415.
ance, parenting and disruptive behavior among young Petts, R. J. (2009). Family and religious characteristics’
girls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(2), influence on delinquency trajectories from adolescence
139–152. to young adulthood. American Sociological Review,
Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course 74(3), 465–483.
persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxon- Piquero, A. (2001). Testing Moffitt’s neuropsychological
omy. Psychological Review, 100, 674–701. variation hypothesis for the prediction of life-course
Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Natural histories of delinquency. In persistent offending. Psychology Crime and Law, 7
Cross-national longitudinal research on human devel- (1–4), 193–215.
opment and criminal behavior (pp. 3–61). Dordecht: Piquero, A. R., & White, N. A. (2003). On the relationship
Springer. between cognitive abilities and life-course-persistent
Delinquency Trajectories 925

offending among a sample of African Americans: Simons, R. L., Simons, L. G., Burt, C. H., Brody, G. H., &
A longitudinal test of Moffitt’s hypothesis. Journal of Cutrona, C. (2005). Collective efficacy, authoritative
Criminal Justice, 31(5), 399–409. parenting and delinquency: A longitudinal test of a
Piquero, A. R., Farrington, D. P., Nagin, D. S., & Moffitt, model integrating community-and family-level pro-
T. E. (2010). Trajectories of offending and their relation cesses. Criminology, 43(4), 989–1029.
to life failure in late to middle age: Findings from the Simons, L. G., Chen, Y.-F., Simons, R. L., Brody, G., &
Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development. Journal Cutrona, C. (2006a). Parenting practices and child
of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 47, 151–173. adjustment in different types of households. Journal
Piquero, A. R., Jennings, W. G., Piquero, N. L., & Schu- of Family Issues, 27(6), 803–825.
bert, C. A. (2014). Human but not social capital is better Simons, R. L., Simons, L. G., Burt, C. H., Drummond, H.,
able to distinguish between offending trajectories in a Stewart, E. A., Brody, G. H., et al. (2006b). Supportive D
sample of serious adolescent Hispanic offenders. Jour- parenting moderates the effect of discrimination upon
nal of Criminal Justice, 42(4), 366–373. anger, hostile view of relationships, and violence
Pires, P., & Jenkins, J. M. (2007). A growth curve analysis among African American boys. Journal of Health and
of the joint influences of parenting affect, child charac- Social Behavior, 47, 373–389.
teristics and deviant peers on adolescent illicit drug use. Simons, R. L., Simons, L. G., Chen, Y. F., Brody, G. H., &
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36(2), 169–183. Lin, K. H. (2007). Identifying the psychological fac-
Roisman, G. I., Monahan, K. C., Campbell, S. B., tors that mediate the association between parenting
Steinberg, L., & Cauffman, E. (2010). Is adolescence- practices and delinquency. Criminology, 45(3),
onset antisocial behavior developmentally normative? 481–517.
Development and Psychopathology, 22(2), 295–311. Simons, L. G., Su, X., & Simons, R. L. (2013). Conse-
Rutter, M., & Rutter, M. (1993). Developing minds: Chal- quences of corporal punishment for African Ameri-
lenge and continuity across the life span. cans: The importance of context and outcome.
Harmondsworth/New York: Penguin/Basic books. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42, 1273–1285.
Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (1993). Crime in the making: Simons, L. G., Simons, R. L., Landor, A. M., Bryant,
Pathways and turning points through life. Cambridge, C. M., & Beach, S. R. H. (2014). Identifying links
MA: Harvard University Press. between childhood parenting experiences and adult
Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (2003). Life-course romantic relationships. Journal of Family Psychology,
desisters? Trajectories of crime among delinquent 28, 368–379.
boys followed to age 70. Criminology, 41(3), 555–592. Skardhamar, T., & Savolainen, J. (2012). Does employment
Sampson, R. J., Laub, J. H., & Eggleston, E. P. (2004). On contribute to desistance? Offending trajectories of
the robustness and validity of groups. Journal of Quan- crime-prone men around the time of job entry (No.
titative Criminology, 20(1), 37–42. 716). Statistics Norway Research department.
Silverthorn, P., & Frick, P. J. (1999). Developmental path- Steinberg, L., & Monahan, K. C. (2007). Age differences
ways to antisocial behavior: The delayed-onset path- in resistance to peer influence. Developmental Psychol-
way in girls. Development and Psychopathology, ogy, 43, 1531–1543.
11(01), 101–126. Stewart, A., Livingston, M., & Dennison, S. (2008). Tran-
Simons, L. G., & Conger, R. D. (2007). Linking sitions and turning points: Examining the links between
mother–father differences in parenting to a typology child maltreatment and juvenile offending. Child Abuse
of family parenting styles and adolescent outcomes. & Neglect, 32(1), 51–66.
Journal of Family Issues, 28(2), 212–241. Straus, M. A., & Mouradian, V. E. (1998). Impulsive
Simons, R. L., Wu, C. I., Lin, K. H., Gordon, L., & Conger, corporal punishment by mothers and antisocial behav-
R. D. (2000). A cross-cultural examination of the link ior and impulsiveness of children. Behavioral Sciences
between corporal punishment and adolescent antisocial & the Law, 16(3), 353–374.
behavior. Criminology, 38(1), 47–80. Straus, M. A., Sugarman, D. B., & Giles-Sims, J. (1997).
Simons, R. L., Murry, V., McLoyd, V., Lin, K. H., Cutrona, Spanking by parents and subsequent antisocial behav-
C., & Conger, R. D. (2002). Discrimination, crime, ior of children. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent
ethnic identity, and parenting as correlates of depres- Medicine, 151(8), 761–767.
sive symptoms among African American children: Tzoumakis, S., Lussier, P., Le Blanc, M., & Davies,
A multilevel analysis. Development and Psychopathol- G. (2012). Onset, offending trajectories, and crime
ogy, 14(02), 371–393. specialization in violence. Youth Violence and Juvenile
Simons, R. L., Chen, Y.-F., Stewart, E. A., & Brody, G. H. Justice. https://doi.org/10.1177/1541204012458440.
(2003). Incidents of discrimination and risk for delin- Walters, G. D. (2012). Developmental trajectories of delin-
quency: A longitudinal test of strain theory with an quent behavior one pattern or several? Criminal Justice
African American sample. Justice Quarterly, 20, and Behavior, 39(9), 1192–1203.
827–854. Warr, M. (2002). Companions in crime: The social aspects
Simons, R. L., Simons, L. G., & Wallace, L. E. (2004). of criminal conduct. UK: Cambridge University Press.
Families, delinquency, and crime: Linking society's White, N. A., & Piquero, A. R. (2004). A preliminary
most basic institution to antisocial behavior. Los empirical test of Silverthorn and Frick’s delayed-onset
Angeles: Roxbury Publishing. pathway in girls using an urban, African-American,
926 Delinquent Youth with Disabilities

US-based sample. Criminal Behaviour and Mental concern, a disproportionate number of these
Health, 14(4), 291–309. youth are minorities. With a burgeoning juvenile
Williams, D. R., & Mohammed, S. A. (2009). Discrimina-
tion and racial disparities in health: Evidence and court system, both in cost and number of youth
needed research. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, involved, today’s more punitive approach to
32(1), 20–47. working with these delinquent youth is shifting
Wolfgang, M. E., Figlio, R. M., & Sellin, T. (1972). Delin- toward a rehabilitative approach. This shift
quency in a birth cohort. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press. toward rehabilitation is the preferred method to
Woodward, L. J., Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. decrease the unfavorable impacts that these dis-
(2002). Romantic relationships of young people with abilities have on juvenile court outcomes, espe-
childhood and adolescent onset antisocial behavior cially for minority youth.
problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30,
231–244.
Yonai, S., Levine, S. Z., & Glicksohn, J. (2015). Trajecto-
ries of crime and familial characteristics: A longitudinal Youth (Juvenile) Delinquency
national population-based study. Crime & Delin-
quency. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011128712453674.
Young, J. T. (2014). “Role magnets”? An empirical inves- Youth (juvenile) delinquency, commonly referred
tigation of popularity trajectories for life-course persis- to as youth being in trouble with the law, is both a
tent individuals during adolescence. Journal of Youth growing area of concern and an ignored problem.
and Adolescence, 43(1), 104–115. The good news, though, is most youth are never
Zheng, Y., & Cleveland, H. H. (2013). Identifying gender-
specific developmental trajectories of nonviolent and involved with illegal activities nor arrested; and
violent delinquency from adolescence to young adult- for those youth who are arrested once, a majority
hood. Journal of Adolescence, 36(2), 371–381. of them are never arrested a second time. In other
words, most youth grow out of illegal, illicit, or
truant behaviors. However, not every youth (and
family) is so fortunate.
Delinquent Youth with Disabilities The number of youth involved with the United
States’ juvenile justice system has been steadily
Christopher A. Mallett growing over the past 30 years (around two mil-
School of Social Work, Cleveland State lion annually), though the number of youth actu-
University, Cleveland, OH, USA ally under juvenile court supervision has been
declining slightly over the past decade
(Sickmund 2009). This decrease may be a reflec-
Overview tion of the cost of involving so many youth in the
juvenile courts, or may reflect the more recent
Most youth in the United States do not have a movement away from harsh punishments, that
disability. In fact, a vast majority will not ever typically lead to longer juvenile court supervision,
suffer from a mental health disorder, substance toward more rehabilitative approaches. In fact, the
abuse problem, special education disability, nor history of the juvenile courts, first established in
be traumatized by maltreatment. However, the the late 1800s, is analogous to a pendulum that has
children and youth who do have one of these been shifting back and forth from what has been
disabilities or experiences are much more often called the “the tough on crime,” or punishment
found in the juvenile justice system – in other approach, to the “parens patriae” approach, or one
words, under juvenile court supervision – when in which the courts take on a benevolent and
compared to the general youth population. Evi- helpful “parent” role. In today’s juvenile courts,
dence to date has identified that a majority of one problem is clear – a majority of the youth who
youth who have been placed under juvenile do come under juvenile court supervision suffer
court supervision (adjudicated delinquent) have from at least one moderate to severe disability – a
at least one of these troubles, and oftentimes situation that is only recently being recognized
more than one. And in what is an interrelated and addressed.
Delinquent Youth with Disabilities 927

Juvenile delinquency itself is a legal term. It and histories of maltreatment – children who
means that the juvenile court judge has made the have been abused and/or neglected. While these
decision that the youth is in need of court super- maltreatment histories are frequently not catego-
vision or oversight. This necessitates a juvenile rized as a distinct disability, they are harmful to
court hearing and a finding by a judge that the many of the youth and also so common in the
youth (a majority of whom found delinquent are delinquent populations, that it is important to do
between the ages of 14 and 17) requires some so here.
level of additional supervision. This supervision
may be minimal or substantial, may last for a few D
months or a few years, and may include more Youth Disability Definitions/Systems
punitive approaches (for example, electronic
monitoring or placement into a detention center) To further understand what these disabilities
or more rehabilitative approaches (for example, include or entail, there are some common defini-
family counseling or psychological evaluations). tions used nationwide in working with at-risk
Youth take a number of different paths to being children and youth. First, in special education
adjudicated (meaning “court determined”) delin- (public schools) there are four federally recog-
quent. To better understand this, a review of one nized categories: severely behaviorally
state’s definition of “delinquency” is helpful for handicapped (SBH), learning disabled (LD),
most states use this or similar language. The State severely emotionally disturbed (SED), and devel-
of Ohio defines a delinquent youth as one: (a) who opmentally handicapped (DH). Further identified
violates any law of this state, the United States, or specific disabilities include mental retardation,
any ordinance or regulation of a political subdivi- hearing impairments, visual impairments
sion of the state, which would be a crime if com- (including blindness), orthopedic impairments,
mitted by an adult; and, (b) who violates any autism, traumatic brain injury, or other health
lawful order of the court. Normally this means impairments (Individuals with Disabilities Educa-
that a youth commits a crime, thus breaking a tion Act 2004). Second, the mental health and
state law. This crime may be against other people substance abuse disorders are identified through
(for example, assault or domestic violence), psychiatric diagnosis utilizing the American Psy-
against property (for example, burglary or vandal- chiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical
ism), or involve illicit drugs (for example, posses- Manual of Mental Disorders-IV (APA 2000). Dis-
sion or sale of marijuana). However, there are orders commonly found in delinquent populations
other circumstances that are not considered include behavior disorders (conduct, oppositional
crimes, or against the law, if committed by an defiant), affective disorders (depression, mood),
adult, but because the youth is a minor (under attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, substance
the age of 18) the actions/circumstances are illegal (alcohol and drug) disorders, and phobias. Third,
and called status offenses. Some of these status child and youth maltreatment is defined by state
offenses include truancy (not attending school), law, but each state categorizes these acts as phys-
running away from home, staying out past com- ical abuse, sexual abuse, and neglect. Neglect may
munity curfews, and underage possession or con- include medical care neglect, home care neglect,
sumption of alcohol. school neglect (not attending), emotional with-
Whichever path the youth takes to delinquent drawal by parent, and/or a combination of these
acts and eventual delinquency adjudication, one types.
thing is clear: these supervised youth have a much The laws, regulations, and professional stan-
greater chance of having a disability or past trau- dards that have established what these disabilities
matic experience when compared to non- are today have also produced separate service
delinquent youth populations. These disabilities delivery systems to address these distinct disabil-
include mental health disorders, substance use and ity and maltreatment concerns. These systems,
abuse problems, special education disabilities, run at the state and local (county) level, include
928 Delinquent Youth with Disabilities

the public school systems administering special population of juvenile court-involved youth is
education disability services, local mental health disproportionately represented by youth with
and substance abuse agencies, child welfare and disabilities – and in an interrelated problem, a
protective service agencies, and, as is the frame- disproportionate number of these youth are Afri-
work for this essay, the juvenile justice system. can American and Hispanic. One significant lim-
What is wholly problematic today is that a child or itation to note with these juvenile justice system
youth who is at risk and in need of disability prevalence rates is that a majority of the research
services can and does access these systems for studies are with youth being held in juvenile
very different reasons, many times leading to the detention centers and incarceration facilities. It is
youth (and family) being involved with more than necessary to expand this research to the full juve-
one system over time, or more than one system at nile court populations, a majority of whom are not
the same time, often without collaborative efforts held in these facilities. However, while acknowl-
between the systems. edging this limitation, the current disability rates
For example, a youth having suicidal thoughts are troubling. Youth involved with the juvenile
will most likely come to the attention of mental courts, oftentimes referred to as juvenile
health professionals, unless, however, that youth offenders, have a significantly larger number of
is masking these feelings with significant sub- disabilities and disorders when compared to the
stance use, in which case substance abuse pro- general youth population (Mallett 2009) (Table 1).
fessionals will probably become involved. If this
youth instead cannot control these harmful feel-
ings, he or she may release them through aggres- Mental Health Disorders
sive or assaultive behavior, oftentimes leading to
police and potential juvenile court involvement. These delinquent youth populations have high
If, by chance, this same youth has a parent or prevalence rates of mental health disorders,
guardian harming them through abuse, this may 40–70%, which is significantly higher than the
be reported to the county children’s services general youth population of between 9 and 16%.
agency, opening up an investigation and further These disorders range from mild to severe diag-
need for supervision or removal of the youth from nostic levels of impairment and include depression
the home. As can be seen in this example, these or dysthymia (a less severe form of depression)
are separate disability service delivery systems, (15–20%), anxiety disorders (24–29%), mood
but for the at-risk youth and families they may disorders (10–25%), attention-deficit hyperactiv-
be very interconnected. This connectivity is found ity disorder (13–30%), behavioral disorders
when examining the disability prevalence rates of (52–80%), and (more rarely) psychotic or bipolar
these youth within the juvenile justice system. disorders (3–7%).

Disability Prevalence Rates/Delinquency Delinquent Youth with Disabilities, Table 1 Offender


Links and general youth disabilities and maltreatment histories
Juvenile offender General youth
As noted earlier, the juvenile courts handle Disability type populations population
approximately two million cases annually, of Mental health 40–70% 9–16%
disorders
which the most common court outcome is delin-
Substance abuse 25–67% 5–6%
quency adjudication (for 1.6 million of these disorders
youth annually). For many supervised youth in Special 33–41% 4–10%
the courts, this means being placed on probation; education
in other words, having a probation officer respon- disabilities
sible for following up and making sure the youth Maltreatment 40–60% 1.4%
(and family) adhere to the judge’s orders. This histories
Delinquent Youth with Disabilities 929

But of particular concern within this group of Higher rates of substance abuse and disorders
juvenile court-involved youth is the estimated are found for youth in detention and incarceration
15–20% who have been diagnosed with a severe facilities, when compared to earlier juvenile jus-
mental health illness – major depression, bipolar tice system stages (for example, at referral or
disorder, and other mood disorders. While this juvenile court intake). Some studies have found
group of youth is small in number, they account race and gender differences: non-Hispanic Whites
for a large percentage of the resources utilized (Caucasians) higher rates, African American low-
within the youth mental health system. Mood est rates; and females more likely than males to
and affective disorders (including some depres- have a co-occurring mental health disorder. One D
sion) often are seen as anger or irritability which of the ongoing measurement concerns is that stan-
may lead to trouble with other juvenile offenders, dard criteria for substance abuse and disorders are
or the youth may turn these harsh feelings upon not consistently utilized across studies, limiting
themselves with self-harmful or suicidal behav- the broader discussion of the findings. Though
iors. Additionally, youth with anxiety disorders many juvenile courts use some screening, or stan-
tend to be more withdrawn, though not youth dardized assessments that include substance use
with posttraumatic stress disorder – many times or abuse measures, it is difficult for the court
a result of abuse or witnessing violence. Though personnel to identify the youth most in need of
common in the delinquent populations, conduct treatment (Chassin 2008). It is also impractical for
disorder is not merely an “acting out” problem, the juvenile courts to be expected to offer appro-
but includes much more significant and long- priate treatment for the number of youth showing
lasting problematic behavior (Grisso 2008; Teplin signs or significant signs of addiction. However,
et al. 2006). there is substantial knowledge on what programs
Today, there is a limited debate as to which or interventions may work to treat youth sub-
occurs first for these youth – mental health prob- stance abuse.
lems or involvement with the juvenile justice There is no one program or intervention found
system – though there is significantly more evi- to be the most effective in addressing youth sub-
dence of the link from earlier mental health prob- stance abuse. However, there are a number of
lems to later youth delinquency. Aggressive general principles that have been developed
behaviors before age 13, hyperactivity and atten- across programs and treatments and found to be
tion problems, and depression have been found effective. These include the use of more than one
predictive of delinquency, in particular physical treatment type or modality; having treatment be
aggression and stealing behaviors. It should be readily available during multiple treatment epi-
noted that delinquency is almost certainly the sodes; meeting multiple client needs including
result of the accumulation and interaction of mental health and physical health; continuous
youth experiences, called risk factors, which monitoring of the treatment provided, and possi-
include the disabilities discussed here, in addition ble drug use; adequate treatment duration; recog-
to deficits in the family (poverty, single parent- nition of co-occurring disorders; recognition of
ing), school, peers (friends who are delinquent), the long-term recovery process; use of counseling,
and neighborhood (poorer, higher crime areas) medications, and medical detoxification; and sup-
(Hawkins et al. 2000). port for involuntary treatment (National Institute
on Drug Abuse 2006).

Substance Abuse Disorders


Special Education Disabilities
These delinquent youth populations also have
high prevalence rates of substance abuse disor- These delinquent youth populations have high
ders, 25–67%, which is significantly higher than prevalence rates of special education disabilities,
the general youth population of around 5%. 33–41%, primarily learning disabilities, which is
930 Delinquent Youth with Disabilities

significantly higher than the general student pop- and neglect to have an elevated risk for delin-
ulation of between four and ten percent. Learning quency, but not victims of sexual abuse; while
disabilities are language difficulties that impair the others have found sexual abuse to be the explana-
youth’s thinking, speaking, writing, listening, tion. There is more agreement that children or
reading, spelling, or mathematical abilities. Chil- youth who suffer more severe abuse or neglect,
dren with low academic achievement and aca- and those with repeated exposure, do have a
demic failure in elementary school, and in greater risk of moderate to severe delinquency.
particular those with unidentified special educa- Contradictory evidence also exists on the impact
tion needs or disabilities, are at increased risk for that out-of-home placement due to this maltreat-
later violent behaviors. Specifically, youth with ment may have on youth. Though delinquency is a
unidentified special education disabilities may be result of the combination of risk factors (youth,
disproportionately represented in those youth who family, school, peers, and neighborhood), and
are suspended, expelled, and/or drop out of high these factors tend to be cumulative and interactive
school – youth outcomes that are all risk factors for making prediction difficult, being a victim of mal-
later delinquent activities (Mears and Aron 2003). treatment continues to be found even in the pres-
Research knowledge to date, though, is limited in ence of these other risk factors.
further explaining this link between these special
education disabilities and delinquent activities.
There exists some evidence that youth with these Comorbidity (Co-occurring) of
disabilities are at higher risk of re-offending, Disabilities
called recidivism; and other evidence that the
increased risk for these youth is minimal. Comorbidity is often the term used to discuss the
presence of more than one mental health disorder
or the combination of a mental health disorder and
Child Maltreatment a substance abuse disorder. It seems that this phe-
nomenon of multiple disabilities may be very
These delinquent populations also have high prev- common among the delinquent youth population,
alence rates of youth who have a history of mal- though research is very limited. When this area is
treatment (being abused or neglect), 40–60%, an studied, between 30% and 67% of juvenile court-
experience that is very uncommon, a little over involved youth have been identified with one of
1% of all children or youth. However, identifying the following comorbidities: two or three mental
which maltreated children and youth are at risk for health disorders; a mental health disorder and an
later delinquency is difficult because the connec- active substance abuse disorder; a mental health
tion is complicated and unclear. disorder with past maltreatment histories; or a
Research findings explaining this link between mental health disorder, a substance abuse disor-
maltreatment and delinquency have been contra- der, and a special education disability (Garland
dictory, resulting from methodological and et al. 2001; Mallett 2009; Teplin et al. 2006).
research design differences (Tuell 2002). There It is important in future research that juvenile
is some evidence that this group of youth have a court and delinquency outcomes be investigated
greater chance of being arrested as adolescents, to identify what impact these disabilities, or com-
though the pattern varies by gender, race, and binations of disabilities, have on referrals, proba-
maltreatment type (physical abuse, sexual abuse, tion, length of juvenile court supervision,
and neglect). There do seem to be some outcome recidivism (re-offending), secure facility place-
differences based on gender and the type of ment, and other important juvenile justice con-
maltreatment – for example, girls who have been cerns. One of these outcomes, being placed into
physically abused are more at risk to be physically a secure detention or incarceration facility, is a top
violent – but further research is needed. Some concern for the juvenile justice system for three
studies have identified victims of physical abuse reasons: (1) a disproportionate number of
Delinquent Youth with Disabilities 931

minority youth are locked, and on average held disability identification and coordination efforts
longer, in these facilities with little explanation with other youth-caring systems – mental health,
as to the reasons or causes – African American substance abuse, special education, and child
youth are six times more likely than whites; welfare – leads to cost-effective interventions
Hispanic youth three times more likely and delinquency prevention.
(Puzzanchera and Adams 2008); (2) the high These coordination efforts across the different
costs of holding youth in these facilities; and, youth and family systems, though, are very chal-
(3) the negative and harmful outcomes for the lenging, for each system has its own policies,
youth held in these facilities. regulations, and priorities. It is increasingly appar- D
On an average day, 54,500 youth are incarcer- ent that these youth and families have problems
ated in this country’s 591 detention centers and that cross these systems and involve the youth in
2,964 correctional facilities (Sickmund et al. more than one of the systems. For the past two
2008), and, as discussed, many of these youth decades, the lack of coordination among these
have at least one significant disability. Incarcerat- youth-caring systems has been identified as a
ing these youth is ineffective public policy that major barrier to more effective and positive out-
compromises public safety because most of the comes. Often titled “systems of care,” these
youth do not improve after their release from these efforts, undertaken at the state and local levels,
facilities. They are less likely to find employment try to share the resources (programmatic and
and less likely to complete high school. In fact, the funding) across the systems and reallocate these
experience of detention makes it more likely that resources to more efficiently work with the multi-
detained youths will continue to engage in delin- system involved youth and families (Pires 2002).
quent behavior, and may increase the odds that These coordinated efforts are effective when pol-
youth will recidivate (re-offend) – costing the icy makers actively work together and share
courts and communities additional resources – resources; they fail when preservation of one sys-
and in no way improving the youth’s chances for tem over another is the priority. However, these
success, or at least a smoother transition into coordination efforts across the juvenile courts,
young adulthood. mental health and substance abuse systems, child
welfare agencies, and public schools are very
important because of the number of youth are
Next Steps increasingly identified as multiply-disabled and
because their outcomes are not encouraging. One
The juvenile courts are today moving away from system alone cannot be expected to handle the
the “tough on crime” approach that has been com- multiple-problem situations that many of these
monly used over for the last few decades, an delinquent youth present.
approach focused on increased punishments, While this topic of delinquent youth with
increased incarcerations, and less tolerance for disabilities is garnering increased attention,
youthful offenders and re-offenders. Because there remain a number of important knowledge
this approach has not significantly decreased the gaps that should be addressed in conjunction
number of youth in the juvenile justice system, with ongoing systems coordination efforts.
has not decreased youth recidivism, nor shown Future research should include national and
very many positive outcomes, juvenile courts regional studies of these cross-system youth
have been moving toward a rehabilitative and families to identify prevalence, trends, and
approach. This movement is fully supported by coordination effort outcomes; juvenile court
the fact that a large number of these youthful outcomes for youth with these differing disabil-
offenders suffer from at least one significant dis- ities; improved predictive pathways from these
ability, disabilities that may be interrelated to their disabilities to juvenile court involvement; gen-
delinquency behaviors. It is increasingly recog- der and race disability differences and court out-
nized by the juvenile justice system that early comes; and additional proven interventions to
932 Delirium

use at various and, it is hoped, earlier juvenile Administration. Available at http://gucchd.george


justice system contact points to help prevent the town.edu/files/products_publications/TACenter/PRIM
ER_ALL.pdf.
youth from continuing to engage in delinquent Puzzanchera, C., & Adams, B. (2008). National dispro-
behaviors and activities. portionate minority contact databook. Developed by
the National Center for Juvenile Justice for the Office of
Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention.
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice. Avail-
Cross-References able at http://ojjdp.ncjrs.gov/ojstatbb/dmcdb/.
Sickmund, M. (2009). Delinquency cases in juvenile court:
▶ Disability 2005. Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Pre-
vention. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
Available at http://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/ojjdp/
224538.pdf.
References Sickmund, M., Sladky, T. J., & King, W. (2008). Census of
juveniles in residential placement databook. Office of
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). The diagnostic Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention.
and statistical manual of mental disorders-IV – Text Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice. Avail-
revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Asso- able at http://ojjdp.ncjrs.org/ojstatbb/Cjrp/.
ciation Press. Teplin, L., Abram, K., McClelland, G., Mericle, A.,
Chassin, L. (2008). Juvenile justice and substance abuse. The Dulcan, M., & Washburn, D. (2006). Psychiatric dis-
Future of Children, 18(2), 165–184. Available at http:// orders of youth in detention. Washington, DC: Office of
www.futureofchildren.org/futureofchildren/publications Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, Juvenile
/journals/journal_details/index.xml?journalid=31. Justice Bulletin. Available at http://www.ncjrs.gov/
Garland, A., Hough, R. L., McCabe, K. M., Yeh, M., pdffiles1/ojjdp/210331.pdf.
Wood, P. A., & Aarons, G. A. (2001). Prevalence of Tuell, J. (2002). Child maltreatment and juvenile delin-
psychiatric disorders in youths across five sectors of quency: Raising the level of awareness. Washington,
care. Journal of the American Academy of Child and DC: Child Welfare League of America. Available at
Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(4), 409–418. http://www.ncjrs.gov/App/Publications/abstract.aspx?
Grisso, T. (2008). Adolescent offenders with mental disor- ID=199834.
ders. The Future of Children, 18(2), 143–164. Available at
http://www.futureofchildren.org/futureofchildren/publica
tions/journals/journal_details/index.xml?journalid=31.
Hawkins, J., Herrenkohl, T., Farrington, D., Brewer, D.,
Catalano, R. F., Harachi, T., & Cothern, L. (2000).
Predictors of youth violence. Office of Juvenile Justice Delirium
and Delinquency Prevention. Available at http://www.
ncjrs.org/html.ojjdp/jjbul2000_04_5/contents/html.
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act. (2004). Roger J. R. Levesque
20 U.S.C. § 1400 et seq., revised and amended by Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
Pub L. No. 108–466, Stat. 34. Available at http://
www.ed.gov/legislation/FedRegister/finrule/2006-3/08
1406a.pdf.
Mallett, C. (2009). Disparate juvenile court outcomes for Delirium is a cognitive disorder that involves an
disabled delinquent youth: A social work call to action. acute and debilitating decline that results in an
Child & Adolescent Social Work Journal, 26(3), altered form of semiconsciousness due to an indi-
197–208.
Mears, D., & Aron, L. (2003). Addressing the needs of
vidual’s inability to pay attention, perceive, speak,
youth with disabilities in the juvenile justice system: and think appropriately. It is a relatively global
The current state of knowledge. Washington, DC: The impairment that may also involve impairment in
Urban Institute. Available at http://www.urban.org/pub sleep–wakefulness cycles and abnormal psycho-
lications/410885.html.
National Institute on Drug Abuse. (2006). Principles of motor activity. Delirium may be either hyperactive
drug treatment: A research based guide. Bethesda: (marked by agitation, aggression, or combative-
National Institutes of Health, U.S. Dept. of Health and ness) or hypoactive (marked by quiet confusion
Human Services. Available at http://www.nida.nih.gov/ or inability to focus or follow commands) (see
podat/PODAT1.html.
Pires, S. (2002). Building systems of care: A primer. Karnik et al. 2007). Delirium is a set of symptoms
Washington, DC: Human Services Collaboration, resulting from an underlying disease or impair-
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services ment; it is a symptom of a malfunctioning brain.
Delusion 933

The understanding of delirium derives mainly need for more prospective studies that would
from studies involving adults. Studies that do examine the phenomenology, risk factors, and
examine the nature of delirium occurring during treatment of delirium during childhood and
childhood and adolescence reveal that they have adolescence.
similar qualities to those of adults. For example, in
their important study, Turkel and Tavaré (2003)
found that adolescents’ symptoms included disor- References
ganized behavior, sleep disturbance, disturbed
attention, altered state of consciousness, as well Karnik, N. S., Joshi, S. V., Paterno, C., & Shaw, R. (2007). D
Subtypes of pediatric delirium: A treatment algorithm.
as some hallucinations. Equally importantly, the
Psychosomatics, 48, 253–257.
more delirious patients had a higher mortality rate, Schieveld, J. N. M., Leroy, P. L. J. M., Van Os, J., Nicolai,
as has been found with adults. They also found J., Vos, G. D., & Leentjens, A. F. G. (2007). Pediatric
that antipsychotics were sufficient in treating their delirium in critical illness: Phenomenology, clinical
correlates and treatment response in 40 cases in the
pediatric patients with delirium. In a later study,
pediatric intensive care unit. Intensive Care Medicine,
Turkel et al. (2006) reaffirmed that many of the 33, 1033–1040.
same symptoms were reported regardless of age Turkel, S. B., & Tavaré, C. J. (2003). Delirium in children
but that they were often reported at significantly and adolescents. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clin-
ical Neuroscience, 15, 431–435.
different rates, with cases involving children
Turkel, S. B., Trzepacz, P. T., & Tavaré, C. J. (2006).
being more likely to report sleep–wake distur- Comparing symptoms of delirium in adults and chil-
bance, fluctuating symptoms, impaired attention, dren. Psychosomatics, 47, 320–324.
irritability, agitation, and confusion and adults’
cases more likely to involve impaired memory,
depressed mood, speech disturbance, delusions,
and paranoia. The authors, however, were unable Delusion
to determine whether these were true differences
attributable to the life cycle or to inconsistent Roger J. R. Levesque
methodologies. Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
Despite potential controversies regarding the
nature of delirium in youth and adults, there is
no doubt that delirium has been found less fre- Delusions are false beliefs held even when
quently in younger patients. Why that is the case contradicted by reality. As misjudgments of real-
is not entirely clear. In their analysis of this ity, delusions are idiosyncratic and individual
issue, Schieveld et al. (2007) provide reasons misinterpretations or misperceptions. A classic
to believe that the increase in delirium rates example would be delusions of grandeur, which
following an increase in age could be the result are fantastical beliefs that one is famous, omnip-
of its misdiagnosis. They reject the contention otent, or otherwise powerful. Studies indicate
that there actually could be a discrepancy in the that these delusions are the most common
prevalence between aged populations. They in adolescent psychiatric patients (Tillman
attribute the apparent differences to such possi- et al. 2008).
bilities as the psychosocial interventions pro- Delusions can be thought disorders with
vided in pediatric intensive care units, the side enough import to interfere with normal function-
effect of analgosedative drugs that may reduce ing if individuals no longer share enough consen-
delirium, doctor’s discomfort with stigmatizing sually related reality with people around them.
youth with the label and, lastly and according to Importantly, some delusions are normal, as many
the authors most likely, is that doctors and individuals have mild subclinical experiences
nurses do not recognize pediatric delirium. of delusion. Community surveys, as opposed
Their studies confirm that delirium in critically to clinical ones, have long revealed that many
ill children is not negligible and highlight the otherwise well individuals report delusional-like
934 Dependency Court Processes

experiences (Scott et al. 2009), and systematic


reviews of the literature reveal that approximately Dependency Court Processes
75–90% of developmental psychotic experiences
are transitory and disappear over time (van Os Roger J. R. Levesque
et al. 2009). It is clear that some delusions may Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
be part of mental health problems that adolescents
face, but not be severe enough to be part of full-
blown psychotic disorders. For example, one At the center of child welfare systems are depen-
important study found that many adolescents dency courts, also referred to as juvenile or family
(24%) experienced delusions in a sample not courts, that rule on cases of child maltreatment as
meeting requirement for a psychotic disorder authorized by state law. While many states grant
(Altman et al. 1997). These adolescents had been dependency courts jurisdiction over children ages
recruited at a day treatment center and a residen- birth until 18 and sometimes up to 21, all states
tial treatment facility. The authors concluded that provide these courts with considerable power to
subclinical delusions and subclinical hallucina- intervene in families to help care for children
tions were related to different psychiatric ail- deemed at risk. These courts thus are able to rule
ments. Subclinical delusions were more related on cases of abuse and neglect, children’s place-
to schizotypal thought processes, while subclini- ment, and the provision of services to families and
cal hallucinations were most closely related to the children as well as on whether parents’ ties with
dissociative process. Findings like these are of their children should be permanently severed (for
significance in that, even though some delusions thorough reviews, see Levesque 2002, 2008). The
may be normal, for some individuals the episodes court system often focuses on ensuring that ser-
may be the beginning of long-term impairments. vices are provided to youth and, when needed, to
The transitory developmental expression of psy- their families. But, the reality is that a relatively
chosis may become abnormally persistent and small percentage of child welfare cases require
subsequently clinically relevant depending on an court oversight and supervision. This is the case
individual’s exposure to environmental risk (van because families receive voluntary services from a
Os et al. 2009). variety of sources, and, as a result, the large major-
ity of cases served by child welfare agencies are
never adjudicated by the courts. Yet, the courts
References still serve important functions in that they not only
provide general parameters for cases that do avoid
Altman, H., Collins, M., & Mundy, P. (1997). Subclinical litigation but also seek to offer important protec-
hallucinations and delusions in nonpsychotic adoles-
tions for the youth and families that do have cases
cents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
38(4), 413–420. processed through the courts. The wide variety of
Scott, J., Martin, G., Welham, J., Bor, W., Naman, J., cases involving child welfare challenges render it
O’Callaghan, M., et al. (2009). Psychopathology dur- difficult to provide an analysis of all the possible
ing childhood and adolescence predicts delusion-like
scenarios that may be involved in cases that go
experiences in adults: A 21-year birth cohort study. The
American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 567–574. through dependency courts. As a result, this essay
Tillman, R., Geller, B., Klages, T., Corrigan, M., Bolhofner, briefly overviews dependency court processes to
K., & Zimerman, B. (2008). Psychotic phenomena in provide a sense of the nature of this court system
257 young children and adolescents with bipolar
I disorder: Delusions and hallucinations (benign and
devoted to child protection and, in a real sense, to
pathological). Bipolar Disorders, 10, 45–55. youth’s mental health and overall healthy
van Os, J., Linscott, R. J., Myin-Germeys, I., Delespaul, P., development.
& Krabbendam, L. (2009). A systematic review and State statutes inform pre-court involvement in
meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: Evidence
family cases. They define abuse and neglect and
for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment
model of psychotic disorder. Psychological Medicine, they tend to do so in their reporting laws.
39, 179–195. Reporting laws are those that specify when
Dependency Court Processes 935

professionals or others must report suspected Placement should be least restrictive. Again, how-
abuse. Laws also require the adoption of proce- ever, many cases avoid entering the court system
dures for reporting to child protection services given efforts to front-load services in a manner
(CPS) and/or law enforcement, along with immu- that provides voluntary services prior to reaching
nity for those who report, and penalties for failing the point requiring a formal court action.
to report suspicions of abuse or neglect. After Adjudicatory hearings are scheduled expedi-
receiving a report, CPS or law enforcement must tiously (such as within 30 days) after the emer-
expeditiously determine whether the allegations gency hearing. Adjudication hearings are bench
are the type that need not be of concern, should trials before judges (or masters) to determine D
lead to an investigation, or might involve the whether the child is indeed dependent and thus
removal of children from their families. If the qualifies to be under the court’s supervision. This
children are removed, CPS files a petition seeking phase involves the presentation of evidence
continued removal or family reunification. Typi- supporting or refuting allegations of abuse or
cally, however, if children are removed, parents neglect. The typical standard of proof is “clear
engage in voluntary services coordinated by child and convincing” evidence that the child has been
welfare systems. (or will be) abused or neglected. And the rules
If children are removed by child welfare agen- regulating the admittance of evidence, unlike for
cies, courts hold an emergency hearing, usually criminal courts, are considerably lenient. If the
within 24–72 h, to determine the necessity of child is found dependent, then a dispositional
more permanent removal. Parties are notified of hearing is scheduled, often immediately following
the need to appear, and the local child welfare the adjudication or within 40–60 days. Pending
agency attempts to identify the children’s biolog- this hearing, parties may be asked to submit to
ical mother and father. Around the time of the evaluations of parental fitness or needs of a child.
emergency hearing, the child welfare agency The extra time ensures the child welfare agency’s
files the petition alleging abuse or neglect. Each development of a case plan for the court to review.
party is entitled to representation, as states provide Adjudications and disposition hearings are
(with varying degrees of effectiveness) guardians separate processes and serve two different pur-
or attorney ad litem for the child and counsel to poses, one determines dependency and one deter-
the parents. Emergency hearings allow all parities mines what to do about that dependency. Yet,
to present oral arguments regarding the alleged jurisdictions can hold these hearings consecu-
abuse or neglect. To remove the child, the depen- tively for the purposes of timeliness and conve-
dency court must find that the child’s home is nience. Consecutive hearings occur since many, if
contrary to the child’s welfare and must be not all, of the parties are the same at both hearings,
detailed regarding the reasoning supporting the the evidence presented to the court can be similar,
removal. At these hearings, courts must decide consecutive hearings expedite the process, and
whether the state made reasonable efforts to pre- some cases have overlapping outcomes. The
vent the child’s removal, including the provision push toward greater timeliness also comes from
of services to facilitate the child’s continuing to federal laws that have encouraged more expedited
live at home, or family reunification. Services dispositional hearings, mainly on the grounds that
may include visitation by a qualified social waiting too long leaves children in limbo. The
worker, skills training, or support services. If the move toward more expedient hearings and dispo-
case is especially severe, the court may decide that sitions is counterbalanced by the reality that dis-
no services need to be provided and the child positional hearings allow courts to review plans
welfare agency rapidly develops alternative, per- created by the child welfare agency and determine
manent living arrangements. The emergency hear- whether child welfare services were provided
ing also includes a decision on parent–child properly to address the determined needs. Plans
visitation, needs for psychiatric or psychological may contain one of many options, including fam-
evaluations, and where the child is to be placed. ily reunification, termination of parental rights,
936 Dependency Court Processes

assigning legal guardianship, permanent place- be represented during the proceedings. Once paren-
ment with an appropriate family member, or tal rights are terminated, the state assumes the
placement in another setting. Families with con- child’s legal custody and frees the child for adop-
siderable needs likely require considerable time to tions. Adoption, a separate legal proceeding, allows
meet appropriate levels of care. The need for time courts to evaluate home environments and evidence
is highlighted by the manner that, historically, pertaining to prospective adoptive parents’ ability
agencies considerably focused on family to provide appropriate care for the child. Some
reunification and protecting parents’ rights to states allow older adolescents to consent to their
raise their children (see Levesque 1995). That adoptions. Open adoptions are sometimes consid-
approach, however, formally has been abandoned ered if the adoptive parents agree to specified
in favor of quicker permanent dispositions, as parent–child contact. In some cases of children
encouraged by the Adoption and Safe Families with disabilities, adoptive families may receive
Act (1997). The Act, for example, requires state financial and medical assistance after the adoption,
agencies to file a petition to terminate parental known as subsidized adoption. In addition to adop-
rights and, at the same time, identify, recruit, tion and termination of parental rights, court super-
process, and approve a qualified adoptive family vision can be terminated on several other grounds,
on behalf of any child, regardless of age, that has some of which can occur at any point in a case, such
been in foster care for 15 out of the most recent as when the child reaches majority, an able parent
22 months. Thus, the system has grown to recog- comes forward to care for the child, permanency
nize the need to move as expeditiously as possible plans are completed, or the child enters prison,
as it balances the rights of parents with those of military service, or dies. Still, a court action must
children deemed maltreated. The dual mandate for occur for the case to be terminated.
family integration while planning for children’s Dependency courts essentially rule on cases
removal has been highly criticized. Yet, it still involving child maltreatment. They intend to pro-
remains and reflects the challenges facing depen- vide speedy mediation and intervention for chil-
dency courts charged with ensuring that child dren and their families. These courts necessarily
welfare systems meet the needs of children and make difficult choices as they seek to protect the
their families. And it is that dual mandate, coupled rights of parents and those of their children and
with the practical need to respond efficiently to recognize that even though children have been
children’s developmental needs, that now pushes harmed or are at risk of harm, it may not neces-
courts to adjudicate and reach dispositions as sarily follow that it is in their best interests to be
quickly as fairness permits. without their parents, a recognition that leads to
If a child is in foster care or other out-of-home the increasingly complex web of services that can
care, a court must review their case periodically. be provided to youth and families based on a large
During a permanency hearing, the court ensures continuum of both need and available services.
that progress is being made toward the implemen-
tation of the permanency plans relevant parties
decided on in the dispositional hearing. For older
Cross-References
youth, courts also must ensure that they are able to
transition to adulthood through the agency’s offer-
▶ Adoption
ing of housing, work skills training, and mental
health counseling. In the case of adoption, the
courts must first terminate parental rights and References
transfer the rights to another party. This includes
all ties, the right to visit, and any decisions regard- Adoption and Safe Families Act. (1997). Pub. L. No.
105–89, 111 Stat. 2115.
ing care. Proceedings that may lead to the sever- Levesque, R. J. R. (1995). The failures of foster care reform:
ing of parental rights place a heavy burden of Revolutionizing the most radical blueprint. Maryland
proof on the state and parents have the right to Journal of Contemporary Legal Issues, 6, 1–35.
Dependency in Law 937

Levesque, R. J. R. (2002). Child maltreatment and the law: nonrelative placements, unless the children have
Foundations in science, policy and practice. Durham: special needs preventing them from being with
Carolina Academic Press.
Levesque, R. J. R. (2008). Rethinking child maltreatment their relatives. Precisely when others can interfere
law: Returning to first principles. New York: Springer. in parent–child relationships has shifted consider-
ably over the past several decades, and that shift
has allowed considerably more intervention
(Levesque 1994).
Dependency in Law A look at the history of dependency in law
reveals that the privileges of caretakers over their D
Roger J. R. Levesque dependents have become more limited in the past
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA several decades. An obvious example involves
more restrictions toward the use of corporal pun-
ishment; where previously a father could legally
One of the most important concepts relating to the expect to compel obedience from his wife and
rights of youth is the concept of dependency and children with threats and actual use of violence,
how it infiltrates the legal system (see Fineman now he would face abuse charges. Even with these
2004). When relating to youth, dependency has modern attitudes toward egalitarianism, however,
two important meanings. One meaning deals with children still tend to be considered a class below
developmental matters, such as a child’s inherent adults. In many ways, children still are viewed as
need to rely on others due to biological, emo- property, although that view continues to change
tional, cultural, and mental immaturity. Another and evolve (see Woodhouse 1992).
meaning refers to dependency in the way that One of the most powerful legal mechanisms
rules, laws, and norms create and assert depen- for fostering and protecting the dependency of
dency in the way that they foster or limit rights youth is a vision of family life that views the
and responsibilities, such as dependency on a decision making and control within families as
family member or the state. Notably, the legal matters of privacy. In the United States, for exam-
definition of dependence has integrated the con- ple, expectations of family privacy, coupled by the
cept of gender equality and minority rights, as expectation that parents will provide for youth,
indicated by efforts to remove adult women from foster the notion that youth are dependent on
patriarchal control and efforts to provide more their families. As a result, social services for chil-
rights to individuals with disabilities so that they dren are intended to supplement rather than sup-
can act and live more independently. Both of these plant the family, and such agencies are only able
views of dependency have important implications to take an active protective role for a child if they
for youth. can prove that the parents have insufficiently pro-
In the United States, youth are assumed to be vided for their child. This concept is based in the
dependent in all senses of the term. Children are belief that a family should be economically and
considered to be incapable of acting in their own personally capable of caring for its children; as a
best interests due to physical, emotional, and men- result, families that require social services or
tal immaturity and cultural construction of the role “welfare” are stigmatized for being unable to pro-
of youth in America. Biological parents are vide. In contrast to the United States’ focus on the
assumed best able to provide for their child due “individualism” of families, other countries take a
to their greater experience and maturity, and are more active role in supporting families and adopt
accordingly granted pervasive authority over their a much more open view of families. Notably,
children that would not be granted in any other several Western countries have incorporated the
relationship. When the biological parents are responsibilities of United Nations or international
found to be incapable of minding their children, principles of children’s rights into their laws.
relevant social service agencies will seek to place These principles view children as individuals
the child with a relative before attempting with rights within their families. This approach
938 Depressive Symptoms

to children’s rights requires the government to


take greater responsibility for a child’s primary Depressive Symptoms
needs, such as their education, even over the
objections of their parents. Brandyn M. Street and Judy Garber
Although much is made of differences between Department of Psychology and Human
different systems’ ways to approach dependency, it Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville,
is important to highlight similarities and how dif- TN, USA
ferences can lead to important differences in the
way that adolescents are free to exercise indepen-
dent rights and even recognize that they do have Feelings of sadness and despondency in the face
rights in the first instance. The notion of depen- of a disappointment or loss are both natural and
dence in US law, for example, does serve important expected. All adolescents occasionally experience
purposes and the distinction between private fam- distress and sadness. When such dysphoria lingers
ilies and the state is not as clear as some commen- for weeks, months, or longer and limit a person’s
taries might suggest (see Levesque 1994). The ability to function, then the person may be suffer-
US approach still fosters the rights of youth ing from major depressive disorder (MDD),
(as highlighted by the right to education), for exam- which is the most common mental health disorder
ple, and it does provide for limiting the govern- in the United States (Kessler et al. 2005). MDD
ment’s intrusion into their lives. Despite those can have adverse consequences on an adolescent’s
realities, the nature of those rights are framed dif- academic performance, social life, and family
ferently and the notion of dependency tends to give relationships (Bean et al. 2006; Shahar et al.
considerably more power to the rights of parents to 2006) and is associated with other mental health
control their children as they deem fit. Perhaps for problems, suicide, and hospitalizations (Jacobson
the vast majority of circumstances the rights of and Gould 2009; Ohayon and Schatzberg 2010;
individuals within families may not clash with Wolff and Ollendick 2006).
those of the entire family or of parents within Given the severity and persistence of depres-
those families; it is when there are potential clashes sion, health providers need to learn to identify
that the default of dependency can make an impor- mood disorders and to provide evidence-based
tant difference, as in instances involving how youth treatments. This essay reviews the epidemiology,
are treated by mental health, education, medical, phenomenology, diagnosis, course, and preven-
child welfare, and juvenile justice systems. Some tion of depression in adolescents. This essay
of those systems were designed to address parental begins with a discussion of the nature, core fea-
failures, including the need to prevent failures, tures, and phenomenology of depression. Next, it
which can result in challenging circumstances reviews issues concerning the detection of MDD
when states need to protect the rights of parents in community settings and provides an overview
as well as those of their children. of current empirically supported depression pre-
vention programs. It ends with suggestions for a
research agenda concerning depression in
adolescents.
References

Fineman, M. A. (2004). The autonomy myth: A theory of


dependency. New York: Free Press.
Epidemiology
Levesque, R. J. R. (1994). The internationalization of
children’s human rights: Too radical for American ado- Depression is a common disorder in both commu-
lescents? Connecticut Journal of International Law, 9, nity (Lewinsohn et al. 1998) and clinical
237–293.
populations (Birmaher et al. 2002). Rates of
Woodhouse, B. B. (1992). Who owns the child?: Meyer
and Pierce and the child as property. William and Mary depressive disorders increase during early to mid-
Law Review, 33, 995–1122. dle adolescence, particularly in females (Costello
Depressive Symptoms 939

et al. 2003; Hankin et al. 1998), with the peak age severely depressed adolescents, approximately
of onset occurring at about 13–15 years old. 25% develop bipolar disorder within 5 years
Whereas the rates of depression in prepubescent (Birmaher et al. 1996).
children are about equal in boys and girls (Nolen- Longitudinal studies have found that the con-
Hoeksema and Girgus 1994), by about age 14 the tinuity of depression from adolescence to adult-
2:1 female to male ratio begins to emerge (Angold hood (e.g., Lewinsohn et al. 1999; Rao et al. 1999)
et al. 2002; Costello et al. 2003). is stronger than for prepubertal-onset to adoles-
Depressed adolescent females are more likely cent or adult depression (Weissman et al. 1999b).
to experience appetite or weight disturbances, Moreover, individuals who were depressed as D
worthlessness or guilt, and suicidal ideation adolescents have two to seven times increased
(Alloy and Abramson 2007; Bearman and Stice odds of having an episode in adulthood as com-
2008; Bennett et al. 2005) and to have more recur- pared to individuals who did not have depression
rences than males (Lewinsohn and Essau 2002). during adolescence (Rutter et al. 2006). Evidence
Explanations for the increased rate of depression of age differences in heritability (Lau and Eley
in adolescent females have included interactions 2008) and in neurobiological correlates (Kaufman
among biological, psychological, and sociologi- et al. 2001) may account for the different course
cal factors that emerge over time (Cyranowski of child- as compared to adolescent-onset
et al. 2000; Hyde et al. 2008; Nolen-Hoeksema depression.
and Girgus 1994).
Lifetime prevalence rates of MDD in adoles-
cents range from 1.9% to 18.4% (Lewinsohn and Onset and Course
Essau 2002; Lewinsohn et al. 2000; Rao et al.
1999). In one of the most comprehensive epide- The first onset of depression often occurs between
miological surveys in the United States, the 15 and 19-year olds (Haarasilta et al. 2001;
National Comorbidity Study, the lifetime preva- Lewinsohn et al. 1998). Earlier age of onset is
lence rate of MDD in adolescents was reported to associated with lifetime medical problems, sui-
be 14%, and subsyndromal or minor depression cidal ideation, anxiety disorders, and a familial
was found in 10–20% of youth (Kessler and history of depression (Klein et al. 2009). Although
Walters 2002) An even higher percentage of ado- adolescence often is a time of increasing self-
lescents (20–50%) report significant levels of awareness, young people with MDD and their
symptoms on questionnaires (Kessler et al. parents may not recognize or acknowledge the
2001). Depressive symptoms show an increasing possible seriousness of persistent sadness and
trajectory during adolescence (Costello et al. the need for evaluation and treatment. Addition-
2006), are associated with significant impairment ally problematic is the perceived stigma many
(Chabrol 2007; Shahar et al. 2006) and predict the adolescents associate with mental health care
onset of subsequent clinical disorders (Costello (Jaycox et al. 2006). Jaycox and colleagues
et al. 2003; Sung et al. 2004). found that nearly half of adolescents who
Major depressive episodes (MDEs) during screened positive for depression reported that
adolescence last about 7–9 months in clinical their relationships with friends would suffer
samples (Birmaher et al. 2002), about 26 weeks more if their friends knew that they had a recent
in community samples (Lewinsohn et al. 1994), history of depression or had visited a mental
and about 24 weeks in high-risk offspring health professional than if they had asthma or
(Kaminski and Garber 2002). Although most HIV/AIDS.
MDEs remit within 2 years, depression during The course of MDD in adolescents is charac-
adolescence is highly recurrent, with as many as terized by frequent recurrences (Dunn and
40% of youth experiencing another MDE within Goodyer 2006; Weissman et al. 1999a). Emslie
2–5 years after recovery (Birmaher et al. 1996; et al. (1997) reported that 61% of their sample of
Rao et al. 1999). Among psychotic or otherwise depressed youth experienced an additional
940 Depressive Symptoms

depressive episode within 2 years. Rao et al. et al. 2002) because of multiple family, child, and
(1999) reported that 47% of their sample experi- health care system factors. Parents often do not
enced a recurrence of another episode of MDD recognize affective problems in their children
within 5 years, with the first year following recov- (Cantwell et al. 1997) and may mistake a child
ery being the time of highest risk. Variables asso- with depressive symptoms as just “going through
ciated with recurrence include parental a stage.” In addition, youth may not communicate
psychopathology, family conflict, and negative mental health concerns to their primary care pro-
cognitive style (Birmaher et al. 2002). vider, particularly when practitioners have limited
Untreated depression has been found to be a time and experience with evaluating and treating
risk factor for conduct problems, substance use, depressive disorders (Jaycox et al. 2006).
interpersonal conflict, and educational under-
achievement, (Birmaher et al. 2004; Clark et al.
2007; Rohde et al. 2004; Shahar et al. 2006). In Clinical Presentation
addition, adolescents experiencing severe depres-
sion are at increased risk for suicidal ideation and Depression in adolescents is characterized by a
behavior (Gould et al. 2003). According to the pervasive and persistent unhappy or irritable
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention mood and/or loss of interest or pleasure in usual
(2004), suicide is the third most common cause activities (i.e., anhedonia). According to the Diag-
of death among adolescents and depression is the nostic and Statistical Manual, fourth edition in
second highest predictor of completed suicide in revision (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric
adolescent males (Shaffer and Craft 1999). Association 2000), the same criteria are used to
Adolescent depression also has been linked diagnose depression across development, except
with subsequent medical problems including that in children and adolescents irritability can be
migraine headaches, allergies, asthma, and one of the mood symptoms, and only 1-year dura-
smoking progression (Audrain-McGovern et al. tion is required for dysthymia. To qualify for a
2009; Egger et al. 1998; Waxmonsky et al. diagnosis of MDD, five of the following symp-
2006). The association between adolescent toms are required, at least one of which must be
depression and medical problems may be due to either a mood disturbance or a anhedonia:
common biological, behavioral, cognitive, and (1) depressed and/or irritable (in children and
social factors. For example, depression may adolescents) mood for most of the day, (2) loss
undermine the immune system, thereby increas- of interest or pleasure in most activities,
ing susceptibility to disease (Kiecolt-Glaser and (3) decreased or increased appetite and/or signif-
Glaser 2002). Symptoms and behaviors that char- icant weight gain or weight loss, (4) insomnia
acterize depression and that can exacerbate risk and/or hypersomnia, (5) psychomotor agitation
for medical problems include sleep problems, lack and/or retardation, (6) fatigue, loss of energy,
of exercise, and poor appetite and nutrition (7) feelings of worthlessness and/or inappropriate
(Cohen and Rodriguez 1995). Further, depression guilt, (8) difficulty concentrating and/or making
is associated with maladaptive social functioning, decisions, and (9) recurrent thoughts of death
which likely disrupts the social connectedness and/or suicidal ideation (American Psychiatric
that can be beneficial to physical health (Cohen Association 2000). These symptoms should rep-
2004). Thus, adolescent depression may lay the resent a change from previous functioning, be
foundation for the development of physical disor- impairing, and persist for at least 2 weeks. The
ders and functional impairment. average duration of an episode of MDD in youth
Depressive symptoms among adolescents also is between 3 and 9 months; 50–90% can be
are quite common and subclinical levels of symp- expected to recover by 12–72 months from onset
toms significantly predict the onset of a full MDE (Birmaher et al. 2002).
(Rutter et al. 2006). However, 70% of depressed Dysthymic disorder (DD) is a chronic form of
youth go without appropriate treatment (Kataoka depression with fewer symptoms but lasting for at
Depressive Symptoms 941

least 1 year in children and adolescents. Depres- (9%) (Birmaher et al. 1996; Simonoff et al.
sion not otherwise specified (DNOS) is defined by 1997); indeed, anxiety disorders may serve as a
depressed mood, anhedonia, or irritability risk factor for depression (Zalsman et al. 2006).
(in children and adolescents) and up to three Comorbidity rates for dysthymia (30–80%), con-
symptoms of major depression. Adjustment dis- duct problems, and attention-deficit hyperactive
orders with depressed mood are characterized by disorder (ADHD) (10–80%), and substance use
fewer than five depressive symptoms that occur disorder (20–30%) also are quite high.
within 3 months of the onset of a specific stressful Corresponding comorbidity rates for anxiety dis-
situation, and last no longer than 6 months. orders, disruptive behavior disorders, substance D
Some debate exists in the literature regarding use disorder, and ADHD (18%, 8%, 14%, and
whether the experience and expression of depres- 21%, respectively) are lower in community com-
sive symptoms change with development. In a pared to clinical samples of adolescents (Jensen
meta-analysis of studies comparing the occur- et al. 1993; Lewinsohn et al. 1991), although still
rence of depressive symptoms across age groups, quite high. A meta-analysis of studies of commu-
Weiss and Garber (2003) found age differences in nity samples of children and adolescents revealed
many core (e.g., agitation, retardation, fatigue, that the odds ratios for comorbid disorders with
guilt, and sadness) and associated symptoms MDD were 8.2 for anxiety disorders, 6.6 for con-
(e.g., anxiety, somatic complaints). In particular, duct/oppositional defiant disorders, and 5.5 for
depressed adolescents were more likely than pre- ADHD (Angold et al. 1999).
adolescents to experience hopelessness/helpless- Adolescent depression often co-occurs with
ness, lack of energy, hypersomnia, weight loss, externalizing and substance use disorders, partic-
and suicidality (Yorbik et al. 2004), although not ularly in males (Lewinsohn et al. 1993; Yorbik
all of these age differences have been found in et al. 2004), and eating disorders, particularly in
studies using longitudinal analyses. Evidence also females (Lewinsohn et al. 1993). Depression typ-
is mixed regarding whether the underlying factor ically develops after the other comorbid disorder
structure of the depressive syndrome is similar (Lewinsohn et al. 1998), with two exceptions.
across ages (Weiss and Garber 2003). That is, do MDD typically precedes the onset of substance
the same symptoms comprise MDD in children use disorders (Birmaher et al. 1996). Second,
versus adolescents? comorbid conduct disorder often represents a
complication of depression with the conduct prob-
lems persisting after the depression remits
Comorbidity (Birmaher et al. 1996). 61, 93–95.
Impairment in cognitive and social functioning
In both clinic and community samples of adoles- in individuals with MDD is intensified by comor-
cents (Angold et al. 1999; Essau et al. 2000; bid conditions (Biederman et al. 2008; Fergusson
Lewinsohn et al. 1998), depression is associated and Woodward 2002; Rudolph and Clark 2001).
with significant comorbidity. Adolescents who For example, depressed adolescents with ADHD
have depression are approximately 20 times have been found to be at increased risk for longer
more likely to meet diagnosis for an additional episode duration, a higher rate of suicidiality, and
disorder (Angold and Costello 1993). In the Ore- a greater likelihood of requiring psychiatric hos-
gon Adolescent Depression Project (OADP) pitalization (Biederman et al. 2008). Depression
(Lewinsohn et al. 1998), 43% of the adolescents comorbid with other disorders tends to be more
with MDD also had a lifetime occurrence of severe and recurrent, and is less responsive to
another mental disorder. treatment (Birmaher et al. 1996). Depressed ado-
Among clinical samples of adolescents, the lescents with comorbid externalizing disorders
most common comorbidity is anxiety disorders, have more interpersonal impairment (Rudolph
particularly generalized anxiety disorders (55%), and Clark 2001), experience more stressful life
phobias (45%), and separation anxiety disorder events (Daley et al. 1997; Rudolph et al. 2000),
942 Depressive Symptoms

and show lower rates of depression and higher Adolescents (ISCA; Kovacs 1997), the Diagnos-
rates of criminality/antisocial personality disorder tic Interview Schedule for Children Version IV
and alcohol abuse in adulthood (Harrington et al. (DISC IV; Shaffer et al. 2000), the Diagnostic
1991) compared to those without such Interview for Children and Adolescents-Revised
comorbidity. (DICA-R; Reich 1991), and the Child and Ado-
lescent Psychiatric Assessment (CAPA; Angold
and Fisher 1999). Like the K-SADS, these struc-
Assessment tured interviews require training and are routinely
used in clinical research settings.
Age-appropriate assessment of depressive symp-
toms and disorders is a key part of the treatment Self-Report Questionnaires
process. Obtaining information from multiple The Center for Epidemiological Studies Depres-
informants and using a variety of assessment sion Scale (CES-D; Radloff 1991) is a 20-item
methods including clinical interviews, question- self-report measure of depressive symptoms
naires, and behavioral observation provide a com- developed for use with adults, but has been
prehensive evaluation needed to make accurate found to be reliable and valid with adolescents
diagnoses and treatment plans. Even when infor- also (Roberts et al. 1990). The CES-D assesses
mation from multiple sources is available, getting the degree to which individuals have had the core
report directly from the adolescent is important symptoms of depression including negative
(Ferdinand et al. 2004). Parents often are unaware affect, thoughts, and behaviors during the past
of their child’s inner experience and therefore may week. Representative statements include “I felt
be less accurate reporters about their child’s sub- sad” and “I did not feel like eating; my appetite
jective distress (Ferdinand et al. 2004). was poor.” Respondents rate each statement using
a four-point Likert Scale from 0 (rarely or never)
Clinical Interviews to 3 (most of the time). Items are summed to create
Psychiatric interviews typically are conducted a total score; total scores of 16–23 indicate mild
with the parent and adolescent separately depression, 24–30, moderate depression, and
and sequentially. Such detailed inquiry can help scores over 30 reflect more severe levels of
establish rapport with the adolescent and family. depression (Roberts and Chen 1995). Other self-
Observations of adolescents’ nonverbal commu- report measures of depressive symptoms for ado-
nications such as affective expressions, psycho- lescents include the Children’s Depression Inven-
motor restlessness or agitation, and listless speech tory (Kovacs 1985), the Beck Depression
also can be helpful in making an accurate diagno- Inventory (Beck et al. 1961), the Reynolds Ado-
sis (Garber and Kaminski 2000). lescent Depression Scale (Reynolds 1987), the
Several psychometrically adequate diagnostic Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (Angold et al.
interviews exist to assess depressive disorders in 1987), the Depression Self-Rating Scale (Birleson
adolescents. The Schedule for Affective Disorders 1981), and the Dimensions of Depression Profile
and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children for Children and Adolescents (Harter and
(K-SADS; Ambrosini 2000; Kaufman et al. Nowakowski 1987).
1997) is one of the most widely used and compre- Depression self-report measures can be used as
hensive clinician-administered interviews for quick screening tests that patients can complete
diagnosing depression in youth (Hodges 1994). while in the waiting room at intake and also can be
Disadvantages of the K-SADS are that it can be used to monitor changes in symptoms over the
time-consuming and resource demanding; that is, course of treatment (Spitzer et al. 1994). Recog-
it requires extensive training and supervision nition and treatment of depression are especially
(Reynolds and Kamphaus 2003). important in primary care settings because for
Other available structured interviews include most adolescents, their primary care provider
the Interview Schedule for Children and may be the only health professional seen within
Depressive Symptoms 943

the course of a year (Gans et al. 1991). One study Qualitative (e.g., Gillham et al. 2000; Merry and
conducted in a primary care setting found that Spence 2007; Sutton 2007) and quantitative (e.g.,
20% of youth met criteria for a depressive disor- Brunwasser et al. 2009; Horowitz and Garber
der (Kramer and Garralda 1998). Thus, primary 2006; Merry et al. 2004a) reviews of studies testing
care settings may be the first place that depres- interventions to prevent depression in children and
sion in an adolescent is recognized. Unfortu- adolescents have concluded that: (a) some targeted
nately, youth seen in primary care for (i.e., selective, indicated) depression prevention
depression are less likely to receive follow-up programs are efficacious; (b) targeted preventions
care compared to children presenting with have effect sizes greater than those found for uni- D
behavioral and/or attentional problems (Wren versal programs; (c) the effect sizes for targeted
et al. 2005), and they typically do not receive prevention programs generally have been small to
treatment until their problems have begun to moderate; and (d) the effects tend not to endure.
severely impact functioning (i.e., suicidal behav- Most studies have measured change in depressive
ior) (Essau 2005). symptoms; far fewer studies have prevented the
onset of depressive disorders.
Central to the prevention of depression is
Prevention of Depression in Adolescents a recognition that dysfunctional thoughts and
interpersonal relationships characterize depres-
Given that only a small proportion of adolescents sion. Both Cognitive Behavioral Therapy
with depressive disorders receive treatment (CBT)-based preventive interventions, which
(Kataoka et al. 2002), prevention programs have combine cognitive restructuring techniques and
a unique opportunity to reach children before the behavioral activation, and Interpersonal Therapy
age of greatest risk, before their symptoms for Depressed Adolescents (IPT-A)-based
increase in severity, and/or before risk factors approaches, which emphasize building and
culminate in disorder onset. Historically, preven- strengthening interpersonal relationships, have
tion was categorized as primary, secondary, or been found to be efficacious. The two approaches
tertiary (Caplan 1964). Reducing the incidence share a number of components (problem solving,
of new cases of disorder in individuals who have time-limited, attention to increasing engagement
not had the disorder is primary prevention; reduc- in pleasurable activities), which may explain why
ing the duration and severity of symptoms is sec- both produce beneficial adolescent outcomes
ondary prevention (i.e., treatment); and reducing (Clarke et al. 2001; Young et al. 2006).
the recurrence of the disorder and its associated The most extensively tested preventive inter-
impairment in those who have already had it is vention with youth is the CBT-based Penn Pre-
tertiary prevention (maintenance). The Institute of vention Program (PPP) (see Brunwasser et al.
Medicine (IOM) found this distinction to be too 2009 for a review), which has been administered
broad, and instead introduced the classification of to universal, selective, and indicated samples. PPP
prevention based on the population groups to has shown both short- and long-term significant
whom the interventions are directed (Mrazek and effects, and has been replicated several times in
Haggerty 1994). Universal prevention is adminis- different samples (e.g., Cardemil et al. 2007;
tered to all members of a population and does not Quayle et al. 2001; Yu and Seligman 2002).
target particular individuals based on risk. Selec- Most studies of PPP have assessed depressive
tive prevention is given to subgroups of a popula- symptoms, although a few have evaluated depres-
tion whose risk is deemed to be above average sive disorders (Gillham et al. 2006b). Samples
(e.g., offspring of depressed parents). Indicated sizes have been generally adequate, and evidence
prevention is provided to individuals who have of satisfactory fidelity has been demonstrated.
subthreshold levels of signs or symptoms of the A recent meta-analysis (Brunwasser et al. 2009)
disorder, but who do not currently meet criteria for indicated that youth participating in PPP experi-
the diagnosis. enced fewer depressive symptoms at post-
944 Depressive Symptoms

intervention, and at 6-, 8-, and 12-month follow- that focused on the prevention of depression
ups compared to youth receiving no intervention. among offspring of depressed parents. The inter-
The strongest effects were found among studies vention was compared with a written information
with selective and indicated approaches im- self-study control condition. Youth in the family
plemented by research team members. group intervention reported significantly fewer
Another replicated depression prevention pro- internalizing and externalizing symptoms, partic-
gram in youth is based on the Coping with Depres- ularly at 12-month follow-up. In addition, parents
sion course developed by Clarke et al. (1990). The in the family intervention reported significantly
program focuses on monitoring negative moods, fewer symptoms of depression at 2-, 6-, and
changing maladaptive thought patterns, and prob- 12-month follow-up. These findings support the
lem solving. Short-term efficacy was found when a involvement of parents in prevention work with
modified version of the program was tested in a adolescents.
universal sample (Horowitz et al. 2007), and both Overall, results of prevention studies with ado-
short- and longer-term efficacy have been found in lescents indicate benefits of CBT-based interven-
selective and indicated samples (Clarke et al. 2001; tions and interpersonal-based approaches. The
Garber et al. 2009); adolescents in the CBT pre- results also underscore the value of conducting
ventive intervention experienced significantly follow-up assessments and the limitations of uni-
fewer episodes of depression compared to adoles- versal approaches. In light of recent evidence
cents in the usual care condition. In addition, favoring family group intervention, further study
Garber and colleagues showed that the intervention of the inclusion of parents and/or family compo-
effects were larger for adolescents whose parents nents in preventative interventions for youth
were not depressed at baseline. is warranted. Modification and evaluation of
Other noteworthy depression prevention pro- depression prevention programs with children
grams have been developed and evaluated in and adolescents from diverse ethnic and racial
Australia and New Zealand. The Resourceful Ado- backgrounds also is needed (see Cardemil
lescent Program (RAP) (Merry et al. 2004b; et al. 2007).
Shochet et al. 2001) has been tested in large uni-
versal samples, was found to be efficacious both at
post-intervention and follow-up, was replicated by Depression and Real-World Settings
independent researchers, and was found to have
satisfactory adherence. The Problem Solving for Fox et al. (2008) recently called for school-based
Life program (PSFL) (Spence et al. 2003) focuses mental health checkups (screenings) for internal-
on cognitive restructuring and problem solving, is izing disorders. Borrowing from Shaffer et al.’s
administered by teachers, and has been tested in (2004) Columbia Suicide Screen, which assesses
large universal school samples. PSFL was found to adolescents’ risk of suicide, Fox and colleagues
have significant short-term effects (Spence et al. proposed a stepped care approach to assessment,
2003), although this was not replicated (Sheffield prevention, and treatment that includes the fol-
et al. 2006), and it has not shown significant long- lowing: (1) entire classes or groups of adolescents
term effects (Spence et al. 2005). completing a self-report battery assessing inter-
Evidence of short-term efficacy of the Interper- nalizing symptoms and risk factors (e.g., negative
sonal Psychotherapy-Adolescent Skills Training self-cognitions, stressful life events), (2) mental
program (IPT-AST) in both a large universal health professionals scoring the measures and
(Horowitz et al. 2007) and a small indicated submitting written summaries of the results, and
(Young et al. 2006) sample has been found. Young (3) mental health professionals contacting parents
and colleagues also showed that the significant of adolescents who score above established
effects endured through the 6-month follow-up. cutoffs to schedule follow-up assessments, rec-
Recently, Compas et al. (2009) tested a ommend prevention/early intervention, or refer
12-session family cognitive-behavioral group for treatment.
Depressive Symptoms 945

Barriers to implementation of such school- depression, anxiety, and adjustment disorders


based mental health checkups include, but are (when combined) among high-symptom children.
not limited to, cost, consistency with other school Stronger effects were found when the intervention
directives, and the need to make the benefits clear was delivered with high compared to low fidelity,
to policymakers, educators, and parents. To and girls benefited more from PPP than boys.
address these issues, studies should examine the Asarnow et al. (2005) also conducted a study
clinical impact relative to the financial expenses of for youth depression in primary care. Primary care
such mental health checkups in terms of reducing clinics were randomized to treatment as usual
symptoms of depression and the prevention of (TAU) or a quality improvement program (QIP). D
internalizing problems. The QIP involved training depression care man-
Some adolescents and their parents may feel agers in CBT, patient and clinician choice regard-
more comfortable discussing mental health con- ing treatment modality, and educating
cerns with their primary care provider as opposed participating clinicians in depression evaluation,
to being assessed at school. Given that 70% of management, and pharmacological and psychoso-
youth visit primary care providers within the cial treatments. Asarnow and colleagues demon-
course of a year, primary care settings may be strated that depression and functioning were
an ideal setting for early identification and inter- significantly better for youth in the QIP condition
vention with adolescents experiencing emotional compared to those receiving TAU.
distress (Monheit and Cunningham 1992). Unfor- One concern about providing intervention in
tunately, due to features of managed care, the time- managed care settings is the issue of safety. When
limited nature of pediatric visits, and the low rates teens are at risk of harming themselves or others,
of specialty referral from primary care settings, the the option for hospitalization needs to be available
detection rates and outcomes for adolescents with to provide treatment and ensure safety. Some
mental health needs in primary care have tended to communities, however, lack inpatient beds for
be poor (Chang et al. 1988; Lavigne et al. 1993; the crisis care of youth (Thomas 1999). This
Burns 1996; Wells et al. 1989). unmet need might be addressed by extending
To address these assessment issues, Zuckerbrot health coverage to the uninsured and establishing
et al. (2007) evaluated the feasibility and accept- parity laws that mandate equivalent coverage for
ability of a two-stage adolescent depression iden- mental and physical health conditions (Wells et al.
tification method at three sites of a pediatric 2001). Many low-income youth have decreased
primary care practice. First, adolescents com- access to mental health services because they are
pleted the Columbia Depression Scale (CDS) uninsured, and for those who are insured, costs for
while in the waiting room. Providers then mental health services commonly exceed cover-
reviewed the screens and could request that the age limits (Sturm 1997).
patient take the youth-informant Columbia voice Primary care settings hold considerable prom-
DISC-IV, a computer assisted self-interview ver- ise for addressing unmet needs for treatment of
sion of the present state version of the Columbia adolescent depression. Unfortunately, there is a
Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children, serious dearth of information about how to
which generates a diagnostic report for providers. implement and maintain depression screening
Patients expressed satisfaction with the procedure as part of routine clinical practice. Additional
and providers expressed a desire to continue to use research on the financial implications and clini-
the CDS (Zuckerbrot et al. 2007). cal benefits of such screening is needed
In a study testing the effectiveness of PPP (Zuckerbrot et al. 2007). In addition, longitudi-
delivered by therapists in a primary care setting, nal studies within primary care settings should
Gillham et al. (2006a) randomized 271 children develop and test the effectiveness of intervention
(11–12 years old) with elevated depressive symp- strategies for youth with depression and address
toms on the CDI to either PPP or usual care. Over issues of sustainability and dissemination (Wells
the 2-year follow-up, PPP significant prevented et al. 2001).
946 Depressive Symptoms

Summary and Future Directions developmental context. In D. Romer & E. F. Walker


(Eds.), Adolescent psychopathology and the developing
brain: Integrating brain and prevention science
Depression among adolescents is common, recur- (pp. 284–312). New York: Oxford University Press.
rent, highly comorbid with other disorders, and Ambrosini, P. J. (2000). Historical development and pre-
associated with significant impairment. Most sent status of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and
depressed youth do not receive any or appropriate Schizophrenia for School-Age Children (K-SADS).
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Ado-
treatment (Kataoka et al. 2002). Multiple lescent Psychiatry, 39, 49–58.
approaches to assessment of depression include American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and
clinical interviews, behavioral observation, and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).
questionnaires completed by adolescents, parents, Washington, DC: American Psychiatric.
Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (1993). Depressive comor-
and teachers. bidity in children and adolescents: Empirical, theoreti-
Prevention of depression is possible, particu- cal, and methodological issues. American Journal of
larly among at-risk youth. Evidence of short-term Psychiatry, 150, 1779–1791.
effects of some preventive interventions has been Angold, A., & Fisher, P. W. (1999). Interviewer-based
interviews. In D. Shaffer, C. Lucas, & J. E. Richters
found, particularly cognitive and/or interpersonal (Eds.), Diagnostic assessment in child and adolescent
approaches with high-risk populations psychopathology (pp. 34–64). New York: Guilford.
(Brunwasser et al. 2009; Horowitz and Garber Angold, A., Weissman, M. M., John, K., Merikangas,
2006). Further research is needed to develop and K. R., Prusoff, B. A., Wickramaratne, P., et al. (1987).
Parent and child reports of depressive symptoms in
evaluate depression prevention programs that children at low and high risk of depression. Journal of
have enduring effects as well. Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disci-
There has been growing interest in early inter- plines, 28, 901–915.
vention and prevention of depression in both Angold, A., Costello, E. J., & Erkanli, A. (1999). Comor-
bidity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40,
school and primary care settings (Fox et al. 57–87.
2008; Walker et al. 2002; Zuckerbrot et al. Angold, A., Erkanli, A., Silberg, J., Eaves, L., & Costello,
2007). Several barriers to practical action exist E. J. (2002). Depression scale scores in 8-17-year-olds:
including cost, absence of on-site therapists effects of age and gender. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 43, 1052–1063.
trained in evidence-based interventions, short Asarnow, J. R., Jaycox, L. H., Duan, N., LaBorde, A. P.,
duration of pediatric visits, and limited insurance Rea, M. M., Murray, P., et al. (2005). Effectiveness of a
coverage (Burns 1996; Sturm 1997). Neverthe- quality improvement intervention for adolescent
less, these settings represent logical contexts to depression in primary care clinics. Journal of the Amer-
ican Medical Association, 293, 311–319.
reach the largest number of adolescents, and Audrain-McGovern, J., Rodriguez, D., & Kassel, J. D.
thereby reduce the burden of depression. (2009). Adolescent smoking and depression: Evidence
for self-medication and peer smoking mediation.
Addiction, 104, 1743–1756.
Cross-References Bean, R. A., Barber, B. K., & Crane, D. R. (2006). Parental
support, behavioral control, and psychological control
among African American youth. Journal of Family
▶ Externalizing and Internalizing Symptoms Issues, 27, 1335–1355.
Bearman, S. K., & Stice, E. (2008). Testing a gender
Acknowledgment Street was supported in part from an
additive model: The role of body image in adolescent
NIMH-funded training grant (T32MH18921); Garber was
depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36,
supported in part from an NIMH Independent Scientist
1251–1263.
Award (K02 MH66249) and grants from the National
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., &
Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH57822) and the
Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depres-
William T. Grant Foundation (173096).
sion. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.
Bennett, D. S., Ambrosini, P. J., Kudes, D., Metz, C., &
Rabinovich, H. (2005). Gender differences in adoles-
References cent depression: do symptoms differ for boys and girls.
Journal of Affective Disorders, 89, 35–44.
Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (2007). The adolescent Biederman, J., Ball, S. W., Monuteaux, M. C., Mick, E.,
surge in depression and emergence of gender differ- Spencer, T. J., McCreary, M., et al. (2008). New
ences: A biocognitive vulnerability-stress model in insights into the co-morbidity between ADHD and
Depressive Symptoms 947

major depression in adolescent and young adult Clarke, G. N., Hornbrook, M., Lynch, F., Polen, M., Gale,
females. Journal of the American Academy of Child J., Beardslee, W., et al. (2001). A randomized trial of a
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 426–434. group cognitive intervention for preventing depression
Birleson, P. (1981). The validity of depressive disorder in in adolescent offspring of depressed parents. Archives
childhood and the development of a self-rating scale: of General Psychiatry, 58, 1127–1134.
A research report. Journal of Child Psychology and Cohen, S. (2004). Social relationships and health. Ameri-
Psychiatry, 22, 73–88. can Psychologist, 59, 676–684.
Birmaher, B., Ryan, N. D., Williamson, D. E., Brent, D. A., Cohen, S., & Rodriguez, M. S. (1995). Pathways linking
Kaufman, J., Dahl, R. E., et al. (1996). Childhood and affective disturbances and physical disorders. Health
adolescent depression: A review of the past 10 years. Psychology, 7, 269–297.
Part I. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Compas, B. E., Forehand, R., Keller, G., Champion, J. E., D
Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 1427–1439. Rakow, A., Reeslund, K. L., et al. (2009). Randomized
Birmaher, B., Arbelaez, C., & Brent, D. (2002). Course and controlled trial of a family cognitive-behavioral pre-
outcome of child and adolescent major depressive dis- ventive intervention for children of depressed parents.
order. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77,
North America, 11, 619–638. 1007–1020.
Birmaher, B., Bridge, J. A., Williamson, D. E., Brent, Costello, E. J., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., &
D. A., Dahl, R. E., & Axelson, D. A. (2004). Psycho- Angold, A. (2003). Prevalence and development of
social functioning in youths at high risk to develop psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence.
major depressive disorder. Journal of the American Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 837–844.
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, Costello, E. J., Foley, D. L., & Angold, A. (2006). 10-year
839–846. research update review: The epidemiology of child and
Brunwasser, S. M., Gillham, J. E., & Kim, E. S. (2009). adolescent psychiatric disorders: II. Developmental
A meta-analytic review of the penn resiliency pro- epidemiology. Journal of the American Academy of
gram’s effect on depression. Journal of Consulting Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 8–25.
and Clinical Psychology, 6, 1042–1054. Cyranowski, J. M., Frank, E., Young, E., & Shear, K.
Burns, B. (1996). What drives outcomes for emotional and (2000). Adolescent onset of the gender difference in
behavioral disorders in children and adolescents? New lifetime rates of depression. Archives of General
Directions for Mental Health Services, 71, 89–102. Psychiatry, 57, 21–27.
Cantwell, D. P., Lewinsohn, P. M., Rohde, P., & Seeley, Daley, S. E., Hammen, C., Burge, D., Davilla, J., Paley, B.,
J. R. (1997). Correspondence between adolescent Lindberg, N., et al. (1997). Predictors of the generation
report and parent report of psychiatric diagnostic data. of episodic stress: A longitudinal study of late adoles-
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Ado- cent women. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106,
lescent Psychiatry, 36, 610–619. 251–259.
Caplan, G. (1964). Principles of preventive psychiatry. Dunn, V., & Goodyer, I. (2006). Longitudinal investigation
New York: Basic Books. into childhood-and adolescence-onset depression: Psy-
Cardemil, E. V., Reivich, K. J., Beevers, C. G., Seligman, chiatric outcome in early adulthood. British Journal of
M. E. P., & James, J. (2007). The prevention of depres- Psychiatry, 188, 216–222.
sive symptoms in low-income, minority children: Two- Egger, H. L., Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (1998). Head-
year follow-up. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, aches and psychopathology in children and adoles-
313–327. cents. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2004). Sui- Adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 951–958.
cide in the United States fact sheet. Atlanta: Centers for Emslie, G. J., Rush, J., Weinberg, W. A., Kowatch, R. A.,
Disease Control and Prevention. Hughes, C. W., Carmody, T., et al. (1997). A double-
Chabrol, H. (2007). Relations between suicidal ideation blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial of fluoxe-
and dimensions of depressive symptoms in high-school tine in children and adolescents with depression.
students. Journal of Adolescence, 30, 587–600. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 1031–1037.
Chang, G., Warner, V., & Weissman, M. M. (1988). Phy- Essau, C. (2005). Frequency and patterns of mental health
sicians’ recognition of psychiatric disorders in children services utilization among adolescents with anxiety and
and adolescents. American Journal of Diseases of Chil- depressive disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 22(3),
dren, 142, 736–739. 130–137.
Clark, C., Haines, M. M., Head, J., Klineberg, E., Arephin, Essau, C. A., Conradt, J., & Petermann, F. (2000). Fre-
M., Viner, R., et al. (2007). Psychological symptoms quency, comorbidity, and psychosocial impairment of
and physical health and health behaviours in adoles- depressive disorders in adolescents. Journal of Adoles-
cents: A prospective 2-year study in East London. cent Research, 15, 470–481.
Addiction, 102, 126–135. Ferdinand, R. F., van der Ende, J., & Verhulst, F. C. (2004).
Clarke, G. N., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Hops, H. (1990). Parent-adolescent disagreement regarding psychopa-
Instructor’s manual for the adolescent coping with thology in adolescents from the general population as
depression course. Portland: Kaiser Permanente a risk factor for adverse outcome. Journal of Abnormal
Center. Psychology, 113, 198–206.
948 Depressive Symptoms

Fergusson, D. M., & Woodward, L. J. (2002). Mental depression in children and adolescents (pp. 209–234).
health, educational, and social role outcomes of ado- New York: Plenum.
lescents with depression. Archives of General Psychi- Horowitz, J. L., & Garber, J. (2006). The prevention of
atry, 59, 225–231. depressive symptoms in children and adolescents:
Fox, J. K., Halpern, L. F., & Forsyth, J. P. (2008). Mental A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and
health checkups for children and adolescents: A means Clinical Psychology, 74, 401–415.
to identify, prevent, and minimize suffering associated Horowitz, J. L., Garber, J., Ciesla, J. A., Young, J. F., &
with anxiety and mood disorders. Clinical Psychology: Mufson, L. (2007). Prevention of depressive symptoms
Science and Practice, 15, 182–211. in adolescents: A randomized trial of cognitive-
Gans, J. E., McManus, M. A., & Newacheck, P. W. (1991). behavioral and interpersonal prevention programs.
Adolescent health care: use, costs, and problems of Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75,
access. Chicago: American Medical Association. 693–706.
Garber, J., & Kaminski, K. M. (2000). Laboratory and Hyde, J. S., Mezulis, A. H., & Abramson, L. Y. (2008). The
performance-based measures of depression in children A-B-Cs of depression: Integrating affective, biological,
and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, and cognitive models to explain the emergence of the
29, 509–525. gender difference in depression. Psychological Review,
Garber, J., Clarke, G., Weersing, V. R., Beardslee, W. R., 175, 291–313.
Brent, D., Gladstone, T., et al. (2009). Prevention of Jacobson, C. M., & Gould, M. (2009). Suicide and non-
depression in at-risk adolescents: A randomized con- suicidal self-injurious behaviors among youth: Risk
trolled trial. Journal of the American Medical Associa- and protective factors. In S. Nolen-Hoeksema &
tion, 301, 2215–2224. L. M. Hilt (Eds.), Handbook of depression in adoles-
Gillham, J. E., Shatte, A. J., & Freres, D. R. (2000). cents (pp. 207–235). New York: Routledge/Taylor &
Preventing depression: A review of cognitive- Francis Group.
behavioral and family interventions. Applied and Pre- Jaycox, L. H., Asarnow, J. R., Sherbourne, C. D., Rea,
ventive Psychology, 9, 63–88. M. M., LaBorde, A. P., & Wells, K. B. (2006). Adoles-
Gillham, J. E., Reivich, K. J., Freres, D. R., Lascher, M., cent primary care patients’ preferences for depression
Litzinger, S., Shatté, A. J., et al. (2006a). School-based treatment. Administration and Policy in Mental Health
prevention of depression and anxiety symptoms in early and Mental Health Services Research, 33, 198–207.
adolescence: A pilot of a parent intervention component. Jensen, P. A., Shervette, R. E., Xenakis, S. N., & Richters, J.
School Psychology Quarterly, 21, 323–348. (1993). Anxiety and depressive disorders in attention
Gillham, J. E., Hamilton, J., Freres, D. R., Patton, K., & deficit disorder with hyperactivity: New findings.
Gallop, R. (2006b). Preventing depression among early American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1203–1209.
adolescents in the primary care setting: A randomized Kaminski, K. M., & Garber, J. (2002). Depressive spec-
controlled study of the Penn Resiliency Program. Jour- trum disorders in high-risk adolescents; episode dura-
nal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 203–219. tion and predictors of time to recovery. Journal of the
Gould, M. S., Greenberg, T., Velting, D. M., & Shaffer, American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
D. (2003). Youth suicide risk and preventative inter- 41, 410–418.
ventions: A review of the past 10 years. Journal of the Kataoka, S. H., Zhang, L., & Wells, K. B. (2002). Unmet
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, need for mental health care among U. S. children:
42, 386–405. Variation by ethnicity and insurance status. American
Haarasilta, L., Marttunen, M., Kaprio, J., & Aro, H. (2001). Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 1548–1555.
The 12-month prevalence and characteristics of major Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C.,
depressive episode in a representative sample of ado- Moreci, P., et al. (1997). Schedule for Affective Disor-
lescents and young adults. Psychological Medicine, 31, ders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-
1169–1179. Present and Lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial
Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L. Y., Moffit, T. E., Silva, P. A., reliability and validity data. Journal of the American
McGee, R., & Angell, K. E. (1998). Development of Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36,
depression from preadoelscence to young adulthood: 980–988.
Emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal Kaufman, J., Martin, A., King, R. A., & Charney,
study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 128–140. D. (2001). Are child, adolescent, and adult onset
Harrington, R., Fudge, H., Rutter, M., Pickles, A., & Hill, depression one in the same disorder? Biological Psy-
J. (1991). Adult outcomes of childhood and adolescent chiatry, 49, 980–1001.
depression: I. Psychiatric status. Archives of General Kessler, R. C., & Walters, E. E. (2002). The national
Psychiatry, 47, 465–473. comorbidity survey. In M. T. Tsaung & M. Tohen
Harter, S., & Nowakowski, M. (1987). Manual for the (Eds.), Textbook in psychiatric epidemiology
dimensions of depression profile for children and ado- (pp. 343–362). New York: Wiley.
lescents. Denver: University of Denver. Kessler, R. C., Avenevoli, S., & Merikangas, K. R. (2001).
Hodges, K. (1994). Evaluation of depression in children Mood disorders in children and adolescents: An epide-
and adolescents using diagnostic clinical interviews. In miologic perspective. Biological Psychiatry, 49,
H. F. Johnston & W. M. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of 1002–1014.
Depressive Symptoms 949

Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., & Walters, Lewinsohn, P. M., Rohde, P., Seeley, J. R., Klein, D. N., &
E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset Gotlib, I. H. (2000). Natural course of adolescent major
distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National depressive disorder in a community sample: Predictors
Comorbidity Survey replication. Archives of General of recurrence in young adults. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 62, 593–602. Psychiatry, 157, 1584–1591.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R. (2002). Depression and Merry, S. N., & Spence, S. H. (2007). Attempting to
immune function: Central pathways to morbidity and prevent depression in youth: A systematic review of
mortality. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53, the evidence. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 1,
873–876. 128–137.
Klein, D. N., Shankman, S. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Merry, S., McDowell, H., Hetrick, S., Bir, J., & Muller,
Seeley, J. R. (2009). Subthreshold depressive disorder N. (2004a). Psychological andor educational interven- D
in adolescents: Predictors of escalation to full- tions for the prevention of depression in children and
syndrome depressive disorders. Journal of the Ameri- adolescents. Cochrane Database of Systematic
can Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, Reviews, 2, CD003380.
703–710. Merry, S. N., McDowell, H., Wild, C. J., Bir, J., & Cunliffe, R.
Kovacs, M. (1985). The children’s depression inventory. (2004b). A randomized placebo controlled trial of a
Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 21, 995–998. schoolbased depression prevention program. Journal
Kovacs, M. (1997). The Interview Schedule for Children of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
and Adolescents (ISCA): Current and Lifetime (ISCA-C Psychiatry, 43, 538–547.
& L) and Current and Interim (ISCA-C & I) versions. Monheit, A. C., & Cunningham, P. J. (1992). Children
Pittsburgh: Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic. without health insurance. The Future of Children, 3,
Kramer, T., & Garralda, M. E. (1998). Psychiatric disorders 154–170.
in adolescents in primary care. British Journal of Psy- Mrazek, P. J., & Haggerty, R. J. (1994). Reducing risks for
chiatry, 173, 508–513. mental disorders: Frontiersfor preventive intervention
Lau, J. Y. F., & Eley, T. C. (2008). Disentangling gene- research. Washington, DC: National Academy.
environment correlations and interactions in adolescent Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Girgus, J. S. (1994). The emer-
depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia- gence of gender differences in depression during ado-
try, 49, 142–150. lescence. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 424–443.
Lavigne, J. V., Binns, H. J., Christoffel, K. K., Rosenbaum, Ohayon, M., & Schatzberg, A. F. (2010). Social phobia and
D., Arend, R., Smith, K., et al. (1993). Behavior and depression: prevalence and comorbidity. Journal of
emotional problems among preschool children in pedi- Psychosomatic Research, 68, 235–243.
atric primary care: Prevalence and pediatric recogni- Quayle, D., Dzuirawiec, S., Roberts, C., Kane, R., &
tion. Pediatrics, 91, 649–655. Ebsworthy, G. (2001). The effect of an optimism and
Lewinsohn, P. M., & Essau, C. A. (2002). Depression in life skills program on depressive symptoms in preado-
adolescents. In I. H. Gotlib & C. L. Hammen (Eds.), lescence. Behaviour Change, 18, 194–203.
Handbook of depression (pp. 541–559). New York: Radloff, L. S. (1991). The use of the Center for Epidemio-
Guilford. logic Studies Depression Scale in adolescents and young
Lewinsohn, P. M., Rohde, P., Seeley, J. R., & Hops, H. adults. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 149–166.
(1991). Comorbidity of unipolar depression: I. Major Rao, U., Hammen, C., & Daley, S. (1999). Continuity of
depression with dysthymia. Journal of Abnormal depression during the transition to adulthood: A 5-year
Psychology, 100, 205–213. longitudinal study of young women. Journal of the
Lewinsohn, P. M., Hops, H., Roberts, R. E., Seeley, J. R., & American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychia-
Andrews, J. (1993). Adolescent psychopathology: try, 38, 908–915.
I. Prevalence and incidence of depression and other Reich, W. (1991). The diagnostic interview for children and
DSM-III-R disorders in high school students. Journal adolescents – Revised. St. Louis: Washington University.
of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 133–144. Reynolds, W. M. (1987). Reynolds adolescent depression
Lewinsohn, P. M., Clarke, G. N., Seeley, J. R., & Rohde, P. scale: Professional manual. Odessa: Psychological
(1994). Major depression in community adolescents: Assessment Resources.
Age at onset, episode duration, and time to recurrence. Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2003). Handbook
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Ado- of psychological and educational assessment of chil-
lescent Psychiatry, 33, 809–818. dren: Personality, behavior, and context (2nd ed.).
Lewinsohn, P. M., Rohde, P., & Seeley, J. R. (1998). Major New York: Guilford.
depressive disorder in older adolescents: Prevalence, Roberts, R. E., & Chen, Y. W. (1995). Depressive symp-
risk factors, and clinical implications. Clinical Psychol- toms and suicidal ideation among Mexican-origin and
ogy Review, 18, 765–794. Anglo adolescents. Journal of the American Academy
Lewinsohn, P. M., Rohde, P., Klein, D. N., & Seeley, J. R. of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 81–90.
(1999). Natural course of adolescent major depressive Roberts, R. E., Andrews, J. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Hops, H.
disorder: I. Continuity into young adulthood. Journal (1990). Assessment of depression in adolescents using
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale.
Psychiatry, 38, 56–63. Psychological Assessment, 2, 122–128.
950 Depressive Symptoms

Rohde, P., Kahler, C. W., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Brown, Spence, S. H., Sheffield, J. K., & Donovan, C. L. (2005).
R. A. (2004). Psychiatric disorders, familial factors, Long-term outcome of a school-based, universal
and cigarette smoking: III. Associations with cessation approach to prevention of depression in adolescents.
by young adulthood among daily smokers. Nicotine Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73,
and Tobacco Research, 5, 509–522. 160–167.
Rudolph, K. D., & Clark, A. G. (2001). Conceptions of Spitzer, R. L., Williams, B., Kroenke, K., Linzer, M.,
relationships in children with depressive and aggres- deGruy, F. V., Hahn, S. R., et al. (1994). Utility of a
sive symptoms: Social-cognitive distortion or real- new procedure for diagnosing mental disorders in pri-
ity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, mary care. The PRIME-MD study. The Journal of the
41–56. American Medical Association, 272, 1749–1756.
Rudolph, K. D., Hammen, C., Burge, D., Lindberg, N., Sturm, R. (1997). How expensive is unlimited mental
Herzberg, D., & Daley, S. E. (2000). Toward an inter- health care coverage under managed care? Journal of
personal life-stress model of depression: The develop- the American Medical Association, 278, 1533–1537.
mental context of stress generation. Development and Sung, M., Erkanli, A., Angold, A., & Costello, E. (2004).
Psychopathology, 12, 215–234. Effects of age at first substance use and psychiatric
Rutter, M., Kim-Cohen, J., & Maughan, B. (2006). Conti- comorbidity on the development of substance use dis-
nuities and discontinuities in psychopathology between orders. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 75, 287–299.
childhood and adult life. Journal of Child Psychology Sutton, J. M. (2007). Prevention of depression in youth:
and Psychiatry, 47, 276–295. A qualitative review and future suggestions. Clinical
Shaffer, D., & Craft, L. (1999). Methods of adolescent Psychology Review, 27, 552–571.
suicide prevention. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60, Thomas, C. R. (1999). National distribution of child and
70–74. adolescent psychiatrists. Journal of the American
Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C., Dulcan, M., & Schwab- Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 9–15.
Stone, M. (2000). NIMH Diagnostic Interview Sched- Walker, Z., Townsend, J., & Oakley, L. (2002). Health
ule for Children Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): promotion for adolescents in primary care: Random-
Description, differences from previous versions and ized controlled trial. British Medical Journal, 325,
reliability of some common diagnoses. Journal of the 524–529.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychia- Waxmonsky, J., Wood, B. L., Stern, T., Ballow, M., Lillis,
try, 39, 28–38. K., & Cramer-Benjamin, D. (2006). Association of
Shaffer, D., Scott, M., & Wilcox, M. (2004). The Columbia depressive symptoms and disease activity in children
suicide screen: Validity and reliability of a screen for with asthma: Methodological and clinical implications.
youth suicide and depression. Journal of the American Journal of the American Academy of Child and Ado-
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, lescent Psychiatry, 45, 945–954.
71–79. Weiss, B., & Garber, J. (2003). Developmental differences
Shahar, G., Henrich, C. C., Winokur, A., Blatt, S. J., in the phenomenology of depression. Development and
Kuperminc, G. P., & Leadbeater, B. J. (2006). Self- Psychopathology, 15, 403–430.
criticism and depressive symptoms interact to predict Weissman, M. M., Wolk, S., Goldstein, R. B., Moreau, D.,
middle school academic achievement. Journal of Clin- Adams, P., Greenwald, S., et al. (1999a). Depressed
ical Psychology, 62, 147–155. adolescents grown up. Journal of the American Medi-
Sheffield, J. K., Spence, S. H., Rapee, R. M., Kowalenko, cal Association, 281, 1707–1713.
N., Wignall, A., & Davis, A. (2006). Evaluation of Weissman, M. M., Wolk, S., Widkramaratne, P., Goldstein,
universal, indicated, and combined cognitive- R. B., Adams, P., Greenwald, S., et al. (1999b). Chil-
behavioral approaches to the prevention of depression dren with pre-pubertal onset major depressive disorder
among adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical and anxiety grown up. Archives of General Psychiatry,
Psychology, 74, 66–79. 6, 29–36.
Shochet, I., Dadds, M., Holland, D., Whitefield, K., Wells, K. B., Stuart, A., Hays, R. D., Burnam, M. A.,
Harnett, P., & Osgarby, S. (2001). The efficacy of a Rogers, W., Daniels, M., et al. (1989). The functioning
universal schoolbased program to prevent adolescent and well-being of depressed patients: Results from the
depression. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30, medical outcomes study. Journal of the American Med-
303–315. ical Association, 262, 914–919.
Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Meyer, J. M., Silberg, J. L., Wells, K. B., Kataoka, S. H., & Asarnow, J. R. (2001).
Maes, H. H., Loeber, R., et al. (1997). The Virginia Affective disorders in children and adolescents:
twin study of adolescent behavioral development Addressing unmet need in primary care settings. Bio-
influences of age, sex, and impairment on rates logical Psychiatry, 49, 1111–1120.
of disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, Wolff, J. C., & Ollendick, T. H. (2006). The comorbidity of
801–808. conduct problems and depression in childhood and
Spence, S. H., Sheffield, J. K., & Donovan, C. L. (2003). adolescence. Clinical Child and Family Psychology
Preventing adolescent depression: An evaluation of the Review, 9, 201–220.
problem solving for life program. Journal of Consult- Wren, F. J., Schollle, S. H., Heo, J., & Comer, D. M.
ing and Clinical Psychology, 71, 3–13. (2005). How do primary care clinicians manage
Designer Drugs 951

childhood mood and anxiety syndromes? International reasons for engaging in the habitual usage of
Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 35, 1–12. such substances. As expected, respondents who
Yorbik, O., Birmaher, B., Axelson, D., Williamson, D. E.,
& Ryan, N. D. (2004). Clinical characteristics of used amphetamines, cocaine, hallucinogens,
depressive symptoms in children and adolescents with and/or designer drugs tended to use drugs for
major depressive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychi- social or psychological enjoyment (Spooner
atry, 65, 1654–1659. 1999). Notably, adolescents who had particular
Young, J. F., Mufson, L., & Davies, M. (2006). Efficacy of
interpersonal psychotherapy-adolescent skills training: motivations for their drug usage also tended to
An indicated preventive intervention for depression. adhere to a certain type of substance. Large stud-
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, ies of youth at risk, such as those who have been D
1254–1262. involved in the juvenile justice system, indicate
Yu, D. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Preventing depres-
sion in Chinese students. Prevention & Treatment, 5, that alcohol and marijuana use are the substances
Article 9. of choice (with over 70% reporting use), followed
Zalsman, G., Brent, D. A., & Weersing, V. R. (2006). by powder cocaine and ecstasy and other designer
Depressive disorders in childhood and adolescence: drugs (with approximately 10% of youth using
An overview epidemiology, clinical manifestation and
risk factors. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics them) (Braithwaite et al. 2003). Although preva-
of North America, 15, 827–841. lence rates of designer drug use may be relatively
Zuckerbrot, R. A., Maxon, L., Pagar, D., Davies, M., low in comparison to the use of many other sub-
Fisher, P. W., & Shaffer, D. (2007). Adolescent depres- stances, they still represent a wide range of serious
sion screening in primary care: Feasibility and accept-
ability. Pediatrics, 119, 101–108. health risks. For example, since no one can reli-
ably assess or confirm a kitchen chemist’s knowl-
edge and abilities, as well as the manner the drug
reached and circulated in the market, it is impos-
Designer Drugs sible to determine their products’ potency, purity,
or toxic potentiation for physical and psycholog-
Roger J. R. Levesque ical damage, let alone the other types of effects
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA associated with drug use. The relative lack of
information about these types of drugs provides
challenges for basic researchers seeking to fill
The term designer drugs refers to any kind of the void in the understanding about them and
regulated drug chemical substance that has had poses problems for practitioners confronted with
its structure modified so that it can be trafficked treating and responding to users of these
outside of restrictions placed on the regulated substances.
drug it resembles. Current regulations stipulate
that the precise chemical structure and label
attached to a unique compound must be specified Cross-References
before it can fall under the purview of controlled
substances and the regulations associated with ▶ Substance Use Risk and Protective Factors
them. With the easy accessibility of the Internet
and potentially far reach of peer groups, many
adolescents have found increased availability of References
a wide spectrum of recreational drugs, including
Braithwaite, R. L., Conerly, R. C., Robilliard, A. G., Ste-
Ecstasy and hallucinogenic amphetamines (see
phens, T. T., & Woodring, T. (2003). Alcohol and other
Wax 2002). In addition to increased access to drug use among adolescent detainees. Journal of Sub-
these new types of drugs, adolescents report stance Use, 8, 126–131.
increased use of them and appear to use them for Spooner, C. (1999). Causes and correlates of adolescent
drug abuse and implications. Drug and Alcohol Review,
reasons similar to more traditional substances. In a
18, 453–475.
study of illicit drug use among adolescents, for Wax, M. (2002). Just a click away: Recreational drug web
example, many specified that they had specific sites on the Internet. Pediatrics, 109, 1–4.
952 Desistance from Crime and Delinquency

Foundational Research on Desistance


Desistance from Crime and
Delinquency One of the most comprehensive reviews of the
desistance literature can be found in Laub and
Lila Kazemian Sampson (2001). The authors discuss various the-
Department of Sociology, John Jay oretical, conceptual, and methodological issues
College of Criminal Justice, New York, relevant to this topic, highlight the limitations of
NY, USA past studies on desistance, provide an overview of
the theoretical frameworks developed to explain
desistance, and report empirical findings on the
predictors of desistance. While relatively few
Overview texts have focused solely on the topic of desis-
tance, sources on developmental, life-course, and
The association between age and crime is one of criminal-career research often include a segment
the most established facts in the field of crimi- on desistance. Sampson and Laub (1993) and
nology. It is generally agreed that aggregate Laub and Sampson (2003) are essential readings
crime rates peak in late adolescence/early adult- in the area of desistance. The authors present
hood and gradually drop thereafter, but there is findings from their follow-up of the males
still very little consensus regarding the cause of involved in the Gluecks’ original study (Glueck
this decline. In the field of Criminology, desis- and Glueck 1950). Sampson and Laub (1993)
tance is generally defined as the cessation of present findings from the first major reanalysis
offending or other antisocial behavior. Desis- of the Glueck data, with a particular emphasis on
tance is one of the central dimensions studied the role of social bonds in the explanation of crime
in life-course criminology and is also one of the and desistance. In the second major reanalysis,
key criminal-career parameters. In recent years, Laub and Sampson (2003) extend the follow-up
the growing literature on the topic of desistance period to age 70, investigate trajectories of
from crime and deviant behavior has generated a offending, and address the issue of prediction.
large body of knowledge on this dimension of Two other reports also offer key research in this
the criminal career. Despite these efforts, it has area. Maruna (2001) reports results from the Liv-
been suggested that our understanding of the erpool Desistance Study, a follow-up study of
processes underlying desistance remains lim- desisting former offenders and persisting
ited. The topic of desistance is particularly offenders. This text offers a qualitative analysis
important for intervention and prevention pur- of the desistance process among a group of for-
poses, as it guides post-onset efforts aiming to merly incarcerated individuals by examining the
limit the intensity, seriousness, and duration of narratives of “desisting” and “persisting”
criminal careers. The main correlates of desis- offenders. Ezell and Cohen (2005) address vari-
tance identified in the literature include the ous key questions in desistance research, such as
development of stronger bonds to sources of individual distributions of the age-crime curve
informal social control (namely marriage, chil- and the degree of stability and change in offending
dren, employment), associations with prosocial behavior across time. The authors conduct thor-
peers and shifts in routine activities, the cessa- ough analyses using three samples of high-rate
tion of substance use, and cognitive changes offenders under the jurisdiction of the California
(i.e., increased maturation, determination to Youth Authority to elucidate some of these impor-
cease offending, willingness to change, and tant issues. In addition to that study, the National
shifts in identity). It has also been suggested Research Council published a policy-oriented
that desistance is most likely to occur if cogni- report in 2007, which reviewed current reentry
tive changes occur in conjunction with a strong practices, resources, and services available to for-
social support system. merly incarcerated individuals. A discussion of
Desistance from Crime and Delinquency 953

some of the major limitations of research on career patterns. As such, these studies are well
parole, desistance, and reintegration is presented, equipped to investigate the desistance process,
including shortcomings relating to parole hetero- particularly during the transition period between
geneity, intervention effects, and methodological adolescence and early adulthood. Some of these
flaws. Recommendations for policy and research data are available through the Interuniversity Con-
are laid out in this publication. sortium for Political and Social Research
(ICPSR). This section highlights some of the key
longitudinal studies that have accessible data.
Populations Generally Studied There are several important longitudinal studies D
in this area. The Cambridge Study in Delinquent
While adolescent samples are highly relevant to Development is a longitudinal follow-up of
the study desistance, it is also crucial to study this 411 working-class males from London, first
dimension of the criminal career beyond adoles- contacted in 1961–1962 when the boys were 8–9
cence. One of the key issues in desistance research years old, and interviewed on eight subsequent
is the length of follow-up required to study desis- occasions (up to age 46). Parent and teacher
tance. Estimates of the age of termination are reports were documented at younger ages. These
dependent on the length of the follow-up period data are particularly useful for the study of desis-
(Bushway et al. 2003; Le Blanc and Loeber 1998). tance in adolescence and early adulthood; compre-
Farrington and Wikström (1994) argue that short hensive interviews were conducted at ages 14, 16,
follow-up periods can result in misleading find- and 18. Data include psychological, social, behav-
ings. In a follow-up of adjudicated men up to age ioral, and offending variables. ICPSR includes
25, Le Blanc and Fréchette (1989) found that the CSDD data up to 1981 (see Farrington et al.
average age at last conviction was 19.9 years. (2006), for an overview of the study). The
When the follow-up extended to the early 40s, National Youth Survey collected both self-reported
the average age at last conviction was 31 years. and official information about offending over var-
One of the strongest arguments supporting the ious periods of the life course, and the data from
need to study desistance beyond adolescence is this study has produced numerous publications.
portrayed through the fact that most juvenile The NYS began in 1977, with an initial random
delinquents do not become adult offenders sample of 1,725 youths (11–17 years) from US
(Moffitt 1993). In this regard, it is important to households. Data for the NYS for waves I (1976)
collect longitudinal data about offending and through VII (1987), as well as a compilation of
other developmental measures in order to better studies that have been published with these data,
understand patterns of desistance and persistence are available on ICPSR. The Philadelphia Birth
during and after adolescence. Although it is Cohort studies consist of two follow-ups of dis-
possible to study the desistance process using tinct Philadelphia cohorts, one born in 1945 and
cross-sectional or retrospective data, long-term, another in 1958. The first cohort includes 9,945
prospective follow-ups help to establish with Philadelphia males born in 1945. The second
increased certainty whether desistance has actu- cohort consists of 27,160 Philadelphia males and
ally occurred. The intermittent character of females born in 1958. Data includes demographic
offending patterns may sometimes lead to the and social variables, as well as measures of
illusion of desistance. This issue will be discussed offending (see Wolfgang et al. (1972) and Tracy
in a subsequent section. et al. (1990) for a description of the study).
While the body of research on desistance is Unraveling Juvenile Delinquency is a longitudinal
growing, data from these studies are not consis- study of 500 adjudicated and 500 representative
tently accessible to the public. In many instances, Boston males, led by Sheldon and Eleanor Glueck.
these data are collected in the context of larger Information was collected through official records,
longitudinal studies, generally as part of more self-reports, and teacher and parent reports. These
extensive analyses on life-course and criminal- data have been analyzed extensively in Sampson
954 Desistance from Crime and Delinquency

and Laub’s various works on desistance. Data and Sampson 2001, 2003; Maruna 2001). The
from 1939 through 1963 are available on ICPSR complete cessation of offending activities is
(see Glueck and Glueck 1950). The Longitudinal unlikely to occur suddenly, especially among
Study of Criminal Career Patterns of Former Cal- individuals who have been highly active in
ifornia Youth Authority Wards is a longitudinal offending from a young age. For this reason, static
study of serious juvenile offenders released from definitions of desistance (i.e., termination) may
the California Youth Authority between 1965 and mask the progress exhibited by individuals across
1984. Information about offending, substance use, various stages of the desistance process (see
and life events is documented. Only official infor- Bushway et al. 2001, for a discussion on this
mation is available. These data have been used in topic). Therefore, focusing solely on the final
publications on desistance (e.g., see Ezell and state of termination provides little guidance for
Cohen 2005). intervention initiatives, neglecting to offer support
Other longitudinal studies have contributed to and reinforcement in periods when they are most
the literature on desistance, with follow-ups needed (i.e., periods of reassessment and ambiva-
extending from childhood or adolescence into lence toward desistance/persistence). Although
adulthood. While these data are not available to different individuals may cease offending at the
the public, these studies are noteworthy. These same age, their criminal careers may be distin-
include the Pittsburgh Youth Study, the Rochester guished by very different processes (in terms of
Youth Development Study, the Denver Youth Sur- frequency, seriousness, and length).
vey, the Montreal Longitudinal and Experimental Bushway et al. (2001) present a thorough
Study, the Montreal Two Samples Longitudinal discussion on the importance of studying desis-
Study, and the Seattle Social Development Pro- tance as a process and develop a statistical frame-
ject. These studies have made important contribu- work for analyzing the process of desistance. On
tions to the literature on persistence and desistance the premise of this argument, Bushway et al.
from crime across various periods of the life (2003) conduct analyses to compare the predic-
course, including adolescence and the transition tors of desistance using static (cessation of
to adulthood. offending) and dynamic (changes in offending)
definitions, and show that the two operationa-
lizations of desistance may yield different
Conceptual, Measurement, and findings.
Methodological Issues Uggen and Kruttschnitt (1998) contrast predic-
tors of self-reported and official desistance, and
Issues relating to the definition, measurement, and make the distinction between two types of desis-
operationalization of desistance have been raised tance: behavioral desistance refers to the shift
in the desistance literature. Laub and Sampson from self-reported crime to noncrime, whereas
(2001) and Kazemian (2007) offer an overview official desistance refers to the absence of official
of some of these issues. Among the issues that offending (measured through arrests or convic-
they address are questions such as the definition of tions). Conclusions drawn about desistance seem
desistance, the length of follow-up required to to vary according to the type of measurement
study desistance, and the type of data (self-report method privileged (i.e., official vs self-reported
versus official) employed for this type of analysis. desistance; see Le Blanc and Fréchette 1989;
Many studies on desistance have adopted a Nagin et al. 1995; Uggen and Massoglia 2003).
dichotomous or static measure of desistance. As Laub and Sampson (2001) argue that few stud-
a result, these studies have not accounted for ies have offered an operational definition of desis-
changes in rates of offending. An increasing num- tance, and that there is currently no consensus on
ber of researchers have acknowledged the rele- this issue (see also Maruna 2001; Piquero et al.
vance of perceiving desistance as a gradual 2003). Various definitions of desistance have been
process (see Bushway et al. 2001, 2003; Laub developed in the literature. Meisenhelder (1977,
Desistance from Crime and Delinquency 955

p. 319) defined desistance as exiting, i.e., the The Issue of False Desistance
“successful disengagement from a previously
developed, and subjectively recognized, pattern False desistance refers to the erroneous assump-
of criminal behavior.” Drawing on ideas devel- tion that individuals have ceased offending.
oped by Le Blanc and Fréchette (1989), Loeber Desistance often is defined as the last officially
and Le Blanc (1990) offered a definition of desis- recorded or self-reported offense. Because desis-
tance that integrates four dimensions; the authors tance studies may employ cross-sectional data or
argue that before criminal activity ceases relatively short follow-up periods, researchers
completely, lambda declines, offenders become may draw misleading conclusions about whether D
increasingly specialized and engage in more study participants have truly ceased offending. It
minor offenses, and a culmination point is has been argued that definite desistance only
reached. Uggen and Kruttschnitt’s (1998) defini- occurs with death (Blumstein et al. 1982; Elliott
tion of behavioral desistance implies a shift from a et al. 1989; Farrington and Wikström 1994).
state of offending to a state of non-offending, and Piquero (2004, p. 108) defines intermittency as
its maintenance. Maruna (2001) argues that the “the time between successive criminal events,
definition of desistance needs to emphasize controlling of course for exposure time.” Mis-
maintenance rather than termination. Bushway chkowitz (1994) makes the distinction between
et al. (2001, p. 500) define desistance as “the desistance and interruptions in the criminal career.
process of reduction in the rate of offending Elliott et al. (1989) refer to a “suspension” from
(understood conceptually as an estimate of crim- criminal activity, implying that the discontinua-
inality) from a nonzero level to a stable rate tion of offending activity is not permanent. Bur-
empirically indistinguishable from zero.” Indis- nett (2004, p. 169) describes ambivalence in the
tinguishable from zero refers to the idea that decision to desist from crime and argues that
offending rates become indistinguishable from “desistance is a process which involves reversals
those of non-offenders. Because criminal events of decisions, indecision, compromise, and lapses”
often are dependent on circumstantial factors and (see Horney et al. 1995, for similar results). Inter-
chance, the authors further argue that the desis- mittent patterns of offending in criminal careers
tance process should focus on changes occurring may lead to the false interpretation that offenders
in the propensity to offend (i.e., criminality) have ceased offending (Piquero 2004). This issue
rather than on changes in crime. of temporary versus permanent desistance
Other operationalizations of desistance include (or “zigzag” patterns; see Laub and Sampson
convictions at age 21, but not between 21 and 2003; Piquero 2004) has been highlighted by
32 (Farrington and Hawkins 1991), the absence criminal-career researchers (e.g., Barnett et al.
of new officially recorded offenses or probation 1989; Piquero et al. 2003), although very few
violation over a 2-year period (Kruttschnitt et al. studies have explored this question in depth
2000), juvenile delinquents who were not arrested (Piquero 2004). The issue of false desistance is
as adults (Sampson and Laub 1993), and the particularly relevant to studies that focus exclu-
absence of self-reported illegal earnings sively on studying desistance during adolescence,
(behavioral desistance) or arrests (official desis- without follow-ups into adulthood.
tance) over a 3-year follow-up period (Uggen and
Kruttschnitt 1998). Bushway et al. (2003) argue
that the selection of cutoff points is arbitrary and Typologies in Desistance Research
varies from one study to another, making it diffi-
cult to compare findings. Given these conceptual In recent years, empirical evidence in criminal-
disparities, Uggen and Massoglia (2003, career research has “suggested the existence of
pp. 316–317) have argued that “. . .it is difficult distinct types of offenders such as early starters,
to draw empirical generalizations from the grow- late starters, persisters, desisters, occasionals, and
ing literature on desistance from crime.” chronics” (Paternoster and Brame 1997, p. 236).
956 Desistance from Crime and Delinquency

Limitations of the typological approach, mainly age. This framework is more relevant to the expla-
issues linked to parsimony, offender heterogene- nation of desistance in adulthood.
ity, and overlap between the groups have often Two major developmental explanations of
been raised. Maruna (2001) argues that the dichot- desistance have been developed. Moffitt (1993)
omy between persisters and desisters is useful for presents a taxonomy that includes two groups of
statistical purposes, but that it is subject to con- offenders: adolescence-limited and life-course
siderable intra-group variability. Maruna further persisters. The author explains that desistance is
maintains that “the two groups (desisting and more likely for the adolescence-limited offenders
persisting offenders) represent similar individuals because they are capable of reintegrating them-
in different stages of the process of change... and selves into a conventional lifestyle; they are also
not two starkly different ‘types’ of people” (p. 74). characterized by less “injurious childhoods.” The
In their analyses of life narratives, Laub and life-course-persistent offenders show increased
Sampson (2003, p. 240) also demonstrate the continuity in their offending habits and manifest
ambiguities associated with defining groups as early onset, high offending frequency, and later
persisters or desisters, and argue that classifica- desistance. This typology has been tested empiri-
tions grow increasingly complex as the length of cally using various data. Studying the narratives
the observation period increases. The authors of “desisting” and “persisting” Liverpool males,
reject the idea that “offenders can be neatly Maruna (2001) offers another developmental
grouped into distinct categories, each displaying framework for the study of desistance. He sug-
a unique trajectory and etiology of offending” gests that desistance occurs as a result of is
(p. 4). They further add that offender typologies explained by cognitive changes in cognition,
“do not capture the patterning or complexity of thinking styles, and self-perception (such as
criminal offending over the full life course” changes in perceptions of identity).
(p. 248), and that this issue cannot be resolved Various authors have emphasized the link
with the use of more sophisticated statistical tech- between different forms of social bonds and desis-
niques. The rationale underlying the use of tance, and this framework has produced an abun-
offender typologies relates to the idea that explan- dance of empirical research. This perspective is
atory and causal mechanisms for desistance are best summarized in Sampson and Laub (1993).
distinct from those of persistence. Empirical Drawing on Travis Hirschi’s earlier work,
research has yet to establish whether clear differ- Sampson and Laub’s (1993) age-graded theory
ences exist between desisters, persisters, and of informal social control theory emphasizes the
non-offenders. importance of age-graded informal social controls
(i.e., bonds to conventional social institutions) in
the explanation of desistance. In adolescence,
Explanatory Frameworks for Desistance bond to parents, school, and friends are particu-
larly influential. The authors explain that while
Various explanatory frameworks have been devel- there is some degree of stability in offending
oped to explain the desistance process; these have behavior, there is also a substantial degree of
been discussed more extensively in Laub and variation in offending patterns across the life
Sampson (2001). The maturation framework, course. In this text, Sampson and Laub argue
discussed in Glueck and Glueck (1940), stipulates that changes in the strength and structure of social
that physical, intellectual, and emotional develop- bonds may explain the intermittent character of
ment (i.e., maturation) is the main cause for offending patterns across the life course.
decline or cessation of offending behavior. Whereas Sampson and Laub argue that desis-
Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) present their gen- tance from crime occurs as a result of strong bonds
eral theory of crime and argue that aging is a major to social institutions, Gottfredson and Hirschi
reason for the decline in crime observed over time, (1990) maintain that crime is dependent on the
and that offending declines for all offenders with individual’s level of self-control, and that
Desistance from Crime and Delinquency 957

individuals with low self-control are less likely to of strong social bonds in the desistance process,
desist from crime. The authors argue that the and the authors argue that the degree of attach-
association social bonds do not exert a direct ment to the spouse and employment promote
effect on desistance, but rather that this associa- desistance. Farrington and West (1995) and Laub
tion occurs through a process of self-selection. et al. (1998) study the effects of marriage as
Rational choice theory is another framework well as the timing and quality of marriage, on
used to explain desistance. Gartner and Piliavin offending behavior and desistance. Using data
(1988) discuss the effects of age on life-course from the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Devel-
offending patterns and suggest that the decision opment, Farrington and West (1995) find that the D
to desist or persist in crime is a result of an assess- stability of marriage impacts desistance. Laub
ment of costs and benefits associated with given et al. (1998) highlight the gradual nature of
behaviors. In a theoretical piece contrasting ratio- developing social relationships as well as the
nal choice and social boding theories, Hirschi ongoing and cumulative effect of marriage on
(1986) suggests that there is a tendency to over- the desistance process. The influence of the
estimate the offender’s ability to make rational quality of marital bonds in promoting desistance
decisions, and that desistance is likely to be is also discussed in this study. Ouimet and Le
impacted by the fact that offenders do not always Blanc (1996) find that cohabitation with a part-
make accurate assessments of the costs and bene- ner may exert a criminogenic effect before the
fits of crime. age of 21 years, but that this variable is posi-
tively associated with desistance after this age.
Bersani et al. (2009) find that while marriage
Correlates of Desistance Identified in the reduces the odds of conviction for both males
Literature and females, there is a reduced effect of marital
status on desistance among women when com-
While there is still no general consensus regarding pared to their male counterparts. Their results
the causes of desistance, some general themes also show that marriage exerts a more substan-
have emerged from the literature. The study of tial effect on the odds of conviction among the
predictors of desistance has been investigated youngest age group.
through three main lenses: life events, individual Kruttschnitt et al. (2000), Uggen (2000), and
predictors, and interactions between social and Sampson and Laub (1993) investigate the role of
cognitive indicators. An overview of some of employment on crime and recidivism. Using data
these findings can be found in Laub and from the National Supported Work Demonstration
Sampson (2001). Project, Uggen (2000) identifies age-graded
effects of employment on recidivism (with a
Social Predictors more substantial effect observed on older individ-
Numerous studies have examined the effects uals, over 26 years of age), raising the question of
of social bonds on crime and desistance. whether the impact of employment on desistance
Savolainen (2009) examines the impact of mar- may be different with adolescent and adult
riage, cohabitation, parenthood, and employment populations. Warr (1998) and Wright and Cullen
on reconvictions among a sample of Finnish fel- (2004) adopt a social learning or differential asso-
ony offenders, and finds that strong social bonds ciation position, and argue that the effects of mar-
lead to reduced incidences of reconviction. Using riage and employment on crime are mediated by
a retrospective survey of 600 serious offenders, peer associations.
Horney et al. (1995) examine variations in crimi-
nal behavior in the short-term and find that social Cognitive Predictors
bonds contribute to the explanation of these vari- The cognitive predictors of desistance have been
ations. Sampson and Laub’s (1993) analysis of the discussed in literature of diverse nature. Gove
Gluecks’ data strongly emphasizes the crucial role (1985) presents five key internal changes that
958 Desistance from Crime and Delinquency

promote desistance: a shift from self- (2002) analysis of narratives suggests that perma-
centeredness to consideration for others, the nent desistance from crime may be a result of both
development of prosocial values and behavior, cognitive changes and turning points (“hooks for
an increasing ease in social interactions, a greater change”). Shover (1996) interviews a sample of
consideration for other members of the commu- incarcerated burglars and identifies two dimen-
nity, and a growing concern for the “meaning of sions explaining changes in criminal careers: the
life.” Giordano et al. (2002) present a theory of strengthening of conventional bonds, activities
cognitive transformation and discuss its four and rewards, and resolve and determination.
underlying processes, which include openness Focusing on family violence, Fagan (1989)
to change, exposure to prosocial experiences, describes a three-stage process explaining desis-
the development of a new prosocial and non- tance. LeBel et al. (2008) discuss three models
criminal identity, and a shift in the perception of explaining the interaction between social and sub-
the criminal lifestyle. Burnett (2004) and Shover jective factors: the strong subjective model
and Thompson (1992) argue that the degree of (emphasis on individual motivation and desire to
optimism regarding cessation of offending change), the strong social model (direct effect of
behavior and individual determination to cease life events on desistance), and the subjective
offending are important cognitive factors that social model (interaction between life events and
promote the desistance process. The importance the mindset of the individual). Bottoms et al.
of identity transformation in the process of desis- (2004) offer a discussion on the interaction
tance has been discussed by Bottoms et al. between the desire to change and the magnitude
(2004), Burnett (2004), Giordano et al. (2002), of social problems. Beaver et al. (2007) investi-
Maruna (2001), and Shover (1996). Bottoms gate the respective effects of marriage and genetic
et al. (2004) develop a theoretical framework dispositions on desistance from crime, and their
for understanding desistance and offer a discus- findings highlight the importance of integrating a
sion of the gradual nature of desistance as well as biosocial framework in the explanation of
the cognitive processes that are crucial to desistance.
enabling this process. Using data from the
Oxford Recidivism Study, Burnett (2004) Substance Use
addresses various issues relating to desistance Some recent studies have investigated different
from crime, including the intermittent patterns dimensions of the relationship between substance
of offending and the effects of optimism on use and desistance. Hussong et al. (2004) employ
desistance outcomes. Giordano et al. (2002) data from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health
develop a theory of cognitive transformation, and Development Study to investigate the sub-
defined as the cognitive shifts that promote the stance use-desistance link and find that substance
process of desistance. Maruna’s (2001) central use exerts both distal and contemporaneous
thesis revolves around the idea that individuals effects on desistance efforts. In a replication of
need to develop a prosocial identity in order to Hussong et al. (2004) hypotheses, Morizot and Le
desist from crime and to make progress in the Blanc’s (2007) findings also suggest that sub-
process of “making good.” Shover (1996) also stance use may constitute an important inhibitor
addresses the association between change in to desistance, both in the short and long term.
identity and variations in patterns of offending Schroeder et al. (2007) use data from the Ohio
among persistent property offenders. Life-Course Study to assess the mediating role of
social networks in the relationship between drug
Interaction Between Internal and use and desistance. The authors find that drug
External Changes use exerts a stronger and more sustained impact
Various authors have stressed the importance of on offending behavior when compared to alcohol
considering both social and cognitive dimensions use. The study highlights the criminogenic
in the explanation of desistance. Giordano et al. effect of drug use and its associated lifestyle
Desistance from Crime and Delinquency 959

characteristics, as well as the mediating role of desistance, or offending behavior in general. In


some forms of social networks that may also ham- a recent analysis of the Glueck men, Laub and
per desistance efforts. Sampson (2003) find evidence which refutes
some of the basic principles put forth by devel-
opmental criminologists, namely the ability
Gaps in Knowledge of risk factors measured in childhood and
adolescence to predict desistance in adulthood.
Self-Selection and Sequencing According to Moffitt (1993), life-course-
Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) argue that life persistent offenders are characterized by an D
events do not exert a direct causal impact on early onset, high offending frequency, and
desistance and rather reflect levels of self-control. delayed desistance. Moffitt argues that there is a
Because turning points and life events are not great degree of continuity in the offending habits
randomly assigned among individuals, it is diffi- of the life-course-persistent group, although she
cult to assess whether these events are causes or also acknowledges that the propensity to offend
correlates of desistance. Knowledge about inter- may vary with the availability of opportunities
actions between levels of self-control and social for crime. In contrast, Laub and Sampson (2003,
bonds is still limited. One area that deserves more p. 588) “. . .failed to find convincing evidence
attention in desistance research relates to whether that a life-course-persistent group can be pro-
the impact of social bonds on desistance is spectively or even retrospectively identified
maintained when accounting for cognitive predis- based on theoretical risk factors at the individual
positions, and vice versa. Evans et al. (1997, level in childhood and adolescence.” In a com-
p. 491) found that when including a measure of parative analysis of patterns of de-escalation
self-control, “the effects of the social bonds mea- between ages 17–18 and 32, Kazemian et al.
sures (quality of relationships, marital status, (2009) find that social bonds and cognitive pre-
attachment to church, occupational attainment) dispositions measured at age 17–18 are generally
in the equation are minimal.” better predictors of between- rather than within
The way in which individual predispositions individual differences in offending gravity at age
and life events converge to promote the desistance 32, highlighting the difficulties in making accu-
process is a complex problem, one that is further rate long-term predictions about changes in indi-
complicated by the issue of self-selection vidual offending patterns early in the criminal
(Kazemian 2007). Many authors have discussed career.
the complexity of establishing temporal or causal
order between cognitive processes, situational cir- Gender and Desistance
cumstances, and desistance from crime (Bottoms Few studies have investigated the link between
et al. 2004; Laub and Sampson 2001). The gender and desistance. Bersani et al. (2009) con-
unraveling of these sequences is thorny, mainly trast the effect of marriage on desistance between
because situational and cognitive changes are male and female Dutch offenders. Sommers et al.
often interdependent and may occur simulta- (1994) analyze narratives to shed some light on
neously (Maruna 2001). Le Blanc (1993, p. 56) the desistance process of high-rate female
argues that “Some potential variables may occur offenders. Giordano et al. (2002) and Uggen and
in such close proximity to desistance that. . .it is Kruttschnitt (1998) also compare the cognitive
impossible to measure which comes first,” and and social predictors of desistance between male
adds that this issue is even more complicated by and female offenders.
the presence of “reciprocal influences.”

Long-Term Predictions Cross-References


Another contentious issue relates to the ability
to make accurate long-term predictions about ▶ Abstention
960 Desistance from Crime and Delinquency

Acknowledgment Some excerpts from this essay were In Z. S. Blau & J. Hagan (Eds.), Current perspectives
originally published in Kazemian (2007) and Kazemian on aging and the life cycle (Vol. 4, pp. 249–281).
et al. (2009). Greenwich: JAI Press.
Farrington, D. P., & Wikström, P.-O. H. (1994). Criminal
careers in London and Stockholm: A cross-national
comparative study. In E. G. M. Weitekamp & H.-J.
References Kerner (Eds.), Cross-national longitudinal research
on human development and criminal behavior
Barnett, A., Blumstein, A., & Farrington, D. P. (1989). (pp. 65–89). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
A prospective test of a criminal career model. Crimi- Farrington, D. P., Coid, J. W., Jolliffe, D., Soteriou, N.,
nology, 27(2), 373–387. Turner, R., & West, D. J. (2006). Criminal careers up to
Beaver, K. M., Wright, J. P., DeLisi, M., & Vaughn, M. G. age 50 and life success up to age 48: New findings from
(2007). Desistance from delinquency: The marriage the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development
effect revisited and extended. Social Science Research, (No. Home Office Research Study No. 299). London:
37, 736–752. Home Office.
Bersani, B. E., Laub, J. H., & Nieuwbeerta, P. (2009). Gartner, R., & Piliavin, I. (1988). The aging offender and
Marriage and desistance from crime in the Netherlands: the aged offender. In P. B. Baltes, D. L. Featherman, &
Do gender and socio-historical context matter? Journal R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Life-span development and behav-
of Quantitative Criminology, 25(3), 3–24. ior (Vol. 9). Hillside: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Blumstein, A., Cohen, J., & Hsieh, P. (1982). The duration Giordano, P. L., Cernkovich, S. A., & Rudolph, J. L.
of adult criminal careers: Final report to National Insti- (2002). Gender, crime, and desistance: Toward a theory
tute of Justice. Pittsburgh: Carnegie-Mellon University. of cognitive transformation. American Journal of Soci-
Bottoms, A., Shapland, J., Costello, A., Holmes, D., & ology, 107(4), 990–1064.
Muir, G. (2004). Towards desistance: Theoretical Glueck, S., & Glueck, E. (1940). Juvenile delinquents
underpinnings for an empirical study. The Howard grown up. New York: Commonwealth Fund.
Journal of Criminal Justice, 43(4), 368–389. Glueck, S., & Glueck, E. (1950). Unraveling juvenile
Burnett, R. (2004). To reoffend or not to reoffend? The delinquency. New York: The Commonwealth Fund.
ambivalence of convicted property offenders. In Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory
S. Maruna & R. Immarigeon (Eds.), After crime and of crime. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
punishment: Pathways to offender reintegration Gove, W. R. (1985). The effect of age and gender on
(pp. 152–180). Cullompton: Willan. deviant behavior: A biopsychological perspective. In
Bushway, S. D., Piquero, A. R., Broidy, L. M., Cauffman, A. Rossi (Ed.), Gender and the life course
E., & Mazerolle, P. (2001). An empirical framework for (pp. 115–144). New York: Aldine.
studying desistance as a process. Criminology, 39(2), Hirschi, T. (1986). On the compatibility of rational choice
491–515. and social control theories of crime. In D. B. Cornish &
Bushway, S. D., Thornberry, T. P., & Krohn, M. D. (2003). R. V. Clarke (Eds.), The reasoning criminal: Rational
Desistance as a developmental process: A comparison choice perspectives on offending (pp. 105–118).
of static and dynamic approaches. Journal of Quanti- New York: Springer.
tative Criminology, 19(2), 129–153. Horney, J., Osgood, D. W., & Marshall, I. H. (1995).
Elliott, D. S., Huizinga, D., & Menard, S. (1989). Multiple Criminal careers in the short-term: Intra-individual var-
problem youth: Delinquency, substance use, and men- iability in crime and its relation to local life circum-
tal health problems. New York: Springer. stances. American Sociological Review, 60, 655–673.
Evans, T. D., Cullen, F. T., Burton, V. S. J., Dunaway, Hussong, A. M., Curran, P. J., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., &
R. G., & Benson, M. L. (1997). The social conse- Carrig, M. M. (2004). Substance abuse hinders desis-
quences of self-control: Testing the general theory of tance in young adults’ antisocial behavior. Develop-
crime. Criminology, 35(3), 475–504. ment and Psychopathology, 16, 1029–1046.
Ezell, M. E., & Cohen, L. E. (2005). Desisting from crime: Kazemian, L. (2007). Desistance from crime: Theoretical,
Continuity and change in long-term crime patterns of empirical, methodological, and policy considerations.
serious chronic offenders. Oxford: Oxford University Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 23(1),
Press. 28–49.
Fagan, J. (1989). Cessation of family violence: Deterrence Kazemian, L., Farrington, D. P., & Le Blanc, M. (2009).
and dissuasion. In L. Ohlin & M. Tonry (Eds.), Family Can we make accurate long-term predictions about
violence (pp. 377–425). Chicago: University of patterns of de-escalation in offending behavior? Jour-
Chicago Press. nal of Youth and Adolescence, 38(3), 384–400.
Farrington, D. P., & Hawkins, J. D. (1991). Predicting Kruttschnitt, C., Uggen, C., & Shelton, K. (2000). Pre-
participation, early onset, and later persistence in offi- dictors of desistance among sex offenders: The interac-
cially recorded offending. Criminal Behavior and Men- tion of formal and informal social controls. Justice
tal Health, 1, 1–33. Quarterly, 17(1), 61–87.
Farrington, D. P., & West, D. J. (1995). Effects of marriage, Laub, J. H., & Sampson, R. J. (2001). Understanding
separation, and children on offending by adults males. desistance from crime. In M. Tonry (Ed.), Crime and
Detention of Alien Juveniles 961

justice (Vol. 28, pp. 1–69). Chicago: University of Piquero, A., Farrington, D. P., & Blumstein, A. (2003). The
Chicago Press. criminal career paradigm. In M. Tonry (Ed.), Crime and
Laub, J. H., & Sampson, R. J. (2003). Shared beginnings, justice (Vol. 30, pp. 359–506). Chicago: University of
divergent lives: Delinquent boys to age 70. Cambridge, Chicago Press.
MA: Harvard University Press. Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (1993). Crime in the making:
Laub, J. H., Nagin, D. S., & Sampson, R. J. (1998). Tra- Pathways and turning points through life. Cambridge,
jectories of change in criminal offending: Good mar- MA: Harvard University Press.
riages and the desistance process. American Savolainen, J. (2009). Work, family, and criminal desis-
Sociological Review, 63, 225–238. tance: Adult social bonds in a nordic welfare state.
Le Blanc, M. (1993). Late adolescence deceleration of crim- British Journal of Criminology, 49, 285–304.
inal activity and development of self- and social control. Schroeder, R. D., Giordano, P. C., & Cernkovich, S. A. D
Studies on Crime and Crime Prevention, 2, 51–68. (2007). Drug use and desistance processes. Criminol-
Le Blanc, M., & Fréchette, M. (1989). Male criminal ogy, 45, 191–222.
activity from childhood through youth: Multilevel and Shover, N. (1996). Great pretenders: Pursuits and careers
developmental perspectives. New York: Springer. of persistent thieves. Boulder: Westview.
Le Blanc, M., & Loeber, R. (1998). Developmental crim- Shover, N., & Thompson, C. Y. (1992). Age, differential
inology updated. In M. Tonry (Ed.), Crime and justice expectations, and crime desistance. Criminology,
(Vol. 23, pp. 115–198). Chicago: University of Chicago 30(1), 89–104.
Press. Sommers, I., Baskin, D. R., & Fagan, J. (1994). Getting out
LeBel, T. P., Burnett, R., Maruna, S., & Bushway, of the life: Crime desistance by female street offenders.
S. (2008). The ‘chicken and egg’ of subjective and Deviant Behavior, 15, 125–149.
social factors in desistance from crime. European Jour- Tracy, P. E., Wolfgang, M. E., & Figlio, R. M. (1990).
nal of Criminology, 5(2), 130–158. Delinquency careers in two birth cohorts. New York:
Loeber, R., & Le Blanc, M. (1990). Toward a developmen- Plenum.
tal criminology. In M. Tonry & N. Morris (Eds.), Crime Uggen, C. (2000). Work as a turning point in the life course
and justice (Vol. 12, pp. 375–473). Chicago: University of criminals: A duration model of age, employment, and
of Chicago Press. recidivism. American Sociological Review, 67, 529–546.
Maruna, S. (2001). Making good: How ex-convicts reform Uggen, C., & Kruttschnitt, C. (1998). Crime in the break-
and rebuild their lives. Washington, DC: American ing: Gender differences in desistance. Law and Society
Psychological Association. Review, 32(2), 339–366.
Meisenhelder, T. (1977). An explanatory study of exiting Uggen, C., & Massoglia, M. (2003). Desistance from crime
from criminal careers. Criminology, 15(3), 319–334. and deviance as a turning point in the life course. In J. T.
Mischkowitz, R. (1994). Desistance from a delinquent way Mortimer & M. J. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of the
of life? In E. G. M. Weitekamp & H.-J. Kerner (Eds.), life course (pp. 311–329). New York: Kluwer/Plenum
Cross-national longitudinal research on human devel- Publishers.
opment and criminal behavior (pp. 303–327). Dor- Warr, M. (1998). Life-course transitions and desistance
drecht: Kluwer. from crime. Criminology, 36(2), 183–216.
Moffitt, T. E. (1993). ‘Life-course persistent’ and Wolfgang, M. E., Figlio, R. M., & Sellin, T. (1972). Delin-
‘adolescence-limited’ antisocial behavior: A develop- quency in a birth cohort. Chicago: University of Chi-
mental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100, cago Press.
674–701. Wright, J. P., & Cullen, F. T. (2004). Employment, peers, and
Morizot, J., & Le Blanc, M. (2007). Behavioral, self, and life-course transitions. Justice Quarterly, 21(1), 183–205.
social control predictors of desistance from crime: A test
of launch and contemporaneous effect models. Journal
of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 23(1), 50–71.
Nagin, D. S., Farrington, D. P., & Moffitt, T. E. (1995).
Life-course trajectories of different types of offenders. Detention of Alien Juveniles
Criminology, 33(1), 111–139.
Ouimet, M., & Le Blanc, M. (1996). The role of life
experiences in the continuation of the adult criminal Roger J. R. Levesque
career. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 6(1), Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
73–97.
Paternoster, R., & Brame, R. (1997). Multiple routes to
delinquency? A test of developmental and general the-
ories of crime. Criminology, 35(1), 49–84. Adolescents who have not reached the age of
Piquero, A. (2004). Somewhere between persistence and majority and who are not under the control or
desistance: The intermittency of criminal careers. In custody of their parents pose important challenges
S. Maruna & R. Immarigeon (Eds.), After crime and
punishment: Pathways to offender reintegration for legal systems. Those challenges are even
(pp. 102–125). Cullompton: Willan. greater when the adolescents are in the
962 Detention of Alien Juveniles

government’s custody because they are suspected child and able to ensure the minor’s attendance at
of being illegal immigrants. In the United States, a deportation hearing. This regulation was chal-
approximately seventy percent of the juveniles lenged, and in Reno v. Flores (1993), the Supreme
arrested for violation of immigration laws are unac- Court found it constitutional.
companied by their parents or guardians, and they In Flores, a 15-year-old girl was one of four
are unaccompanied typically because they have juveniles who filed a class action suit against the
been sent in advance or have been separated from attorney general of the United States, Janet Reno.
them when they entered the country illegally (Reno The four juveniles had been arrested and held by
v. Flores 1993). Given that they have not reached the INS pending deportation hearings. Among
the age of majority, immigrant minors have reduced other claims, their challenge to the INS regulation
rights and, equally importantly, the government has argued that both the United States Constitution
increased responsibilities to protect them. This ten- and federal immigration laws required that they
sion marks and shapes immigration law relating to be released into the custody of “responsible
juveniles, with the current tendency moving toward adults.” They also claimed that they posed no
the need to detain unaccompanied alien youth. risk of flight or harm to the community, and all
Immigration policies relating to minor adoles- had responsible third parties available to provide
cents have undergone important transformations for them. Given their circumstances, they asserted
since the 1980s. Before the mid-1980s, minors that the release conditions and their confinement
were customarily released on bond to their parents violated their due process and equal protection
or other responsible adults who would care for rights under the Constitution’s Fifth Amendment.
them and ensure their attendance at future depor- In a seven-to-two decision, the Supreme Court
tation proceedings. In 1984, a regional Immigra- rejected the detained juveniles’ argument that they
tion and Naturalization Service (“INS”) had a fundamental right to freedom from physical
commissioner instituted a new policy providing restraint and that their procedural due process
that minors only would be released to a parent or rights had been violated. To prevail in their chal-
lawful guardian, except in “unusual and extraor- lenge, the Court noted, the juveniles would need
dinary circumstances.” The justifications offered to establish that no set of circumstances existed
were the need to protect the INS from potential under which the regulation would be valid. They
legal liability and to assure the minor's welfare. were unable to do so. The Court held that the best
The new policy resulted in no longer releasing interest of the child was not an absolute and
children to anyone but a parent or legal guardian, exclusive constitutional criterion for the govern-
and the “unusual and extraordinary” exception ment’s exercise of the custodial responsibilities. If
was limited only to minors requiring medical a juvenile has no available parent or legal guard-
care. The INS refused to hold hearings to deter- ian, INS custody does not violate the Constitution
mine whether detention would be in a child’s best as long as the government has no intention of
interest and refused to provide procedures by punishing the child and the conditions of the gov-
which its initial, automatic detention orders ernment detention facility are decent and humane.
could be reviewed regularly. As a result, and Although the Court recognized that the Fifth
sometimes because parents feared deportation if Amendment entitles aliens to procedural due pro-
they claimed their children, minors were summar- cess of law in deportation proceedings, it con-
ily detained even though they posed no risk to the cluded that the requirements of due process were
community and whose attendance at deportation satisfied by giving the detained juveniles the right
hearings could be assured by responsible individ- to a hearing before an immigration judge. That
uals. The policy eventually became a formal INS hearing, as long as a waiver of it is revocable and
regulation, which required detention unless an there is no evidence of excessive delay in holding
adult relative or legal guardian was available to it, assured the alien's due process rights. The Court
assume custody, even where there was another also held that the Attorney General had the power
responsible adult willing and able to care for the to use discretion to continue custody over arrested
Developmental 963

aliens. The Attorney General had struck an appro-


priate balance between the INS’s concern that the Developmental
juveniles’ welfare will not permit their release to
just any adult and the INS’s assessment that it has Roger J. R. Levesque
neither the expertise nor the resources to conduct Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
individual child placement studies.
The concurrence and dissenting court opinions
gave more credence to the juveniles’ claims. The The term developmental refers to the process of
concurring opinion emphasized the view that the maturation and the changes that occur (or not) in D
minors had a constitutionally protected interest in an organization, a structure, or an individual over
freedom from institutional confinement. The dis- time. The term can be applied in one of two ways.
senting opinion characterized the right at issue as First, it can be applied in a temporal sense, which
the right not to be detained rather than the right of would involve descriptions of changes that take
detained juveniles to be released to one custodian place over an extended period of time. Second, it
over another. The dissenting opinion criticized the could also refer to the process of development itself,
incarceration of minor children as serving no such as the factors that affect one’s development.
legitimate governmental purpose, arguing that, For example, developmental science describes
as aliens, the detained minors sought the same changes that occur, and investigate the potential
equal justice before the law that is enjoyed by all causes of differences and similarities in various
persons present in the United States, including modes of development. It also describes the changes
procedural protections in conjunction with any that do not occur when they would have been
deprivation of liberty. The majority, however, expected. Thus, examining development means
was not persuaded by these arguments. focusing on both continuity and discontinuity.
The case is of significance, in addition to illus- The complex changes that occur during ado-
trating the workings of immigration law, for the lescence consist of biological changes, sexual
manner the legal system can approach the rights of development, social progression, identity
adolescents. The case reveals how the law can changes, cognitive changes, and physical growth
presume that minors should be held under the (see Gowers 2005). Although past research has
state’s control in the absence of a parent or other focused on psychological, physical, and social
legally recognized caretaker. The preference for development, recent research has moved toward
parents and other legally recognized caretakers is examining how the brain affects these domains.
not unusual; what is unusual is the preference that These multiple ways of looking at adolescent
a juvenile be detained in a governmental institu- development focus on, for example, the effects
tion over release to responsible adults. As noted of changes that can occur due to cognitive brain
above, this preference is particularly problematic structures or the lack of changes that can be
for youth given that they are more likely to be expected due to those structures (Steinberg
unaccompanied by parents or those deemed 2005). Importantly, despite the focus on neurolog-
legally responsible for their care. ical aspects of development, there remains a
strong recognition that the timing, length and
experience of adolescence highly depends on cul-
tural forces (Gowers 2005). Although there is a
Cross-References belief that adolescent research is dominated by
research in the United States, and that it presents
▶ Immigration a narrow view of adolescent experiences, even
researchers in the United States now focus con-
References siderably on cultural diversity and the leading
journals that publish research on adolescence are
Reno v. Flores. (1993). 507 U.S. 292. increasingly international (see Levesque 2007).
964 Developmental Assets

References Benson et al. (2003), the first focuses on develop-


mental opportunities, experiences, and resources
Gowers, S. (2005). Development in adolescence. Psychia- that enhance educational, socioemotional, and
try, 4, 6–9.
health outcomes for children and adolescents.
Levesque, R. J. R. (2007). The ethnicity of adolescent
research. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36, This conceptual territory of developmental
375–389. strengths is sometimes characterized as “the other
Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development side of the coin” to approaches that focus on
in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 69–74.
developmental risks, deficits, and problems. The
organizing principle in this latter approach is to
reduce threats to human development (e.g., pov-
erty, family dysfunction, victimization, neglect,
Developmental Assets abuse) and/or to reduce health-compromising
behaviors (e.g., alcohol and other drug use, vio-
Peter L. Benson1 and Peter C. Scales2 lence, adolescent pregnancy). In contrast, the
1
Search Institute, Minneapolis, MN, USA developmental strength approach is organized
2
Search Institute, Manchester, MO, USA around promoting or increasing concepts such as
competence, mastery, caring, connection, belong-
ing, skill, and resilience. Although both deficit
Overview reduction and strength building are necessary and
complementary human development strategies,
The concept of developmental assets, first the former approach has in recent decades domi-
introduced in 1990, links positive features of nated the national research and policy agenda
developmental ecologies with personal skills and (Scales 1999).
competencies in order to further the understand- Community frames a second line of inquiry
ing of healthy development. Each of the 40 devel- and action assuming a greater presence in the
opmental assets in the model is a characteristic of intellectual and policy limelight. Concepts such
the individual (internal assets) or his/her ecologi- as community development, community building,
cal settings (external assets) that is related to the and healthy communities have their origins in
increased probability of positive outcomes. urban and neighborhood renewal and the attention
Developmental assets complement the extensive social science has paid to neighborhood effects on
research on risk factors. The former represent juvenile delinquency and crime (see Leventhal and
“nutrients” that promote healthy development; Brooks-Gunn 2000). Increasingly, however, com-
the latter represent impediments that reduce the munities understand the contexts that shape posi-
probability of health and wellbeing. In this essay, tive adolescent development as well as an arena for
the authors explain how the developmental asset comprehensive, community initiatives that pro-
model was developed, review the extensive mote child and adolescent well-being (Benson
research that documents the impact of develop- 2006; Damon and Gregory 2003; Lerner 2003).
mental assets on both the reduction of risk behav- The framework of developmental assets, first
iors and the promotion of thriving (including posited in 1990 (Benson 1990) and refined in
school success), document subsets of assets 2005 (Benson 2006), was explicitly designed to
related to specific outcomes, review how the address these two issues. As described in a series
model applies cross-culturally, and discuss of publications (Benson 2002, 2003a; Benson
the use of the model in programs and policy. et al. 1998; Scales et al. 2000; Scales and Leffert
The essay concludes with a discussion of 1999, 2004), the framework establishes a set of
recommended next steps in research and practice. developmental experiences and supports hypoth-
Two interdisciplinary arenas of scholarship and esized to have import for all young people during
practice are gaining greater prominence in child the second decade of life. Recent work is taking a
and adolescent psychology. As described by broader life span perspective, positing that
Developmental Assets 965

developmental assets reflect developmental pro- Although each element in the asset taxonomy
cesses that have age-related parallels in infancy, is grounded in research, the framework and its
childhood, and young adulthood (Leffert et al. measurement and reporting processes have four
1997; Mannes et al. 2003; Scales et al. 2004, applied purposes (Benson 2007). First, the frame-
2010; VanderVen 2008). This work will be work seeks to provide an understandable vocabu-
addressed later in this essay. lary (for both lay and professional audiences) for
The framework synthesizes research in a num- core elements of positive human development,
ber of fields with the goal of selecting for inclusion with emphasis on developmental processes, expe-
those developmental nutrients that: (a) have been riences, and resources known to promote short- D
demonstrated to prevent high-risk behavior (e.g., term and long-term well-being. Second, it is
substance use, violence, dropping out of school), intended to create a unified picture of positive
enhance thriving, or strengthen resilience; (b) have development capable of uniting citizens and mul-
evidence of generalizability across social location; tiple socializing systems around a shared vision.
(c) contribute balance to the overall framework In this way, it is an attempt to create a common
(i.e., of ecological and individual-level factors); language that has the potential of contributing to a
(d) are within the capacity of communities to effect public consensus on what “our” children and ado-
their acquisition; and (e) are within the capacity of lescents need to succeed.
youth to proactively procure. Third, it seeks to empower and mobilize resi-
Because the developmental assets framework dents (both adults and youth), families, neighbor-
for adolescents ages 12–18 was designed not only hoods, youth organizations, religious institutions,
to inform theory and research, but also to have and other community sectors to take action.
practical significance for the mobilization of com- Finally, through a survey and reporting process
munities, the 40 assets are placed in categories that presents a portrait of developmental assets
that have conceptual integrity and can be among a community’s youth, the framework and
described easily to the residents of a community. its local measurement serve as a kind of call to
As seen in Tables 1 and 2, they are grouped into action to strengthen developmental processes and
20 external assets (i.e., environmental, contextual, experiences for all youth within a community, not
and relational features of socializing systems) and just “at-risk” or particularly vulnerable youth.
20 internal assets (i.e., skills, competencies, and The developmental asset framework is closely
values). The external assets comprise four catego- aligned with the field of positive youth develop-
ries: (a) support, (b) empowerment, (c) boundaries ment. As noted in a recent, systematic review of
and expectations, and (d) constructive use of time. the literature, the core concept in positive youth
The internal assets are also placed into four cate- development has to do with how the positive life
gories: (a) commitment to learning, (b) positive experiences that are embedded in relationships
values, (c) social competencies, and (d) positive and the culture of programs and institutions pro-
identity. The scientific foundations for the eight mote healthy development (Benson et al. 2006).
categories and each of the 40 assets are described A rich vocabulary has developed to describe these
in more detail in Scales and Leffert (1999, 2004). development-enhancing factors. Variously, those
An exploratory factor analysis conducted with have been called supports, protective factors, the
150,000 6th–12th grade students showed that 5C’s (caring, connection, etc.), opportunities,
14 scales emerged for middle-school students promises, developmental resources, developmen-
and 16 for high-school students, all conceptually tal strengths, and developmental assets.
reflecting the eight a priori asset categories; in Cutting across several models of positive youth
addition, a second-order factor analyses identified development (Lerner 2004; Roth and Brooks-
two major superordinate scales, labeled Individ- Gunn 2000; Pittman et al. 2000; Connell et al.
ual Assets and Ecological Assets, that mirrored 2001; Zeldin 1995) – including developmental
the a priori designation of assets into internal and assets – is a considerable consensus on six theoret-
external classes (Theokas et al. 2005). ical principles:
966 Developmental Assets

Developmental Assets, Table 1 The framework of developmental assets: external assets


Category External assets Definition
Support 1. Family support Family life provides high levels of love and support.
2. Positive family Young person and her or his parent(s) communicate positively, and young
Communication person is willing to seek advice and counsel from parents.
3. Other adult Young person receives support from three or more nonparent adults.
relationships
4. Caring Young person experiences caring neighbors.
neighborhood
5. Caring school School provides a caring, encouraging environment.
climate
6. Parent Parent(s) are actively involved in helping young person succeed in
involvement in school.
schooling
Empowerment 7. Community Young person perceives that adults in the community value youth.
values youth
8. Youth as resources Young people are given useful roles in the community.
9. Service to others Young person serves in the community 1 hour or more per week.
10. Safety Young person feels safe at home, school, and in the neighborhood.
Boundaries and 11. Family Family has clear rules and consequences and monitors the young person’s
expectations boundaries whereabouts.
12. School School provides clear rules and consequences.
boundaries
13. Neighborhood Neighbors take responsibility for monitoring young people’s behavior.
Boundaries
14. Adult role Parent(s) and other adults model positive, responsible behavior.
models
15. Positive peer Young person’s best friends model responsible behavior.
influence
16. High Both parent(s) and teachers encourage the young person to do well.
expectations
Constructive use 17. Creative Young person spends 3 or more hours per week in lessons or practice in
of time activities music, theater, or other arts.
18. Youth programs Young person spends 3 or more hours per week in sports, clubs, or
organizations at school and/or in community organizations.
19. Religious A young person spends 1 hour or more per week in activities in a religious
community institution.
20. Time at home Young person is out with friends “with nothing special to do” two or fewer
nights per week.
From Benson (2006), Copyright 2006 by John Wiley & Sons

1. All youth have the inherent capacity for posi- 4. All youth benefit from these relationships, con-
tive growth and development. texts, and ecologies. Support, empowerment,
2. A positive developmental trajectory is enabled and engagement are, for example, important
when youth are embedded in relationships, developmental assets for all youth, generaliz-
contexts, and ecologies that nurture their ing across race, ethnicity, gender, and family
development. income. However, the strategies and tactics for
3. The promotion of positive development is fur- promoting these developmental assets can vary
ther enabled when youth participate in multi- considerably as a function of social location.
ple, nutrient-rich relationships, contexts and 5. Community is a viable and critical “delivery
ecologies. system” for positive youth development.
Developmental Assets 967

Developmental Assets, Table 2 The framework of developmental assets: internal assets


Category Internal assets Definition
Commitment to 21. Achievement Young person is motivated to do well in school.
learning motivation
22. School engagement Young person is actively engaged in learning.
23. Homework Young person reports doing at least 1 hour of homework every school
day.
24. Bonding to school Young person cares about her or his school.
25. Reading for Young person reads for pleasure 3 or more hours per week. D
pleasure
Positive values 26. Caring Young person places high value on helping other people.
27. Equality and social Young person places high value on promoting equality and reducing
Justice hunger and poverty.
28. Integrity Young person acts on convictions and stands up for her or his beliefs.
29. Honesty Young person “tells the truth even when it is not easy.”
30. Responsibility Young person accepts and takes personal responsibility.
31. Restraint Young person believes it is important not to be sexually active or to use
alcohol or other drugs.
Social 32. Planning and Young person knows how to plan ahead and make choices.
competencies decision making
33. Interpersonal Young person has empathy, sensitivity, and friendship skills.
competence
34. Cultural Young person has knowledge of and comfort with people of different
competence cultural/racial/ethnic backgrounds.
35. Resistance skills Young person can resist negative peer pressure and dangerous
situations.
36. Peaceful conflict Young person seeks to resolve conflict nonviolently.
resolution
Positive identity 37. Personal power Young person feels he or she has control over “things that happen to
me.”
38. Self-esteem Young person reports having high self-esteem.
39. Sense of purpose Young person reports that “my life has a purpose.”
40. Positive view of Young person is optimistic about her or his personal future.
personal future
From Benson (2006), Copyright 2006 by John Wiley & Sons

6. Youth are major actors in their own develop- 156-item self-reporting instrument administered
ment and are significant (and underutilized) anonymously in classroom or program settings,
resources for creating the kinds of relation- the Search Institute Profiles of Student Life: Atti-
ships, contexts, ecologies, and communities tudes and Behavior survey (and a companion
that enable positive youth development. Search Institute survey for the upper elementary
grades called Me and My World). In the case of
schools and school districts, efforts usually are
Developmental Assets Among US Youth made to conduct a complete census of all sixth
through twelfth grade students. Completed survey
Studies of developmental assets have blossomed forms are returned to Search Institute for analyses
in a variety of venues, including universities, with a comprehensive report prepared and sent
national youth serving organizations, federally within weeks of the survey date.
funded initiatives, state departments of health More than 2,500 school districts in the USA
and education, county agencies, and public and have commissioned this study of developmental
private schools. The primary research tool is a assets. In recent years, these include a balance of
968 Developmental Assets

urban, suburban, and rural districts. Urban dis- approaches tend to bifurcate youth into two
tricts have included New York City, Seattle, Min- camps, with the developmental “have nots”
neapolis, Albuquerque, Portland, San Jose, and labeled as at risk, vulnerable, high risk, or mar-
Dallas. Since that time, more than three million ginalized. The asset approach, supported by thou-
students in grades 4–12 have participated. sands of community asset profiles, is to place
School districts typically commission the development on a continuum that runs from
study at the request of community leaders for “asset-depleted” on the one end (0–10 assets) to
the purposes of launching (or sustaining) a “asset-rich” on the other (31–40 assets). By show-
community-wide asset-building initiative. The ing communities that a majority of their 6th–12th
survey is primarily used as a means of communi- grade students experience half or fewer of the
cating aggregate data on a community’s youth. 40 assets, the report strategically and purposefully
The developmental asset report often becomes a creates a dissonance in the public’s understanding
widely shared document and is used both to frame of what the issue is. In more classic community
community-wide visioning and planning and to development verbiage, this is the process of
serve as a focal point to mobilize around raising expanding citizen ownership of and engagement
healthy youth. A dichotomous form of reporting in the issue of child and adolescent developments.
the assets, whereby each asset is simplified into a In language that resonates more with community
single percentage of youth who have, or do not residents, local studies shift the understanding
have, each asset, has become an effective method from “some kids need more” to “all kids need
for communicating the asset profile to diverse more.” This reframing to “all kids” is an essential
community audiences. This allows for a simple strategy for motivating multiple systems within
summation of the average number of assets youth communities to pool their developmental
in any given community report having. (Binary, resources in a coherent, long-term, multisystem,
dichotomous scoring is useful for simplifying and citizen-engaged initiative to promote devel-
public communications about the assets, but of opmental assets. It also has other significant
course fails then to use all of the information advantages. Given the complex and long-term
contained in the data. In its scholarly papers, the dimensions of community change – as well as
Institute also uses continuous versions of the asset the dearth of scientific knowledge about creating
variables for data analyses.) In addition to serving developmentally attentive communities – the
as a research tool, the developmental asset survey work connects hundreds of communities in a
and its reporting process are intended to trigger mutually supporting web of action and learning
community-wide dialogue, reflection, and plan- directed toward the comprehensive transforma-
ning. In essence, a local asset study reframes tion of community life. The “all kids” reframing,
how a community knows and understands its and the underlying societal dimensions it presup-
role in the development of children and adoles- poses, puts cities of many sizes and geographies
cents. The study of developmental assets at a local on a common playing field, seeking knowledge
level is intended to trigger several forms of and wisdom about similar issues (e.g., the expan-
reframing. One, of course, is the reframing of sion of civic engagement, the creation of devel-
how a community of people and systems under- opmentally attentive schools, the building of
stands the nature of successful development. The social trust within neighborhoods, and the recrea-
goal here is to help communities expand their tion of intergenerational community). Positioned
shared understanding of healthy development this way, communities across the country discover
to encompass not only “problem free,” but also a commonality of interest and an eagerness to
“asset-rich.” connect, learn, and teach. And rather than
Another reframing has to do with a deflecting energy away from marginalized youth,
community’s collective understanding of the pop- the “all kids” reframing reenergizes and
ulation of children and adolescents to be targeted strengthens a community’s work with high-risk
by community interventions. Deficit reduction youth.
Developmental Assets 969

Finally, embedded within the developmental A concept of “levels of assets” is introduced


asset framework and the community report is a into each community study. These are defined
comprehensive approach to the etiology of devel- simply as the total number of 40 assets broken
opmental strengths. The external assets directly into quartiles: 0–10, 11–20, 21–30, and 31–40.
speak to the role of multiple developmental ecol- Benson (2006) labeled these four as at-risk
ogies, including family, neighborhood, school, (0–10), vulnerable (11–20), adequate (21–30),
youth organizations, congregations/synagogues/ and optimal development (31–40).
mosques, and programs. The internal assets invite The percentages in each category by key
inquiry and conversation about the multiple demographic variables are displayed in Table 3. D
sources of competencies, values, identity, com- Note that 59% of youth possess 20 or fewer of the
mitment, and purpose, and the degree to which 40 developmental assets.
communities possess the harmony of voice and
deep and sustained relationships necessary for
these capacities to develop. As such, the taxon- Prediction and Explanation
omy of developmental assets provides counter-
weight to approaches that are overdependent on A number of studies have consistently shown that
programs and professionalized services for their the developmental assets are additive or cumula-
implementation. tive in explaining both risk behaviors and indica-
A number of research publications have been tors of thriving (e.g., school success, prosocial
based on analyses of three large aggregated data behavior, leadership). There are two versions of
sets (99,462 in school year 1996–1997, 217,277 this hypothesis. One is called the vertical pileup of
in 1999–2000 and 148,189 in calendar 2003). All assets and points to the idea that the number of
were based on samples from more than 200 cities assets at a single point in time is associated with
spread across all regions of the country. (These risk and thriving. The horizontal pile-up idea
samples were large and reasonably diverse, but implies contextual breadth. That is, outcomes
they are not “national” samples. They were cre- improve as a function of the number of asset-
ated through district self-selection, and not by rich contexts in which a person is embedded
drawing them from a probabilistic sampling (Benson et al. 2003). It also implies that assets
frame for the US youth population.) Based on experienced earlier in life positively affect out-
the 2003 sample, Benson (2006, 2009) provided comes later in life.
a demographic portrait of assets. Using the binary In their study of nearly 100,000 6th- to 12th-
form for each asset (so that an individual’s “score” grade students, Benson et al. (1999) reported that
ranges from 0 to 40), six major findings are: (1) the consistently, across outcome variables, the higher
average number of assets was 18.6; (2) females the number of assets students reported experienc-
average 19.9 developmental assets versus 17.2 for ing, the less likely they were to report engaging in
boys; (3) the average tends to decrease as grade a variety of patterns of high-risk behaviors. For
increases (from 23.0 in grade 6 to 17.8 in grade 12); example, 53% of young people who reported
(4) the means for major categories of race and 0–10 assets had used alcohol three or more times
ethnicity fall in a fairly narrow range, from 16.9 in the past month or had gotten drunk at least once
to 19.0; (5) youth in low-income families average in the last 2 weeks, but only 3% of students with
17.0 versus 19.8 in moderate-income or high- 31–40 assets reported such problems alcohol use.
income families; and (6) contrary to common pub- Even students with average levels of assets
lic perception, the average number of assets stays (11–20) were ten times more likely (30%) to
quite constant across city-size categories (from have problems with alcohol use than were asset-
18.2 in small towns with a population under rich students. The same profoundly different
2,500 to 18.6 in small cities with populations experiences with risk behaviors – with asset-rich
between 10,000 and 50,000; the average in larger students many times less likely to engage in the
cities of 250,000 or more is 18.2). behaviors than even average-asset youth, much
970 Developmental Assets

Developmental Assets, Table 3 Percentage of youth: levels of assets


Level of developmental assets (in %)
0–10 11–20 21–30 31–40
Assets Assets Assets Assets
At-risk Vulnerable Adequate Optimal
Total 17 42 32 8
Male 22 45 28 6
Female 13 40 36 11
Grades 6–8 15 38 36 12
Grades 9–12 19 45 30 6
American Indian 23 42 27 8
Asian and Pacific Islander 15 46 31 8
Black and African American 13 47 34 6
Hispanic 23 45 27 5
White 17 40 33 10
Multiracial 18 46 30 7
Low family income 22 45 28 5
Moderate or high family income 14 40 35 11
Based on aggregate sample of 148,189 6th–12th grade students surveyed in 2003 from more than 200 US communities.
Percentages may not sum to 100 due to rounding

less asset-poor youth – were reported for all the total regression models (with demographics)
other risk patterns, including tobacco use, illicit explained 66% of the variance of the risk index,
drug use, sexual intercourse, depression and/or and assets explained 57%. Across the risk index
attempted suicide, antisocial behavior, violence, and various risk behavior patterns, the develop-
school problems, driving and alcohol, and mental assets most often retained as meaningful
gambling. predictors were positive peer influence, the value
In a different study of the same sample, Leffert of restraint, peaceful conflict resolution, school
et al. (1998) reported that clusters of developmen- engagement, resistance skills, and time at home.
tal assets explained a considerable proportion of Achievement motivation was a unique predictor
the variance of those high-risk behavior patterns. of school problems, and a sense of purpose and
The 40 assets were allowed to enter stepwise self-esteem uniquely explained depression and/or
regressions as predictor variables after first enter- suicide attempts.
ing gender, grade, racial/ethnic background, fam- The concept of thriving encompasses not only
ily composition, and maternal education (as a the relative absence of pathology but also more
proxy for socioeconomic status). A predictor var- explicitly indicators of healthy and even optimal
iable was considered meaningful if it added at development (Benson 2008). There is some con-
least 1% to the variance of the risk behavior pat- ceptual similarity between the notion of develop-
terns. Although slightly different clusters of assets mental assets and that of thriving indicators, in
were meaningful in explaining different out- that both concepts focus on the presence of
comes, the total models (with demographic vari- strengths in young people’s lives. However, assets
ables) accounted for 21–41% of the variance, and are conceptualized as building blocks of success,
the assets contributed 16–35%. Developmental whereas thriving indicators are seen as signs or
assets were most meaningful in predicting depres- markers of success. In explanatory terms, devel-
sion and/or suicide attempts, school problems, opmental assets are considered predictors of the
violence, and alcohol use and had somewhat less outcomes represented by thriving indicators.
explanatory power for tobacco use, sexual inter- Experiencing the assets defines conditions under
course, and gambling. An overall risk index of which the attainment of those thriving outcomes is
24 different risk behaviors also was created. The made more likely.
Developmental Assets 971

Benson et al. (1999) reported that developmen- graders, they found that each increase in the quar-
tal assets showed similar patterns of relations with tile level of the asset domains studied (0–2, 3–5,
thriving indicators as with high-risk behavior pat- 6–8, or 9–12 assets) was associated with a signif-
terns. For all eight thriving indicators they stud- icant increase in the mean score on prosocial
ied, the proportion of students saying they orientation. They also found that, controlling for
experienced them increased with each increase grade in school, race/ethnicity, and parental edu-
in asset level. For example, only 7% of students cation, both boys and girls with above average
with 0–10 assets said they got mostly As at school, levels of prosocial orientation were nearly four
whereas 53% of those with 31–40 assets reported times more likely to report actual volunteer ser- D
such thriving. Although the patterns typically vice of at least 1 h per week in the past year.
were not as dramatic as the relation of assets to Sesma and Roehlkepartain (2003) demon-
risk behaviors, the trends were all the same. There strated that the pileup of assets finding generalizes
were substantial differences between higher- and to six racial ethnic groups: African American,
lower-asset youth on all of these thriving indica- American Indian, Asian American, Latino/Latina,
tors: school success, helping others, valuing White, and multiracial. In all six categories, the
diversity, maintaining physical health, exhibiting pileup of assets substantially reduces risk behav-
leadership, resisting danger, delaying gratifica- ior and increases thriving.
tion, and overcoming adversity. Benson et al. (2004a) examined the role of
Scales et al. (2000) allowed the 40 assets to developmental assets in substance abuse preven-
enter stepwise regressions after the demographic tion, examining the odds that students report sub-
variables had first been entered as a block and stance use if: (a) they live in a single-parent
retained as meaningful predictors those assets family; (b) they are of low socioeconomic status,
that explained at least 1% of the variance in a or (c) if they have relatively few developmental
thriving indicator. They reported that clusters of assets (0–10). The magnitude of the association
the assets explained 47–54% of a thriving index when comparing demographic “risk” factors with
(comprising all the indicators) across racial/ethnic asset depletion is substantial. For example, youth
groups and 10–43% of seven specific thriving from single-parent families are 1.3 times more
outcomes across racial/ethnic groups, over and likely than those from two-parent families to
above demographic variables. With demographic engage in problematic alcohol use. In comparison,
variables included, the total models explained young people with a low asset level (0–10 assets)
48–57% of the variance of the thriving index and are 5.5 times more likely to engage in problematic
11–48% of the individual thriving indicators. The alcohol use than those with an above-average
asset clusters best explained valuing diversity, asset level (21–40 assets). Similarly, though low-
school success, physical health, helping others, income youth are twice as likely as higher-income
leadership, and delay of gratification and, likely youth to use illicit drugs, low-asset youth are 9.4
due to measurement issues, were less strong an times more likely to use illicit drugs than those
explanation (10–17%) for overcoming adversity. with above-average asset levels. Similarly, Ben-
Similarly, Scales and Benson (2005) created a son and Scales (2009) found that every increase in
prosocial orientation measure by combining sev- quartile level of assets was associated with a lower
eral items tapping adolescents’ attitudes toward probability of 6th–12th grade youth engaging in
helping others, and several items asking about antisocial behavior or violence. For example, 61%
intentions to help those in need, working to of those with 0–10 assets reported engaging in
improve their school, or tutoring or coaching violent behaviors in the last year, versus just 6%
younger children over the next year. They then who had 31–40 assets. They also found that asset
examined the concurrent relation to prosocial ori- level in middle school helped to predict violence
entation and the number of developmental assets level 3 years later in high school, through its
adolescents reported. In a racially/ethnically relation to lower levels of concurrent violence.
diverse sample of more than 5,000 6th–12th Other lines of research have explored the
972 Developmental Assets

hypothesis that external and internal assets work Developmental Assets and
in harmony to promote developmental success. School Success
The strong relationship between asset levels and
both risk behaviors and thriving indicators has Scales and his colleagues (Scales and
been documented in large sample studies in Roehlkepartain 2003; Scales and Taccogna
major urban centers. These include New York 2000; Starkman et al. 2006; Scales and Benson
City, Seattle, Portland, Albuquerque and Minne- 2007) describe, based on many cross-sectional
apolis (Benson 2007). Scales et al. (2004) repli- studies of 6th–12th grade students, the robust
cated the “pileup” of assets having a positive and consistent relationship between levels of
relationship with developmental outcomes, with assets and self-reported school attendance and
a sample of 1,294 10–12 year olds. As with 12–18 grades. Similar patterns have been reported by
year olds, asset levels strongly predicted both risk school districts in Michigan and California that
and thriving. have conducted studies of the asset-to-
A longitudinal study in a small Midwestern achievement link (see Scales and Roehlkepartain
city in the USA suggests that levels of develop- 2003) for a review of these studies.
mental assets are strongly related to positive out- Scales et al. (2006) extended this line of
comes both concurrently and 3 years later; levels research to a longitudinal investigation using
of developmental assets strongly predict academic school records to measure achievement. In this
and behavioral measures embedded in students’ study, 370 students in the 7th–9th grades were
personal records; and developmental assets are followed for 3 years (through the 10th–12th
two to four times more powerful in predicting grades). The greater the number of developmental
academic achievement, risk behaviors, and thriv- assets students reported in grades 7–9, the higher
ing than are such demographic factors as race/ their actual GPA 3 years later. Increases in devel-
ethnicity and poverty (Roehlkepartain et al. opmental assets were significantly associated with
2003). This longitudinal study also shows, as increases in GPA. Furthermore, the effect size of
would be expected, that changes in a young per- assets on GPA was significantly larger than the
son’s developmental assets profile (with changes as effect size typically found for documented school
minimal as an increase or decrease in two or three reform efforts. As Walser (2006) noted in a review
assets) have significant (and even dramatic) impact of this work in the Harvard Education Letter
on markers of developmental success (Scales and (2006), “achievement is as much about student
Roehlkepartain 2003; Scales et al. 2006). A recent development as it is about rigor and curriculum”
line of inquiry spearheaded by Taylor and Lerner (p. 2).
provides supportive evidence that the link between
developmental assets and thriving documented in
many studies of youth can be seen also among The Study of Asset-Building
youth in particularly challenging social contexts Communities
such as those surrounding youth in urban gangs
(Taylor et al. 2002). Complementary to research on the role of devel-
Scales and Roehlkepartain (2004) introduced opmental assets has been a stream of work on
the idea of certain assets functioning as “gateway” conceptualizing how communities can be orga-
assets. Focusing on the asset of service to others, nized and mobilized to promote the assets. The
they suggest this action generates the experience dynamics and processes by which communities
of other assets (e.g., adult role models, youth as mobilize their developmental capacity constitute a
resources, personal power). In turn, service gen- relatively unexplored line of inquiry, both theo-
erates “positive outcomes, including school suc- retically and empirically (Blyth and Leffert 1995).
cess, because those service experiences are part of An initial framework for understanding the asset-
an overall web of assets that provide a strong building capacity of communities provides a set of
foundation for healthy development” (p. 26). core principles and strategies, grounded in the
Developmental Assets 973

literatures of community development, social for this level of community engagement is the
marketing, and organizational change (Benson creation of a normative culture in which all resi-
2003b). Among these are the principles of devel- dents are expected, by virtue of their membership
opmental redundancy (the exposure to asset- in the community, to promote the positive devel-
building people and environments within multiple opment of children and adolescents. For example,
contexts), developmental reach (a focus on nur- only 15% of young people are “rich” in the
turing most or all assets in children and adoles- 12 assets particularly linked to relationships with
cents), and developmental breadth (extending, by adults outside their own families, i.e., report
purpose and design, the reach of asset-building experiencing 9–12 of those assets; yet, a national D
energy to all children and adolescents). study of US adults found that those who experi-
In activating these core principles, five sources ence a normative sense of social expectation to get
of asset-building potential are hypothesized to involved with youth are more likely to consider
exist within all communities, each of which can that involvement important, and to be in networks
be marshaled by means of a multiplicity of com- of adults who connect positively with other peo-
munity mobilization strategies. These sources of ple’s kids (Scales et al. 2003).
potential asset-building influence include: While the developmental assets framework –
and the reframing principles on which it is based –
• Sustained relationships with adults, both is designed to create a readiness for new commu-
within and beyond family nity action, models of asset-rich developmental
• Peer group influence (when peers choose to ecologies are needed to give this energy focus
activate their asset-building capacity) and direction. Drawing on a range of sources,
• Socializing systems, including families, neigh- including reviews of system change research,
borhoods, schools, playgrounds, congregations, field studies, and interviews with practitioners, a
youth organizations, and places of employment series of publications paint conceptual pictures of
• Community-level social norms, ceremony, rit- asset-building contexts and concrete examples
ual, policy, and resource allocation of how the principles are applied in a wide variety
• Programs, including school-based and of settings. These include, for example, schools
community-based efforts to nurture and build (Starkman et al. 2006), neighborhoods (Saito et al.
skills and competencies 2000), congregations (Roehlkepartain 1998),
and whole communities (Benson 2006). Research
In brief, asset-building communities are on strategies for creating and sustaining asset-
distinguished as relational and intergenerational building community initiatives are reviewed
ecologies, with a critical mass of citizens and in Benson et al. (2006) and Enfield and
socializing institutions (e.g., families, schools, Owens (2009).
neighborhoods, youth organizations, religious
communities) choosing to attend to the develop-
mental needs of all children and adolescents. Lifespan Extensions
Developmental assets become a language of the
common good, uniting sectors, citizens, and pol- The developmental assets framework originated
icy in the pursuit of shared goals for all children as an approach for understanding and promoting
and adolescents. The commitment of a commu- positive development in adolescence, or roughly
nity and its people, institutions, and organizations grades 6–12. Theoretical and empirical work also
is both long term and inclusive. have been conducted to extend the framework, in
Ultimately, rebuilding and strengthening the developmentally appropriate accents, to younger
developmental infrastructure in a community are children from birth-grade 5, as well as to young
conceived less as a program implemented and adults, roughly ages 18–25.
managed by professionals and more as a mobili- The initial formulation of developmental assets
zation of public will and capacity. A major target for young children was published in 1997 (Leffert
974 Developmental Assets

et al. 1997), and was a straightforward extension small number of assets, and a reformulation of the
of the concepts from adolescence to younger chil- Institute’s Developmental Assets Profile (see
dren from birth onward. For the first time, the below) for parents of younger children has been
conceptual utility of the framework throughout drafted. But this effort is at a very early stage of
the first two decades of life could be seen, as development, and so the measurement of the
versions of the 40 assets were articulated for developmental assets in early childhood remains
infants and toddlers (ages 0–2), preschoolers an area needing considerable work.
(3–5), elementary-age children (6–11), and ado- An extensive review of more than 600 studies
lescents (12–18). This work grew out of asking confirms that the developmental assets concepts
the question, what are the earlier roots of the originally developed to describe adolescence are
40 assets in adolescence? An extensive review of well-suited to describing healthy development in
the literature, and the insights of both local and middle childhood (Scales et al. 2004). Middle
national practitioners and researchers, contributed childhood is defined mostly as grades 4–5, with
to the development of these frameworks for youn- a swing year of grade 6 for many children. It is
ger children. during these years that children approach the cusp
More recently, Search Institute focused on of early adolescence. In many respects, children
elaborating the conceptualization of the assets have one foot firmly planted in childhood, and
framework in early childhood, undertaking an with the other are gingerly reaching toward the
even more extensive review and consultation ini- vastly different world of adolescence.
tiative around the first decade of life. The resulting As a result, there is both similarity and differ-
Early Childhood Developmental Assets Frame- ence between the assets frameworks for adoles-
work is thus grounded in the original assets frame- cence and middle childhood. There is great
work for adolescents, but also well aligned with continuity, not only in the underlying structure
influential scientific, theoretical, and practice- of the assets (e.g., the family-support asset con-
oriented approaches such as the National notes high levels of love and support, regardless
Research Council’s From Neurons to Neighbor- of developmental level), but also in how the assets
hoods report, Brazleton and Greenspan’s seminal are operationalized (e.g., one of the criteria for
Irreducible Needs of Children, and the Head Start family support is how well the child and parent
program’s outcomes framework. In accenting the get along).
areas that are most developmentally important for Beyond these simple continuities, however, lie
early childhood, the framework notes that the a number of differences corresponding to the
pairing of support and social competencies, and varying capacities and skills of children in those
constructive use of time and commitment to learn- developmental periods. For example, sense of
ing, are “primary” assets – conceptually fusing purpose is a developmental asset for both middle
these pairs of internal and external asset childhood and adolescence, but younger children
categories – that are “fundamental in develop- are not expected to have as much insight into the
ment” (VanderVen 2008, p. 159). purpose and meaning of their lives as adolescents.
The theoretical, conceptual, and practice-based Another example is “school” engagement in the
contributions of the early childhood assets frame- adolescent framework. In the middle childhood
work are considerable. However, the Institute has framework, this asset is called “learning” engage-
yet to embark on a major program of measurement ment to emphasize the importance of children at
of the assets in early childhood, in large part this age both being engaged at school and devel-
because the rather lengthy self-report survey oping an interest in learning regardless of where
needed to study assets in adolescence is less fea- that learning occurs.
sible to use with younger children. A Search Insti- Both the more than 600 middle childhood stud-
tute collaboration with the YMCA of the USA has ies reviewed in Scales et al. (2004) and data from
enabled initial development and testing of a brief the Search Institute assets survey for 4th–6th
survey of parents of children in grades K-5 on a graders, Me and My World, strongly suggest that
Developmental Assets 975

the same relation of assets to outcomes exten- 25 in order to enjoy current well-being and suc-
sively documented for adolescents pertain to cessful transition to later life.
children in middle childhood. The more develop- As early as 1997, Benson (1997, 2006) artic-
mental assets they experience, the better their ulated an initial description of adult assets. In
odds of not just “doing okay” in their present 2004, in a report to the Bill and Melinda Gates
and future but of thriving, of being the very best Foundation, Search Institute and the Social
unique persons they can be. Development Research Group at the University
For example, in a sample of nearly 5,000 of Washington collaborated to make more
4th–6th graders from the 2003–2004 school explicit a statement of successful development D
year, one analysis found that asset-depleted chil- in the next developmental period beyond adoles-
dren (0–10 assets) are much more likely than cence, that of emerging or young adulthood
children with even as few as 11–20 assets to (Benson et al. 2004b).
engage in antisocial behavior (22% vs 8%), and Subsequent to the Gates Foundation project,
children with 11–20 assets are more likely than from 2004 to 2006, Search Institute worked with
asset-rich children to engage in this risk pattern Child Trends and the America’s Promise Alliance
(8% versus 2%). Similarly, 72% of asset-rich to articulate and develop measurement tools to
4th–6th graders say they have good coping skills, assess whether America’s children and youth
compared with 47% of children with 21–30 were experiencing five “promises” (based on the
assets, 25% of students with only 11–20 assets, developmental assets framework) that are known
and just 16% of asset-depleted students (Scales to be linked to positive developmental outcomes
et al. 2005). Thus, the developmental assets (the promises are: caring adults, safe places and
framework seems validly to describe a broad constructive use of time, a healthy start, effective
array of relationships, opportunities, values, and education, and opportunities to make a difference;
skills that promote a healthy middle childhood see Scales et al. 2008; America’s Promise
and increase the odds of a successful adolescence. Alliance 2007). Although the eventual measure-
Search Institute is now expanding the territory ment focused on school-age children and youth,
of developmental assets into the arena of young the authors generated indicators of positive devel-
adulthood. Different authors use different terms – opmental experiences for each of the age group-
usually either emerging adult or young adult – for ings of 0–5, 6–11, 12–17, and, most relevant for
the same period of young adulthood, which is also this discussion, ages 18–21. That conceptual work
variously defined in terms of age range, but which informed the evolving framework of assets for
may be defined as approximately ages 18–25. An young adults.
extensive research review conducted for the Gates Similarly, from 2006 to 2008, under a grant
Foundation (Benson et al. 2004b) amply demon- from the John Templeton Foundation, Institute
strates that although the transition to new roles researchers collaborated with a global group of
characterizes the essence of young adulthood, the more than 100 advisors and research partners in
social and psychological processes at work are North America, South America, Europe, Africa,
much the same as in the first two decades of life. Asia, and Oceania to develop a new theory of
Other efforts have addressed pathways to success- spiritual development among childhood and
ful young adult development, and define what youth, and to begin to measure salient dimensions
successful young adulthood entails (Schorr and of spiritual development with new measurement
Marchand 2007), but are essentially about what tools (Roehlkepartain et al. 2008). Because of the
adolescents need subsequently to have a success- global nature of the work, and the tendency for
ful young adulthood, much as the assets frame- much of the world outside the USA to define
work is about what adolescents need while they “youth” as including older ages than is common
are adolescents. The new assets framework for in US definitions, the focus spanned the ages from
young adulthood, however, describes what about 12–25. Much of this work around theory
young adults need during those years from 18 to and measures of spiritual development involved a
976 Developmental Assets

focus on naming the positive developmental expe- survey that was more sensitive to detecting
riences of emerging or young adults. changes over time, and of a high enough psycho-
The result of all this conceptual and empirical metric quality to be used at an individual level.
work is an initial description of the developmental The DAP also is much briefer than the A&B
assets framework for young adults (Scales et al. survey. The trade-off for the value-adds of the
2010). Most of the asset names used for adoles- DAP is that it does not provide scores for each
cents can validly be used as well in the young of the 40 individual assets, but rather for the eight
adult period, although the definition must change asset categories (Support, Positive Values, etc.).
somewhat to reflect developmental realities. An added feature, however, is that it also provides
Some asset names must change. For example, a scores by environmental context, so it can be
number of assets refer to the context of the school, readily seen how youth are experiencing assets
which for young adults must broaden to include in the family, school, community, social, and per-
not only post-secondary school settings, but sonal contexts (details in Search Institute 2005).
workplace settings and the military as well. Dif- The DAP has been used in more than
ferent young adults will of course have differing 200 research and program evaluation applications
constellations of relationships too, with some since 2005. Most have been with relatively small
being married, some being parents, some with samples of less than 100, and often fewer than
partners and some not, etc., but the conceptuali- 50 youth. However, several dozen studies have
zation and measurement of assets in this period been done with sample sizes of at least 400 for
must be broad enough to adequately reflect that one-time cross-sectional surveys, and at least
diversity of life trajectories. In collaboration with 200 for pre/post-program evaluation studies. In
students and faculty at several universities, Search 2010, the Institute began collecting the databases
Institute began pilot testing of a new survey of from those relatively large studies, as a prelude to
assets for young adulthood in 2010, with the conducting a meta-analysis of DAP research that
intention to have, within several years, psycho- has occurred over the survey’s first 5 years.
metrically sound surveys that can be used with A particularly intriguing study was conducted
young adults in college, workplace/community, in Binghamton, New York. Selected DAP items
and military settings. were regrouped into prosociality, social support,
and neighborhood social capital scales, and scores
linked to spatial mapping of middle- and high-
The Developmental Assets Profile (DAP) school students’ (N = 1,840) residential locations
and other Census data (Wilson et al. 2009).
The Search Institute Profiles of Student Life: Atti- Results showed that individual prosociality scores
tudes and Behavior survey (the A&B survey), were strongly linked to social support, and that it
along with its other surveys, has been adminis- took multiple sources of social support (e.g.,
tered to more than three million young people in extracurricular activities, neighborhood, school,
thousands of communities over the last 20 years. religion) to incrementally add up to explaining a
The strength of the A&B survey is providing an substantial percentage (52%) of the variance in
aggregate portrait of the 40 developmental assets prosociality. Moreover, students’ perceptions of
experienced by young people, with the data cata- their neighborhood quality, based on DAP items
lyzing and mobilizing communities into taking such as “I have neighbors who help watch out for
action to promote positive youth development. me,” were strongly linked to whether pedestrians
However, it was not designed for assessing picked up a planted, “lost letter” and mailed it,
change over time at either the aggregate or indi- impressive evidence of the validity of their DAP
vidual level. responses. In turn, the students’ DAP perceptions
The Developmental Assets Profile (DAP) was of neighborhood quality were also confirmed by
designed in 2005 to capture the developmental crime statistics, school delinquency notices, and
experience of young people in grades 6–12 in a even the extent of holiday decorations observed in
Developmental Assets 977

the neighborhoods between Halloween and and whether youth in the experimental programs
Christmas. The DAP measures of neighborhood enjoy greater improvements than youth in the
quality also were more important than neighbor- comparison communities. Initial results are
hood median income in predicting prosociality. expected in 2012.
Among the program evaluation studies, the Perhaps the most significant DAP-related
DAP was used as one of the tools in a study of development of the last few years is the explo-
the Gates-funded Community Access to Technol- sive growth in use of the survey internationally.
ogy program. Ten organizations and nearly Developmental asset surveys have been used in a
900 youth aged 10–21 in Washington state partic- number of other countries for many years, par- D
ipated. The researchers found that participation in ticularly in Canada and Australia, but since the
the program – which increases access to digital introduction of the DAP, it has become the most
technology through after-school programs – was common developmental assets survey used glob-
linked to significant gains in developmental ally, with users now in nearly 60 countries world-
assets: 62% of those in the “low” asset level of wide. Among other languages, the DAP has
DAP scores moved to either the “fair” or “good” been, or is being translated, into Albanian, Ara-
levels after the program, and one-third moved bic, Armenian, Chinese, French, Japanese,
from the fair or good levels to “excellent” (MGS Nepali, Portuguese, Spanish, and Tagalog. An
Consulting 2008). extensive and well-documented local language
One of the most ambitious DAP-related pro- versioning process carried out in the Philippines
jects is being undertaken by Search Institute and in a collaboration between Search Institute and
Vision Training Associates in collaboration with Education Development Center (EDC) provided
the RAND Corporation, the University of South- the model for this process (James-Wilson and
ern Maine, and the Communities for Children and Sesma 2007).
Youth initiative of the Governor’s Children’s Cab- Working with a variety of local experts and
inet. Assets-Getting-to-Outcomes for Maine practitioners, as well as with colleagues from
(AGTO4ME) is a 5-year project funded by the international consultation and relief organizations
National Institute of Drug Abuse, using a quasi- such as EDC, World Vision International
experimental design to assess the success of a (Albania, Armenia, Mexico), and Save the Chil-
youth program improvement model (http:// dren (Bangladesh, Egypt, Gaza, Lebanon,
maineassets.org, see also National Institute on Morocco, Yemen), Search Institute provides train-
Drug Abuse 2008). Twelve community coalitions ing on the developmental assets framework,
comprising scores of organizations serving Maine administration and scoring of the DAP, and assists
youth are participating. Six were randomly the indigenous teams to ensure that translated
assigned to be trained in the developmental assets versions retain fidelity to the original English
framework, and the AGTO process (Fisher et al. intent of the questions, while still being modified
2006). AGTO is a ten-step process that blends enough to be culturally meaningful.
asset building as an improvement strategy, with An intensive local language versioning process
critical elements of program planning, implemen- of training, consultation, and pilot and field testing
tation, and outcome measurement. Those coali- carried out over 4 months in Albania is illustra-
tions are being compared with six community tive. A field test sample of 259 youth in grades
coalitions not receiving the assets and AGTO 4–12 took the translated DAP in 2009. Most of the
training and technical assistance. Data are being scales achieved acceptable reliability, both inter-
collected from organizations’ staff on how the nal consistency and stability, but some did not,
steps in the AGTO process are being carried out, showing the need for further revisions before they
and from youth participants in the 12 coalitions’ could be used for program evaluation and
programs, using the DAP to determine whether improvement purposes. About 40% of the
changes in the youths’ experiences of develop- 58 items required several rounds of translation-
mental assets can be linked to program activities, back translation-revision to capture the English
978 Developmental Assets

meanings adequately, and a number of different resonates to local experts as a uniquely useful
administration strategies had to be tested. Nev- tool for stimulating positive youth development
ertheless, the majority of the scales did reach efforts around the world.
acceptable levels of comprehension, reliability,
and validity (through moderate to high correla-
tions with validation measures) that those could Conclusion
be recommended for use in Albania (Scales
2009). In the little more than 20 years since its introduc-
The use of the DAP in Bangladesh is also tion, the developmental assets approach has
provocative. It is the most rigorous evaluation become acknowledged as one of the most wide-
design Save the Children staff have observed. It spread and influential frameworks for understand-
involves a Random Control Trial (RCT) based on ing and strengthening positive youth development
a village randomization approach, with pre- and (Eccles and Gootman 2002; Small and Memmo
post-tests done 6-months apart, and includes a 2004). Google Scholar shows that the develop-
panel data set of 11,000 girls, four different mental assets approach and/or Search Institute has
types of interventions, and measures for contam- been referenced in more than 17,000 peer-
ination and spill-over affect for the control group reviewed journal articles. In 2009 alone, more
of girls, as well as contamination and level of than 10,000 schools and youth programs were
involvement measurement for the intervention using Search Institute resources, and in the last
girls (email from Larry Dershem to Peter Scales, 15 years, more than 20 million of the Institute’s
October 6, 2009). In another case, an Arabic ver- books and other resources have been disseminated
sion of the DAP was used in Yemen by Save the worldwide. In the last decade, more than 300,000
Children as part of the evaluation of a youth leaders in education, health, social services, reli-
empowerment project funded by USAID. About gion, youth development, and other fields have
600 youth ages 12–24 were surveyed. Among been trained in the assets framework, and more
many interesting findings, older Yemeni youth than five million people have visited the Institute’s
aged 19–24 tended to have lower DAP scores web site. More than 60 countries across the globe
than adolescents ages 12–18 (Bakeer et al. are actually using the assets approach, and people
2009). This corresponds to what Search Institute from three times that number of countries have
has found in younger US samples, both cross- been exposed to these ideas through visiting the
sectional as was the Yemen study, and longitudi- Search Institute web site. The expansion of the
nal, when one follows the same group of youth assets framework to younger children and young
over several years: Middle-school youth tend to adults and emergence of new surveys such as the
have more assets than high-school youth, DAP seem likely to fuel a continued increase in
suggesting the need for even more intentional both scholarship around the assets and the grow-
asset-building efforts for older youth. This sug- ing application of this strength-based approach to
gests that, despite the substantial cultural differ- policies, programs, and practice intended to ben-
ences between Yemen and the US, the DAP efit young people around the world.
survey has validity in both settings.
These examples confirm that, although the
technical challenges to cross-cultural use of the References
DAP are not insignificant, the mounting experi-
ence Search Institute has gained with its global America’s Promise Alliance. (2007). Every child, every
partners makes it clear that the challenges can be promise. Alexandria: America’s Promise Alliance.
met. The content of the survey is seen as broadly Bakeer, S. B., Maas, L., Sabbarini, L., Conklin, S., &
Dershem, L. (2009). Measuring impact of young peo-
relevant to positive youth development, it can be ple’s empowerment in Yemen – Setting a baseline.
successfully adapted with acceptable scientific Istanbul conference presentation on Save the Children,
strength in differing cultural contexts, and it Aden.
Developmental Assets 979

Benson, P. L. (1990). The troubled journey: A portrait of Benson, P. L., Scales, P. C., Hamilton, S. F., & Sesma,
6th–12th grade youth. Minneapolis: Search Institute. A. (2006). Positive youth development: Theory,
Benson, P. L. (1997). All kids are our kids: What commu- research, and applications. In W. Damon & R. M. Ler-
nities must do to raise caring and responsible children ner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed.,
and adolescents. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. pp. 894–941). New York: Wiley.
Benson, P. L. (2002). Adolescent development in social Blyth, D. A., & Leffert, N. (1995). Communities as con-
and community context: A program of research. New texts for adolescent development. Journal of Adoles-
Directions for Youth Development, 95, 123–147. cent Research, 10, 64–87.
Benson, P. L. (2003a). Developmental assets and asset Connell, J. P., Gambone, M. A., & Smith, T. J. (2001).
building communities: Conceptual and empirical Youth development in community settings: Challenges
foundations. In R. M. Lerner & P. L. Benson (Eds.), to our field and our approach. In P. L. Benson & K. J. D
Developmental assets and asset-building communities: Pittman (Eds.), Trends in youth development: Visions,
Implications for research, policy, and practice realities, and challenges (pp. 291–307). Boston:
(pp. 19–43). Norwell: Kluwer. Kluwer.
Benson, P. L. (2003b). Toward asset-building commu- Damon, W., & Gregory, A. (2003). Bringing in a new era in
nities: How does change occur? In R. M. Lerner & the field of youth development. In R. M. Lerner & P. L.
P. L. Benson (Eds.), Developmental assets and Benson (Eds.), Developmental assets and asset-
asset-building communities: Implications for building communities: Implications for research, pol-
research, policy, and practice (pp. 213–221). Norwell: icy, and practice (pp. 47–64). New York: Kluwer/
Kluwer. Plenum.
Benson, P. L. (2006). All kids are our kids: What commu- Eccles, J., & Gootman, J. A. (2002). Community programs
nities must do to raise caring and responsible children to promote youth development. Washington, DC:
and adolescents (2nd ed.). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. National Research Council and Institute of Medicine,
Benson, P. L. (2007). Developmental assets: An overview Committee on Community-Level Programs for Youth,
of theory, research, and practice. In R. K. Silbereisen & National Academy Press.
R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Approaches to positive youth Enfield, R., & Owens, P. E. (2009). Building and sustaining
development (pp. 33–58). Thousand Oaks: Sage. community-based youth development collaboratives.
Benson, P. L. (2008). Sparks: How parents can help ignite Community Development, 40(4), 381–397.
the hidden strengths of teenagers. San Francisco: Fisher, D., Imm, P., Chinman, M., & Wandersman,
Jossey-Bass. A. (2006). Getting to outcomes with developmental
Benson, P. L., & Scales, P. C. (2009). Positive youth assets: Ten steps to measuring success in youth pro-
development and the prevention of youth aggression grams and communities. Minneapolis: Search Institute.
and violence. European Journal of Developmental Sci- James-Wilson, D., & Sesma, A. (2007). Pilot testing of
ence, 3, 218–234. search institute’s 40 developmental assets framework
Benson, P. L., Leffert, N., Scales, P. C., & Blyth, D. A. and dap tools in the philippines – Final report. Minne-
(1998). Beyond the ‘village’ rhetoric: Creating healthy apolis: Search Institute.
communities for children and adolescents. Applied Leffert, N., Benson, P. L., & Roehlkepartain, J. L. (1997).
Developmental Science, 2(3), 138–159. Starting out right: Developmental assets for children.
Benson, P. L., Scales, P. C., Leffert, N., & Roehlkepartain, Minneapolis: Search Institute.
E. C. (1999). A fragile foundation: The state of devel- Leffert, N., Benson, P. L., Scales, P. C., Sharma, A. R.,
opmental assets among American youth. Minneapolis: Drake, D. R., & Blyth, D. A. (1998). Developmental
Search Institute. assets: Measurement and prediction of risk behaviors
Benson, P. L., Scales, P. C., & Mannes, M. (2003). Devel- among adolescents. Applied Developmental Science,
opmental strengths and their sources: Implications for 2(4), 209–230.
the study and practice of community building. In R. M. Lerner, R. M. (2003). Developmental assets and asset-
Lerner, F. Jacobs, & D. Wertlieb (Eds.), Handbook of building communities: A view of the issues. In R. M.
applied developmental science (Applying Develop- Lerner & P. L. Benson (Eds.), Developmental assets
mental Science for Youth and Families, Vol. 1, and asset-building communities: Implications for
pp. 369–406). Thousand Oaks: Sage. research, policy, and practice (pp. 3–18). New York:
Benson, P. L., Roehlkepartain, E. C., & Sesma, A., Kluwer/Plenum.
Jr. (2004a). Tapping the power of community: The Lerner, R. M. (2004). Liberty: Thriving and civic engage-
potential of asset building to strengthen substance ment among America’s youth. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
abuse prevention efforts. Search Institute Insights & Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). The neighbor-
Evidence, 2(1), 1–14. hoods they live in: The effects of neighborhood resi-
Benson, P. L., Scales, P. C., Hawkins, J. D., Oesterle, S., & dence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychological
Hill, K. G. (2004b). Successful young adult develop- Bulletin, 126, 309–337.
ment. A report submitted to The Bill & Melinda Gates Mannes, M., Benson, P. L., Kretzmann, J., & Norris,
Foundation. Retrieved 19 Jan 2010, from http://www. T. (2003). The American tradition of community devel-
search-institute.org/system/files/GatesFdnReport–Emer opment: Implications for guiding community engage-
gingAdulthood2004.pdf. ment in youth development. In R. M. Lerner, F. Jacobs,
980 Developmental Assets

& D. Wertlieb (Eds.), Handbook of applied develop- Scales, P. C., & Leffert, N. (2004). Developmental assets:
mental science: Promoting positive child, adolescent, A synthesis of the scientific research on adolescent
and family development through research, policies and development (2nd ed.). Minneapolis: Search Institute.
programs. Applying developmental science for youth Scales, P. C., & Roehlkepartain, E. C. (2003). Boosting
and families: Historical and theoretical foundations student achievement: New research on the power of
(Vol. 1, pp. 469–499). Thousand Oaks: Sage. developmental assets. Search Institute Insights & Evi-
MGS Consulting. (2008). Community access to technology dence, 1(1), 1–10.
program evaluation report – Year 3. Seattle: MGS Scales, P. C., & Taccogna, J. (2000). Caring to try: How
Consulting. building students’ developmental assets can promote
National Institute on Drug Abuse. (2008). Enhancing pre- school engagement and success. NASSP Bulletin,
vention capacity with developmental assets and getting 84(619), 69–78.
to outcomes (1R01DA023277-01A2) (M. Chinman, Scales, P. C., Benson, P. L., Leffert, N., & Blyth, D. A.
Principal Investigator). Bethesda: National Institute (2000). Contribution of developmental assets to the
on Drug Abuse, 6/1/08–3/31/13. prediction of thriving among adolescents. Applied
Pittman, K. J., Irby, M., & Ferber, T. (2000). Unfinished Developmental Science, 4(1), 27–46.
business: Further reflections on a decade of promoting Scales, P. L., Benson, P. L., Mannes, M., Hintz, N. R.,
youth development. In Public/Private Ventures (Ed.), Roehlkepartain, E. C., & Sullivan, T. K. (2003).
Youth development: Issues, challenges and directions Other people’s kids: Social expectations and American
(pp. 17–64). Philadelphia: Public/Private Ventures. adults’ involvement with children and adolescents.
Roehlkepartain, E. C. (1998). Building assets in congrega- New York: Kluwer/Plenum.
tions: A practical guide for helping youth grow up Scales, P. C., & Roehlkepartain, E. C. (2004a). Service to
healthy. Minneapolis: Search Institute. others: A gateway asset for school success and healthy
Roehlkepartain, E. C., Benson, P. L., & Sesma, A. (2003). development. In National Youth Leadership Council
Signs of progress in putting children first: Developmen- (Ed.), Growing to greatness, The state of service-
tal assets among youth in St. Louis Park, 1997–2001. learning project (pp. 26–32). St. Paul: National Youth
Minneapolis: Unpublished report prepared by Search Leadership Council.
Institute for St. Louis Park’s Children First Initiative. Scales, P. C., Sesma, A., & Bolstrom, B. (2004b). Coming
Roehlkepartain, E. C., Benson, P. L., Scales, P. C., into their own: How developmental assets promote
Kimball, L., & King, P. E. (2008). With their own positive growth in middle childhood. Minneapolis:
voices: A global exploration of how today’s young Search Institute.
people experience and think about spiritual develop- Scales, P. C., Sesma, A., Bolstrom, B., & Roehlkepartain,
ment. Minneapolis: Center for Spiritual Development E. C. (2005). Between childhood and adolescence:
in Childhood and Adolescence, Search Institute. Insights into promoting healthy development in middle
Roth, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). What do adolescents childhood. Minneapolis: Search Institute. Unpublished
need for healthy development: Implications for youth paper.
policy. Social Policy Report, 14(1), 3–19. Scales, P. C., Benson, P. L., Roehlkepartain, E. C., Sesma,
Saito, R. N., Sullivan, T. K., & Hintz, N. R. (2000). The A., & van Dulmen, M. (2006). The role of develop-
possible dream: What families in distressed communi- mental assets in predicting academic achievement:
ties need to help youth thrive. Minneapolis: Search A longitudinal study. Journal of Adolescence, 29,
Institute. 691–708.
Scales, P. C. (1999). Reducing risks and building develop- Scales, P. C., Benson, P. L., Moore, K. A., Lippman, L.,
mental assets: Essential actions for promoting adoles- Brown, B., & Zaff, J. F. (2008). Promoting equal devel-
cent health. Journal of School Health, 69(3), 113–119. opmental opportunity and outcomes among America’s
Scales, P. C. (2009). Developmental assets among Alba- children and youth: Results from the National Promises
nian youth: Field test results from the Search Institute Study. Journal of Primary Prevention, 29(2), 121–144.
Developmental Assets Profile – Report to the Albania Scales, P. C., Roehlkepartain, E. C., & Benson, P. L.
LIFE Project and World Vision International. Minne- (2010). Beyond adolescence: A framework of develop-
apolis: Search Institute. mental assets for young adulthood. Insights & Evi-
Scales, P. C., & Benson, P. L. (2005). Prosocial orientation dence. Minneapolis: Search Institute.
and community service. In K. A. Moore & L. Lippman Schorr, L. & Marchand, V. (2007). Pathway to successful
(Eds.), What do children need to flourish? Conceptual- young adulthood (Pathways Mapping Initiative).
izing and measuring indicators of positive development Washington, DC: Center for the Study of Social Policy.
(pp. 339–356). New York: Kluwer/Plenum. Retrieved 19 Jan 2010, from http://www.cssp.org/
Scales, P. C., & Benson, P. L. (2007). Building developmen- uploadFiles/Youth%20Pathway%20PDF%209-07.pdf.
tal assets to encourage students’ school success Texas Search Institute. (2005). Developmental assets profile:
Instructional Leader (Texas Elementary Principals and User manual. Minneapolis: Search Institute.
Supervisors Association), 20(3), 1–3, 8–10, 12. Sesma, A., Jr., & Roehlkepartain, E. C. (2003). Unique
Scales, P. C., & Leffert, N. (1999). Developmental assets: strengths, shared strengths: Developmental assets
A synthesis of the scientific research on adolescent among youth of color. Search Institute Insights & Evi-
development. Minneapolis: Search Institute. dence, 1(2), 1–13.
Developmental Contextualism 981

Small, S., & Memmo, M. (2004). Contemporary models of development cannot be examined independent of
youth development and problem prevention: Toward the people and places that comprise an adoles-
an integration of terms, concepts, and models. Family
Relations, 53, 3–11. cent’s life space (i.e., contexts). Key contexts for
Starkman, N. A., Scales, P. C., & Roberts, C. R. (2006). adolescents include family, peers, school, and
Great places to learn: How asset-building schools help community (Hill 1983). Moreover, there is a
students succeed (2nd ed.). Minneapolis: Search reciprocal relationship between adolescents and
Institute.
Taylor, C. S., Lerner, R. M., von Eye, A., Bobek, D. L., these contexts such that not only do contexts and
Balsano, A. B., Dowling, E., et al. (2002). Stability of the individuals comprising them influence adoles-
attributes of positive functioning and of developmental cents, but adolescents also shape the contexts D
assets among African American adolescent gang mem- (Lerner 1991, 1995). The interplay between per-
bers and community-based organization members. New
Directions for Youth Development, 95, 35–55. son and context is dynamic and developmental.
Theokas, C., Almerigi, J., Lerner, R. M., Dowling, E., This means that families, peers, schools, and com-
Benson, P., Scales, P. C., et al. (2005). Conceptualizing munities respond to the maturational changes of
and modeling individual and ecological asset compo- an adolescent and their responses in turn further
nents of thriving in early adolescence. Journal of Early
Adolescence, 25(1), 113–143. influence the development of the adolescent.
VanderVen, K. (2008). Promoting positive development in More simply put, adolescents “grow up” not
early childhood: Building blocks for a successful start. only because of developmental, maturational pro-
New York: Springer. cesses but also as a result of their interactions
Walser, N. (2006). “R” is for resilience: Schools turn to
“asset development” to build on students’ strengths. with other people and in a variety of settings.
Harvard Education Letter, 22, 1–3. Furthermore, these individuals and settings are
Wilson, D. S., O’Brien, D. T., & Sesma, A. (2009). Human simultaneously changing. The positive youth
prosociality from an evolutionary perspective: Varia- development (PYD) orientation (Lerner et al.
tions and correlations at a city-wide scale. Evolution
and Human Behavior, 30, 190–200. 2003) is based on principles of developmental
Zeldin, S. (1995). An introduction to youth development: contextualism.
Questions for community collaborations (Center for
Youth Development Policy Report, #16). Washington,
DC: Academy for Educational Development.
Specific Principles of Development in
Context

In articulating developmental contextualism,


Developmental Contextualism Richard Lerner (1991, 1995) explained the per-
spective as one that views “fused and changing
Rhonda A. Richardson relations among biological, psychological, and
Human Development and Family Studies, Kent social contextual levels” (Lerner 1995, p. 15) as
State University, Kent, OH, USA comprising the process of developmental change.
That is, a developmental contextual perspective
views the essential process of human develop-
Overview ment as being composed of changing, reciprocal
relations between individuals and the multiple
Developmental contextualism is a perspective that contexts within which they live (Richardson
views human development as inextricably and 2005). Therefore, understanding adolescent
reciprocally linked to the multiple contexts of development requires attention to the interaction
individuals’ lives (Lerner 1991, 1995). While the between the continuously changing adolescent
perspective is applicable to human development and the social contexts within which the adoles-
across the life span, the focus of this essay is on its cent spends time.
relevance for understanding and facilitating posi- There are three key themes within develop-
tive adolescent development. Essentially, devel- mental contextualism: (1) Children actively influ-
opmental contextualism implies that adolescent ence their own development; (2) Development is a
982 Developmental Contextualism

life span phenomenon; and (3) Human develop- distinctiveness (Lerner 1995; Muuss 1996).
ment is embedded within social contexts (Lerner Thus, by affecting the other people that are
1995). The remainder of this essay explains each influencing him or her, the adolescent is shaping
of these themes in detail. In addition, the applica- a source of his or her own development (Lerner
bility of these three principles to understanding 1995). According to developmental
positive adolescent development is illustrated by contextualism, adolescent development is partly
focusing on their relevance to parent–adolescent a function of this influence of the adolescent on
connectedness. The latter has been identified as a the individuals occupying the contexts that com-
critical factor facilitating positive development in prise his or her life. The primary contexts for
adolescence (Lezin et al. 2004). Finally, the newly adolescents are family, school, peers, and commu-
emerged positive youth development (PYD) per- nity (Hill 1983).
spective (Lerner et al. 2003) is summarized to An illustration of this principle was provided
demonstrate the applicability of a developmental by Richardson (2005), who identified aspects of
contextual perspective for understanding and intraindividual change that are particularly salient
facilitating positive adolescent development. and may influence the family context during the
Adolescents’ influences on their own develop- early years of adolescence. Specifically, she noted
ment. According to this principle, every adoles- that increases in height, the emergence of second-
cent brings unique personal characteristics to his ary sex characteristics such as breast development
or her encounters with others. While some of in girls, and heightened moodiness stemming
these characteristics, such as gender and temper- from hormonal fluctuations and altered levels of
ament, can be considered “fixed” or stable, many limbic system neurotransmitters may influence a
of them are changeable, particularly during ado- parent’s behaviors toward his or her adolescent.
lescence. For instance, maturational changes in Extrapolating from the limited body of empirical
physical characteristics include both hormonal research examining such effects, Richardson
processes and outward (somatic) changes such suggested, for example, that “moody children
as dramatic increases in height and weight and may be harder for parents to like and enjoy, lead-
changes in body shape. Recent research has iden- ing to fewer shared activities and less time spent
tified adolescence as a significant period of brain together” (p. 30) and “the larger physical size
growth with consequences for cognitive and and emerging adult physique of children in
behavioral characteristics of the age group such early adolescence may prompt parents to begin
as limited impulse control, increased emotional- granting more autonomy (p. 30).” In this way,
ity, and an inclination toward risk-taking (Straugh intraindividual changes may partially account for
2004). A number of psychological changes also the decrease in parent–child closeness or connect-
typically occur during adolescence, such as iden- edness that typically occurs in adolescence. Par-
tity development and individuation (Steinberg ents report that during adolescence their children
2008). All of these changes within an individual become harder to communicate with, more argu-
are referred to as intraindividual change. Of mentative, and more moody due to the physical,
course, no two individuals change in exactly the cognitive, and emotional changes taking place
same way; that is, there are interindividual differ- (Shearer et al. 2005; Steinberg and Silk 2002).
ences in intraindividual change. The adolescent’s fixed trait of gender is also of
An understanding of adolescent development significance as illustrated by the finding that
begins with an awareness of distinctive personal mothers are closer to sons than to daughters
characteristics and intraindividual change. How- (Shanahan et al. 2007).
ever, developmental contextualism further Development is a life span phenomenon.
emphasizes that as a consequence of their individ- A second theme of developmental contextualism
uality different adolescents present unique stimu- is the principle that development is a life span
lation to the people in their lives who in return phenomenon (Lerner 1995). This implies that
respond to the adolescent based on this adults, as well as children and adolescents,
Developmental Contextualism 983

develop as distinct individuals over time. Those experience, Richardson noted that inexperienced
who occupy an adolescent’s life contexts possess parents are more likely than experienced parents
their own unique individuality, part of which is to expect that they and their children will become
stable and part of which is fluid. These distinctive closer in adolescence, and suggested that this
qualities will influence their responses to the ado- expectation of increased closeness might lead par-
lescent. That is, adolescents receive different ents to be more intentional about spending time
responses from different people (Lerner 1995; together or expressing acceptance of the adoles-
Muuss 1996). To understand adolescent develop- cent. Recent research illustrates the interplay of
ment, then, one must consider how the character- parent gender and experience in relation to D
istics of each individual comprising an parent–adolescent connectedness (Shanahan
adolescent’s family, peer group, school, and com- et al. 2007). Declines in paternal warmth over
munity interact with the unique qualities of each the course of adolescence were found to be less
adolescent. The result is a very complex mosaic of pronounced for second-borns than for firstborns,
adolescent development. suggesting that fathers learn from experience,
The concept of reciprocal interaction captures whereas maternal warmth decreased for both
this complexity. Reciprocal interaction refers to first- and second-born siblings.
the process by which both the adolescent and the Positive development depends on the goodness
members of his or her life contexts mutually influ- of fit between the person and the context (Lerner
ence and respond to the behavior of one another in 1995). This refers to the idea that when there is a
a circular fashion. Specifically, as a consequence good match between the characteristics and
of the different stimulation received from each behaviors of the adolescent and those occupying
adolescent, and in relation to their own character- his or her life contexts, positive development will
istics of individuality, each family member, peer, ensue. Eccles et al. (1993) illustrated this principle
teacher, and community member will have a in an analysis of young adolescents’ experiences
unique relationship with each adolescent they in schools and families. These authors argued that
encounter and vice versa. This exchange of influ- declines in academic motivation and performance
ence in turn contributes to further change in each during middle school are largely attributable to a
individual. For example, a moody adolescent may mismatch between developmental characteristics
stimulate frustration in a parent who has a reactive of young adolescents (e.g., desire for indepen-
temperament, and the resultant irritable response dence and autonomy) and educators’ teaching
from the parent may exacerbate the adolescent’s approaches (e.g., limited opportunities for student
moodiness, which in turn would increase the par- decision-making). Similarly, conflict in the
ent’s frustration. The result might be a reduction in parent–adolescent relationship is largely a func-
connectedness between parent and adolescent. tion of a poor fit between the adolescent desire for
Conversely, the same moody adolescent may autonomy and parent tendency toward
stimulate an empathic response from his or her maintaining control (Eccles et al. 1993). Con-
more easygoing other parent. This may result in a versely, Richardson (2004) suggested that when
more regulated emotional response from the ado- adults understand the multiple characteristics and
lescent and reduce the likelihood of future emo- processes of adolescence they are better suited to
tional outbursts toward that parent, thus support and guide adolescents through the
preserving connectedness between the two. changes and decisions they face.
Richardson (2005) suggested a number of par- From a developmental contextual perspective,
ent characteristics that may have a bearing on then, to facilitate positive adolescent develop-
parental connectedness to their adolescents, such ment, it is important that contexts are developmen-
as gender, beliefs about adolescence, and prior tally responsive. This means that the contexts and
experience. Adolescents generally spend more individuals have an understanding of the develop-
time with and feel closer to mothers than to fathers mental nature and needs of adolescents and pos-
(Steinberg 2008). With regard to beliefs and sess qualities that complement those. This
984 Developmental Contextualism

principle is the basis for the National Middle developmentally responsive family context that
School Association’s (NMSA) position that a suc- will enhance positive adolescent development.
cessful middle school is one in which the organi- Human development is embedded within social
zation, curriculum, pedagogy, and programs are contexts. Developmental contextualism recog-
based on the developmental readiness, needs, and nizes that humans are inherently social beings,
interests of young adolescents (NMSA 2003). and in fact it is through interactions with other
Specifically, among the characteristics of the human beings that an individual’s personal char-
school context that are associated with higher acteristics come to have meaning and that change
levels of student achievement and overall devel- occurs (Lerner 1995). From this perspective, then,
opment are educators who understand the devel- adolescent development does not occur in a social
opmental uniqueness of the age group; teaching vacuum but rather depends upon contacts and
approaches that accommodate the diverse skills, exchanges with those who constitute the primary
abilities, and prior knowledge of adolescents; and social contexts of adolescence: family members,
specialized professionals who are readily avail- peers, school personnel, and community mem-
able to offer the assistance many students need bers. Drawing upon ecological systems theory
in negotiating their lives both in and out of school (Bronfenbrenner 1979), developmental
(NMSA 2003). contextualism recognizes the significance of prox-
Richardson (2004) proposed that a develop- imal microsystems (e.g., family, friends, teachers)
mentally responsive family context for adolescent as well as the more distal macrosystem (e.g.,
development is one in which parents are willing to culture, society). With regard to the latter, for
engage in meaningful, open communication with example, it is conceivable that new communica-
their children. Indeed, positive family communi- tion technologies such as text messaging and
cation contributes to connectedness and has been social networking web sites could have implica-
found to be a strong predictor of adolescents’ tions for connectedness between adolescents and
acquisition of positive values (Hillaker et al. their parents. On the one hand, the technologies
2008). To be effective communicators, parents enable quicker, more accessible contact, but on
need to be attuned to what the adolescent believes the other hand they may depersonalize that con-
is appropriate for them to discuss, yet the topics tact and ultimately diminish the feelings of con-
adolescents would like to talk about with parents nectedness that are correlated with positive
encompass a wider range of concerns than what adolescent development. In this way, the broader
parents may assume (Richardson 2004). In other macrosystem has implications for adolescent
words, there appears to be a limited goodness of fit development via its influence on the family
between parents and adolescents regarding their microsystem.
preferred important topics of communication. Interconnections between the various micro-
While adults may assume that the important topics systems of an adolescent are also recognized
for parent–adolescent discussion are sensitive as significant from a developmental contextual
subjects such as alcohol and drug use and sexual- perspective. Referred to as mesosystems
ity, adolescents themselves want to discuss under- (Bronfenbrenner 1979), these linkages between
standing and handling conflict with their parents, contexts can be thought of as creating a “safety
expressing love and support, building and net” or “web” that surrounds the adolescent and
maintaining trust, discovering and appreciating bolsters positive development. As one example,
individual qualities, and learning about their par- the National Middle School Association empha-
ents’ past (Richardson 2004). Richardson and sizes the importance of school-initiated family
Pevec (2007) developed a guidebook to assist and community partnerships (NMSA 2003). For
parents and young adolescents in using movies instance, teachers can establish good channels of
to start meaningful conversations about these and communication with students’ parents by distrib-
additional topics that kids want to discuss. Strate- uting weekly written or electronic newsletters that
gies such as this may contribute to a may prompt parent involvement. In turn, parent
Developmental Contextualism 985

participation such as helping with homework, are personal characteristics of an adolescent asso-
attending school conferences, and discussing ciated with positive behavioral outcomes and
school assignments, contributes to greater aca- comprise four categories: commitment to learn-
demic success for youth (Pena 2000; Steinberg ing, positive values, social competencies, and
2008). Notably, consistent with the developmen- positive identity. External assets are provided by
tal contextual perspective, some parents may feel the family, peer, school, and community contexts,
better equipped than others for involvement with and encompass support, empowerment, bound-
their child’s school based on personal factors such aries and expectations, and constructive use of
as self-efficacy, prior educational experiences, time (Leffert et al. 1998). Thus, both individual D
and language-speaking ability (Pena). and contextual factors are recognized as important
Ultimately, the adolescent is part of a larger, for positive adolescent development.
enmeshed system of fused relations among Recent research has demonstrated that internal
multiple social contexts. This principle of and ecological assets commingle to promote pos-
embeddedness underlies the positive youth devel- itive development (Theokas et al. 2005). For
opment (PYD) approach that has emerged over example, a measure of positive identity capturing
the past decade (Lerner et al. 2003; Silbereisen youths’ feelings about themselves and their future
and Lerner 2007). PYD is a perspective that views is statistically intertwined with measure of youths’
youth as resources to be developed rather than connections to family, school, and community.
problems to be fixed. It draws on principles of This implies that adolescents’ individual identities
developmental contextualism to consider the are inherently linked with their experiences in the
strengths, competencies, and contributions that contexts that define their lives. Multiple such link-
youth can make and ways to align these with ages between individual and ecological assets
resources and supports in the environment to max- exist and are predictive of a composite of “thriv-
imize healthy development (Silbereisen and ing” behaviors such as school success, leadership,
Lerner 2007). From this viewpoint, every adoles- and helping others (Theokas et al. 2005). The
cent has the capacity for positive development research of Theokas and colleagues demonstrates
(Lerner et al. 2003). Fulfilling that capacity the empirical soundness of adopting a develop-
depends on the goodness of fit between individual mental contextual perspective for considering
strengths of adolescents and resources for healthy processes of positive youth development.
development existing across the key contexts of Informed by developmental contextualism, the
adolescents’ lives (i.e., families, peer groups, positive youth development perspective recog-
schools, communities). In other words, the devel- nizes the importance of interindividual differ-
oping adolescent contributes positively to self, ences in intraindividual change, reciprocal
family, peer groups, school, and community and interaction, goodness of fit, developmentally
those contexts simultaneously support the devel- responsive contexts, and contextual embeddedness
opment of the adolescent. Lerner et al. have used for understanding and facilitating optimal develop-
the term “thriving” to describe adolescents who ment in adolescence.
are involved in mutually beneficial interactions
with their multilevel contexts.
Conceptually, PYD demonstrates the relevance
References
of a developmental contextual perspective for
understanding and facilitating positive adolescent Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human devel-
development. There is emerging empirical sup- opment. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
port for this applicability. The Search Institute Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M.,
has identified 40 developmental assets that con- Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., et al. (1993). Development
during adolescence: The impact of stage-environment
tribute to positive adolescent development fit on young adolescents’ experiences in schools
(Leffert et al. 1998). This list of assets is divided and in families. The American Psychologist, 48(2),
into two sets: internal and external. Internal assets 90–101.
986 Developmental Periods

Hill, J. (1983). Early adolescence: A framework. Journal Steinberg, L. (2008). Adolescence (8th ed.). New York:
of Early Adolescence, 3, 1–21. McGraw-Hill.
Hillaker, B. D., Brophy-Herb, H. E., Villarruel, F. A., & Steinberg, L., & Silk, J. S. (2002). Parenting adolescents.
Haas, B. E. (2008). The contributions of parenting to In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting
social competencies and positive values in middle (Children and parenting, 2nd ed., Vol. 1,
school youth: Positive family communication, pp. 103–133). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum
maintaining standards, and supportive family relation- Associates.
ships. Family Relations, 57, 591–601. Straugh, B. (2004). The primal teen. New York:
Leffert, N., Benson, P. L., Scales, P. C., Sharma, A. R., Doubleday.
Drake, D. R., & Blyth, D. A. (1998). Developmental Theokas, C., Almerigi, J. B., Lerner, R. M., Dowling,
assets: Measurement and prediction of risk behaviors E. M., Benson, P. L., Scales, P. C., et al. (2005). Con-
among adolescents. Applied Developmental Science, ceptualizing and modeling individual and ecological
2(4), 209–230. asset components of thriving in early adolescence.
Lerner, R. M. (1991). Changing organism-context relations Journal of Early Adolescence, 25(1), 113–143.
as the basic process of development: A developmental-
contextual perspective. Developmental Psychology, 27,
27–32.
Lerner, R. M. (1995). America’s youth in crisis. Thousand
Oaks: Sage.
Lerner, R. M., Dowling, E. M., & Anderson, P. M. (2003). Developmental Periods
Positive youth development: Thriving as the basis of
personhood and civil society. Applied Developmental Roger J. R. Levesque
Science, 7(3), 172–180. Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
Lezin, N., Rolleri, L., Bean, S., & Taylor, J. (2004). Parent-
child connectedness: Implications for research, inter-
ventions, and positive impacts on adolescent health.
Scotts Valley: ETR Associates. Developmental periods reference the human life
Muuss, R. E. (1996). Theories of adolescence. New York:
McGraw-Hill. cycle, which in modern times has been deemed as
NMSA. (2003). This we believe: Successful middle schools comprising the following periods: the neonatal
for young adolescents. Columbus: National Middle period (0–1 month), infancy (birth to 1 year),
School Association. middle childhood (ages 6–10), preadolescence
Pena, D. C. (2000). Parent involvement: Influencing fac-
tors and implications. Journal of Educational (ages 10–12), adolescence (ages 12–21), adult-
Research, 94, 42–54. hood (ages 21–65), and old age (age 65 and
Richardson, R. A. (2004). Early adolescence talking older). More recently, researchers have conceptu-
points: Questions that middle school students want to alized the adolescent period itself as including
ask their parents. Family Relations, 53, 87–94.
Richardson, R. A. (2005). Developmental contextual con- three periods: early adolescents (ages 11–13),
siderations of parent-child attachment in the later mid- middle (ages 14–16), and late (ages 17–19) (see
dle childhood years. In K. A. Kerns & R. A. Richardson Clark-Lempers et al. 1991). That increasingly
(Eds.), Attachment in middle childhood (pp. 24–45). accepted delineation has been subjected to further
New York: Guilford.
Richardson, R. A., & Pevec, A. M. (2007). What kids elaboration by the concept of emerging adulthood
REALLY want to ask: Using movies to start meaningful (ages 18–30) (see Arnett 2000). These different
conversations (A guidebook for parents and children developmental periods are known for being
ages 10–14). Acton: VanderWyk & Burnham. impacted by “multiple, integrated levels of orga-
Shanahan, L., McHale, S. M., Crouter, A. C., & Osgood,
D. W. (2007). Warmth with mothers and fathers from nization” (Lerner and Galambos 1998, p. 415), or,
middle childhood to late adolescence: Within- and a myriad of dimensions including the biological,
between-families comparisons. Developmental Psy- individual-psychological, social-interpersonal,
chology, 43(3), 551–563. institutional, cultural, and historical. During ado-
Shearer, C. L., Crouter, A. C., & McHale, S. M. (2005).
Parents’ perceptions of changes in mother-child and lescence, each individual undergoes distinctive
father-child relationships during adolescence. Journal shifts in each of these areas, and a person’s devel-
of Adolescent Research, 20(6), 662–684. opmental trajectory also consists of the relation-
Silbereisen, R. K., & Lerner, R. M. (2007). Approaches to ships between and among all these factors, as no
positive youth development: Aview of the issues. In R. K.
Silbereisen & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Approaches to positive single component acts alone to produce the sum
youth development (pp. 3–30). Thousand Oaks: Sage. total of a person’s adult persona.
Deviancy 987

References given that they depend on the nature and attitudes


of dominant groups.
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of The more flexible approach to deviancy partic-
development from the late teens through the twenties.
ularly applies to adolescents, and those who study
American Psychologist, 55, 469–480.
Clark-Lempers, D. S., Lempers, J. D., & Ho, C. (1991). the period of adolescence typically must consider
Early, middle, and late adolescents' perceptions of their additional factors. When examined in the context
relationships with significant others. Journal of Adoles- of the adolescent period, deviance can mean more
cent Research, 6, 296–315.
than different from the norm or more than unde-
Lerner, R. M., & Galambos, N. L. (1998). Adolescent
sirable behavior. When relating to youth, deviant D
development: Challenges and opportunities for
research, programs, and policies. Annual Review of behavior also includes behaviors that are a source
Psychology, 49, 413–446. of concern or behaviors that are deemed inappro-
priate by adult authority as well as behaviors that
typically elicit some kind of social control
response (see Jessor and Jessor 1977). Adoles-
Deviancy cents’ different position in society, then, translates
into different ways of considering them as poten-
Roger J. R. Levesque tially deviant and as exerting control over them to
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA suppress their deviancy.
Different kinds of deviant behaviors have been
deemed particularly pronounced among adoles-
The term deviancy describes characteristics, cents. Those behaviors range from potentially
behaviors, or thought patterns that significantly minor deviances such as lying to parents or
differ from that of the rest of the population. teachers and shoplifting to potentially more seri-
Deviance can be fixed or variable over both time ous ones like alcohol and drug abuse and physical
and contexts. Additionally, deviance can be violence. Importantly, some argue that these
thought of in terms of not only too much of an behaviors tend to constitute a single entity called
undesirable behavior but also too little of a desired general deviance or problem behavior (see
behavior, and it can occur as extremely intense Donovan et al. 1988). Other researchers view the
and isolated events such as mental illness. Devi- behaviors as distinct forms of deviance and cham-
ance tends to be determined by considering what pion the need to differentiate them given, for
is not deviant, given that the norms of a popula- example, that they differently depend on adoles-
tion generally dictate what that population deems cents’ developmental phases (see McGee and
deviant. Newcomb 1992; Osgood et al. 1988). Regardless
Researchers studying deviancy tend to use one of the approach taken to understanding youth’s
of two common approaches to defining it. The first deviancy, it is clear that society marks it as impor-
common way to determine whether thoughts or tant to respond to as it even has created entire
actions are deviant is to use statistical approaches justice systems devoted to adolescent deviance
to create definitive categories for what is consid- (i.e., the juvenile justice system) and to address
ered deviant by determining what thoughts or types of deviance in this period of human devel-
behaviors statistically differ from the rest of the opment that are ignored in others (e.g., through
population. The second common approach places the creation of status offenses).
more emphasis on dysfunctional and undesirable Although controversy surrounds whether there
behaviors and thoughts that are significantly dif- is such a phenomenon as a problem behavior syn-
ferent from the rest of the population. Importantly, drome, researchers continue to give considerable
regardless of the approach, categories of deviancy attention to the concept of deviant lifestyles.
still need to involve important judgment calls Indeed, the concept of deviant lifestyles increas-
since views of behaviors may not necessarily be ingly contributes to productive ways of viewing
absolute and may require a relativistic approach adolescents’ troubling and troubled behavior and
988 Deviancy

actions. Most notably, researchers have shown that focus on the mechanisms through which peers
important links between deviant lifestyles and influence the initiation and maintenance of devi-
problem behavior, and research now also links ancy (see, e.g., Thornberry and Krohn 2001).
deviant lifestyles to victimization (see Chen et al. Although this very rich tradition of research con-
2007). For example, studies report how adoles- tinues to explore the relative weight to be given to
cents’ deviant lifestyles, such as involvement in different factors, few doubt the importance of
delinquent behavior, association with deviant peers in the development and maintenance of
peers, and unsupervised activities, directly increase deviancy.
the risk of personal victimization (see Lauritsen Although the study of adolescent deviancy
et al. 1991; Sampson and Lauritsen 1990). This tends to focus on peers, it is important to highlight
research builds on well established sets of findings that deviancy also may be due to individual or
highlighting how associations with deviant peers broad structural factors and that those factors
serve as risk factors for victimization. likely influence not only individual behaviors
Efforts to understand links between deviant but also behaviors while in peer contexts. These
lifestyles, peer influences, and deviant behavior factors may be of significance in that they may
(including victimization) constitute one of the key well make spurious the links between particular
areas of research in this area. Researchers suggest adolescents’ deviant behaviors and those of their
several mechanisms that can help to explain the peers. For example, some adolescents simply may
links. Notably, the social influence hypothesis be prone to antisocial behavior and have difficult
suggests that adolescents engage in deviant temperaments that also make them prone to devi-
behavior due to the influence of their deviant ancy (see Dishion and Owen 2002). It also may be
peers. For example, adolescents who are more that some adolescents have a tolerance of devi-
heavily embedded in contexts of deviant peer ance and a general tendency toward non-
groups likely have more peer role models, encour- conformity or rebelliousness (Curran et al. 1997;
agement, access, and rewards for deviant behav- Wills and Cleary 1999). On another very different
ior, such as substance use (see Hussong 2002). level of analysis, it may well be that social struc-
The process by which adolescents influence one tural differences create barriers and privileges for
another likely is both directional and groups of youth, resulting in some finding oppor-
bi-directional (see Farrell 1994). That is, tunities in society and others feeling alienated, as
according to the well established selection reflected in research highlighting sex differences
hypothesis of peer influences, adolescents who in delinquent behavior and even in what consti-
initiate deviant behavior may actively select tutes delinquent behavior in that society is much
friends who closely match their behaviors and less tolerant of girls’ deviancy (see Feld 2009)
attitudes. According to the bi-directionality thesis (e.g., running away tends to be seen as more
of peer influences, selection processes are at work deviant for girls than for boys; see Kempf-
during the formation of friendship but that selec- Leonard and Johansson 2007). These potentially
tion eventually contributes to the development of important factors help to reinforce the need for
social processes that reinforce the maintenance, researchers to proceed cautiously as they identify
escalation, or even desistance of deviance in the factors that seem to influence adolescents’ deviant
context of these friendships. The understanding of behavior.
these factors has been well developed by propo- The above lines of research certainly have
nents of social learning theory (e.g., Akers and made important progress in shaping the under-
Lee 1999) who have identified processes, such as standing of adolescent deviance, but they are not
differential association, vicarious learning, and without their limitations. Perhaps the strongest
differential reinforcement within peer contexts, limitation in this area of research is the lack of
as playing important roles in the development of focus on discerning what constitutes deviancy.
deviant behavior. The factors also have been well As a result, measures tend to focus on behaviors
developed by proponents of interactional theories and the focus on deviant behaviors varies
Deviancy 989

considerably. Some adopt a general approach that Curran, P. J., Stice, E., & Chassin, L. (1997). The relation
is synonymous with problem behavior, as assessed between adolescent alcohol use and peer alcohol use:
A longitudinal random coefficients model. Journal of
by the frequency of involvement in nine problem Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 130–140.
behaviors (e.g., lying, cheating, stealing, and Dishion, T. J., & Owen, L. D. (2002). A longitudinal anal-
aggression) during a certain time period, typically ysis of friendships and substance use: Bidirectional
within the past 6 month or year (see, e.g., Donovan influence from adolescence to adulthood. Developmen-
tal Psychology, 38, 480–491.
et al. 1991). Other approaches simply index con- Donovan, J. E., Jessor, R., & Costa, F. M. (1988). Syn-
duct problems, such as questions that would be drome of problem behavior in adolescence:
answered by teachers who rate youth on their ten- A replication. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy- D
dency to engage in antisocial and rule-breaking chology, 56, 762–765.
Donovan, J. E., Jessor, R., & Costa, F. M. (1991). Adoles-
behavior, including destroying property (see cent health behavior and conventionality-
Kamphaus et al. 1997). Researchers tend to borrow unconventionality: An extension of problem behavior
measures from projects that were not aimed at theory. Health Psychology, 10, 52–61.
understanding deviance but at exploring, for exam- Farrell, A. D. (1994). Structural equation modeling with
longitudinal data: Strategies for examining group dif-
ple, delinquency. When coupled with differences in ferences and reciprocal relationships. Journal of Con-
who rates youth (self-reports, teacher or parent sulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 477–487.
reports, or investigator observations), what consti- Feld, B. C. (2009). Violent girls or relabeled status
tutes deviancy becomes even more important to offenders? An alternative interpretation of the data.
Crime & Delinquency, 55, 241–265.
consider. Hussong, A. M. (2002). Differentiating peer contexts and
The study of deviancy has a long history, as does risk for adolescent substance use. Journal of Youth and
the focus on adolescents’ deviancy. That history has Adolescence, 31, 207–220.
led to important research detailing the nature, Jessor, R., & Jessor, S. L. (1977). Problem behavior and
psychosocial development – A longitudinal study of
causes, and consequences of deviancy among youth. New York: Academic Press.
youth and has led to important developments in Kamphaus, R. W., Huberty, C. J., DiStefano, C., &
the study of deviance itself. Research has particu- Petosky, M. D. (1997). A typology of teacher-rated
larly addressed how the causes of deviancy vary child behavior for a national U.S. Sample. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 453–463.
from internal, to structural, to both, to social. Still, Kempf-Leonard, K., & Johansson, P. (2007). Gender and
research in this area remains limited in important runaways: Risk factors, delinquency, and juvenile jus-
ways. Despite those limitations, the study of ado- tice experiences. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice,
lescents’ deviancy, like the period of adolescence 5, 308–327.
Lauritsen, J., Sampson, R., & Laub, J. (1991). The link
itself, provides researchers with important opportu- between offending and victimization among adoles-
nities to understand the nature of deviance. cents. Criminology, 29, 265–291.
McGee, L., & Newcomb, M. D. (1992). General deviance
syndrome: Expanded hierarchical evaluations at four
ages from early adolescence to adulthood. Journal of
Cross-References Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 766–776.
Osgood, D. W., Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., &
▶ Delinquency Bachman, J. G. (1988). The generality of deviance in
late adolescence and early adulthood. American Socio-
▶ Deviancy Training logical Review, 53, 81–93.
Sampson, R., & Lauritsen, J. (1990). Deviant lifestyles,
proximity to crime, and the offender-victim link in
References personal violence. Journal of Research in Crime and
Delinquency, 27, 110–139.
Akers, R. L., & Lee, G. (1999). Age, social learning, and Thornberry, T. P., & Krohn, M. D. (2001). The develop-
social bonding in adolescent substance use. Deviant ment of delinquency: An interactional perspective. In
Behavior, 19, 1–25. S. O. White (Ed.), Handbook of youth and justice
Chen, X., Thrane, L., Whitbeck, L., Hoyt, D., & Johnson, K. (pp. 289–305). New York: Plenum.
(2007). Onset of conduct disorder, use of delinquent Wills, T. A., & Cleary, S. D. (1999). Peer and adolescent
subsistence strategies, and street victimization among substance use among 6th–9th graders: Latent growth
homeless and runaway adolescents in the Midwest. analyses of influence versus selection mechanisms.
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 22, 1156–1183. Health Psychology, 18, 1–11.
990 Deviancy Training

increase in delinquency has been shown to be


Deviancy Training mediated by the rate of reinforcement and level
of peer delinquency. High or increased levels of
Roger J. R. Levesque reinforcement of and exposure to deviance also
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA have been shown to increase levels of conduct
problems at home, at school, and on the play-
ground (Snyder et al. 2005). Peer affiliations also
One of the most important findings to emerge from have been demonstrated to be a critical determi-
deviancy prevention programs is that some of them nant of violent perpetration, especially for some
actually provide environments that increase the minority groups (Haynie and Payne 2006) and for
outcomes that the programs sought to prevent. early adolescents (Pardini et al. 2005; Matjasko
The concept of “deviancy training” has been used et al. 2010). This line of research suggests that
to explain, in part, why some prevention interven- adolescents are more likely than adults to assign
tions that were assumed to be helpful actually harm higher status, attention, and approval to deviant
participants by fostering delinquent dispositions peers, and they are more likely to act on those
(see Dishion et al. 1999). The term “deviancy views (Osgood et al. 1996).
training” was derived from social learning research Taken together, research on deviancy training
that had described the process and effects of produced important findings. This line of research
youths’ discussions that positively respond to rule points to the importance of structured activities in
breaking, types of discussions that researchers first after-school programs in reducing the promotion
identified as occurring among some groups of boys of delinquent behavior by peers. While after-
and their friends. Dishion and others had identified school programs and other youth-oriented pro-
deviancy training as occurring when youth spent grams can promote the acquisition of prosocial
time in groups with peers, when their interactions skills and values, the structure of these programs
had reinforced rule breaking, and their norm- also may decrease delinquent behaviors in adoles-
violating talk had increased future problem behav- cents when the programs carefully monitor activ-
iors (Dishion et al. 1996; Patterson et al. 2000; ities and decrease the extent to which they are
Snyder et al. 2005). This process now has been unstructured. Importantly, although these findings
well documented. have been found in interventions and other pro-
Researchers have found a positive association grams designed to provide activities for youth, it
between delinquent behavior and both unstruc- is notable that youth already otherwise spend con-
tured and unsupervised activities (Osgood et al. siderable time in unstructured activities that
1996). Unstructured activities tend to increase appear to be normative to their development.
adolescents’ involvement in delinquent behav-
iors, even though structured, but unsupervised,
activities do not. Delinquent behavior at individ- Cross-References
ual and school levels also has been reported to
increase with higher unsupervised, unstructured ▶ Delinquency
activity (Osgood and Anderson 2004). Even with ▶ Deviancy
nondeviant peers, unstructured time correlates ▶ Peer Influences
with increased deviance. Researchers have noted
that the combination of reduced social control and References
increased social rewards for deviance may be the
cause of the deviancy training phenomenon Dishion, T. J., Spracklen, K. M., Andrews, D. W., &
(Osgood et al. 1996). Peers long have been Patterson, G. R. (1996). Deviancy training in male
adolescent friendships. Behavior Therapy, 27, 373–390.
found to influence deviant behavior (see, e.g., Dishion, T. J., McCord, J., & Poulin, F. (1999). When
Patterson et al. 2000). The relationship between interventions harm: Peer groups and problem behavior.
early involvement with delinquent peers and The American Psychologist, 54, 755–764.
Diary Methods 991

Haynie, D. L., & Payne, D. C. (2006). Race, friendship Although researchers adopting diary methods
networks, and violent delinquency. Criminology, 44, can use the method in a variety of ways, this
775–805.
Matjasko, J. L., Needham, B. L., Grunden, L. N., & Farb, approach can be grouped in two major categories:
A. F. (2010). Violent victimization and perpetration time-based and event-based. Event-based diary
during adolescence: Developmental stage dependent entries depend on when the phenomenon occurs
ecological models. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, and are particularly useful for rare, specialized, or
39, 1053–1066.
Osgood, D. W., & Anderson, A. L. (2004). Unstructured unpredictable phenomena. Time-based
socializing and rates of delinquency. Criminology, 42, approaches involve entries collected at regular or
519–549. randomized time intervals chosen by researchers D
Osgood, D. W., Wilson, J. K., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, and are slightly more common given that they
J. G., & Johnston, L. D. (1996). Routine activities and
individual deviant behavior. American Sociological provide a greater sense of consistency across par-
Review, 61, 635–655. ticipants. Both approaches are useful for aggre-
Pardini, D. A., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, gating, generalizing, and reporting frequencies.
M. (2005). Developmental shifts in parent and peer To know when to record observations, partici-
influences on boys’ beliefs about delinquent behavior.
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 15, 299–323. pants often wear wristwatches, pagers, or phones
Patterson, G. R., Dishion, T. J., & Yoerger, K. (2000). that indicate when to enter data into the collection
Adolescent growth in new forms of problem behavior: medium. The medium may be a journal, electronic
Macro- and micro-peer dynamics. Prevention Science, device, or Internet. And what they report also can
1, 3–13.
Snyder, J., Schrepferman, L., Oeser, J., Patterson, G., vary considerably; researchers can use event
Stoolmiller, M., Johnson, K., & Snyder, A. (2005). checklists, checklists used with Experience Sam-
Deviancy training and association with deviant peers pling Method (ESM) assessments, and open-
on young children: Occurrence and contribution to ended responses that are categorized after they
early onset conduct problems. Development and Psy-
chopathology, 17, 397–413. have been recorded (see Stone et al. 1991).
Thus, the diary method can address questions
that center on the need to aggregate experiences
over time, examine temporal patterns of experi-
ences, and understand factors affecting changes in
Diary Methods these experiences. As with any other method, the
research questions determine which approach will
Roger J. R. Levesque secure the most useful data.
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA Diary methods have important strengths that
highlight the benefits that can accrue from efforts
that are self-reported and center on ongoing expe-
One of the most effective ways to obtain data riences within everyday situations. Recording
relating to individuals’ experiences is to ask data using the diary method seeks to minimize
them to report their moods, behaviors, situations, retrospection and idealized beliefs about the self;
thoughts, or other information as they experience it instead seeks to capture transient states as they
the phenomenon under study. This approach gen- are lived. Doing so is of significance given that the
erally is known as the diary method, which was length of time between the phenomenon and
devised to have participants record their experi- reporting affects the generation of responses. In
ences as soon as they could after it was experi- this regard, the diary method is particularly robust
enced (and sometimes during it). This approach compared to other methods in that attempting to
provides unique data and has become part of an report over an aggregated time period usually
increasingly popular method that takes advantage overemphasizes certain moments and may be
of emerging technologies as well as new statistical either less reliable or less valid. In addition, diaries
methods that allow researchers to understand bet- capture contextual effects on the phenomenon and
ter the “lived experiences” of phenomena (see help researchers understand the development of
Bolger et al. 2003; Laurenceau and Bolger 2005). the phenomenon. The method allows for clearly
992 Diary Methods

identifying antecedents, concomitants, and conse- modeling. The methods can help account for sta-
quences. Diaries also are able to demonstrate indi- tistical interdependence between repeated mea-
vidual differences in the phenomenon, which may sures, unbalanced data from categories of
or may not be generalizable to some groups of participants, and the incorporation of within- and
people. Diary methods, then, have substantial between-personal predictors. Diary-based studies,
strengths that provide researchers with important then, can provide information about the average
opportunities to create highly useful data. person, between-person variability, and predictors
While the diary method can result in the col- and determinants of this variability. These devel-
lection of highly useful data, it also can have opments highlight how both technical as well as
drawbacks. Diaries can be burdensome to partic- more practical developments have sharpened the
ipants, and those burdens can result in compli- effectiveness and general utility of diary methods.
ance issues. The technique also is not well The diary method has allowed researchers to
understood in terms of the effects of special atten- study phenomena outside of the laboratory,
tion given to the phenomenon under study; stud- increasing the robustness of the findings. Diaries
ies have demonstrated that the observer effect capture transient states not easily elicited in labo-
may change the behavior of research participants ratory settings, as well as behaviors that are diffi-
who know that they are under observation, a cult to replicate in a laboratory. As this method of
concern exacerbated by reliance on self-reports data collection increases in popularity, more
(see Schwartz 1999). While observer effects in research needs to address the effects of this
the context of self-reports have not been the sub- method on participants’ behaviors, thoughts, feel-
ject of much focus (perhaps because of a lack of ings, and other experiences.
research interest in it), it remains a concern that
could be applicable to this method. The diary
method, then, evinces limitations peculiar to the Cross-References
method itself as well as some shared with others
that rely on self-reports. ▶ Qualitative Methods
Despite notable limitations, the diary method ▶ Quantitative Methods
has been developed in new ways that increase its ▶ Self-Report
promise as a highly useful research tool. These
efforts have centered on increasing the robustness References
of data obtained from participants and on using
statistical techniques that permit highly sophisti- Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods:
cated use of the gathered data (see Snijders and Capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychol-
ogy, 54, 579–616.
Bosker 1999; Reis and Gable 2000). For example, Laurenceau, J.-P., & Bolger, N. (2005). Using diary
researchers now may use diary methods along methods to study marital and family processes. Journal
with other methods of data collection to increase of Family Psychology, 19, 86–97.
the depth of data. Researchers also may combine Reis, H. T., & Gable, S. L. (2000). Event sampling and
other methods for studying daily experience. In H. T.
diaries with collateral data collected from other Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research
environmental or interpersonal sources. They also methods in social and personality psychology
may include reminders or instructions for partici- (pp. 190–222). New York: Cambridge University Press.
pants. Also important to note is the extent to Schwartz, N. (1999). Self-reports: How the questions
shape the answers. The American Psychologist, 54,
which the diary method produces inherently 93–105.
multilevel data, which has important implications Snijders, T. A. B., & Bosker, R. (1999). Multilevel analy-
relating to cutting-edge statistical techniques. sis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel
Techniques to analyze multilevel data have devel- modeling. London: Sage.
Stone, A. A., Kessler, R. C., & Haythornthwaite, J. A.
oped rapidly and increase in popularity as they (1991). Measuring daily events and experiences: Deci-
include multilevel modeling, general mixed sions for the researcher. Journal of Personality, 59,
models, random regression, and hierarchal linear 575–607.
Dietary Patterns 993

past decade, research using this method in adoles-


Dietary Patterns cent populations has emerged. There are multiple
reasons to expand our knowledge on dietary
Gretchen J. Cutler, Andrew Flood and intake in adolescents. The prevalence of over-
Dianne Neumark-Sztainer weight and obesity has increased dramatically in
Division of Epidemiology and Community adolescents, and many health problems previ-
Health, School of Public Health, University of ously associated only with adult obesity are now
Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA being seen in adolescents. In addition, overweight
adolescents tend to become overweight adults, D
leading to further consequences for their future
Overview health. Even with abundant research on single
foods, nutrients, and food groups, we know little
The use of dietary patterns to describe an individ- about the role that dietary intake plays in the
ual’s or a population’s overall dietary intake and to development of overweight or obesity in adoles-
relate that intake to health outcomes has increased cents. It will also be crucial to learn more about
greatly in the last few decades. Traditional methods the characteristics associated with high consump-
in nutritional epidemiology typically examine sin- tion of healthy versus unhealthy dietary patterns.
gle foods or nutrients, which informs our under- Knowing the characteristics of adolescents con-
standing of associations between specific measures suming the least healthful diets will help identify
of dietary intake and health outcomes. But people subgroups most at-risk for future health problems.
eat a variety of foods every single day; thus the This knowledge will be important for improving
study of dietary patterns can offer a different per- the efficiency, and better targeting, of interven-
spective. The increased emphasis on the examina- tions aimed at improving dietary intake. Research
tion of dietary patterns within the field of using the single food/nutrient/food group
nutritional epidemiology has been a direct result approach to examine dietary intake in adolescent
of the issues that arise when the combinations of populations has without a doubt provided impor-
foods that are included in an individual’s usual diet tant information and should continue. With this in
are overlooked. Examining the contributions of mind, there are very few studies that have exam-
single foods and nutrients to one’s overall dietary ined dietary patterns in adolescents, but these
intake and health outcomes will always be impor- methods may be especially informative. At a
tant, but foods and nutrients work together, thus, if time when the number of adolescents meeting
we want to truly understand the impact of usual diet current dietary recommendations is extremely
on health it is important to examine them together. low, and the number of health issues they face is
This essay will examine what dietary pattern anal- increasing, the use of promising new methods to
ysis is, why it has emerged as an important alter- study their overall diet is especially warranted.
native to traditional methods used in nutritional
epidemiology, and how its use may further our
knowledge on the relationship between diet and Defining Dietary Patterns
health. The essay will specifically focus on how
dietary patterns have been examined in adolescents A dietary pattern can be defined as the distribution
to date, and future directions for research using this of foods by frequency and/or amount in an indi-
new method. vidual’s habitual diet (Togo et al. 2001). Dietary
patterns are mainly examined to assess overall
dietary intake, and to relate that overall dietary
Introduction intake to health outcomes or other factors
(Moeller et al. 2007). The examination of dietary
Dietary pattern analysis has been used success- patterns has increased in popularity over the past
fully in adult populations for some time, and in the few decades for several reasons. Traditionally,
994 Dietary Patterns

research examining dietary intake has focused on Dietary Pattern Analysis: Score-Based
intake of single foods or nutrients, but this Approaches
approach has many limitations. As mentioned
above, people eat a variety of foods every single In a score-based approach, dietary patterns are
day. By not taking the complexity of the human defined theoretically according to criteria of a
diet into account, traditional methods are ignoring healthy diet that have been determined a priori
the fact that foods and nutrients work together. By based on current nutrition knowledge and dietary
examining only a single food or nutrient, we are recommendations (Michels and Schulze 2005).
missing out on any interactive and synergistic The Healthy Eating Index (HEI) is a well known
effects between it and the other foods or nutrients diet index in which subjects are ranked by their
in the diet (Michels and Schulze 2005; Moeller score to determine the healthfulness of their diet
et al. 2007; Togo et al. 2001). In addition, we (Moeller et al. 2007). The original HEI was
cannot measure every substance in our usual released in 1995, and was recently revised to
diet, and these unknown substances may also reflect the 2005 dietary guidelines represented in
impact our health. Examining the whole diet My Pyramid and was renamed the HEI-2005
through dietary patterns is an attempt to address (Guenther et al. 2008). Scores on the HEI-2005
these limitations, and although not without can range from 0 to 100, with higher scores indi-
issue, dietary pattern analysis has emerged as an cating higher diet quality. Individuals are scored
important alternative method in nutritional on multiple dietary components including total
epidemiology. fruit, whole fruit, total vegetables, dark green
Dietary patterns have usually been defined in and orange vegetables and legumes, total grains,
two ways. They have been defined a priori using whole grains, milk, meat and beans, oils, saturated
a score-based approach, and they have been fat, sodium, calories from solid fats, alcoholic
defined a posteriori using a data-driven approach beverages, and added sugars (Guenther
(Michels and Schulze 2005; Togo et al. 2001). In et al. 2008).
a score-based approach, dietary patterns are To show an example of how a diet index is used
defined through a diet index in which foods or and how scoring criteria is applied, a simplified
nutrients are grouped according to predefined diet index has been created and is presented in
criteria of a healthy diet. In a data-driven Table 1. This simplified diet index has a scoring
approach, dietary patterns are defined through range of 0–12 points, with a higher score indicat-
statistical methods that identify patterns of die- ing a healthier diet. Each study subject would
tary intake from data collected on a specific receive a score for each diet index component,
research population. Neither of these methods is and the scores would be summed to create a total
considered more useful than the other, and both diet index score. To receive the maximum score in
are considered to have multiple benefits and lim- this simplified diet index, a study subject would
itations (Moeller et al. 2007). Both are described have to report that they consume three or more
in more detail below. servings per day of vegetables, fruits, and whole
It is also noteworthy that a third approach to grains, 0 servings of snack foods or sweetened
dietary pattern analysis has recently emerged. beverages, and they would also have to report
This approach is called reduced rank regression that they consume breakfast four or more times a
(RRR), and it combines characteristics of the a week. The dietary data used to create a diet index
priori and a posteriori methods described above score such as this one could be collected in a
(Michels and Schulze 2005). As there have been variety of ways, including food frequency ques-
no studies using RRR in an adolescent population tionnaires (FFQ), 24-h recalls, a diet history, or a
to date, the bulk of this essay will focus on the dietary interview. Benefits of a diet index include
most commonly used dietary pattern analysis the fact that they are usually simple to compute,
methods, and RRR will be briefly described later they are easily reproducible in other populations,
in the essay. and it is easy to compare the results found in one
Dietary Patterns 995

Dietary Patterns, Table 1 Simplified diet index example: components and scoring criteria (score range = 0–12)
Minimum score Maximum score
Diet index component Servingsa Score Servings Score Servings Score
Vegetables 0 (0) 1–2 (1) 3 (2)
Fruits 0 (0) 1–2 (1) 3 (2)
Whole grains 0 (0) 1–2 (1) 3 (2)
Snack foods 3 (0) 1–2 (1) 0 (2)
Sweetened beverages 3 (0) 1–2 (1) 0 (2)
Timesa Score Times Score Times Score D
Eat breakfast 0 (0) 1–3 (1) 4 (2)
a
Servings per day, Times per week

population to another (Moeller et al. 2007). The YHEI was found to be useful at assessing diet
Although several studies have had success using quality in low-income African American adoles-
the diet index approach to examine relationships cents, but was not associated with body composi-
between dietary intake and health behaviors and tion in this study (Hurley et al. 2009).
outcomes, this approach has rarely been taken in In 2009, Kosti et al. published a paper describ-
research on adolescent populations. ing a diet index that was developed to incorporate
Two diet indices have recently been developed various dietary and lifestyle behaviors associated
for use in adolescent populations, and in contrast with adolescent overweight/obese status (Kosti
to the majority of studies using the data-driven et al. 2009). Lifestyle behaviors assessed included
dietary pattern approach, they have added mea- hours of extracurricular sports/activities a week,
sures of healthy behaviors in addition to the usual hours of TV/electronic game use a week, fre-
measures of dietary intake. In 2004, the HEI was quency of eating breakfast, eating breakfast
adapted for use in children and adolescents cereals, and eating foods not prepared at home,
(Hurley et al. 2009). This adaption, the Youth number of eating episodes per day, and obesity
HEI (YHEI), was developed to rate diet quality status of parents (Kosti et al. 2009). In regards to
while also addressing eating behaviors critical to diet, the Diet-Lifestyle Index measures intake of
healthy childhood and adolescent growth and vegetables, fruits, whole grains, type of dairy
development (Hurley et al. 2009). These behav- products consumed (low fat vs. full fat), visible
iors include eating breakfast, attending family fat on meat, and sweets and added sugars (Kosti
dinners, and avoiding snack foods and soft drinks et al. 2009). Scores for the Diet-Lifestyle Index
(Hurley et al. 2009). In regards to dietary intake, can range from 11 to 57, with higher scores indi-
the YHEI measures intake of vegetables, fruits, cating greater adherence to recommended eating
whole grains, low-fat and full-fat dairy products, and lifestyle behaviors that may protect against
lean proteins and higher fat meats, visible fat on obesity (Kosti et al. 2009). Similar to the YHEI,
meat, butter and margarine, snack foods high in dietary data was used from an FFQ to create the
salt or sugar, and sweetened beverage intake Diet-Lifestyle Index score (Kosti et al. 2009). For
(Hurley et al. 2009). Total scores for the YHEI non-dietary intake data, information from a study
can range from 0 to 100, and similar to the HEI, questionnaire was used (Kosti et al. 2009). The
higher scores indicate better diet quality (Hurley Diet-Lifestyle Index was inversely associated
et al. 2009). Data from a food frequency question- with the odds of being overweight/obese in a
naire (FFQ) served as the basis for calculating the population of Greek adolescents (Kosti
YHEI (Hurley et al. 2009). Specifically, the et al. 2009).
Youth/Adolescent Food Frequency Questionnaire The YHEI and the Diet-Lifestyle Index mea-
was used, which, in addition to questions on die- sure slightly different healthy behaviors, which
tary intake, also assesses eating behaviors such as may be due in part to the fact that the Diet-
frequency of eating breakfast (Hurley et al. 2009). Lifestyle Index was specifically developed to
996 Dietary Patterns

measure eating and lifestyle behaviors that are Factor analysis reduces the dietary data col-
associated with overweight/obesity in adoles- lected from participants into factors (i.e., dietary
cents. The inclusion of healthy eating and lifestyle patterns) based on the intercorrelations between
behaviors in these diet indexes may be a useful dietary items. It is a data reduction technique
way to assess important factors above and beyond that constructs new variables (i.e., dietary pat-
dietary intake in this population. More research terns) from the original dietary variables. Prin-
using these diet indices developed for adolescent cipal components analysis is the most common
populations is needed. type of factor analysis used in nutritional epide-
miology (Moeller et al. 2007). As with a diet
index, dietary data can be collected through
Dietary Pattern Analysis: Data-Driven food frequency questionnaires (FFQ), 24-h
Approaches recalls, diet history, or dietary interview
(Moeller et al. 2007). Identified dietary patterns
In data-driven approaches, often called data- are usually given qualitative names that describe
driven dietary pattern analysis, dietary patterns the types of foods in that pattern with regard to
are defined through statistical methods that iden- their nutritional quality (e.g., healthy,
tify patterns of dietary intake from data collected unhealthy), cultural or geographic description
on a specific research population (Moeller et al. (e.g., traditional, western, southern), or the com-
2007). Empirically defined patterns have the bination of foods seen in the pattern (e.g., junk
advantage of not being defined a priori, and thus food, snack food). After dietary patterns are
they do not depend on the investigator’s idea of a identified, each participant is given a score for
healthful eating pattern. Using this method also each dietary pattern, which indicates the agree-
allows researchers to use the dietary data from ment between the participant’s diet and that spe-
their specific studies, and thus can provide insight cific pattern (Togo et al. 2001). These scores can
into the diet behavior of the study participants. then be used in further analyses to examine the
Empirically defined patterns can also provide relationship between adherence to certain die-
important information on distinct diet behavior tary patterns and health outcomes or other char-
in many different populations. In addition, this acteristics (Togo et al. 2001).
method can lead to the identification of new die- An example of dietary patterns identified
tary patterns, and thus, the development of new through principal components factor analysis in
hypotheses on the relationship between diet and an adolescent cohort is given in Table 2. In this
health. Factor and cluster analysis are the methods study, dietary patterns were identified among a
most commonly used to identify data-driven die- cohort of 4,746 middle and high school boys and
tary patterns in nutritional epidemiology. girls who participated in Project EAT (Eating

Dietary Patterns, Table 2 Dietary patterns identified over a 5-year period using factor analysis in adolescent boys and
girls (n = 4,761 at baseline) by Cutler et al. (2009)
Dietary pattern Description of dietary pattern
Vegetable Mixed vegetables, broccoli, zucchini/squash/eggplant, green/red peppers, tofu, spinach,
cooked carrots, raw carrots, corn, celery, beets
Fruit Pears, grapes, peaches/plums/apricots, oranges/grapefruit, strawberries, cantaloupe/melons,
bananas, apples and apple sauce
Starchy foods Crackers, lasagna, pretzels, English muffins/bagels, mashed potatoes, grilled cheese,
macaroni and cheese, pancakes, spaghetti with sauce
Sweet and salty snack Brownies, cake, cookies, sweet rolls, snack cakes, ice cream, donuts, corn chips, nachos,
foods potato chips, candy with chocolate, popsicles, fruit roll-ups
Fast fooda Hamburgers, cheeseburgers, non-diet soda, French fries, fried food not at home, fried food at
home
a
Identified only at 5-year follow-up
Dietary Patterns 997

Among Teens) (Cutler et al. 2009). Dietary pat- 2005; Moeller et al. 2007). In cluster analysis,
terns were identified at baseline, and were identi- individuals are placed into groups on the basis of
fied again 5 years later in the same cohort who had shared dietary intake, and each group shares a
now transitioned from middle school to high similar dietary pattern (Togo et al. 2001). In con-
school and from high school to post-high school trast to factor analysis, where subjects receive a
(Cutler et al. 2009). The following qualitative score for each identified dietary pattern, in cluster
names were given to the dietary patterns identified analysis, subjects belong to only one dietary pat-
at baseline after examining the foods that contrib- tern group (i.e., cluster). There are no scores
uted to each pattern: “vegetable,” “fruit,” “starchy involved in cluster analysis. The only output for D
food,” and “sweet and salty snack food” (Cutler each subject is the fact that he or she belongs to a
et al. 2009). Factor analysis has often been used to specific dietary pattern group, and does not belong
describe dietary patterns in a population and to the other dietary pattern groups identified. Fur-
examine their stability over time. In the Project ther analyses then often examine the relationship
EAT cohort, when dietary patterns were identified between dietary pattern group membership and
again 5 years later, relatively similar patterns were health outcomes or other characteristics (Togo
found (Cutler et al. 2009). Interestingly, a new et al. 2001). Ritchie et al. (2007) used cluster
pattern labeled “fast food” was identified in the analysis to examine how dietary patterns in ado-
Project EAT cohort at the 5-year follow-up, indi- lescence are related to adiposity in young adult
cating a slight change in the usual diet of adoles- black and white females (Ritchie et al. 2007).
cents as they age (Cutler et al. 2009). This may be A description of the dietary patterns identified by
due to the independence in food choices that are Ritchie et al. is given in Table 3. They found that a
gained as adolescents grow older, coupled with “healthy” pattern, followed by 12% of the white
taste preferences for fast food and a limited ability girls, was related to more favorable nutrient
or desire to cook for themselves (Cutler et al. intakes and a smaller increase in waist circumfer-
2009). ence over time (Ritchie et al. 2007). None of the
Cluster analysis is similar to factor analysis, dietary patterns identified in the black girls were
but instead of reducing the dietary data into die- related to adiposity in young adulthood (Ritchie
tary patterns consisting of specific foods, the die- et al. 2007).
tary data are used to categorize study subjects into Another recent study in adolescents used
separate groups of individuals based upon differ- cluster analysis in a group of adolescents,
ences in their dietary intake (Michels and Schulze but, in contrast to Ritchie et al., they created

Dietary Patterns, Table 3 Dietary patterns identified using cluster analysis in adolescent girls by Ritchie et al. (2007)
Dietary pattern n Description of dietary pattern
Black Girls
Customary 643 General intake of many food groups
Snack-type foods 276 High intake of diet drinks, coffee/tea, yogurt, crackers pretzels
Meal-type foods 269 High intake of plain breads and grains, other breakfast grains, most types of sandwiches
and protein sources
Sweets and cheese 23 High intake of sweets, flavored milk and cheese, and low intake of other food groups
White Girls
Convenience 521 High intakes of pizza, fried fish and poultry, and ramen
Sweets and snack- 380 High intake of sweetened and diet drinks, juice, candy, crackers, pretzels, nuts/popcorn
type
Fast food 118 High intake of flavored milk, burger sandwiches, fried potatoes, red meat, processed
meats/sandwiches
Healthy 141 Low intake of sweetened drinks, baked desserts, chips, red meat, burgers, pizza, and fried
potatoes
998 Dietary Patterns

clusters of diet, activity, and other obesity- Dietary Pattern Analysis: Reduced Rank
related behaviors (Boone-Heinonen et al. Regression
2008). Similar to the YHEI and the Diet-
Lifestyle Index, Boone-Heinonen et al. wanted Reduced Rank Regression (RRR) is an approach
to take health behaviors/obesity-related behav- to dietary pattern analysis that has recently
iors into account along with dietary intake emerged as an alternative to the use of factor or
(Boone-Heinonen et al. 2008). Through cluster cluster analysis or the use of a diet index. RRR is
analysis, several obesogenic behavior patterns unique in that it combines the use of a data-driven
were identified, many of which were related to approach, similar to factor or cluster analysis, with
obesity in adolescent females (Boone-Heinonen the use of prior knowledge, as is done when
et al. 2008). developing a diet index (Michels and Schulze
Although often used in adult populations, the 2005). In short, RRR uses dietary data to develop
use of data-driven dietary pattern analysis is a dietary patterns that predict intermediate out-
relatively new method in adolescent populations. comes of a specific disease, and these intermediate
Until recently, most research on dietary intake in outcomes are chosen using prior knowledge of
this population has focused on single foods, risk factors for that disease (Michels and Schulze
nutrients, and food groups. Only a handful of 2005). Ultimately, the relationship between the
studies have examined dietary patterns in adoles- identified dietary patterns and the disease of inter-
cent populations. The majority of these studies est are examined (Michels and Schulze 2005). For
have been published in the last 3 years, indicat- example, McNaughton et al. recently used RRR to
ing the increased interest in this new method. identify a dietary pattern that was associated with
Adolescent dietary pattern studies have taken insulin resistance in a cohort of men and women
place in multiple countries and have focused on (35–55 years of age) (McNaughton et al. 2008).
varied research questions, but they are unified in Insulin resistance was chosen as the intermediate
their desire to examine the whole diet in this outcome because of prior knowledge indicating
important age group. The majority of studies in that insulin resistance is closely associated with
adolescent populations have used factor analysis, the development of type 2 diabetes (McNaughton
and a few have used cluster analysis. The number et al. 2008). The identified dietary pattern was
of dietary patterns identified and their chosen characterized by high consumption of low-calo-
label vary, but similar to studies done with adult rie/diet soft drinks, onions, sugar sweetened bev-
populations, a version of a “healthy” pattern and erages, burgers and sausages, potato chips and
an “unhealthy” or “Western” pattern are usually other snacks, and white bread, and a low con-
found. Many studies in adolescents have also sumption of medium to high-fiber breakfast
found a pattern characterized by snack-type cereals, jam, French dressing/vinaigrette, and
foods that is high in fat and sugar, possibly due wholegrain bread (McNaughton et al. 2008).
to the high level of snacking done by this age McNaughton et al. then examined whether the
population. A “traditional” pattern has also been identified dietary pattern was prospectively asso-
commonly found, specifically in countries ciated with type 2 diabetes, and found that the
experiencing a nutritional transition. While the pattern was significantly associated with risk of
dietary pattern approach has thus far been used in type 2 diabetes (McNaughton et al. 2008). Other
adolescent populations to examine multiple outcomes examined using RRR have included
research questions, most research has focused obesity, cardiovascular disease, and all-cause
on a few major issues including stability mortality (McNaughton et al. 2008). Very few
of dietary intake over time, associations studies have used the RRR approach in adult
between dietary patterns and adiposity, and studies (Michels and Schulze 2005), and to date,
associations between dietary patterns and this approach has not been used in adolescent
socio-demographic, socioeconomic, and/or populations. Results from studies using this
lifestyle factors. approach in adult populations have been
Dietary Patterns 999

promising, and thus, future research using this Although patterns will never be exactly the same
method in adolescent populations is warranted across studies and populations, both factor and
(Michels and Schulze 2005). cluster analysis show reasonable reproducibility
of several major patterns in adult studies (Moeller
et al. 2007). The same has been true in adolescents
Controversies/Limitations studies thus far, with the majority identifying sim-
ilar dietary patterns even though they have taken
The use of theoretically defined dietary patterns place in various countries.
such as diet indexes have raised some concerns. A further caution in using factor analysis is that D
Since they are defined a priori they must be based investigators must remember that an individual’s
on subjective evaluation of the existing literature overall dieting pattern is a combination of all the
on the relationships among many aspects of diet identified patterns. That is to say, in a population in
and multiple health outcomes (Michels and which a few major dietary patterns have been iden-
Schulze 2005). And with many different individ- tified, an individual study subject can score high
uals and groups devising what they view as an (or low) on all of them, none of them, or a combi-
ideal diet, there can be many different definitions nation of some but not others. Finally, the ultimate
of a healthful eating pattern (Michels and Schulze goal is often to examine the relationship between
2005). Furthermore, although it is possible to dietary patterns and health outcomes, but factor and
devise an index that describes an idealized dietary cluster analysis are not designed to derive dietary
pattern, this does not mean that any significant patterns that are predictive of these outcomes
number of people actually follow this pattern, or (Moeller et al. 2007). The dietary pattern that is
that there is any evidence that following this pat- most predictive of a specific disease or health out-
tern per se results in any real health benefits. come cannot be easily answered with these
Caution is needed in interpreting a single sum- methods alone (Michels and Schulze 2005). For
mary score. If various components of an index are this reason the use of alternative methods such as
not related to each other, then they do not collec- reduced rank regression (RRR) has been proposed
tively represent a single underlying construct (Moeller et al. 2007). None of the approaches
(Moeller et al. 2007). Also, a summary score is currently used to examine the diet-disease relation-
only instructive when it is very high or very low ship are perfect, and the use of multiple methods
(Moeller et al. 2007). Subtleties in midrange can will likely provide the most insight.
be lost, which is important since most do not have
a very good or very poor diet (Moeller et al. 2007).
Finally, studies in adolescents are extremely lim- Future Directions
ited, making it difficult to assess the usefulness of
this method in this population. Although research is limited, dietary pattern anal-
Data-driven methods such as factor or cluster ysis has proven to be a useful approach to sum-
analysis also have several limitations, including marize dietary intake in adolescent populations, to
the fact that dietary patterns derived in one popu- relate that intake to health or other outcomes, and
lation may not be reproducible in another popula- to examine what influences dietary intake. There
tion due to differences in dietary habits/traditions, are limited data on reproducibility and validity of
or due to differences in decisions made in the dietary pattern analysis methods in adolescent and
analytic process (Moeller et al. 2007). Throughout adult populations, and future research should pay
the process of dietary pattern analysis there are more attention to these and other methodological
subjective decisions made on the dietary intake issues (Moeller et al. 2007). In addition, most
variables to include, energy adjustment, number studies have been cross-sectional, and more lon-
of patterns derived, number of patterns reported, gitudinal studies are needed. Important questions
and the number analyzed (Moeller et al. 2007). for future research using dietary pattern analysis
Even the naming process involves subjectivity. in adolescent populations include the following:
1000 Dietary Patterns

• What dietary patterns are shown by different intake. Dietary pattern analysis can provide valu-
adolescent populations? able new insights into these relationships.
• Are these patterns stable as adolescents transition Although not free of limitations, dietary pattern
into young adulthood and then into adulthood? analysis provides an opportunity to examine ado-
• Do the identified patterns relate to health out- lescent dietary intake in new ways. Ultimately, the
comes (e.g., overweight/obesity, type 2 diabe- goal should be to use results from studies using
tes, etc.)? dietary pattern analysis to inform interventions
• Which patterns are most predictive of these and guidelines focused on improving the health
health outcomes? of our youth. For this reason, much attention must
• What factors influence adherence to healthy or be paid to quality of the methods used and to the
unhealthy dietary patterns? thoughtful interpretation of the results.
• How do other factors (lifestyle, behavioral,
environmental) interact with dietary patterns?
• Can dietary patterns do a better job at pre- Cross-References
dicting weight status in adolescents than ana-
lyses using single foods/nutrients? ▶ Obesity and Overweight
• How can these findings be used to better plan
and target interventions focused on improving References
dietary intake and decreasing overweight/obe-
sity in youth? Boone-Heinonen, J., Gordon-Larsen, P., & Adair, L. S.
(2008). Obesogenic clusters: Multidimensional adoles-
cent obesity-related behaviors in the U.S. Annals of
Finally, future research using dietary pattern
Behavioral Medicine: A Publication of the Society of
analysis in adolescent populations should not be Behavioral Medicine, 36(3), 217–230.
limited to using the same methods and asking the Cutler, G. J., Flood, A., Hannan, P., & Neumark-Sztainer,
same questions only with different data sets, as D. (2009). Major patterns of dietary intake in adoles-
cents and their stability over time. The Journal of
this will restrict the field of dietary pattern
Nutrition, 139(2), 323–328.
research (Moeller et al. 2007). Investigators need Guenther, P. M., Reedy, J., & Krebs-Smith, S. M. (2008).
to continue to ask new questions, and to be inquis- Development of the Healthy Eating Index-2005. Jour-
itive enough to try new methods when current nal of the American Dietetic Association, 108(11),
1896–1901.
ones do not provide the answers.
Hurley, K. M., Oberlander, S. E., Merry, B. C., Wrobleski,
M. M., Klassen, A. C., & Black, M. M. (2009). The
Summary Healthy Eating Index and Youth Healthy Eating Index
are unique, non-redundant measures of diet quality
among low-income, African American adolescents.
The use of dietary pattern analysis methods to
The Journal of Nutrition, 139(2), 359–364.
explain dietary intake and relate that intake to Kosti, R. I., Panagiotakos, D. B., Mariolis, A., Zampelas,
health outcomes has greatly increased in adult A., Athanasopoulos, P., & Tountas, Y. (2009). The
populations over the last few decades. Results Diet-Lifestyle Index evaluating the quality of eating
and lifestyle behaviours in relation to the prevalence
from the handful of studies using dietary pattern
of overweight/obesity in adolescents. International
analysis in adolescent populations have given Journal of Food Sciences and Nutrition, 60(Suppl 3),
support to the usefulness of this approach in this 34–47.
age group as a complement to the traditional McNaughton, S. A., Mishra, G. D., & Brunner, E. J.
(2008). Dietary patterns, insulin resistance, and inci-
methods of examining single foods and nutrients
dence of type 2 diabetes in the Whitehall II study.
in nutritional epidemiology. Adolescence is a cru- Diabetes Care, 31(7), 1343–1348.
cial time period, and we need more information on Michels, K. B., & Schulze, M. B. (2005). Can dietary
adolescent’s overall dietary intake, how this patterns help us detect diet-disease associations? Nutri-
tion Research Reviews, 18(2), 241–248.
intake changes over time, how diet is associated
Moeller, S. M., Reedy, J., Millen, A. E., Dixon, L. B.,
with health outcomes such as obesity and type Newby, P. K., Tucker, K. L., et al. (2007). Dietary
2 diabetes, and what influences their dietary patterns: Challenges and opportunities in dietary
Digital Divides 1001

patterns research an experimental biology workshop, access are unlikely to result in digital equality.
April 1, 2006. Journal of the American Dietetic Asso- Rather, direct interventions from both the public
ciation, 107(7), 1233–1239.
Ritchie, L. D., Spector, P., Stevens, M. J., Schmidt, M. M., and private sectors are needed to reduce and ulti-
Schreiber, G. B., Striegel-Moore, R. H., et al. (2007). mately eliminate the digital divides. This is a
Dietary patterns in adolescence are related to adiposity critical point that will be returned to later in this
in young adulthood in black and white females. The discussion.
Journal of Nutrition, 137(2), 399–406.
Togo, P., Osler, M., Sorensen, T. I., & Heitmann, B. L. The last two objectives relate directly to youth.
(2001). Food intake patterns and body mass index in The fifth objective is to demonstrate that the digital
observational studies. International Journal of Obesity divides is almost as pervasive among youth in the D
and Related Metabolic Disorders: Journal of the Inter- USA as they are among adults. The so called “Net
national Association for the Study of Obesity, 25(12),
1741–1751. Generation” is not the entire generation but rather
an exclusive subset of it. (The Net Generation
consists of individuals who have grown up with
computer technology and the Internet as a com-
monplace. The distinguishing mark of this genera-
Digital Divides tion is that its members spent their formative years
during the rise of the World Wide Web; they usu-
Linda A. Jackson ally have no memory of [or nostalgia for] pre-
Michigan State University, East Lansing, Internet history; and most were born after 1993.)
MI, USA The youth digital divides have more negative
implications than the adult digital divides, widen-
ing the gap between the “haves” and “have-nots”
This essay examines the nature of the digital on a variety of dimensions, including academic
divides, which are known as the divides between performance, career choices, and social and politi-
individuals and groups who have access to infor- cal integration into mainstream society. The sixth
mation technology (IT) and can use it effectively and final objective of this essay is to demonstrate
and those who lack access and/or lack the critical that digital equality has the potential to “level the
skills needed to use information technology effec- playing field” by providing underserved youth and
tively. To do so, this essay has six objectives. adults with the same opportunities for cognitive,
First, it provides evidence that there is a digital social, and psychological development as their
divides in Internet access in the USA that is based more affluent peers. In today’s world, being
on race, income, and education; second, that there “connected” is essential for achieving the educa-
are digital divides in the USA in the intensity and tional and occupational outcomes needed for suc-
nature of Internet use that is based not only on cess in the twenty-first century. Research from the
race, income, and education, but also on gender; authors’ HomeNetToo Project is used to describe
and third, that the intensity and nature of the the changing nature of the digital divides in the
Internet “use” divides have consequences as far USA, the potential benefits of IT use to the aca-
reaching as the initial Internet “access” divides. demic performance of underserved youth, and gen-
The existence of digital divides, broadly defined der differences in select dimensions of academic
to include access, intensity, and nature of Internet performance and information technology (IT) use
use indicate that vital information about health, among children in the USA and China. By IT we
government, jobs, education, and commerce, all mean the use of electronic computers and computer
of which are migrating relentlessly to the Internet, software to convert, store, protect, process, trans-
will become increasingly less available to those mit, and securely retrieve information as well as,
who need this information the most – members of more broadly, the ability to use effectively such
underserved groups. The fourth objective is to devices as cell phones and videogames. The dis-
demonstrate that efforts to reduce the digital cussion leads ultimately to policy recommenda-
divides in the USA and elsewhere through public tions aimed at reducing the digital divides to
1002 Digital Divides

achieve social, economic, and political parity for all researchers has documented the existence and
subgroups in the USA. persistence of a racial digital divide with respect
Since the Internet first entered the public con- to physical access to the Internet (e.g., Hoffman
sciousness (circa 1995) there have been countless et al. 2001; Pew 2000a, 2003a, 2005a, 2006a, b;
discussions about the digital divides, including NTIA 1999, 2000). In most of this research,
debates about the extent of their existence and “access” is defined as whether or not the respon-
likely that their persistence (Drori 2005; van dent has gone online within a designated period of
Dijk 2005; Jackson 2008; Pew Internet and Amer- time (e.g., the last 6 months). In 2000, 50% of
ican Life Project 2000a, 2002a, 2003a, 2005a, Caucasian Americans had Internet access, com-
2006a, b); NTIA (1999, 2000). Initially, the term pared to 36% of African-Americans (Pew 2000a).
“digital divides” was used to refer to the gap Six years later, this racial digital divide in Internet
between those who had access to digital technol- access persisted. In 2006, 73% of Caucasian
ogies, especially the Internet, and those who did Americans compared to 61% of African-
not (NTIA 1999, 2000). More recently, the term Americans had Internet access. The racial digital
has been used to refer to the gap between those access divide is most pronounced among the
who have regular, “effective” access to digital lower and higher income groups. About 25% of
technologies and those who do not. Thus, discus- African-Americans compared to 32% of Cauca-
sions have shifted away from physical access and sian Americans with incomes below $20,000 (net
toward the digital skills and literacy needed for annual household) had access to the Internet; 65%
success in the twenty-first century global work- of African Americans compared to 82% of
force (Digital Inclusion Panel 2005; Drori 2005; Caucasian Americans with incomes greater than
Livingstone 2003; Van Dijk 2005). $50,000 had access. At incomes between $20,000
Are there a digital divides? As the discussion to and $49,999, similar percentages of African-
follow demonstrates, the answer to this question Americans (55%) and Caucasian Americans
depends on how are digital divides defined (Chen (57%) had Internet access. African-Americans
and Wellman 2003; Drori 2005; Driori et al. 2006; have consistently lagged about 10 percentage
Livingstone 2003; Pew Internet and American points behind Caucasian Americans in physical
Life Project 2000a, 2002a, 2003a, 2005a, 2006a, access to the Internet (Pew 2006a).
b; Van Dijk 2005). If the multidimensional nature Since the landmark reports by the NTIA, titled
of the digital divides is acknowledged, then solu- “Falling thru the net: Defining the digital divides”
tions will require a multiple levels approach. (1999) and “Digital Inclusion” (2000), evidence
Access to the Internet, especially limited access of an income-based digital divide in access to the
in public places (e.g., schools, libraries), will not Internet has been unequivocal. Quite simply, as
be enough to achieve Internet equity. A multiple income increases so too does the likelihood of
levels approach would include training activities having Internet access. Data obtained by the Pew
designed to achieve Internet skills and technology Internet and American Life Project indicated that
literacy while emphasizing the crucial role that 44%, 69%, 81%, and 89% of Americans earning
technology plays not only in the twenty-first cen- less than $30,000, $30,000 to $49,999, $50,000 to
tury global workforce but also in everyday life, as $74,999, and greater than $75,000, respectively,
computers become more and more integrated into had access to the Internet (Pew 2006a).
the tools of daily life (e.g., cars, TVs). Overall, the survey research just discussed sug-
gests the following conclusions: (1) A racial digital
divide exists with respect to Internet access that
The First Digital Divide: Access to cannot be explained by income or education. Cau-
Information Technology (IT) casian Americans are more likely to have access to
the Internet than are African-Americans, a differ-
For over a decade, national survey research ence that has existed and persisted since the earliest
conducted by the Pew, NTIA and independent days of the Internet; (2) The racial digital divide
Digital Divides 1003

cannot be explained for by race differences in Americans in the same income bracket. However,
income or education. Even within similar levels of on an encouraging note, the rate of increase in
these characteristics African-Americans are less broadband access between 2003 and 2006 was
likely than Caucasian Americans to have access to much higher for African-Americans (and the mid-
the Internet (Hoffman et al. 2001); (3) Given access dle class) than for Caucasian Americans (and the
to the Internet, African-Americans use the Internet upper and lower classes; Pew 2006d).
less intensely and for different purposes than do Researchers, educators, and policy makers are
Caucasian Americans (Jackson et al. 2004; Pew concerned that the new digital divide based on
2000a, 2003b, 2005a, 2006a, b, c). broadband access will have implications as far D
Since the early days of the Internet (circa 1995) reaching as the original physical access divide
efforts have been made in the USA and elsewhere (Children’s Partnership Foundation 2007; Drori
to eliminate the digital divide based on access to 2005; van Dijk 2005). They argue that broadband
the Internet. To some extent, these efforts have access changes the users’ relationship with the
been successful (e.g., Pew 2003a, 2006a; NTIA Internet entirely, including how often they go
2000). Physical access to the Internet has become online, how long they stay online, and what they
more commonplace for African-Americans and do online. Whereas dial-up access is disruptive,
low-income Americans of all races/ethnicities in broadband access is seamless, a distinction that
part because the cost of technology (e.g., com- may result in deepening social and economic
puters) has decreased and in part because public divides (Livingstone 2003; van Dijk 2005).
access has increased (e.g., access in schools, Because these disparities are based primarily on
libraries, and other public spaces). However, income, their overall effect is likely to be that “the
new digital divides are developing just as the rich get richer and the poor get poorer” as existing
access divides decline. Solutions to these new disparities in digital skills and literacy are exacer-
digital divide in the USA will require more than bated with the addition of the broadband divide
equity in physical access to technology. (Norris 2001). Research has already demonstrated
that broadband users spend more time online,
engage in more activities (7/day for broadband
The Second Digital Divide: Broadband users versus 3/day for dial-up users), and engage
Versus Dial-Up Access in a greater variety of activities than do dial-up
users (Pew 2006d). Broadband users are more
Recent evidence indicates that a second digital likely to turn to the Internet first when they have
divide is developing based on broadband access a health question (versus calling a health profes-
(i.e., high speed, high bandwidth access) to the sional). Broadband users are more confident about
Internet at home (Pew 2002a, 2005a, 2006d). In their Internet search abilities. They use the Inter-
2000, less than 5% of home Internet users had net to save time and money, and to get the best
broadband access (Pew 2006a, b, d). In March information available for themselves and their
2006, 42% had broadband access. The new broad- families. Broadband users are more likely than
band access divide is based primarily on income dial-up users to create content online (e.g., blogs
(Pew 2006d). Here too, as income increases so or web pages) and to share online self-created
does the likelihood of having broadband Internet content (e.g., stories, artwork, or videos; Chil-
access at home. In October 2005, Internet users dren’s Partnership Foundation 2007; Pew 2006d).
living in the highest-income households ($75,000
and up) were not only the most likely to have
Internet access (93%), but were also the most The Third Digital Divide: Intensity and
likely to have broadband access (71%). African- Nature of Internet Use
Americans are less likely to have broadband
access at home, again lagging behind by about As physical access to the Internet in the USA
10 percentage points compared to Caucasian becomes commonplace, whether connection is
1004 Digital Divides

by broadband or dial up, attention has shifted to a resulting in a gender gap in Internet use among
“third digital divide” pertaining to the intensity African-Americans that no longer exists among
and nature of Internet use. It is quite obvious, Caucasian Americans. The HomeNetToo project
especially to educators, that some uses of the also found that African-Americans used the Inter-
Internet contribute more to cognitive, social and net less often than did their Caucasian American
psychological development, and technology skills peers.
and literacy than do other uses (Annenberg Digital Not only the intensity but also the nature of
Future Project 2004, 2006; Jackson et al. 2004, Internet use differs for African-Americans and
2006a, b; Driori et al. 2006; Livingstone 2003). Caucasian Americans who have Internet access.
Research thus far suggests that individuals are In a Pew (2000b) online survey, African-
more likely to use the Internet in intense and Americans were proportionally more likely than
“engaging” ways when they have broadband online Caucasian Americans to search for infor-
access at home. But beyond this finding are deeper mation about major life issues, such as finding a
issues about the nature of Internet use, particularly job or a place to live. African-Americans were
with regard to what kinds of uses contribute to more likely than Caucasian Americans to seek
technology skills and literacy, and who is likely to entertainment online through music, video, and
use the Internet in these ways. audio clips. They were also more likely to search
For example, since the primary vehicle for for religious or spiritual information than were
delivering the Internet is the computer, there is online Caucasian Americans (Pew 2006a, b, c, d;
concern that people lacking computer skills and Jackson et al. 2001a).
confidence will use the Internet less intensely and Race differences in the intensity and nature of
in less engaging ways than those with more com- Internet use, when access is not an issue, are as
puter skills and confidence. Because the primary pronounced in later surveys as they were in the
reason for going online is to communicate with 2000 Pew Survey. In 2006, compared to other
others, there is concern that people who lack com- racial/ethnic groups, online African-Americans
munication partners online will use the Internet used the Internet less frequently (Pew 2000a).
less intensely and in less engaging ways than They were less likely than online Caucasian
those who use it to connect with family and Americans to use the Internet to connect to family
friends. Technology skills and use are strongly and friends or to engage in other social network-
influenced by peer group support. Therefore, ing activities (e.g., chat; Pew 2007). They were
there is concern that people lacking peer group less likely than online Caucasian Americans to
support for technology use will use the Internet contribute content to the Internet (web pages,
less intensely and in less engaging ways than do blogs; Livingstone 2003; Pew 2005b, c). More-
those who have peer support. Thus, having access over, these race differences in the intensity and
to the Internet, even home access, and even broad- nature of Internet use held across all income and
band access, does not guarantee that the user will age levels (see also Hoffman et al. 2001).
develop the technology skills and literacy needed The HomeNetToo Project, funded by the
to participate in the twenty-first century – eco- National Science Foundation (http://www.msu.
nomically, politically, and socially. edu/user/jackso67/homenettoo/), recorded the
A racial digital divide is evident in the intensity Internet activity of 90 families, 117 adults, and
of Internet use when access is no longer an issue. 143 children, who resided in a medium-size urban
In a 2000 survey by Pew (2000a) a greater pro- community in the Midwestern United States. Inter-
portion of Caucasian Americans than African- net activity was automatically and continuously
Americans who had access to the Internet went recorded between January 2001 and June 2002
online on a typical day (56% versus 36%), or sent (Jackson et al. 2004; Jackson et al. 2006a, b).
or received e-mail (49% versus 27%). Moreover, Most of the children in the project were African-
growth in the online African-American popula- American (83%), male (58%), and living in single-
tion has been driven primarily by females, parent households (75%) where the median annual
Digital Divides 1005

income was $15,000 (USD). Most of the adults in Internet use among adult participants. How-
were African-American (67%) and female (80%). ever, there was some evidence that education
The majority reported having some college educa- influenced adult Internet use later in the project.
tion or earning a college degree (62%), making the Adults with more education were using the Inter-
sample better educated than the typical low-income net more after 1 year of home Internet access than
sample (e.g., Annenberg Digital Future Project were adults who had less education (Jackson
2004; Pew 2000a). Average age was 38 years old et al. 2004).
for adult participants and 13 years old for child Gender differences in access to the Internet
participants. worldwide and in the nature and intensity of Inter- D
Analyses focused on four measures of Internet net use in the USA have been well documented
use: time online (minutes per day), number of since the Internet first entered the public con-
sessions (logins per day), number of domains sciousness (circa 1995; Jackson et al. 2001b). In
visited (per day), and number of e-mails sent the USA, males and females are equally likely to
(per day). Findings indicated that African- access the Internet (Pew 2005a), but a gender gap
American adults initially used the Internet less persists worldwide, with men more likely to have
than did Caucasian American adults, in terms of Internet access than women (Bradbrook and
both number of sessions and number of domains Fisher 2004; Chen and Wellman 2003; Norris
visited. Race differences in adults’ Internet use 2001; van Dijk 2005). Even within the USA,
became more pronounced as the project pro- gender gaps exist with respect to the nature and
gressed. African-Americans spent less and less intensity of Internet use (Pew 2003b, 2005a,
time online, participated in fewer Internet ses- 2006a, b, c, d; Van Dijk 2005). Men are also
sions, and visited fewer domains than did Cauca- more likely than are women to use the Internet
sian Americans (Jackson et al. 2004). However, for commercial transactions and know more about
there was no race difference in e-mail use, Internet technology than do women. Women are
which was relatively infrequent throughout the more likely than men to use the Internet to com-
16-month project. Two explanations were offered municate with friends and family (Boneva et al.
for the low level of e-mail use. First, the study was 2006; Jackson et al. 2001b; Pew 2006a, b, c, d).
conducted in 2001, a time when home Internet
access was less common than it is today, particu-
larly among low-income families. Second, and The Digital Divides in the “Net”
relatedly, low-income families were unlikely Generation
to have family and friends to communicate with
online. Moreover, few had jobs that required Are there a digital divides in Internet access,
e-mail communication from home. Additional intensity, or nature of use among today’s “Net
evidence obtained in ethnographic interviews of Generation” (Tapscott 1998)? The answer to this
a sample of adult participants indicated that com- question again depends on how you define the
municating with strangers online was viewed as digital divide (Pew 2003a; Drori 2005; Jackson
inappropriate, unnecessary, and potentially dan- et al. 2001a; Hoffman et al. 2001; Van Dijk 2005).
gerous, a view that was held more tenaciously by In the USA, access to the Internet from school is
African-American than Caucasian American nearly universal, with about 99% of schools hav-
adults. ing Internet access (Children’s Partnership
Other findings from the HomeNetToo adults 2005a). Thus, if the digital divides is defined in
indicated that older participants (age 38 and terms of having access to the Internet from any-
above) engaged in fewer Internet sessions and where, then there are no divides among the Net
visited fewer domains than did younger partici- Generation in the USA. However, there is a digital
pants. These differences became more pro- divide among youth worldwide (e.g., Norris
nounced as the project progressed. On the other 2001), especially with respect to home Internet
hand, there were no gender or income differences access, how homes are connected to the Internet
1006 Digital Divides

(broadband or dial-up), and how the Internet is ways from those who use it in more mundane
used once access is obtained (Boneva et al. 2006; ways. These differences in Internet use may have
Jackson 2008; Pew 2007, 2005a, 2006a, b). implications for participation in the twenty-first
Among the 88% of youth who report using the century knowledge economy, implications as pro-
Internet in the USA, home access is higher for found as the initial Internet access divide of the
those whose parents have more education and twentieth century.
income, for Caucasian American, Hispanic Amer- Findings from the HomeNetToo project indi-
ican, and Asian American children than for cated that African-American children and youn-
African-American children, and for urban and ger children used the Internet less than did
suburban children than for rural children, Caucasian American children and older children,
although the latter gap is quickly narrowing. As respectively, even though all groups had equal
was the case for adults, a digital divide exists access to the Internet at home (Jackson et al.
between youth who have broadband access at 2006a). African-American children spent less
home and youth who have dial-up access. The time online, participated in fewer Internet ses-
broadband divide are based primarily on parental sions, visited fewer domains, and sent fewer
income and, to a lesser extent, race (Pew 2002b, e-mails than did Caucasian American children.
2006d). However, race differences in Internet use
Researchers of the Net Generation have decreased as the project progressed. E-mail activ-
suggested that there is also a growing digital ity was low and varied considerably over the
divide between youth who use the Internet as a 16-month trial, as it did for adults, probably
rich, diverse, engaging, and stimulating resource because these low-income families had few com-
and those who use it as a narrow, unengaging, if munication partners online.
occasionally useful resource (Jackson 2008; In the NSF-funded research, the Children and
Livingstone 2003). Like the access and broadband Technology Project (http://www.msu.edu/user/
divides, these Net Generation’s digital use divide jackso67/CT/children/) evidence indicated a
are structured primarily along racial/ethnic and “new” racial digital divide in IT use among
socioeconomic background (Pew 2001, 2002a, 12-year-old children (Jackson et al. 2010a, b).
2003b; Driori et al. 2006; Van Dijk 2005). The The sample consisted of 515 children,
percentages of youth on each side of these new 172 African-Americans, and 343 Caucasian
divides are difficult to estimate. But it is clear from Americans, whose average age was 12 years. Par-
the following statistics that large numbers of ents also participated in the project. All partici-
youth are not using the Internet in intense and pants completed mailed surveys requesting a wide
engaging ways, but rather are using it as routine variety of information, including information
and mundane extensions of offline activities. about the frequency of their computer and Internet
According to a report by the Pew, titled “Teens use, the frequency with which they used the Inter-
and the Internet” (Pew 2006e), only a minority of net for communication purposes (e-mail and
teens are using the Internet in ways that may be instant messaging), how often they played video
described as engaging or stimulating. For exam- games, and how often they used cell phones.
ple, only 19% of online youth say they have African-American boys were the least intense
created a web page or blog, although a larger users of computers and the Internet and African-
percentage, 32%, have created or worked on a American girls were the most intense users. Boys,
web page for school, a friend, or an organization. regardless of race, played video games far more
Only 33% of online youth have shared original than did girls, whereas girls, but especially
content on the Internet, such as artwork, photos, African-American girls, used cell phones more
stories, or videos. Only 19% have remixed content than did boys of either race.
they found online to make something new. Race An extension of the Children and Technology
and parental income appear to distinguish Project compared the IT use of the sample of USA
between youth who use the Internet in engaging children to their same-age peers in China (Jackson
Digital Divides 1007

et al. 2010a, b). This study focused on three basic Adolescents who never used the Internet say that
questions. First, are there differences between Chi- lack of interest, time, and access were the major
nese and USA children in their computer and Inter- reasons for not using it.
net use? Second, are there differences between
Chinese and USA children in their use of other
technologies, namely, video games and cell Implications of the Digital Divides:
phones? Third, does gender moderate the influence Adults
of culture – Chinese or USA, on the use of com-
puters, the Internet, and other technologies? Most discussions of the implications of the digital D
Based on samples of 600 Chinese and divides have focused on adults. The assumption,
600 USA children, the study found cultural and often implicit, is that there are no digital divides
gender differences in technology use as well as among the NetGeneration or, if there are, they are
interactions between culture and gender. USA small and will disappear as technology diffusion
children used computers and the Internet more continues (Pew 2006c; Drori 2005; Van Dijk
intensely than did Chinese children, with Chinese 2005). In contrast, this discussion acknowledges
females being the least intense users. Males the existence of digital divides among both youth
played video games more intensely than did and adults and addresses the implications of these
females, with USA males playing more intensely divides for both age groups.
than did Chinese males. USA females lead all What are the implications of the digital divides
other groups in cell phone use whereas Chinese in Internet access and use among adults in the
females were the least likely to use cell phones. USA today? Five broad implications have been
identified (Harris 2003).
First, the digital divides have implications for
The Truly Disconnected social equality, primarily through access to infor-
mation. The argument here is that those who lack
In 2005, 22% of USA adults said they never used access to the Internet are disadvantaged because
the Internet or e-mail and that they do not live in they lack access to information needed to main-
Internet-connected households (Pew 2005b). tain and enhance the quality of their lives. For
Interestingly, the percentage of the USA adult example, research indicates that adults use the
population in this category, labeled the “Truly Internet to find information about health, jobs,
Disconnected,” has remained unchanged since and housing. In a March 2005 survey, 12% of
2002, despite a 10-point increase in the percent- online adults (17 million people) said the Internet
age of all adults who go online. In addition to race played a crucial or important role in their ability to
and income, age and education are strong predic- help another person cope with a major illness.
tors of being truly disconnected. Members of the Another 7 million said it played a crucial or
truly disconnected group are overwhelmingly important role in helping them to cope with their
above the age of 70, and have less than a high own major illness (Pew 2006c). Information about
school education. In both 2002 and 2005, the truly social services unrelated to health, such as com-
disconnected said the primary reason they do not munity and day care services, bus schedules, and
go online is because they do not want or need the law enforcement services are available on the
Internet (Pew 2002a, 2005b). Other reasons Internet 24/7 and at little or no cost. The
included cost, lack of support for use, and being Internet also provides a plethora of entertainment
too busy to learn and use the new technology. resources, such as music, videos, games, sports,
In the USA, 13% of adolescents do not use the and celebrity Web sites, all of which may contrib-
Internet at all (Pew 2006b). About half (47%) of ute to the quality of life and, more generally, to
these nonusers had been online but dropped off for social equality (Pew 2006d).
a variety of reasons, including bad experiences, Relevant to the implications of the digital
parental restrictions, or not feeling safe online. divides for social equality, as well as implications
1008 Digital Divides

for economic equality and social mobility, of the confounding “offline” factors that also limit
discussed next, is the near limitless capacity of social mobility and economic progress it may be
the Internet to support social networking. Internet more helpful to posit a relationship mediated by
users can create and maintain social networks both online and offline factors, rather than a causal
online that are difficult to imagine offline. Those link. Movement out of lower socioeconomic
on the “have-not” side of the divides may be groups will become increasingly difficult for the
disadvantaged because they are less socially disconnected as social and economic opportuni-
connected than the “haves.” A March 2005 survey ties move to the online world. Thus, failure to
indicated that being able to stay in touch with close the digital divides may exacerbate existing
family and friends, both within and beyond one’s social class differences, resulting in the rich get-
geographic community, was a major reason for ting richer and the poor getting poorer (Drori
using the Internet (Pew 2006e). Moreover, much 2005).
of the evidence indicates that online social con- The digital divides have even broader implica-
nections and resources do not detract from offline tions for sustaining democracy. The participation
connections and resources. Instead, online social of all citizens is fundamental to a democratic
activity supplements rather than replaces offline society. Access to information about government,
social activity (Jackson 2008; Kraut et al. 2001). political issues and politicians, and national and
For example, Internet users report that having local issues is essential to having an informed
social support on the Internet has helped them to citizenry motivated to participate in its own gov-
get through “major life moments,” such as chang- ernance. Participation in the political system
ing jobs, dealing with a serious illness, and finding already has a substantial online component.
a new place to live. There exists today a plethora of Web sites, blogs,
The adult digital divides have implications for and chat rooms devoted to government, politics,
economic equality. Educational and occupational and political candidates, and issues of conse-
opportunities are migrating relentlessly to the quence for sustaining a democratic society and
online world, leaving people without access at a an involved citizenry. Although voting has yet to
considerable disadvantage. For example, almost move to the online world, largely for reasons of
half of young adult Internet users who chose a security, efforts are underway to make online vot-
college during the preceding 2 years said the ing an option in the not-so-distant future.
Internet played a crucial or important role A fifth reason to close the adult digital divides
(Digital Inclusion Panel 2005; Pew 2003b, is that the economic growth and competitiveness
2006b). These opportunities are available 24/7 of society may well depend on closing it. The
and in the comfort of one’s home, making them development of an information infrastructure and
particularly attractive to parents and caregivers for its active use by all citizens is inextricably linked
whom inflexible working hours and travel are to economic growth and competitiveness.
major barriers to continuing education and Advances in digital technologies are associated
employment. Information about housing and with increased productivity in most if not all sec-
products is also available 24/7 on the Internet, as tors of today’s economy, as is the development of
is the ability to purchase products and have them new “cutting-edge” technologies. Thus, eliminat-
delivered conveniently to one’s doorstep. ing the digital divides makes good economic
Related to issues of social and economic equal- sense.
ity raised by the digital divides is the issue of Another way to view the implications of the
social mobility. Without access to the Internet digital divides for adults is to consider how adults
and the social and economic equality facilitated on the “have” side of the divides use the Internet.
by it, adults on the “have-not” side of the divides Findings from the Pew Internet and American
are likely to remain on that side. But the implied Life Project with respect to adults Internet use
causal relationship between Internet access and are summarized in Table 1. As can be seen in the
social mobility may be a bit overstated. In light table, the primary reasons adults go online are for
Digital Divides 1009

Digital Divides, Table 1 Adult Internet activities It is by now clear that eliminating digital
Activity Percent divides will require more than just increasing
E-mail 90 access to the Internet, although increasing access
Instant messaging 46 is a necessary first step. In a recent discussion of
Chat 26 the digital divides in the UK, the Digital Inclusion
Information: specific question 83 Panel Report (2005) elaborated “5 Cs” of digital
Information: product 83 inclusion that apply equally well to the USA.
Information: health/specific 80 They are connectivity, capability, content, confi-
Information: hobby/interest 77 dence, and continuity. D
News 71 Connectivity refers to the way people access
Information: health/general 66 the Internet. Connectivity at home is generally
Browse for fun 65
considered the ultimate goal of digital inclusion
Purchase product 61
because home access has numerous benefits,
Government Web site 56
including 24/7 availability, privacy, and the ability
Information: school/training 53
to tailor the technology to personal needs. How-
Information: work-related 52
Information: sports 44
ever, community-based access also has its advan-
Information: financial 42 tages. Community-based access, as in technology
Listen to music 38 centers and libraries, can provide needed expertise
Play games 37 while focusing on community projects, needs, and
Download music 32 concerns (e.g., safety, car-pooling, day care).
Information: religious/spiritual 30 Community-based access also has the potential
Banking 30 to provide the continuity (discussed later) needed
Auction 22 to develop technology skills and literacy.
Create content 19 Capability, the second “C” of digital inclusion,
Stocks: buy and sell 12 refers to the development of technology skills and
Note: Data are from the Pew Internet and American Life literacy that improve the quality of life and
Project (2006b). Internet evolution, Internet penetration increase employability. Developing these skills
and impact. Retrieved January 6, 2008, from http://www.
pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Internet_Impact.pdf
in low-income adults is particularly challenging
because many no longer have access to traditional
educational channels, nor do they have the time
and resources needed for additional job training.
communication and information. Most adults use The private and flexible nature of e-learning is
e-mail and most go online to search for informa- particularly helpful for members of this group,
tion. An overwhelming majority search for infor- provided that some minimal level of technology
mation to address a specific health issue. More skills is already present, and provided there is the
than half have purchased a product or made travel technical and social support needed for successful
reservations online. Many go online to get news. e-learning.
More than half have visited government Web The third “C” of digital inclusion is content.
sites, looked for school or training information, There is still a shortage of relevant content on the
or conducted work-related searches. Almost half Internet – content that will motivate members of
of these adults have used the Internet to get finan- disadvantaged groups to go online. A study by
cial information, and almost one third have The Children’s Partnership (2005a, b, 2007) iden-
engaged in online banking. About one third have tified some of these content-related barriers.
downloaded music files, listened to music, or Among 1,000 relevant Web sites sampled, only
played games online. Almost two thirds say they 6% had local information that users needed and
go online to browse just for the fun of it. Only one wanted. Only 1% was developed for adults with
fifth have created content online, such as a Web limited literacy. Only 1% was created in a cultur-
site or a posting to a bulletin board. ally relevant manner. Only 2% had information
1010 Digital Divides

available in several languages. As the Web con- an alternative not typically available to adults. How-
tinues to develop issues of content relevance may ever, although 99% of USA public schools have
disappear, but not without concerted effort and a Internet access, it is not clear how school access
supportive public policy. Research has shown that translates into individual student use. In many
the key to relevant content is that it be created by schools, Internet access is available in only one
and for the community. Thus, to the extent that location, typically the library or media center. The
corporations, government, educators, and the number of computers in these locations is limited,
entertainment industry are the content providers and sometimes extremely so, with ten or fewer
online, relevance may continue to be an issue for connected computers to serve hundreds of students.
members of disadvantaged groups. Hours of computer availability are also limited, as
The fourth “C” of digital inclusion is confi- are hours of student availability for technology use.
dence. Lack of confidence is one of the most The types of activities that students are permitted to
frequently cited barriers to adult Internet use engage in at school are also limited.
(39%), after “don’t want to/no need” (57%) and The implications of the youth digital divides
“no Internet access” (44%), and ahead of cost become abundantly clear when one considers
concerns (15%).Instilling confidence about tech- what youth do when they go online. Table 2 lists
nology use among the less-educated and low- specific Internet activities by youth. The over-
income groups is a challenging task that will whelming majority of youth use e-mail and instant
require a multifaceted approach. But research is messaging, visit entertainment Web sites, play
already available to guide the development of games, and get news. More than half of the online
techniques that will enhance confidence in tech- youth visit social networking Web sites, down-
nology use. For example, one method known to load music or video files, and search for informa-
increase learner confidence is to provide success- tion about college or politics. Fewer youth make
ful experiences with the technology. Another is to purchases online. About one third search for infor-
provide social support for learning, especially mation about health, dieting and physical fitness,
support from family and friends. jobs or religious/spiritual information. About one
The final “C” of digital inclusion is continuity.
Too often efforts to increase technology use in Digital Divides, Table 2 Youth Internet activities
low-income communities are one-shot interven-
Activity Percent
tions that leave participants adrift after the inter-
E-mail 89
vention is over. Community-based interventions, Entertainment sites 84
especially those that address a pressing commu- Play games 81
nity need, allow for greater continuity in both News 76
technical and social support. Continuity increases Instant messaging 75
the likelihood that the technology skills and con- Download music 51
fidence needed for social inclusion and occupa- Information: college/education 57
tional attainment and advancement will develop Social networking Web sites 55
and continue to flourish. Information: politics 55
Purchase product 43
Download videos 31
Implications of the Digital Divides: Information: health/diet/fitness 31
Youth Information: jobs 30
Information: religious/spiritual 26
What are the implications of the youth digital Information: health-related (sex, drugs) 22
divides in Internet access and use in the USA? An Note: Data are from the Pew Internet and American Family
Life Project (2005d). Teens and technology: Youth are lead-
optimistic perspective would argue that there would ing the transition to a fully wired and mobile nation.
be fewer implications for youth than for adults. Retrieved January 8, 2008, from http://www.pewinternet.
After all, youth can access the Internet from school, org/pdfs/PIP_Teens_Tech_July2005web.pdf
Digital Divides 1011

fifth search for information about health topics The effects of Internet use on youth have been
that are difficult for them to talk about in “real a topic of much public concern and academic
life” (e.g., drug and sex-related health issues; debate, but little systematic research (Jackson
Children’s Partnership Foundation 2005a, b, 2008). On the one hand is the “utopian perspec-
2007; Pew 2006a, b). tive,” which argues that the Internet provides
Youth report that they use social networking youth with unprecedented opportunities for
Web sites to help manage their real-world friend- self-expression, creativity, and active learning
ships: 91% say they use these sites to stay in touch while simultaneously facilitating the develop-
with friends they see frequently in real life; 82% use ment of technology skills – skills that are critical D
them to stay in touch with friends they rarely see in to educational and occupational attainment and
person. Almost three fourths (72%) use social net- advancement as adults. On the other hand is the
working Web sites to make plans with friends. “dystopian perspective,” which argues that Inter-
Almost half (49%) use them to make new friends, net use has negative effects, particularly for
typically peers they are already acquainted with in youth, because it provides inaccurate and
real life. They view online communication and unreliable information and exposes users to
friendships as supplements to, rather than substitutes potentially harmful content (e.g., pornography)
for real-life social communication and friendships and experiences (e.g., bullying). Moreover, from
(Bargh and McKenna 2004; Boneva et al. 2006; the dystopian perspective, time online is time
Gross 2004; Pew 2001, 2003b; Roberts et al. taken away from family, friends, and other
2005; Subrahmanyam et al. 2001; Wellman and healthy pursuits, such as reading books, doing
Haythornwaite 2002). Other research, discussed school work, and participating in sports and rec-
next, has examined the effects of Internet use on reational activities.
youth, although “effects” research is nascent and the Research on the consequences of Internet use
findings are far from conclusive (Jackson 2008). for youth, though nascent, is more supportive of
Gender differences in youth’s Internet use have the utopian perspective and the dystopian per-
been found, although similarities are more com- spective. As discussed earlier, youth use the Inter-
mon than differences. Girls lead the way in using net to manage their social lives rather than replace
the Internet’s communication tools, such as them, and many consider the Internet indispens-
e-mail, instant messaging, blogging, and visiting able for doing so. As is true for adults, youth’s
social networking Web sites. Boys lead the way in online social activities appear to have no negative
playing games, downloading files, and file shar- consequences for their offline social lives. On
ing. Both sexes use the Internet as a social the contrary, research supports the “the rich get
communication tool. richer” hypothesis; youth who already have rich
Consistent with findings just discussed, the social networks in real life enrich them further
most popular Web sites for youth are social net- with online activities.
working Web sites (e.g., MySpace, FaceBook) Also supporting the utopian perspective, youth
and Web sites that support social networking say that the Internet is as important to their school
(PLyrics, Snapvine). Also consistent with the pre- work as it is to their social lives. More than half
ceding discussion, other popular Web sites are (60%) of online youth regard the Internet as the
those that supplement real-life activities. Thus, most useful tool for finding information for
fashion magazine Web sites are popular among homework – more useful than books (21%), par-
adolescent girls (e.g., http://www.teenmag.com/). ents (11%), CDs (3%), friends (2%), or TV (1%).
Both sexes frequent the MTV Web site (http:// For the majority of families, education is the most
www.mtv.com/), teen news Web sites (e.g., important reason for purchasing a home computer
http://www.teenspeaknews.com/), teen chat sites and connecting to the Internet (Pew 2006a). The
(http://www.teenchat.com/), teen culture sites home computer has become one of the indispens-
(http://www.spankmag.com/), and music sites able “symbolic goods” of contemporary parent-
(http://www.teenmusic.com/). ing. Enthusiasm about the Internet is driven not
1012 Digital Divides

only by its ability to deliver content but also by its Bavelier 2003, 2006, 2007). However, supporting
potential to provide individualized, interactive, the dystopian perspective, there is also strong evi-
autonomous, and learner-centered learning oppor- dence, including evidence from the authors’
tunities, although how often this occurs is unclear research (Jackson et al., in press), that videogame
(Jackson 2008; Livingstone 2003). playing undermines academic performance (GPA)
In the HomeNetToo project, discussed earlier, and, that playing violent videogames increases
home Internet use by low-income youth was aggressive cognition and behavior, both immedi-
found to increase academic performance (Jackson ately and in the long run (Anderson et al. 2007).
et al. 2006b). In this longitudinal study, which Equally unequivocal is that boys are more likely to
occurred between January 2001 and June 2002, play videogames than are girls.
the Internet use of 140 youth (average age
13 years old) was continuously recorded for
16 months. Most of the youth were African- Future Trends
American (83%), male (58%), and living in
single-parent households (75%) in which the Research reviewed here indicates that digital
median annual income was $15,000 or less divides in Internet access and use exist for both
(USA dollars). Findings indicated that youth youth and adults and have important implications
who used the Internet more subsequently had for both groups. For adults, the digital divides have
higher scores on standardized tests of reading implications for the development of the technology
achievement and higher grade point averages skills and literacy needed to participate fully in the
6 months, 1 year, and 16 months later than did twenty-first century, especially in the workforce.
youth who used it less. Social and economic equality and social mobility
The Children and Technology Project, also depend on the acquisition of IT skills and literacy.
discussed earlier, also found benefits to Internet Sustaining a democratic society and maintaining a
use for youth (average age was 12 years). Youth competitive economic edge require a citizenry that
who used the Internet more, either to search for is fully participating in an information-based econ-
information or to communicate with others (i.e., omy. Systematic, concerted efforts are needed to
instant messaging and e-mail) performed better on turn the “have-nots” in today’s adult population
a standardized test of reading skills than did youth into the “haves,” for their own sake and that of
who used it less (Jackson et al. 2010a, b). More- their children and the society.
over, IT use predicted youth’s academic perfor- For youth, the digital divides have even more
mance. The longer the time the youth had been pressing implications. Although Internet effects
using computers and the Internet the better was his research is still in its early stages, the evidence
or her academic performance. suggests that social connectedness, academic per-
Any discussion of the effects of youth’s IT formance, and ultimately occupational attainment
use on their development would be incomplete and advancement may depend on youth’s partici-
without some discussion of the most popular IT pation in the digital world. Being disconnected
activity of youth, videogame playing. Research on from new technology is fast becoming tantamount
the effects of videogame playing has produced to being disconnected from social and profes-
remarkably strong and consistent results. sional resources that may contribute to personal,
Supporting the utopian perspective, there is strong social, and professional development. The likely
evidence from both correlational and experimental link between Internet use and academic perfor-
research that videogame playing improves visual- mance for youth is an even more compelling
spatial skills, which relate to the ability to think reason to eliminate digital divides. Those on the
and learn through visual processes. This form of “have-not” side of the divides may be at an ever
nonverbal learning is viewed as important in learn- increasing disadvantage in developing the tech-
ing science, technology, engineering, and mathe- nology skills and literacy needed to participate in
matics (known as the STEM areas; Green and the twenty-first century global economy.
Digital Divides 1013

Understanding the digital divides and their must be introduced early in development, just as
implications will require additional research soon as the cognitive structures needed to support
aimed at establishing cause–effect relationships them have developed.
between technology use and a variety of positive
and negative outcomes. A more fine-tuned analy-
sis of what it means to “use” IT, such as the Cross-References
Internet, will also shed light on how and when
positive or negative outcomes might be expected. ▶ Media Literacy and Health Promotion
For example, using the Internet to search for D
health information is likely to produce different
Acknowledgments We wish to thank the National Science
cognitive outcomes and academic performance Foundation for funding the HomeNetToo Project (NSF-ITR,
outcomes than is using the Internet to discuss December 1, 2005–November 30, 2009) and the Children
inappropriate behavior in chat rooms. The next and Technology Project (NSF-HSD, September 1,
2000–August 31, 2005).
generation of research on technology use and its
Thanks go to the 15 graduate and 30 undergraduate
effects must focus on specific uses, specific students who have assisted in data collects for these
effects, and on establishing a causal link between projects.
the two.

References
Conclusions
Anderson, C. A., Gentile, D. A., & Buckley, K. E. (2007).
Violent video game effects on children and adolescents:
Digital divides are multidimensional and are best
Theory, research and public policy. New York: Oxford
conceptualized as a number of divides in technol- University Press.
ogy access and use. Those on the “have” side of Annenberg Digital Future Project. (2004). Surveying the
the divides have 24/7 access to the Internet at digital future–Year 4: Ten years, ten trends. Retrieved
3 Mar 2008, from http://www.digitalcenter.org/down
home with a broadband connection so that Inter-
loads/DigitalFutureReport-Year4-2004.pdf.
net activities fit seamlessly into the activities of Annenberg Digital Future Project. (2006). Online world as
everyday life. The “haves” use the Internet important to Internet users as real world? 6th annual
intensely and for multiple purposes, such as survey. Press release, November 29. Retrieved 3 Mar
2008, from http://www.digitalcenter.org/pages/cur
acquiring information about products, politics,
rent_report.asp?intGlobalId=19.
and purchases. Youth on the “have” side of the Bargh, J. A., & McKenna, K. Y. A. (2004). The Internet
digital divides use the Internet to facilitate social and social life. Annual Review of Psychology, 55,
connectedness and to gain information that bene- 573–590.
Boneva, B. S., Quinn, A., Kraut, R. E., Kiesler, S., &
fits their academic performance. In contrast, the
Shklovski, I. (2006). Teenage communication in the
“have-nots” lack the social, academic, and profes- Instant Messaging era. In R. Kraut, M. Brynin, &
sional benefits enjoyed by the “haves.” S. Kiesler (Eds.), Computers, phones, and the Internet:
Because the digital divides are multi- Domesticating information technology (pp. 201–218).
New York: Oxford University Press.
dimensional, eliminating them will require a
Bradbrook, G., & Fisher, J. (2004). Digital equality:
multidimensional approach. From a policy per- Reviewing digital inclusion activity and mapping the
spective, universal broadband access is a neces- way forwards. Retrieved 15 May 2008, from www.
sary first step toward eliminating digital divides. btplc.com/Societyandenvironment/PDF/Digitaldivide
2025.pdf.
Internet Service Providers may need government
Buckingham, D., & Scanlon, M. (2006). Selling learning:
incentives to reduce the recurring cost of broad- Towards a political economy of edutainment. Media,
band connection for low-income subscribers. Culture and Society, 27, 43–49.
Internet content developers may need to expand Chen, W., & Wellman, B. (2003). Charting and bridging
digital divides: Comparing socio-economic, gender,
their design capabilities to appeal to a more
life stage, and rural-urban Internet access and use in
diverse set of users (Buckingham and Scanlon eight countries. Retrieved 5 May 2008, from http://
2006). Furthermore, the technologies themselves www.amd.com/us-en/assets/content_type/.
1014 Digital Divides

Children’s Partnership Foundation. (2005a). Measuring affective, and cognitive correlates of Internet use. Jour-
digital opportunity for America’s children: Where we nal of Applied Social Psychology, 31, 2019–2046.
stand and where we go from here. Retrieved 21 Apr Jackson, L. A., Ervin, K. S., Gardner, P. D., & Schmitt, N.
2008, from http://www.childrenspartnership.org/AM/ (2001b). Gender and the Internet: Women communi-
Template.cfm?Section=Reports1&Template=/CM/Co cating and men searching. Sex Roles, 44, 363–380.
ntentDisplay.cfm&ContentFileID=1089. Jackson, L. A., von Eye, A., Barbatsis, G., Biocca, F.,
Children’s Partnership Foundation. (2005b). Impacts of Fitzgerald, H. E., & Zhao, Y. (2004). The social impact
technology on outcomes for youth: A 2005 review. of Internet use on the other side of the digital divides.
Retrieved 21 Apr 2008, from http://www.children- Communications of the Association for Computing
spartnership.org/AM/Template.cfm?Section=Reports1& Machinery, 47, 43–47.
Template=/CM/ContentDisplay.cfm&ContentID=7931. Jackson, L. A., von Eye, A., Biocca, F. A., Barbatsis, G.,
Children’s Partnership Foundation. (2007). Helping Zhao, Y., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (2006a). Children’s home
our children succeed: What’s broadband got to do Internet use: Predictors and psychological, social and
with it? Retrieved 21 Apr 2008, from http://www. academic consequences. In R. Kraut, M. Brynin, &
childrenspartnership.org/AM/Template.cfm?Section= S. Kiesler (Eds.), Computers, phones and the Internet:
Reports1&Template=/CM/ContentDisplay.cfm&Cont Domesticating information technology (pp. 145–167).
entID=10692. New York: Oxford University Press.
Digital Inclusion Panel Report. (2004). Department of Jackson, L. A., von Eye, A., Biocca, F. A., Barbatsis, G.,
Trade and Industry. London. www.dti.gov.uk/ Zhao, Y., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (2006b). Does home
publications. Internet use influence the academic performance of
Digital Inclusion Panel Report. (2005). Enabling a digitally low-income children? Findings from the HomeNetToo
United Kingdom. Retrieved 17 Nov 2010, from http:// project. Special Section on Children, Adolescents, and
webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20050310190350/ the Internet. Developmental Psychology, 42, 429–435.
http://cabinetoffice.gov.uk/publications/reports/digital/ Jackson, L. A., Zhao, Y., Kolenic, A., Fitzgerald, H. E.,
digitalframe.pdf. Harold, R., & von Eye, A. (2008a). Race, gender and
Driori, G. S., Meyer, J. W., & Hwang, H. (Eds.). (2006). information technology use: The new digital divides.
Globalization and organization: World society and CyberPsychology and Behavior, 755–757.
organizational change. Oxford: Oxford University Jackson, L. A., Zhao, Y., Qiu, W., Kolenic, A., Fitzgerald,
Press. H. E., Harold, R., & von Eye, A. (2008b). Culture,
Drori, G. S. (2005). Global e-litism: Digital technology, gender and information technology use:
social inequality and transnationality. New York: A comparison of Chinese and U.S. youth. Computers
Worth. in Human Behavior, 24, 2817–2829.
Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2003). Action video games Jackson, L. A., Zhao, Y., Kolenic, A., Fitzgerald, H. E.,
modify visual attention. Nature, 423, 534–537. Harold, R., & von Eye, A. (2010a). Race, gender and
Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2006). Effects of video game information technology use: The new digital divides.
playing on the spatial distribution of visual selective CyberPsychology and Behavior, 437–442.
attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Jackson, L. A., Zhao, Y., Qiu, W., Kolenic, A., Fitzgerald,
Perception and Performance, 32, 465–478. H. E., Harold, R., & von Eye, A. (2010b). Cultural
Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2007). Action video experi- differences in morality in the real and virtual worlds:
ence alters the spatial resolution of vision. Psycholog- A comparison of Chinese and U.S. youth. Cyber-
ical Science, 18, 88–94. psychology and Behavior, 279–286.
Gross, E. F. (2004). Adolescent Internet use: What we Kraut, R., Kiesler, S., Boneva, B., Cummings, J.,
expect, what teens report. Journal of Applied Develop- Helgeson, V., & Crawford, A. (2001). Internet paradox
mental Psychology, 25, 633–649. revisited. Journal of Social Issues, 58, 49–74.
Harris, R. W. (2003). Information and communication Livingstone, S. (2003). Children’s use of the Internet:
technologies for poverty alleviation. Retrieved Reflections on the emerging research agenda. New
17 Nov 2010, from http://157.150.195.10/depts/toc/ Media & Society, 5, 147–166.
toc9.pdf. National Telecommunications and Information Adminis-
Hoffman, D., Novak, T., & Schlosser, A. (2001). The tration. (1999). Falling thru the net: Defining the digital
evolution of the digital divides: Examining the relation- divides. Retrieved 3 Nov 2006, from http://www.ntia.
ship of race to internet access and usage over time. In doc.gov/ntiahome/fttn99/.
B. Compaine (Ed.), The digital divides. Cambridge, National Telecommunications and Information Adminis-
MA: MIT Press. tration. (2000). Falling thru the net: Digital inclusion.
Jackson, L. A. (2008). Adolescents and the Internet. In Retrieved 3 Nov 2008, from http://search.ntia.doc.gov/
D. Romer & P. Jamieson (Eds.), The changing portrayal pdf/fttn00.pdf.
of American youth in popular media (pp. 377–410). Norris, P. (2001). Digital divide: Civic engagement, infor-
Annenberg Public Policy Center at the University of mation poverty, and the Internet worldwide. New York:
Pennsylvania, New York: Oxford University Press. Cambridge University Press.
Jackson, L. A., Ervin, K. S., Gardner, P. D., & Schmitt, N. Pew Internet and American Family Life. (2005a). A decade
(2001a). The racial digital divides: Motivational, of adoption: How the internet has woven itself into
Disability 1015

American life. Retrieved 3 Mar 2008, from http://www. Retrieved 3 Feb 2008, from http://www.pewinternet.
pewinternet.org/pdfs/Internet_Status_2005.pdf. org/ppt/Fox_FTC_Nov_6_%202006.pdf.
Pew Internet and American Family Life. (2005b). Teen Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2006b). Internet
content creators and consumers. Retrieved 2 Mar evolution, Internet penetration and impact. Retrieved
2008, from http://www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_ 22 Feb 2008, from http://www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/
Teens_Content_Creation.pdf. PIP_Internet_Impact.pdf.
Pew Internet and American Family Life. (2005c). Teens Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2006c). Finding
and technology: Youth are leading the transition to a Answers Online in Sickness and in Health. Retrieved
fully wired and mobile nation. Retrieved 4 Feb 2008, 30 Jan 2008, from http://www.pewinternet.org/PPF/r/
from http://www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Teens_ 183/report_display.asp.
Tech_July2005web.pdf. Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2006d). Home D
Pew Internet and American Family Life. (2006). Teens and Broadband adoption. Retrieved 14 Feb 2008, from
the Internet. Findings submitted to the House Subcom- http://www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Broadband_trends
mittee on Telecommunications and the Internet. 2006.pdf.
Retrieved 23 Mar 2008, from http://www.pewinternet. Roberts, D. F., Foehr, U., & Rideout, V. (2005). Kids and
org/ppt/Pew%20Internet%20findings%20-%20teens% media in America. New York: Cambridge University
20and%20the%20internet%20-%20final.pdf. Press.
Pew Internet and American Family Life. (2007). Social Subrahmanyam, K., Greenfield, P., Kraut, R., & Gross, E.
networking websites and teens: An overview. (2001). The impact of computer use on children’s and
Retrieved 21 Mar 2008, from http://www.pewinternet. adolescents’ development. Applied Developmental
org/pdfs/PIP_SNS_Data_Memo_Jan_2007.pdf. Psychology, 22, 7–30.
Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2000a). African- Tapscott, D. (1998). Growing up digital: The rise of the Net
Americans and the Internet. Retrieved 5 Jan 2008, from generation. New York: McGraw-Hill.
http://www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_African_Americans_ Van Dijk, J. (2005). The deepening divide, inequality in the
Report.pdf. information society. Thousand Oaks/London/New
Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2000b). Tracking Delhi: SAGE.
online life: How women use the Internet to cultivate Wellman, B., & Haythornwaite, C. (2002). The Internet in
relationships with family and friends. Retrieved 3 Mar everyday life. Oxford: Blackwell.
2008, from http://www.pewinternet.org/.
Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2001).Teenage
life online: The rise of the instant-message generation
and the Internet’s impact on friendships and family
relationships. Retrieved 9 Mar 2008, from http://
www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Teens_Report.pdf. Disability
Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2002a). The
digital disconnect: The widening gap between Internet Roger J. R. Levesque
savvy students and their schools. Retrieved 13 Jan
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
2008, from http://www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_
Schools_Internet_Report.pdf.
Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2002b). The broad-
band difference. Retrieved 23 Mar 2008, from http:// The term disability generally refers to limitations
www.pewinternet.org/reports/toc.asp?Report=63.
of functions that are secondary to the disorder of a
Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2003a). The
ever-shifting Internet population: A new look at Inter- specific system. The word can be differentiated
net access and the digital divides. Retrieved 13 Jan from the label of handicapped, which instead ref-
2008, from http://www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_ erences the obstacles encountered by individuals
Shifting_Net_Pop_Report.pdf.
who pursue goals as a result of their disability.
Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2003b). Online
Pursuits. Retrieved 12 Jan 2008, from http://www. Handicap frequently represents a degree of emo-
pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Online_Pursuits_Final.pdf. tional, economic, social, or physical dependence
Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2005a). Digital that a person can experience as the result of a
Divisions. Retrieved 8 Jan 2008, from http://www.
disability. A person’s “handicap” reflects an inter-
pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Digital_Divisions_Oct_5_
2005.pdf. action between a disability and the person who is
Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2005b). The disabled, concerning the way the individual
Strength of Internet Ties. Retrieved 8 Jan 2008, from behaves in a broader context and environment
http://www.pewinternet.org/PPF/r/172/report_display.
which itself consists of social, attitudinal, archi-
asp.
Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2006a). Internet tectural, and legal dimensions (Walker and
Usage Trends: Through the demographic lens. Greene 1991). Globally, almost 180 million
1016 Disciplining

people between the ages of 10 and 24 operate with individual, family, societal, and cultural norms
some type of an identifiable disability (Groce and expectations. Traditionally, the term discipline
2004). Disabilities during adolescence are impor- has been synonymous with punishment and has
tant to consider given that all adolescents face been characterized by use of an aversive or
physical, cognitive, and social changes and that unpleasant consequence (Forehand and McKinney
those changes may be experienced differently by 1993); however, as will be evident from the
adolescents with disabilities. research reviewed here, a more comprehensive
conceptualization situates the construct of disci-
pline in a much broader framework, including the
Cross-References expectations parents set forth for their adolescents,
the consequences they use, and how they monitor
▶ Delinquent Youth with Disabilities behaviors. What initially appeared to be a straight-
forward and simple construct has proven to be a
References complex and multifaceted phenomenon – for both
parents and researchers – and boasts a long history
Groce, N. E. (2004). Adolescents and youth with disability: of change across the past 400 years in the United
Issues and challenges. Asia Pacific Disability Rehabil- States (see Forehand and McKinney 1993).
itation Journal, 15(2), 13–32. In order to understand the role of discipline in
Walker, L. S., & Greene, J. W. (1991). The functional
disability inventory: Measuring a neglected dimension
healthy adolescent development, one must
of child health status. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, acknowledge both the wide array of strategies
16(1), 39–58. embodied by the term as well as its place within
the larger context of parenting. Generally speak-
ing, authoritative parenting has emerged from
over 40 years of research as a style of parenting
Disciplining most clearly associated with positive child out-
comes. An authoritative approach, which com-
Emily P. Garai1, Laura G. McKee2, bines disciplinary strategies with warmth and
Virginia Peisch3 and Rex Forehand3 positive regard, is appropriate for use with all
1
Atlanta Psychological Services, Atlanta, ages of children; however, specific adaptations
GA, USA for teenagers are necessary because of the
2
Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA unique biological, cognitive, social, and environ-
3
Department of Psychological Science, Dewey mental changes that occur during adolescence
Hall, University of Vermont, Burlington, (Holmbeck et al. 1995, 2010). The following dis-
VT, USA cussion provides an overview of the development
of authoritative parenting as a framework within
which to examine discipline, an argument for why
Broadly, the purpose of discipline is to mold, cor- this type of parenting is important, a discussion of
rect, train, and modulate behavior. Appropriate specific discipline strategies that are effective with
discipline is particularly important during adoles- adolescents, and a review of the literature
cence given both the new demands placed on youth suggesting the importance of considering disci-
because of increased independence as well as the pline within the context of other parenting behav-
potential for conflictual relationships with parents iors (i.e., warmth), the family, and culture.
as peers and romantic partners become more
important (e.g., Eisenberg et al. 2008). Parents of
adolescents utilize various disciplinary strategies to Authoritative Parenting
socialize their teenager’s behavior to be in accor-
dance with rules of conduct deemed safe, accept- Two frameworks for studying parenting have
able, and appropriate according to established evolved since the mid-1960s, and research
Disciplining 1017

camps have either focused on individual parenting levels of anxiety and depression (Steinberg
dimensions or broad typologies of parenting (for and Silk 2002). Alternatively, other parenting
reviews see McKee et al. 2008, 2013). The first behaviors and constellations (e.g., harsh or critical
framework, based on work by Schaefer (1965), parenting behaviors, indulgent-permissive typol-
identified three behavioral dimensions – warmth, ogies) have been linked with both internalizing
hostility, and behavioral control – as important (e.g., anxiety, depression) and externalizing (e.g.,
elements of parenting. Researchers have contin- aggression) problems (for a review see McKee
ued to explore the effects of these three salient et al. 2013).
parenting concepts on child and adolescent psy- Although individual parenting dimensions D
chopathology and healthy adaptation. The second (e.g., behavioral control) and constellations of
framework, based on work by Baumrind (1966, behavior (e.g., authoritative) are associated with
1967), advanced the field by focusing not on positive child outcomes, the form of these behav-
individual behaviors in isolation but rather on iors and their delivery must vary to match the
three constellations of parenting behaviors developmental age and stage of the child
consisting of various combinations of warmth (Forehand and Wierson 1993). During the adoles-
and control labeled authoritative, authoritarian, cent years, children have the capacity to question
and permissive. While Baumrind’s approach to authority and seek psychological autonomy. As a
studying parenting was novel in that she catego- consequence, authoritative parenting includes
rized parents according to their behaviors regard- verbal give-and-take between the parent and
ing both control and warmth, the parenting teen, delineation of clear expectations, enforce-
concepts she highlighted were remarkably similar ment of rules when appropriate, and the encour-
to the warmth and behavior control dimensions agement of independence (Holmbeck et al. 1995;
(e.g., parental expectations, consequences, and also see Holmbeck et al. 2010). In essence,
monitoring) being studied by Schaefer and others. authoritative parenting for teenagers provides the
Maccoby and Martin (1983) then modified and child with direction but allows considerable free-
expanded Baumrind’s original system into a dom within broad limits.
schema in which parenting could be classified
into four constellations, again based on different
combinations of warmth and behavioral control Discipline Procedures with Adolescents
(i.e., discipline): authoritarian-autocratic (i.e., low
levels of warmth and high levels of control), Background and overview. Drawing from the par-
indulgent-permissive (i.e., high levels of warmth ent training literature, several specific parenting
and low levels of control), indifferent-uninvolved skills identified as components of an authoritative
(i.e., low levels of both warmth and control), and parenting style have become part of clinical inter-
authoritative-reciprocal (i.e., high levels of both ventions for adolescents struggling with behavior
warmth and control). Both the dimensional problems (e.g., Barkley and Robin 2014; Forgatch
approach and the typological approach to the and Patterson 1998; Patterson and Forgatch 2005;
study of parenting have yielded research germane Wells et al. 2008). Successful programs promote
to healthy child and adolescent development and positive parenting skills (i.e., warmth, sensitivity,
psychopathology. For example, each of the four realistic rules that are monitored), including effec-
parenting styles has been associated with different tive discipline strategies, while also attempting to
developmental outcomes for teens. Most notable, eliminate negative parenting skills (e.g., harsh
Steinberg’s (2001) review of 25 years of research criticism, guilt induction, physical discipline).
on parenting adolescents and a recent update by The intervention literature provides evidence
Baumrind and colleagues (2010) both conclude regarding which parenting skills and behaviors
that an authoritative parenting style is associated constitute effective discipline; as we will note
with positive outcomes for adolescents, including below, experimental manipulation of parenting
academic success, social skillfulness, and lower behavior in intervention programs is a particularly
1018 Disciplining

powerful tool when attempting to determine 2002; Dishion and McMahon 1998; Holmbeck
which disciplinary tactics promote adolescent et al. 1995).
development. Before turning to a more in-depth consider-
The parent behavior management literature, ation of each of the three components of disci-
which addresses skills for parents of children pline, a recent review of parenting behaviors that
ranging from early childhood (e.g., McMahon have been identified as mediators in parenting
and Forehand 2003) through middle childhood programs is worthy of note. Among other parent-
(e.g., Kazdin 2008) and adolescence (Patterson ing behaviors, Forehand et al. (2014a) examined
and Forgatch 2005), delineates three components constructs they labeled as discipline (e.g., lax,
of discipline that are typically implemented in consistent versus inconsistent), which would be
stages. First, clearly defined rules must be devel- consequences in our framework, and monitoring
oped. Parents who provide clear rules and expec- (e.g., awareness of activity and location of youth).
tations for their teens give them the opportunity to These authors found that the construct of disci-
succeed: When adolescents know the expecta- pline (i.e., consequences for inappropriate behav-
tions of their parents (e.g., telling their parents ior) had the most support as a mediator (i.e.,
where they will be going for the evening), they accounted for youth behavior change as a result
are more likely to meet that expectation. Also, of parent participation in a parenting program),
having clear expectations allows for psychologi- whereas monitoring alone received the least sup-
cal autonomy to develop as parents can provide port. These findings suggest that parental conse-
increasingly more freedom (e.g., the teen can quences are a key component to effective
choose where to go as long as the parents are discipline; nevertheless, we would advocate that
informed). Clear and appropriate expectations clearly defined rules and expectations as well as
are related to increased self-esteem, academic monitoring are necessary components for disci-
functioning, and behavioral compliance (e.g., pline to be most effective with adolescents.
Patterson and Forgatch 2005; also see Holmbeck We now turn to an examination of each of the
et al. 1995, for a review). components of discipline. We first examine how
The second component of discipline involves each of these components changes as a function of
consequences, which are tied to the adherence and the child’s age and then focus on the adolescent
violation of the rules established by parents. Allo- years.
wing a teenager to earn rewards for appropriate Rules and Expectations. Regardless of devel-
behavior and/or experience negative conse- opmental age or stage, the three components of
quences for inappropriate behavior increases pos- discipline – clearly defined rules and expectations,
itive behaviors while reducing negative ones (e.g., contingencies for complying with or breaking the
Wells et al. 2008). There is a long history of rules, and sufficient parental monitoring – have
research to support the role of parental positive been shown to decrease negative behaviors and
and negative consequences in promoting child promote positive behaviors of children. However,
and adolescent positive outcomes and decreasing the implementation of effective discipline changes
problem behaviors (e.g., see McMahon et al. 2006, as children progress through various developmen-
for a review of child conduct problems). tal stages; as a result, sensitivity and thoughtful-
The final component of discipline is monitor- ness regarding the delivery of discipline is integral
ing an adolescent’s behavior. Monitoring is nec- to successful positive parenting. Consider house
essary to ensure that rules are being followed and rules as an example of parental expectations. As
allows parents the opportunity to provide conse- with younger children, having a limited number of
quences for the child’s behavior. Research has clearly articulated rules is important for teenagers:
shown monitoring to be related to higher levels A child’s opportunities for success are enhanced
of self-esteem, lower levels of delinquent behav- by allowing her or him to fully understand and
ior, and lower rates of susceptibility to peer remember the rules. Additionally, parents can
pressure (for reviews, see Crouter and Head more effectively monitor fewer rules and enforce
Disciplining 1019

them consistently. However, the process of gener- up rewards for an adolescent; see Galeazzi
ating these rules is different for adolescents versus et al. 1988, for an example of a survey to identify
younger children. Adolescents no longer accept rewards for adolescents).
“just because” as a justification for the existence Before delineating appropriate consequences,
of rules. Rather, they wish to understand, or be it is important to note which disciplinary strategies
convinced of, the importance of household limi- should be avoided because they are, at best, inef-
tations (Forehand and Wierson 1993) and thus fective, and at worst, harmful to adolescents. First,
require a more developed rationale for house corporal punishment is not recommended for use
rules from their parents. It is in this realm that with a child of any age. Corporal punishment has D
the give-and-take component of authoritative par- been defined as “the use of physical force with the
enting is important. House rules need to be nego- intention of causing a child to experience pain but
tiated and agreed upon by both parent and teen in not injury for the purposes of correction or control
order to increase the likelihood of compliance and of the child’s behavior” (Straus 1994, p. 4). In a
to facilitate the development of adolescent inde- review of 88 studies, Gershoff (2002) examined
pendence (Forehand and Weirson 1993; Patterson the relation between corporal punishment and
and Forgatch 2005). Both parents and teens must immediate and long-term outcomes for children
acquire effective communication skills (e.g., be a and adolescents. Although corporal punishment
good listener, paraphrase back what has been was related to immediate compliance, it was also
heard, honestly and calmly express feelings) and related to numerous negative short-term (e.g.,
problem-solving skills (e.g., define the problem, aggression) and long-term (e.g., mental health
generate solutions, evaluate solutions, select problems) outcomes. Gershoff (2002) concluded
agreed upon option, implement solution, evaluate that corporal punishment has many adverse con-
implementation of solution) to reach compro- sequences that mitigate the short-term benefit of
mises that are at least minimally acceptable to immediate compliance. Subsequent research has
both parties (Barkley and Robin 2014; Forgatch supported these conclusions (e.g., Callender
and Patterson 2005). et al. 2012; Ferguson 2013). It is important to
Rewards and Consequences. In addition to note that consequences do not have to be physical
changes in the generation of house rules, enforce- to lead to negative outcomes for children. For
ment of rules must also evolve for adolescents. The example, McKee et al. (2007) found that harsh
authoritative parent provides a teenager with verbal criticism was related to young adolescents’
age-appropriate rewards for clearly defined desir- behavior problems, even after positive parenting
able behaviors and age-appropriate negative con- was taken into account. Furthermore, Rakow
sequences for clearly defined undesirable et al. (2009) reported a positive relation between
behaviors. For younger children (i.e., 2–7 year parental guilt induction and child internalizing
olds), rewards often consist of praise or attention problems such that children whose parents uti-
from parents, while time-out is used as the primary lized unwarranted, inappropriate blame and dec-
consequence for negative behaviors (Forehand larations of disappointment more often were more
and Long 2010; McMahon and Forehand 2003). likely to evidence higher levels of internalizing
Although parent attention remains important for symptoms. And, encouragingly, McKee
teens, it is often not sufficient because peers et al. (2014) found that a family-based preventive
increase in importance and tangible objects (e.g., intervention effectively reduced parent use of
iPod, cell phones) take on more value (Forehand guilt induction, leading to a reduction in internal-
and Weirson 1993). Therefore, identifying adoles- izing problems.
cent age-appropriate rewards and consequences is As noted, time-out is the disciplinary strategy
critical. The following discussion focuses on neg- of choice for young children (Forehand and Long
ative consequences for inappropriate behaviors as 2010; McMahon and Forehand 2003) and is also a
these are typically considered “disciplinary strate- component of some parenting intervention pro-
gies” (see Barkley and Robin 2014, for how to set grams for children as old as 12 years (Wells
1020 Disciplining

et al. 2008) and even up through the teen years to date) may be particularly salient and motivat-
(Barkley and Robin 2014). However, when used ing. As such, removal of these reinforcers can
with 10–12 year olds, the procedures are modified serve as another disciplinary strategy. Of course,
substantially in terms of length and location of the it is important once again to consider what is
time-out. For example, instead of a three-minute realistic: Extended “grounding” of a teenager is
time-out in a chair in a corner as suggested for not only aversive for the parent living with the
young children (McMahon and Forehand 2003), adolescent but also negatively impacts the parent-
Wells et al. (2008) recommends 10–20 min time- teen relationship and reduces opportunities for
outs in a room devoid of entertainment for older positive and normative social development with
children, and Barkley and Robin (2008) state that peers (see Barkley and Robin 2008, for a discus-
time-out for adolescents “means telling your teen sion on grounding).
to leave the room or sit down and be quiet until he In order for consequences to be effective, the
cools down” (p. 182). Thus, “traditional” time-out way they are executed is also important. First,
(McMahon and Forehand 2003) is not appropriate consistency is essential (Barkley and Robin
or recommended for use with teenagers. Not sur- 2014). Parents should not state consequences,
prisingly, as the value of parental attention either positive or negative, that they are unwilling
decreases, time-out from such attention is less to impose, monitor, and carry through to comple-
effective and other consequences should be tion! Second, parents should administer conse-
considered. quences calmly. Parents have been encouraged
In their seminal treatment manual for parents to become like a “brick wall” or a “robot” when
of adolescents with behavior problems, Patterson delivering discipline so that they are able to
and Forgatch (1987) asserted, “For parents with calmly state and consistently enforce the rules
adolescents, the five-minute work chore is without being drawn into bickering (Hembree-
clearly the punishment of choice” (p. 189). This Kigin and McNeil 2007). Third, Patterson and
conclusion had not changed 18 years later as Forgatch (2005) highlight the importance of not
Patterson and Forgatch made the same assertion lecturing as a source of discipline. While lecturing
in 2005 (p. 143). The five-minute work chore may intuitively seem to parents to elucidate the
(i.e., short tasks to be completed at home in rationale for a given punishment, it actually pro-
addition to an adolescent’s typical responsibili- vides attention for inappropriate negative behav-
ties) is given immediately for failure to comply iors, is often used to induce guilt, and, particularly
with established rules or for engaging in clearly with adolescents, encourages arguments. Rather
defined inappropriate behavior. Another exam- than lecturing when discipline is imposed,
ple of an appropriate disciplinary strategy for Patterson and Forgatch (2005) recommend stating
teens is removal of a privilege for a period of the rule that was violated, imposing the conse-
time (Wells et al. 2008). For example, computer quence (e.g., work chore), and discussing the rea-
or cell phone usage could be removed. The sons for the consequence at a later time. This
length of time such privileges are lost can depend eliminates the role of attention and arguments
on the severity of rule violating behavior when negative consequences are used and pro-
displayed by the adolescent; however, being vides a chance for parents and adolescents to
realistic about the length of time is critical: discuss house rules later in a calm give-and-take
Removing a privilege “until you are no longer environment that bolsters the parent-child alli-
living in my home” is not only very unlikely to ance. Fourth, parents should carefully plan how
be enforced but also negatively impacts the they will talk to and implement discipline with
parent-adolescent relationship. their adolescent. Planning can be a key to effective
Because adolescents value the opinion of and use of consequences. Finally, use of the commu-
access to peers (e.g., Chan et al. 2012; Forehand nication and problem-solving skills we delineated
and Wierson 1993), social reinforcers (e.g., later earlier is critical (also see Barkley and Robin
curfew, permission to go to certain parties, ability 2008; Forgatch and Patterson 2005).
Disciplining 1021

Monitoring. The third component of discipline, have the cognitive and emotional skills to guide
monitoring, also changes as children age. When their own behavior. When parents gradually turn
children are younger, parents have ready access to over monitoring and decision-making, adoles-
them and can monitor and supervise closely in cents have the opportunity to develop into self-
order to provide for safety and other basic needs. directed individuals who make responsible
In many ways, monitoring younger children is decisions.
easier than keeping tabs on adolescents, who are An emerging area of importance for monitor-
more mobile and independent. As a rule of thumb, ing that deserves mention concerns adolescent
parents of teens should consider the four W’s as technology use. Youth technology use (e.g., D
they monitor their adolescents: Who are you smartphones, television, video games, computers,
with?; What are you doing?; Where will you tablets) averages over 7 h per day (Rideout
be?; and When will you be home? Monitoring is et al. 2010), far exceeding the 2 h recommended
associated with more knowledge about youth by the American Academy of Pediatrics (2013).
activities, which can help parents deter adolescent Not only do adolescents have a higher usage than
problem behaviors (Criss et al. 2015). Although younger age groups, they also appear to be less
there has been debate about monitoring (Stattin shaped by general and technology-specific parent-
and Kerr 2000), evidence (e.g., Lahey et al. 2008) ing behaviors (Sanders et al. in press-a, b).
continues to provide strong support for this par- Although some research suggests that parent
enting behavior being important for healthy ado- mediation (e.g., restrictions, discussion of con-
lescent development. Furthermore, monitoring is tent) of youth media consumption impacts usage
becoming recognized as a complex behavior and influences behavior and emotional outcomes
consisting not only of the four W’s (i.e., knowl- (Vaala and Bleakely 2015), other data suggest that
edge about the adolescent) but also of parental parental restrictions may not eliminate online risk
involvement with the adolescent and adolescent (i.e., Livingstone and Helsper 2008) and in some
openness about her/his activities (e.g., Criss cases may be associated with increased electronic
et al. 2015). As a component of discipline, moni- communication (i.e., Len-Ríos et al. 2015). The
toring is an important, but as yet understudied and development of parenting programs for teaching
not sufficiently understood, construct. parents how to monitor and control their adoles-
Regardless of the complexity of this construct, cents’ technology represents a critical need as our
it is important for parents to be clear with adoles- society moves forward in the digital age.
cents about what the four W’s involve and what
their expectations are for the adolescent. These
should be reviewed with the adolescent before The Context of Discipline
she or he departs from the house. Furthermore, if
one or more of the four W’s is violated (e.g., an The context in which discipline occurs is impor-
adolescent goes somewhere other than agreed tant in determining its effectiveness. We will con-
upon or does not come home on time), then one sider three contexts as examples: other parenting
of the consequences discussed previously should behavior (i.e., warmth), the family, and culture.
be implemented. Warmth. Implementation of the three steps of
For adolescents, monitoring also can include discipline in the appropriate fashion is necessary,
having the teenager involved in the implementa- but not sufficient, to produce positive parenting:
tion and self-reinforcement of rules and conse- Discipline is only one of the two components of
quences by letting them take responsibility for authoritative parenting. Discipline must occur
monitoring some of their own behaviors within the context of a high quality parent-child
(Forehand and Weirson 1993). If parents continue interaction (Patterson and Forgatch 1987), and in
throughout the adolescent years to maintain all order to create an atmosphere conducive to a pos-
control, successful development of internal con- itive relationship, parents must be attentive to, and
trol can be hindered. Adolescents increasingly supportive of, their adolescents (Forehand and
1022 Disciplining

Wierson 1993). One specific parenting skill, paren- et al. 2000, for a review) and subsequent youth
tal warmth, defined by McLeod et al. (2007) as “a problem behaviors (for reviews, see England and
sense of positive regard expressed by the parent Sim 2009; Goodman et al. 2011). Fortunately,
toward the child, pleasant interactions shared preventive intervention programs, which include
between the parent and child, or parenting involve- a parenting component, are being developed and
ment in children’s activities” (p. 161), has been evaluated (e.g., Compas et al. 2009, 2011), includ-
shown to be associated with positive mental health ing which components of the program are critical
outcomes for youth (Masten and Coatsworth 1998; for adolescent outcomes. For example, Compas
McKee et al. 2008, 2013). When considered in and his colleagues have found that parenting is
combination with discipline, warmth is particu- one mediator of adolescent behavior change (e.g.,
larly important. For example, recent research has Compas et al. 2010).
shown that low levels of warmth plus harsh disci- Although parental depression is a major influ-
pline is especially detrimental for adolescents ence in how parents raise their children and ado-
(Wang et al. 2015). lescents, it is by no means the only parent
Like discipline techniques, methods of “adjustment” variable that can influence parent-
expressing warmth evolve as a child becomes an ing. Parent ADHD symptoms, anxiety, excessive
adolescent. While in early childhood warmth may alcohol use, and physical illness have all been
be expressed verbally through attending to or found to influence parenting, including the use
praising a child’s behavior or by affectionate of discipline (e.g., see Johnston et al. 2012, for a
appropriate touches (McMahon and Forehand review of parent ADHD).
2003), in adolescence it becomes more complex The family context of parenting goes beyond
and often subtle. For example, ongoing parental the parents’ own personal “adjustment” as the
attention is not desirable for most teens: rather, a relationship between the two caregivers can
parent who is available to talk at a moment’s shape parenting. It is important to note that when
notice, attends performances (e.g., sporting events, both caregivers are parents (whether biological or
theater productions) by the adolescent, and allows nonbiological), the context may be one of a mar-
an adolescent to choose some family activities ried, cohabiting, divorced, or remarried family.
(e.g., which movie to watch or restaurant to eat) Regardless of this context or the gender of the
is displaying an age-appropriate warmth which parents, a substantial literature indicates that inter-
will facilitate the parent-teen relationship. Without personal conflict, particularly when it occurs in
warmth, discipline will hinder the development of front of, or involves, the youth, is detrimental (for
a positive parent-adolescent relationship and ado- reviews see Buehler et al. 1997; Evans et al. 2008;
lescent outcome (e.g., Lowe and Dotterer 2013). Jouriles et al. 1991; also see Peisch et al. in press),
Therefore, discipline should be reserved for a partially because conflict disrupts parenting (e.g.,
select few behaviors and imposed only for a short use of effective discipline). Such data highlight the
period of time within the context of a parent-child need to create effective interventions for families
relationship dominated by positive reinforcement affected by adult conflict (see Reynolds et al. 2014).
and support (Patterson and Forgatch 2005). Beyond identifying variables that can be dis-
Family. Discipline, as well as other parenting, ruptive to parenting behaviors, such as discipline,
occurs within the context of the family. Substan- recent research is beginning to focus on personal
tial attention in the research literature has been and interpersonal variables that promote effective
devoted to how parental and interparental “adjust- parenting. For example, mindful parenting (e.g.,
ment” can influence parenting and, in turn, youth present-centered attention during parent-child
outcome. For example, it has been estimated that interactions, regulation of reactivity to child’s
7.5 million parents will experience an episode of behavior) has been shown to enhance effective
depression during a child’s formative years parenting with youth ranging in age from 3 to
(England and Sim 2009), leading to negative par- 17 years old (Parent et al. 2016). When interper-
enting (e.g., harsh discipline) (see Lovejoy sonal variables are considered, substantial work
Disciplining 1023

has focused on the construct of coparenting (i.e., authoritative stance with regard to amount of con-
two adults jointly provide care for children) (see trol but with only moderate warmth and autonomy
McHale and Lindahl 2011) regardless of the fam- support (Baumrind et al. 2010; see also Sorkhabi
ily form (e.g., married, cohabiting) in which it and Mandara 2013, for a review). For example,
occurs. When parents effectively coparent harsh discipline has been related to detrimental
together, both are involved in daily childrearing outcomes for Chinese youth living in both the
activities and youth adaptive behavior is enhanced United States (e.g., Benner and Kim 2010), Taiwan
(e.g., Forehand et al. 2014b, 2015). (Wang et al. 2015) and China (Chang et al. 2003),
Culture. In addition to other parenting behav- while warmth has been associated with adaptive D
iors and the family system as important contexts outcomes for Chinese Canadians (Yoo and Miller
within which specific disciplinary strategies must 2011). Furthermore, a cross-cultural examination
be considered, the cultural norms of different of adolescent youth in twelve cultures has validated
racial, ethnic, and socioeconomic groups may extant measures of positive parenting and suggests
shape parent socialization goals. Harkness and that such parenting behaviors are associated with
Super (2006), for example, suggest that parent beneficial youth outcomes around the globe
ethnotheories about how best to raise children (McNeely and Barber 2010).
are informed by the larger culture. Although Given increasing globalization and the
decades of research have replicated a robust core diverse makeup of US society, clinicians must
of findings regarding the impact of positive par- be aware of cultural values that may impact par-
enting behaviors such as warmth and behavioral enting ethnotheories and behaviors. Forehand
control on youth social and emotional health, and Kotchick (1996) originally called for
scholars have questioned whether research, largely therapists and researchers to investigate the
conducted in the West with European American effectiveness of cultural adaptations of parenting
families, is equally applicable to families from interventions. More recently, Ortiz and Del
different ethnic and cultural backgrounds. Vecchio (2013) revisited these concerns and rec-
Some research has suggested that authoritative ommendations. Although they found that the
parenting may not be as critical for youth well- diversity of families included in parent training
being as hypothesized, depending upon the context research had increased and that a number of
in which it is delivered. In her oft-cited papers, culturally tailored interventions had been devel-
Chao (1994, 2001), for instance, makes the case oped; unfortunately, they also reported that such
that Chinese parenting, which she describes as adaptations have not been tested against the orig-
“training” youth to engage in socially appropriate inal versions and thus fail to provide evidence of
behavior, is deployed and interpreted in a collec- superiority.
tivist context that values complete obedience and
deference to family and authority. As such, she and
others (i.e., Rudy and Grusec 2006) indicate that Conclusions
parenting behaviors considered controlling or puni-
tive through a more individualist lens may not be As is evident, discipline is not a simple construct.
perceived as negative, harsh, or lacking warmth by It requires an awareness of an adolescent’s devel-
Chinese youth and parents. In other words, if the opmental level, thoughtful planning around
intent is benign and the parenting behaviors judged expectations and rules, and a commitment to
as reasonable, then the youth developmental out- implementing appropriate consequences and
comes will be positive even when warmth is low. monitoring adolescent behavior. Finally, disci-
This notion has been challenged by recent empiri- pline should be implemented in the context of
cal work and by those arguing a cultural equiva- parental warmth to promote healthy adolescent
lence model. This perspective claims that youth in adaptation and parental use of discipline and per-
all cultural contexts benefit from authoritative and haps its effectiveness depends in part on the fam-
directive parenting, which is equivalent to an ily and cultural context.
1024 Disciplining

References families of depressed parents. Journal of Consulting


and Clinical Psychology, 78, 623–634.
American Academy of Pediatrics. (2013). Children, ado- Compas, B. E., Forehand, R., Champion, J. E., Keller,
lescents, and the media. Pediatrics, 132, 958–961. G. Hardcastle, E. J., Cole, D. A., . . . Roberts,
Barkley, R. A., & Robin, A. L. (2008). Your defiant teen: L. (2011). Family group cognitive-behavioral preven-
10 steps to resolve conflict and rebuild your relation- tive intervention for families of depressed parents: 18-
ship. New York, NY: Guilford Press. and 24-month outcomes. Journal of Consulting and
Barkley, R. A., & Robin, A. L. (2014). Defiant teens: Clinical Psychology, 79, 488–499.
A clinician’s manual for assessment and family inter- Criss, M. M., Lee, T. K., Morris, A. S., Cui, L., Bosler,
vention (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Publications. C. D., Shreffler, K. M., & Silk, J. S. (2015). Link
Baumrind, D. (1966). Effects of authoritative parental con- between monitoring behavior and adolescent adjust-
trol on child behavior. Child Development, 37, ment: An analysis of direct and indirect effects. Journal
887–907. of Child and Family Studies, 24(3), 668–678.
Baumrind, D. (1967). Child care practices anteceding three Crouter, A. C., & Head, M. R. (2002). Parental monitoring
patterns of preschool behavior. Genetic Psychological and knowledge of children. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.),
Monographs, 75, 43–88. Handbook of parenting (Vol. 3, pp. 461–483).
Baumrind, D., Larzelere, R. E., & Owens, E. B. (2010). Mahway: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Effects of preschool parents’ power assertive patterns Dishion, T. J., & McMahon, R. J. (1998). Parental monitor-
and practices on adolescent development. Parenting: ing and the prevention of child and adolescent problem
Science and Practice, 10, 157–201. behavior: A conceptual and empirical formulation. Clin-
Benner, A. D., & Kim, S. Y. (2010). Understanding Chi- ical Child and Family Psychology Review, 1, 61–75.
nese American adolescents’ developmental outcomes: Eisenberg, N., Hofer, C., Spinrad, T. L., Gershoff, E. T.,
Insights from the family stress model. Journal of Valiente, C., Losoya, S., & Maxon, E. (2008). Under-
Research on Adolescence, 20, 1–12. standing mother-adolescent conflict discussion: Con-
Buehler, C., Anthony, C., Krishnakumar, A., Stone, G., current and across-time prediction from youths’
Gerard, J., & Pemberton, S. (1997). Interparental con- dispositions and parenting. Monographs of the Society
flict and youth problem behaviors: A meta-analysis. for Research in Child Development, 73(2), 1–160.
Journal of Child and Family Studies, 6(2), 233–247. England, M. J., & Sim, L. J. (Eds.). (2009). Depression in
Callender, K. A., Olson, S. L., Choe, D. E., & Sameroff, parents, parenting, and children: Opportunities to
A. J. (2012). The effects of parental depressive symp- improve identification, treatment, and prevention.
toms, appraisals, and physical punishment on later Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
child externalizing behavior. Journal of Abnormal Evans, S. E., Davies, C., & DiLillo, D. (2008). Exposure to
Child Psychology, 40(3), 471–483. domestic violence: A meta-analysis of child and ado-
Chan, D. K., Lonsdale, C., & Fung, H. H. (2012). Influ- lescent outcomes. Aggression and Violent Behavior,
ences of coaches, parents, and peers on the motivational 13(2), 131–140.
patterns of child and adolescent athletes. Scandinavian Ferguson, C. J. (2013). Spanking, corporal punishment and
Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 22(4), negative long-term outcomes: A meta-analytic review
558–568. of longitudinal studies. Clinical Psychology Review,
Chang, L., Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., & McBride- 33(1), 196–208.
Chang, C. (2003). Harsh parenting in relation to child Forehand, R., & Kotchick, B. A. (1996). Cultural diversity:
emotion regulation and aggression. Journal of Family A wake-up call for parent training. Behavior Therapy,
Psychology, 17, 598–606. 27(2), 187–206.
Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authori- Forehand, R., & Long, N. (2010). Parenting the strong-
tarian parenting style: Understanding: Chinese parent- willed child (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw Hill.
ing through the cultural notion of training. Child Forehand, R., & McKinney, B. (1993). A historical over-
Development, 65, 1111–1120. view of child discipline in the United States. Journal of
Chao, R. K. (2001). Extending research on the conse- Child and Family Studies, 2, 219–226.
quences of parenting style for Chinese Americans and Forehand, R., & Wierson, M. (1993). The role of develop-
European Americans. Child Development, 72, mental factors in planning behavioral interventions:
1832–1843. Disruptive behavior as an example. Behavior Therapy,
Compas, B. E., Forehand, R., Keller, G., Champion, J. E., 22, 671–688.
Rakow, A., Reeslund, K. L, . . .. Cole, D. A. (2009). Forehand, R., Lafko, N., Parent, J., & Burt, K. B. (2014a).
Randomized controlled trial of family cognitive- Is parenting the mediator of change in behavioral parent
behavioral preventive intervention for children of training for externalizing problems of youth? Clinical
depressed parents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology Review, 34(8), 608–619.
Psychology, 77, 1007–1020. Forehand, R., Parent, J., Golub, A., & Reid, M. (2014b).
Compas, B. E., Champion, Forehand, R., J. E., Cole, D. A., Correlates of male cohabiting partner’s involvement in
Reeslund, K. L., Fear, J., . . . Roberts, L. (2010). Medi- childrearing tasks in an inner-city African American sam-
ators of 12-month outcomes of a family group ple of families. Journal of Family Psychology, 28(3),
cognitive-behavioral preventive intervention with 336–345.
Disciplining 1025

Forehand, R., Parent, J., Golub, A., Reid, M., & Lafko, N. Len-Ríos, M. E., Hughes, H. E., McKee, L. G., & Young,
(2015). Involvement in child rearing and firm control H. N. (2015). Early adolescents as publics: A national
parenting by male cohabiting partners in black survey of teens with social media accounts, their media
low-income stepfamilies forecasting adolescent prob- use preferences, parental mediation, and perceived
lem behaviors. Behavior Modification, 39(5), 654–669. internet literacy. Public Relations Review, 42, 101–108.
Forgatch, M. S., & Patterson, G. R. (1998). Behavioral Livingstone, S., & Helsper, E. J. (2008). Parental media-
family therapy. In F. M. Dattilio (Ed.), Case studies in tion of children’s internet use. Journal of Broadcasting
couple and family therapy: Systematic and cognitive and Electronic Media, 52, 581–599.
perspectives (pp. 85–107). New York: Guilford Press. Lovejoy, M. C., Graczyk, P. A., O’Hare, E., & Neuman, G.
Forgatch, M. S., & Patterson, G. R. (2005). Parents and (2000). Maternal depression and parenting behavior:
adolescents living together, Part 2: Family problem A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, D
solving (2nd ed.). Champlain: Research Press. 20(5), 561–592.
Galeazzi, A., Franceschina, E., Cautela, J., Holmes, G. R., Lowe, K., & Dotterer, A. M. (2013). Parental monitoring,
& Sakano, Y. (1998). A comparison of Italian, Japanese parental warmth, and minority youths’ academic out-
and American students’ responses to the Adolescent comes: Exploring the integrative model of parenting.
Reinforcement Survey Schedule. Journal of Clinical Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42(9), 1413–1425.
Psychology, 54(2), 267–278. Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the
Gershoff, E. T. (2002). Corporal punishment by parents context of the family: Parent–child interaction. In
and associated child behaviors and experiences: A E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychol-
meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological ogy: Socialization, personality, and social development
Bulletin, 128(4), 539–579. (Vol. 4, pp. 1–102). New York: Wiley.
Goodman, S. H., Rouse, M. H., Connell, A. M., Broth, Masten, A. S., & Coatsworth, D. (1998). The development
M. R., Hall, C. M., & Heyward, D. (2011). Maternal of competence in favorable and unfavorable environ-
depression and child psychopathology: A meta- ments: Lessons learned on successful children. Ameri-
analytic review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology can Psychologist, 53, 205–220.
Review, 14(1), 1–27. McHale, J. P., & Lindahl, K. M. (Eds.). (2011).
Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (2006). Themes and varia- Coparenting: A conceptual and clinical examination
tions: Parental ethnotheories in western cultures. In of family systems. Washington, DC: American Psycho-
K. H. Rubin & O. Boon Chung (Eds.), Parental beliefs, logical Association.
parenting, and child development in cross-cultural per- McKee, L., Roland, E., Coffelt, N., Massari, C., Olson,
spective (pp. 61–80). New York: Psychology Press. A. L., Forehand, R., Jones, D., Gaffney, C. A., & Zens,
Hembree-Kigin, T. L., & McNeil, C. B. (2007). M. S. (2007). Harsh discipline and child problem
Parent–child interaction therapy. New York: Springer. behavior: The roles of positive parenting and gender.
Holmbeck, G. N., Paikoff, R. L., & Brooks-Gunn, Journal of Family Violence, 22, 187–196.
J. (1995). Parenting adolescents. In M. H. Bornstein McKee, L., Colletti, C., Rakow, A., Jones, D. J., &
(Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1: Children and Forehand, R. (2008). Parenting and child externalizing
parenting (pp. 91–118). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum behaviors: Are the associations specific or diffuse?
Associates, Inc. Aggression and Violent Behavior: A Review Journal,
Holmbeck, G. N., Devine, K. A., & Bruno, E. F. (2010). 13, 201–215.
Developmental issues and considerations in research McKee, L. G., Jones, D. J., Forehand, R., & Cuellar,
and practice. In J. R. Weisz & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), J. (2013). Assessment of parenting style, parenting
Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and ado- relationships, and other parenting variables. In
lescents (2nd ed., pp. 28–39). New York: The Guilford D. H. Saklofske, C. R. Reynolds, & V. Schwean (Eds.),
Press. The Oxford handbook of child psychological assessment
Johnston, C., Mash, E. J., Miller, N., & Ninowski, J. E. (pp. 788–821). New York: Oxford University Press.
(2012). Parenting in adults with attention-deficit/ McKee, L. G., Parent, J., Forehand, R., Rakow, A., Wat-
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Clinical Psychology son, K. H., Dunbar, J. P., . . . & Compas, B. E. (2014).
Review, 32(4), 215–228. Reducing youth internalizing symptoms: Effects of a
Jouriles, E. N., Farris, A. M., & McDonald, R. (1991). family-based preventive intervention on parental guilt
Marital functioning and child behavior: Measuring spe- induction and youth cognitive style. Development and
cific aspects of the marital relationship. In J. P. Vincent Psychopathology, 26(02), 319–332.
(Ed.), Advances in family intervention, assessment and McLeod, B. D., Wood, J. J., & Weisz, J. R. (2007). Exam-
theory (pp. 25–46). London: Jessica Kingsley Limited. ining the association between parenting and childhood
Kazdin, A. E. (2008). The Kazdin method for parenting the anxiety: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review,
defiant child. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 27, 155–172.
Lahey, B. B., Van Hulle, C. A., D’Onofrio, B. M., Rodgers, McMahon, R. J., & Forehand, R. L. (2003). Helping the
J. L., & Waldman, I. D. (2008). Is parental knowledge of noncompliant child: Family-based treatment for oppo-
their adolescent offspring’s whereabouts and peer associ- sitional behavior (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.
ations spuriously associated with offspring delinquency? McMahon, R. J., Wells, K. C., & Kotler, J. S. (2006).
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 807–823. Conduct problems. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley
1026 Discrimination

(Eds.), Treatment of childhood disorders Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2000). Parental monitoring:
(pp. 137–268). New York: Guilford. A reinterpretation. Child Development, 71, 1072–1085.
McNeely, C. A., & Barber, B. K. (2010). How do parents Steinberg, L. (2001). We know some things: Parent–child
make adolescents feel loved? Perspectives on support- relationships in retrospect and prospect. Journal of
ive parenting from adolescents in 12 cultures. Journal Research on Adolescence, 11, 1–19.
of Adolescent Research, 25, 601–631. Steinberg, L., & Silk, J. S. (2002). Parenting adolescents. In
Ortiz, C., & Del Vecchio, T. (2013). Cultural diversity: Do M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: vol. 1:
we need a new wake-up call for parent training? Behav- Children and parenting (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 103–133).
ior Therapy, 44(3), 443–458. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Parent, J., McKee, L. G., Rough, J. N., & Forehand, R. Straus, M. A. (1994). Beating the devil out of them: Cor-
(2016). The association of parent mindfulness with poral punishment in American families. New York:
parenting and youth psychopathology across three Lexington Books.
developmental stages. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy- Vaala, S. E., & Bleakely, A. (2015). Monitoring, mediat-
chology, 44(1), 191–202. ing, and modeling: Parental influence on adolescent
Patterson, G., & Forgatch, M. (1987). Parents and adoles- computer and internet use in the United States. Journal
cents: Living together, part 1: The basics (2nd ed.). of Children and Media, 9, 40–57.
Champaign: Research Press. Wang, Y.-C. L., Chan, H.-Y., Lin, C.-W., & Li, J.-R.
Patterson, G., & Forgatch, M. (2005). Parents and adoles- (2015). Associations of parental warmth and harsh
cents: Living together, part 1: The basics (2nd ed.). discipline with developmental trajectories of depres-
Champaign: Research Press. sive symptoms among adolescents in Chinese society.
Peisch, V. D., Forehand, R., Parent, J., Golub, A., & Reid, Journal of Family Psychology, 29, 895–906.
M., & Price, M. (in press). Intimate partner violence in Wells, K. C., Lochman, J. E., & Lenhart, L. A. (2008).
cohabiting families: Reports by multiple informants Coping power: Parent group program. Oxford: Oxford
and associations with adolescent outcomes. Journal of University Press.
Family Violence. Yoo, C., & Miller, L. D. (2011). Culture and parenting:
Rakow, A., Forehand, R., McKee, L., Coffelt, N., Cham- Psychological adjustment among Chinese Canadian
pion, J., & Compas, B. (2009). The relation of parental adolescents. Canadian Journal of Counseling and Psy-
guilt induction to child internalizing problems when a chotherapy, 45, 34–52.
caregiver has a history of depression. Journal of Child
and Family Studies, 18, 367–377.
Reynolds, J., Houlston, C., Coleman, L., & Harold, G.
(2014). Parental conflict: Outcomes and interventions
for children and families. Chicago: Policy Press.
Rideout, V. J., Foehr, U. G., & Roberts, D. F. (2010). Discrimination
Generation M2: Media in the Lives of 8-to 18-Year-
Olds. Menlo Park: Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation. Roger J. R. Levesque
Rudy, D., & Grusec, J. E. (2006). Authoritarian parenting Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
in individualist and collectivist groups: Associations
with maternal emotion and cognition and children’s
self-esteem. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 68–78.
Sanders, W., Parent, J., Forehand, R., Jones, D., & While it is common to prefer members of an indi-
Sullivan, A. (in press-a). Parental perceptions of tech- vidual’s own group, discrimination involves the
nology and technology-focused parenting: Associa-
tions with youth screen time and psychosocial unfair treatment of others based on their member-
adjustment. Journal of Applied Developmental ship to these inherent groups. Although individuals
Psychology. can opt to belong to groups that are discriminated
Sanders, W., Parent, J., Forehand, R., & Lafko, N. (in press- against, often members belong to discriminated
b). The roles of general and technology-related parent-
ing in managing youth screen time. Journal of Family groups to which they do not choose to belong,
Psychology. such as those based on categories of race, gender,
Schaefer, E. S. (1965). A configurational analysis of chil- age, sexual orientation, and physical appearance.
dren’s reports of parent behavior. Journal of Consulting The extent to which categories, and individuals
Psychology, 29, 552–557.
Sorkhabi, N., & Mandara, J. (2013). Are the effects of within those categories, are discriminated against
Baumrind’s parenting styles culturally specific or cul- varies from society to society as well as from group
turally equivalent? In R. E. Larzelere, A. S. Morris, & to group depending on both broad social forces and
A. W. Harrist (Eds.), Authoritative parenting: Synthe- narrow individual circumstances.
sizing nurturance and discipline for optimal child
development. Washington, DC: American Psychology The variety of categories to which individuals
Assocation. can belong means that adolescents can be
Discrimination 1027

discriminated against for a variety of reasons. make important progress, but they tend to be the
They can be discriminated due to their age, gen- subject of criticism to the extent that they, for
der, race, sexual orientation, appearance, and so example, are not necessarily applicable to other
forth, all of which can vary considerably to the contexts and may not be readily obvious and sig-
extent that these factors can interact and have nificant enough to be seen as resulting in actual
different meanings in different contexts. A recent discrimination, which happens to be one of the key
study of adolescent immigrants in the United points about subtle forms of discrimination itself: it
States (Medvedeva 2010), for example, found is difficult to identify and, because of that, combat.
that adolescents who felt discriminated against Researchers may investigate discrimination on D
by school peers were more likely to report speak- an individual or societal level. At the individual
ing and reading English less than “very well.” level, individuals are likely to discriminate when
Those who did report speaking and reading they hold negative stereotypes of an entire social
English “very well” were more likely to feel dis- group and who are prejudiced toward them. These
criminated against by teachers and counselors at prejudices and negative views emerge from the
school and also reported perceived societal dis- need to increase self-esteem or out of a sense of
crimination. Other studies also have found, for moral judgment. At the societal level, social con-
example, a persistent negative effect of accultura- formity plays an important role. Social norms are
tion on the link between discrimination and ado- powerful motivators of action, such that members
lescents’ developmental outcomes, such that of a group being discriminated against sometimes
adolescents who are more acculturated experience adopt discriminatory views about their own social
more deleterious effects of discrimination on both group. Importantly, if individuals perceive that
socioemotional and academic outcomes (see discrimination is not a social norm, then they
Benner and Kim 2009). will not discriminate. Thus, interventions fre-
Discrimination appears on a continuum, rang- quently will focus on changing social norms in
ing from overt hostility directed at an entire group hopes of reducing discrimination.
to subtle social exclusion of an individual. Subtle One of the key factors relating to discrimina-
discrimination is the most prevalent form of dis- tion, and that helps to perpetuate it, involves the
crimination and commonly goes unnoticed by challenge of identifying it. Identifying discrimi-
members of the dominant social group. Indeed, nation may prove difficult, for example, due to
this is a form of discrimination that often goes multiple individual perceptions of any one event.
unnoticed because the discrimination that results In many instances, there may not be enough evi-
is purported to result from factors not related to the dence to prove a definite attribution, which can
specified discrimination, for example, individuals lead to a lack of perception and official reporting.
can be treated differently for belonging to a group, To further complicate matters, while members of
such as a racial group, but the reason for the differ- inferior social groups, such as women and minor-
ent treatment can be viewed as based on economic ities, report discrimination against their social
considerations (see Dovidio and Gaertner 2004). group, they do not report experiencing it person-
The challenge of identifying more subtle forms of ally (Crosby 1984). In addition, the reporting of
discrimination transfers to research. While explicit discrimination is difficult, and it is so for a variety
discrimination may be measured through surveys of reasons (see Stangor et al. 2003). Reporting
or document analysis, subtle forms of discrimina- may be difficult because it may be psychologi-
tion require sophisticated techniques. Researchers cally costly to victims who might have to come to
may measure verbal and nonverbal behavior, such terms with the unfair advantages for others. Social
as how much help a subject gives to another, how consequences to reporting discrimination, such as
long they converse with them, or how far away embarrassment, ostracism, and termination, might
they sit. Researchers may also use computer pro- also cause a victim to decide against reporting the
grams that measure subjects’ reaction times. These incident. Indeed, numerous factors have been
methods have helped the study of discrimination identified that relate to the likelihood of reporting
1028 Disease

discrimination, such as belief in a just world, need ▶ Racial and Ethnic Discrimination
for approval, need for control, identification with ▶ Racism
the in-group, stigma consciousness, and mood. In ▶ Segregation and Desegregation
addition, victims of discrimination may make
internal or external attributions for the discrimi-
References
nation. If an internal attribution is made, such that
the victim feels to blame for their victimization, Benner, A. D., & Kim, S. Y. (2009). Experiences of dis-
lowered self-esteem, increased self-blame, and crimination among Chinese American adolescents and
misidentification with the in-group may occur. the consequences for socioemotional and academic
Importantly, again, these effects vary depending development. Developmental Psychology, 45,
1682–1694.
on people’s own experiences and abilities, such as Crosby, F. J. (1984). The denial of personal discrimination.
their age or cognitive abilities (see Seaton 2010). The American Behavioral Scientist, 27, 371–386.
Although it may be challenging to identify dis- Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2004). Aversive Racism.
crimination and especially difficult to report some In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
psychology (pp. 1–52). San Diego: Academic.
instances of it, it is clear that discrimination may Medvedeva, M. (2010). Perceived discrimination and lin-
have important psychological consequences for guistic adaptation of adolescent children of immigrants.
victims, particularly if it is experienced frequently. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 940–952.
Individuals who feel discriminated against report Seaton, E. K. (2010). The influence of cognitive develop-
ment and perceived racial discrimination on the psy-
more psychological distress, depression, and lower chological well-being of African American youth.
levels of life satisfaction. At the societal level, Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 694–703.
discrimination can contribute to the high percent- Stangor, C., Swim, J. K., Sechrist, G. B., DeCoster, J., Van
age of minorities in poverty and their lack of access Allen, K. L., & Ottenbeit, A. (2003). Ask, answer and
announce: Three stages in perceiving and responding to
to higher paying jobs. Discrimination that contrib- discrimination. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.),
utes to a lack of access to resources also has been European review of social psychology (Vol.
linked to such negative outcomes as higher rates of 14, pp. 277–311). London: Psychology Press.
earlier mortality, access to less and poorer quality Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and
the intellectual test performance of African Americans.
health care, delayed diagnosis, and failure to man- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69,
age chronic diseases. In addition to these observ- 797–811.
able effects, discrimination also may have indirect
effects on individuals. Perceived past, present, or
future discrimination influences a variety of out-
comes. Individuals may alter the way they interact Disease
with others, or even inadvertently perform worse
on tasks, for example, stereotype threat occurs Roger J. R. Levesque
when an individual becomes anxious about perfor- Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
mance stereotypes, such as women’s not being
good at math, and thus performs worse than they
would have otherwise (Steele and Aronson 1995). Overview
Clearly, discrimination, both at the individual and
broader social level, has negative and long-term Disease generally refers to conditions harmful to
outcomes for its victims. an individual’s well-being or health. Although
different cultural definitions and conceptions of
disease have been proposed, in the context of
Cross-References adolescence the concept of disease most com-
monly adopts a medical focus, with the harm
▶ Affirmative Action most often taking physical and/or psychological
▶ Equal Access Act dimensions. Research on disease in adolescence
▶ Equal Protection Laws differs from research on adults in that it involves
Disease 1029

additional dynamics relating to treatment and treatment options and discourages blaming one-
decision-making because adolescents generally self in scenarios where there is really no one to
are under the authority of parents and caregivers. blame. This approach has important repercus-
Recognizing diseases and adherence/compliance sions. As an example, adopting a disease model
to treatment and the role of self-perception on can be beneficial in that it can be more psycho-
outcomes have been found to vary greatly logically reassuring than it would otherwise be
among adolescents; they also have been found to through other models (Meurk et al. 2014).
be affected by parents, peers, and even how dis- Despite the medical model’s important
ease is explained or understood. Despite impor- strengths, there are other models that influence D
tant developments in understanding how efforts to address diseases. Notably, the dominant
adolescents experience diseases, much remains medical model of disease takes a different view of
to be investigated, particularly in terms of how disorders and problems than models that are
cultural and individual conceptions of disease can deemed more strength based. Strengths perspec-
influence outcomes. tives focus on individuals’ strengths, capacities,
competencies, and possibilities (including those
of their social environment) and seek ways to
Defining and Conceptualizing Disease facilitate those strengths, such as building better
social environments for adolescents. Strengths-
Disease can be defined as a condition potentially based approaches focus on empowerment, resil-
harmful to the well-being or health of an individ- ience, and membership in an appropriate group or
ual or as circumstances that place individuals at community (Saleebey 1996; Simmons et al.
the mercy of potentially disadvantageous conse- 2016). The strengths approach developed to coun-
quences. The factors that need to exist in order for ter the negative view of the disease model, and, as
a condition to qualify as a disease are established may be expected by those roots, the strengths
and defined from norms and expectations within model has been criticized, for example, for down-
the culture in which they occur, as adverse conse- playing real problems and approaching the causes
quences in one culture may not be viewed in the of disease in ways that defuse responsibility (see,
same light as another cultural perspective or view- e.g., Saleebey 1996). Despite criticisms, it is
point (Michaud et al. 2001). Disease can affect important to note that the strengths-based
various dimensions of a person. Although dis- approach has many variations, with some becom-
eases tend to be viewed as physical, they can ing highly influential (e.g., positive psychology;
also impact emotional and psychological aspects see Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi 2000).
of mental health and well-being. Indeed, it is
likely that diseases are an expression of physical
as well as psychosocial conditions. Several terms Disease in Adolescence
are often used interchangeably with disease, with
those terms including abnormality, affliction, In the study of adolescence, conceptions of dis-
defect, deficiency, disability, disorder, distur- ease are important for at least two reasons.
bance, dysfunction, illness, and impairment. As Although adolescents ostensibly appear to be in
these terms connote, a focus on disease tends to the prime of their health (Call et al. 2002), the
involve a medical focus. tumultuous quality of this developmental period
The medical focus is important to highlight often belies this expectation. Several important
because conceptions of disease guide responses diseased are diagnosed during the adolescent
to it. For example, the dominant medical model of period, or soon thereafter, such as severe psy-
disease addresses severe condition and stresses chotic disorders like schizophrenia (Tarbox and
diagnosis, labeling, medication, control, and Pogue-Geile 2008). The adolescent years provide
manipulation of individuals. Among other things, opportunities to identify clues to the development
the model increases willingness to explore of pathological processes and could potentially
1030 Disease

improve early identification of diseases as well as or therapeutic actions – but also to commonly
suggest potential targets for intervention (Tarbox believe that it is the other party’s fault (Hommel
and Pogue-Geile 2008). The adolescent period et al. 2011). Even when adolescents claim to fol-
also is an ideal time for preventing the develop- low medical treatment instructions, other issues
ment of diseases since the period is one marked by arise. For example, adolescents with asthma, a
engaging in risk behaviors linked with a variety of common diagnosis in adolescence, have been
diseases, such as those relating to substance use found to report using medication, yet their medi-
and sexual activity (see D’Amico et al. 2005; cations were not dispensed and they did not have
Brookmeyer and Henrich 2009). access to anyone else’s (Dahlén et al. 2016). In
Although the adolescent period is important addition, even though parents and adolescents
for understanding and addressing diseases in gen- could agree about the need for treatment, they
eral, there is much about the adolescent period that may not agree on how to go about obtaining
distinguishes it from others in terms of how to important aspects of treatment. Parents have
approach disease during adolescence. Adoles- been found to generally focus on longer-term
cents’ dependency on others contributes to the concerns, while adolescents generally focus
need to explore how the intersection of the roles more on immediate quality of life (Lipstein et al.
of parent and child with caregiver and patient can 2016). These divergences may be understandable,
affect – generally, either improve or complicate – but they complicate how to address diseases that
how adolescents face disease. For example, posi- have long-term implications as much as those that
tive self-perceptions correlate positively with raise acute concerns during adolescence.
health-related quality of life, which has led
researchers to highlight the benefits of explaining
to adolescents how their own bodies are fighting/ Conclusion
dealing with disease and help increase their self-
confidence to improve alongside the course of Understandings of disease in adolescence have
their medical treatment (Mussatto et al. 2014). tended to coalesce around a medical focus. As a
Relatedly, adolescents’ tolerance for pain and dis- result, it explores the intersection of how parents,
comfort generally is lower than adults’. This medical professionals, and caregivers might bal-
makes managing adolescents’ pain and discom- ance their good intentions with the complexities
fort differently complicated because of adoles- of adolescent personality and maturity in the face
cents’ development. Adolescent patients who of sometimes quite “adult” challenges. Although
are most difficult to assess by doctors and the medical model has been adopted to address
caregivers – most often those who could most disease, the period of adolescence raises impor-
use their attention – are known to be more anxious tant cultural, social, and psychological issues for
than their calmer peers (Josset-Raffet et al. 2016). getting and responding to diseases.
Although psychological dimensions are known to
be important to address for all patients, it appears
that considering adolescents’ developmental References
and psychological needs plays a particularly
important role. Brookmeyer, K. A., & Henrich, C. C. (2009).
The need to consider adolescents’ psychologi- Disentangling adolescent pathways of sexual risk tak-
ing. The Journal of Primary Prevention, 30, 677–696.
cal and developmental status relates to other Call, K. T., Riedel, A. A., Hein, K., McLoyd, V., Petersen,
dimensions of efforts to address diseases that A., & Kipke, M. (2002). Adolescent health and well-
afflict adolescents. Adolescents face unique chal- being in the twenty-first century: A global perspective.
lenges relating to adherence and compliance with Journal of Research on Adolescence, 12, 69–98.
D’Amico, E. J., Ellickson, P. L., Collins, R. L., Martino, S.,
treatment. For example, parents and adolescents & Klein, D. J. (2005). Processes linking adolescent
have been found to not only forget to take certain problems to substance-use problems in late young
disease treatment regimens – such as medications adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66, 766–775.
Dispositions in Law 1031

Dahlén, E., Wettermark, B., Bergström, A., Jonsson, E. W.,


Almqvist, C., & Kull, I. (2016). Medicine use and Dishonor
disease control among adolescents with asthma.
European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 72(3),
339–347. ▶ Humiliation
Hommel, K. A., Odell, S., Sander, E., Baldassano, R. N., &
Barg, F. K. (2011). Treatment adherence in paediatric
inflammatory bowel disease: Perceptions from adoles-
cent patients and their families. Health & Social Care in
the Community, 19(1), 80–88. Dispositions in Law
Josset-Raffet, E., Duparc-Alegria, N., Thiollier, A. F., D
Dugue, S., Faye, A., Ithier, G., et al. (2016). Per- Roger J. R. Levesque
ception and assessment of pain by caregiver and
adolescent with sickle cell disease: The impact of Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
the patient’s anxiety. Archives de Pédiatrie, 23(2),
143–149.
Lipstein, E. A., Dodds, C. M., Lovell, D. J., Denson, L. A., Dispositions, when used in law, refer to the final
& Britto, M. T. (2016). Making decisions about chronic
disease treatment: A comparison of parents and their statement or ruling on a matter made by a deci-
adolescent children. Health Expectations, 19(3), sion maker, such as a court. In criminal proce-
716–726. dure, for example, it is the imposed sentence or
Meurk, C., Carter, A., Partridge, B., Lucke, J., & Hall, other final statement made regarding a case or
W. (2014). How is acceptance of the brain disease
model of addiction related to Australians’ attitudes particular issue. When dealing with adolescents
towards addicted individuals and treatments for addic- in the juvenile justice system, dispositional rul-
tion? BMC Psychiatry, 14, 373–390. ings are deemed dispositions rather than
Michaud, P. A., Blum, R. W., & Slap, G. B. (2001). Cross- sentences. In juvenile courts, judges typically
cultural surveys of adolescent health and behavior:
Progress and problems. Social Science and Medicine, are deemed to have considerable discretion to
53, 1237–1246. determine the needed dispositions in specific
Mussatto, K. A., Sawin, K. J., Schiffman, R., Leske, J., cases. Although sometimes referred to by other
Simpson, P., & Marino, B. S. (2014). The importance of names, there tends to be six available disposi-
self-perceptions to psychosocial adjustment in adoles-
cents with heart disease. Journal of Pediatric Health tional alternatives available to juvenile courts:
Care, 28(3), 251–261. Commitment to a mental health institution, cus-
Saleebey, D. (1996). The strengths perspective in social todial confinement in a secure facility, custodial
work practice: Extension and cautions. Social Work, confinement in nonsecure facilities such as foster
41, 296–395.
Seligman, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive homes, home supervision or probation, commu-
psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, nity service, and reparation or restitution. Each of
55, 5–14. these alternatives is meant to be imposed after a
Simmons, C. A., Shapiro, V. B., Accomazzo, S., & dispositional hearing, which tends to occur sep-
Manthey, T. J. (2016). Strengths-based social work:
A meta-theory to guide social work research and prac- arate from an adjudication hearing meant to
tice. In N. Coady & P. Lehmann (Eds.), Theoretical determine whether the juvenile was responsible
perspectives for direct social work practice (3rd ed., for alleged offenses.
pp. 131–154). New York: Springer Publishing In the United States, the wide variety of possi-
Company.
Tarbox, S. I., & Pogue-Geile, M. F. (2008). Development ble dispositions typically exists in theory as courts
of social functioning in preschizophrenia children and tend to focus on a few alternatives. Probation
adolescents: A systematic review. Psychological Bulle- remains the most common disposition. Yearly,
tin, 134, 561–583. approximately 60% of youth who are adjudicated
delinquent participate in some form of
community-based probation (see Snyder and
Sickmund 2006). Probation programs use case
Disgrace management to monitor youth and enforce the
courts’ rulings. These programs vary from infre-
▶ Humiliation quent monitoring to intensive monitoring. The
1032 Dispositions in Law

second most frequently used disposition, which social service interventions, along with identified
covers over 20% of adjudicated cases, involves treatment needs, may increase youths’ chances for
institutional placements (Snyder and Sickmund receiving harsh juvenile court sanctions, as
2006), and these also vary considerably in their revealed by studies indicating that alcohol and
focus and intensity as they range from group drug abuse predict more severe dispositions
home placements and detention centers to train- (Campbell and Schmidt 2000; Lyons et al.
ing schools. Despite several potential alterna- 2001). How these factors are affected by legal
tives, then, the vast majority of courts tends to factors (such as offense history, severity of instant
rely persistently on two major approaches to offense, and past incarceration), however, remains
dealing with youth who are found to have debatable. One important study has reported that
offended. only drug use problems predicted confinement
Decisions making use of dispositional alter- beyond legal factors, but it did note that other
natives typically involve two sometimes compet- individual factors under study were not available
ing approaches. One approach focuses on to courts (Cauffman et al. 2007). Other studies,
community protection and identification of risk, however, tend to show that legal factors consis-
as exemplified by a continuum of “graduated tently predict the severity of dispositions
sanctions.” Graduated sanctions are those that (Campbell and Schmidt 2000; Leiber and Mack
range from least restrictive to most restrictive, 2003; Leiber and Stairs 1999). Equally important
such as from diversion to confinement (see to consider are findings showing that demo-
Howell 1995, 2003). These alternatives theoret- graphic variables like gender and race also may
ically are based on such legal factors as offense influence the severity of dispositions. Although
severity, offense history, and risk of future studies report important nuances and not all find
offending. These concerns are different from similar results, studies do tend to find that
the other main approach which focuses on the girls receive more restrictive dispositions than
offenders’ treatment needs, such as the need to boys once they are referred to juvenile court
promote their healthy development. Although (Chesney-Lind and Sheldon 2003; MacDonald
both concerns can be different and competing, and Chesney-Lind 2001), and research on race
they also can be related. They can be related in shows a disproportionate confinement of minor-
that the graduated sanctions could speak to the ity youth as well as other more severe disposi-
issue of the severity of sanctions needed in a tions for at least some groups of minority youth
particular case while treatment needs can deter- (Leiber and Fox 2005). Thus, although much
mine the nature of the intervention. Together, research indicates that courts follow legal factors
these approaches confront the key issues in deter- that appear to privilege a graduated sanctions
mining dispositions: the offense, the offender, approach, it is not clear whether they do so, nor
and community’s needs. is it clear what effect following legal factors has
Researchers have examined approaches taken on different groups.
by courts when they choose dispositional alterna- The study of dispositions in juvenile justice
tives. In practice, their options are considerably decision making is of significance beyond efforts
limited by the services available to them, and to ensure that the system works fairly. Disposi-
courts tend to take a legalistic approach that tional decisions are supposed to be based on a
focuses on the offenses more so than offenders’ wide variety of factors and protect youth from
special needs. When courts do focus on offenders’ arbitrary and unfair outcomes. In addition, the
special needs, that consideration runs the risk of actual dispositions dispensed by juvenile courts
increasing the severity of the disposition. Thus, are key to understanding the extent to which they
juvenile courts tend to view mental health needs abide by their mandate of customizing disposi-
as indicating a need for more restrictive, and tions to youth based on a case-by-case consider-
severe, dispositions rather than a need for more ation of each youth’s distinct rehabilitative
therapeutic alternatives. Prior involvement in needs. Theoretically, this customization should
Dissociation 1033

result in different juvenile court dispositions for


adolescent offenders whose personality charac- Dissociation
teristics and social backgrounds differ but who
may commit similar offenses. If research finds Roger J. R. Levesque
that legal factors are more important to determin- Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
ing dispositional outcomes, as research now
tends to find, then the juvenile court approxi-
mates the formality of the criminal court and, Overview
debatably, is moving away from its original D
mission. Dissociation refers to disconnects or disruptions
in an individuals’ thinking or bodily functions.
Those disruptions, in turn, cause abnormal inter-
References actions between consciousness and awareness.
Dissociation’s relevance to adolescence comes
Campbell, M. A., & Schmidt, F. (2000). Comparison of from research indicating that adolescent victims
mental health and legal factors in the disposition out- of trauma are more likely to experience dissocia-
come of young offenders. Criminal Justice and Behav- tion as well as see long-term negative mental
ior, 27, 688–715.
and/or physical consequences if improperly
Cauffman, E., Piquero, A. R., Kimonis, E., Steinberg, L.,
Chassin, L., & Fagan, J. (2007). Legal, individual, and addressed. Distinctions are drawn between
environmental predictors of court disposition in a sam- psychoform and somatoform dissociation, with
ple of serious adolescent offenders. Law and Human the later involving both physical and mental
Behavior, 31, 519–535.
Chesney-Lind, M., & Sheldon, R. G. (2003). Girls, delin-
symptoms and the former only mental symptoms.
quency, and juvenile justice (3rd ed.). Belmont: Several theories of dissociation have been pre-
Wadsworth. sented over the past few decades, with the most
Howell, J. C. (1995). Guide for implementing the compre- influential being the behavioral trauma theory and
hensive strategy for serious, violent, and chronic juve-
the behavioral states model. Most effects of dis-
nile offenders. Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile
Justice and Delinquency Prevention. sociation are negative, although some situations
Howell, J. C. (2003). Preventing and reducing juvenile do reveal a few positive and curative results.
delinquency: A comprehensive framework. Thousand Advances in assessment have helped increase
Oaks: Sage.
accuracy in distinguishing dissociation from per-
Leiber, M. J., & Fox, K. C. (2005). Race and the impact of
detention on juvenile justice decision making. Crime & sonality disorders. The study of dissociation as it
Delinquency, 51, 470–497. relates to adolescents would benefit from explor-
Leiber, M. J., & Mack, K. Y. (2003). The individual and ing more precisely the neurological sources of the
joint effects of race, gender, and family status on juve-
symptoms (mental and especially physical) of
nile justice decision-making. Journal of Research in
Crime and Delinquency, 40, 34–70. dissociation and the processes underlying these
Leiber, M. J., & Stairs, J. M. (1999). Race, contexts, and experiences.
the use of intake diversion. Journal of Research in
Crime and Delinquency, 36, 56–86.
Lyons, J., Baerger, D., Quigley, P., Erlich, J., & Griffin,
E. (2001). Mental health service needs of juvenile The Nature of Dissociation
offenders: A comparison of detention, incarceration,
and treatment settings. Children’s Services: Social Pol- Dissociation literally means disconnecting or
icy, Research, and Practice, 4, 69–85.
splitting apart elements that have something in
MacDonald, J. M., & Chesney-Lind, M. (2001). Gender
bias and juvenile justice revisited: A multiyear analysis. common. In psychiatry and other clinical sci-
Crime & Delinquency, 47, 173–195. ences, dissociation refers to an experience of hav-
Snyder, H. N., & Sickmund, M. (2006). Juvenile ing disruptions relating to one’s thinking (such as
offenders and victims: 2006 national report.
one’s memory, consciousness, perceptions, or
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Office
of Justice Programs, Office of Juvenile Justice and identity) or bodily functions (such as sensations
Delinquency Prevention. and movement) (see Diseth 2005). In
1034 Dissociation

dissociations, these disruptions result in different someone on whom the victim is dependent will
streams of consciousness that flow concurrently in lead to memory disruptions and dissociations as a
ways that occur simultaneously or outside of response to the need to maintain the needed (but
awareness (see American Psychiatric Association dysfunctional) attachment (Freyd 1996). Under
2000). The different forms of disruptions are asso- this model, dissociation is a mechanism used to
ciated with different traumas and prevalence rates. block trauma-related information, a finding
Somatoform dissociation involves both physical supported by research indicating higher incidence
and mental symptoms and is general indicative of dissociation symptoms of intrafamilial versus
a deeper-rooted problem, while psychoform dis- extrafamilial juvenile victims of violence
sociation involves only mental symptoms and is (Plattner et al. 2003). The role of lived experience
generally the most prevalent (see Nilsson et al. can be especially critical because hearing voices
2015). Moreover, dissociative disorders are dif- can stem from irrepressible memories of real-life
ferentiated from borderline personality disorders voices during traumatic events (Longden et al.
by their general lack of overt identity alteration, 2012). In fact, recent research consistently reveals
although misdiagnoses between these two pathol- that female adolescents who were sexually abused
ogies persist (Sar et al. 2016). exhibit higher levels of persistent dissociation
Epidemiological studies have historically not than their peers, especially if the abuse involved
conclusively assessed the prevalence of dissocia- family members (see Lev-Wiesel and Zohar
tive disorders in the general population of adoles- 2014). And the danger runs both ways: dissocia-
cents, although recent research has identified tion also has been linked to being the aggressor in
more specifics including the link between trauma- sexual abuse situations among adolescents
tized populations and populations with dissocia- (Leibowitz et al. 2011).
tive symptoms (Gusic et al. 2016). It does appear, The second major theory relating to dissocia-
however, that a high proportion of patients in tion is the discrete behavioral states model
adolescent psychiatry experience significant dis- (Putnam 1997). That model purports that patho-
sociative symptomatologies. Studies reporting the logical dissociation develops as a result of chil-
prevalence of dissociative symptoms among sex- dren’s failure to learn to integrate states. The
ually abused or maltreated children provide esti- model proposes that parents are instrumental in
mates ranging from 19 to 73%, with inpatient teaching very young children to move fluidly
adolescents cohorts ranging from 23 to 45% (see between states, such as a distressed to a neutral
Silberg 2000). These findings help support the one. These parenting needs are at risk of not being
generally accepted view that dissociation relates appropriately met when there are atypical experi-
to chronic traumas and disruptions in develop- ences, such as violence. And, as with the other
ment. These disruptions include a history of sex- model, evidence does suggest that, for example,
ual abuse, family disruptions such as removal children’s perceptions of their mothers’ parenting
from the home, and witnessing the abuse of family relates to the development of post-trauma symp-
members (for reviews, see Ogawa et al. 1997; toms, including dissociative symptoms. A leading
Silberg 2000). study revealed, for example, that children related
to their mothers in ways that revealed disorga-
nized or avoidant attachment styles were at higher
Dissociation and Development risk for developing dissociation in adolescence
(Ogawa et al. 1997). Both of these theories and
Findings have led to the development of several research supporting them highlight well how fam-
developmental theories relating disruptions to dis- ily members can increase or decrease the risk of
sociation, two of which are illustrative and espe- children’s developing dissociative symptoms.
cially relate to developmental issues. The first is Although many adhere to the general model
betrayal trauma theory (Freyd 1996). Betrayal that traumatic childhood experiences can result in
trauma theory posits that violence perpetrated by dissociative states, considerable controversy has
Dissociation 1035

emerged to challenge that view. Among others, “decline in suffering” which can in turn prevent
critics used socio-cognitive models to argue that suicide attempts and/or enable adolescents just
dissociative states, especially multiple personality enough introspection to seek the help they need
disorder, were artifacts of psychotherapy that (Levinger et al. 2016). And dissociation can pre-
were created, legitimized, and maintained by sent a first step on the path toward long-term
social reinforcement, as they have been socially healing from childhood trauma, to later be
created by other mechanisms in other societies replaced by deep introspection (Gusic et al. 2016).
(see Spanos 1994). One of the most important Despite some possible positive aspects to dis-
claims that gained remarkably wide and popular sociation, there is no getting around the generally D
attention was that dissociative disorders resulting negative focus. Notably, the DSM-IV-TR
from “recovered memories” of early trauma actu- (2000) restricts mental dissociation to its patho-
ally are based on “false memories” (see Lofthus logical forms, such as amnesia, depersonalization,
1993; Merskey 1995). Although this often was derealization, fragmentation, absorption, and
seen as a broad indictment of this area of research, identity confusion. And in some cases, psycholo-
the criticisms are much more narrow than popular gists are obliged to turn to ego-state therapy to
commentaries suggest given that they rest on treat adolescents with dissociation because it can
delayed recalls. Still, evidence does reveal that get so deeply embedded in their personalities and
the delayed recalls could contain both true and because it is one of the few generally positive
false memories. There may not be agreement treatment methods (da Silva and Fritz 2012).
that there is a direct link between trauma and Researchers have developed a number of ques-
dissociation, but there is agreement that trauma tionnaires to assess dissociative tendencies in both
creates vulnerability and that early traumatization clinical and research settings (Kihlstrom et al.
is one of the strongest predisposing factors for 1994). By far, the most popular of measure has
dissociation, and this is especially the case for been the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES;
repeated and severe traumas of emotional, physi- Carlson and Putnam 1993), which has been dem-
cal, or sexual character (see Diseth 2005). onstrated to screen effectively those who might be
Although dissociation tends to be viewed in a at risk for dissociative disorder (see Kihlstrom
negative manner, not surprisingly given its associ- 2005). Other instruments, such as the Perceptual
ation with trauma, it is important to emphasize that Alterations Scale, the Questionnaire on Experi-
some forms of dissociation are part of normal, ences of Dissociation, the Dissociation Question-
everyday life. Dissociation can range from norma- naire, and the Dissociative Processes Scale,
tive dissociation, such as daydreaming, to psycho- strongly correlate to one another (for a review,
pathological dissociations, such as would be found see Kihlstrom 2005). The different questionnaires
in multiple personality disorders (see Putnam also focus on absorption, which, in this context,
1995). Dissociation also can be a way to adapt to refers to an individual’s (actually a personality’s)
stress, which again can be a normal response or tendency to be open to experiences and alter con-
become pathological when it leads to functional sciousness by markedly narrowing or expanding
loss or altered behavior. As expected, when there their focus of attention and blurring the boundary
is significant and overwhelming trauma, massive between self and world (e.g., the extent to which a
psychological responses help children cope, such personality is open to hypnosis and hypnotizabil-
as through psychic numbing, denial, repression, ity) (Roche and McConkey 1990). Cutoff scores
aggression, and dissociation (Putnam 1997). of the DES reveal that 5–15% of individuals in the
Research does confirm that dissociation can be general population are deemed “at risk” for disso-
used in positive ways. It can be used to reinforce ciative disorder (Kihlstrom et al. 1994), but it is
activities or pay attention to negative ones to important to recognize that the range likely inflates
enhance performance. For example, in suicidal the presence of dissociation given that the measure
adolescents or adolescents who have already taps into normal levels of absorption and lead to a
attempted suicide, dissociation can cause a significant number of high false positives that can
1036 Dissociation

be best addressed through more structured clinical Da Silva, J., & Fritz, E. (2012). The experiences of educa-
interviewing which, itself, remains fraught with tional psychologists who utilise ego-state therapy to
address dissociation in adolescents. South African
controversy in this area. Furthermore, one of the Journal of Psychology, 42(2), 169–181.
most powerful recent developments in assessing Diseth, T. H. (2005). Dissociation in children and adoles-
dissociation in adolescents has come in the form of cents as reaction to trauma – An overview of concep-
the Somatoform Dissociation Questionnaire-20 tual issues and neurobiological factors. Nordic Journal
of Psychiatry, 59, 79–91.
(SDQ-20) and its brief version, SDQ-5. Although Freyd, J. J. (1996). Betrayal trauma: The logic of forgetting
differences manifest between somatoform and childhood abuse. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
psychoform dissociation, these measures exhibit Press.
high-quality psychometric properties and thus Gušić, S., Cardeña, E., Bengtsson, H., & Søndergaard,
H. P. (2016). Types of trauma in adolescence and their
represent yet another useful tool in assessing dis- relation to dissociation: A mixed-methods study. Psy-
sociation in both clinical and research settings chological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and
(Nilsson et al. 2015). Policy, 8(5), 568–576.
Kihlstrom, J. F. (2005). Dissociative disorders. Annual
Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 227–253.
Kihlstrom, J. F., Glisky, M. L., & Angiulo, M. J. (1994).
Conclusion Dissociative tendencies and dissociative disorders.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 117–124.
Dissociation represents a powerful mental and/or Leibowitz, G., Laser, J., & Burton, D. (2011). Exploring
the relationships between dissociation, victimization,
physical experience that can be caused by a num- and juvenile sexual offending. Journal of Trauma &
ber of possible conditions during adolescence. It Dissociation, 12(1), 38–52.
deserves the growing attention given by new Lev-Wiesel, R., & Zohar, G. (2014). The role of dissocia-
research because its nuances are one of the few tion in self-injurious behavior among female adoles-
cents who were sexually abused. Journal of Child
definitive links between physical and mental Sexual Abuse, 23(7), 824–839.
pathologies sharing a root in perception. Research Levinger, S., Holden, R., & Ben-Dor, D. (2016). Examin-
has yet to favor one leading model over another, ing the importance of mental pain and physical disso-
as the trauma theory and the behavioral states ciation and the fluid nature of suicidality in young
suicide attempters. Omega, 73(2), 159–173.
model are supported by different forms of Lofthus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories.
research – and the distinction between The American Psychologist, 48, 518–537.
psychoform and somatoform dissociation further Longden, E., Madill, A., & Waterman, M. G. (2012).
underscores the complexity of these potential dif- Dissociation, trauma, and the role of lived experience:
Toward a new conceptualization of voice hearing. Psy-
ferences in causes. Dissociation as a means of chological Bulletin, 138(1), 28–76.
coping with stress, suicidality, or even one’s own Merskey, H. (1995). Multiple personality disorder and the
growing personality has been explored with false memory syndrome. The British Journal of Psy-
increasing success through the use of new assess- chiatry, 166, 281–283.
Nilsson, D., et al. (2015). Somatoform dissociation among
ment methods. While there are seemingly few Swedish adolescents and young adults: The psycho-
positive and many negative dimensions of disso- metric properties of the Swedish versions of the SDQ-
ciation, it appears useful to accept the wide variety 20 and SDQ-5. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 69(2),
of dissociations that can occur and continue to 152–160.
Ogawa, J. R., Sroufe, L. A., Weinfield, N. S., Carlson,
grow this nascent body of research. E. A., & Egeland, B. (1997). Development and the
fragmented self: Longitudinal study of dissociative
symptomatology in a nonclinical sample. Development
and Psychopathology, 9, 855–879.
References Plattner, B., Silvermann, M. A., & Redlich, A. D. (2003).
Pathways to dissociation: Intrafamilial versus extra-
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and familial trauma in juvenile delinquents. The Journal
statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). of Nervous and Mental Disease, 191, 781–788.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Putnam, F. W. (1995). Development of dissociate disor-
(Revised Text). ders. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Develop-
Carlson, E. B., & Putnam, F. W. (1993). An update on the mental psychopathology (pp. 581–608). New York:
dissociative experiences scale. Dissociation, 6, 16–27. Wiley.
Dissociative Disorders 1037

Putnam, F. W. (1997). Dissociation in children and ado- which is formerly known as multiple personality
lescents: A developmental perspective. New York: disorder characterized by a patient’s appearing to
Guilford.
Roche, S. M., & McConkey, K. M. (1990). Absorption: posses two or more distinct personalities (e.g., a
Nature, assessment, and correlates. Journal of Person- host and one or more alters) that alternate in their
ality and Social Psychology, 59, 91–101. control of the individual’s conscious experience,
Sar, V., Alioğlu, F., Akyuz, G., Tayakısı, E., Öğülmüş, thought and action separated, in varying degrees,
E. F., & Sönmez, D. (2016). Awareness of identity
alteration and diagnostic preference between borderline by amnesia. The second is the dissociative fugue
personality disorder and dissociative disorders. Journal which involves amnesia relating to part or all of an
of Trauma & Dissociation, 18(1), 1–17. individual’s life. In this context, fugues involve a D
Silberg, J. L. (2000). Fifteen years of dissociation in loss of identity and often physical relocation, such
maltreated children: Where do we go from here?
Child Maltreatment, 5, 119–136. as forgetting who one is and adopting a new
Spanos, N. P. (1994). Multiple identity enactments and identity in a new location. The third, dissociative
multiple personality disorder: A sociocognitive per- amnesia, involves the loss of autobiographical
spective. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 143–165. memory for certain past experiences; memory
loss must be more extensive than normal forget-
fulness and not explainable by a physical or neu-
rological condition. Dissociative amnesia
Dissociative Disorders typically involves the inability to recall con-
sciously traumatic periods, especially from child-
Roger J. R. Levesque hood. Depersonalization disorder, the fourth DD,
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA is characterized by a sudden sense of being out-
side oneself and having the sense of watching
ones actions as though one were watching a
Some of the most controversial forms of psycho- movie. The last DD, dissociative disorders not
pathology fall under the broad umbrella of dis- otherwise specified, is a catchall category used to
sociative disorders (DDs). This group of label patients who display some dissociative
disorders shares the common characteristic of symptoms but not to the extent that they qualify
having an alteration in consciousness that affects for one of the major diagnoses.
memory and identity (American Psychiatric Controversy continues to surround this group
Association 2000). These disorders have been of disorders. There is some consensus that they
attributed to pathological levels of psychological typically develop as a way to cope with trauma,
stress, with the levels of stress disrupting the especially chronic physical, sexual, or emotional
normal integration of personality and mental abuse or even a frightening or highly unpredict-
life to the extent that some aspects of experience, able home environment (see Kihlstrom 2005).
thought, and action are split off from conscious- But, some rightly argue that evidence in this
ness. It is this split that leads to the use of the term respect mainly consists of retrospective self-
“dissociation.” Disorders from this group are reports made by the patients, which makes it
based on clinically relevant dissociative charac- difficult to determine whether reports reflect indi-
teristics not because of the mere presence of viduals’ vivid imaginations, their views of their
dissociation or its use as a defense in due to own problems, or their therapists’ suggestions
trauma; rather dissociative states are relevant (Id.). Although considerable progress has been
because the dissociation continues and has made toward understanding the nature of disso-
important effects once the immediate trauma ciative disorders, as well as their assessment and
has passed. treatment, including as they relate to children and
In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual adolescents (see Silberg 2000), reviews continue
(DSM-IV-TR, American Psychiatric Association to find much of this area of research, especially
2000) identifies five major dissociative disorders. those relating to etiological pathways, unconvinc-
The first is the dissociative identity disorder, ing (Kihlstrom 2005).
1038 Dissociative Identity Disorder

Cross-References early, intense treatment offers the greatest likeli-


hood of full recovery. Illness identification
▶ Dissociation methods for the specific components of the disor-
▶ Dissociative Identity Disorder der that most often affect children and adoles-
cents, amnesia and depersonalization, as well as
modes of treatment, will be described in this
References
essay. Recognizing that dissociative identity dis-
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and order includes a continuum of symptoms during
statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., Rev. development, the terms child and adolescent are
Text). Washington, DC: Author. used interchangeably in this essay.
Kihlstrom, J. F. (2005). Dissociative disorders. Annual
Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 227–253.
Silberg, J. (2000). Guidelines for the evaluation and treat-
ment of dissociative symptoms in children and adoles- Dissociative Identity Disorder
cents. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 1, 105–134.
Dissociation may be seen as a developmental
disruption in the integration of adaptive memory,
sense of identity, and the self-regulation of emo-
Dissociative Identity Disorder tion. Adaptive memory provides elastic capacity
to draw constructive lessons, and create linkages,
Scott Weber from difficult or painful experiences and includes
Nursing and Public Health, University of the capacity to actually alter memories to integrate
Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA more constructively with identity and baseline
collective memories. Sense of identity is the
foundation of context upon which memories and
Overview experiences interrelate to derive meaning of
experiences. Identity can qualitatively modify
Dissociated cognition is defined as thinking and experiences before they are fully integrated into
feeling that have not become successfully inte- memory, but identity itself can likewise be altered
grated into the usual sense of self. This results in if stimuli are so powerful or painful so as to leave a
discontinuities in conscious awareness and dis- lasting imprint or scar.
ruptions in ongoing linkages and connections in Self-regulation of emotions involves the medi-
the development of identity. Dissociation is ating capacity an individual uses, in an organized
believed to be one of the most common underly- manner, to interpret experiences, emotions, and
ing psychological processes among children and memories. According to Siegel (2007), integra-
adolescents receiving mental health treatment, but tion is broadly defined as “how the mind creates
most of the dissemination of information about a coherent self-assembly of information and
dissociation has occurred among psychiatrists and energy flow across time and context” (p. 316)
psychologists. Clinicians familiar with the char- and create whole, cohesive identifies. Neuronal
acteristics of dissociative identity disorders can firings create associations between these compo-
identify early symptoms of dissociation in chil- nents of memory, identity, and this unimpeded
dren and adolescents; those who practice in psy- energy flow allows us to regulate, process infor-
chiatric settings, emergency departments, and mation, and organize memories – and thereby
community health should be especially cognizant create cohesion. Siegel (2007) sees the develop-
of the clinical features of dissociation in pediatric ment of an integrated self as an ongoing process
patients. Prognosis in children and adolescents by which the mind continues to make increasingly
can vary widely among patients and between the organized connections that allow adaptive action.
specific types of dissociative disorder. Experi- There is no clear consensus on the exact
enced clinicians and researchers underscore that etiological pathway for the development of
Dissociative Identity Disorder 1039

dissociative symptomatology, but newer theoreti- it should be noted that each individual’s reaction
cal models stress impaired parent-child attach- to life events is a constructive process that is
ment patterns (Liotta 1999; Barach 2006; Ogawa idiosyncratic, and what might overwhelm one
et al. 2007). A second theory emphasizes trauma- child or adolescent may not overwhelm another.
based disruptions in the development of self- On the other hand, events which have not been
regulation of state transitions (Putnam 2006; thought of as major trauma, such as repetitive
Siegel 2007). Newer thinking ties maladaptive losses of attachment figures, peer rejection, obser-
attachment patterns directly to dysfunctional vation of domestic violence, medical procedures,
brain development that may inhibit integrative chronic living instability, and emotional abuse, D
connections in the developing child’s brain have nevertheless been found in the backgrounds
(Schore 2001). of children and adolescents displaying dissocia-
Children and adolescents may present with a tive symptoms. There may be individual differ-
variety of dissociative symptoms that reflect a lack ences in children’s susceptibility to dissociative
of coherence or integration in the self-assembly of symptoms which may be related to other traits,
mental functioning, including the following typi- such as fantasy-proneness (Rhue et al. 1995) or
cal characteristics: other inherited personality traits (Jang et al. 2008).
The contribution of genetic factors in the devel-
1. Inconsistent consciousness may be reflected in opment of dissociative symptoms is somewhat
symptoms of fluctuating attention, such as more controversial (Grabe et al. 1999).
trance states or “black outs.” Among adolescents, there are four recognized
2. Autobiographical forgetfulness and fluctua- categories of dissociative symptom disorders
tions in access to knowledge may reflect inco- which reflect the manifestations of the illness in
herence in developmental memory processes. each person. For lay individuals, it might make
3. Fluctuating moods and behavior, including more sense to think of these as four continuous
rage episodes and regressions, may reflect dif- disorders rather than as subcomponents of the
ficulties in self-regulation. same diagnostic cluster. With added experience
4. The child’s belief in alternate selves or imagi- and exposure in practice, it will become easier to
nary friends that control the child’s behavior recognize interrelationships between the catego-
and may reflect disorganization in the develop- ries. It is important to recognize that these precise
ment of a cohesive self. subcategories might not be relevant for younger
5. Depersonalization and derealization may children who are more likely to manifest more
reflect a subjective sense of dissociation from generalized, non-specific symptoms, including
normal body sensation and perception, or from potentially with pronounced features of one or
a sense of self (Schore 2001). more of the main subcategories. Younger children
usually display symptoms of either amnesia or
Dissociative symptoms have been found to depersonalization. By contrast, true identity dis-
correlate with traumatic histories of significant order, or what was previously called “fugue,” is
sexual abuse or physical abuse (Trickett et al. extremely rare even among adolescents in their
2001; Macfie et al. 2005; Coons 2006; Dell and late teens.
Eisenhower 2008; Hornstein and Putnam 2009), Current DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric
as well as war trauma (Cagiada et al. 2005) and Association 2004) are available for adults; there
natural disasters (Laor et al. 2002.) Dissociative are currently no separate pediatric criteria in
symptoms in children have also been associated DSM-IV-TR but an initiative is underway to
with parenting styles described as severely develop pediatric and adolescent criteria for
neglectful (Brunner et al. 2000; Sanders and DSM-V. Child and adolescent patients most
Giolas 2001; Ogawa et al. 2007), including clearly reflect traits from the depersonalization
behavior that is severely rejecting and highly and derealization criteria as well as the amnesia
inconsistent (Mann and Sanders 2004). However, criteria; as noted, identity disorders are rare
1040 Dissociative Identity Disorder

among children but may be partially expressed in warfare, and war-related atrocities; “ethnic
older adolescent patients. cleansing”; and involvement in organized
criminal activities such as child pornography
or other types of sexual or behavioral exploi-
Trauma and Abuse, and Development of tation (Bidrose and Goodman 2006)
Dissociated Cognition • Severe neglect or highly inadequate
attachment
There is a strong correlation between traumatic • Devastating loss, including death of a parent or
events and dissociative symptoms (Edwards et al. loss of a parent through separation; and cata-
2004; Kluft 2004). Thus, some children and ado- strophic illness or experiences with impending
lescents will present for evaluation with both dis- death of the child, or illness or death of other
sociative symptoms and a documented legal and family members and close friends
social service history of trauma. Others will pre-
sent with only the symptoms or the history but not Trauma and Amnesia
both. Still others will present with neither symp- A child’s memory may or may not be affected by
toms nor history. Trauma history may be masked trauma. Some autobiographical amnesia is under-
by a type of protective amnesia in the child or by stood to be a normal developmental phenomenon
the family’s denial. It should not be considered (Siegel 1996). While the frequency of traumatic
surprising if there is later disclosure by the child, amnesia in children and adolescents is unknown,
the family, or other witnesses to abusive behaviors clinical experience suggests that children and ado-
over the course of an extended assessment period. lescents may have sudden recall of previously
unavailable traumatic memories during or outside
Trauma Assessment of therapy sessions. This process has been
Clinicians in community health or emergency set- documented in cases where the traumatic events
tings who suspect child abuse, trauma or other were corroborated (Duggal and Sroufe 1998;
underlying cause of dissociative illness in chil- Corwin and Olafson 2007). Because children’s
dren should request an immediate referral to a memory is enhanced from practice and rehearsal
professional who is trained to evaluate pediatric and is superior for events that are familiar
or adolescent patients with these disorders. These (Ornstein 2005), sexual abuse experiences may
professionals could include child psychiatrists, not be as accessible to recall, especially when
psychologists, pediatric or psychiatric social shrouded in secrecy. Memory for traumatic events
workers, nurses, nurse practitioners, or clinical for children in incest-committing families may
nurse specialists who have training and experi- also be affected by the intense contradictory
ence working with children and adolescents with pulls for attachment and for safety, as when the
dissociative symptoms or pediatric mental health. source of abuse is also the source of nurturing
This initial practitioner might, in turn, refer a (Freyd 2006). In addition, violent threats from a
pediatric patient to an advanced specialist who perpetrator may produce powerful incentives to
focuses in working with patients with these clin- dismiss certain traumatic events from conscious
ical symptoms and history. The initial evaluating awareness so that an individual can more effec-
clinician should conduct a thorough assessment of tively cope with day-to-day expectations.
traumatic events in the child’s or adolescent’s An important point to remember in assessing
history. This assessment should include a review children and adolescents for trauma is that these
of the following domains, including the function- patients may lack verbal memories for traumatic
ality of the patient within each domain: events but might instead be able to demonstrate
knowledge of events through sensorimotor
• Physical abuse, emotional and psychological modalities or somatic symptoms (Burgess et al.
abuse, and sexual abuse; severe peer rejections 1995; Fivush et al. 2007; Stein and Waters 2009).
or isolation; witnessing marked violence, Evaluators should assess how children’s play, art,
Dissociative Identity Disorder 1041

behavioral re-enactments, and somatic concerns between amnesia and a simple unwillingness to
may be reflections of, or communicate informa- report (Hornstein 1998).
tion about, highly traumatic signal events and Other useful techniques have proven effective
life patterns. In assessing and documenting the for assessing memory problems, but these
severity of dissociative symptoms, all treating methods require significant training and should
advanced practice clinicians need to take into be carefully considered and undertaken only by
consideration how severely the symptoms disrupt trained therapists who are experienced in
normal developmental experiences such as implementing these modalities with children and
playing with peers or attending school, and how adolescent patients. These techniques include care- D
far the child’s behaviors and experiences deviate fully interviewing the child with the parent out of
from what is characteristic of normal develop- the room, helping the patient express feelings asso-
mental patterns (Schore 2001). ciated with the forgotten behaviors, role-playing
the forgotten behavior, providing abundant contex-
Assessing Memory Changes in Children tual clues, and interventions to decrease the child’s
Memory loss and changes resulting from severe considerable sense of shame. If the patient reports
trauma range from completely restricted to imaginary playmates or peers, the therapist should
broadly affecting major categories of a child’s or take special care to differentiate normal imaginary
adolescent’s memory. Trance states or “black playmates or peers and fantasy material from path-
outs” may occur and may span the range from ological dissociative symptoms. Dissociative
momentary absences of attention (normal in chil- pathology is suggested when the involvement in
dren and adolescents), to longer periods of non- fantasy interferes with normal activity, when the
responsiveness, to excessive sleeping or fainting, child feels his behavior is outside of his or her
all the way to states described as “coma” or control, when the child experiences the imaginary
“coma-like” (Cagiada et al. 2005). These lapses playmates as real, and when the child perceives
must be distinguished from the neurological phe- imaginary figures in conflict with each other
nomena of petit mal seizures in epilepsy. The (Trujillo et al. 2006; Silberg 2008).
evaluator should determine what elicits these
absences in attention, how long they last, what Assessing Changes in Affect and Behavior
seems to interrupt them, how they end, what con- Children normally go through changes in affect
sequences these disruptions have for the individ- and behavior during the course of an interview.
ual, and what the child or adolescent experiences The therapist should be very careful not to pathol-
during these states. ogize normal state shifts. To gauge changing
True amnesia about one’s own recent behav- affect, the therapist might inquire about the sub-
ior is quite rare and is considered diagnostic of a jective sense of discontinuity and about any stim-
more severe dissociative process. More common uli that preceded a change in state, mood, ability,
is amnesia for past traumatic signal events and or perceived identity (Carrion and Steiner 2000).
life patterns. More common still is transient for- Frequently, the stimulus may be a feeling associ-
getting which quickly disappears once the child ated with a traumatic event or associated thoughts
or adolescent is working with an empathic ther- which patients find too frightening or em-
apist. Parents may report that the child has no barrassing to acknowledge. Children with disso-
memory of an event; children may use the ciative symptoms often have very colorful ways
expression “I forgot” as a distraction, out of of describing these phenomena that make it clear
guilt or shame, or because of lack of rapport that their changes feel dramatic, uncontrollable,
with the assessing therapist. In this context, one and puzzling. Even during assessment, the thera-
therapist suggests asking children about whether pist should try to connect these initially puzzling
they forget good things (good grades, birthday changes with the child’s own perceptions, feel-
parties), as well as misbehavior or angry epi- ings, goals, intentions, and communicative mean-
sodes that may help the therapist to discriminate ings even if these are not obvious initially.
1042 Dissociative Identity Disorder

Making these connections during assessment useful to inquire as to relief experienced by such
will give therapists a better idea of the patient’s self-harm; the infliction of physical pain com-
receptivity to treatment as well as a sense of the monly reduces emotional pain (Johnson 2002).
severity of the dissociation. Shifts that are dra-
matic and perplexing to the child or adolescent
may involve sudden regression, rage behavior, Standardized Assessment Measures
apparent loss of consciousness, and they could
include suddenly talking about oneself in the Psychotherapists who work with dissociative
third person or with a new name. These shifts patients should understand that no single instru-
suggest difficulty in the integration of affect, ment could definitively diagnose clinical dissocia-
consciousness, and identity. The therapist tive identity disorder. However, the measures
should assess the reactions of caregivers, family described in Table 1 are well established in the
members, and others to these shifts in order to field and may be employed as adjuncts in a multi-
gain a context for understanding the ongoing faceted process of differential assessment.
influences shaping, promoting, or mitigating
dissociation.
Treatment of Dissociative Identity
Post-Traumatic Symptoms Disorders in Children and Adolescents
Post-traumatic symptoms include positive or
overt symptoms, such as nightmares, night terrors, Clinicians at the front lines of the healthcare team
disturbing hallucinations, intrusive traumatic are generally in ideal practice environments for
thoughts and memories, re-experiencing or flash- initial identification of key features of dissocia-
backs, and traumatic re-enactments. Also tion in children and adolescents with whom they
included could be negative or covert symptoms work. Yet, significant lack of awareness of this
such as numbing and avoidance. Standardized disorder cluster still exists even among experi-
screening instruments that might be helpful in enced clinicians in community health, emer-
identifying these symptoms include the Trauma gency care, and in pediatric and adolescent
Symptom Checklist for Children (Briere 2005), mental health care – settings in which clinicians
the Trauma Symptom Checklist for Young Chil- are most likely to initially screen individuals with
dren (Briere et al. 2001), or the Children’s PTSD dissociative identity disorder and related illness.
Inventory (Saigh et al. 2000). Practitioners in these settings could have signif-
icant positive impact in immediately referring
Self-Injurious Behavior children and adolescents to advanced practice
Self-injurious behavior is common among disso- clinicians who are most expert in evaluation
ciative teens and young adults. This behavior may of individuals with dissociative symptoms.
include cutting, burning, scratching, or head- Early identification and care of children and
banging. The behavior might be secretive, it
might serve an affect-regulating function, and it Dissociative Identity Disorder, Table 1 Assessment
might be performed while the patient is in a dis- tools for pediatric dissociative identity disorders
sociative trance-like state. The behavior could Adolescent Dissociative Experiences Scale (A-DES), a
also be used to facilitate or interrupt such a state. first-stage screen; 30 items; average Cronbach alpha
The therapist should carefully inquire about all coefficient = 0.93
stages of self-harm (cutting, burning, hitting) Dissociative Experiences Scale for Children (DES-C),
first-stage screen; 44 items; average Cronbach alpha
including planning, preparing, engaging, and coefficient = 0.94
recuperation, since some or all of these stages Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Dissociative
might be performed in a dissociated state Disorders-Revised (SCID-D-R), diagnostic screen;
(depersonalized, numbed, trance state, robotic 277 items; average Cronbach alpha coefficient = 0.95;
state, dream-like state). Therapists might find it may be used for children, adolescents, or adults
Dissociative Identity Disorder 1043

adolescents with these disorders is the main to understand the deeply personal experience of
determinant of improved prognosis. dissociation.
Several therapeutic modalities that have
proven especially successful in treating children Intervention Techniques
and adolescents with dissociative identity disor- Gestalt techniques involving dramatizing, and
der. It is important to know that treatment thus “giving voice to,” a variety of opposing and
approaches must be tailored to each patient. conflicting feelings may stimulate this process of
While the state of the science of treatment self-awareness and promote acceptance of disso-
methods for dissociation is somewhat uneven ciated feelings (Waters and Silberg 1998a, b; D
depending on the modality, new evidence is pro- Shirar 2006). The therapist could model the
viding firmer foundations for improved applica- acceptance of all contrasting feelings and remind
tion of these approaches, especially gestalt and the patient of these feelings when they are not
play therapies for both children and older adoles- otherwise immediately accessible. This technique
cents (Laporta 2002; McMahon and Fagan 2003). helps to break down dissociative barriers.
Another focus is to assist the child with reso-
lution of conflicting feelings, wishes, loyalties,
Examples of Therapeutic Goals identifications, or contrasting expectations. The
child may perceive these as conflicts between
The treating psychotherapist can help the patient internal voices, imaginary friends, or conflicting
achieve a sense of cohesive identity and whole- identities. The therapist helps the child find ways
ness if the child has had a difficult time achieving to express these conflicts directly, examine both
this sense of integrated selfhood. The therapist can sides of the conflict, and problem-solve toward
enhance motivation for growth and future success integrative solutions so that, over time, there is
by helping the patient believe in his/her own skill, no longer a need for dissociative escape or a
potential, and capacity for wellness. A goal should fragmented sense of identity.
be to enhance motivation for success and future One of the long-established and highly effec-
accomplishments (Siegel 2007). This approach tive treatment methods used in working with chil-
promotes integration and helps defeat the resis- dren and younger adolescents is play therapy in
tance to change. Therapists can employ the tech- which these various conflicts, contrasting role
nique of teaching about how dissociation expectations, or dissociated feelings are enacted
prohibits growth and change. This encourages in play and brought to conscious awareness with
the insightful adolescent patient to consciously the therapist’s guidance. This process can encour-
abandon dissociative strategies over time age a natural integration and development of
(Schore 2001). cohesive identity in younger adolescents (Albini
Another goal is to promote self-acceptance of and Pease 1999; Laporta 2002). This process may
behavior and self-knowledge about feelings pre- be facilitated by specific play activities or by
viously considered unacceptable. Dissociation imagery and even hypnotic techniques among
may protect the individual from awareness or highly trained and experienced clinicians
experience of deeply personal feelings of rage, (Waters and Silberg 1998b; Gil 2001; McMahon
disappointment, grief, self-doubt, fear, shame, and Fagan 2003; Kluft 2005; Shirar 2006). With
physical pain, or sexual pain, abuse, or humilia- teenagers, some therapists recommend having the
tion (Schore 2001). Enhancing insight and aware- client write letters to the self to dramatize aloud
ness of these features will promote integration. internal dialogues as a way to facilitate resolution
Education about sexuality and affect may assist of competing wishes or feelings. These exercises
with this process (Wieland 2007). Teaching how promote self cohesion without reinforcing the
to recognize early implicit memories and how separateness of identities. Therapists should take
they can affect one’s developmental integration note of situations where these conflicts cannot be
can also help both adolescents and their parents resolved without a concomitant change in the
1044 Dissociative Identity Disorder

environment (Gil 2001). For example, patients these do not interfere with daily functioning
may be caught in custody battles, with widely (Waters 1998). Some traumatized adolescent
divergent expectations between parents that may patients engage in flashbacks during parental
lead to a fragmented sense of identity. The patient fights or when asked to do chores; thus, under-
alone often cannot resolve this inner conflict standing the complete context in which these
unless the environmental pressure is lessened. flashbacks occur is essential before designing
An effort to desensitize traumatic memories interventions.
and correct learned attitudes toward life resulting Adolescent patients with dissociative symp-
from traumatic events may be attempted (Shirar toms arising from trauma may develop a variety
2006). Talk to the patient about overwhelming of learned attitudes which may include helpless-
experiences and their associated affects and ness, belief in a bad self, being destined for bad
perceptions. This helps to desensitize the child’s things, and being unlovable. These beliefs need to
conditioned fear responses to any frightening be corrected during therapy (Wieland 1997;
memories. The patient’s access to these memories American Academy of Child and Adolescent
may be variable as treatment progresses. How- Psychiatry 1998; Deblinger and Heflin 2006).
ever, as the child moves towards a more cohesive Dissociative children may hold contradictory
sense of self, such memories may become more beliefs or contradictory attachment styles with
accessible. These memories may become more associated beliefs which may make this aspect of
accessible as the child moves toward a more cohe- treatment particularly challenging (Liotta 1999).
sive sense of self. The therapist should be careful Carefully pointing out these contradictions within
to gradually introduce discussions about trau- the context of an accepting therapeutic relation-
matic content and not overwhelm the child ship can eventually lead to successful integration.
(James 1989). Play therapy with trauma patients The therapist should promote autonomy and
tends to involve more active intervention for encourage the patient to independently regulate
reworking of traumatic themes than other kinds and express affects and to self-regulate state
of play therapy. changes.
Eye movement desensitization and Therapists and caregivers can help adolescent
reprocessing (EMDR) therapy, when conducted patients to identify precursors to state changes so
by a trained and experienced clinician, can assist that they become better at self-monitoring (James
adolescent patients in working through experi- 1989; Allers et al. 2001; Gil 2001). A highly
ences for which they have very little or no explicit effective treatment approach for older children
memory, or experiences that they find too difficult with post-traumatic stress disorder has been
to talk about in detail (Greenwald 2003; Tinker developed by Cohen and colleagues (Cohen
and Wilson 2008). Ego-strengthening and et al. 2004, 2005). Self-injury may become a
calming techniques are advisable prior to using mood-altering addictive behavior that serves to
EMDR to avoid destabilization (Greenwald distract from emotional pain or expression of feel-
2003). Techniques that help improve the child’s ings of despair and anger. Individuals engaging in
sense of efficacy and mastery are always encour- repetitive self-harm must learn alternatives
aged when dealing with traumatic content (Gil including more direct expression of feelings and
1996, 2001; Friedrich 2001; James 2004). improved methods of mood regulation; family
Traumatic re-experiencing may take the form members must assess ways in which they can
of flashbacks, in which the adolescent patient learn to reinforce positive methods of affect-self-
experiences past events as if they were currently regulation.
actually happening. Imagery techniques that Self-monitoring may be encouraged by use of
guide the individual toward mastery experiences special code words whereby a parent, caregiver, or
may be helpful (Friedrich 2001; Williams and teacher can let a child know a shift has occurred
Velazquez 2006). Families can set designated or, conversely, these words or slogans can stimu-
times to discuss unpleasant memories so that late focused attention. Cognitive-behavioral
Dissociative Identity Disorder 1045

techniques can help patients learn to manage self- In educating parents, change any literal views
destructive and impulsive behavior, including the family may have about the reality of “alter
impulsive sexual behavior. Individual behaviors egos” as separate from the child, while explaining
can be reinforced for identifying changes in how dissociation evolves in a traumatized child.
moods or states, interrupting dysfunctional impul- Parents can be taught to encourage the child’s
sive habits, and engaging in drawing, writing, direct expression of thoughts and feelings without
talking to adults, or other expressive alternatives. reinforcing dysfunctional dissociative strategies.
Therapists can teach adolescent patients tech- The therapist can also help the family identify
niques of promoting active attention that may current reinforcers that maintain the symptom- D
help interrupt sexual misbehavior or other disrup- atology, such as family indulgence or overly puni-
tive behavior that occurs during dissociative states tive responses in the face of regressive behavior.
(Johnson 2002). Teaching use of positive imagery For children residing in original homes in
and relaxation for self-soothing and stress reduc- which maltreatment or unintentional exposure to
tion may be very useful. traumatic events occurred, acknowledgment of
Therapists and caregivers can encourage chil- the trauma and an apology for the lack of protec-
dren to communicate feelings of anger, fear, and tion is a basic starting point for much of the family
regressive needs to their caregivers so that these therapy work (Deblinger and Heflin 2006). Dyad
are not enacted in dysfunctional ways. In addi- work with the parent figure and the child can be
tion, the therapist can teach caregivers to tolerate particularly valuable in helping the therapist
these direct expressions. This interaction pro- understand the subtle negative dynamics that
motes healthy attachment though it may be diffi- may be occurring in the home. This would be a
cult for families that are uncomfortable with time when the therapist, parent and child can
affect and have poor boundaries. The goal should develop ideas for handling problem situations
be to help families and adolescents view the at home.
therapist as a stable attachment figure, particu-
larly for patients and families from chaotic envi- Hypnotherapy
ronments. Group therapy may be useful, Hypnotherapy for children and adolescents,
particularly if it is psycho-educational in orien- conducted by thoroughly trained and experienced
tation (Silberg 1997; Brand 1998). Promotion of therapists, has been described for rapidly
positive peer interactions may help build long- accessing ego states and promoting integration
term resiliency for children and adolescents with (Kluft 2000, 2005; Bowman et al. 2005; Dell
dissociation. and Eisenhower 2008) or for containment of
intense affect, ego strengthening, education, and
Family Therapy support. Hypnotherapy is not advised for memory
Work with the primary caregivers may include retrieval (Williams and Velazquez 2006). In cases
education about dissociation (Waters 1998), spe- where hypnosis is deemed appropriate, the thera-
cific guidance about parenting strategies which pist should gain informed consent from caregivers
facilitate therapy (Boat 2007), family sessions to or guardians, as well as clients. The therapist
encourage the family to accept all aspects of the should explore all legal implications, given that
child (Waters and Silberg 1998b), correcting inter- witness credibility for any upcoming court hear-
active patterns that promote dissociation ings could be affected (American Psychological
(Benjamin and Benjamin 1993; Silberg 2001), Association 2008).
helping parents process guilt or denial about trau-
matic events (Keren and Tyano 2000), working Pharmacotherapy
through of feelings about issues of safety and There are no controlled studies on the use of
betrayal to help establish trust (Waters 1998), medications with dissociative children, adoles-
and straightforward parenting advice or training cents, or adults. However, some clinicians have
which is part of all good child therapy. found that psychotropic medication may be of
1046 Dissociative Identity Disorder

benefit for children and adolescents with dissocia- understanding and reward effort and approxima-
tive symptoms and disorders as an adjunct to tions of desired goals (Benjamin and Benjamin
psychotherapy to ameliorate targetable symp- 1993). Pre-arranged code words or signals that
toms, such as incapacitating anxiety, insomnia, stimulate attention and focus may be useful
lability, behavioral dyscontrol, inability to focus (Waterbury 1998).
attention, and depression (Silberg et al. 1997; Therapists should ensure that school staff
Putnam 2006; Nemzer 2008). A consultation encourages children and adolescents to monitor
with a psychiatric primary care clinician with themselves to access the greatest potential possi-
prescribing authority and experience in working ble, should set clear and firm limits about expec-
with dissociative children, or similar advanced tations, and provide regular opportunities for the
professional, should be obtained. Medications child to communicate about sources of stress in
may also be utilized to treat co-morbid conditions the school environment or at home that may
such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, impact on performance (Deblinger and Heflin
major depression, obsessive-compulsive disorder, 2006). Discourage special attention from other
or post-traumatic stress disorder. If they are sepa- students related to extreme shifts in behavior.
rate members of the care team, close communica- Designate one member of the staff to deal with
tion and teamwork between the prescribing any dramatic shifts, such as age regression (Shirar
clinician, pediatrician, and the psychotherapist is 2006). School environments would be ideal set-
essential. tings to incorporate expressive arts into the cur-
riculum for expression of feelings (Waterbury
Art Therapy 1998). Teachers must be attentive to the child’s
The goal with art therapy is to strike a balance or teenager’s needs to have stable attachment fig-
between art that encourages mastery and art that is ures in the school setting (counselors, advisors, or
regressive, limiting the latter if it simply becomes teachers) with whom they have a special relation-
a form of traumatic re-enactment without trauma ship and with whom they can frequently commu-
resolution. Most child therapists use some art in nicate (Koback et al. 2008).
their work, and art therapy from a licensed art
therapist may be a useful adjunct to individual
treatment in some cases (Sobol and Schneider Summary
2006).
Clinicians who screen and do first-level assess-
Educational Interventions ments of youth with dissociative symptoms can
Children and adolescents with dissociative symp- recognize basic features of this disorder and can
toms may require special education modifications help to ensure that the proper referrals to special-
if their disruptive behavior, mood instability, and ized care are provided. A beginning understand-
poor attention interfere with academic function- ing of the characteristics of dissociative identity
ing. With supportive staff, however, many chil- disorders as they affect pediatric patients is nec-
dren with dissociative symptoms can succeed in essary. As children get older their symptoms may
regular classrooms (Deblinger and Heflin 2006). emerge into adult forms of dissociation. Early
Therapists will find it essential to work closely intervention helps to reduce the severity of symp-
with school staff in this domain of patient care. toms and can significantly improve long-term
School staff, in turn, should help encourage the prognosis.
child or adolescent to stay focused even while Primary care clinicians and school staff can
mood and attention fluctuate. It should be lead the way in helping children and adolescents
assumed that the child or adolescent will bring who suffer from dissociative symptom disorders
his or her full potential to the school setting and to become whole and healed, and to make the
an attitude of accountability is encouraged. The transition to a healthy adulthood. An interdisci-
school staff should convey an attitude of plinary program of treatment for children and
Dissociative Identity Disorder 1047

adolescents with dissociative identity disorders Briere, J. (2005). The trauma symptom checklist for chil-
must address therapeutic needs in all domains of dren. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Briere, J., Johnson, K., Bissada, A., Damon, L., Crouch, J.,
growth and development, including school, home, Gil, E., et al. (2001). The trauma symptom checklist for
and relationships with peers. young children (TSCYC): Reliability and association
with abuse exposure in a multi-site study. Child Abuse
& Neglect, 25, 1001–1014.
Brunner, R., Parzer, P., Schuld, V., & Resch, F. (2000).
Cross-References Dissociative symptomatology and traumatic factors in
adolescent psychiatric patients. Journal of Nervous and
▶ Dissociation Mental Disease, 188, 71–82. D
Burgess, A. W., Hartman, C. R., & Baker, T. (1995). Mem-
▶ Dissociative Disorders
ory presentation of childhood sexual abuse. Journal of
Psychosocial Nursing, 33, 9–16.
Cagiada, S., Camaido, L., & Pennan, A. (2005). Success-
References ful integrated hypnotic and psychopharmacological
treatment of a war-related post-traumatic psychologi-
Albini, T. K., & Pease, T. E. (1999). Normal and patholog- cal and somatoform dissociative disorder of two years
ical dissociations of early childhood. Dissociation, 2, duration (psychogenic coma). Dissociation, 10,
144–150. 182–189.
Allers, C. T., White, J. F., & Mullis, F. (2001). The treat- Carrion, V. G., & Steiner, H. (2000). Trauma and dissoci-
ment of dissociation in and HIV-infected sexually ation in delinquent adolescents. Journal of the Ameri-
abused adolescent male. Psychotherapy, 37, 201–206. can Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39,
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 353–359.
(1998). Practice parameters for the assessment and Cohen, J. A., Deblinger, E., Mannarino, A. P., & Steer,
treatment of children and adolescents with post- R. A. (2004). A multisite, randomized, controlled trail
traumatic stress disorder. Journal of the American for children with sexual abuse-related PTSD symp-
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37(10), toms. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
4s–26s. Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 393–402.
American Psychiatric Association. (2004). DSM-IV-TR – Cohen, J., Mannarino, A. P., & Knudsen, K. (2005).
Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders Treating sexually abused children: 1 year follow-up of
(4, text revisionth ed.). Washington, DC: American a randomized controlled trial. Child Abuse & Neglect,
Psychiatric Association Publications. 29, 135–145.
American Psychological Association. (2008). Guidelines Coons, P. M. (2006). Clinical phenomenology of 25 chil-
for psychological evaluations in child protection mat- dren and adolescents with dissociative disorders. Child
ters. Washington, DC: American Psychological and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America,
Association. 5, 361–374.
Barach, P. (2006). Multiple personality as an attachment Corwin, D., & Olafson, E. (2007). Videotaped discovery of
disorder. Dissociation, 4, 117–123. a reportedly unrecallable memory of child abuse. Com-
Benjamin, L. R., & Benjamin, R. (1993). Interventions parison with a childhood interview taped 11 years
with children in dissociative families: A family treat- before. Child Maltreatment, 2, 91–112.
ment model. Dissociation, 7, 47–53. Deblinger, E., & Heflin, A. H. (2006). Treating sexually
Bidrose, S., & Goodman, G. S. (2006). Testimony and abused children and their nonoffending parents. Thou-
evidence: A scientific case study of memory for child sand Oaks: SAGE.
sexual abuse. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, Dell, P. F., & Eisenhower, J. W. (2008). Adolescent multi-
197–213. ple personality disorder: a preliminary study of eleven
Boat, B. W. (2007). Caregivers as surrogate therapists in cases. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
treatment of a ritualistically abused child. In W. N. Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 359–366.
Friedrich (Ed.), Casebook of sexual abuse treatment Duggal, S., & Sroufe, L. A. (1998). Recovered memory of
(pp. 1–26). New York: WW Norton. childhood sexual trauma: A documented case from a
Bowman, E. S., Blix, S. F., & Coons, P. M. (2005). longitudinal study. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 11,
Multiple personality in adolescence: Relationship to 301–322.
incestual experience. Journal of the American Acad- Edwards, V. J., Holden, G. W., Felitti, V. J., & Anda, R. F.
emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 24, (2004). Relationship between multiple forms of child-
109–114. hood maltreatment and adult mental health in commu-
Brand, B. (1998). Supportive group therapy for adoles- nity respondents. Results from the adverse childhood
cents with dissociative disorders. In J. L. Silberg experiences study. American Journal of Psychiatry,
(Ed.), The dissociative child: Diagnosis, treatment & 160(8), 1453–1460.
management (2nd ed., pp. 219–230). Lutherville: Fivush, R., Pipe, M. E., Murachver, T., & Reese, E. (2007).
Sidran. Events spoken and unspoken; Implications of language
1048 Dissociative Identity Disorder

and memory development for the recovered memory symptoms in Turkish children exposed to the 1999
debate. In M. Conway (Ed.), Recovered memories and earthquakes. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Dis-
false memories (pp. 34–63). New York: Oxford Uni- ease, 190, 824–832.
versity Press. Laporta, L. D. (2002). Childhood trauma and multiple
Freyd, J. (2006). Betrayal trauma. Boston, MA: Harvard personality disorder: The case of a 9-year-old girl.
University Press. Child Abuse & Neglect, 16, 615–620.
Friedrich, W. N. (2001). Hypnotherapy with traumatized Liotta, G. (1999). Disorganization of attachment as a
children. The International Journal of Clinical and model of understanding dissociative psychopathology.
Experimental Hypnosis, 39, 67–81. In J. Solomon & C. George (Eds.), Attachment disor-
Gil, E. (1996). Treating abused adolescents. New York: ganization (pp. 291–317). New York: Guilford.
Guilford. Macfie, J., Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2005). The devel-
Gil, E. (2001). The healing power of play. New York: opment of dissociation in maltreated preschool-afed
Guilford. children. Development and Psychopathology, 13,
Grabe, H., Spitzer, C., & Freyberger, H. J. (1999). Rela- 233–254.
tionship of dissociation to temperament and character Mann, B. J., & Sanders, S. (2004). Child dissociation and
in men and women. The American Journal of Psychi- the family context. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy-
atry, 156, 1811–1813. chology, 22, 373–388.
Greenwald, R. (2003). Using EMDR with children. Pacific McMahon, P. P., & Fagan, J. (2003). Play therapy with
Grove: EMDR Institute. children with multiple personality disorder. In R. P.
Hornstein, N. L. (1998). Complexities of psychiatric dif- Kluft & C. G. Fine (Eds.), Clinical perspectives on
ferential diagnosis in children with dissociative symp- multiple personality disorder (pp. 253–276).
toms and disorders. In J. L. Silberg (Ed.), The Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
dissociative child: Diagnosis, treatment & manage- Nemzer, E. (2008). Psychopharmacological Interventions
ment (2nd ed., pp. 27–45). Lutherville: Sidran. for children and adolescents with dissociative disor-
Hornstein, N. L., & Putnam, F. W. (2009). Clinical phe- ders. In J. L. Silberg (Ed.), The dissociative child:
nomenology of child and adolescent dissociative disor- Diagnosis, treatment & management (2nd ed.,
ders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and pp. 231–272). Lutherville: Sidran.
Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 1077–1085. Ogawa, J. R., Sroufe, L. A., Weinfield, N. S., Carlson,
James, B. (1989). Treating traumatized children. E. A., & Egeland, B. (2007). Development and the
New York: Free Press/Simon & Schuster. fragmented self: Longitudinal study of dissociative
James, B. (2004). Handbook for treatment of attachment- symptomatology in a nonclinical sample. Development
trauma problems in children. New York: Free Press/ and Psychopathology, 9, 855–979.
Simon & Schuster. Ornstein, P. A. (2005). Children’s long-term retention of
Jang, K. L., Paris, J., Zweig-Frank, H., & Livesley, W. J. salient personal experiences. Journal of Traumatic
(2008). Twin study of dissociative experience. The Stress, 8, 581–605.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 186, 345–351. Putnam, F. W. (2006). Dissociation in children and ado-
Johnson, T. C. (2002). Some considerations about sexual lescents. New York: Guilford.
abuse and children with sexual behavior problems. Rhue, J. W., Lynn, S. J., & Sandberg, D. (1995). Dissoci-
Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 3(4), 83–105. ation, fantasy and imagination in childhood:
Keren, M., & Tyano, S. (2000). A case study of PTSD in A comparison of physically abused, sexually abused,
infancy: Diagnostic, neurophysiological, developmen- and non-abused children. Contemporary Hypnosis, 12,
tal and therapeutic aspects. Israel Journal of Psychiatry 131–136.
& Related Science, 37(3), 236–246. Saigh, P. A., Yasik, A. E., Oberfield, R. A., Green, B. L.,
Kluft, R. P. (2000). The psychoanalytic psychotherapy of Halmandaris, P. V., Rubenstein, H., et al. (2000). The
dissociative identity disorder in the context of trauma children’s PTSD inventory: Development and reliabil-
therapy. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 20, 259–286. ity. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 13, 369–380.
Kluft, R. P. (2004). Hypnotherapy of childhood multiple Sanders, B., & Giolas, M. H. (2001). Dissociation and
personality disorder. American Journal of Clinical childhood trauma in psychologically disturbed adoles-
Hypnosis, 27, 201–210. cents. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 50–54.
Kluft, R. P. (2005). Outpatient treatment of dissociative Schore, A. (2001). The effects of early relational trauma on
identity disorder and allied forms of dissociative disor- right brain development, affect regulation, and infant
ders not otherwise specified in children and adoles- mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22,
cents. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of 201–269.
North America, 5, 471–494. Shirar, L. (2006). Dissociative children. New York: WW
Koback, R., Little, M., Race, E., & Acosta, M. C. (2008). Norton.
Attachment disruptions in seriously emotionally dis- Siegel, D. J. (1996). Cognition, memory and dissociation.
turbed children: Implications for treatment. Attachment Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North
& Human Development, 3, 243–258. America, 5, 509–533.
Laor, N., Wolmer, L., Kora, M., Yucel, D., Spirman, S., & Siegel, D. J. (2007). The developing brain. New York:
Yazgan, Y. (2002). Posttraumatic, dissociative and grief Guilford.
Diversion Programs 1049

Silberg, J. L. (1997). Afterword. In J. L. Silberg (Ed.), The Wieland, S. (2007). Techniques and issues in abuse-
dissociative child: Diagnosis, treatment & manage- focused therapy. Thousand Oaks: SAGE.
ment (2nd ed., pp. 331–347). Lutherville: Sidran. Williams, D. T., & Velazquez, L. (2006). The use of hyp-
Silberg, J. L. (2001). Treating maladaptive dissociation in a nosis in children with dissociative disorders. Child and
young teenage girl. In H. Orvaschel, J. Faust, & Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 5,
M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of conceptualization and 495–508.
treatment of child psychopathology (pp. 449–474).
Oxford: Elsevier Science.
Silberg, J. L. (2008). Interviewing strategies for assessing
dissociative disorders in children and adolescents. In
J. L. Silberg (Ed.), The dissociative child: Diagnosis, Diversion Programs D
treatment & management (2nd ed., pp. 47–68).
Lutherville: Sidran.
Silberg, J. L., Stipic, D., & Tagizadeh, F. (1997). Dis- Roger J. R. Levesque
sociative disorders in children and adolescents. In Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
J. Noshpitz (Ed.), The handbook of child & adoles-
cent psychiatry (Vol. III, pp. 329–355). New York:
Wiley.
Sobol, B., & Schneider, K. (2006). Art as an adjunctive Diversion involves the use of a variety of mea-
therapy in the treatment of children who dissociate. In sures other than formal adjudications in the juve-
J. L. Silberg (Ed.), The dissociative child: Diagnosis, nile court and juvenile justice systems. Decisions
treatment & management (2nd ed., pp. 219–230). to divert youth are made at intake, which is the
Lutherville: Sidran.
Stein, P., & Waters, F. S. (2009). Chronic traumatic stress first point at which a juvenile is taken into custody
in children as an etiological factor in the development on suspicion of having committed a delinquent
of obsessive-compulsive disorder and attention-deficit/ act, status offense, or criminal act. Diversion
hyperactivity disorder. Workshop presented at the Fif- involves using other agencies, often private
teenth International Meeting of the International Soci-
ety for Traumatic Stress, Miami. ones, that will take on the challenge of rehabili-
Tinker, R. H., & Wilson, S. A. (2008). Through the eyes of tating offenders, typically those who are first-time
a child. New York: WW Norton. offenders and status offenders who pose no threat
Trickett, P. K., Noll, J. G., Reiffman, A., & Putnam, to others or themselves. The use of diversion pro-
F. (2001). Variants of intrafamilial sexual abuse expe-
rience: Implications for short and long-term develop- grams rests on the theory that, for some youth, the
ment. Development and Psychopathology, 13(4), formal justice system may do them more harm
1001–1019. than good (see Lundman 1993). Although for-
Trujillo, K., Lewis, P. O., Yeager, C. A., & Gidlow, mally outside of the juvenile justice system,
B. (2006). Imaginary companions of school boys and
boys with dissociative identity disorder: A normal to these programs are meant to further the juvenile
pathological continuum. Child and Adolescent Psychi- courts’ notion that formal legal sanctions and pro-
atric Clinics of North American, 5, 373–391. cessing runs the risk of inadvertently stigmatizing
Waterbury, M. (1998). School interventions for dissocia- some youth for having committed relatively petty
tive children. In J. L. Silberg (Ed.), The dissociative
child: Diagnosis, treatment & management (2nd ed., acts and lead them to engage in more rather than
pp. 315–330). Lutherville: Sidran. less problem behavior. In addition to furthering
Waters, F. W. (1998). Parents as partners in the treatment of these ideals, diversion programs also are meant to
dissociative children. In J. L. Silberg (Ed.), The disso- help the juvenile and criminal courts avoid further
ciative child: Diagnosis, treatment & management
(2nd ed., pp. 273–296). Lutherville: Sidran. overburdening their systems and focus on more
Waters, F. W., & Silberg, J. L. (1998a). Promoting integra- serious offenders.
tion in dissociative children. In J. L. Silberg (Ed.), The Considerable theory supports the view that
dissociative child: Diagnosis, treatment & manage- legal interventions by the juvenile justice system
ment (2nd ed., pp. 167–190). Lutherville: Sidran.
Waters, F. W., & Silberg, J. L. (1998b). Therapeutic phases may actually perpetuate delinquency, which is
in the treatment of dissociative children. In J. L. Silberg what lead to the development of diversion pro-
(Ed.), The dissociative child: Diagnosis, treatment & grams to deal with cases that might be more
management (2nd ed., pp. 135–156). Lutherville: appropriately remedied in informal settings within
Sidran.
Wieland, S. (1997). Hearing the internal trauma. Thou- the community. This view has been held for a long
sand Oaks: SAGE. time; the President’s Commission on Law
1050 Diversion Programs

Enforcement and the Administration of Justice’s context, when programs bring more youth to the
report (1967) called for and led to the creation of formal juvenile justice system when they were
youth services bureaus to develop alternative pro- meant to do the opposite and when the youth
grams for juvenile offenders within local commu- would have never entered the system in the first
nities. The establishment of these bureaus, which place given that the system would have otherwise
quickly appeared in most communities, began a ignored their offenses (see Decker 1985). The
movement toward diverting youth, especially sta- second concern involves the disparate treatment
tus offenders and nonserious delinquents, from of minority youth; youth from some minority
the juvenile court. groups fare worse than others at each subsequent
The movement to use diversion has not been stage of processing by the juvenile justice system
without its challenges. In addition to the reality (see Leiber and Stairs 1999). Although the rea-
that not that many diversion programs exist, diver- sons for the disparate treatment are difficult to
sion programs remain challenged by the need to entangle, research in this area raises concerns
match offenders’ needs with programs’ services. about prejudice, discrimination, and civil rights
To complicate matters even more, evaluations of violations. Together, research in this area reports
diversion programs report mixed findings. Many important findings suggesting successes and mis-
studies have demonstrated that diversion pro- steps, and it highlights the need for efforts to
grams work, while some have found no impact, proceed cautiously as they seek to assist youth,
while still others have found them to be detrimen- families, and their communities.
tal (see Dembo et al. 2005). Reviews suggest that
the most successful diversion programs provide
more intensive and comprehensive services and References
use more experienced caseworkers (see Dryfoos
1990), and such provisions tend to be much less Campbell, J. S., & Retzlaff, P. (2000). Juvenile diversion
cost effective than was hoped and can actually be interventions: Client characteristics and program out-
comes. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 32, 57–73.
as costly as traditional programs. Still, leading Decker, S. H. (1985). A systemic analysis of diversion: Net
evaluations have found that interventions, regard- widening and beyond. Journal of Criminal Justice, 13,
less of whether conducted in traditional juvenile 207–216.
justice settings or in alternative programs, do Dembo, R., Wareham, J., Schmeidler, J., & Chirikos,
T. (2005). Evaluation of the impact of a policy change
result in increases in levels of perceived labeling on diversion program recidivism and justice agency
and self-reported delinquency among youth (see, costs: 12-month follow-up. Journal of Offender Reha-
e.g., Lipsey et al. 1981). These findings support bilitation, 41, 93–122.
those from other studies indicating that client Dryfoos, J. (1990). Adolescents at risk: Prevalence and
prevention. New York: Oxford University Press.
characteristics, especially arrest history, likely Leiber, M., & Stairs, J. (1999). Race, contexts, and the use
play an important role in determining the out- of intake diversion. Journal of Research in Crime and
comes of interventions (see Campbell and Delinquency, 36, 56–86.
Retzlaff 2000; Sullivan et al. 2007). These types Lipsey, M. W., Cordray, D. S., & Berger, D. E. (1981).
Evaluation of a juvenile diversion program: Using mul-
of findings are of considerable importance in that tiple lines of evidence. Evaluation Review, 5, 283–306.
they counter the reasons for using diversion pro- Lundman, R. J. (1993). Prevention and control of delin-
grams as they also highlight the problems encoun- quency (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
tered by traditional programs. President’s Commission on Law Enforcement and the
Administration of Justice. (1967). Task force report:
The lack of successes reported for at least some Juvenile delinquency and youth crime. Washington,
programs exacerbates other potentially negative DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
consequences of using diversion programs that Sullivan, C. J., Veysey, B. M., Hamilton, Z. K., & Grillo,
are deemed effective. Two potentially negative M. (2007). Reducing out-of-community placement and
recidivism – Diversion of delinquent youth with mental
repercussions are illustrative. The first concern is health and substance use problems from the justice
that diversion programs can result in “net- system. International Journal of Offender Therapy
widening” effects. Net-widening occurs, in this and Comparative Criminology, 51, 555–577.
Divorce 1051

children will experience their parents’ divorce.


Divorce Given the large number of families affected by
divorce, it is important to understand how
Jennifer E. Lansford experiencing parental divorce affects adolescents’
Center for Child and Family Policy, Duke adjustment. Two central questions are addressed
University, Durham, NC, USA in this entry. First, how does the experience of
parental divorce affect the adjustment of adoles-
cents? Second, what are the methodological
Overview issues, controversies, and gaps in knowledge that D
should be taken into account in understanding
This entry reviews research on associations links between parental divorce and adolescents’
between parental divorce and adolescents’ adjust- adjustment?
ment. First, evidence regarding associations The preponderance of the research evidence
between parental divorce and adolescents’ adjust- suggests that divorce has some negative effects
ment is presented. Although, on average, adoles- on adolescents’ adjustment but that these effects
cents whose parents divorce are at risk for more may be small in magnitude and not universal
externalizing and internalizing problems, lower (Lansford 2009). Externalizing behaviors (such
levels of academic achievement, and more prob- as aggression and delinquency), internalizing
lematic social relationships than are adolescents problems (such as depression and anxiety), aca-
whose parents do not divorce, most adolescents demic achievement (such as grades in school),
whose parents divorce do not have long-term and quality of social relationships (with peers
adjustment problems. Second, consideration is and parents) are the most frequently examined
paid to methodological issues, including the nec- indicators of adjustment in the divorce literature
essarily correlational nature of the data, control- (see Amato 2010 for a review). The largest effects
ling for potential confounds, and taking into appear to relate to adolescents’ relationships with
account not only the divorce of adolescents’ bio- their fathers. Studies that have compared the
logical parents but also exposure to subsequent adjustment of adolescents whose parents have
remarriages and divorces. Third, controversies divorced with the adjustment of adolescents
involving mechanisms that have been hypothe- whose parents have not divorced generally have
sized to account for links between parental found that adolescents whose parents have
divorce and adolescents’ adjustment are divorced are at greater risk for externalizing and
reviewed. These include attention to interparental internalizing problems, lower academic achieve-
conflict, economic disadvantage, disrupted par- ment, and more difficulties in social relationships,
enting, and parents’ distress as mediators of rela- both during adolescence and as they develop into
tions between divorce and adolescents’ adulthood.
adjustment. Fourth, gaps in knowledge regarding However, not all adolescents whose parents
the extent to which genetic factors interact with have divorced demonstrate these problems. In
environmental factors to shape adolescents’ fact, Hetherington and Kelly (2002) concluded
adjustment following their parents’ divorce are that 25% of individuals whose parents divorce
discussed. The entry culminates in a summary of have serious long-term behavioral, psychological,
conclusions. or social problems in adulthood, in comparison to
10% of individuals whose parents have stayed
together. Therefore, 75% of individuals whose
Studying Parental Divorce’s Effects on parents divorce do not have serious long-term
Adolescents impairment that lasts beyond adolescence into
adulthood. Similarly, Amato (2003) estimated
In the United States, approximately 50% of first that 10% of children who experience parental
marriages end in divorce, and 50% of American divorce experience poorer psychological
1052 Divorce

adjustment in adulthood than they would have if In most studies, age is tied to the time of data
their parents had stayed together, 18% of children collection rather than the time of divorce, making
who experience parental divorce have more con- it difficult to interpret age differences because it is
flict in their own marriages than they would have not clear whether they merely reflect a recency
if their parents had stayed together, and 35% of effect. The most common methodological
children who experience parental divorce have approach is to study children in a particular devel-
lower quality relationships with their fathers than opmental stage (e.g., early childhood, middle
they would have if their parents had stayed childhood, adolescence) and to compare the
together. Even when differences are found adjustment of those whose parents have divorced
between adolescents whose parents divorced and with the adjustment of those whose parents have
those whose parents stayed together, the effect not divorced. A limitation of this approach is that
sizes typically are small. As a whole, an extensive the length of time between the parents’ divorce
research literature suggests that the majority of and the time of participation in the study will vary
adolescents who experience parental divorce do considerably across the sample. To overcome this
not have long-term adjustment difficulties, but the limitation, Lansford et al. (2006) used the time of
risk of externalizing and internalizing problems, divorce as an anchor point and modeled trajecto-
poorer academic achievement, and problematic ries of adjustment over a period from 1 year prior
social relationships is greater, on average, for ado- to the divorce to 3 years after the divorce, using
lescents who experience parental divorce than for prospective longitudinal data collected annually
adolescents who do not. for 12 years. Results suggested that parental
The timing of divorce may be important, divorce during childhood (between kindergarten
although studies have shown mixed results. Com- and grade 5) exerted more adverse effects than
pared to adolescents, children may be less capable divorce during adolescence (between grades
of realistically assessing the causes and conse- 6 and 10) on internalizing and externalizing prob-
quences of divorce, may feel more anxious about lems, whereas divorce during adolescence exerted
abandonment, may be more likely to blame them- more adverse effects on academic achievement.
selves, and may be less able to take advantage of Methodologically, longitudinal trajectory ana-
resources outside the family to cope with the lyses are important because although one can
divorce. This explanation could account for find- control for prior adjustment in analyses predicting
ings from some studies that children experience adjustment at a single point in time, such analyses
more problems after parental divorce than do ado- do not allow for an examination of how develop-
lescents. Other research suggests that adjusting to ment unfolds over time. Adolescents often have
parents’ remarriage following divorce may be adjustment difficulties in close temporal proxim-
harder for adolescents than for children. One pos- ity to their parents’ divorce, but after an initial
sibility is that divorce is most likely to affect adjustment period, some of these difficulties may
aspects of adjustment that are developmentally subside. Other difficulties, however, may not
salient at the time of the divorce. For example, manifest themselves immediately after the
because of the developmental salience of aca- divorce but rather may not become apparent
demic achievement, identity development, sub- until adolescence or adulthood, at developmen-
stance use, and emerging romantic relationships tally salient points in time. For example, parental
during adolescence, these aspects of adjustment divorce may curtail educational achievement or
may be especially affected by divorce that occurs disrupt social relationships in ways that are not
during adolescence. Indeed, adolescents who apparent until children try to enter the labor mar-
have experienced parental divorce have been ket, establish romantic partnerships, or become
found to be at risk for early-onset sexual activity parents. A more comprehensive understanding
and substance use in adolescence, as well as other of adolescents’ long-term adjustment is gained
internalizing and externalizing problems that may by examining trajectories that extend from before
be less age dependent. the parents’ divorce to well after the divorce.
Divorce 1053

In addition to age, there are several other miti- more likely to have other risk factors not shown by
gating factors that contribute to whether and how parents who do not divorce, controlling for pre-
divorce is related to adolescents’ adjustment. One divorce adjustment, modeling trajectories of
of the most notable of these factors is adolescents’ adjustment that begin prior to parental divorce,
adjustment prior to their parents’ divorce. There is and handling selection effects in other ways is
evidence from prospective longitudinal studies fol- important (Amato and Anthony 2014).
lowing participants from childhood into adulthood Another measurement issue is that many ado-
that children whose parents eventually divorce lescents whose biological parents divorce are
have more adjustment problems prior to the exposed not just to that divorce but also to subse- D
divorce than do children whose parents do not quent remarriages and divorces. In studies that do
divorce, suggesting that differences in adjustment not take into account adolescents’ exposure to
after the divorce are not attributable entirely to the multiple marital transitions, adolescents’ adjust-
divorce per se. Another mitigating factor is chil- ment to divorce may be confounded with their
dren’s positive attributes (e.g., attractiveness, easy adjustment to additional marital transitions. Ado-
temperament, social competence). Positive attri- lescents exposed to multiple marital transitions
butes can promote resiliency following parental generally are at higher risk for adjustment diffi-
divorce, perhaps in part because children with pos- culties than are adolescents exposed to fewer mar-
itive attributes are more likely to receive support ital transitions, and there is evidence that parental
from teachers, peers, or others outside the family. remarriage (and issues associated with dating and
Many studies have investigated whether there are sexuality that remarriage raises) may be especially
gender differences in links between divorce and difficult for adolescents given the developmental
adolescents’ adjustment. The findings have been salience of emerging romantic relationships in
mixed and inconclusive, with some studies their own lives.
reporting worse outcomes for girls who experience
parental divorce, some studies reporting worse out-
comes for boys who experience parental divorce, Controversies
and some studies reporting no gender differences in
outcomes associated with parental divorce. A main controversy in the divorce literature is
whether divorce per se accounts for differences
in adjustment between adolescents whose parents
Measurement Issues have divorced versus those whose parents have
stayed together or whether differences between
Studies of links between parents’ divorce and ado- these groups of adolescents can be accounted for
lescents’ adjustment are necessarily correlational by other factors that are associated with parental
because children cannot be randomly assigned to divorce. Theory and research on effects of divorce
family structure groups. Therefore, uncontrolled on adolescents’ adjustment have suggested
variables may be responsible for links between interparental conflict, economic disadvantage,
parental divorce and adolescents’ adjustment. To disrupted parenting, and parental distress mecha-
overcome these measurement limitations, rigorous nisms as explanations of why divorce is associ-
studies have collected longitudinal data and con- ated with adolescents’ adjustment (e.g., Weaver
trolled for potential confounds. Examining longi- and Schofield 2015). Each of these mechanisms is
tudinal trajectories of adjustment makes it possible discussed in turn. The preponderance of the evi-
to track change over time from before the divorce dence suggests that taking into account these
occurs to some period after the divorce. Because explanatory mechanisms attenuates, but does not
adolescents whose parents eventually divorce entirely eliminate, associations between divorce
show poorer adjustment prior to the divorce than and adolescents’ adjustment.
do children whose parents do not divorce and First, an interparental conflict perspective has
because parents who eventually divorce are also received the most empirical support as an
1054 Divorce

explanation of why divorce is associated with resources also can be disruptive for adolescents
adolescents’ adjustment. High levels of by leading to changes such as moves into more
interparental conflict have negative and long- dangerous neighborhoods or worse schools. Finan-
lasting implications for adolescents’ adjustment, cial hardships can lead to behavioral and emotional
especially if this conflict leads adolescents to feel problems and may mediate the link between paren-
caught between their parents or forced to take tal divorce and adolescents’ adjustment.
sides. Adolescents exposed to overt interparental Third, according to a disrupted parenting per-
conflict such as screaming, insulting, hitting, or spective, divorce disrupts parents’ ability to par-
threatening have been found to show higher levels ent well, which, in turn, is detrimental to
of externalizing problems than adolescents not adolescents’ adjustment. Regardless of family
exposed to overt interparental conflict. Exposure structure, parenting problems are related to ado-
to covert interparental conflict such as having the lescents’ adjustment difficulties. For example,
adolescent carry messages between parents or low levels of monitoring and supervision are
putting down the other parent in discussions related to increases in adolescents’ problem
with the adolescent has been associated with behavior such as substance use and risky sexual
higher levels of internalizing problems for adoles- behavior. Following divorce, parents may not be
cents. Dissolution of a high-conflict marriage able to monitor or supervise their adolescents as
might reduce adolescents’ exposure to the stress closely because there is now one adult rather than
of daily conflict between parents and the modeling two adults to do the monitoring, and adolescents
of dysfunctional interpersonal interactions this may therefore spend more time at home without a
entails, resulting in improved adjustment for ado- parent present. Furthermore, parent-adolescent
lescents. The dissolution of a low-conflict mar- conflict often increases following divorce, and
riage, on the other hand, may come as more of a parents’ discipline becomes less consistent.
surprise for adolescents and they may experience Because positive relationships with parents can
more of a sense of loss rather than relief, contrib- serve a protective function in terms of reducing
uting to an increase in adjustment problems. adolescents’ vulnerability to negative peer influ-
Taken together, the implication is that reducing ences, a decline in parent–child relationship qual-
adolescents’ exposure to interparental conflict ity can contribute to adolescents’ behavior
benefits their adjustment, regardless of whether problems. However, just as adolescents with
parents are married or divorced (see Sandler divorced parents have been found to show worse
et al. 2008). Therefore, the effects of divorce will adjustment before the divorce than adolescents
depend, at least in part, on how the divorce relates whose parents do not divorce, parents who even-
to adolescents’ exposure to interparental conflict. tually divorce engage in more problematic parent-
Second, an economic disadvantage perspective ing practices before the divorce than do parents
suggests that socioeconomic changes following who do not divorce. Therefore, neither parenting
parental divorce mediate the association between problems nor adolescent adjustment problems can
divorce and adolescents’ adjustment. A drop in be attributed solely to the divorce itself.
household income often accompanies divorce. Fourth, a parental distress perspective suggests
According to the US Bureau of the Census, 43% that parents’ well-being mediates the link between
of children in single mother and 26% of children parental divorce and children’s adjustment. This
in single father households live in poverty, com- mechanism often is linked with economic disadvan-
pared to 10% of children in two-parent families. tage and disrupted parenting perspectives to explain
Because adolescents most often live with their the links as follows: Divorce and its attendant eco-
mothers after their parents divorce, they may not nomic implications increase parents’ depression,
have access to the same financial resources as anxiety, and stress, which disrupt parents’ ability
before the divorce (especially if they are not receiv- to parent well, which then negatively affects ado-
ing regular child support payments from non- lescents’ adjustment. Conversely, this perspective
residential fathers). Having fewer financial suggests that if parents cope well with the divorce
Divorce 1055

and are able to maintain high-quality relationships with one another to shape adolescents’ adjustment.
with their adolescents, then adolescents’ adjustment For example, future research would benefit from
will be less affected by the divorce. examining how genetic factors influence adoles-
In investigations of all four explanations for cents’ susceptibility to particular environmental
why divorce is associated with adolescents’ experiences, including parental divorce, and
adjustment problems, controlling for the explana- whether problems associated with divorce are
tory factor (i.e., interparental conflict, economic evidenced primarily by adolescents at genetic risk.
disadvantage, parenting problems, parent distress)
attenuates the link between parental divorce and D
adolescents’ adjustment. However, the associa- Conclusions
tion between divorce and adolescents’ adjustment
typically does not disappear entirely after control- Instead of simply comparing the adjustment of
ling for these other factors. Researchers generally adolescents whose parents have and have not
have concluded that divorce poses a risk factor divorced, researchers have used increasingly
above and beyond these other risk factors for sophisticated longitudinal methods to understand
adolescent adjustment difficulties. how trajectories of adjustment followed from
before to several years after parental divorce unfold
as well as mechanisms that may account for links
Gaps in Knowledge between parental divorce and adolescents’ adjust-
ment. Controlling for adolescents’ adjustment prior
A main gap in knowledge involves the extent to to the divorce as well as other potential confounds
which genetic factors interact with environmental attenuates the association between the experience
factors to shape adolescents’ adjustment following of parental divorce and adolescents’ adjustment. It
their parents’ divorce. Parents with problems such is important to keep in mind the large variability
as depression, substance use, or antisocial behavior among individuals in adolescents’ trajectories of
are at risk of selecting spouses with similar prob- adjustment before and after parental divorce.
lems, and these problems increase both marital Although, on average, adolescents whose parents
conflict and the likelihood that parents will divorce. divorce are at risk for more externalizing and inter-
Offspring of parents with these problems are at risk nalizing problems, lower levels of academic
of similar problems because of both genetic and achievement, and more problematic social relation-
shared environmental experiences (D’Onofrio ships than are adolescents whose parents do not
et al. 2006). It is clear that genetic and environmen- divorce, most adolescents whose parents divorce
tal contributions both shape whether individuals do not have long-term adjustment problems.
will eventually divorce and, if they do, how their A combination of interparental conflict, economic
adolescents may adjust to the divorce. For exam- disadvantage, disrupted parenting, and parental
ple, the role of genetic factors in divorce has been distress that precedes and follows divorce interacts
supported by twin studies showing that monozy- in complex ways with divorce itself to affect adjust-
gotic twins are more concordant than dizygotic ment during adolescence and as adolescents
twins in their divorces. The role of environmental develop into adults.
factors has been supported by adoption studies
showing that children who experience their adop-
tive parents’ divorce have higher levels of behavior References
problems than do children whose adoptive parents
do not divorce. Future research will help address Amato, P. (2003). Reconciling divergent perspectives:
gaps in knowledge regarding the exact nature of Judith Wallerstein, qualitative family research, and
children of divorce. Family Relations, 52, 332–339.
which genetic versus environmental factors con- Amato, P. R. (2010). Research on divorce: Continuing
tribute to distinct developmental outcomes and trends and new developments. Journal of Marriage
how genetic and environmental factors interact and Family, 72, 650–666.
1056 Domestic Violence

Amato, P. R., & Anthony, C. J. (2014). Estimating the over one third of women (35.6%) and over one
effects of parental divorce and death with fixed effects fourth of men (28.5%) report experiencing phys-
models. Journal of Marriage and Family, 76, 370–386.
D’Onofrio, B. M., Turkheimer, E., Emery, R. E., Slutske, ical, sexual, or stalking behavior by an intimate
W. S., Heath, A. C., Madden, P. A., et al. (2006). partner (Black et al. 2011). Adding to these
A genetically informed study of the processes underly- alarming statistics is the fact that many instances
ing the association between parental marital instability of domestic violence take place in the presence of
and offspring adjustment. Developmental Psychology,
42, 486–499. children and adolescents. Results from the
Hetherington, E. M., & Kelly, J. (2002). For better or National Survey on Children’s Exposure to Vio-
worse. New York: Norton. lence conducted between 2013 and 2014 suggest
Lansford, J. E. (2009). Parental divorce and children’s that 5.8% of children between ages 0 and 17 in the
adjustment. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4,
140–152. United States witnessed a physical assault
Lansford, J. E., Malone, P. S., Castellino, D. R., Dodge, between partners during the past year and that
K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Trajectories of 15.8% of children witness a physical assault
internalizing, externalizing, and grades for children between partners during their lifetime (Finkelhor
who have and have not experienced their parents’
divorce. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 292–301. et al. 2015). Further, 43% of households that law
Sandler, I., Miles, J., Cookston, J., & Braver, S. (2008). enforcement in a large Northeast county investi-
Effects of father and mother parenting on children’s gated for domestic violence crimes had children
mental health in high- and low-conflict divorces. Fam- and nearly all of these children (95%) were
ily Court Review, 46, 282–296.
Weaver, J. M., & Schofield, T. J. (2015). Mediation and exposed to this violence (Fusco and Fantuzzo
moderation of divorce effects on children’s behavior 2009). Some estimates have suggested that 9%
problems. Journal of Family Psychology, 29, 39–48. of adolescents are exposed to domestic violence
within their homes annually (Zinzow et al. 2009).
Because domestic violence is ongoing in many
households (Rand and Saltzman 2003), many of
Domestic Violence these adolescents have been exposed to repeated
instances of violence between caregivers, begin-
Sarah E. Evans1, Ruby Charak2, Laura E Watkins3 ning in childhood and continuing throughout their
and David DiLillo1 development.
1
Department of Psychology, Clinical Psychology
Training Program, University of
Nebraska – Lincoln, Lincoln, NE, USA Understanding Definitions and
2
Department of Psychological Science, The Terminology
University of Texas-Rio Grande Valley, College
of Liberal Arts, Edinburg, TX, USA Domestic violence “broadly refers to the intimate
3
Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of context within which one partner is abused by
Medicine, VA Connecticut Healthcare System, another” (Holt et al. 2008, p. 798). This term
National Center for PTSD-Clinical Neuroscience generally refers to physically, sexually, or psycho-
Division, West Haven, CT, USA logically aggressive acts perpetrated by one inti-
mate partner against another. Acts of physical
aggression encompassed by domestic violence
Overview include hitting, slapping, or kicking, while acts
of sexual aggression range from sexual coercion
Domestic violence is a widespread problem to forced sexual intercourse. Psychological
throughout the world. A global research review aggression includes acts such as insulting, humil-
utilizing samples from 48 countries revealed that iating, or intimidating, and behaviors such as
10–69% of women reported experiencing physi- stalking or isolating a victim from friends or fam-
cal violence at the hands of an intimate partner or ily. Although domestic violence is a commonly
spouse (Heise et al. 1999). In the United States, used term to refer to violence that occurs between
Domestic Violence 1057

caregivers within the home, in some countries intervene), or experience the aftermath (i.e.,
domestic violence refers to partner violence and injury, emergency response) of aggressive acts
also encompasses child or elder abuse, or abuse by that occur between caregivers (Evans et al.
any member of a household (World Health Orga- 2008).
nization [W.H. O.] 2012). Other terms used for
violence between caregivers, include, intimate
partner aggression, intimate partner violence, Categories of Exposure
interparental violence, domestic abuse, and mari-
tal violence. The terms “wife battering” and In response to the lack of consensus regarding D
“spousal abuse” came about because much of what constitutes “exposure to domestic vio-
the early domestic violence research focused on lence,” research trends have shifted from classi-
male-perpetrated violence toward females (Wolak fying adolescents dichotomously as exposed and
and Finkelhor 1998). Recently, these terms have nonexposed to the recognition that exposure to
fallen out of favor due to criticisms that they are domestic violence is a multifaceted, dimensional
less inclusive. Literature suggests that women experience. Holden (2003) concluded that there
perpetrate domestic violence as well (Straus are as many as ten different levels of exposure to
2005; Swan et al. 2008; Williams et al. 2008). domestic violence, including, among others, pre-
These findings indicate that many children and natal exposure, eyewitness exposure, and partic-
adolescents are exposed to domestic violence in ipation in the acts. While adolescents may
which their mother is the perpetrator, to bidirec- visually witness the acts as they occur, it is
tional acts of violence between caregivers, and to equally plausible that exposure could include
violence in same-sex relationships. overhearing the acts from another room, seeing
Different terms have been used to describe only the injury sustained or emergency response
adolescents who are affected by domestic vio- following the acts, or even just learning about the
lence. The earliest research in the field often used events from a family member at a later time.
the terms “observer” or “witness,” but recent Further complicating matters, additional vari-
research has begun to use the term “exposed” ability exists with regard to the severity of the
when referring to youth who have endured acts to which youth are exposed. For example,
these acts within their home (Holden 2003). Def- some adolescents may have been exposed to
initions of exposure vary with regard to the verbal aggression only, while others may have
threshold that must be met to consider a youth been exposed to physical aggression of varying
“exposed.” Characteristics of the acts, such as the severity, ranging from slapping or throwing an
frequency, the duration of exposure, and the level object all the way to strangulation and assault
of exposure (i.e., saw acts, heard acts from with a deadly weapon. Still, others may have
another room, learned about acts later) all have been exposed to sexual aggression in the form
the potential to influence classification. While of low severity acts such as verbal coercion but
some studies classify adolescents as victims if also in the form of unwanted touching and even
they have ever been exposed to domestic vio- sexual assault (Holt et al. 2008).
lence, other studies only include adolescents
who have been exposed to domestic violence
during the past year. Still others require that Methodological Issues
adolescents be exposed to at least two severely
violent acts in order to be classified as a victim. The methodological issues that permeate research
Despite the lack of standardization across studies on exposure to domestic violence have been well
regarding what constitutes exposure, most documented (Clements et al. 2008; Edleson
researchers agree that exposure to domestic vio- 1999a; Fantuzzo and Lindquist 1989). In general,
lence occurs when youth visually witness, over- studies of exposure to domestic violence and its
hear, are directly involved in (i.e., attempt to effects have used a narrow range of samples, and
1058 Domestic Violence

yet a diverse range of assessments. Highlighted regard to whether the youth were exposed to the
here are some of the major methodological issues acts and the nature of those acts (Holden 2003;
concerning the study of exposure to domestic O’Brien et al. 1994; Sternberg et al. 1998). Addi-
violence. tionally, child abuse and other adversities often
co-occur with domestic violence, and hence while
Sampling Issues investigating the risk of domestic violence the
One criticism of the exposure to domestic vio- influence of other risk factors such as child mal-
lence literature is the overreliance on samples treatment should be accounted for. Finally, differ-
obtained from battered women’s shelters ent types and levels of exposure to domestic
(Edleson 1999a). Several concerns have been violence have different short- and long-term con-
raised with regard to these samples. Many previ- sequences on a child’s functioning (Kitzmann
ous studies recruiting from such settings have et al. 2003). Thus, a more comprehensive investi-
assumed that the youth were exposed to domestic gation would include the unique effect of expo-
violence rather than directly assessing levels of sure to domestic violence and the additive effect
exposure (Fantuzzo and Lindquist 1989; Kashani of coexisting risk factors while taking into
and Allan 1998). Moreover, it has been postulated account levels of severity.
that studying children and adolescents obtained
from this setting may result in findings that do not Measurement Options
generalize to all youth for two reasons. First, there Another methodological issue, and likely contrib-
has been speculation that children in these settings utor to the lack of consensus in defining exposure
have been exposed to more severe violence, to domestic violence, is that the standardized
resulting in more extreme outcomes. Furthermore, options for assessing “exposure” are relatively
some researchers have argued that residing in a limited. As a result, many studies to date have
shelter is itself a stressor that may induce out- assumed the youth were exposed based on the
comes similar to those hypothesized to occur in sample source (i.e., domestic violence shelter).
victims of exposure to domestic violence Other studies have simply utilized unstandardized
(Margolin 1998; McIntosh 2003). screening questions to classify youth dichoto-
mously as either exposed or nonexposed. Increas-
Measurement Issues ingly, researchers have adapted a standardized
There is inherent difficulty in measuring both caregiver self-report measure or interview to
instances of domestic violence and youth. This assess caregivers’ perceptions of whether their
setting may result in findings that do not general- child had been exposed to domestic violence.
ize to all youth for two reasons. First, they have The original and revised Conflict Tactics
witnessed, and may assume the youth were not Scales (CTS; Straus 1979; Straus et al. 1996) are
exposed because they were asleep or outside of the most common measure used to assess the
the home during the acts (Holden 2003; Jaffe et al. occurrence and frequency of domestic violence
1990; McIntosh 2003). Other times, caregivers and exposure to domestic violence (Hamby and
may be hesitant to provide the information for Finkelhor 2001). The CTS is used to detect fre-
fear that doing so might result in legal conse- quency and severity of aggressive behaviors
quences to themselves (Clements et al. 2008; occurring within the context of an intimate rela-
Kantor and Little 2003). Few studies have tionship. The instrument assesses acts that
attempted to collect information regarding the occurred both over the past 12 months and over
nature of the acts and resulting exposure from the entire history of the relationship. It includes
more than one informant (Clements et al. 2008). items designed to measure different aspects of
Those that have attempted to collect information conflict including physical aggression and
from multiple informants reveals low agreement resulting injury, psychological aggression, and
between the youth exposed to the violence, care- sexual coercion. Studies using the CTS have gen-
givers, and law enforcement officials, both with erally fallen into three categories: those that have
Domestic Violence 1059

administered the CTS to a caregiver and then Domestic Violence Scale (CEDV; Edleson et al.
assumed that dependents were exposed to the 2008; Shin and Edelson 2007). Designed to assess
acts, and those that utilized a version of the CTS self-reported exposure to domestic violence in
adapted to assess more explicitly whether minors youth ages 10–16, an initial study indicates that
in the household were exposed to the acts, and this measure has demonstrated test–retest and
those that utilized a version of the CTS adapted to convergent validity (Edleson et al. 2008).
assess more explicitly whether minors in the
household were exposed to the acts (e.g., Kolbo
1996). Unfortunately, given the lack of a standard- Outcomes of Exposure to Domestic D
ized definition of “exposure,” adaptations of the Violence
CTS vary widely (Jouriles et al. 2001). Moreover,
most studies using the CTS have been done with Despite these methodological and measurement
school-aged children rather than adolescents issues, an increasing body of empirical studies
(Evans et al. 2008). Other less used standardized has established that exposure to domestic violence
measures of exposure to domestic violence is associated with adverse outcomes among both
administered to caregivers include the Overt Hos- children and adolescents. Moreover, numerous
tility/O’Leary-Porter Scale (Porter and O’Leary reviews (Clements et al. 2008; Edleson 1999a;
1980) and the Family Disagreements Interview Fantuzzo and Lindquist 1989; Holden 2003;
(Salzinger et al. 1992). To date, however, there Holt et al. 2008; MacMillan and Wathen 2014;
appear to be no caregiver measures of child or Voisin and Sung Hong 2012) and meta-analytic
adolescent exposure to domestic violence that studies (Chan and Yeung 2009; Evans et al. 2008;
have been subjected to meticulous psychometric Kitzmann et al. 2003; Wolfe et al. 2003b) have
study (Edleson et al. 2007; Jouriles et al. 2001). synthesized the literature documenting child and
Several researchers have developed measures adolescent outcomes associated with exposure to
of child or adolescent exposure to domestic domestic violence. In general, these outcomes
violence that have been subjected to meticulous have been classified as biological/physical, cog-
psychometric evaluation, including the Children’s nitive/academic, emotional/internalizing, behav-
Perception of Interparental Conflict Scale ioral/externalizing, and social/interpersonal.
(CPICS; Grych et al. 1992), the Violence Expo-
sure Scale (Fox and Leavitt 1996), the Things Biological/Physical
I Have Seen and Heard Scale (TISH; Richters A growing body of literature conceptualizing
and Martinez 1992), and the Victimization Scale exposure as a traumatic event has attempted to
(Nadel et al. 1996). An additional measure, the determine whether children and adolescents
Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire (JVQ; exposed to domestic violence evidence effects in
Finkelhor et al. 2005) contains both a youth self- their biological development. Consistent with
report and a caregiver self-report scale. However, other childhood traumatic experiences,
the majority of these measures only assess expo- researchers have suggested that, exposure to
sure in preschool and school-aged or middle domestic violence may cause biological alter-
school children. Only one of these measures ations to the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
(i.e., the JVQ) was designed to assess exposure (HPA) axis stress response system and parts of
in adolescents up to age 17 (Finkelhor et al. 2005; the brain (Gunnar and Donzella 2002; Saltzman
Hamby et al. 2011). Additionally, the suitability et al. 2005; Tsavoussis et al. 2014). Cortisol is the
of another (i.e., the CPICS) for use with adoles- major hormonal product of HPA system activation
cents age 17 through 19 was explored post- and can be used as a physiological indicator of
development (Bickham and Fiese 1997). One stress. Several studies have examined cortisol
promising option that may address the shortage reactivity among children who have been exposed
of empirically validated measures of exposure to to domestic violence. Overall, findings have
domestic violence is that of the Child Exposure to suggested greater cortisol reactivity to stressful
1060 Domestic Violence

or emotion-eliciting tasks among children domestic violence at this developmental level.


exposed to domestic violence as compared to Additionally, longitudinal, prospective research
children who have not been exposed (Davies following exposed children into adolescence and
et al. 2011; Hibel et al. 2011; Saltzman et al. beyond is needed to determine the long-term
2005; Towe-Goodman et al. 2012). However, impact of exposure to domestic violence on bio-
some research has not replicated these findings, logical and physical development.
with children exposed to violence showing no
differences in cortisol reactivity (Hibel et al. Cognitive/Academic
2009) or demonstrating lower cortisol reactivity Few studies have assessed associations between
in response to a laboratory simulation of exposure to domestic violence and cognitive or
interparental conflict (Sturge-Apple et al. 2012). academic abilities in adolescents. While findings
Further, the majority of this work has examined suggest that exposure to domestic violence is asso-
infants, toddlers, or young children. Thus, addi- ciated with decreased attention, IQ, reading ability,
tional research is needed to clarify impact of and verbal skills (Graham-Bermann et al. 2009;
domestic violence exposure on cortisol reactivity Huth-Bocks et al. 2001; Horn Ratner et al. 2006;
particularly among adolescents. Koenen et al. 2003; Towe-Goodman et al. 2012),
Similarly, differences in brain structure have most studies to date have been conducted with
been found among children exposed to domestic young children, rather than adolescents. One
violence. For example, among child and adoles- investigation of youth age 11–13 did link exposure
cent victims of co-occurring child maltreatment to family violence with decreased academic per-
and exposure to domestic violence who were formance (Thompson and Massat 2003). More-
diagnosed with posttraumatic stress disorder, sev- over, the meta-analysis conducted by Kitzmann
eral differences in brain development were noted et al. (2003) examined linkages between exposure
when compared to nontraumatized controls to domestic violence and academic outcomes.
(De Bellis et al. 1999; 2002). Further, young Results revealed significantly lower outcomes for
adults who were exposed to domestic violence youth exposed to domestic violence. However,
as children, but not physical or sexual maltreat- this effect was found across all age groups, rather
ment, demonstrated alterations in the brain when than specifically for adolescents. Given the dearth
compared to individuals who had not been of research in this area, it is not possible to draw
exposed to domestic violence or other forms of conclusions regarding the impact of exposure to
child maltreatment (Choi et al. 2012). However, violence on the academic and cognitive function-
none of these studies examined brain develop- ing of adolescents specifically.
ment among a sample of only adolescents, and it
is impossible to say whether the differences noted Emotional/Internalizing
in the De Bellis studies are attributable to expo- In contrast to the dearth of research on the cogni-
sure to domestic violence alone or to the tive effects of exposure to domestic violence, a
co-occurrence of multiple forms of exposure and large body of literature has examined emotional,
child maltreatment. behavioral, and social outcomes. Adolescents
In addition to impacting biological function- who have been exposed to domestic violence
ing, domestic violence exposure may also affect demonstrate greater overall psychological dis-
children’s physical health. Two studies demon- tress, including anxiety, depression, and external-
strated a link between exposure to IPV and izing behaviors (Ellonen et al. 2013) and more
increased risk for children being overweight or general internalizing symptoms (Moylan et al.
obese (Boynton-Jarrett et al. 2010; Jun et al. 2010). More specifically, teens exposed to domes-
2012). However, no research has examined this tic violence tend to exhibit greater levels of
relationship among adolescents. Thus, further depression (Zinzow et al. 2009), anxiety (Mrug
research is needed utilizing adolescent samples and Windle 2010; Pelcovitz et al. 2000), and
to determine the full extent of exposure to posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms (Lang
Domestic Violence 1061

and Smith Stover 2008; Zinzow et al. 2009) than However, not all studies have found these gender
adolescents not exposed to domestic violence. differences (Grant et al. 2004), and these differ-
Effect sizes from meta-analyses range from ences appear to be reversed when examining sub-
d = .38 to .50 for internalizing symptoms stance or alcohol use problems (Fagan and Wright
(Chan and Yeung 2009; Evans et al. 2008; 2011; Smith et al. 2010).
Kitzmann et al. 2003; Wolfe et al. 2003b). Three
of four meta-analyses reveal positive linkages Social/Interpersonal
between exposure to domestic violence and Finally, evidence also links exposure to domestic
trauma symptoms (Chan and Yeung 2009; Evans violence to disturbances in adolescents’ interper- D
et al. 2008; Kitzmann et al. 2003), although the sonal functioning. Meta-analyses reveal that chil-
number of studies aggregated to produce these dren and adolescents exposed to domestic
outcomes was only k = 4, k = 6, and k = 12, violence have significantly poorer social out-
respectively. Interestingly, some research suggests comes than do nonexposed youth (Chan and
that both child and adolescent females may be Yeung 2009; Kitzmann et al. 2003). Specifically,
more prone to experience internalizing and trauma adolescents exposed to domestic violence may
symptoms in response to exposure to domestic have less secure attachment styles (Levendosky
violence than are males (Buka et al. 2001; Moretti et al. 2002) and more disturbances in peer rela-
et al. 2006). Further research is needed to clarify tionships (Dawud-Noursi et al. 1998). Youth who
potential gender differences in emotional and are exposed to domestic violence also may rely on
internalizing symptoms among adolescents avoidance as a coping strategy for social problems
exposed to domestic violence. (McGee and Baker 2002).
In addition, exposure to domestic violence may
Behavioral/Externalizing impact beliefs about aggression and aggressive
Similar to internalizing symptoms, and consistent behavior during adolescence. Adolescent males
with the arguments of social learning theorists, exposed to domestic violence appear more likely
adolescents exposed to domestic violence have to approve of violent acts in general and to believe
an increased risk for a range of behavioral or that acting violently bolsters one’s social reputa-
externalizing symptoms. Data from numerous tion (Carlson 1991; Spaccarelli et al. 1995). More-
empirical studies suggest greater rates of conduct over, some evidence suggest that adolescents who
problems and delinquency (Baldry 2007; Ellonen are exposed to domestic violence may be more
et al. 2013; Ireland and Smith 2009), as well as aggressive toward others (Moretti et al. 2006;
more aggression toward parents, peers, and ani- Mrug and Windle 2010; Sousa et al. 2011). How-
mals (Duncan et al. 2005; McCloskey and Lichter ever, other research has found a lack of an associ-
2003; Moretti et al. 2006). Further, witnessing ation between witnessing domestic violence and
domestic violence during adolescence has been adolescent violence (Fagan and Wright 2011).
linked to greater frequency of drug use among Driven in part by the intergenerational transmis-
girls, but not boys (Fagan and Wright, 2011), sion of violence theory (see Widom 1989), one
and increased likelihood of problematic alcohol domain of adolescent aggressive behavior that has
use in young adulthood for women, but not men received attention in relation to exposure to
(Smith et al. 2010). Effect sizes from meta- domestic violence is that of teen dating violence
analyses range from d = .42 to .47 for externaliz- (Ireland and Smith 2009). Consistent with social
ing symptoms (Chan and Yeung 2009; Evans et al. learning theory, it has been postulated that adoles-
2008; Kitzmann et al. 2003; Wolfe et al. 2003b). cents who are exposed to domestic violence learn
In contrast to the gender differences with internal- styles of expressing anger that put them at risk for
izing symptoms, several studies suggest that perpetrating violence in teen dating relationships
males are more prone to experience externalizing (Wolfe and Foshee 2003). Indeed, several
symptoms than are females (Baldry 2007; research studies suggest that adolescents exposed
Flannery et al. 2001; Sternberg et al. 2006). to domestic violence within the home are at an
1062 Domestic Violence

increased risk of both perpetrating and being vic- violence has a detrimental impact on adolescent
timized by dating violence (Foshee et al. 2008; health. Specifically, studies have found that expo-
O’Keefe 1998), with adolescent males being more sure to intimate partner violence during childhood
likely to perpetrate violence than are females increases the risk of substance use disorder among
(O’Keefe 1998). However, other studies have older adolescents (Skeer et al. 2009), and expo-
failed to establish a link between exposure to sure during the past year during adolescence is
domestic violence and subsequent dating violence associated with psychological distress and sub-
(Gagne et al. 2005). Thus, research linking expo- stance use in young adults (Schiff et al. 2014).
sure to domestic violence and later aggressive Hence, recent studies have started to address the
behavior is somewhat mixed. Further research issue of short- and long-term effects of domestic
identifying potential mediators or moderators violence on adolescents although the need for
may help explain this inconsistent relationship. studies examining different types of psychopath-
ological outcomes is warranted.

Developmental Considerations
Theoretical Considerations
Because coping abilities and cognitive appraisal
in response to violent acts may evolve with age, Several theories have been postulated to explain
effects of exposure to domestic violence may the mechanisms by which domestic violence may
manifest differently in youth of different develop- impact children and adolescents. Attachment the-
mental stages. However, no known critical or ory states that, in interacting with caregivers and
sensitive period for the child’s exposure to IPV other family members, children develop mental
has been established. In tandem with this, representations, or working models, of them-
although research has been conducted on children selves. If the family environment is volatile and
in various developmental stages, no clear pattern childcare is compromised, then a young child
of age-related symptoms has emerged (Margolin representations, or working models results in the
1998). Of the four meta-analytic studies, only one child developing attachments of poor quality
revealed significant age effects (i.e., Wolfe et al. (Shemmings and Shemmings 2011). Insecure or
2003b), finding that exposure to domestic vio- disorganized attachment patterns in turn being a
lence resulted in higher effect sizes for school chain reaction of aggression and interpersonal
age children than for adolescents or preschoolers. problems in children and adolescents. Researchers
However, follow-up analyses excluding two stud- advocating a social learning perspective (Bandura
ies hypothesized to be outliers resulted in the age 1986), particularly a cognitive-ecological model
effect disappearing. Findings from empirical stud- within this broader view, suggest that through
ies suggest that adolescents may not be as affected exposure to domestic violence, children and ado-
by exposure to domestic violence as are younger lescents learn to use aggression as a means of
children (Levendosky et al. 2002). A few expla- resolving conflict (Hotaling and Sugarman
nations have been posited for these findings. It 1986). Social learning theory has been used to
could be that adolescents are better able to remove explain the intergenerational transmission of vio-
themselves from the situation, are generally more lence in which children who are abused or who
emotionally distant from caregivers due to their witness violence are at higher risk of perpetrating
developmental stage, or are better able to under- similar acts later in life. According to Margolin
stand that the acts are not their fault (Graham- (1998), “the intergenerational data, taken as a
Bermann and Levendosky 1998; Levendosky whole, indicate that the childhood experience of
et al. 2002). witnessing interparental violence can lay the foun-
On the other hand, the few longitudinal studies dation for an aggressively oriented behavioral
that have been conducted so far on children and repertoire as well as different expectations regard-
adolescents suggest that exposure to domestic ing aggression in adult intimate relationships”
Domestic Violence 1063

(p. 63). To date, however, support for a social all exposed children and adolescents have diffi-
learning perspective on exposure to domestic vio- culties. For example, the meta-analysis by
lence, and the intergenerational transmission of Kitzmann et al. (2003) revealed that 37% of
violence, has been mixed (see Stith et al. 2000 exposed youth were functioning as well or even
for a review). Moreover, while social learning better than youth in nonexposed samples. Thus,
theory is helpful in explaining the externalizing recent attention has been devoted to explaining
and behavioral symptoms (i.e., childhood aggres- the variability in the functioning of youth who are
sion, adolescent delinquency, and dating violence) exposed to such acts. Several theories have been
observed in violence-exposed youth, it is less offered to explain the variability in functioning D
useful in accounting for the broad range of inter- observed in many empirical studies. One area
nalizing sequelae. that has received much attention is the role that a
In contrast, family systems theorists postulate youth’s perception of the acts plays in subsequent
that exposure to domestic violence creates a par- adjustment. Grych and Fincham’s (1990)
adox that leads youth to interpret their families, cognitive-contextual model posits that youth
which are supposed to provide safety and protec- respond to domestic violence by both trying to
tion, as chaotic (Margolin 2005). Simultaneously, interpret the meaning of the conflict and identify
violence perpetrated between caregivers may the role they might have played in the events. This
result in increased risk of psychopathology theory holds that the impact of the conflict is
among the victimized caregiver. These processes shaped by contextual as well as developmental
work in tandem and cause strain to the family unit, factors. Thus, different contextual, or mediating,
which in turn may increase stress levels and sub- variables (i.e., exposure to other family dysfunc-
sequent psychopathology among the exposed tion or community violence; co-occurring child
youth (Margolin 1998, 2005). Similarly, trauma maltreatment) as well as the age and cognitive
theory posits that exposure to domestic violence is level of the youth will influence how the child
an environmental stressor that threatens a youth’s perceives the conflict and ultimately reacts to it
sense of personal safety and security (de Bellis (Grych and Fincham 1993). Consistent with this
2001). This theory holds that these perceived theory, studies have shown that when children
threats lead to disturbances in cognitive, biologi- blame themselves for conflict or perceive the con-
cal, and psychological functioning that are seen in flict as a threat to their security, they exhibit more
traumatized youth. Moreover, these disturbances adjustment problems (Fosco and Grych 2008;
at a young age can set the stage for the develop- Gerard et al. 2005). Examining externalizing
ment of additional emotional and behavioral dis- behavior, this framework emphasizes that sense
turbances as the youth matures (Margolin 2005). of threat leads to hypervigilance and heightens
In addition to these theories, the ecological theory sensitivity to signs of anger and conflict both
elucidates the role of violence faced across multi- within and outside of the home (Fosco et al.
ple nested levels or contexts, such as family, 2007). Another mechanism between exposure to
school, neighborhood, and society on an adoles- intimate partner violence and externalizing behav-
cent family (Bronfenbrenner 1979). Oftentimes ior is the belief in children that the aggression is
child witnesses of domestic violence face addi- justified (Fosco et al. 2007; Jouriles et al. 2014).
tional adversities, such as poverty, low parental Other mediational theories have focused on the
education, and neighborhood crime that increase relationship between the youth and the parent. For
the risk of adolescent psychopathology. example, Davies and Cummings’s (1994) emo-
tional security hypothesis posits that the effects
of interparental conflict stem from the quality of
Mediating and Moderating Factors the relationship between the youth and the parent
as well as the quality of the caregiver to their
While research suggests that exposure to domestic security; they exhibit more adjustment problems.
violence can be detrimental to development, not This theory holds the poor quality parent–child
1064 Domestic Violence

relationship, or assuaged if the parent–child bond 1998; Edleson 1999b). The various emotional and
is relatively strong (Sturge-Apple et al. 2012). behavioral problems of child maltreatment have
Similarly, while originally developed to explain been well documented and are similar to the
the youth also have been hypothesized to medi- sequelae of exposure to domestic violence
ate associations between exposure and subse- (Widom 2000). Several researchers have argued
quent psychology (Buchanan et al. 1991). that the field of child maltreatment have been well
Loyalty conflicts are thought to occur when, in documented and are similar to the sequelae of
the context of intimate partner conflict, youth exposure to domestic violence between youth
feel pressured to align themselves with one par- who have and have not been exposed to domestic
ent against the other. Indeed, some research indi- violence (Clements et al. 2008; Edleson 1999a;
cates that greater loyalty conflict is associated Fantuzzo and Lindquist 1989; Hanson et al.
with adjustment problems among adolescents 2006).
(Buchanan et al. 1991). Interestingly, however, There are also protective factors that buffer or
and consistent with the emotional security moderate the association between exposure to
hypothesis, loyalty conflicts were lower in ado- intimate partner violence and problem behavior
lescents who perceived themselves as closer to among youth. Parenting characteristics such as
their parents. acceptance and respect for the child, and peer
Other contextual variables present within the trust and peer communication has been found to
home environment may also explain the variabil- reduce problem behavior in youth exposed to
ity in the functioning associated with youth’ expo- domestic violence (Tajima et al. 2010). Positive
sure to domestic violence. The possibility of one sibling relationship is also shown to act as a buffer
or more third variables accounting for the nega- between self-blame for interparental conflict and
tive outcomes displayed by youth exposed to anxiety symptoms among adolescents (Iturralde
domestic violence seems probable given that et al. 2013). Hence, the presence of some mediat-
higher levels of domestic violence occur in house- ing or moderating variables can lead to exacerba-
holds that are also affected by poverty, community tion of problem behavior, while others can
violence, and general family dysfunction (Carlson alleviate symptoms of problem behavior in
1984; Higgins and McCabe 2003; Tolman and adolescents.
Raphael 2000). These moderating variables
could be antecedent events in existence prior to
exposure and are likely to continue to act as Is Exposure to Domestic Violence a Form
stressors during the domestic violence exposure. of Child Maltreatment?
Furthermore, other risk factors for youth psycho-
pathology, such as parental psychopathology and Evidence of a dual exposure to domestic violence
child maltreatment, may also co-occur with expo- and child maltreatment is suggestive of its addi-
sure to domestic violence (Owen et al. 2009). For tive and negative sequelae in adolescents (Moylan
instance, one of the most common mental health et al. 2010). However, one controversy within the
sequelae among victimized women is depression field is whether exposure to domestic violence
(World Health Organization 2000), while alcohol- itself should be classified as a form of child mal-
ism and drug abuse are prevalent in perpetrators of treatment. Many advocates and researchers have
domestic violence (Kantor et al. 1994; Thompson argued that exposure to domestic violence should
and Kingree 2006). Each of these types of parental be classified as either as a form of neglect (i.e.,
psychopathology has been associated with behav- failure to protect) or as psychological maltreat-
ioral and emotional difficulties similar to those ment (i.e., Holden 2003; Kalichman 1999).
found in youth exposed to domestic violence Indeed, some Australian jurisdictions mandate
(Ritter et al. 2002). Finally, child maltreatment, the report of exposure to domestic violence as an
especially physical abuse, often overlaps with independent form of child maltreatment (Higgins
exposure to domestic violence (Appel and Holden et al. 2009). While some US states classify
Domestic Violence 1065

exposure to domestic violence as child maltreat- Program, a system of care program successfully
ment, and more specifically as a form of child implemented within the New Orleans, Louisiana
neglect, other states mandate an enhanced penalty Police Department, has been shown to increase
if an assault is committed in the presence of a law enforcement sensitivity to children in domes-
minor (Child Welfare Information Gateway tic violence situations as well as their capacity
2013; Edleson 2004). However, Australia and to refer such children to appropriate treatment
the United States do not classify exposure to (Osofsky et al. 2004). However, to date, the
domestic violence as a reportable form of mal- majority of these prevention programs have been
treatment on a federal level (Bross et al. 2002). implemented only with young children, rather D
Critics of efforts to classify exposure as a form than with adolescents.
of child maltreatment argue that doing so would Given the overwhelming evidence
broaden mandatory reporting requirements, suggesting that exposure to domestic violence
which would increase burden on the already over- negatively impacts children and adolescents,
extended child protection system (Edleson 2004; emerging research has begun to explore the
Kantor and Little 2003). An additional argument effectiveness of treatments for victims of expo-
against legally mandated reporting of exposure to sure to domestic violence (for reviews, see
domestic violence is that these laws may actually Cohen et al. 2006b; Vickerman and Margolin
have a detrimental effect due to the potential to 2007). In general, clinicians working with ado-
blur the boundaries of culpability. That is, crimi- lescents exposed to domestic violence should
nalizing these acts by labeling them as child mal- approach treatment in a developmentally sensi-
treatment may further deter adult victims of tive manner that encourages an open discussion
domestic abuse from seeking help for fear that about the events (Kerig et al. 2000). Cognitive
they would be charged with failure to protect restructuring techniques and therapeutic inter-
dependents who witnessed the acts, particularly pretation of the events may be utilized to help
if they decide to return to the relationship in which adolescents minimize maladaptive feelings and
domestic violence was legally documented attributions, and gain an understanding of the
(Kantor and Little 2003). symptoms they have developed as a result of
violence exposure. Finally, long-term treatment
may help adolescents reestablish trust and break
Prevention and Treatment the cycle of violence within their homes as
adults (Kerig et al. 2000).
Increasingly, advocates and researchers are recog- Consistent with these recommendations, sev-
nizing the need for prevention and treatment pro- eral established treatments have been utilized to
grams aimed at reducing instances of exposure to reduce both the internalizing and externalizing
domestic violence and its consequences. Several sequelae of exposure to domestic violence.
initiatives involving collaborations between Trauma-Focused Cognitive Behavioral Therapy
researchers, child protective services, policy (TF-CBT; Cohen et al. 2006a), originally devel-
makers, and law enforcement officials have been oped to treat child sexual abuse, has been utilized
developed to diminish the risk of reoccurring vio- to reduce trauma symptoms in adolescents ages
lence and to reduce the trauma associated with 13 and 18 exposed to domestic violence, and is
such acts (Jaffe et al. 2012). One example of considered a “well supported and efficacious”
such a program is Project Safe Care, a home- treatment (de Arellano et al. 2008). Recently, the
based parent-training program aimed at family trauma-narrative component of TF-CBT is being
preservation, which has been found to be effective tested with a web-based digital storytelling cur-
at reducing families’ subsequent involvement in riculum with children as young as 9 years
the legal system due to allegations of child (Anderson & Cook 2015). Group treatment with
maltreatment (Gershater-Molko et al. 2002, children and adolescents exposed to domestic
2003). Additionally, the Violence Intervention violence are widely used interventions and are
1066 Domestic Violence

also more economical in contrast to individual Conclusion


therapy (Edelson and Greenleaf 2006). The Kids
Club (Graham-Bermann 2000; Graham-Bermann Many children and adolescents are exposed to
et al. 2007) has been used to target trauma symp- domestic violence between caregivers within
toms with 13-year-old adolescent victims of phys- their homes. Clear links have been established
ical abuse and exposure to domestic violence. In between exposure to this violence and detrimental
an evaluation study of youth ages 5–13, the Kids physiological, cognitive, emotional, behavioral,
Club was successful at reducing trauma symp- and social outcomes among youth. Several theo-
toms, but not depressive symptoms or impulsivity ries have been proffered to explain these out-
(Graham-Bermann 2000). The London Commu- comes, including social learning theory, family
nity Group Treatment Programme for Child Wit- systems theory, and trauma theory. Recent
nesses of Women Abuse (Jaffe et al. 1986), a research has moved beyond establishing linkages
group intervention for exposed youth age 4-16, to attempting to understand the influence of medi-
has been found to be effective at changing atti- ating and moderating factors that explain variabil-
tudes regarding the acceptability of violence ity in the functioning of youth who have been
reducing participants self-blame for the acts, and exposed to domestic violence. Finally, as more
increasing awareness about community resources becomes known about the effects of exposure to
and avenues for help. Finally, the Youth Relation- domestic violence on children and adolescents,
ship Project (Wolfe et al. 1996) is a secondary treatment and prevention programs have been
prevention program for 13- to 17-year-old adoles- developed to treat both the internalizing and exter-
cent victims of physical abuse and exposure to nalizing sequelae of exposure as well as to reduce
domestic violence, aimed at promoting healthy the risk of subsequent exposure.
social and dating relationships. This treatment Although the study of exposure to domestic
has been shown to be effective in reducing inci- violence has advanced steadily in the past few
dents of emotional and physical violence perpe- decades, much remains to be learned. For example,
trated by at-risk youth toward adolescent dating to date, the majority of the literature has examined
partners (Wolfe et al. 2003a). the correlates of domestic violence in combined
In addition to these clinical interventions, samples of children and adolescents, which tends
efforts are being directed toward formulating to obscure developmental differences in coping
universal prevention strategies owing to the and responses. More longitudinal studies are
vast number of children and adolescents being needed to reveal the particular impact of exposure
exposed to domestic violence. In this context, to domestic violence on adolescents and to track
school- and community-based intervention the developmental trajectory of this group. As this
programs have gained momentum. One such literature base expands, it will undoubtedly con-
school-based program is Safe Dates, with a tinue to inform legal policy related to adolescent
focus of changing norms regarding partner vio- exposure to domestic violence as well as the for-
lence, gender roles, and social skills relating to mulation of treatments to address the negative cor-
relationship violence among adolescents relates of these events in youth and their families.
(Foshee et al. 2005, 2014). Another universal
program directed toward prevention of future
incidents of intimate partner violence targeting
References
dating violence and related risk behaviors is the
Fourth R (Wolfe et al. 2009). This classroom Anderson, K. M., & Cook, J. R. (2015). Challenges and
based intervention conducted by trained opportunities of using digital storytelling as a trauma
teachers has been effective in decreasing dating narrative intervention for children. Advances in Social
Work, 16, 78–89.
violence and increasing assertiveness and com-
Appel, A. E., & Holden, G. W. (1998). The co-occurrence
munication skills among adolescents (Wolfe of spouse and physical child abuse: A review and
et al. 2009). appraisal. Journal of Family Psychology, 12(4), 578.
Domestic Violence 1067

Baldry, A. C. (2007). “It does affect me”: Disruptive Clements, C. M., Oxtoby, C., & Ogle, R. L. (2008). Meth-
behaviors in preadolescents directly and indirectly odological issues in assessing psychological adjust-
abused at home. European Psychologist, 12, 29–35. ment in child witnesses of intimate partner violence.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 9, 114–127.
action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs: Cohen, J. A., Mannarino, A. P., & Deblinger, E. (2006a).
Prentice-Hall. Treating trauma and traumatic grief in children and
Bickham, N. L., & Fiese, B. H. (1997). Extension of the adolescents. New York: Guilford.
children’s perception of interparental conflict scale for Cohen, J. A., Mannarino, A. P., Murray, L. K., & Igelman,
use with late adolescents. Journal of Family Psychol- R. (2006b). Psychosocial interventions for maltreated
ogy, 11, 246–250. and violence-exposed children. Journal of Social
Black, M. C., Basile, K. C., Breiding, M. J., Smith, S. G., Issues, 62, 737–766. D
Walters, M. L., Merrick, M. T., & Stevens, M. R. Davies, P. T., Sturge-Apple, M. L., & Cicchetti, D. (2011).
(2011). The National intimate partner and sexual vio- Interparental aggression and children’s adrenocortical
lence survey (NISVS): 2010 summary report. Atlanta: reactivity: Testing an evolutionary model of allostatic load.
National Center for Injury Prevention and Control, Development and Psychopathology, 23(3), 801–814.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579411000319.
Boynton-Jarrett, R., Fargnoli, J., Suglia, S. F., Zuckerman, Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (1994). Marital conflict
B., & Wright, R. J. (2010). Association between mater- and child adjustment: An emotional security hypothe-
nal intimate partner violence and incident obesity in sis. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 387–411.
preschool-aged children: Results from the fragile fam- Dawud-Noursi, S., Lamb, M., & Sternberg, K. (1998). The
ilies and child well-being study. Archives of Pediatrics effects of domestic violence on children’s adjustment at
& Adolescent Medicine, 164, 540–546. school. Megamot, 40, 72–102.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human devel- de Arellano, M. A., Ko, S. J., Danielson, C. K., & Sprague,
opment: Experiments by nature and design. Cam- C. M. (2008). Trauma-informed interventions: Clinical
bridge: Harvard University Press. and research evidence and culture-specific information
Bross, D. C., Miyoshi, T. J., Miyoshi, P. K., & Krugman, project. Los Angeles: National Center for Child Trau-
R. D. (2002). World perspectives on child abuse. In The matic Stress.
International Society for the Prevention of Child Abuse De Bellis, M. D. (2001). Developmental traumatology:
and Neglect (ISPCAN) and the Kempe Center for the The psychobiological development of maltreated
Prevention and Treatment of Child Abuse and Neglect. children and its implications for research, treatment,
Denver: University of Colorado Denver School of and policy. Development and Psychopathology, 13,
Medicine. 539–564.
Buchanan, C. M., Maccoby, E. E., & Dornbush, S. M. (1991). De Bellis, M. D., Keshavan, M. S., Clark, D. B., Casey, B.
Caught between parents: Adolescents’ experience in J., Giedd, J. N., Boring, A. M.,… & Ryan, N. D.
divorced home. Child Development, 62, 1008–1029. (1999). Developmental traumatology part II: Brain
Buka, S. L., Stichick, T. L., Birdthistle, I., & Earls, F. J. development. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1271–1284.
(2001). Youth exposure to violence: Prevalence, risks, De Bellis, M. D., Keshavan, M. S., Shifflett, H., Iyengar,
and consequences. The American Journal of Orthopsy- S., Beers, S. R., Hall, J., et al. (2002). Brain structures
chiatry, 71, 298–310. in pediatric maltreatment-related PTSD: A socio-
Carlson, B. E. (1984). Children’s observation of demographically matched study. Biological Psychiatry,
interparental violence. In A. R. Roberts (Ed.), Battered 52, 1066–1078.
women and their families (pp. 147–167). New York: Duncan, A., Thomas, J. C., & Miller, C. (2005). Signifi-
Springer. cance of family risk factors in development of child-
Carlson, B. E. (1991). Outcomes of physical abuse and obser- hood animal cruelty in adolescent boys with conduct
vation of marital violence among adolescents in place- problems. Journal of Family Violence, 20, 235–239.
ment. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 6, 526–534. Edleson, J. L. (1999a). Children’s witnessing of adult
Chan, Y., & Yeung, J. F. (2009). Children living with domestic violence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence,
violence within the family and its sequel: A meta-anal- 14, 839.
ysis from 1995–2006. Aggression and Violent Behav- Edleson, J. L. (1999b). The overlap between child mal-
ior, 14, 313–322. treatment and woman battering. Violence Against
Child Welfare Information Gateway. (2013). Child wit- Women, 5, 134.
nesses to domestic violence. Washington: U.S. Depart- Edleson, J. L. (2004). Should childhood exposure to adult
ment of Health and Human Services, Children’s domestic violence be defined as child maltreatment
Bureau. under the law. St. Paul: University of Minnesota.
Choi, J., Jeong, B., Polcari, A., Rohan, M. L., & Teicher, http://www.mincava.umn.e.
M. H. (2012). Reduced fractional anisotropy in the Edleson, J. L., Ellerton, A. L., Seagren, E. A.,
visual limbic pathway of young adults witnessing Schmidt, S. O., Kirchberg, S. L., & Ambrose,
domestic violence in childhood. NeuroImage, 59(2), A. T. (2007). Assessing child exposure to adult
1071–1079. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage. domestic violence. Children and Youth Services
2011.09.033. Review, 29, 961–971.
1068 Domestic Violence

Edleson, J., & Greenleaf, D. (2006). King county safe and Foshee, V. A., Reyes, L. M., Agnew-Brune, C. B., Simon,
bright futures project final report: Developing a plan for T. R., Vagi, K. J., Lee, R. D., & Suchindran, C. (2014).
infants, children, and youth exposed to domestic violence. The effects if the evidence-based safe dates dating
Seattle & King County: Office of Public Health and abuse prevention program on other yput violence out-
Science & Department of Health and Human Services. comes. Prevention Science, 15, 907–916.
Edleson, J. L., Shin, N., & Johnson Armendariz, K. K. Fox, N. A., & Leavitt, L. A. (1996). Violence exposure
(2008). Measuring children’s exposure to domestic scale for children-revised. Philadelphia: University of
violence: The development and testing of the child Pennsylvania.
exposure to domestic violence (CEDV) scale. Children Fusco, R. A., & Fantuzzo, J. W. (2009). Domestic violence
and Youth Services Review, 30, 502–521. crimes and children: A population-based investigation of
Ellonen, N., Piispa, M., Peltonen, K., & Oranen, M. (2013). direct sensory exposure and the nature of involvement.
Exposure to parental violence and outcomes of child Children and Youth Services Review, 31(2), 249–256.
psychosocial adjustment. Violence and Victims, 28(1), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2008.07.017.
3–15. https://doi.org/10.1891/0886-6708.28.1.3. Gagne, M., Lavoie, F., & Herbert, M. (2005). Victimization
Evans, S. E., Davies, C., & DiLillo, D. (2008). Exposure to during childhood and revictimization in dating relation-
domestic violence: A meta-analysis of child and ado- ships in adolescent girls. Child Abuse & Neglect,
lescent outcomes. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 29, 1155.
13, 131. Gerard, J. M., Buehler, C., Franck, K., & Anderson,
Fagan, A. A., & Wright, E. M. (2011). Gender differences O. (2005). In the eyes of the beholder: Cognitive
in the effects of exposure to intimate partner violence appraisals as mediators of the association between
on adolescent violence and drug use. Child Abuse & interparental conflict and youth maladjustment. Jour-
Neglect, 35(7), 543–550. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. nal of Family Psychology, 19, 376.
chiabu.2011.05.001. Gershater-Molko, R., Lutzker, J., & Wesch, D. (2002).
Fantuzzo, J. W., & Lindquist, C. U. (1989). The effects of Using recidivism data to evaluate project SafeCare:
observing conjugal violence on children: A review and Teaching bonding, safety, and health care skills to
analysis of research methodology. Journal of Family parents. Child Maltreatment, 7, 277.
Violence, 4, 77. Gershater-Molko, R., Lutzker, J., & Wesch, D. (2003).
Finkelhor, D., Hamby, S. L., Omrod, R., & Turner, Project safecare: Improving health, safety, and parent-
H. (2005). The juvenile victimization questionnaire: ing skills in families reported for, and at-risk for child
Reliability, validity and national norms. Child Abuse maltreatment. Journal of Family Violence, 18, 377.
& Neglect, 29, 383. Graham-Bermann, S. A. (2000). Evaluating interventions
Finkelhor, D., Turner, H. A., Shattuck, A., & Hamby, S. L. for children exposed to family violence. Journal of
(2015). Prevalence of childhood exposure to violence, Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 4, 191.
crime, and abuse: Results from the National Survey of Graham-Bermann, S. A., Howell, K. H., Miller, L. E.,
Children’s exposure to violence. JAMA Pediatrics, Kwek, J., & Lilly, M. M. (2009). Traumatic events
169(8), 746–754. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics. and maternal education as predictors of verbal ability
2015.0676. for preschool children exposed to intimate partner vio-
Flannery, D. J., Singer, M. I., & Wester, K. (2001). Vio- lence (IPV). Journal of Family Violence, 25(4),
lence exposure, psychological trauma, and suicide risk 383–392. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10896-009-9299-3.
in a community sample of dangerously violent adoles- Graham-Bermann, S. A., & Levendosky, A. A. (1998).
cents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Traumatic stress symptoms in children of battered
Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 435. women. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 13, 111.
Fosco, G. M., DeBoard, R. L., & Grych, J. H. (2007). Graham-Bermann, S. A., Lynch, S., Banyard, V., DeVoe,
Making sense of family violence. European Psycholo- E. R., & Halabu, H. (2007). Community-based inter-
gist, 12, 6–16. vention for children exposed to intimate partner vio-
Fosco, G. M., & Grych, J. H. (2008). Emotional, cognitive, lence: An efficacy trial. Journal of Consulting and
and family systems mediators of children adjustment to Clinical Psychology, 75, 199.
interparental conflict. Journal of Family Psychology, Grant, K. E., Katz, B. N., Thomas, K. J., Orant, J. H., Meza,
22, 843. C. M., DiPasquale, A., et al. (2004). Psychological
Foshee, V. A., Bauman, K. E., Ennett, S. T., Suchindran, symptoms affecting low-income urban youth. Journal
C., Benefield, T., & Linder, G. F. (2005). Assessing the of Adolescent Research, 19, 613.
effects of the dating violence prevention program “safe Grych, J. H., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Marital conflict and
dates” using random coefficient regression modeling. children’s adjustment: A cognitive-contextual frame-
Prevention Science, 6, 245–258. work. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 267.
Foshee, V. A., Karriker-Jaffe, K. J., Reyes, H. L. M., Grych, J. H., & Fincham, F. D. (1993). Children’s
Ennett, S. T., Suchindran, C., Bauman, K. E., et al. appraisals of interparental conflict. Initial investigation
(2008). What accounts for demographic differences in of the cognitive-contextual framework. Child Develop-
trajectories of adolescent dating violence? An exami- ment, 64, 215–230.
nation of intrapersonal and contextual mediators. The Grych, J. H., Seid, M., & Fincham, F. D. (1992). Assessing
Journal of Adolescent Health, 42, 596. marital conflict from the child’s perspective: The
Domestic Violence 1069

children's perception of interparental conflict scale. Hotaling, G. T., & Sugarman, D. B. (1986). An analysis of
Child Development, 63, 558–572. risk markers in husband to wife violence: The current
Gunnar, M. R., & Donzella, B. (2002). Social regulation of state of knowledge. Violence and Victims, 1, 101.
the cortisol levels in early human development. Huth-Bocks, A. C., Levendosky, A. A., & Semel, M. A.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 27, 199–220. (2001). The direct and indirect effects of domestic
Hamby, S. L., & Finkelhor, D. (2001). Choosing and using violence on young children’s intellectual functioning.
child victimization questionnaires. Washington: Office Journal of Family Violence, 16, 269–290.
of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, Ireland, T. O., & Smith, C. A. (2009). Living in partner-
US Department of Justice. violent families: Developmental links to antisocial
Hamby, S. L., Finkelhor, D., Turner, H., & Ormrod, behavior and relationship violence. Journal of Youth
R. (2011). Children’s exposure to intimate partner vio- and Adolescence, 38, 323. D
lence and other family violence: Nationally represen- Iturralde, E., Margolin, G., & Shapiro, L. A. S. (2013).
tative rates among US youth. Washington: Office of Positive and negative interactions observed between
Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention Juvenile siblings: Moderating effects for children exposed to
Justice Bulletin US Government. parents’ conflict. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
Hanson, R. F., Self-Brown, S., Fricker-Elhai, A. E., 23, 716–729.
Kilpatrick, D. G., Saunders, B. E., & Resnick, H. S. Jaffe, P., Wolfe, D. A., & Campbell, M. (2012). Growing
(2006). The relations between family environment and up with domestic violence: Advances in psychotherapy.
violence exposure among youth: Findings from the Evidence-based practice. Cambridge: Hogrefe.
national survey of adolescents. Child Maltreatment, Jaffe, P. G., Wolfe, D. A., & Wilson, S. K. (1990). Children
11, 3. of battered women. Newbury Park: Sage.
Heise, L., Ellsberg, M., & Gottemoeller, M. (1999). Ending Jaffe, P., Wilson, S., & Wolfe, D. A. (1986). Promoting
violence against women. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins changes in attitudes and understanding of conflict
University School of Public Health, Center for Com- among child witnesses of family violence. Canadian
munications Programs, Population Information Journal of Behavioural Science, 18, 356.
Program. Jouriles, E. N., McDonald, R., Norwood, W. D., & Ezell,
Hibel, L. C., Granger, D. A., Blair, C., Cox, M. J., & E. (2001). Issues and controversies in documenting the
Family Life Project Key, I. (2009). Intimate partner prevalence of childrens’ exposure to domestic violence.
violence moderates the association between mother- In S. A. Graham Bermann & J. L. Edleson (Eds.),
infant adrenocortical activity across an emotional chal- Domestic violence in the lives of children (pp. 13–34).
lenge. Journal of Family Psychology, 23(5), 615–625. Washington: American Psychological Association.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016323. Jouriles, E. N., Vu, N. L., McDonald, R., & Rosenfield,
Hibel, L. C., Granger, D. A., Blair, C., Cox, M. J., & D. (2014). Children’s appraisals of conflict, beliefs
Family Life Project Key, I. (2011). Maternal sensitivity about aggression, and externalizing problems in fami-
buffers the adrenocortical implications of intimate part- lies characterized by severe intimate partner violence.
ner violence exposure during early childhood. Devel- Journal of Family Psychology, 28, 915–924.
opment and Psychopathology, 23(2), 689–701. https:// Jun, H. J., Corliss, H. L., Boynton-Jarrett, R., Spiegelman,
doi.org/10.1017/S0954579411000010. D., Austin, S. B., & Wright, R. J. (2012). Growing up in
Higgins, D. J., & McCabe, M. P. (2003). Maltreatment and a domestic violence environment: Relationship with
family dysfunction in childhood and the subsequent developmental trajectories of body mass index during
adjustment of children and adults. Journal of Family adolescence into young adulthood. Journal of Epide-
Violence, 18, 107. miology and Community Health, 66(7), 629–635.
Higgins, D., Bromfield, L., Richardson, N., Holzer, P., & https://doi.org/10.1136/jech.2010.110932.
Berlyn, C. (2009). Mandatory reporting of child abuse Kalichman, S. C. (1999). Mandated reporting of suspected
and neglect. Melbourne: National Child Protection child abuse (2nd ed.). Washington: American Psycho-
Clearinghouse, The Australian Institute of Family logical Association.
Studies. Kantor, G. K., & Little, L. L. (2003). Defining the bound-
Holden, G. W. (2003). Children exposed to domestic vio- aries of child neglect: When does domestic violence
lence and child abuse: Terminology and taxonomy. equate with parental failure to protect. Journal of Inter-
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, personal Violence, 18, 338.
6, 151–160. Kantor, G. K., Jasinski, J. L., & Aldarondo, E. (1994).
Holt, S., Buckley, H., & Whelan, S. (2008). The impact of Sociocultural status and incidence of marital violence
exposure to domestic violence on children and young in Hispanic families. Violence and Victims, 9, 207.
people: A review of the literature. Child Abuse & Kashani, J. H., & Allan, W. D. (1998). The impact of family
Neglect, 32, 797. violence on children and adolescents. Thousand Oaks:
Horn Ratner, H., Chiodo, C. C., Sokol, R. J., Ager, J., & Sage.
Delaney-Black, V. (2006). Violence exposure, IQ, aca- Kerig, P. K., Fedorowicz, A. E., Brown, C. A., & Warren,
demic performance, and childrenposure, IQ, academic M. (2000). Assessment and intervention for PTSD in
performance, and childrene effects. Merrill-Palmer children exposed to violence. Journal of Aggression,
Quarterly, 52, 264. Maltreatment & Trauma, 3, 161.
1070 Domestic Violence

Kitzmann, K. M., Gaylord, N. K., Holt, A. R., & Kenny, 953–961. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.
E. D. (2003). Child witnesses to domestic violence: 02222.x.
A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Nadel, H., Spellman, M., Alvarez-Canino, T., Lausell-
Clinical Psychology, 71, 339. Bryant, L., & Landsberg, G. (1996). The cycle of
Koenen, K. C., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., & violence and victimization: A study of the school-
Purcell, S. (2003). Domestic violence is associated based intervention of a multidisciplinary youth
with environmental suppression of IQ in young chil- violence-prevention program. American Journal of
dren. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 297. Preventive Medicine, 12, 109.
Kolbo, J. R. (1996). Risk and resilience among children O’Brien, M., John, R. S., Margolin, G., & Erel, O. (1994).
exposed to family violence. Violence and Victims, Reliability and diagnostic efficacy of parentsy of par-
11, 113. ents’ diagnostic efficacy of parents’report regarding
Lang, J. M., & Smith Stover, C. (2008). Symptom patterns childrens’ exposure to martial aggression. Violence
among youth exposed to intimate partner violence. and Victims, 9, 45.
Journal of Family Violence, 23, 619. O’Keefe, M. (1998). Factors mediating the link between
Levendosky, A. A., Huth-Bocks, A. C., & Semel, M. A. witnessing interparental violence and dating violence.
(2002). Adolescent peer relationships and mental Journal of Family Violence, 13, 39.
health functioning in families with domestic violence. Osofsky, J. D., Rovaris, M., Hammer, J. H., Dickson, A.,
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 31, 206. Freeman, N., & Aucoin, K. (2004). Working with
MacMillan, H. L., & Wathen, C. N. (2014). Children’s police to help children exposed to violence. Journal
exposure to intimate partner violence. Child and Ado- of Community Psychology, 32, 593.
lescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 23(2), Owen, A. E., Thompson, M. P., Shaffer, A., Jackson, E. B.,
295–308. viii-ix. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2013. & Kaslow, N. J. (2009). Family variables that mediate
12.008. the relation between intimate partner violence (IPV)
Margolin, G. (1998). The effects of domestic violence on and child adjustment. Journal of Family Violence,
children. In P. K. Trickett & C. Schellenbach (Eds.), 24, 433.
Violence against children in the family and the commu- Pelcovitz, D., Kaplan, S. J., DeRosa, R. R., Mandel, F. S.,
nity (pp. 57–102). Washington: American Psychologi- & Salzinger, S. (2000). Psychiatric disorders in
cal Association. adolescents exposed to domestic violence and physi-
Margolin, G. (2005). Childrens’ exposure to violence: cal abuse. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
Exploring developmental pathways to diverse out- 70, 360.
comes. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20, 72–81. Porter, B., & O’Leary, K. D. (1980). Marital discord and
McCloskey, L. A., & Lichter, E. L. (2003). The contribu- childhood behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal
tion of marital violence to adolescent aggression across Child Psychology, 8, 287.
different relationships. Journal of Interpersonal Vio- Rand, M. R., & Saltzman, L. E. (2003). The nature and
lence, 18, 390. extent of recurring intimate partner violence against
McGee, Z. T., & Baker, S. R. (2002). Impact of violence on women in the United States. Journal of Comparative
problem behavior among adolescents: Risk factors Family Studies, 34, 137.
among an urban sample. Journal of Contemporary Richters, J. E., & Martinez, P. (1992). Things i have seen
Criminal Justice, 18, 74. and heard: A structured interview for assessing young
McIntosh, J. E. (2003). Children living with domestic childrens violence exposure. Rockville: National Insti-
violence: Research foundations for early intervention. tute of Mental Health.
Journal of Family Studies, 9, 219–234. Ritter, J., Stewart, M., Bernet, C., Coe, M., & Brown, S. A.
Mohr, W. K., Noone Lutz, M. J., Fantuzzo, J. W., & Perry, (2002). Effects of childhood exposure to familial alco-
M. A. (2000). Children exposed to family violence: holism and family violence on adolescent substance
A review of empirical research from a developmentall use, conduct problems, and self-esteem. Journal of
research from a develop. Trauma, Violence & Abuse, Traumatic Stress, 15, 113.
1, 264. Saltzman, K. M., Holden, G. W., & Holahan, C. J. (2005).
Moretti, M. M., Obsuth, I., Odgers, C. L., & Reebye, The psychobiology of children exposed to marital vio-
P. (2006). Exposure to maternal vs. paternal partner lence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psy-
violence, PTSD, and aggression in adolescent girls chology, 34, 129.
and boys. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 385. Salzinger, S., Feldman, R. S., Hammer, M., & Rosario,
Moylan, C. A., Herrenkohl, T. L., Sousa, C., Tajima, E. A., M. (1992). Constellation of family violence and their
Horrenkohl, R. C., & Russi, M. J. (2010). The effects of differential effects on children’s behavior disturbance.
child abuse and exposure to domestic violence on ado- Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 14, 23.
lescent internalizing and externalizing behavior prob- Schiff, M., Plotnikova, M., Dingle, K., Williams, G. M.,
lems. Journal of Family Violence, 25, 53–63. Najman, J., & Clavarino, A. (2014). Does adolescent’s
Mrug, S., & Windle, M. (2010). Prospective effects of exposure to parental intimate partner conflict and vio-
violence exposure across multiple contexts on early lence predict psychological distress and substance use
adolescents’ internalizing and externalizing problems. in young adulthood? A longitudinal study. Child Abuse
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(8), and Neglect, 38, 1945–1954.
Domestic Violence 1071

Skeers, M., McCormick, M. C., Normand, S. T., Buka, Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Boney-McCoy, S., &
S. L., & Gilman, S. E. (2009). A prospective study of Sugarman, D. B. (1996). The revised conflict tactics
familial conflict, psychological stress, and the develop- scale (CTS2). Journal of Family Issues, 17, 283.
ment of substance use disorders in adolescence. Drug Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., Cicchetti, D., & Man-
and Alcohol Dependence, 104, 65–72. ning, L. G. (2012). Interparental violence, maternal
Shemmings, D., & Shemmings, Y. (2011). Understanding emotional unavailability and children’s cortisol func-
disorganized attachment: Theory and practice for tioning in family contexts. Developmental Psychology,
working with children and adults. London: Jessica 48, 237–249.
Kingsley. Swan, S. C., Gambone, L. J., Caldwell, J. E., Sullivan,
Shin, N., & Edleson, J. L. (2007). A new scale for assessing T. P., & Snow, D. L. (2008). A review of research on
child exposure to domestic violence. Protecting Chil- women’s use of violence with male intimate partners. D
dren, 22, 6. Violence and Victims, 23, 301.
Smith, C. A., Elwyn, L. J., Ireland, T. O., & Thornberry, Tajima, E. A., Herrenkohl, T. I., Moylan, C. A., & Derr,
T. P. (2010). Impact of adolescent exposure to intimate A. S. (2010). Moderating the effects of childhood expo-
partner violence on substance use in early adulthood. sure to intimate partner violence: The role of parenting
Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 71, 219–230. characteristics and adolescent peer support. Journal of
https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2010.71.219. Research on Adolescence, 21, 376–394.
Sousa, C., Herrenkohl, T. I., Moylan, C. A., Tajima, E. A., Tolman, R. M., & Raphael, J. (2000). A review of research
Klika, J. B., Herrenkohl, R. C., & Russo, M. J. (2011). on welfare and domestic violence. Journal of Social
Longitudinal study on the effects of child abuse and Issues, 56, 655.
children’s exposure to domestic violence, parent-child Towe-Goodman, N. R., Stifter, C. A., Mills-Koonce,
attachments, and antisocial behavior in adolescence. W. R., Granger, D. A., & Family Life Project Key,
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 26(1), 111–136. I. (2012). Interparental aggression and infant patterns
https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260510362883. of adrenocortical and behavioral stress responses.
Spaccarelli, S., Coatsworth, J. D., & Bowden, B. S. (1995). Developmental Psychobiology, 54(7), 685–699.
Exposure to serious family violence among incarcer- https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20618.
ated boys: Its association with violent offending and Tsavoussis, A., Stawicki, S. P., Stoicea, N., & Papadimos,
potential mediating variables. Violence and Victims, T. J. (2014). Child-witnessed domestic violence and its
10, 163. adverse effects on brain development: A call for soci-
Sternberg, K. J., Lamb, M. E., & Dawud-Noursi, S. (1998). etal self-examination and awareness. Frontiers in Pub-
Using multiple informants to understand domestic vio- lic Health, 2, 178. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2014.
lence and its effects. In G. W. Holden, R. Geffner, & 00178.
E. N. Jouriles (Eds.), Children exposed to marital vio- Vickerman, K. A., & Margolin, G. (2007). Posttraumatic
lence (pp. 121–156). Washington: American Psycho- stress in children and adolescents exposed to family
logical Association. violence: II treatment. Professional Psychology:
Sternberg, K. J., Lamb, M. E., Guterman, E., & Abbott, Research and Practice, 38, 620.
C. B. (2006). Effects of early and later family violence Voisin, D. R., & Hong, J. S. (2012). A mediational model
on children’s behavior problem and depression: linking witnessing intimate partner violence and bully-
A longitudinal, multi-informant study. Child Abuse & ing behaviors and victimization among youth. Educa-
Neglect, 30, 283. tional Psychology Review, 24, 479–498. https://doi.org/
Stith, S. M., Rosen, K. H., Middleton, K. A., Busch, A. L., 10.1007/s10648-012-9197-8.
Lundeberg, K., & Carlton, R. P. (2000). The Widom, C. S. (1989). Does violence beget violence?
intergenerational transmission of spouse abuse: A critical examination of the literature. Psychological
A meta-analysis. Journal of Marriage and the Family, Bulletin, 106, 3.
62, 640. Widom, C. S. (2000). Childhood victimization: Early
Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and adversity, later psychopathology. National Institute of
violence: The conflict tactics (CT) scales. Journal of Justice Journal, 242, 2.
Marriage and the Family, 41, 75. Williams, J. R., Ghandour, R. M., & Kub, J. E. (2008).
Straus, M. A. (1992). Children as witnesses to marital Female perpetration of violence in heterosexual inti-
violence: A risk factor for lifelong problems among a mate relationships: Adolescence through adulthood.
nationally representative sample of American men and Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 9, 227.
women. In D. F. Schwarz (Ed.), Children and violence: Wolak, J., & Finkelhor, D. (1998). Children exposed to
Report of the twenty-third roundtable on critical partner violence. In J. L. Jasinski & L. M. Williams
approaches to common pediatric problems (Eds.), Partner violence: A comprehensive review of
(pp. 98–109). Columbus: Ross Laboratories. 20 years of research (pp. 73–113). Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Straus, M. A. (2005). Women’s violence toward men is Wolfe, D. A., Crooks, C., Jaffe, P., Chiodo, D., Hughes, R.,
a serious social problem. In D. R. Loseke, R. J. Ellis, W., & Donner, A. (2009). A school-based pro-
Gelles, & M. M. Cavanaugh (Eds.), Current con- gram to preent adolescent dating violence: A cluster
troversies on family violence (pp. 55–77). Newbury randomized trial. Archives of Pediatrics and Adoles-
Park: Sage. cent Medicine, 163, 692–699.
1072 Doping

Wolfe, K. A., & Foshee, V. A. (2003). Family violence, addressed adolescents’ use of other supplements.
anger expression styles, and adolescent dating vio- Similarly, little is known about potential health
lence. Journal of Family Violence, 18, 309.
Wolfe, D. A., Wekerle, C., Gough, B., Reitzel-Jaffe, D., risks of such substances in adolescents. There has
Grasley, C., Pittman, A., et al. (1996). The youth rela- been a worldwide effort to standardize and regu-
tionships manual: A group approach with adolescents late the use of performance enhancing substances
for the prevention of woman abuse and the promotion by adult athletes, but attempts over the past
of healthy relationships. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Wolfe, D. A., Wekerle, C., Scott, K., Straatman, A., decade to extend such efforts to younger athletes
Grasley, C., & Reitzel-Jaffe, D. (2003a). Dating vio- have generally found that widespread screening
lence prevention with at-risk youth: A controlled out- and testing is not feasible. Additionally, there are
come evaluation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical many dietary supplements that are used by ado-
Psychology, 71, 279.
Wolfe, D. A., Crooks, C. V., Lee, V., McIntyre-Smith, A., lescents as performance enhancers and/or to
& Jaffe, P. G. (2003b). The effects of children’s expo- improve body image that are not regulated and
sure to domestic violence: A meta-analysis and cri- are widely available. Further research is needed
tique. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, to determine the prevalence of use as well as
6, 171–187.
World Health Organization. (2000). Women’s mental reasons for use of performance enhancing sub-
health: An evidence based review. Geneva: WHO. stances and dietary supplements among the gen-
World Health Organization. (2012). Understanding and eral adolescent population. This research should
addressing violence against women. Geneva: WHO. also focus on factors leading to their use as well
Zinzow, H. M., Ruggiero, K. J., Resnick, H., Smith, D.,
Saunders, B., & Kilpatrick, D. (2009). Prevalence and as potential long-term physical and psychologi-
mental health correlates of witnessed parental and com- cal impacts.
munity violence in a national sample of adolescents.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 441.
Introduction

The use of performance enhancing substances in


Doping sports, also known generally as doping, has been
pushed to the forefront of professional sports and
Edward M. Castillo1 and R. Dawn Comstock2,3 the Olympics in recent decades and has even
1
Department of Emergency Medicine, University sparked US Congressional investigational hear-
of California, San Diego, CA, USA ings on the extent of doping in sports. This has
2
College of Medicine, Department of Pediatrics resulted in an ongoing debate about the ethical
and College of Public Health, Division of implications as well as potential negative impact
Epidemiology, The Ohio State University, to the health of those who use these substances.
Columbus, OH, USA However, doping is not new. Athletes have always
3
Center for Injury Research and Policy, Columbus utilized methods to improve physical performance
Children’s Hospital, Columbus, OH, USA including improved nutrition, physical fitness,
and specialized training. The use of exogenous
substances to assist these efforts has been prac-
Overview ticed for thousands of years and dates back to as
early as ancient Greece (Emmanquel 1947).
Performance enhancing substances and dietary Although these substances have been com-
supplements have long been used by athletes to monly used throughout competitive sports, the
improve physical performance and endurance, use of performance enhancing substances among
but the use of such substances among adolescents adolescents is not well documented. This is par-
is not well documented. The majority of peer- ticularly troubling because of sparse safety data
reviewed studies focusing on adolescents have on the health effect of such substances in this
examined this population’s use of anabolic population. Few studies have focused on the use
steroids and creatine, while little work has of performance enhancing substances and dietary
Doping 1073

supplements among adolescents with most studies Committee’s (USOC) Select Task Force on Exter-
to date focusing on a single supplement (i.e., nalization. The USADA is an independent, non-
anabolic steroids, creatine, etc.). Additionally, profit, nongovernmental agency that manages a
safety data for such substances is essentially comprehensive, national anti-doping program
lacking for adolescents and must be extrapolated that includes education, testing, and research and
from studies in adults if available. This is insuffi- administers its own protocols and policies as well
cient given the biophysiological differences as those of the USOC. During the same time that
between adolescents and adults. The purpose of the USADA was being established, the Interna-
this essay is to provide an overview of dietary tional Olympic Committee established the World D
supplement use among adolescents in the USA Anti-Doping Agency, which developed the World
with a focus on the substances used to enhance Anti-Doping Program. The program consists of
physical performance, also known as ergogenic three main sections including (1) the World Anti-
aids, which are commonly used by athletes. It will Doping Code (Code), which was developed to
also address alternative reasons adolescents use standardize anti-doping rules and sanctions on a
these types of substances. worldwide basis; (2) the International Standards
for Testing, Therapeutic Use Exemptions, Labo-
ratory Practices, Protection of Privacy and Per-
Overview of Performance Enhancing sonal Information, and The Prohibited List; and
Substances and Sports (3) Models of Best Practice, which provides and
encourages up-to-date solutions in different areas
The term, “performance enhancing substances” is of anti-doping. These efforts led to a standard
commonly used to refer to a wide range of sub- definition of doping, which defined by the Code,
stances that can ultimately be separated into three is the occurrence of one or more of the following
main categories: (1) illegal substances or those anti-doping rule violations (US Anti-Doping
that can only be obtained legally through a pre- Agency 2010a):
scription from a medical doctor and that are
banned by some or all sports, such as anabolic • The Presence of a Prohibited Substance or its
steroids and the hormone precursor androstenedi- Metabolites or Markers in an Athlete’s Sample.
one; (2) legally available substances that are • Use or Attempted Use by an Athlete of a Pro-
banned by some or all sports, in general or in hibited Substance or a Prohibited Method.
competition only, such as diuretics and caffeine; • Refusing or failing without compelling justifi-
and (3) legally available substances that are not cation to submit to Sample collection after
banned, such as many types of dietary supple- notification as authorized in applicable anti-
ments. Determining which category a substance doping rules or otherwise evading Sample
falls into can be difficult because the lists of sub- collection.
stances banned by the various sporting governing • Violation of applicable requirements regarding
bodies is frequently amended and, while less fre- Athlete availability for Out-of-Competition
quent, a substance’s legal status can also change Testing including failure to file required where-
(e.g., the US Food and Drug Administration’s abouts information and missed tests which are
banning of the sale of supplements containing declared based on rules which comply with the
ephedrine alkaloids in 2004) (Castillo and International Standard for Testing. Any com-
Comstock 2007). bination of three missed tests and/or filing fail-
In the last decade, there has been a worldwide ures within an 18-month period as determined
effort to standardize and regulate the use of per- by Anti-Doping Organizations with jurisdic-
formance enhancing substances by athletes. In the tion over the Athlete shall constitute an anti-
USA, the United States Anti-Doping Agency doping rule violation.
(USADA) was established in 2000 following a • Tampering or Attempted Tampering with any
recommendation by the United States Olympic part of Doping Control.
1074 Doping

• Possession of Prohibited Substances and Pro- Supplement Health and Education Act of 1994
hibited Methods. states that the dietary supplement business in the
• Trafficking or Attempted Trafficking in any USA had annual sales of $4 billion (US Congress
Prohibited Substance or Prohibited Method. 1994), with a more recent estimate by the supple-
• Administration or Attempted administration to ment trade publication Nutritional Business Journal
any Athlete In-Competition of any Prohibited of $27.3 billion (http://nutritionbusinessjournal.
Method or Prohibited Substance, or adminis- com). Presently, the US federal government allows
tration or Attempted administration to any Ath- for the marketing of any substance that affects the
lete Out-of-Competition of any Prohibited structure or function of the body as a dietary or
Method or any Prohibited Substance that is dietary supplement without regulation. However,
prohibited Out-of-Competition, or assisting, the manufacturers must clearly state on product
encouraging, aiding, abetting, covering up, or labels that the dietary supplement is not evaluated
any other type of complicity involving an anti- by the FDA and the product is not intended to
doping rule violation or any Attempted anti- diagnose, treat, or prevent any disease. Also detailed
doping rule violation. in the Dietary Supplement Health and Education
Act of 1994, “The term ‘dietary supplement’
Currently the Code has been formally adopted means a product (other than tobacco) intended to
by many sports governing organizations and has supplement the diet that bears or contains one or
been instrumental in standardizing worldwide more of the following dietary ingredients: a vitamin;
anti-doping efforts, especially among the Olym- a mineral; an herb or other botanical; an amino acid;
pic movement. a dietary substance for use by man to supplement the
According to the US Anti-Doping Agency, a diet by increasing the total dietary intake; a concen-
performance enhancing substance can be placed trate metabolite, constituent, extract, or combination
on the prohibited list (i.e., is banned by some or all of any ingredient described above; is intended for
sports) if it meets two of three criteria: it has the ingestion in the form of a capsule, powder, softgel,
potential to enhance or enhances sport perfor- or gelcap; or is labeled as a dietary supplement.”
mance, it represents an actual or potential health These somewhat relaxed guidelines and definitions
risk to the athlete, or it violates the spirit of sport. are attributed to lobbying by the multibillion-dollar
To aid athletes, the US Anti-Doping Agency dietary supplement industry, with continued discus-
annually publishes a guide to help athletes under- sion of more stringent controls not yet resulting in
stand how prohibited substances affect them with stricter guidelines or FDA oversight.
specific attention paid to medications available on
the US market (US Anti-Doping Agency 2010b).
The US Food and Drug Administration deter- Uses of Performance Enhancing
mines the legal status of substances (i.e., if a Substances by Adolescents
prescription is required to obtain the substance
legally). While performance enhancing sub- Performance enhancing supplement use in adoles-
stances have traditionally been used by athletes cents has not been adequately studied. Although
to improve performance, a growing number of there have been a number of reports in the peer-
individuals, both athletes and nonathletes, have review scientific literature on the prevalence of ana-
adopted the use of such substances to improve bolic steroid and creatine use among adolescents,
body appearance as well. Additionally, athletes few reports provide information on the knowledge,
and nonathletes alike have increasingly adopted attitudes, behaviors, and beliefs (KABs) regarding
the use of the growing number of dietary supple- their use, the reasons for using the substances among
ments which similarly claim to improve physical athletes as well as nonathletes, and the prevalence of
performance and body appearance. use of other supplements.
The use of dietary supplements has dramati- The KABs held by adolescents regarding the
cally risen over the last two decades. The Dietary use of performance enhancing substances is
Doping 1075

important to understanding the trends in their ydrug testing of high school athletes (Holland-
behavior and reasons for their use. Data from Hall 2007). The most studied performance
adolescent populations is limited and the majority enhancing substances in adolescent populations
of it relating to KABs and performance enhancing include anabolic steroids (Buckley et al. 1988;
substances has come from national surveys such Krowchuk et al. 1989; Whitehead et al. 1992;
as the Youth Sports Survey (YSS 2006) and the Johnson et al. 1989; Tanner et al. 1995; Scott
Monitoring the Future Study (Johnston et al. 1996; Faigenbaum et al. 1998; Stilger and
et al. 2009). Yesalis 1999; Radakovich et al. 1993; DHHS
The Youth Sports Survey was administered by 2008; YRBSS 2007; Elliot et al. 2007) and crea- D
the Josephson Institute Center for Sports Ethics to tine (Smith and Dahm 2000; Metzl et al. 2001;
5275 randomly selected high school students Ray et al. 2001; McGuine et al. 2001, 2002;
throughout the USA (www.charactercounts.org). Reeder et al. 2002; Kayton et al. 2002), with a
This survey asked two specific questions regard- few other studies investigating the use of human
ing performance enhancing substances. The growth hormone (Rickert et al. 1992) or general
results of these two questions were that 7% of “dietary supplements” by adolescents (Reeder
boys and 10% of girls disagreed with the state- et al. 2002; Kayton et al. 2002).
ment, “No athlete should use performance Anabolic steroids have long been used by pro-
enhancing drugs because it’s unhealthy” and fessional and recreational athletes to improve ath-
15% of boys and 11% of girls disagreed with the letic performance and physical appearance and
statement, “No athlete should use performance were not classified as a controlled substance
enhancing drugs because it’s cheating.” These until the Anabolic Steroid Control Act of 1990.
findings suggest that adolescent athletes may not Their use among US adolescent athletes report-
hold negative beliefs about the appropriateness of edly first occurred in 1959 (Yersalis et al. 1993).
use of these types of substances (YSS 2006). Anabolic steroids are synthetic substances that
The Monitoring the Future Study is an ongoing mimic testosterone and are used for increasing
study of the behaviors, attitudes, and values of fat-free muscle mass and strength. They have
approximately 50,000 American secondary potentially severe adverse effects. Although only
school students, college students, and young available by prescription now, they continue to be
adults (www.monitoringthefuture.org). This sur- used illegally by both adults and adolescents.
vey specifically asked respondents their percep- Studies in the peer-reviewed literature focus pri-
tions regarding steroid use. When asked how marily on high school students, with a few studies
much people risked harming themselves if they including younger students (Castillo and
took steroids, the percentage of 12th graders Comstock 2007). The prevalence of use among
responding that use of steroids posed “great risk” adolescent athletes has been reported to range
ranged from a high of 70.7% in 1992 to a low of from as low as 1% (Krowchuk et al. 1989) to as
55.0% in 2003. The range of respondents who high as 11% (Johnson et al. 1989) with more
indicated that they disapproved of people taking recent reports of 3.9% from a national survey
steroids ranged from a high of 92.1% in 1992 to a (YRBSS 2007).
low of 86.0% in 2003 (Johnston et al. 2009). The trends in use and reasons for using ana-
bolic steroids have remained consistent in most
studies (Castillo and Comstock 2007). Adoles-
Sports-Related Use of Performance cents involved in sports used steroids more often
Enhancing Substances than those who did not, but use was also noted
among nonathletes (Buckley et al. 1988;
Although adolescent athletes are generally held to Whitehead et al. 1992; Scott et al. 1996). Use
the same standard as collegiate or Olympic ath- varied by sport, with football being the most com-
letes regarding doping, the high costs associated mon sport played by users. Although participating
with testing and lack of testing resources represent in football was common among users, users also
1076 Doping

participated in other sports including gymnastics, among 10th-grade boys was about 5%. Other
weight training, basketball, and baseball (Buckley studies that have reported the prevalence of use
et al. 1988; Whitehead et al. 1992; Radakovich of various other performance enhancing sub-
et al. 1993). Reasons for using steroids ranged stances among adolescents have been limited
from improving athletic performance and improv- (Castillo and Comstock 2007).
ing strength among athletes to improving appear-
ance or building self-esteem among nonathletes
(Buckley et al. 1988; Scott et al. 1996; Non-Sports-Related Use of Performance
Radakovich et al. 1993). Enhancing Substances
Creatine is possibly the most popular perfor-
mance enhancing substance currently used by Although performance enhancing substances
adolescents. Similar to anabolic steroid studies, have been commonly used by athletes to improve
the prevalence of creatine use among adolescents physical fitness and athletic performance, there
has largely focused on high school students with has been an increase in their use among nonath-
one including middle school students (Castillo letes and athletes alike to improve their physical
and Comstock 2007). Creatine is a naturally appearance. As with most behaviors, several fac-
occurring compound in the body that supplies tors contribute to the use of dietary supplements
energy to muscle and is used to enhance recovery including age, history of smoking, alcohol use,
after a workout, build muscle mass, and improve and physical activity. In adolescents, dietary sup-
strength. Among high school athletes, the preva- plements are becoming increasingly popular
lence of use ranged from 8.2% to as high as 30.0% among individuals attempting to improve body
among football players. Its use also varies by sport appearance and achieve healthier lifestyles. This
and gender. McGuine et al. (2002) reported the increase continues despite the limited information
distribution of use in sports by gender. Among on efficacy and safety for the vast majority of
girls, creatine use was most frequent among ath- products available.
letes who participated in track (5.4%), gymnastics Through the normal course of development,
(4.7%), tennis (4.6%), and volleyball (3.8%). adolescents often become obsessed with their
Among boys, creatine use was most frequent body image and peer acceptance. Performance
among athletes who participated in football enhancing substances become a method to easily
(30.1%), swimming (28.4%), hockey (28.0%), self-esteem through improved physical fitness and
and baseball (26.5%). performance, often with disregard to the potential
There are many other types of performance negative short- and long-term health effects. Stud-
enhancing substances available to adolescents ies focusing on the association between body
today, both legally and illegally. However, the image and performance enhancing substance use
prevalence for the use of the majority of perfor- are limited. A study by Kanayama et al. (2006)
mance enhancing substances among adolescents investigated body image issues among male adult
is largely unknown. A few reports have described anabolic-androgenic steroid users. They con-
prevalence of use of a few additional types of cluded that those who used steroids showed
performance enhancing substances such as more issues with body image and although they
human growth hormone (HGH). HGH is used to could not conclude that it was the reason for their
increase muscle mass and strength and has steroid use, they believed it was a possibility.
received an increase in media attention and prod- Field et al. (2005) investigated the exposure to
uct marketing in the past few years. However, mass media, body shape concerns, and the use of
little is currently known about the prevalence of supplements to improve weight and shape among
its use as a performance enhancing supplement. adolescents. They suggest that both girls and boys
Rickert et al. (1992), currently the only peer- who thought about wanting toned or well-defined
reviewed publication that has reported the preva- physiques were at increased risk of using poten-
lence of use among adolescents, reported the use tially unhealthy supplements or other potentially
Doping 1077

unhealthy means to achieve their desired image enhancing substances are also being used to
goals. improve physical fitness and physique among
Additional research is starting to investigate both athletes and nonathletes. Further research is
the risk profiles among performance enhancing needed to determine the prevalence of use as well
substances users. A recent study by Buckman as reasons for use of such substances among the
et al. (2009) sought to determine if those who general adolescent population. This research
reporting using performance enhancing sub- should also focus on factors leading to their use
stances were also more likely to use alcohol and as well as the potential long-term physical and
other social drugs. Users of performance enhanc- psychological impact they may have. D
ing substances are often thought of as athletes who
focus on healthy behaviors; however, little is
known about their substance abuse risk profile.
They found that performance enhancing sub- References
stance users were more likely to report more prob-
Buckley, W. E., Yesalis, C. E., 3rd, Friedl, K. E., et al.
lematic alcohol-use behaviors and alcohol and (1988). Estimated prevalence of anabolic steroid
drug use-related problems. Although the sample use among male high school seniors. Journal
population for this study was male college of the American Medical Association, 260,
3441–3445.
athletes, it leads to some important questions
Buckman, J. F., Yusko, D. A., White, H. R., & Pandina,
regarding the association between performance R. J. (2009). Risk profile of male college athletes who
enhancing substances and substance abuse and use performance-enhancing substances. Journal of
may even pose broader implications for adoles- Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 70, 919–923.
Castillo, E. M., & Comstock, R. D. (2007). Prevalence of
cents. Additional research is needed to assess the
use of performance-enhancing substances among
risk of increased alcohol and social illicit drug use United States adolescents. Pediatric Clinics of North
among young adults as well as adolescents who America, 54, 663–675.
also use performance enhancing substances. Dietary Supplement Health and Education Act of 1994
(DSHEA). FDA summary. http://www.cfsan.fda.gov/
~dms/dietsupp.html. Accessed 12 Jan 2010.
Elliot, D. L., Cheong, J., Moe, E. L., & Goldberg,
Summary L. (2007). Cross-sectional study of female students
reporting anabolic steroid use. Archives of Pediatrics
& Adolescent Medicine, 161, 572–577.
The use of performance enhancing substances and
Emmanquel, E. (1947). History of pharmacy (Istoria
dietary supplements among adolescents appears pharmakeutikis) (p. 126). Athens: Pryssos.
to be common, but is not well studied. In the last Faigenbaum, A. D., Zaichkowsky, L. D., Gardner, D. E.,
decade, there has been a worldwide effort to stan- et al. (1998). Anabolic steroid use by male and female
middle school students. Pediatrics, 101, E6.
dardize and regulate the use of performance
Field, A. E., Austin, S. B., Camargo, C. A., Jr., et al.
enhancing substances among athletes. Although (2005). Exposure to the mass media, body shape con-
this has improved regulation and compliance cerns, and use of supplements to improve weight and
among professional and Olympic athletes, the shape among male and female adolescents. Pediatrics,
116, e214–e220.
resources for screening the much larger popula-
Holland-Hall, C. (2007). Performance-enhancing sub-
tion of adolescent athletes are not available. There stances: Is your adolescent patient using? Pediatric
have been a number of reports in the peer-review Clinics of North America, 54, 651–662.
scientific literature on the prevalence of perfor- Johnson, M. D., Jay, M. S., Shoup, B., et al. (1989).
Anabolic steroid use by male adolescents. Pediatrics,
mance enhancement substance use among adoles-
83, 921–924.
cents, but they have focused on anabolic steroid Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., &
and creatine use. Few reports provide information Schulenberg, J. E. (2009). Monitoring the future
on the knowledge, attitudes, behaviors, and national survey results on drug use, 1975–2008. vol-
ume 1: Secondary school students, NIH Publication
beliefs regarding their use, the reasons for using
No. 09-7402 (p. 721). Bethesda: National Institute on
these substances, and the prevalence of use of Drug Abuse. http://www.monitoringthefuture.org.
other supplements. Additionally, performance Accessed 3 Feb 2010.
1078 Double Standards

Kanayama, G., Barry, S., Hudson, J. I., et al. (2006). Body US Anti-Doping Agency. (2010a). Athlete handbook. US
image and attitudes toward male roles in anabolic- Anti-Doping Agency. http://www.usantidoping.org.
androgenic steroid users. The American Journal of Accessed 15 Mar 2010
Psychiatry, 163, 697–703. US Anti-Doping Agency. (2010b). Athlete guide to the
Kayton, S., Cullen, R. W., Memken, J. A., et al. (2002). 2010 prohibited list. US Anti-Doping Agency. http://
Supplement and ergogenic aid use by competitive www.usantidoping.org. Accessed 15 Mar 2010.
male and female high school athletes (poster). Whitehead, R., Chillag, S., & Elliott, D. (1992). Anabolic
Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 34, steroid use among adolescents in a rural state. The
S193. Journal of Family Practice, 35, 401–405.
Krowchuk, D. P., Anglin, T. M., Goodfellow, D. B., et al. Yersalis, C. E., Courson, S. P., & Wright, J. (1993). History
(1989). High school athletes and the use of ergogenic of anabolic steroid use in sport and exercise. In C. E.
aid. American Journal of Diseases of Children, 143, Yersalis (Ed.), Anabolic steroids in sport and exercise
486–489. (pp. 35–47). Champaign: Human Kinetics.
McGuine, T. A., Sullivan, J. C., & Bernhardt, D. T. (2001). Youth Risk and Behavior Surveillance System (YRBSS) –
Creatine supplementation in high school football National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and
players. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, 11, Health Promotion. Healthy Youth! YRBSS, Youth
247–253. Online: Comprehensive Results. (2007). http://apps.
McGuine, T. A., Sullivan, J. C., & Bernhardt, D. A. (2002). nccd.cdc.gov/yrbss/. Accessed 15 Mar 2010.
Creatine supplementation in Wisconsin high school Youth Sports Survey (YSS). (2006). Josephson Institute.
athletes. WMJ, 101, 25–30. http://charactercounts.org/index.html. Accessed
Metzl, J. D., Small, E., Levine, S. R., et al. (2001). 10 Mar 2010.
Creatine use among young athletes. Pediatrics, 108,
421–425.
Radakovich, J., Broderick, P., & Pickell, G. (1993). Rate of
anabolic-androgenic steroid use among students in
junior high school. The Journal of the American Double Standards
Board of Family Practice, 6, 341–345.
Ray, T. R., Eck, J. C., Covington, L. A., et al. (2001). Use
of oral creatine as an ergogenic aid for increased sports Roger J. R. Levesque
performance: Perceptions of adolescent athletes. South- Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
ern Medical Journal, 94, 608–612.
Reeder, B. M., Rai, A., Patel, D. R., et al. (2002). The
prevalence of nutritional supplement use among high
school students: A pilot study (poster). Medicine and Overview
Science in Sports and Exercise, 34, S193.
Rickert, V. I., Pawlak-Morello, C., Sheppard, V., et al. The concept of double standard refers to flexible
(1992). Human growth hormone: A new substance of principles that apply differently to certain groups
abuse among adolescents? Clinical Pediatrics, 31,
723–726. than others. In the context of adolescence, double
Scott, D. M., Wagner, J. C., & Barlow, T. W. (1996). standards most frequently apply in the realms of
Anabolic steroid use among adolescents in Nebraska education, parenting, and sexuality. Gender par-
schools. American Journal of Health-System Phar- ticularly plays a large role in adolescents’ double
macy, 53, 2068–2072.
Smith, J., & Dahm, D. L. (2000). Creatine use among a standards related to sexual debut, sexting, and
select population of high school athletes. Mayo Clinic social groupings, among other behaviors, charac-
Proceedings, 75, 1257–1263. teristics, and attitudes. Adolescents are especially
Stilger, V. G., & Yesalis, C. E. (1999). Anabolic- susceptible to internalizing these double standards
androgenic steroid use among high school
football players. Journal of Community Health, 24, for several reasons: they have not yet fully formed
131–145. their own belief systems, they are susceptible to
Tanner, S. M., Miller, D. W., & Alongi, C. (1995). Ana- peer influences, and the double standards are
bolic steroid use by adolescents: Prevalence, motives, especially ubiquitous in adolescence. Research
and knowledge of risks. Clinical Journal of Sport Med-
icine, 5(2), 108–115. has explored the dynamics of double standards
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS), in adolescence. Although researchers have not
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Admin- identified effective ways to reduce double stan-
istration. (2008). Results from the 2008 National Sur- dards, they still do have important implications for
vey on Drug Use and Health: National findings.
Rockville. www.nsduhweb.rti.org. Accessed 12 Mar adolescents as well as those who interact
2010. with them.
Double Standards 1079

Varieties of Double Standards Researchers routinely report studies


supporting the commonly held belief that people
“Double standard” is a pejorative term that judge sexual behaviors differently depending on
denotes the use of a different set of principles for the gender of a sexual actor (see Milhausen and
evaluating different groups on similar behaviors, Herold 2001; Fugère et al. 2008). For example,
abilities, characteristics, or attitudes. Most typi- girls and women are believed to be derogated and
cally, the most salient forms of double standards stigmatized for nonmarital sexual contacts, while
relate to intelligence, sexual activity, physical boys and men are thought to receive praise and
looks, as well as political or sexual orientation. positive attributions from others for similar D
At its core, the existence of double standards behaviors. While this double standard is not as
implies that one set of principles is ideal and overt and prevalent as it was during the 1950s and
acceptable for one group but not for another. 1960s, it still exists in an undercurrent of language
Double standards figure prominently in social and social behavior. For example, individuals in
groups. Factors such as race, age, gender, religion, social situations tend to adhere to the sexual dou-
social class, and other markers of difference serve ble standard because they believe that they are
as the factors that determine where individuals displaying the accepted social norm, and since
end up in terms of having the acceptable ideal or they view the sexual double standard as the
having the unacceptable (taboo) concepts applied norm, they act accordingly to avoid social con-
to them. The double standard rests on categories demnation (Marks and Fraley 2006, 2007).
based on stereotypes, and they violate notions Despite the above well-accepted findings, evi-
of equality and impartiality when individuals dence of the sexual double standard’s existence
are held accountable according to different stan- has been elusive. Some research shows, for exam-
dards. Among the more troubling dynamics of ple, that some rate the sexes who have had an
double standards is that their ubiquity is self- above average amount of sexual activity similarly
reinforcing – individuals who engage with double with some equally derogating men and women
standards often find that they are so wide held that who had similar sexual histories (see Gentry
it is easiest to believe them even without personal 1998; Marks and Fraley 2005). These similarities,
experience with any direct evidence upon which however, may disappear when studying unusual
to base their beliefs (De Bock et al. 2012). sexual acts (e.g., multiple partners at once) rather
than common ones (see Browning et al. 2000;
Jonason and Marks 2009). Furthermore, recent
Double Standards and Adolescence research exploring specifically adolescent sexual
debut has found the double-standard to have pre-
Although many double standards relate to adoles- dictable but often unvoiced negative effects on
cent development, including those in the educa- both adolescent boys and girls. Girls who do not
tional system and the effects on student outcomes have their sexual debut by a certain age might be
and self-esteem (see Himmler and Schwager seen as “prudes,” while those who do so “too
2013), the most studied are those that relate to early” are “whores,” while for boys it is a large
sexual double standards based on gender. Research issue of image in terms of demonstrating sexual
in the past has tended not to focus adolescents “prowess” (Kreager et al. 2016).
(instead focusing on college students and adults) Research also reveals that, although the stan-
(Crawford and Popp 2003; Visser and McDonnell dard may be held widely by the general public,
2012). But the relevance of this double standard for findings on the sexual double standards remain
adolescent sexual development and gender equivocal and nuanced as it reveals itself in mul-
inequality as well as other areas of research make tiple ways that might not be expected. Research
it an important concept to understand in and of that has examined the place of other sexual double
itself in addition to a concept that serves as an standards during adolescence reports support
exemplar of how and why double standards exist. for it. Among other factors, the rise of online
1080 Double Standards

communication and use of social media among points. It shows how perceptions are highly
adolescents has had strong impacts on strengthen- influenced by social norms. Those norms are
ing double standards. For example, adolescent influenced by a variety of factors, such as gender,
males are quite likely to feel that they “deserve” social class, and ethnicity. This area of study also
nude photos (“sexting”) from their female peers, shows how, in some situations, those who suffer
while females who ask for such photos in from derogation appear to accept the double stan-
response are quite often stigmatized (Ringrose dards. The reliance on social stereotypes, even by
et al. 2013). Additionally, research has examined those who may be negatively affected by them,
adolescents’ self-reported numbers of sexual part- further reveals the challenges facing efforts that
ners to their relative acceptance by peers (Kreager seek to address them. Adolescents’ entrenchment
and Staff 2009). That research reveals that the in double standards can be reinforced by develop-
association between lifetime sexual partnerships ing identities, values, senses of self, peer influ-
and peer status varies significantly by gender; ences, and online communication and social
greater numbers of sexual partners correlates pos- media. While there may be much to learn, enough
itively with boys’ peer acceptance but correlates evidence reveals the importance of increasing
negatively with girls’ peer acceptance. Boys with awareness of double standards and their effects,
many sexual “conquests” were expected to be especially as they relate to adolescents and their
well liked, while permissive girls ended with a development.
low status in school-based networks, regardless
of whether their sexual behaviors occur within
“romantic” relationships. Importantly, the study
also noted that the relationship between boys’ Cross-References
sexual behaviors and peer acceptance was moder-
ated by socioeconomic origins. Sexually permis- ▶ Sexism
sive boys from disadvantaged backgrounds had
more friendships than permissive boys from more
advantaged backgrounds; that link was not References
supported for girls. Notably, permissive boys did
not lose status among girls, and permissive girls Browning, J. R., Hatfield, E., Kessler, D., & Levine, T.
(2000). Sexual motives, gender, and sexual behavior.
did not appear to gain status from male peers, even Archives of Sexual Behavior, 29, 135–153.
though they were clearly sexually active. The Crawford, M., & Popp, D. (2003). Sexual double stan-
study supported the existence of an adolescent dards: A review and methodological critique of two
sexual double standard and suggests that sexual decades of research. Journal of Sex Research, 40,
13–26.
norms vary by both gender and socioeconomic De Bock, T., Vermeir, I., & Van Kenhove, P. (2012).
origins. Moreover, adolescents’ individual identi- “What’s the harm in being unethical? These strangers
ties being not fully formed renders them increas- are rich anyway!” exploring underlying factors of dou-
ingly susceptible to holding on to double ble standards. Journal of Business Ethics, 112(2),
225–240.
standards as an easy way of conforming and Fugère, M. A., Escoto, C., Cousins, A. J., Riggs, M. L., &
avoiding overly complex decision-making, Haerich, P. (2008). Sexual attitudes and double stan-
whether in terms of romantic relationships dards: A literature review focusing on participant gen-
(Tolman et al. 2016) or even consumer behaviors der and ethnic background. Sexuality and Culture, 12,
169–182.
(Williams and Steffel 2014). Gentry, M. (1998). The sexual double standard: The influ-
ence of number of relationships and level of sexual
activity on judgments of women and men. Psychology
Conclusion of Women Quarterly, 22, 505–511.
Himmler, O., & Schwager, R. (2013). Double standards in
educational standards – Do schools with a disadvan-
The study of all double standards but especially taged student body grade more leniently? German Eco-
sexual double standards reveals important key nomic Review, 14(2), 166–189.
Dress Codes and Uniforms 1081

Jonason, P., & Marks, P. (2009). Common vs. uncommon within one of three theories (a) human develop-
sexual acts: Evidence for the sexual double standard. ment theory, (b) risk taking theory, or (c) social
Sex Roles, 60, 357–365.
Kreager, D. A., & Staff, J. (2009). The sexual double norm theory. A variety of variables and methods
standard and adolescent peer acceptance. Social Psy- are used to study specific segments of the popula-
chology Quarterly, 72, 143–164. tion of kindergarten through high school students,
Kreager, D., Staff, J., Gauthier, R., Lefkowitz, E. S., & as well as teachers, parents, and school personnel.
Feinberg, M. E. (2016). The double standard at sexual
debut: Gender, sexual behavior and adolescent peer Controversies regarding student dress and the
acceptance. Sex Roles, 75(7), 377–392. rationale of proponents and opponents are
Marks, M. J., & Fraley, R. C. (2005). The sexual double explained with findings from specific studies. D
standard: Fact or fiction? Sex Roles, 52, 175–186. Gaps in knowledge and measurement issues pro-
Marks, M. J., & Fraley, R. C. (2006). Confirmation bias
and the sexual double standard. Sex Roles, 54, 19–26. vide suggestions for further research. Dress code
Marks, M. J., & Fraley, R. C. (2007). The impact of social policies should be designed as part of a school’s
interaction on the sexual double standard. Social Influ- social structure to facilitate students’ thriving
ence, 2, 29–54. behaviors and growth into self-actualizing adults.
Milhausen, R. R., & Herold, E. S. (2001).
Reconceptualizing the sexual double standard. Journal
of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 13, 63–83.
Ringrose, J., Harvey, L., Gill, R., & Livingstone, S. (2013). Introduction
Teen girls, sexual double standards and “sexting”: Gen-
dered value in digital image exchange. Feminist The-
ory, 14(3), 305–323. Education is an important function of state and
Tolman, D., Davis, B. R., & Bowman, C. P. (2016). “That’s local governments “because our society rests for
just how it is”: A gendered analysis of masculinity and its continuance upon the growth of children into
femininity ideologies in adolescent girls’ and boys’ well-developed and self-actualizing adults”
heterosexual relationships. Journal of Adolescent
Research, 31(1), 3–31. (Myhra 1999, p. 399). The school environment
Visser, R., & McDonnell, E. (2012). ‘That’s OK. He’s a has unique facets (a) a captive audience of
guy’: A mixed-methods study of gender double- (b) students legally defined as minors who are
standards for alcohol use. Psychology & Health, (c) a vulnerable population susceptible to peer
27(5), 618–639.
Williams, E., & Steffel, M. (2014). Double standards in the pressure, (d) cognitively immature, with (e) a
use of enhancing products by self and others. Journal of known proclivity for risky behaviors, and
Consumer Research, 41(2), 506–525. (f) officials with power to act in loco parentis in
(g) a limited-public forum. Freedom of expression
in the school environment allows for endorsement
of unpopular and controversial opinions, but it
Dress Codes and Uniforms must be counterbalanced by society’s interest in
teaching students the parameters of civil behavior
Beth W. Freeburg1 and Jane E. Workman2 or respect for others (LaVigne 1999). Dress code
1
School of Education, Saint Louis University, policies outline the constraints of civil behavior
St. Louis, MO, USA and are guidelines by which students’ right of
2
School of Architecture, Fashion Design and expression is limited because it may infringe on
Merchandising Program, Southern Illinois others’ right to an education (Murphy 1991).
University, Carbondale, IL, USA Dress code policies vary along a continuum of
restrictiveness: (a) no dress code policy at all,
(b) general dress code that outlines broad princi-
Overview ples, (c) itemized dress code policy usually pro-
scriptive in nature (what not to wear),
The purpose of this chapter is to summarize (d) voluntary uniform policy, (e) mandatory uni-
research related to dress codes and uniforms in form policy with an opt-out provision, and (f) a
K-12 settings. The summary includes 21 major mandatory uniform policy without an opt-out pro-
studies that are either atheoretical or framed vision (Paliokas 2005). US public schools have
1082 Dress Codes and Uniforms

traditionally had dress code policies; private physical security; (b) the research focused on ele-
schools (e.g., parochial schools) have typically mentary, middle, and high school populations;
prescribed uniforms (DelCampo and DelCampo (c) data sources included teacher survey
1998). There is a nationwide trend toward adop- responses, interviews, observations, student sur-
tion of more restrictive dress code policies as a veys and observations, newspaper articles, stu-
response to increasing school violence (DaCosta dent handbooks, and large databases referred to
2006; Paliokas 2005; Sanchez et al. 2012; Yeung as big data; and (d) data were analyzed using
2009). quantitative descriptive statistics, t-tests, correla-
tion, analysis of variance, multivariate analysis of
variance, regression, content analysis, and
Major Theorists and Researchers chi-square; and qualitative interviews, focus
group analysis, and content analysis of documents
Scholars in various disciplines (e.g., sociology, (e.g., minutes from meetings, letters, brochures)
education, psychology, family studies, school and field notes.
law) have studied dress codes and uniforms from One necessary characteristic of quality
a variety of perspectives. This research can be research is grounding in a theory that provides a
subsumed under the umbrella of three major the- basis for explaining why something happens and
ories: human development theory, risk-taking the- establishes a context for understanding events
ory, and social norm theory. Guidelines for (Wellington and Szczerbiński 2007). This under-
student dress involve a context of social interac- standing can potentially lead to generalizability or
tion that can be described by its temporal, physi- broad application. A theory includes:
cal, and social environments (Workman and (a) interrelated concepts, definitions, and proposi-
Freeburg 2009). Human development theory tions used for specifying relationships among var-
examines dress codes and uniforms from the per- iables; (b) a systematic strategy used to explain
spective of the temporal environment, that is, the behavior; (c) applicability in multiple content
past, present, and future. One focus is the life areas; (d) unique terms; and (e) operationalized
stage of individuals and the accomplishment of components that are testable.
developmental tasks necessary to progress to the
next stage. Risk-taking theory examines dress
codes and uniforms from the perspective of the Human Development Theory
physical environment, that is, material artifacts,
geographic location, and the setting in determin- Human development theory is most often associ-
ing how to ameliorate risks to a vulnerable popu- ated with the work of Erikson (1997). From the
lation. Social norm theory examines dress codes theory of human development came the notion of
and uniforms from the perspective of the social developmental tasks of early adolescence such as
environment, that is, characteristics of people with physical maturation, emotional development, peer
their statuses, roles, and relationships. Reviews of group membership, and heterosexual relation-
case law represent an historical look at the process ships. School dress code and uniform policies
of social control regarding dress code and uniform are sometimes rationalized based on the idea of
policies. assisting adolescents with achievement of devel-
Empirical research regarding dress codes and opmental tasks.
uniforms includes 21 studies (see Table 1). A sum- Most public school dress code policies are
mary of the studies follows: (a) variables exam- found at the junior and senior high level when
ined were the rationale for dress codes, content of students are in early adolescence (ages 12–18).
dress codes, sanctions for violating dress codes, Students in early adolescence tend to think in
reasons for violating dress codes, and the relation- concrete terms (Erikson 1997). Concrete thinking
ship of dress to student behavior, school climate, involves literal interpretation of visual stimuli.
safe-school planning, and psychological and Thus, it is important in dress code policies to
Dress Codes and Uniforms 1083

Dress Codes and Uniforms, Table 1 Research regarding dress codes and uniforms: grades K–12
Researchers Year Variables Population Data source Method
Alleyne et al. 2003 Student dress (including Middle Black educators, Descriptive
uniforms) and student behavior school sixth graders, statistics, focus
eighth graders group data
analysis
Arnold and 2003 Offensive T-shirts, violent High school High school Descriptive
Workman behavior, consequences of students statistics, t-tests,
violent behavior, substance use, correlation
attitude toward school D
Baxter and 2008 Cultural meaning of fashion in High school Male high school Ethnography:
Marina marginalized subculture, subculture students interviews
demands for conformity
Brunsma and 1998 Student uniforms, attendance, Tenth grade Big Data: Descriptive
Rockquemore behavioral problems, substance National statistics and
abuse, academic achievement Educational weighted
Longitudinal regression
Study 1988
DaCosta 2006 School uniform policy, High School Big Data: Student Interviews over
compliance, academic Life in High 2-year period
performance Schools Project
(SLP)
Firmin et al. 2006 School uniform policies Christian Administrators, Interviews
K-12 schools faculty, staff,
students, parents
Freeburg 2004 Dress code rationale High school Student Content analysis
et al. handbooks
Han 2011 Mandatory school uniform K-12 Public Big Data: School Descriptive
policy and students’ problem schools Survey on Crime statistics,
behaviors and Safety’s analysis of
(SSOCS) variance, and
2003–2004 multiple
regression
Herbon and 2000 Content of dress and High school Student Content analysis
Workman appearance codes handbooks
LaPoint et al. 2003 Uniforms, dress-related Middle Black urban Descriptive
problems school middle school statistics,
students chi-square
Morris 2005 Race, class, gender inequities, Middle Students, Ethnography:
and regulation of students’ school teachers, aides, interviews,
bodies administrators observations, and
field notes
Murray 1997 Uniforms, school climate Middle Urban sixth, Descriptive
school seventh, and statistics,
eighth grade analysis of
students variance,
correlation
Ogle and 2002 Media dress-related responses, Columbine Dress-related text Content analysis,
Eckman who responded, rationale for (CO) High in two Colorado grounded theory
response School newspapers
O’Neal 1998 Risky behaviors related to dress High school Interviews Focus groups
Sanchez et al. 2012 Opinions about school Middle Student survey, Descriptive
uniforms by gender, grade school school police statistics,
level, benefits, race/ethnicity, records, and chi-square
and school violence and safety disciplinary data
(continued)
1084 Dress Codes and Uniforms

Dress Codes and Uniforms, Table 1 (continued)


Researchers Year Variables Population Data source Method
Studak and 2007 Civil behavior, safe-school US schools Dress-related text Content analysis
Workman planning, dress codes in the newspaper
Workman 2003 Ownership and visibility of Middle and Students in grades Descriptive
alcohol promotional clothing high school 7–12 statistics, t-tests,
items, alcohol use, perceived chi-square
parental approval of drinking
Workman and 2006 Dress codes, psychological High school Student Content analysis
Freeburg security, physical security handbooks
Workman 2004 Dress code violation sanctions High school Student Content analysis
et al. handbooks
Workman 2011 Message t-shirts and school High school Students and Descriptive
et al. safety (physical and teachers: Survey statistics,
psychological) with pictures ANOVA/
MANOVA, cross
tabs
Yeung 2009 School uniforms and student Kindergarten Big data: ECLS-K Descriptive
achievement and and NELS statistics, t-tests,
secondary and multiple
school regression

define and give specific examples of prescribed psychological and physical danger even if the
(e.g., appropriate dress) or proscribed dress (e.g., protection restricts some constitutional rights
immodest dress) for both boys and girls. (Myhra 1999). The constitution does not grant
By providing boundaries and expectations individuals an absolute right to free expression
(e.g., through dress code policies), schools can of ideas; this is especially true within the unique
provide adolescents with the resources needed to context of the school (Miller 2002).
thrive (Scales et al. 2000). Adolescent thriving
can be assessed by indicators such as success in
school, helping others, physical health, ability to Risk-Taking Theory
delay gratification, and valuing diversity. These
positive indicators are in direct opposition to com- Risk-taking theory examines dress codes and
mercial messages emphasizing instant gratifica- uniforms from the perspective of the physical
tion, selfish indulgence, and materialistic values. environment, that is, the material artifacts,
Schools can help students develop effective self- geographic location, and the setting in deter-
regulation by reinforcing a commitment to posi- mining how to ameliorate risks to a vulnerable
tive values and social competencies such as civil population. Material artifacts refer to tangible
behavior. Adolescent well-being should be fore- objects (e.g., dress); setting refers to the place
most in any rationale for dress code policies where the event takes place (e.g., school build-
(Freeburg et al. 2004). ing and grounds). Irwin (1993) defined risk
Because of adolescents’ developmental stage, taking as deliberately choosing behaviors with
they have not developed an ability to consistently uncertain and possibly negative outcomes.
make mature decisions. “The First Amendment Experimenting with a wide range of behaviors,
rights of children are not as extensive as those of including risky behaviors, is a normal part of
adults because of children’s unique position rela- adolescent development (Irwin 1993). Cogni-
tive to their parents and the state and because of tive immaturity may partially explain why ado-
their less developed capacity to engage in rational lescents make impulsive decisions, rationalize
deliberation” (Myhra 1999, p. 365). Schools have impulsive choices by minimizing the potential
extensive power to protect students from possible for personal risk, and appear insensitive to
Dress Codes and Uniforms 1085

potential consequences of their actions (Bell reason for dress code revisions. Dress code revi-
and Bell 1993; Gardner 1993). sions had an unstated but underlying goal of
School boards are entrusted to provide children encouraging civil behavior. These findings relate
in their districts the skills necessary to cope in dress code guidelines to the educational mission
society; thus, “school boards have a substantial of the school.
interest in creating and maintaining a safe and For some school districts, the rationale for a
effective learning environment” (Murphy 1991, dress code policy is reducing risks to physical and
p. 1338). A commonly accepted notion is that it psychological safety (Workman and Freeburg
is better to prevent or decrease significant risks to 2006). For example, in efforts to be fashionable, D
a population than to deal with problems after they students want expensive items – designer clothes,
have occurred. Three principles used to guide sneakers, or sports team gear – and have attacked
school officials’ decisions are risk to others, risk other students to acquire them (Why Dress Codes
to self, and protection of vulnerable persons. n.d.). However, safety involves more than threats
Avoidance of risk of serious harm to others is of physical violence. Safety also implies social
known as the harm principle. Risk to self refers and psychological security, for example, freedom
to behavior that appears to affect only the person from fear of intimidation or peer discrimination.
engaging in it. Protection of vulnerable persons Focus group interviews with 300 students in
involves safeguarding interests of individuals grades 9–12 examined why students engaged in
who are not in a position to defend themselves. risky behaviors related to dress (O’Neal 1998).
Dress code and uniform policies are policies with Students wore dress that had potential for negative
implications for risks to health, safety, and morals outcomes (e.g., physical abuse) in order to avoid
of the school population. other negative outcomes (e.g., social or psycho-
Content analyses of dress code policies in pub- logical abuse). Social and psychological abuse
lic secondary school handbooks and in news (e.g., humiliation, intimidation, isolation, and
reports revealed stated rationale, listed dress peer discrimination) were considered more likely
items allowed and prohibited, and specified sanc- to occur than physical abuse.
tions for violation of dress code policies. Analysis For some schools, the rationale for a dress code
of school handbooks from 150 schools revealed policy is reducing risks to students’ health, for
16 different rationales for dress codes of which example, banning items with references to alco-
students’ right to a nondisruptive educational hol, tobacco, or illegal drugs. These products are
environment was most frequent, followed by stu- harmful and minors are not legally permitted to
dents’ right to a safe and healthy environment use them. Arnold and Workman (2003) examined
(Freeburg et al. 2004). Secondary school dress the relationship between problem behavior among
codes generally specified required, acceptable, high school students and ownership of T-shirts
and unacceptable clothing items with unaccept- advertising alcohol, tobacco, and weapons. Stu-
able items (e.g., tank tops, halter/backless/midriff dents who owned these T-shirts, compared with
tops) most frequently listed (Herbon and Work- those who did not, were more likely to self-report
man 2000). Sanctions for dress code violations engaging in violent behavior, experiencing conse-
were found to be hierarchical in nature (e.g., ver- quences of problem behavior, engaging in sub-
bal warning, sent home to change, suspension) stance use, and having a negative attitude toward
with severity of sanctions increasing as the num- school. There were positive correlations between
ber of repeat violations increased (Workman ownership of a T-shirt that advertised alcohol and
et al. 2004). Studak and Workman (2007) exam- students’ drinking behavior; ownership of a
ined news reports (104 articles from 44 US news- T-shirt that advertised tobacco and students’
papers) to identify incidents that precipitated dress smoking behavior; and ownership of a T-shirt
code revisions. School safety (violence or threat that depicted a weapon and students’ having
of violence in school, gang activity/influence, used a gun or knife to take something from
neighborhood violence) was the predominant someone else.
1086 Dress Codes and Uniforms

Other dress code policies are instituted to lessons about how schools regulate the appear-
reduce risks to morality, for example, policies ance of their students.
prohibiting vulgar or obscene language on cloth- One goal of schools is to provide an atmo-
ing (Weisenberger 2000). Immorality includes sphere conducive to learning. As one method of
sexual promiscuity, obscenity, and vulgarity. The social control, schools may employ narrowly
mass media devote vast resources to commercial- defined time, place, and manner regulations by
ization of the fashion and entertainment industries regulating, while students are in school, student
(Linn 2003), where traditional values and behav- expression “that might reasonably be perceived to
ioral constraint give way to innovativeness and advocate drug or alcohol use, irresponsible sex, or
pushing boundaries. Commercial appeals to conduct otherwise inconsistent with the shared
sophistication and fashionability may pressure values of a civilized social order” (Murphy
female students to dress and act in sexually pro- 1991, p. 1349). Regulations that are content
vocative ways (Linn 2003). The Supreme Court based have a goal of “suppression of the message
noted that a legitimate concern of schools is to itself, while a content-neutral regulation is one in
protect children from exposure to material that is which the impact on expression is only incidental”
sexually explicit, indecent, or lewd (DeMitchell (LaVigne 1999, p. 393). Uniform policies are
2002). content neutral because they suppress all clothing
choice as a medium of expression for students
while at school; they are not targeted to suppres-
Social Norm Theory sion of one particular dress style, one specific
clothing item, or one particular message (Miller
Social norm theory assumes that (a) socialization 2002). Another form of content-neutral regulation
is one form of social control, (b) sanctions encour- available to schools is dress code policies. Spe-
age compliance with norms, and (c) appropriate cific messages are not prohibited and sufficient
behavior is learned through interaction with other options remain outside of school for expression
people within a specific social environment of the messages.
(Schwartz and Ewald 1968; Workman and School boards have a compelling interest in
Freeburg 2009). There are six steps in the process controlling threats to a safe and effective learning
of social control: norms (e.g., dress code policies), environment (Murphy 1991). The school’s com-
violation of norms (e.g., students who ignore or pelling interest in controlling student appearance
deliberately violate dress codes), recognition of in order to maintain discipline within the school
violation (e.g., teachers notice students who are environment may outweigh students’ right of free
in violation of the dress code), report of violation expression (Killen 2000).
(e.g., teachers report student violators to the prin- While attending school, students – who have
cipal), sanctions (e.g., consequences specified in the legal status of minors – are deprived of paren-
the school handbook), and enforcement of sanc- tal protection and have limited ability to protect
tions (e.g., principal enforces sanctions by send- themselves. If students’ right to an education is
ing student violator to detention). To apply this put at risk because of violence, disciplinary prob-
theory to dress code policies, it is apparent that: lems, negative attitudes toward school, and peer
(a) such policies are socialization devices; pressure, school officials have an obligation to
(b) sanctions associated with violation of dress take action. More and more, school officials take
codes are written to encourage compliance with action by invoking their constitutionally accept-
norms; and (c) students learn appropriate and able power to enact dress code policies as one
inappropriate behavior while observing and means to control student behavior. Historically,
interacting with peers, teachers, administrators, school officials get their authority to control stu-
and other school personnel in the school environ- dent behavior, including students’ dress and
ment. Morris (2005) identified a “. . .hidden cur- appearance, from the principle of in loco parentis
riculum” (p. 28) purported to teach unspoken (DeMitchell 2002). In loco parentis “encompasses
Dress Codes and Uniforms 1087

the common law view of the legal status of minors attend school; a captive audience may include
in the public school setting” (DeMitchell 2002, some who are unwilling to be exposed to contro-
p. 19). School employees assume the duty to versial messages; the First Amendment assumes a
protect because they have supervisory responsi- willing audience (Murphy 1991). When students
bility over students and have assumed the role of wear message T-shirts, the message is persistently
parental substitute. Schools have implemented displayed – unwilling members of the audience
dress code policies with stated purposes of con- cannot ignore it. For example, 23 students were
trolling the classroom atmosphere so that fear, suspended for wearing clothing with Confederate
violence, and intimidation are nonexistent; inno- flag symbols in violation of the high school’s D
cent nongang bystanders are protected; and learn- dress code (Shapiro and Balingit 2015).
ing is encouraged (Watkins and Hooks 1999). A spokesperson for the school said the symbols
Certain items of clothing symbolize lifestyles were banned following racially motivated fights
related to gangs, drugs, violence, and disrespect linked to students wearing Confederate symbols.
for authority (Killen 2000; LaPoint et al. 1993; The Confederate flag has a long history of contro-
Murphy 1991). Thus, some schools have versy: some see it as a symbol of Southern heri-
established policies prohibiting signs of gang tage and others see it as a symbol of hatred,
membership such as jewelry, caps, bandannas, slavery, and bigotry. Both students and teachers
and clothing that is all one color, especially red gave the lowest ratings for feelings of safety to
or blue (Watkins and Hooks 1999). being in the presence of someone wearing a Con-
Murphy (1991) analyzed the implications of federate flag T-shirt (Workman et al. 2011).
gang-related clothing as a form of overt intimida- Graphic messages are not an issue when message
tion. Gang membership can be conveyed by wear- T-shirts are prohibited. Such narrowly defined
ing specific colors, accessories, or garments. regulations only require students to change their
Through clothing, gang members express pride appearance while in school – not permanently
in associating with a particular gang. Dress code (Ruggiero 2002). Thus, the stage is set for contro-
policies prohibiting gang clothing may help con- versy. According to Mitchell and Knechtle (2003,
trol violence because, without symbols to ignite p. 491),
violence, fights between rival gangs will be less
When weighing the possible likelihood of eliminat-
likely. Gang clothing can be intimidating because ing school violence, creating an environment con-
students are aware that gangs maintain control ducive for learning, structuring a level playing field,
using violence and harassment. Gang members reducing competition, and enhancing school pride
who wear gang clothing or symbols in school are and self-esteem versus stymieing the freedom of
children to select their school wardrobe, it can be
trying to exert control while in school. When gang argued that the sides of the scale are not even close
clothing is prohibited other students are less to being balanced.
intimidated because gang members no longer out-
wardly declare control by their dress and covert Morris’s study (2005) of the hidden curriculum
threats of violence are diminished. was framed in reproductive theory, a variant of
social control theory that relies on social norms.
There were attempts to control middle school
Controversies and Related Research students’ clothing on nonuniform days and at
school events. Findings revealed inequities across
Students’ clothing choices are silent personal gender, class, and race, which generated students
expression – expressed in school where they are who oppose school, or at minimum, felt out of
required to be (Nishigai 2001). Dress code poli- place there. For example, wearing all blue – shirt
cies limit students’ freedom to use dress as per- and pants – was interpreted to indicate gang mem-
sonal expression because it may infringe on other bership only when worn by Latino boys. Asian
students’ right to an education. Students are a students were treated as a “. . .model-minority”
captive audience because they are required to (p. 39) no matter their clothing choices. Morris
1088 Dress Codes and Uniforms

concluded that schools unevenly convey the Opponents


styles of the dominant culture through unspoken
lessons, in addition to formal school dress codes. Opponents of dress code and uniform policies
allege that benefits may be based on intuitively
accepted premises, as there is little statistical evi-
Proponents dence to support claims of benefits (Brunsma and
Rockquemore 1998; DaCosta 2006; Sarke 1998;
Proponents of prescriptive dress code policies Yeung 2009). They point out that dress code and
allege that these policies promote student safety uniform policies restrict youth from
by reducing negative peer pressure, gang-related experimenting with identity expression, an impor-
activity, and fighting (LaPoint et al. 2003; LaPoint tant developmental task of adolescence. Oppo-
et al. 1992). As a security measure, dress code nents also assume that these policies contribute
policies are less intrusive for students than other to punitive zero tolerance in student discipline
security measures, such as metal detectors, secu- policies (Skiba 2001). The constitutional issue of
rity guards, locker checks, locked doors, and zero- freedom of expression is a key component of
tolerance policies. opposition to dress code and uniform policies
Further, prescriptive dress code policies are because policies sometimes fail to acknowledge
alleged to contribute to positive school attitudes, cultural, religious, and ethnic dimensions of dress
lessen pressure on families to buy expensive (LaPoint et al. 2003), for example, prohibition of
trendy clothing for their children, and decrease hip-hop dress or Muslim headscarves.
absenteeism, disciplinary sanctions, and sub- Students are frequently opposed to dress code
stance abuse (Holloman et al. 1996; LaPoint and uniform policies. Guided by ecological
et al. 1992, 2003). Principals reported that dress human development perspectives, DaCosta
code policies were effective in improving behav- (2006) examined the messages conveyed through
ior, reducing peer sexual harassment, and prepar- school uniform policies. For example, are stu-
ing students for the world of work (DeMitchell dents’ positive identities associated with school
et al. 2000). Advocates of school uniform policies uniforms (e.g., pride and inspiration) or are uni-
credit uniforms with enhancement of discipline, forms “. . .markers for who can ‘pass,’ or who can
respect, academic performance, attendance, posi- proceed through the school day without threat of
tive attitudes, school climate, and safety (Sarke harassment or disciplinary ‘profiling’?” (p. 50).
1998). In addition, uniforms have been linked to Student narratives revealed that the majority of
reduced peer pressure and reduced financial strain students in this urban high school were opposed
on parents (Why Dress Codes n.d.). to the uniform policy that included an alternate
Han (2011) used a nationally representative uniform used as punishment. In fact, Han (2011)
School Survey on Crime and Safety findings found that at the high school level, mandatory
(i.e., big data) to examine the relationships uniform policies increased student problem
among a mandatory school uniform policy, stu- behaviors. Two additional studies determined
dent problem behaviors, and various school safety that students found little benefit in dress code
efforts in urban high schools where most such policies. Firmin et al. (2006) found that students
policies are adopted. Problem behaviors ranged in two Christian schools reported little progress in
in severity from minor (e.g., class disruption) to eliminating competition, encouraging appropriate
serious and violent (e.g., rape, physical attack, dress, decreasing distractions in the learning envi-
robbery). School safety efforts included parental ronment, and reducing the cost of clothing stu-
involvement, crime prevention programs for stu- dents for school. However, students did believe
dents, teacher training programs, and community that uniform policies encouraged more modest
efforts. School uniform policies significantly dress. Sanchez et al. (2012) found that seven and
reduced student problem behaviors at the elemen- eighth grade students did not perceive any benefits
tary and middle school level. in wearing school uniforms.
Dress Codes and Uniforms 1089

To understand high school students’ resistance human development theory, risk-taking theory,
to dress code policies, Baxter and Marina (2008) and social norm theory could be combined to
framed their research using social control theory, create a theory to guide research regarding dress
specifically focused on violation of norms and the codes and uniforms.
reasons behind the violations. In addition to for- Researchers have examined to varying extents
mal and informal interviews, extensive field notes the relationship of dress codes and uniforms to
based on personal observations were the sources both thriving and disruptive student behaviors.
of data. Students violated the dress code as a: There is limited research and, therefore, knowl-
(a) means to challenge authority; (b) way to gain edge gaps in understanding on the relationship of D
status by customizing their hair and clothes; student dress with thriving behaviors, such as
(c) way to celebrate the self in ways essential for academic achievement and proschool attitudes
recognition in the subculture; and (d) show of (Brunsma and Rockquemore 1998; Murray
strength, power, and upward mobility within 1997). There is a great deal of empirical data
their subculture. regarding some behaviors that create risk to
There is no guarantee that dress codes will be health, safety, and morality in the school environ-
an effective deterrent to school violence, but that ment (e.g., violence, bullying, drinking, smoking,
is also true of other security measures (e.g., metal and drug abuse). Data about other risks are sparse
detectors, security guards). Likewise, there is little (e.g., verbal and visual harassment, teasing).
evidence that uniforms improve achievement in There are little or no data on visual harassment
school; they are “. . . not a panacea” for educa- using dress as a method of invoking fear and
tional reform (Yeung 2009, p. 871). Dress code intimidation – powerful emotions that hinder a
policies have inherent weaknesses, for example, student’s ability to concentrate on academic
policies may be vague or overbroad, not achievement (LaPoint et al. 1993).
completely address the problems they were cre- Regarding thriving behaviors, data can be
ated to solve, and the same issues are subject to gathered to determine the extent to which dress
litigation in different jurisdictions (Killen 2000; codes and uniforms increase attendance, self-
Freeburg et al. 2004; Herbon and Workman esteem, belonging, and school spirit (Brunsma
2000). When school officials and students spend and Rockquemore 1998; Scales et al. 2000). For
their time and resources on legal disputes, they example, Murray (1997) studied the effects of
cannot focus on their primary mission of educa- uniforms on school climate by comparing stu-
tion and learning (Nishigai 2001). dents in a school with a uniform policy to students
in a school without a uniform policy. The two
middle schools were from the same urban area
Gaps in Knowledge and comparable in student characteristics such as
racial composition, state categorization, and num-
Quality research studies most generally are built ber of students receiving free/reduced price lunch.
on a theoretical foundation or overarching struc- In nine of ten subscales, students with a uniform
ture that is used to investigate research questions policy rated their school climate more positively
and explain findings. Three studies, two using big than students without a uniform policy. Murray
data and another using qualitative methods, exam- concluded that school uniforms do have a positive
ined the impact of school uniforms on multiple effect on student perceptions of school climate.
student behaviors and outcomes without a theo- Regarding disruptive behaviors, data can be
retical framework. A number of existing theories compiled on dress and its relationship to student
could be applied to components of dress code and problem behaviors such as bullying, coercion,
uniform research. One gap in knowledge is the disruptive behavior, harassment, intimidation,
absence of an all-encompassing theory to provide substance use (alcohol, tobacco, drugs), threats,
a context for understanding and explaining the verbal abuse, and violence (Holloman 1995;
effects of dress codes and uniforms. Elements of Holloman et al. 1996; LaPoint et al. 1992, 1993;
1090 Dress Codes and Uniforms

O’Neal 1998). For example, in the Columbine of variables via numeric data (quantitative) and
(CO) High School shootings, jocks’ baseball nonnumeric (qualitative) data. Appropriate
caps with specific rounded bills implicitly com- numeric research methods include surveys and
municated threats of impending bullying behavior observations. One example of numeric research
(Ogle and Eckman 2002). School officials have a is big data sets which are increasingly being used
substantial interest in reducing the number of to understand both thriving and disrupting student
innocent students who are intimidated through behaviors. Big data features: “. . .multi-sources,
such symbols (Murphy 1991). Based on anecdotal huge volume, and [is] fast changing” (Jin
evidence, some administrators believe that dress et al. 2015 p. 63). Appropriate nonnumeric
codes have been effective in reducing violence in research methods include ethnography, content
their schools. Other types of problem behavior analysis, narrative inquiry, artifact analysis, and
possibly linked to dress can be documented via historical sources. Issues related to validity and
anecdotal reports, for example, humiliation, neg- reliability are associated with both types of data.
ative attitudes toward school, peer discrimination, School officials can be proactive in their eval-
and implied threats of violence. uation of the impact of student dress. Regarding
A lack of documented evidence may lead to a disruptive behaviors, data are necessary to answer
conclusion that a dress code policy has been two questions: can a reasonable school official
implemented as a way to instill good taste in foresee a disruption and would a reasonable
students’ personal appearance. This conclusion school official find the disruption to be substantial
suggests a superficial policy aimed at student and material (Miller 2002). If school officials can
expression and not connected to student behav- logically predict substantial disruption, they can
ioral outcomes (Brunsma and Rockquemore prohibit student expression (i.e., particular items
1998). Student expression should be restricted of dress) before any disruption occurs (Sarke
only when necessary to attain an important pur- 1998). But school officials must provide evidence
pose (Sarke 1998). When carefully documented that the school’s educational mission is endan-
evidence leads to a conclusion that the dress code gered by a foreseeable and perhaps impending
policy is aimed at students’ behavior for reasons risk of disruption. School records can provide
not related to the expressive component, then the evidence of past instances of actual or threatened
policy is permissible if it can endure inspection disruption relating to disputed clothing items
under the four-part test of United States v. O’Brien (Miller 2002). When courts apply the substantial
(Myhra 1999). The O’Brien test requires the interference test, school districts may be required
school to prove that the dress code policy is: to show how regulating student dress will actually
(a) within the constitutional power of the school; reduce the risk of disruption (LaVigne 1999).
(b) that it advances a significant interest of the Regarding thriving behaviors, data can provide
school; (c) that the school’s interest is not related evidence suggesting the impact of school dress
to limiting free expression; and (d) that the limita- codes and uniforms on achievement (Yeung
tion of free expression is no greater than necessary 2009), ease of going to school, confidence, and
to advance the school’s interest (LaVigne 1999). self-esteem (Sanchez et al. 2012) and youth
resourcefulness during challenging situations to
maintain their sense of freedom (DaCosta 2006).
Measurement and Measurement Issues Multiple sources of data are needed to provide
sufficient evidence supporting the need for stu-
Measurement and the analysis of data are the dent dress codes or uniforms. Observational anal-
foundations of social science research (White ysis should include an awareness of current events
and Breckenridge 2014). Together they are used influencing student behavior such as fashion
to evaluate the impact of dress code and uniform trends and popular entertainers. For example,
policies on student behavior. Specific research one dress code policy prohibited students from
questions can be answered by the measurement wearing plaid shirts because of an assumption
Dress Codes and Uniforms 1091

that plaid shirts were a gang symbol. However, students’ dress. For example, school officials
plaid shirts were a fashion trend that year, and it could compile statistics detailing students
was impossible to enforce the regulation. Some attacking other students for their expensive
dress items can be prohibited because of their clothes or jewelry (Sarke 1998).
potential to promote unhealthy practices, for In deciding whether dress code policies are
example, alcohol promotional items such as beer constitutional, courts are guided by tests derived
T-shirts (Workman 2003). Evidence of veracity of from Supreme Court decisions. Pure speech may
observational and anecdotal evidence is mixed but be restricted only if there is a risk of significant
given the difficulty of measuring effectiveness, disruption to learning. Dress may be considered D
such evidence should not be discounted (Volokh speech if the wearer intends to communicate a
and Snell 1998). Anecdotal evidence about deter- particular message, and it is likely others would
rents to school violence supports a ban on items understand the message (Killen 2000). Because of
such as Gothic clothing and Marilyn Manson in loco parentis and the status of schools as limited
T-shirts (Cohen 2000). School officials can show public forums, school officials are allowed rela-
a connection between certain items of dress and tively more regulation of speech than officials in
potential risks to health, safety, or morals by rely- other contexts (Murphy 1991).
ing on actual experience (e.g., anecdotal evi-
dence) or on opinions from experts (Lane
et al. 1994). Testimony by an authority can estab- Conclusions
lish a link between dress code policies and
increased safety, for example, “evidence satisfied If the past is any indication, policies regulating
the reasonable relationship standard, including student dress and appearance will likely encounter
testimony that the dress code reduced distractions legal challenges. When school dress code policies
to learning, increased campus safety, and leveled are written, the purpose for the regulations should
socioeconomic barriers” (Zirkel 2000, p. 79). be expressly stated. For example, “the board
Newspaper articles that report incidents in stated that its objective was to maintain order
schools can indicate relationships among risks to and decorum, avoid disruptions, maintain and
health, safety, or morality of the school population promote discipline, avoid distractions, prevent
due to particular dress and appearance symbols. In disturbances, contribute to the administration of
one school, hooded shirts and cargo pants with the classroom, and promote safety” (Ruggiero
pockets were prohibited because they provided a 2002, p. 719). Dress code policies can be designed
hiding place for school contraband, including as part of a school’s social structure to facilitate
weapons and cell phones (Breitman and Barker student growth into self-actualized adults – a way
2006). In another school, two girls were to assist adolescents to thrive by providing bound-
reprimanded for wearing Lycra and denim strap- aries and expectations for dress. Simultaneously,
less dresses that revealed too much cleavage positive boundaries and expectations for dress can
(Callas 2002). In both situations, students were counteract commercial messages and potentially
required to change to clothes the school provided help students develop a positive identity, effective
or to face being sent home. self-regulation, positive values, and social com-
School officials may believe certain items of petencies such as civil behavior.
clothing invite theft and the way to control risk of
theft is to regulate student dress; in response, they
establish dress code policies prohibiting students References
from wearing expensive clothing or jewelry.
However, it is insufficient for school officials to Alleyne, S., LaPoint, V., Lee, J., & Mitchell, H. (2003).
Black educators’ views on middle school students’
believe that student dress choices contribute to
dress and uniforms: Addressing the challenges from
theft; they must have evidence to show that the commercialism. Journal of Negro Education, 72(4),
risk of theft is at least partially attributable to 418–426.
1092 Dress Codes and Uniforms

Arnold, D., & Workman, J. E. (2003). The relationship Holloman, L. (1995). Violence and other antisocial behav-
between ownership of offensive t-shirts and problem iors in public schools: Can dress codes help solve the
behavior among high school students. Journal of Fam- problem? Journal of Family and Consumer Sciences,
ily and Consumer Sciences, 95(3), 32–39. 87(4), 33–38.
Baxter, K., & Marina, P. (2008). Cultural meaning and Holloman, L., LaPoint, V., Alleyne, S., Palmer, R., &
hip-hop fashion in the African-American male youth Sanders-Phillips, K. (1996). Dress-related behavioral
subculture of New Orleans. Journal of Youth Studies, problems and violence in public school setting: Preven-
11(2), 93–113. tion, intervention, and policy–A holistic approach.
Bell, N., & Bell, R. (1993). Adolescent risk taking. Journal of Negro Education, 65(3), 267–281.
Newbury Park: Sage. Irwin, C. (1993). Adolescence and risk taking: How are
Breitman, R., & Barker, O. (2006, August 17). Hoodies, they related? In N. Bell & R. Bell (Eds.), Adolescent
cargos aren’t going back to school; dress codes stress risk taking (pp. 7–28). Thousand Oaks: Sage.
safety. USA Today, p. 1D. Jin, X., Wah, B. W., Cheng, X., & Wang, Y. (2015). Sig-
Brunsma, D., & Rockquemore, K. (1998). Effects of stu- nificance and challenges of big data research. Big Data
dent uniforms on attendance, behavior problems, sub- Research, 2, 59–64.
stance use, and academic achievement. Journal of Killen, R. (2000). The Achilles’ heel of dress codes: The
Educational Research, 92(1), 53–63. definition of proper attire in public schools. Tulsa Law
Callas, T. (2002, November 19). New shirts not meant to be Journal, 36, 459–486.
hit with students; dress-code violators at Rancocas Lane, K., Swartz, S., Richardson, M., & VanBerkum,
Valley High in N.J. get sartorial shock. The Philadel- D. (1994). You aren’t what you wear. The American
phia Inquirer, p. B7. School Board Journal, 181, 64–65.
Cohen, M. (2000). A student profiling technique will not LaPoint, V., Holloman, L., & Alleyne, S. (1992). The role
effectively deter juvenile violence in our schools. New of dress codes, uniforms in urban schools. National
York Law School Journal of Human Rights, 17, 298–340. Association of Secondary School Principals Bulletin,
DaCosta, K. (2006). Dress code blues: An exploration of 76, 20–26.
urban students’ reactions to a public high school uniform LaPoint, V., Holloman, L., & Alleyne, S. (1993). Dress
policy. The Journal of Negro Education, 75(1), 49–59. codes and uniforms in urban schools. The Education
DelCampo, D., & DelCampo, R. (1998). Will stricter dress Digest, 58, 32–34.
codes improve the educational environment? In LaPoint, V., Alleyne, S., Mitchell, H., & Lee, J. (2003).
D. DelCampo & R. DelCampo (Eds.), Taking sides: Attitudes of youth of color on student dress and uni-
Clashing views on controversial issues in childhood forms: A case of commercialism in schools. Journal of
and society (pp. 262–263, 277). Guilford: McGraw- Negro Education, 72(4), 406–417.
Hill. LaVigne, C. (1999). Bloods, crips, and Christians: Fighting
DeMitchell, T. (2002). The duty to protect: Blackstone’s gangs or fighting the First Amendment? Baylor Law
doctrine of in loco parentis: A lens for viewing the Review, 51, 389–417.
sexual abuse of students. Brigham Young University Linn, S. (2003). Children and commercial culture:
Education and Law Journal, 1, 17–52. Expanding the advocacy roles of professionals in edu-
DeMitchell, T., Fossey, R., & Cobb, C. (2000). Dress codes cation, health, and human service. Journal of Negro
in the public schools: Principals, policies, and precepts. Education, 72(4), 478–486.
Journal of Law and Education, 29(1), 31–49. Miller, A. (2002). Balancing school authority and student
Erikson, E. (1997). The life cycle completed. New York: expression. Baylor Law Review, 54, 623–675.
Norton. Mitchell, H. W., & Knechtle, J. C. (2003). Uniforms in
Firmin, M., Smith, S., & Perry, L. (2006). School uniforms: public schools and the first amendment: A constitu-
A qualitative analysis of aims and accomplishments in tional analysis. Journal of Negro Education, 72(4),
two Christian schools. Journal of Research on Chris- 487–494.
tian Education, 15(2), 143–168. Morris, E. W. (2005). “Tuck in that shirt!” Race, class,
Freeburg, E. W., Workman, J. E., & Lentz, E. S. (2004). gender, and discipline in an urban school. Sociological
Rationale for dress codes: A review of school hand- Perspectives, 48(1), 25–48.
books. Journal of Family and Consumer Sciences, Murphy, P. (1991). Restricting gang clothing in public
96(1), 77–82. schools: Does a dress code violate a student’s right of
Gardner, W. (1993). A life-span rational-choice theory of free expression? Southern California Law Review, 64,
risk taking. In N. Bell & R. Bell (Eds.), Adolescent risk 1321–1362.
taking (pp. 66–83). Newbury Park: Sage. Murray, R. K. (1997). The impact of school uniforms on
Han, S. (2011). A mandatory uniform policy in urban school climate. NASSP Bulletin, 81, 106–112.
schools: Findings from the School Survey on Crime Myhra, A. (1999). No shoes, no shirt, no education: Dress
and Safety 2003-04. International Journal of Educa- codes and freedom of expression behind the postmod-
tional Policy and Leadership, 5(8), 1–12. ern schoolhouse gates. Seton Hall Constitutional Law
Herbon, B., & Workman, J. E. (2000). Dress and appear- Journal, 9, 337–400.
ance codes in public secondary school handbooks. Jour- Nishigai, M. (2001). From categorizing to balancing liberty
nal of Family and Consumer Sciences, 92(5), 68–76. interests in constitutional jurisprudence: An emerging
Drinking Game Playing: A Prevalent and Risky Activity Among Youth 1093

sliding-scale test in the seventh circuit and public school Educational Policy and Management. http://eric.
uniform policies. Wisconsin Law Review, 6, 1583–1617. uoregon.edu/publications/policy_reports/dress_codes/
O’Neal, G. (1998). Adolescent dress and risk taking. Fam- whynow.html
ily Science Review, 11(3), 190–204. Workman, J. E. (2003). Alcohol promotional clothing items
Ogle, J. P., & Eckman, M. (2002). Dress-related responses and alcohol use by underage consumers. Family and
to the Columbine shootings: Other-imposed and self- Consumer Sciences Research Journal, 31(3), 331–354.
designed. Family and Consumer Sciences Research Workman, J. E., & Freeburg, B. W. (2006). Safety and
Journal, 31(2), 155–194. security in a school environment: The role of dress
Paliokas, K. (2005). Dress codes as a means of reducing code policies. Journal of Family and Consumer Sci-
violence in public schools. Retrieved May 17, 2005, ences, 98(2), 19–24.
from http://www.edpolicy.gwu.edu/resources/chal Workman, J. E., & Freeburg, B. W. (2009). Dress and D
lenges/index.html#top society. New York: Fairchild.
Ruggiero, A. (2002). Mandatory public school uniforms: Workman, J. E., Freeburg, E. W., & Lentz-Hees, E. (2004).
Restricting rights or protecting pupils? Bridgeport Law Sanctions for dress code violations in secondary
Review/Quinnipiac Law Review, 21, 691–729. schools. Journal of Family and Consumer Sciences,
Sanchez, J. E., Yoxsimer, A., & Hill, G. C. (2012). Uni- 96(4), 40–46.
forms in middle school: Student opinions, discipline Workman, J. E., Webb, A., & Freeburg, B. W. (2011).
data, and school police data. Journal of School Vio- Message t-shirts and school safety. Journal of Family
lence, 11, 345–356. and Consumer Sciences, 103(3), 19–27.
Sarke, D. (1998). Coed naked constitutional law: The Yeung, R. (2009). Are school uniforms a good fit? Educa-
benefits and harms of uniform dress requirements in tional Policy, 23(6), 847–874.
American public schools. Boston University Law Zirkel, P. (2000). Dress codes. NASSP Bulletin, 84(612),
Review, 78, 153–175. 78–82.
Scales, P., Benson, P., Leffert, N., & Blyth, D. (2000).
Contribution of developmental assets to prediction of
thriving among adolescents. Applied Developmental
Science, 4(1), 27–46.
Schwartz, B. M., & Ewald, R. H. (1968). Culture and
society. New York: Ronald Press. Drinking Game Playing: A Prevalent
Shapiro, T. R., & Balingit, M. (2015, September 17). Virginia and Risky Activity Among Youth
high school students suspended for wearing Confederate
flag apparel. The Washington Post. Retrieved March Shannon R. Kenney1, Byron L. Zamboanga2,
6, 2016, from https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/
education/wp/2015/09/17/virginia-high-school-students- Justin F. Hummer3 and Joseph W. LaBrie4
1
suspended-for-wearing-confederate-flag-apparel/ Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior,
Skiba, R. (2001, July). Preventing school violence and Alpert Medical School of Brown University,
disruption: Alternatives to zero tolerance. Paper pre- Providence, RI, USA
sented to the meeting of the National Prevention Coa- 2
lition, National Mental Health Association, Alexandria. Department of Psychology, Smith College,
Studak, C. M., & Workman, J. E. (2007). Civil behavior, Northampton, MA, USA
3
safe-school planning, and dress codes. Journal of Fam- Department of Psychology, University of
ily and Consumer Sciences, 99(3), 23–28. Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Volokh, A., & Snell, L. (1998, January). School violence 4
prevention: Strategies to keep schools safe (Policy Loyola Marymount University, Los Angeles,
Study No. 234). Reason Public Policy Institute. CA, USA
Retrieved May 17, 2005, from http://www.rppi.org/edu
cation/ps234.html
Watkins, W. D., & Hooks, J. S. (1999). The legal aspects of
school violence: Balancing school safety with students’
rights. Mississippi Law Journal, 69, 641–713.
Weisenberger, C. (2000). Constitution or conformity: Overview
When the shirt hits the fan in public schools. Journal
of Law & Education, 29(1), 51–61.
Wellington, J. J., & Szczerbiński, M. (2007). Research Drinking games (DG) can be defined as a social
methods for social sciences. London: Continuum. drinking activity that consists of rules designed to
White, P., & Breckenridge, R. S. (2014). Trade-offs, limi- facilitate intoxication as participants perform
tations, and promises of big data in social science some type of cognitive and/or motor tasks
research. Review of Policy Research, 31(4), 331–338.
Why dress codes and why now? (n.d.). Eugene: University (Zamboanga et al. 2013). Given that the overarch-
of Oregon, College of Education, Clearinghouse on ing goal of DG is for participants to become
1094 Drinking Game Playing: A Prevalent and Risky Activity Among Youth

intoxicated in a relatively short period of time, blackouts, DUI, risky sexual behaviors, and alco-
participation typically leads to heavy episodic or hol dependence (e.g., Borsari et al. 2007; Hummer
binge drinking (four drinks in a row for females or et al. 2013; Nagoshi et al. 1994; Zamboanga
five drinks in a row for males) and high blood et al. 2010b).
alcohol concentrations (BACs), which in turn
increase the risk for negative consequences, such
as blacking out, sexual victimization, and driving Types of Games
under the influence (DUI) (see Zamboanga
et al. 2014a, 2016b for reviews). Drinking game Drinking games vary in structure, competitive-
involvement is also correlated with excessive ness, duration, requisite thinking and coordina-
drinking behaviors and other high risk activities tion, beverage choice, and risk for intoxication
including prepartying, risky sex, unsafe driving, (LaBrie at al. 2013; Zamboanga et al. 2007,
and drug use (e.g., Borsari et al. 2007, 2013; 2013). For example, verbal games (e.g., I
Fairlie et al. 2015; Hummer et al. 2013; Johnson Never), in which responses or questions control
and Stahl 2004; Nagoshi et al. 1994; Simons consumption, tend to rely on players’ capacity to
et al. 2005a, b; Zamboanga et al. 2010a). Research share information or remember rules, whereas
suggests that DG participation peaks in late ado- media games (e.g., Roxanne), in which words or
lescence (Sharmer 2005; Polizzotto et al. 2007) actions from television, movies, or music cue
and that a majority of adolescents matriculate into consumption, test players’ perception and listen-
college having already participated in DG ing skills (Borsari 2004). LaBrie and colleagues
(Borsari et al. 2003; Kenney et al. 2010). Despite (2013) systematically derived five categories of
the established prevalence and risk associated DG based on their rules and player behaviors.
with DG playing among adolescents, DG have Even competition games (e.g., Beer Pong, Flip
not been the focus of harm reduction interventions Cup), in which players or teams alternate turns,
or prevention efforts, and research on DG involve- were found to be the most popular among adoles-
ment among precollege adolescents is limited cents and young adults. Because these types of
(Zamboanga et al. 2016b). competitive DG are structured around specific
skill set goals (e.g., throwing a ping pong ball
into a cup of beer from across a ping pong
Introduction table), players are incentivized to force opponents
to drink as much as possible while retaining as
As early as the fifth century BC, ancient Greeks much personal visuospatial and motor functioning
enjoyed drinking “symposia” in which a “master as possible, in spite of inebriation. Targeted and
of drinking” was designated to predetermine both skills games (e.g., Quarters, Jeopardy) require
the potency and amount of wine to be consumed strategy and involve making others drink or
as well as impose penalties for violation of game avoiding drinking oneself, whereas communal
rules (e.g., failure to recite a passage of poetry or games have no official winner or loser; players
exhibit the dexterity to aim a drop of wine through simply agree on some rule-based action, phrase,
an intricate maze) (Garland 1982). Modern day or event that determine when and how much to
DG, however, are played predominantly by ado- drink (e.g., Never Have I Ever, Movie DG).
lescents and young adults and have evolved to Chance games (e.g., dice and playing card
include an estimated 500 types of DG, including games) involve simple rules but minimal skill.
Kings, Power Hour, and Quarters, that test Finally, extreme consumption games (e.g., chug-
players’ drinking endurance and speed as they ging, keg stands), which involve high volume
consume large amounts of alcohol in relatively drinking but are not rule-bound, are associated
short spans of time. The increasing popularity of with the highest levels of drinking and intoxica-
these hazardous drinking activities leads to esca- tion of all DG (LaBrie et al. 2013; Zamboanga
lated risk among youth, including hangovers, et al. 2007). Regardless of type, however, all DG
Drinking Game Playing: A Prevalent and Risky Activity Among Youth 1095

are governed by reversal of competence or the the cup must be reset and re-flipped. Only after the
notion that the longer the game is played, the first teammate is done flipping the cup success-
more inebriated and inept players become fully can the next person proceed. Whichever
(Green and Grider 1990). team finishes drinking and flipping all of their
As a testament to the perpetuation and evolu- respective cups is the winner.
tion of DG, in early 2014, an online drinking game Power Hour: With its variant Centurion
known as NekNominate (aka neck and nominate (or Century Club), Power Hour is a drinking
or neknomination) gained widespread popularity event in which every player consumes one shot
and attention due to its potentially lethal outcomes of beer every minute for 1 h. Each shot contains D
(Moss et al. 2015; Wombacher et al. 2016). In approximately 1.5 fluid ounces, for a total of 90 fl.
neknominate, a participant films him or herself oz. of consumption during the power hour. For
drinking in increasingly extreme circumstances purposes of blood alcohol content, this amount of
(e.g., chugging large quantities of high ethanol beer is roughly equivalent to 7.5 drinks. In the
content liquor, drinking alcohol out of a toilet), Centurion variant, beer shots are consumed 1/min
then nominates others to do the same within 24 h. for 100 min, roughly equating to 10 drinks. All
Four of the most popular DGs are described as players successfully completing the total number
follows: of shots without retiring are considered winners.
Beer Pong (Beirut): Beer Pong is a team- Quarters: Quarters can be played with any
based game in which players throw a ping pong number of players. All players sit around a table
ball across a table with the intent of landing the or other hard surface, with a single shallow glass
ball in a cup of beer on the other end of the table. placed in the middle. The goal is to try to bounce a
The game typically consists of two, two-player quarter off the table and into the glass. If success-
teams. Six to ten cups are set up on each side of ful, the player selects any other player to have a
the table, typically in a triangle formation. The drink and receives another turn. If the player fails,
goal of the game is to eliminate the other teams’ play passes to the next in the circle. If the player
cups before one’s own cups are eliminated. When successfully bounces a quarter into the glass three
a ball lands in a cup, which is generally filled with times in a row, this player may make a rule. As
2 to 3 oz of beer, that cup is eliminated and the with other DG, the rule can be anything that
defending team must consume all of the beer includes drinking as an outcome. For example,
inside that cup. Once a team has eliminated all nobody is allowed to say a chosen word, or every-
of the opposing cups (with the opponents having one must drink before each attempted bounce, or
consumed all the beer in the cups), the losing team perhaps no one is allowed to point with his or her
must also consume all the beer remaining in the fingers. If any rule is broken during the course of
winning team’s cups. play, the violator must consume alcohol.
Flip Cup (Cup): Flip Cup is also a team-based
drinking game in which two teams stand on oppo-
site sides of a table, facing one another. In front of Prevalence
each teammate is a plastic cup filled with a stan-
dard amount of beer (generally 2 to 4 oz.). At the Studies based on high school and matriculating
start of the race, the first member of each team college student samples show high rates of game
drinks his or her beverage. When finished, the cup playing among adolescents (Zamboanga et al.
is placed face up at the edge of the table, and the 2016b). In a study of 477 first-year college stu-
player who drank it attempts to flip the cup, by dents, 54% reported playing a drinking game dur-
setting it on the edge of the table and flicking or ing the last months of high school. Those students
lifting the bottom until it flips and lands face down who played a drinking game in their last month of
on the table, sitting on its mouth. The player may high school averaged nearly four game playing
not use two hands to help “guide” the cup to flip events per month and played DG during 44% of
over. If he or she is not successful on the first try, all high school drinking occasions (Kenney
1096 Drinking Game Playing: A Prevalent and Risky Activity Among Youth

et al. 2010). In another study of 1252 students et al. 2010; Pedersen and LaBrie 2006). These
attending precollege summer orientations, data findings coincide with trends in college women’s
compiled from three consecutive years revealed binge drinking that are increasingly converging
that 66% of females and 60% of males had played with those of male peers (O’Malley and Johnston
DG and, of these, approximately one in five 2002; Wechsler et al. 2002; Young et al. 2005).
reported weekly and nearly half reported monthly Although males appear to drink more while par-
or biweekly DG playing (Borsari et al. 2003). ticipating in DG than do females (Johnson and
Evidence suggests that initiating DG playing dur- Sheets 2004), both males and females report
ing high school is predictive of DG participation drinking significantly more during DG than dur-
in college (Borsari et al. 2003; Johnson ing other drinking situations (Johnson et al. 1998;
et al. 1998; Kenney et al 2010; Douglas 1987). Pedersen and LaBrie 2006).
Although heavy drinking and DG playing among In regard to race and ethnicity, the vast major-
college students has often been attributed to new- ity of research shows that DG are more prevalent
found freedom from parental monitoring and among White students compared to other racial-
immersion into collegiate social cultures that con- ethnic groups (Haas et al. 2012; Pedersen and
done and even revere unsupervised underage LaBrie 2006; Kenney et al. 2010; Zamboanga
drinking (Hingson et al. 2005; O’Malley and et al. 2015a, b). Moreover, event-level self-reports
Johnston 2002; Toomey et al. 2007; White indicate that among students participating in DG,
et al 2006; White and Jackson 2004), considerable non-White students, despite lower rates of partic-
evidence suggests that college drinking may pri- ipation overall, experience greater ensuing nega-
marily be an extension of preestablished high-risk tive consequences than White students (Pedersen
drinking behaviors, such as DG, that persist or and LaBrie 2006). These studies are limited given
intensify when students enter collegiate environ- that the ethnic minority student samples were
ments (Baer et al. 1995; Hersh and Hussong 2006; grouped into a single non-White category. In a
Leibsohn 1994; Wechsler et al. 1994). Regardless, recent study, (Zamboanga et al. 2015b) examined
DG are thought to act as ritualized drinking con- DG behaviors in a large, multisite ethnically
texts by which incoming college students are diverse sample of college students. The authors
socialized into heavy drinking peer cultures found that prevalence rates were highest among
(Adams and Nagoshi 1999). White students, followed by Hispanic and Asian
Although the majority of college students American students who did not differ from one
report past month DG participation, younger stu- another but had higher participation rates than
dents, particularly first-year students, have the Black students. Whether or not similar patterns
highest rates of DG involvement (Adams and of ethnic and racial differences exist in DG behav-
Nagoshi 1999. In fact, in a survey of 3830 student iors among current high school students and non-
drinkers from 58 US colleges, 73% of first-years college students is not entirely clear at the present
had played a DG in the past month as compared to time. Thus, representative samples with adoles-
38% of sophomores, juniors, and seniors (Engs cents and young adult populations are needed to
and Hanson 1993). In another study of male adequately assess the role of racial-ethnic status
and female undergraduates aged 18 to 25 years, and acculturation (i.e., the process of social, psy-
18- and 19-year-olds were five times more chological, and behavioral adaptation that unfolds
likely to engage in DG than older students when people from different cultural backgrounds
(Sharmer 2005). Although previously considered come in contact with each other; Zamboanga
a male-dominated activity (Borsari 2004; et al. 2014b), both of which are found to predict
Clapp et al. 2000; Engs et al. 1996; Johnson DG participation and risk (e.g., Schwartz
et al. 1998), recent research shows comparable et al. 2014; Zamboanga et al. 2016a).
rates of DG engagement among male and female Research indicates that playing DG represents
high school and college students (Borsari a social normative behavior in certain subgroups
et al. 2013; Engs and Hanson 1993; Kenney of students, namely, high school and collegiate
Drinking Game Playing: A Prevalent and Risky Activity Among Youth 1097

athletes (Borsari et al. 2013; Grossbard students reporting both prepartying and DG in
et al. 2007; Hummer et al. 2011; Lisha and the past 30 days experienced similar levels of
Sussman 2010 for review; cf. Zamboanga negative consequences as high schoolers
et al. 2008) and fraternity/sorority members reporting just DG participation.
(Sher et al. 2001; West 2001). Not only may However, in a large, multisite study examining
athletes and Greek-affiliated students value the event-specific data among heavier drinking col-
social and competitive nature of DG, but high lege students (Hummer et al. 2013), playing DG
rates of heavy drinking in these populations con- led to higher BACs and resulted in more negative
tribute to higher rates of high risk drinking activ- consequences in both prepartying and non- D
ities (e.g., Hummer et al. 2011). In fact, heavy prepartying contexts. However, whereas the
drinkers have been shown to be nearly seven heightened risk associated with game playing
times more likely to play DG than students not when prepartying impacted men and women sim-
classified as heavy drinkers (Sharmer 2005). ilarly with respect to BACs, the experience of
Nonetheless, it is important to note that college negative consequences was more pronounced for
students unaffiliated with formal athletics or female students. Therefore, playing DG while
Greek houses engage in DG and heavy drinking prepartying seems to be the riskiest combination
and experience negative consequences as a result for heavier drinking college women, as those who
of DG (Smith et al. 2005). Residential status engage in both drinking behaviors concurrently
appears to be a strong predictor of DG-related have more resultant problems than heavier drink-
risk. One study found that of students enrolled in ing women who preparty sans games.
college, those living away from home (i.e., on In another study of male and female college
campus) encountered significantly greater drink- athletes (Hummer et al. 2011), 36% reported
ing opportunities (i.e., gatherings with DG, alco- typically playing DG while prepartying. Individ-
hol, and intoxicated peers present) than those uals within the prepartying/DG group drank more
living with parents (White et al. 2008). Surpris- and experienced more negative consequences,
ingly however, no significant differences emerged both overall and while prepartying, than the
between college and same aged noncollege prepartying-only group and the nonprepartying
attending respondents’ drinking opportunities group. Perhaps most concerning are the specific
(White et al. 2008). Thus, despite lack of direct types of consequences experienced at a higher rate
data, it appears that DG may be quite common by the prepartying/DG group, relative to the other
among noncollege youth. two groups. These included blacking out, passing
Even though they consistently represent out, waking up in an unexpected place, regrettable
unique forms of high risk drinking contexts sexual situations, and increased tolerance. Taken
(Borsari et al. 2007; Kenney et al. 2010; Zambo- together, these findings indicate that engaging in
anga et al. 2010b), DG often occur as a form of DG while prepartying can lead to riskier drinking
prepartying (aka. front-loading, pregaming or pre- outcomes, especially among heavier drinking
funking; refer to the prepartying entry for addi- groups of college students and women.
tional information). Prepartying or pregaming can
be defined as “drinking (which may or may not
involve getting ‘buzzed’ or drunk) alone or with Motivations
people before going to an event or gathering
where more alcohol may or may not be con- Among youth, DG are believed to serve multiple
sumed.” (Zamboanga and Olthuis 2016, p. 954). functions; for example, they may offer contexts in
Still, DG have no temporal requirement and can which youth can conform to peer group behavior,
be played at any point during the course of the ease stress associated with social interaction, and
entire drinking occasion. Consistent with Kenney foster group camaraderie. Adolescents may
et al’s (2010) study of incoming college students, engage in drinking game playing due to a conflu-
Tomaso et al. (2015) found that high school ence of potential risk factors associated with the
1098 Drinking Game Playing: A Prevalent and Risky Activity Among Youth

developmental period. These include the Consequences


decreased salience of parents and increased
salience of peers, greater exposure to the model- The rapid intoxication associated with DG makes
ing of peer behavior, transition to adulthood and self-regulation appreciably more difficult and
orientation toward autonomy and non- negative consequences more likely. Studies have
conventional behaviors, and emphasis on drink- linked game playing to alcohol-related problems
ing as a social lubricant associated with fun in high school students (e.g., memory lapses,
(Jessor et al. 1991; Palmqvist et al. 2003; passing out, vomiting, and engaging in regrettable
Palmqvist and Santavirta 2006). High school stu- sex; Borsari et al. 2013) as well as college students
dents commonly endorse positive reinforcing (e.g., hangovers, illicit prescription drug use, mar-
social (e.g., “to be sociable”) and enhancement ijuana use, tobacco smoking, campus violations,
(e.g., “because I like the feeling”) motives for reliance on alcohol, sexual victimization, DUI,
participating in DG (Borsari et al. 2013; Tomaso and car accidents; Borsari et al. 2007; Johnson
et al. 2015; Van Tyne et al. 2012), which are and Stahl 2004; Nagoshi et al. 1994; Simons
associated with increased DG participation. et al. 2005a, b; Zamboanga et al. 2010a). Unfor-
College transitions marked by heavy alcohol tunately, engaging in DG during high school
consumption, increased drinking opportunities, appears to amplify both proximal and prospective
and unprecedented personal stressors (Hingson risk. Participating in DG during high school may
et al. 2002; White et al. 2006) are particularly predispose adolescents to risky high school
risk-enhancing for incoming students with prior drinking – game players consumed over 5 times
high school game playing experience as these more weekly drinks than nongame players and
students may be inclined to perceive heavy drink- 64% of game players engaged in binge drinking
ing as a means by which to ease social discomfort during typical drinking occasions – and college
and establish their identity amid unfamiliar peers. drinking (over and above high school consump-
However, considering the normative role that tion) and consequences (over and above high
heavy drinking plays in collegiate cultures, drink- school and college consumption) (Kenney
ing game playing in college may differ fundamen- et al. 2010). Not surprisingly, DG participation is
tally from precollege drinking game playing. also highly predictive of binge drinking in college
Considering that 55% of incoming college stu- populations. Event-level studies of drinking game
dents report playing DG to get drunk and half to occasions have evidenced that game players, com-
socialize or meet new people (Borsari et al. 2003), pared to nongame players, experienced an esti-
it is not surprising that DG have been embraced by mated 50% greater likelihood for binge drinking
this population. Self-reports have revealed that (Pedersen and LaBrie 2006) and significantly
college students most commonly play DG for higher BACs during DG events
social reasons, for instance, to act as a social (Clapp et al. 2008). Other studies support that
lubricant (Borsari 2004; Polizzotto et al. 2007; students experience negative consequences (e.g.,
Zamboagna et al. 2007). Moreover, perceived blacking out, passing out, vomiting) as a direct
positive expectancy effects (e.g., sociability or result of DG participation (e.g., Polizzotto
liquid courage) are positively associated with et al. 2007; Ray et al. 2014; Usdan et al. 2008).
drinking game participation in both high school Fueled by high rates of DG involvement, first-
and incoming college student samples (Borsari year undergraduates are found to face the greatest
et al. 2013; Haas et al. 2012; Nagoshi likelihood for adverse outcomes related to DG
et al. 1994; Zamboanga et al. 2005, 2010b, (e.g., Adams and Nagoshi 1999; Newman
2012). Players are likely to view getting drunk et al. 1991; Polizzotto et al. 2007; Sharmer
positively, take pride in their ability to drink 2005). This may be particularly important with
large quantities, and anticipate approval for regard to incoming students as risky drinking
heavy drinking (Green and Grider 1990; Newman may compromise successful negotiation of the
et al. 1991). transition to college and lead to problematic
Drinking Game Playing: A Prevalent and Risky Activity Among Youth 1099

drinking trajectories that may persist throughout change in DG participation post-intervention.


college (Task Force of the National Advisory Nonetheless, the overall mixed results indicate
Council on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism that more research is needed to explicate the
2002; Schulenberg et al. 2001). There is empirical extent to which DG participation is modifiable
evidence that relative to their male peers, females through intervention and what intervention strat-
experience more alcohol consequences after egies and formats may be most useful. Indeed, if
participating in DG, such as increased likelihood effective, incorporating information on DG into
for unprotected, unplanned, or unwanted sexual multicomponent alcohol interventions may be
intercourse, and date rape or sexual victimization seamless and cost-effective (see Larimer and D
(Johnson and Stahl 2004; Pedersen and LaBrie Cronce 2007 for review). Researchers have also
2006). Not surprisingly, simulated lab studies called for targeted initiatives, including increased
demonstrate that women achieve higher education about DG (Cameron et al. 2010), inter-
BACs than men while playing DG (Cameron ventions that provide feedback on peer norms and
et al. 2011; Correia and Cameron 2010). BACs related to DG (Cameron et al. 2010;
Women’s physiological susceptibility to intoxi- Silvestri et al. 2013), and enhanced supervision
cation and findings that some males use DG to in campus housing (Sharmer 2005). At the high
take sexual advantage of women (Johnson and school level, DG-specific information may sup-
Stahl 2004; Newman et al. 1991) point to the plement school-based educational, parental
urgent need to explicate this context-specific awareness, and peer social norms campaigns. At
risk behavior. the college level, DG -specific social norms or
harm-avoidance strategies (i.e., cognitive-
behavioral skills-based, personal feedback-
Prevention and Harm Reduction based, or motivational interventions) may be ben-
eficial. Indeed, college students tend to over-
Despite the established relationship between DG estimate the occurrence of DG playing among
participation and alcohol misuse and conse- their peers, and these overestimations are predic-
quences, few studies have examined the effect of tive of individual consumption and game playing
alcohol intervention—with and without DG con- rates (Pedersen and LaBrie 2008). Educating stu-
tent—on DG behaviors. Studies that have exam- dents about more moderate norms while provid-
ined BMI and DG-related outcomes have shown ing additional information, such as the risks
mixed results. While some trials demonstrate that associated with game playing, may positively
incoming students randomized to standard impact individuals’ decision making about
web-based (i.e., AlcoholEdu; Croom et al. 2009) whether and how often to participate in DG.
and brief motivational (Wood et al. 2010) inter- Still, the high prevalence and heightened risk
ventions report lower levels of DG participation associated with DG playing in adolescent
relative to controls, a recent dual-site trial found populations highlights the importance of early
reduced DG participation among students partic- high school or junior high school initiatives
ipating in Alcohol-Wise (Alcohol Education + geared to prevent game playing and reduce
eCHECKUP TO GO) at one site but not the harms associated with risky drinking before dan-
other (Croom et al. 2015). Recently, Fernandez gerous patterns may be established. In addition to
and colleagues (in press) examined the effect of harm reduction initiatives focused on DG, pro-
DG-specific feedback and discussion in two inde- viding opportunities for safer alternative social-
pendent trials (Trial 1, N = 198; Trial 2, N = 412) contextual activities may be advantageous by
of college alcohol BMIs, both of which demon- appealing to youth drawn to DG for social rea-
strated prior efficacy in reducing alcohol-related sons. For example, school-sponsored field trips
problems post-intervention (see Carey et al. 2009 or social events preceding athletic games or
and Borsari et al. 2012). Fernandez et al. found dances may offer proxies for socializing and
that in-session DG-related content did not predict networking.
1100 Drinking Game Playing: A Prevalent and Risky Activity Among Youth

Methodological Issues and Directions the role of game playing during this important
for Future Research developmental period. For instance, qualitative
studies or student focus groups may illuminate
The majority of research on DG has compared the extent to which socially learned peer group
students’ overall self-reported engagement in behavior or individual-level characteristics may
DG with overall alcohol consumption and conse- contribute to engagement in these high risk drink-
quences at the aggregate level. However, event- ing contexts and associated current and prospec-
level analyses that examine specific DG playing tive alcohol risks.
events with consumption and consequences are
needed to explicate the direct impact of DG
(e.g., Borsari et al. 2007; Clapp et al. 2003, Hum- References
mer et al. 2013; Pedersen and LaBrie 2006). For
example, assessing typical durations of game Adams, C. E., & Nagoshi, C. T. (1999). Changes over one
semester in drinking game playing and alcohol use and
playing events as well as gauging the differential
problems in a college student sample. Substance Abuse,
impact of drinks consumed before, during, and 20(2), 97–106.
after game playing would provide insight into Baer, J. S., Kivlahan, D. R., & Marlatt, G. A. (1995). High-
specific risks associated with this high risk con- risk drinking across the transition from high school to
college. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
text. Novel assessment methods, such as Ecolog-
Research, 19(1), 54–61.
ical Momentary Assessment (EMA; Stone and Borsari, B. (2004). Drinking games in the college environ-
Shiffman 1994), enable youth to report on feel- ment: A review. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Educa-
ings and behaviors in real-time and in natural tion, 48(2), 29–51.
Borsari, B., Bergen-Cico, D., & Carey, K. B. (2003). Self-
drinking environments using mobile devices. reported drinking-game participation of incoming col-
This innovative technology may be well suited lege students. Journal of American College Health,
for gathering context-specific information related 51(4), 149–154.
to DG. Finally, given the limitations of self-report Borsari, B., Boyle, K. E., Hustad, J. T. P., Barnett, N. P.,
Tevyaw, T. O., & Kahler, C. W. (2007). Drinking before
methods (e.g., memory recall bias) coupled with
drinking: Pregaming and drinking games in mandated
the notion that DG involve complex group students. Addictive Behaviors, 32(11), 2694–2705.
dynamics that may not be accurately assessed Borsari, B., Hustad, J. T. P., Mastroleo, N. R., Tevyaw, T.
using self-report measures (i.e., players may not O., Barnett, N. P., Kahler, C. W., . . . Monti, P. M.
(2012). Addressing alcohol use and problems in man-
always be aware of the social forces that occur
dated college students: a randomized clinical trial using
while playing), rigorous, and controlled experi- stepped care. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
mental studies on DG will also greatly advance chology, 80(6), 1062–1074. https://doi.org/10.1037/
our knowledge in this field (see Zamboanga and a0029902
Borsari, B., Zamboanga, B. L., Correia, C., Olthuis, J. V.,
Peake 2016) Van Tyne, K., Zadworny, Z.,. . . & Horton, N. J. (2013).
Further, most of what is known about DG is Characterizing high school students who play drinking
restricted to collegiate populations, thus leaving a games using latent class analysis. Addictive Behaviors,
void as it relates to younger adolescents and non- 38(10), 2532–2540.
Cameron, J. M., Heidelberg, N., Simmons, L., Lyle, S. B.,
college attending youth (see Zamboanga
Mitra-Varma, K., & Correia, C. J. (2010). Drinking
et al. 2016 for review). Given the prevalence and game participation among undergraduate students
prospective risk associated with DG playing attending National Alcohol Screening Day. Journal of
among precollege adolescents, future research American College Health, 58, 499–506.
Cameron, J. M., Leon, M. R., & Correia, C. J. (2011).
should further examine early and mid--
Extension of the simulated drinking game procedure
adolescents’ motivations for playing DG (e.g., to multiple drinking games. Experimental and Clinical
social facilitation, peer pressure, competition/ Psychopharmacology, 19, 295–302.
social camaraderie) as well as the peer cultures Carey, K. B., Henson, J. M., Carey, M. P., & Maisto, S. A.
(2009). Computer versus in-person intervention for
in which they are embedded. Research providing
students violating campus alcohol policy. Journal of
insight into adolescent’s predispositions toward Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 74–87. https://
risky drinking behaviors will help to better define doi.org/10.1037/a0014281
Drinking Game Playing: A Prevalent and Risky Activity Among Youth 1101

Clapp, J. D., Shillington, A. M., & Segars, L. B. (2000). alcohol related problems. Journal of Studies on Alcohol
Deconstructing contexts of binge drinking among col- and Drugs, 68(1), 97–105.
lege students. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Haas, A. L., Smith, S. K., Kagan, K., & Jacob, T. (2012).
Abuse, 26(1), 139. Pre-college pregaming: Practices, risk factors, and rela-
Clapp, J. D., Lange, J., Min, J. W., Shillington, A., John- tionship to other indices of problematic drinking during
son, M., & Voas, R. (2003) Two studies examining the transition from high school to college. Psychology
environmental predictors of heavy drinking by college of Addictive Behaviors, 26(4), 931–938.
students. Prevention Science, 4, 99–108. Hersh, M. A., & Hussong, A. M. (2006). High school
Clapp, J. D., Min, J. W., Shillington, A., Reed, M. B., & drinker typologies predict alcohol involvement and
Croff, J. K. (2008) Person and environment predictors psychosocial adjustment during acclimation to college.
of blood alcohol concentrations: A multi-level study of Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35(5), 738–751. D
college parties. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Hingson, R. W., Heeren, T., Zakocs, R. C., Kopstein, A., &
Research, 32(1), 100–107. Wechsler, H. (2002). Magnitude of alcohol-related mor-
Correia, C. J., & Cameron, J. M. (2010). Development of a tality and morbidity among U. S. college students ages
simulated drinking game procedure to study risky alco- 18-24. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63(2), 136–144.
hol use. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacol- Hingson, R., Heeren, T., Winter, M., & Wechsler, H.
ogy, 18, 322–328. (2005). Magnitude of alcohol-related mortality and
Croom, K., Lewis, D., Marchell, T., Lesser, M. L., Reyna, morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18-24:
V. F., Kubicki-Bedford, L.,. . .& Staiano-Coico, L. Changes from 1998 to 2001. Annual Review of Public
(2009). Impact of an online alcohol education course Health, 26, 259–279.
on behavior and harm for incoming first-year college Hummer, J. F., LaBrie, J. W., & Lac, A. (2011). Warming
students: Short-term evaluation of a randomized trial. up and staying loose: The prevalence, style, and influ-
Journal of American College Health Association, 57, ence of prepartying and drinking game behavior among
445–454. college student athletes. Athletic Insight, 13(2), 1–19.
Croom, K., Staiano-Coico, L., Lesser, M. L., Lewis, D. K., Hummer, J. F., Napper, L. E., Ehret, P. J., & LaBrie, J. W.
Reyna, V. F., Marchell, T. C.,. . . & Ives, S. (2015). The (2013). Event-specific risk and ecological factors asso-
glass is half full: Evidence for efficacy of Alcohol-Wise ciated with prepartying among heavier drinking college
at one university but not the other. Journal of Health students. Addictive Behaviors, 38, 1620–1628.
Communication, 20(6), 627–638. https://doi.org/ Jessor, R., Donovan, J. E., & Costa, F. M. (1991). Beyond
10.1080/10810730.2015.1012239 adolescence: Problem behavior and young adult devel-
Douglas, P. (1987). Bizz-buzz, turtles, quarters, and one opment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
horse club: The role of drinking games among high Johnson, T. J., & Sheets, V. L. (2004). Measuring college
school and college students. Alcohol Health and students’ motives for playing drinking games. Psychol-
Research World, 54–57, 92. ogy of Addictive Behaviors, 18(2), 91–99.
Engs, R. C., & Hanson, D. J. (1993). Drinking games and Johnson, T. J., & Stahl, C. (2004). Sexual experiences
problems related to drinking among moderate and heavy associated with participation in drinking games. Jour-
drinkers. Psychological Reports, 73(1), 115–120. nal of General Psychology, 131(3), 304–320.
Engs, R. C., Diebold, B. A., & Hanson, D. J. (1996). The Johnston, T. J., Wendel, J., & Hamilton, S. (1998). Social
drinking patterns and problems of a national sample of anxiety, alcohol expectancies, and drinking-game par-
college students, 1994. Journal of Alcohol and Drug ticipation. Addicitive Behaviors, 23, 65–79.
Education, 41(3), 13–33. Kenney, S., Hummer, J., & LaBrie, J. (2010). An exami-
Fairlie, A. M., Maggs, J. L., & Lanza, S. T. (2015). Pre- nation of prepartying and drinking game during high
partying, drinking games, and extreme drinking among school and their impact on alcohol-related risk upon
college students: A daily-level investigation. Addictive entrance into college. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
Behaviors, 42, 91–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. cence, 39, 999–1011.
addbeh.2014.11.001. LaBrie, J. W., Ehret, P. J., & Hummer, J. F. (2013). Are
Fernandez, A. C., Yurasek, A. M., Merrill, J. E., Miller, M. they all the same? An exploratory, categorical analysis
B., Zamboanga, B. L., Carey, K. B., & Borsari, B. (in of drinking game types. Addictive Behaviors, 38,
press). Do brief motivational interventions reduce 2133–2139.
drinking game frequency in mandated students? An Larimer, M. E., & Cronce, J. M. (2007) Identification,
analysis of data from two randomized controlled trials. prevention, and treatment revisited: Individual-focused
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. college drinking prevention strategies 1999-2006.
Garland, J. (1982). Greek drinking parties. History Today, Addictive Behaviors, 32, 2439–2468.
32, 18–21. Leibsohn, J. (1994). The relationship between drug and
Green, T. A., & Grider, S. A. (1990). Reversal of compe- alcohol use and peer group associations of college
tence in college drinking games. Play and Culture, freshmen. Journal of Drug Education, 24(3), 177–192.
3(2), 117–132. Lisha, N. E., & Sussman, S. (2010). Relationship of high
Grossbard, J., Geisner, I. M., Neighbors, C., Kilmer, J. R., school and college sports participation with alcohol,
& Larimer, M. E. (2007). Are drinking games sports? tobacco, and illicit drug use: A review. Addictive
College athlete participation in drinking games and Behaviors, 35, 399–407.
1102 Drinking Game Playing: A Prevalent and Risky Activity Among Youth

Moss, A. C., Spada, M. M., Harkin, J., Albery, I. P., on heavy drinking: A social norms perspective. Psychol-
Rycroft, N., & Nikcevic, A. V. (2015). ogy of Addictive Behaviors, 15(1), 42–51.
‘Neknomination’: Predictors in a sample of UK univer- Silvestri, M. M., Cameron, J. M., Borsari, B., & Correia,
sity students. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 1, 73–75. C. J. (2013). Examining alcohol and alcohol-free ver-
Nagoshi, C. T., Wood, M. D., Cote, C. C., & Abbit, S. M. sions of a simulated drinking game procedure. Journal
(1994). College drinking game participation within the of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 74, 329–336.
context of other predictors of alcohol use and problems. Simons, L., Klichine, S., Lantz, V., Ascolese, L., Deihl, S.,
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 8(4), 203–213. Schatz, B., & Wright, L. (2005a). The relationship
Newman, I. M., Crawford, J. K., & Nellis, M. J. (1991). between social-contextual factors and alcohol and
The role and function of drinking games in a university polydrug use among college freshman. Journal of Psy-
community. Journal of American College Health, choactive Drugs, 37(4), 415–424.
39(4), 171–175. Simons, L., Lantz, V., Klichine, S., & Ascolese,
O’Malley, P. M., & Johnston, L. D. (2002). Epidemiology L. (2005b). Drinking games, binge drinking and risky
of alcohol and other drug use among American college sexual behaviors among college students. Journal of
students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol Supplement, 63 Alcohol and Drug Education, 49, 23–36.
(Supplement 14), 23–39. Smith, M., Finneran, J., & Droppa, M. (2005). High risk
Palmqvist, R., & Santavirta, N. (2006). What friends are drinking among non-affiliated college students. Jour-
for: The relationships between body image, substance nal of Alcohol and Drug Education, 58(1), 28.
use, and peer influence among Finnish adolescents. Stone, A. A., & Shiffman, S. (1994). Ecological momen-
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 192–206. tary assessment (EMA) in behavioral medicine. Annals
Palmqvist, R. A., Martikainen, L. K., & von Wright, M. R. of Behavioral Medicine, 16, 199–202.
(2003). A moving target: Reasons given by adolescents Task Force of the National Advisory Council on Alcohol
for alcohol and narcotics use, 1984 and 1999. Journal Abuse and Alcoholism. (2002). A call to action:
of Youth and Adolescence, 32(3), 195–203. Changing the culture of drinking at U.S. colleges
Pedersen, E. R., & LaBrie, J. (2006). Drinking game par- (NIH publication No. 02-5010). Rockville: National
ticipation among college students: Gender and ethnic Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.
implications. Addictive Behaviors, 31(11), 2105–2115. Tomaso, C. C., Zamboanga, B. L., Haas, A. L., Olthuis,
Pedersen, E. R., & LaBrie, J. (2008). Normative misper- J. V., Kenney, S. R., & Ham, L. S. (2015). All it takes is
ceptions of drinking among college students: A look at one: Drinking games, prepartying, and negative drink-
the specific contexts of prepartying and drinking ing consequences among high school students. Journal
games. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 69, of Substance Use, 20, 77–84.
406–411. Toomey, T. L., Lenk, K. M., & Wagenaar, A. C. (2007).
Polizzotto, M. N., Saw, M. M., Tjhung, I., Chua, E. H., & Environmental policies to reduce college drinking: An
Stockwell, T. R. (2007). Fluid skills: Drinking games update of research findings. Journal of Studies on Alco-
and alcohol consumption among Australian university hol & Drugs, 68(2), 208–219.
students. Drug and Alcohol Review, 26(5), 469–475. Usdan, S., Martin, R., Mays, D., Cremeens, J., Weitzel,
Ray, A. E., Stapleton, J. L., Turrisi, R., & Mun, E. (2014). J. A., & Bernhardt, J. (2008). Self-reported conse-
Drinking game play among first-year college student quences of intoxication among college students: Impli-
drinkers: An event-specific analysis of the risk for cations for harm reduction approaches to high-risk
alcohol use and problems. The American Journal of drinking. Journal of Drug Education, 38, 377–387.
Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 40(5), 353–358. Van Tyne, K., Zamboanga, B. L., Ham, L. S., Othuis, J. V.,
Schulenberg, J., Maggs, J. L., Long, S. W., Sher, K. J., & Pole, N. (2012). Drinking motives as mediators of
Gotham, H. J., Baer, J. S., Kivlahan, D. R., Marlatt, the association between alcohol expectancies and risky
G. A., & Zucker, R. A. (2001). The problem of college drinking behaviors among high school students. Cog-
drinking: Insights from a developmental perspective. nitive Therapy and Research, 36, 756–767.
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, Wechsler, H., Davenport, A., Dowdall, G., & Moeykens,
25(3), 473–477. B. (1994). Health and behavioral consequences of
Schwartz, S. J., Zamboanga, B. L., Tomaso, C. C., Kondo, binge drinking in college: A national survey of students
K. K., Unger, J. B., Weisskirch, R. S., Ham, L. S., at 140 campuses. JAMA: Journal of the American
Meca, A., Cano, M. A., Whitbourne, S. K.,. . . & Medical Association, 272(21), 1672–1677.
Ravert, R. D. (2014). Association of acculturation Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Kuo, M., Seibring, M., Nelson,
with drinking games among Hispanic college students. T. F., & Lee, H. (2002). Trends in college binge drink-
The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, ing during a period of increased prevention efforts.
40(5), 359–366. Journal of American College Health, 50(5), 203–217.
Sharmer, L. (2005). Campus living arrangement as a risk West, L. A. (2001). Negotiating masculinities in American
factor for participation in drinking games among under- drinking subcultures. The Journal of Men’s Studies,
graduates. Journal of College and University Student 9(3), 371–392.
Housing, 33(2), 37–43. White, H. R., & Jackson, K. (2004). Social and psycholog-
Sher, K. J., Bartholow, B. D., & Nanda, S. (2001). Short- and ical influences on emerging adult drinking behavior.
long-term effects of fraternity and sorority membership Alcohol Research & Health, 28(4), 182–190.
Driving and Drivers 1103

White, H. R., McMorris, B. J., Catalano, R. F., Fleming, expectancies and risky drinking behaviors among high
C. B., Haggerty, K. P., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). school athletes: “I’d rather keep my head in the game”.
Increases in alcohol and marijuana use during the tran- Prevention Science, 13(2), 140–149.
sition out of high school into emerging adulthood: The Zamboanga, B. L., Pearce, M. W., Kenney, S. R., Ham,
effects of leaving home, going to college, and high L. S., Woods, O. E., & Borsari, B. (2013). Are ‘extreme
school protective factors. Journal of Studies on Alco- consumption games’ drinking games? Sometimes it’s a
hol, 67(6), 810–822. matter of perspective. The American Journal of Drug
White, H. R., Fleming, C. B., Kim, M. J., Catalano, R. F., & And Alcohol Abuse, 39(5), 275–279. https://doi.org/
McMorris, B. J. (2008). Identifying two potential 10.3109/00952990.2013.827202.
mechanisms for changes in alcohol use among Zamboanga, B. L., Olthuis, J. V., Kenney, S. R., Correia,
college-attending and non-college-attending emerging C. J., Van Tyne, K., Ham, L. S., & Borsari, B. (2014a). D
adults. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1625–1639. Not just fun and games: A review of college drinking
Wombacher, K., Renob, J. E., & Veilc, S. R. (2016). game research from 2004–2013. Psychology of Addic-
NekNominate: Social norms,social media, and binge tive Behaviors, 28(3), 682–695.
drinking. Health Communication, 22, 1–7. Zamboanga, B. L., Tomaso, C. C., Kondo, K. K., &
Wood, M. D., Fairlie, A. M., Fernandez, A. C., Borsari, B., Schwartz, S. J. (2014b). Surveying the literature on
Capone, C., LaForge, R., & Carmona-Barros, R. (2010). acculturation and alcohol use among Hispanic college
Brief motivational and parent interventions for college students: We’re not all on the same page. Substance
students: A randomized factorial study. Journal of Con- Use & Misuse, 49(8), 1074–1078. https://doi.org/
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 349–361. 10.3109/10826084.2014.887731.
Young, A. M., Morales, M., McCabe, S. E., Boyd, C. J., & Zamboanga, B. L., Iwamoto, D. K., Pesigan, I. A., &
D’Arcy, H. (2005). Drinking like a guy: Frequent binge Tomaso, C. C. (2015a). A ‘player’s’ game? Masculinity
drinking among undergraduate women. Substance Use and drinking games participation among White and
& Misuse, 40(2), 241–267. Asian American male college freshmen. Psychology
Zamboanga, B. L., & Olthuis, J. V. (2016). What Is pre- of Men & Masculinity, 16(4), 468–473. https://doi.
gaming and how prevalent Is It among U.S. college org/10.1037/a0039101.
atudents? An introduction to the special issue on pre- Zamboanga, B. L., Pesigan, I. A., Tomaso, C. C.,
gaming. Substance Use and Misuse, 51(8), 953–960. Schwartz, S. J., Ham, L. S., Bersamin, M.,. . . & Hurley,
https://doi.org/10.1080/10826084.2016.1187524. E. A. (2015b). Frequency of drinking games participa-
Zamboanga, B. L., & Peake, P. K. (2016). Moving the tion and alcohol-related problems in a multiethnic sam-
house party to the Lab: A call for experimental studies ple of college students: Do gender and ethnicity
on Drinking games. Manuscript under review. matter?. Addictive Behaviors, 41, 112–116. https://
Zamboanga, B. L., Bean, J. L., Pietras, A. C., & Pabon, doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.10.002
L. C. (2005). Subjective evaluations of alcohol expec- Zamboanga, B. L., Audley, S., Iwamoto, D. K., Tomaso,
tancies and their relevance to drinking game involve- C. C., Ramos, G., & Schwartz, S. J. (2016a). “What’s in
ment in female college students. Journal of Adolescent a game?” Acculturation and drinking game behaviors
Health, 37(1), 77–80. among Asian American young adults. Asian American
Zamboanga, B. L., Calvert, B. D., O’Riordan, S. S., & Journal of Psychology, 7(3), 195–204. https://doi.org/
McCollum, E. C. (2007). Ping-Pong, endurance, card, 10.1037/aap0000048.
and other types of drinking games: Are these games of Zamboanga, B. L., Tomaso, C. C., Cloutier, R. M.,
the same feather? Journal of Alcohol and Drug Educa- Blumenthal, H., Kenney, S. R., & Borsari, B. (2016b).
tion, 51(2), 26–39. Drinking game participation among high school and
Zamboanga, B. L., Rodriguez, L., & Horton, N. J. (2008). incoming college students: A narrative review. Journal
Athletic involvement and its relevance to hazardous of Addictions Nursing, 27(1), 24–31.
alcohol use and drinking game participation in female
college athletes: A preliminary investigation. Journal
of American College Health, 56(6), 651–656.
Zamboanga, B. L., Schwartz, S. J., Van Tyne, K., Ham,
L. S., Olthuis, J. V., Huang, S.,. . . &Weisskirch, Driving and Drivers
R. (2010a). Drinking game behaviors among college
students: How often and how much? The American Roger J. R. Levesque
Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 36(3), 175–179. Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
https://doi.org/10.3109/00952991003793869
Zamboanga, B. L., Schwartz, S. J., Ham, L. S., Borsari, B.,
& Van Tyne, K. (2010b). Alcohol expectancies, pre-
gaming, drinking games, and hazardous alcohol use in For several decades, motor vehicle crashes have
a multiethnic sample of college students. Cognitive constituted the greatest single public health threat
Therapy and Research, 34(2), 124–133.
Zamboanga, B. L., Ham, L. S., Olthuis, J. V., Martens, to adolescents in many countries. Compared to
M. P., Grossbard, J. R., & Van Tyne, K. (2012). Alcohol other age groups, adolescents are substantially
1104 Driving and Drivers

over represented in traffic accidents, including Simpson 2002). Decades of research have con-
traffic-related fatalities. In the United States, for cluded that driver education fails to reduce crash
example, adolescent drivers have the highest involvement among beginning drivers, and that
crash rate per mile driven of any age group they actually can produce adverse outcomes; yet,
(Shope and Bingham 2008). In 2004, drivers the programs still retain tremendous popular appeal
aged 16–20 accounted for 13.3% of the drivers as a means to improve driver safety (Williams and
in fatal crashes and 17.9% of the drivers in all Ferguson 2004). Among the most notable law
crashes even though they were only 6.3% of the enforcement methods has been those that increased
licensed drivers (Hedlund 2008). The youngest the national minimum drinking age to 21 and insti-
drivers are at highest risk for crashes, with their tuted more strict drunk-driving laws. Unlike other
rates of crashing consistently declining with each efforts, these have been shown to be widely suc-
year of increasing age per mile traveled. For cessful, for example, they have been deemed to
example, in 2000, drivers aged 16 years had account for a drop in traffic fatalities, down by
nearly three times as many crashes as did by 62% from 1982 to 2004 (Hedlund 2008). More
those of 19 years of age (Williams 2003). These recently, there have been efforts to control adoles-
crashes account for a large proportion of adoles- cents’ driving by enacting restrictions on adoles-
cents’ fatalities. In 2004, crashes accounted for cents’ driving. These more intermediate measures
41% of all deaths among teens aged 13–19 years are known as graduated drivers licenses (GDL).
in the US (Shope and Bingham 2008). These Some of the most common GDL approaches are
statistics are important to consider in light of those that limit learners’ permits for specified
those showing that other unintentional ▶ injuries periods and control them, who can drive with
accounted for 15% of all teen deaths, while homi- youth during those times. These latter approaches
cide accounted for 15% and suicide for another have been known to reduce crashes but, as seen
14% (Shope and Bingham 2008). Importantly, below, they do reveal more limited effects than
although adolescents can be more resilient than anticipated (Simons-Morton et al. 2008). Society,
adults, teens who survive crash related injuries policy makers, and researchers, then, all recognize
often experience significant deficits in quality of the need to address adolescents’ uneven risks asso-
life, and they do not recover preinjury function or ciated with their driving, and they have led to the
quality of life up to 2 years postinjury compared to development of some programs deemed effective.
national norms (Holbrook et al. 2007). The high Despite these efforts, the reality is that adolescents’
risks that adolescents face, and the highly negative driving remains marked by much higher negative
consequences of those risks rightly have led to outcomes than those of other age groups.
considerable societal interest and concern. Many reasons account for the varying success
Given the long history of traffic accidents of efforts aimed to reduce adolescents’ traffic
involving adolescents, several efforts have sought accidents. Stricter law enforcement regarding
to improve adolescents’ driving safety. Among the drinking alcohol and driving have been effective;
most popular approaches have been the use of but it is not clear how else more strict law enforce-
driver education, more aggressive law enforce- ment approaches can be effective. Despite its pop-
ment, and efforts to regulate adolescent’s actual ularity, strict law enforcement may not be as
driving. These efforts have increased in popularity, effective with adolescents, when compared with
although not all have been as successful in increas- other groups, in that it assumes that awareness
ing safety as was hoped. The most common pro- leads to behavior change to avoid consequences.
grams associated with adolescents are those The current understanding of adolescents leads to
involving driver education. These programs have the conclusion that they may not make calcula-
been deemed helpful to the extent that they have tions of costs and benefits in ways that would
been shown to increase both knowledge and skills; increase safety. Adolescents simply do not weigh
regrettably, they have not increased the number of benefits and costs the same way that adults do
safe drivers as much as expected (Mayhew and (e.g., driving passed the speed limit may make
Driving and Drivers 1105

sense when trying to impress peers) (Shope and drive more on weekends (Shope and Bingham
Bingham 2008). In addition, classroom instruc- 2008). Similarly, adolescents engage in risk behav-
tions also are not necessarily as effective as would ior related to accidents, such as the drinking of
be expected. Knowledge about a subject does not alcohol, in intermittent ways that place them at
necessarily lead adolescents to change behaviors higher risk. Most notably, adolescents may drink
or attitudes, as evidenced by research highlighting alcohol less than other age groups, but when they
the limitations of drivers education program do, they are at greater risk of accidents (Williams
(Williams and Ferguson 2004). Equally impor- 2003). As a last example, adolescents also are less
tantly, the graduated drivers licenses may not be likely to use seat belts, which increase the risk of D
as effective as hoped if they rely on parents, for injury and death (Williams et al. 2003). Adoles-
parents may not be the most effective models for cents, then, are likely to increase their risk of being
safe driving (Simons-Morton et al. 2008). involved in vehicle accidents due to factors that
What may be unusual about the above efforts is relate to increased risk and relate to the period of
that they developed from highly respected adolescence. Policies can influence some of these
research about the adolescent period. The study factors, but the point is that there are so many
of adolescence often does not influence policies so factors that place adolescents at risk that reducing
directly and quickly. Regrettably, it appears that some factors still leaves many others.
the efforts remain incomplete and limited. The That adolescents create other risk factors is
efforts are limited for at least two reasons. First, important to highlight, and several studies have
society still has an attachment to having adoles- shown links among several factors and adoles-
cents learn how to drive in their mid teen years. cents’ driving behavior, crashes, and traffic
Second, despite all of the efforts to limit and offenses. Adolescents are more likely to engage
control adolescents’ actual driving, the reality is in common risky driving behaviors. These include
that these methods remain incomplete to the speeding, making illegal lane changes, driving too
extent that they do not take into account the closely to other vehicles, failing to yield the right
more complex skills and behaviors that must be of way, failing to perceive hidden risks, driving
performed in the presence of several risk factor. while drowsy or sleepy, and being more suscepti-
The programs have reduced some risk factors, but ble to distractions from passengers, phones, eat-
the number of risk factors that remain when ado- ing, drinking, and a host of other distractions
lescents are permitted to drive remains high. This (Shope and Bingham 2008). In addition, of
is problematic to the extent that, as the number of course, adolescents are less experienced drivers.
risks increases, so does the chance for accidents. The lack of experience contributes to driving
Some of the reasons that adolescents are more errors simply because the acquisition of any com-
likely to be involved in crashes have to do with the plex set of skills requires a significant amount of
nature of adolescence itself. For example, one of time. All novice drivers, adolescents included,
the key factors contributing to an increase in ado- necessarily learn from their errors and progress
lescents’ crash risk is passengers. Since cars are an in driving more safely relies on receiving feed-
important mode for socializing, adolescents often back from their driving. These errors and feed-
have passengers. Adolescents often travel in motor back are necessary for adolescents and other
vehicles with other adolescents, and given that learners to automatize the processes involved in
adolescents often drive with passengers and drive safe driving. Thus, the point is that all drivers need
less safely than adults, it is not surprising that to drive to learn how to drive safely; there is no
adolescents are at greater risk when they drive way around the need to develop expertise that
together. This greater risk due to driving with must be time-intensive and involve much practice.
peers appears unique to adolescents; indeed, the It is important to emphasize that adolescent
risk has the obverse effect on adults (Williams novice drivers generally are not like other novice
2003). In addition, adolescents also are at increased drivers. Research on brain development makes
risk of crashes because they drive more at night and this point rather clear. A gap exists between
1106 Driving and Drivers

physical maturity (largely complete by the time differently and not necessarily view risks as poten-
adolescents reach driving age) and mental and tially relevant to themselves. This is important in
cognitive maturity (incomplete until a decade that there appears to be groups of adolescents who
later) (Dahl 2008). This gap helps account for weigh the benefits of risk differently than others.
risk-taking behavior and other social-interaction Harre (2000) offered one of the most important
problems. What this translates into for adolescents models to highlight this issue. By combining
is their need to test their limits and abilities, seek notions of perceived risks and perceived benefits,
their identities as individuals, and evolve in their Harre generated five adolescent risk-approach
relationships with peers. In terms of driving, what “types” arrayed on two dimensions: objective
this means is that the development of the capacity crash risk (high or low) and perceived crash risk
to make decisions, which may be just as good as (high or low). For example, adolescents high on
that of adults, may be less of a factor in high-risk both dimensions are typical thrill seekers while
driving than in other areas of development. those who are habitually cautious drivers are low
Despite efforts to limit whom adolescents drive on both dimensions. There does seem to be a type
with, the reality is that peers can affect driving by and disposition toward risk and accidents, but it is
being drivers and passengers in other cars on the important to keep in mind context and its influence.
road and they also may influence driving by being Even considering context, though, it is reasonable
pedestrians or others not in cars, perhaps in con- to accept, for example, that some adolescents may
tact via cell phones or text messages. be temperamentally disposed to accept high risk,
Although there is a focus on adolescents as a which they accurately judge, because they are
group at risk, it is important to emphasize that active risk seekers. For adolescents, then, perceived
some adolescents are more at risk than others. benefits play an equal and important role in engag-
Important individual differences increase the ing in risk behavior, and the weight given to ben-
risk. For example, males are more at risk for traffic efits and risks seems to be different for adolescents
accidents, just as those who are more thrill seekers when compared to other groups as well as adoles-
are at higher risk (Shope et al. 2003). How ado- cents compared to one another.
lescents’ parents have served as role models also One of the most remarkable points to emerge
matters. How parents drive, and how they social- in efforts to reduce adolescents’ traffic-related
ize their adolescents’ driving, strongly influence accidents and their negative effects is that efforts
adolescents’ driving; adolescents’ driving is sim- have sought to benefit from the understanding of
ilar to that of their parents (Taubman-Ben-Ari the adolescent period to develop more effective
et al. 2005). These points are critical to keep in policies and other social responses. The contin-
mind. The limitations of relying on parents, for ued high rates of traffic accidents reveal that
example, are enormous. For that reason, it turns current efforts remain limited. When compared
out that the management practices of many par- to adult drivers, adolescents are at considerable
ents may not be sufficient to provide the safety excess risk of vehicle crashes. This excess risk
effects that current policy trends have tried to continues despite important successes in
encourage. Although GDL policies tacitly recog- addressing adolescents’ abilities to drive more
nize the importance of parental involvement in safely. Researchers have identified many factors
teaching and managing novice teenage drivers, that lend themselves to control, such as driving
parents are not systematically prepared to take with peers, accessing alcohol, and driving at
on the role (Simons-Morton et al. 2008). night. Regrettably, a high number of other fac-
Another important example of the need to con- tors, and their combinations together, increase
sider individual differences among adolescents adolescents’ risk of vehicle accidents. These
comes from research examining ▶ Risk-Taking studies and statistics reveal truly sobering
and understanding risk. Adolescents may not be results, and they highlight the need to demon-
at a particular disadvantage when it comes to strate the effectiveness of measures assumed to
understanding risk; but they may value benefits curb adolescents’ vehicle accidents.
Drug Sellers and Distributors 1107

References Hanneman 1974; Centers and Weist 1998;


Moore 1995). Typically, drug sales occur within
Dahl, R. D. (2008). Biological, developmental, and a complex drug economy that transports drugs
neurobehavioral factors relevant to adolescent driving
from producers to users through interpersonal net-
risks. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 35
(3 S), S278–S284. works that operate based on fragile relationships
Harre, N. (2000). Risk evaluation, driving, and adolescents: (Atkyns and Hanneman 1974; Centers and Weist
A typology. Developmental Review, 20, 206–226. 1998; Moore 1995). The success of this distribu-
Hedlund, J. (2008). Teen driving. American Journal of
tion system requires a great number of people to
Preventive Medicine, 35(3 S), S258–S260.
work together against the narcotics officials D
Holbrook, T. L., Hoyt, D. B., Coimbra, R., Potenza, B.,
Sise, M. J., Sack, D. I., et al. (2007). Trauma in adoles- attempting to expose and stop distributors.
cents causes long-term marked deficits in quality of Research on adult drug sellers and distributors
life: adolescent children do not recover preinjury qual-
and studies of gangs shed light on the drug econ-
ity of life or function up to two years postinjury com-
pared to national norms. Journal of Trauma: Injury, omy and the organizational roles within this econ-
Infection, and Critical Care, 62, 577–583. omy (Aguilar and Jackson 2009; Dunlap et al.
Mayhew, D. R., & Simpson, H. M. (2002). The safety 2004; Moore 1995). This body of knowledge
value of driver education and training. Injury Preven-
suggests that drug distributing organizations are
tion, 8(2), ii3–ii7.
Shope, J. T., & Bingham, C. R. (2008). Teen driving: Motor- complex, members maintain tenuous relation-
vehicle crashes and factors that contribute. American ships, and some organizations have transnational
Journal of Preventive Medicine, 35(3 S), S261–S271. networks. Law enforcement officials often use the
Shope, J. T., Raghunathan, T. E., & Patil, S. M. (2003).
tenuous relationships within the drug economy to
Examining trajectories of adolescent risk factors as
predictors of subsequent high-risk driving behavior. progress from arrests of street-level sellers to
The Journal of Adolescent Health, 32, 214–224. larger distributors or wholesalers. Unfortunately,
Simons-Morton, B. G., Ouimet, M. C., & Catalano, R. F. a deeper description of the drug economy
(2008). Parenting and the young driver problem. Ameri-
is beyond this discussion; however, several
can Journal of Preventive Medicine, 35(3 S), S294–S303.
Taubman-Ben-Ari, O., Mikulincer, M., & Gillath, O. (2005). researchers have described different facets of this
From parents to children – similarity in parents and type of system (cf. Adler 1993; Bourgois 2002;
offspring driving styles. Transportation Research Part Hoffer 2005; LeBlanc 2004). The basic structure
F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 8, 19–29.
of drug distribution organizations does provide a
Williams, A. F. (2003). Teenage drivers: Patterns of risk.
Journal of Safety Research, 34, 5–15. context for the roles adults and youth occupy in
Williams, A. F., & Ferguson, S. A. (2004). Driver educa- these settings.
tion renaissance? Injury Prevention, 10, 4–7. Much of what is known about drug sellers in
Williams, A. F., McCartt, A. T., & Geary, L. (2003). Seatbelt
general and adolescent drug dealers, in particular,
use by high school students. Injury Prevention, 9, 25–28.
is derived from incarcerated populations or data
gathered from secondary sources (Atkyns and
Hanneman 1974; Moore 1995). In the last
Drug Sellers and Distributors 15 years, some researchers published qualitative
studies of adolescent self-identified drug sellers
Jemel P. Aguilar and this knowledge amplified what researchers
Center for Social Work Research, Austin School know about adolescents’ involvement in the
of Social Work, University of Texas at Austin, drug economy (Centers and Weist 1998; Moore
Austin, TX, USA 1995). These studies integrated qualitative tech-
niques that require prolonged engagement, using
community representatives to engage in data col-
Criminological and social science research indi- lection, and other means to explore active drug
cates that some adolescents engage in the sale or users’ lives and activities. This essay makes uses
distribution of illicit substances including mari- this body of research, regardless of how it is
juana, cocaine, crack, and methamphetamine collected, to understand adolescent drug sellers.
(Aguilar and Jackson 2009; Atkyns and Additionally, this essay outlines the ways in
1108 Drug Sellers and Distributors

which adolescents enter into and the roles adoles- Dunlap et al. 2004; Jacinto et al. 2008; Joseph and
cents occupy in the drug economy. Next, the ado- Pearson 2002). Some researchers describe low-
lescents’ roles are then linked with violence using income, high unemployment neighborhoods as
Goldstein’s (1985) tripartite model of the drugs- closed in that few residents are able to develop
violence nexus. Goldstein’s model helps to frame the social and economic resources outside the
the different initiators and outcomes associated neighborhood to aid them in escaping from these
with drug use or selling. The essay ends with a areas. For example, Dunlap et al. (2007) studied
discussion of the implications of drug selling the social forces and individual consequences
research for adolescent drug treatment and policy. associated with drug selling in post-Katrina New
For the purposes of this essay, drug selling refers Orleans and found that “illegal activities, both
to the marketing of illicit substances, while drug before and after Hurricane Katrina, are increas-
distribution refers to the activities of individuals ingly the only viable micro-level methods of
that both sell small amounts of illegal drugs and acquiring money” (p. 116). Consequently, with
provide other sellers with illegal drugs that they few resources available to them and the tightening
can market for profit (Aguilar and Jackson 2009; job market limiting what is available for employ-
Centers and Weist 1998; Moore 1995). This essay ment, adolescents are getting an early and easy
does not include research describing the market start in the illicit drug market. Thus, some adoles-
for prescribed drugs such as Xanax, or Adderall. cents certainly are entering into the drug economy
Although the findings from the illicit drugs may for economic or money gain.
apply or shed light on the sale of prescription Adolescents who join illicit drug markets
drugs, this essay instead focuses on other drugs for economic gain may access the drug market
involved in the drug market. through friends or family members already
involved in this economy (Dunlap et al. 2007).
Several researchers are documenting how these
Pathways into the Drug Economy adolescents typically grow up in families and
neighborhoods teeming with drug abuse, selling,
Researchers interested in drug selling and distri- and distribution. These adolescents, however, do
bution find that some adolescents enter into the not simply “join up”; instead, they are mentored
drug economy because of the limited social and into the illicit drug economy in minor, highly
economic opportunities available to them monitored roles. At first, adolescents may act as
(Aguilar and Jackson 2009; Anderson 2005; Cen- go-betweens for drug sellers, a role that asks them
ters and Weist 1998; Dembo et al. 1993; Dunlap to act as lookouts and warn sellers of a police
et al. 2007; Jacinto et al. 2008; Joseph and Pearson presence in the neighborhood, as runners who
2002; Moore 1995; Sales and Murphy 2007). In transport drugs or money from the distributor to
the last 10 years, researchers and journalists sellers in the network, or as those who hold the
documented how job flight, the movement of drugs for a seller working in specific area and
employment out of the inner city into suburban bring the amount needed for sale to the seller
areas, is negatively affecting the employment when the buyer and seller are ready. Initially,
opportunities of African American men and new drug sellers will operate their business
young boys. Job flight, coupled with the overrep- under the tutelage of a more experienced drug
resentation of African American men in the seller in that they sell a portion of the senior
criminal justice system, is creating almost insur- drug seller’s supply, keep a percentage of the
mountable barriers to employment for African profits, and then are able to purchase some addi-
American adult men and boys. Thus, with diffi- tional drugs for their own use or for sale. As the
culties obtaining gainful and legal employment relationship between the senior and junior drug
many African American youth are entering into seller grows, the senior drug seller takes on the
the drug economy as a means to gain an income role of distributor by obtaining a supply of drugs
(Aguilar and Jackson 2009; Dunlap et al. 2007; from the wholesaler, using a stash house to
Drug Sellers and Distributors 1109

prepare the drug for market, and coordinating the Distributors typically operate out of stash houses.
sale of the drug to lower-level drug sellers. Junior Stash houses are temporary places where distrib-
drug sellers typically are the sales force for the utors can prepare large quantities of illicit drugs
senior drug sellers and are engaged in the day-to- for retailers to sell. Stash houses often are secret
day sales and interactions with customers. Senior locations maintained by drug organizations and
distributors also may involve other adolescents may be run by individuals or family members who
and youth in drug sales through different roles occupy different roles within the drug enterprise.
that are necessary to operate such an enterprise. Natarajan and Belanger’s typology of roles is a
useful framework for understanding the broadly D
defined positions that anyone may occupy on the
Roles in the Illicit Drug Economy selling side of the drug economy.
Additional research studies on adolescent
Researchers identified the roles that youth occupy populations and in drug-using venues have dis-
in the drug economy that go beyond the stereo- covered more specific roles and activities that
typical street-based seller. These roles are enacted drug selling youth occupy. For example, the
within a context such as a rave or stash house, retailer role has been found to involve many
identify the drug sellers’ relationships to their roles. Several researchers (Jacinto et al. 2008;
customers, and position the drug sellers within Joseph and Pearson 2002; Werb et al. 2008) sug-
the overall drug economy. Essentially, roles in gest that adolescent retailers can be grouped into
the drug economy are an important aspect of three types: facilitators, dealers, and user-sellers.
socializing someone to their status in the network Although adolescents may drift in and out of these
and the expectations associated with that status roles, the typology helps further explain some of
(Adler 1993; Bourgois 2002; Hoffer 2005; the pathways into drug selling, the settings sellers
LeBlanc 2004). Natarajan and Belanger’s (1998) will access, and some of the associated risky
study of drug distributing organizations identifies behaviors that sellers will engage in. Facilitators
general roles and responsibilities embedded are peripherally linked with drug distribution net-
within drug distribution organizations. These works and use these relationships simply to obtain
roles are worth highlighting. drugs (Adler 1993; Bourgois 2002; Feigelman
Natarajan and Belanger’s model identifies et al. 1993; Sales and Murphy 2007). Facilitators
three roles: retailer, broker, and distributor. The typically sell lesser quantities of illicit substances
retailer role is the one most commonly associated and their primary customer base is close friends
with the drug economy in that a number of youth (Jacinto et al. 2008; Joseph and Pearson 2002).
are arrested for drug-related crimes because they Jacinto et al.’s (2008) study of ecstasy sellers
are acting as the street-based vendor for a drug indicates that most sellers began using ecstasy
distributor (Centers and Weist 1998; Moore and then started selling the drug to friends within
1995). Natarajan and Belanger define retailers as the first year after use. Most of the sellers they
those individuals who sell small quantities of interviewed did not intend to become drug sellers;
illicit substances in public settings. Many times, instead, these adolescents described initially
retailers will obtain their illicit substances from assisting their friends acquire the drug (i.e.,
brokers. Brokers are those individuals who act as being the middleperson between a consumer and
representatives of distributors and provide seller). Jacinto and her colleagues also found that
retailers with the drugs to sell in the public sphere. adolescents in the “middleperson” role do not
Distributors are those who manage large quanti- necessary view the drug selling as necessarily
ties of drugs received from traffickers, prepare criminal. According to their findings “real
drugs by “cutting” or introduce additional com- dealers” can be identified by the level of drug
ponents to lessen the purity of the drug and sales because real dealers had to contend with
thereby increase the amount available to sale, the associated stigma of the dealer label, drug
and package the drugs for distribution to retailers. distribution was a full-time occupation, and
1110 Drug Sellers and Distributors

some facilitators do not make a significant profit behaviors (Aguilar and Jackson 2009; Bourgois
from their selling. Because freelancers invest in 2002; Brunelle et al. 2000; Feigelman et al. 1993;
drug selling relationships as a way of maintaining LeBlanc 2004; Li et al. 2001).
a convenient supply of illicit substances for their The final type of adolescent drug seller is a
friends, freelancers’ business practices different drug-addicted adolescent (Joseph and Pearson
from many other drug sellers in that they are less 2002). These adolescents use in connection with
likely to create elaborate and formal business their selling and attempt to subsidize or cover the
structures and engage in the practice of providing costs of their drug usage by selling. Brunelle
drug samples and advice for maximizing the et al.’s (2000) examination of Goldstein’s tripar-
“drug experience” (Jacinto et al. 2008, p. 420). tite model of drug-violence nexus elaborates on
While many youth present themselves as drug the drug user-seller type. In this study, Brunelle
sellers, some youth selling illicit drugs reject the et al. found that viewing drug-using dealers from
identity of drug seller (Aguilar and Jackson 2009; an economic perspective does not quite capture
Joseph and Pearson 2002; Moore 1995). For youth’s motivations to engage in drug selling.
example, the respondents in Jacinto et al.’s They found that drug-using youth have regular
(2008) study discussed above rejected the second contact with drug sellers and, through these con-
type of drug distributor identity, Dealer. Dealers tacts, may become increasingly involved in the
are adolescents who sell large quantities of illicit drug economy to support their drug habits. Earn-
drugs, offer a variety of drug types to their cus- ing money through the drug economy also permits
tomer base (i.e., marijuana, crack, methamphet- youth to hide their income from parents and/or
amine, cocaine), and use their income from drug law enforcement in that their earnings are not
sales to support themselves (Joseph and Pearson officially documented (as in the form of a pay-
2002). In other words, their profit margin from check stub) and/or evidence of their spending is
drug sales becomes an important aspect of their not recorded. Until drug using–selling youth
survival. As one of Jacinto et al.’s participants come to the attention of law enforcement or their
stated “. . .a dealer is out there pushing. That’s parents, most likely through their crime-related
how they get paid. If that’s your only source of behaviors, their drug selling can mask their
income and that’s what you do, you’re a dealer” involvement in selling and drug use and their
(p. 432). Thus, using drug distribution to support drug use can mask their drug selling (Brunelle
oneself is a significant differentiator of small or et al. 2000). Moreover, drug-addicted drug sellers
large drug sellers. are more likely to be accepted by their friendship
In contrast to Natarajan and Belanger’s (1998) networks because the seller peddles their drugs to
typology of drug seller and distributor roles, the friends and their friends are also more likely to be
research literature suggests that some adolescent a part of the illicit drug economy (Flom et al.
dealers act as retailers and brokers. For adolescent 2001; Jacinto et al. 2008; Joseph and Pearson
drug distributors, moving large quantities of drugs 2002). This type of drug seller is under-
into the drug economy brings with it some aspects researched, most likely because they “fly under
to their work that does not exist with adolescent the radar” of both substance abuse services and
facilitators. Stash houses are one aspect of the law enforcement.
large-scale distributor identity. Stash houses pro-
vide drug distributors a place to keep their drugs
while they are being prepared for sale. Adoles- Gangs and Selling Roles
cents who sell larger quantities are more deeply
involved in the drug economy in that they are Valdez and Sifaneck’s (2004) research on gangs
depending on their drug distribution for their and the illicit drug economy suggests a typology
income, more likely are employing others to dis- of drug seller and distributor roles in relation to
tribute their illicit substances, may sell multiple the gang with which these individuals are affili-
substances, and are engaged in several risk ated. Valdez and Sifaneck outline four different
Drug Sellers and Distributors 1111

types of drug selling gang members: homeboys, violent crime (Aguilar and Jackson 2009;
hustlers, slangers, and ballers. Homeboys are Bourgois 2002; Brunelle et al. 2000; Feigelman
user-seller gang members that are a part of street et al. 1993; LeBlanc 2004; Li et al. 2001). Without
gangs and operate their illicit businesses outside doubt, drug use links to violence.
the gang network. Similar to seller-users, home- Paul Goldstein (1985) has outlined a tripartite
boys typically buy illicit drugs for both their own model of the drug violence nexus that provides a
consumption and sale. Alternatively, hustlers sell conceptual framework for understanding not only
drugs from within the gang network, meaning that drug use and violence but also drug selling and
they affiliate their drug sales with their gang even violence. Goldstein offers three models that link D
though the gang’s purpose is not to distribute violence and the drugs. These are the pharmaco-
drugs. Unlike homeboys, hustlers are well inte- logical model, the economic-compulsive model,
grated into their gang network, benefit from the and the systemic model. The pharmacological
protection of the gang, and use the gang’s social model focuses on the biological changes that
networks to increase their sales. Slangers are the occur from using substances or during withdrawal
third type of gang-related drug sellers (Valdez and (Goldstein 1985). The economic-compulsive
Sifaneck 2004). Slangers are a part of gang net- model links a person’s aggressive and violent
works that sell and distribute illicit substances, use tendencies with his/her need to obtain money for
drugs, and return the profits from drug sales back a supply of drugs. Several studies indicate that
to the gang network. Akin to the dealer type, youth commit serious and violent crimes while
slangers are typically a part of well-organized using illicit substances (Bourgois 2002; Brunelle
networks that traffic large shipments of illicit et al. 2000; LeBlanc 2004). Systemic violence is
drugs for distribution. Similar to slangers, ballers the last model posited by Goldstein. Systemic
are a part of well-organized gang units that dis- violence refers to the typically aggressive interac-
tribute illicit substances, except ballers’ gang tions between people in the drug economy. Unlike
units are much larger. Ballers are more likely to the pharmacological and economic-compulsive
be connected to large distribution networks that models discussed earlier, the systemic model is
maintain a hierarchy and move large quantities of much more difficult to document and study
illicit drugs in and out of the network. Similar to because individuals in the drug economy are less
the other types of gang-related drug sellers, ballers likely to report violence committed by or against
benefit from their gang members in that they have them as developing from their participation in the
protection from rivals and gain an income from drug economy. Researchers such as Brunelle et al.
their drug sales. (2000) are applying Goldstein’s framework to
samples of adolescent drug sellers and users. In
Brunelle et al. (2000), the authors found that
Drug Selling Youth and Violence youth report using substances to commit crimes
or avoid negative feelings that develop from com-
From a review of the roles that adolescents occupy mitting the crime, or enhance other feelings asso-
in the drug selling economy, it becomes clear that ciated with the crime. Moreover, Sales and
youth engage in a range of activities that involve Murphy’s (2007) study of ecstasy using drug
the selling and distribution of illicit substances, sellers indicates that these youth also often will
that not all sellers are alike or involved in drug use drugs while selling.
sales for similar reasons, and that their immersion
in the drug economy differs based on the type of
drug seller. For those adolescents actively Implications and Future Directions
involved in drug selling and distribution net-
works, violence is a part of the practice of selling Research suggests that substance users will com-
illicit drugs. Moreover, some types of adolescent mit violent acts to obtain money needed for fur-
drug sellers are associated with serious and ther drug use. For example, serious and violent
1112 Drug Sellers and Distributors

crimes may become a viable option for those user- delinquents: A tripartite model and more. Contempo-
sellers who are not able to support their drug use rary Drug Problems, 27, 835–866.
Centers, N. L., & Weist, M. D. (1998). Inner city youth and
exclusively by sales. Additionally, journalistic drug dealing: A review of the problem. Journal of
accounts of youth in the drug economy indicate Youth and Adolescence, 27(3), 395–411.
that some user-sellers are involved in violent Dembo, R., Hughes, P., Jackson, L., & Mieczkowski,
crimes against other drug sellers (LeBlanc 2004; T. (1993). Crack cocaine dealing by adolescents in
two public housing projects: A pilot study. Human
Sikes 1997). The violence committed by Organization, 52(1), 89–96.
adolescent user-sellers also seems to straddle Dunlap, E., Sturzenhofecker, G., Sanabria, H., & Johnson,
Goldstein’s pharmacological, economic, and sys- B. D. (2004). Mothers and daughters: The
temic models in that youth may act violently as a intergenerational reproduction of violence and drug
use in home and street life. Journal of Ethnicity in
result of the biological changes caused by the Substance Abuse, 3(2), 1–23.
illicit drug, as a way to gain money and additional Dunlap, E., Johnson, B. D., & Morse, E. (2007). Illicit drug
drugs, or as part of retaliation against an opposing markets among New Orleans evacuees before and soon
drug seller. Further research into the combination after Hurricane Katrina. Journal of Drug Issues, 37(4),
981–1006.
of types of adolescent drug sellers and Goldstein’s Feigelman, S., Stanton, B. F., & Ricardo, I. (1993). Per-
models of the drugs-violence nexus can help ceptions of drug selling and drug use among urban
differentiate between adolescent sellers’ motiva- youths. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 13(3),
tions to act violently within the drug economy. 267–284.
Flom, P. L., Friedman, S. R., Jose, B., & Curtis, R. (2001).
Researchers, practitioners, and policy makers can Peer norms regarding drug use and drug selling among
use the current knowledge about the different household youth in a low-income “drug supermarket”
types of adolescent drug sellers to stagger inter- urban neighborhood. Drugs: Education, Prevention, &
ventions for various drug sellers as more youth Policy, 8(3), 219–232.
Goldstein, P. J. (1985). The drugs/violence nexus:
enter into the drug marketplace because of A tripartite conceptual framework Journal of Drug
increasingly limited economic opportunities in Issues, 39, 143–174.
the legal market. Hoffer, L. D. (2005). Junkie business: The evolution and
operation of a heroin dealing network. Belmont:
Wadsworth.
Jacinto, C., Duterte, M., Sales, P., & Murphy, S. (2008).
Cross-References “I’m Not a Real Dealer”: The identity process of
ecstasy sellers. Journal of Drug Issues, 38(2), 419–444.
Joseph, J., & Pearson, P. G. (2002). Black youths and
▶ Delinquency illegal drugs. Journal of Black Studies, 32(4), 422–438.
LeBlanc, N. (2004). Random family: Love, drugs, trouble
and coming of age in the Bronx. New York: Scribner.
References Li, X., Stanton, B., Cottrell, L., Burns, J., Pack, R., &
Kaljee, L. (2001). Patterns of initiation of sex and
Adler, P. (1993). Wheeling and dealing: An ethnography of drug-related activities among urban low-income
an upper level drug dealing and smuggling community African-American adolescents. The Journal of Adoles-
(2nd ed.). New York: Columbia University Press. cent Health, 28(1), 46–54.
Aguilar, J. P., & Jackson, A. K. (2009). From the streets to Moore, S. E. (1995). Adolescent black males’ drug traf-
institutions: Female adolescent drug sellers’ percep- ficking and addiction: Three theoretical perspectives.
tions of their power. Affilia: The Journal of Women Journal of Black Studies, 26(2), 99–116.
and Social Work, 24(4), 369–381. Natarajan, M., & Belanger, M. (1998). Varieties of drug
Anderson, T. L. (2005). Dimensions of women’s power in trafficking organizations: A typology of cases prose-
the illicit drug economy. Theoretical Criminology, 9(4), cuted in New York City. Journal of Drug Issues, 28(4),
371–400. 1005–1026.
Atkyns, R. L., & Hanneman, G. J. (1974). Illicit drug Sales, P., & Murphy, S. (2007). San Francisco’s freelancing
distribution and dealer communication behavior. Jour- ecstasy dealers: Towards a sociological understanding
nal of Health and Social Behavior, 15(1), 36–43. of drug markets. Journal of Drug Issues, 37(4),
Bourgois, P. I. (2002). In search of respect: Selling crack in 919–950.
El Barrio (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Sikes, G. (1997). 8 ball chicks: A year in the violent world
Press. of girl gangs. New York: Anchor Books.
Brunelle, N., Brochu, S., & Cousineau, M.-M. (2000). Valdez, A., & Sifaneck, S. J. (2004). “Getting high and
Drug-crime relations among drug-consuming juvenile getting by”: Dimensions of drug selling behaviors
Due Process of Law 1113

among American Mexican gang members in South government’s action likely violates those rights.
Texas. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, The fundamental concern that “due process of
41(1), 82–105.
Werb, D., Kerr, T., Li, K., Montaner, J., & Wood, E. (2008). law” addresses, then, is the nature of those rights.
Risks surrounding drug trade involvement among In this regard, it is much more complex than
street-involved youth. The American Journal of Drug might be expected, in that it actually involves
and Alcohol Abuse, 34(6), 810–820. two fundamental components.
The fundamental components are easy to
understand. The more well-known component is
understood as procedural due process. That aspect D
Due Process of Law of due process holds the government subservient
to the law that protects individuals from govern-
Roger J. R. Levesque mental actions (often called state actions). The
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA focus is on protecting individuals from the gov-
ernment by providing them with the tools they
need to protect themselves from, most notably,
Overview errors that the government would do when they
infringe on peoples’ rights. At its core, it involves
Due process rests on the belief that the govern- the process by which the government infringes on
ment must be fair and respect the rights owed to peoples’ rights and the protection that people have
individuals. Although the tendency is to focus on from the government.
procedural rights (essentially the protections in The less popularly known, but equally
place to protect individuals from errors), due important, component of due process is under-
process also involves substantive rights (the stood as substantive due process. This aspect
actual rights deemed worth protecting). In the of due process involves limiting laws, includ-
USA, the Supreme Court has devoted much ing legal procedures, so that they are funda-
attention to these rights, and the rights have mentally fair, just, and do not inappropriately
evolved considerably over time. Part of that evo- infringe on people’s liberties. This component
lution has involved including adolescents as of due process, which centers on the general
individuals worthy of due process rights protec- command that laws must not be unfair,
tions. That development, however, remains lim- involves judicial decisions that reach interpre-
ited as adolescents’ due process rights are both tations of what constitutes the needed protec-
limited and also undeveloped. Much of the future tions under substantive due process. It
developments in adolescents’ rights likely will identifies the types of rights that need protec-
focus on these two domains of due process, mak- tion in the first place; they are what procedural
ing them critical to understanding the rights of due process rights protect.
adolescents. Both forms of due process serve as impor-
tant sources of protection for adolescents.
They serve, for example, as the basis for ado-
Due Process lescents’ right to privacy, including their rights
to relationships, and numerous rights that
The principle of due process of law refers to guide how social and legal systems respond
the government’s need to respect the legal to them (e.g., educational systems, child wel-
rights that they owe to individuals according to fare systems, juvenile courts, and criminal
legal mandates. The popularly understood com- courts) (Levesque 2008, 2016). Given its cen-
ponent of due process involves the need for the trality to the manner the government responds
government to follow a prescribed course of law to adolescents, this entry briefly reviews the
before it can infringe on individuals’ rights. legal sources and nature of due process and
Without following those legal mandates, the its significance in US law.
1114 Due Process of Law

The Legal Foundation of Due Process As expected, much significance attaches to


whether a right is to be deemed fundamental.
The legal basis for due process protections stems The Supreme Court has taken a narrow view,
from the Fifth Amendment to the US Constitu- with essentially their needing to be explicit or
tion. That amendment provides that “no person implicit recognition of the right in the constitu-
shall be. . . deprived of life, liberty, or property, tion. The court also generally restricts fundamen-
without due process of law.” The federal courts tal rights to those deemed deeply rooted in the
originally interpreted the clause as restricting gov- nation’s history and tradition (see Washington
ernmental procedures used to deprive persons of v. Glucksberg 1997). One of the most remarkable
life, liberty, and property. That approach centered features of substantive due process is that its con-
on the fairness of proceedings (see Murray’s tent, which still guides the court today, essentially
Lessee v. Hoboken Land and Improvement was enumerated in a footnote to a case, a now
Co. 1855). By the turn of the twentieth century, famous footnote 4 of United States v. Carolene
however, courts had interpreted the amendment to Products Company (1938). That footnote
include a substantive component. It viewed it as highlighted three types of rights under substantive
requiring that the deprivations of rights be due process: the first eight amendments in the Bill
supported by some legitimate justification of Rights, restrictions on the political process
(Allgeyer v. Louisiana 1897). This notion that (e.g., the rights of voting, association, and free
courts could hold governmental actions unconsti- speech); and the rights of “discrete and insular
tutional, regardless of the validity of the proce- minorities” (particularly those who lack sufficient
dures by which they executed those actions, has numbers or power to seek redress through the
since become a firmly rooted principle in consti- political process). Among the rights that the
tutional law and a well-accepted part of the sepa- court has recognized as fundamental are those
ration of powers among different governmental protecting marriage, marital privacy, use of con-
institutions. traception, bodily integrity, and abortion; also
In terms of substantive due process, a central included are the rights to have children and
issue that arises is what rights gain the most directing their upbringing (Levesque 2008,
protection. Fundamental rights receive the most 2016). Still, debates continue among scholars
protection, meaning that governmental efforts regarding whether fundamental rights should be
to limit those rights will lead to considerably limited to those enumerated in the constitution or
exacting scrutiny to ensure that the government could be expanded to embrace rights implied by
has a compelling interest to do so and that the more generalized conceptions of liberty (see, e.g.,
government closely tailored the infringement to Wolf 2002). The debates are of significance in that
achieving those interests (Korematsu v. United they go to the heart of substantive due process,
States 1944). Conversely, governmental actions which is that it limits not only executive and
that do not affect a fundamental right are judicial powers but also legislative actions. Typi-
given considerably more leeway and will be cally, the power to create laws should remain with
found permissible as long as the regulation the legislatures; substantive due process gives the
that impairs that interest rationally relates to a courts considerable power to strike down state and
legitimate state interest. This entry is meant to federal statutes that seek to limit various activities.
be quite deferential, and it is so because the Procedural due process rights attract consider-
government is not infringing on a right that ably less controversy, although they are not
should be highly protected (see United States immune from ongoing debates. The reason proce-
v. Carolene Products Company 1938). These dural rights attract less controversy is that the
interpretive rules mean that courts treat rights constitution explicitly enumerates many of them.
differently, with the most important ones This enumeration is especially the case for crim-
gaining the strongest judicial protection from inal prosecutions, as the Bill of Rights offers a
the state itself. host of explicit procedural right guarantees. These
Due Process of Law 1115

rights mainly have been applied to individual example, it would relate to adolescents’ rights to
states in addition to the federal government, a public benefits (e.g., “welfare” and educational
process known as incorporation that was done benefits), protections from intrusions in their
under the power of the Fourteenth Amendment. relationships (e.g., against the child welfare sys-
As a result, this protection reaches all governmen- tem), and protections from both their parents and
tal actions that can result in an individual’s depri- the state (e.g., in cases involving their institution-
vation, whether civil or criminal in nature. alization). This wide range of cases makes it
Generally, due process rights aim to give individ- difficult to underestimate the significance of pro-
uals a fair opportunity to affect the result or judg- cedural due process. D
ment of statutes, regulations, and enforcement
actions that would deprive individuals of what
generally is described as rights involving life,
Conclusion
liberty, or property. These protections center the
focus of due process on the concept of fundamen-
The right to due process of law serves as a limita-
tal fairness. The Supreme Court often uses lan-
tion on governments. It has both “procedural”
guage borrowed from a 1934 case finding that due
and “substantive” components. These compo-
process is violated “if a practice or rule offends
nents mean that due process rights impose
some principle of justice so rooted in the traditions
unenumerated restrictions on legal procedures
and conscience of our people as to be ranked as
(which involve the manner laws operate or are
fundamental” (Snyder v. Massachusetts 1934,
enumerated and executed) and also on legal sub-
p. 105). In legal proceedings against individuals,
stance (what laws regulate, such as what they seek
due process involves, for example, adequate
to prohibit). Both components are central to
notice, a hearing so that they can be heard, and a
understanding the nature of rights as well as to
neutral arbiter. These general principles guide
how they can be protected. Both also are central to
responses to many controversies involving chal-
understanding and protecting adolescents’ rights.
lenges to state actions.
Although due process rights tend to be less
controversial than substantive due process rights,
considerable room exists for controversy. That Cross-References
controversy often involves the rigor with which
the requirements of procedural due process ▶ Equal Protection Laws
should be applied to a particular deprivation.
The court has formulated a balancing test to References
determine the amount of rigor required by the
constitution. That test involves the consideration Allgeyer v. Louisiana. (1897). 165 U.S. 578.
of three factors: “first, the private interest that Eldridge v. Williams. (1976). 424 U.S. 319.
will be affected by the official action; second, Korematsu v. United States. (1944). 323 U.S. 214.
Levesque, R. J. R. (2008). Rethinking child maltreatment
the risk of an erroneous deprivation of such law: Returning to first principles. New York: Springer.
interest through the procedures used, and the Levesque, R. J. R. (2016). Adolescents, Privacy and the
probable value, if any, of additional or substitute Law: A Developmental Science Perspective. New
procedural safeguards; and, finally, the govern- York: Oxford University Press.
Murray’s Lessee v. Hoboken Land & Improvement Co.
ment’s interest, including the function involved (1855). 59 U.S. 272.
and the fiscal and administrative burdens that the Snyder v. Massachusetts. (1934). 291 U.S. 97.
additional or substitute procedural requirement United States v. Carolene Products Company. (1938).
would entail” (Eldridge v. Williams 1976, 304 U.S. 144.
Washington v. Glucksberg. (1997). 521 U.S. 702.
p. 335). This test eventually would figure prom- Wolf, A. B. (2002). Fundamentally flawed: Tradition and
inently in a wide range of cases, including those fundamental rights. University of Miami Law Review,
dealing with adolescents and their families. For 57, 101–156.
1116 Duty to Protect in Law

Constitutional law when youth have been harmed


Duty to Protect in Law after having been returned to their family mem-
bers, even when the state knew of the high risk of
Roger J. R. Levesque harm. This entry reviews this important case and
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA further highlights key rules regarding the duty to
protect others.
The DeShaney case involved a 4-year-old
Overview child, Joshua, who was severely beaten by his
custodial father. The Winnebago County Depart-
Adolescents suffer many preventable harms. ment of Social Services (“DSS”) and several of its
Among the most prevalent harms that they suffer case workers had received complaints that Joshua
are at the hands of family members and others was being abused by his father, and it had taken
who care for them. In such instances, concerns various steps to protect him from harm. The child
have been raised about the government’s duty to had been removed from his home but returned to
protect. The Supreme Court of the United States the father, who had agreed to a variety of condi-
has addressed this issue by stating the obligations tions that would help reduce the risk of harm to
that governments have under the US Constitution. Joshua. DSS, however, had not removed him from
The Court has rejected the notion that the govern- his father’s custody. Quite tragically, Joshua even-
ment has an obligation to protect youth from tually was beaten so severely by his father that he
private harms, which are harms not caused suffered brain damage resulting in his being
directly by the government. This article examines forced to spend the rest of his life confined to a
that leading case by detailing its rationales and mental health institution.
importance. Joshua and his mother sued DSS under
42 U.S.C. § 1983. Section 1983 of the Civil
Rights Acts establishes a cause of action for
Protecting Adolescents from Harm violations of constitutional rights. Under that
statute, a claim can be made that a right had
Much debate centers on the extent to which the been violated by someone who was acting
government has a duty to protect youth from the “under the color of law,” that is, pursuant to
harms they suffer at the hands of family members. their roles as state actors. Joshua and his mother
This is the case because there continues to be high alleged that DSS had deprived Joshua of his
rates of maltreatment, in the hundreds of thou- liberty interest in bodily integrity, in violation
sands per year in the USA alone, with a prevailing of the Fourteenth Amendment’s Due Process
sense that much maltreatment goes unreported Clause, by failing to intervene to protect him
(see Levesque 2008). That recognition has led to against his father’s violence. The lower courts
the creation of child welfare systems meant to all held against Joshua, with all of them essen-
help protect children from harm, and those sys- tially confirming that the Due Process Clause of
tems include permitting states to intrude aggres- the Fourteenth Amendment does not impose an
sively in families and remove children from them affirmative duty on the part of state and local
when there is a high risk of harm. Yet, the governments to protect their citizens from pri-
Supreme Court clearly has stated that the state vate instances of violence. They further had
cannot be held responsible, under the Constitu- found that the causal connection between the
tion’s Due Process Clause, for the harms children state’s (here being DSS) conduct and Joshua’s
suffer from private parties, such as family mem- injuries simply was too attenuated to establish a
bers. The case, DeShaney v. Winnebago County deprivation of constitutional rights actionable
Department of Social Services (1990), although under § 1983. The Supreme Court agreed to
not well received by some, has settled the matter hear the case, and it too held as the lower
in favor of protecting the state from liability under courts had.
Duty to Protect in Law 1117

The Supreme Court’s majority opinion flatly exceptions grew out of the marked distinction
stated that nothing in the language of the Due between nonfeasance and misfeasance, with mis-
Process Clause itself requires a state to protect feasance occurring when, through affirmative
the life, liberty, and property of its citizens against actions, another is rendered more vulnerable to
invasion by private actors. Under this view, the harm. Leaving someone more vulnerable imposes
Due Process Clause works to limit state action, a duty to act affirmatively to assist. Placing an
but it does not guarantee certain minimal levels of individual in peril creates a “special relationship”
safety and security. This means that the Due Pro- that renders the misfeasor liable for harm suffered
cess Clause cannot be the source of an affirmative due to his actions and also for his subsequent D
duty on the part of the state to protect these inter- inaction in failing to provide assistance. This rec-
ests from private invasion. In its reasoning, the ognition results not only when there is misfeasance
majority noted that the purpose of the Due Process but also when there are certain relationships that
Clause was to protect citizens from the state, not give rise to a duty to aid another in distress. Exam-
to require that the state protect citizens from one ples of special relationships include employer–-
another. The Court did acknowledge that, under employee relationships, shopkeepers and patrons,
certain limited situations, the state does have an jailors and prisoners, parents and children as well
affirmative duty to act, such that it has the need to as school officials and students (see, e.g., Levesque
provide adequate care to incarcerated prisoners or 2002). Thus, many examples support the duty to
those involuntarily held by the states. By doing so, care, but those examples tend to rest on the need to
the Court confirmed the narrow view that when show the existence of a special relationship that
the state acts to hold individuals against their will, creates the duty to care.
the Constitution imposes a duty to ensure their The common law principle relating to special
safety and general well-being, a duty that no lon- relationships is what eventually led the Court to
ger exists after the individual is released. hold that the Fourteenth Amendment imposes a
Importantly, the dissenting justices had argued duty on a state to provide adequate care, including
that the state’s initial action (in which it took protection, when the state has assumed the
affirmative, protective actions) was of signifi- responsibility for their well-being or otherwise
cance and that the focus should be on that rather rendered them more vulnerable to harm (see
than on the actions it failed to take to protect. The Estelle v. Gamble 1976; Youngberg v. Romeo
argument was that, by intervening, the state had 1982). Unlike the DeShaney case, however, the
removed other sources of aid that could have instances above that give rise to a state’s duty are
helped Joshua and others similarly situated. notable for the existence of a complete and total
Because Wisconsin law directed its citizens to custodial relationship between the individual and
depend on local departments of social services to the state. Whether the Court would impose a spe-
protect children from abuse, the dissent argued cial relationship in circumstances of less than total
that the state had placed Joshua into a special dependence, such as when the state does not have
relationship with the state that required the state physical custody but still restricts an individual’s
to protect him from harm. The majority was not ability to care for themselves, still remains to
persuaded by this line of reasoning. be answered. As the Court in DeShaney (1990,
It is worth highlighting the legal recognition of p. 201) noted:
exceptions to the rule that individuals or a state
[w]hile the State may have been aware of the
does not have a duty to care. At common law, dangers that Joshua faced in the free world, it
individuals did not have a duty to aid others in played no part in their creation, nor did it do
distress. Nonfeasance, or inaction, that results in anything to render him more vulnerable to them.
harm was permissible. Originally, the rule was That the State once took temporary custody of
Joshua does not alter the analysis, for when it
absolute in that the failure to take affirmative returned him to his father’s custody, it placed him
steps to prevent harm did not create liability. The in no worse position than that in which he would
rule, however, has eroded over the years. The have been had it not acted at all.
1118 Dyscalculia

The Court does not view the state as placing


him in a worse position, nor does the Court view it Dyslexia
as restricting the child’s ability to care for itself or,
in this case, receive care from its parent. Roger J. R. Levesque
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA

Conclusion
The term dyslexia refers to a disability that takes
two forms. It can be a learning disability, known
It is difficult to overstate the significance of the
DeShaney case. The case highlights the view that as developmental dyslexia, unexplainable by def-
icits in general intelligence, motivation or learn-
the Constitution is a negative charter of rights
ing opportunities. It also can take the form of
rather than one that sets affirmative duties. This
means that the Constitution focuses on pro- acquired dyslexia, or alexia, which describes a
specific pattern of reading deficits acquired during
tecting individuals from the government, not
adulthood as a result of brain injuries or diseases.
from private actors. This approach results in
placing limits on state action rather than trying Generally, however, focus has been on develop-
mental dyslexia. This disability is associated with
to have the state take affirmative steps to ensure
numerous types of deficits in domains relating to
rights. Although the distinctions between nega-
tive and affirmative obligations can become quite language acquisition, writing, and mathematical
abilities, as well as motor coordination, dexterity,
unclear, the general principle affirmed in
and attention deficits (for a review, see Habib
DeShaney seeks to clarify the stance that the
government seeks to protect individuals from it 2000). Generally, however, dyslexia is character-
ized by difficulties with the accurate and/or fluent
and to limit the government’s role rather than
recognition of words, as well as poor spelling and
expand it.
decoding abilities. Dyslexia reveals itself initially
in difficulty learning to read effortlessly or with
understanding, and subsequently by erratic spell-
References ing and deficits that affect written as opposed to
spoken language.
DeShaney v. Winnebago County Department of Social
Dyslexia arguably is the most common
Services. (1990). 489 U.S. 189.
Estelle v. Gamble. (1976). 429 U.S. 97. neurobehavioral disorder affecting children. Dys-
Levesque, R. J. R. (2002). Child maltreatment and the law: lexia indicates a persistent, chronic condition;
Foundations in science, policy and practice. Durham: unlike many other conditions, it does not represent
Carolina Academic Press.
a transient developmental lag. Prevalence rates
Levesque, R. J. R. (2008). Rethinking child maltreatment
law: Returning to first principles. New York: Springer. range from 5% to 17.5% (Shaywitz and Shaywitz
Youngberg v. Romeo. (1982). 457 U.S. 307. 2005). Dyslexia tends to appear more frequently in
males, with studies reporting ratios ranging from
2:3 to 4:5 (Habib 2000). Researchers have yet to
understand why males are more likely to show
Dyscalculia signs of dyslexia, but prevalence studies continue
to note the sex differences.
▶ Specific Learning Disorder Dyslexia typically is unexpected given the nor-
mal range of intelligence and other abilities
exhibited by individuals who show dyslexic
symptoms. The disorders associated with dyslexia
Dysgraphia often relate to oral-language impairment, espe-
cially subtle impairment in perception and articu-
▶ Specific Learning Disorder lation of speech; and these subtle impairments
Dyslexia 1119

currently are viewed as the most likely mechanism by dyslexia given a sibling is already affected is
leading to the reading disorders. Indeed, the lead- estimated at 43–60% (Scerri and Schulte-Körne
ing theory of dyslexia’s causes, the phonological 2010). Evidence for a genetic origin of dyslexia
deficit model, views it as a specific impairment in has been increasing rapidly. The role of genetics
the representation, storage, and/or retrieval of is not surprising given the role of families. But,
speech sounds (for a review, Ramus et al. 2003). although different forms of dyslexia may occur
Importantly, however, it is critical to recognize that within the same families, different genes have
the relationship between oral-language deficits and been implicated in different aspects of reading
dyslexia is far from straightforward, with some disorders, with some forms of dyslexia appearing D
individuals with dyslexia exhibiting no oral- more heritable than others, which suggests a role
language impairment and others who read without for environments. Research has identified several
difficulty. Dyslexia is a dimensional disorder, (sometimes controversial) environmental factors
meaning that reading disability and ability occur that may link to dyslexia, such as maternal anti-
along a continuum. This wide range of possible bodies, immune disorders, fatty-acid deficien-
disorders helps to explain why there continues to cies, toxic metals, and exposures to high levels
be many theories relating to the causes of dyslexia, of prenatal testosterone (Scerri and Schulte-
as theories can explain only some but not all pos- Körne 2010). These findings have led to the
sible features (see Scerri and Schulte-Körne 2010). conclusion that genetic transmission appears
Given the wide range of dysfunctions that can complex and nonexclusive (see Habib 2000;
support the manifestations of dyslexia, the most Pennington 1999).
effective remedial approaches typically involve More recently, research has focused consider-
extensive, specific, and specialized teaching best ably on the neurological bases of developmental
provided by one or more competent professionals. dyslexia. A wide range of studies that have used
Depending on the nature of the disability, reading postmortem brain specimens and, more recently,
can become possible, although reading is likely to brain imaging suggest differences in the temporo-
be effortful and sometimes accompanied by errors parieto-occipital brain regions between dyslexic
in comprehension and more or less profound and non-impaired readers (for a review, see Habib
spelling impairment (Habib 2000). It is this spell- 2000). For example, functional magnetic reso-
ing impairment that, during adolescence and nance imaging has been helpful in beginning to
adulthood, tends to persist as the permanent distinguish among types of reading disabilities
mark of the developmental disorder. This likely (Shaywitz and Shaywitz 2005). These lines of
is the case in that efforts to intervene and treat tend research indicate that dyslexia involves a disrup-
to not focus on at risk children. The lack of focus tion of left hemisphere posterior brain systems.
on children at risk is of significance in that at-risk This area of research further suggests that disrup-
readers can become both accurate and fluent tions in the posterior reading systems result in
readers if they have appropriate instruction; this attempts to compensate by shifting to other, ancil-
is in contrast to older, reading-disabled children lary systems. For example, it is not uncommon for
who, with intensive, evidence-based remedial children to engage the right-hemisphere sites that
interventions can markedly improve but not as allow poor readers to use other perceptual pro-
fluently as they would have with earlier interven- cesses to compensate for poor phonologic skills.
tions (for a review, see Alexander and Slinger- This shift helps explain why some may develop
Constant 2004). accuracy in reading words but remain slow, non-
Studies report significant familial clustering of automatic readers (Shaywitz and Shaywitz 2005).
dyslexia. Family history is one of the most impor-
tant risk factors, with research indicating that from
23% to 65% of children who have dyslexia have a Cross-References
parent who has the disorder (Scarborough 1990).
The probability of an individual being affected ▶ Specific Learning Disorder
1120 Dysphoria

References depression, especially during middle adolescence


(Kistner et al. 1999). If adolescents believe that
Alexander, A. W., & Slinger-Constant, A.-M. (2004). Cur- their peers are hesitant to accept them, whether
rent status of treatments for dyslexia: Critical review.
this is actually the case or not, this type of experi-
Journal of Child Neurology, 19, 744–758.
Habib, M. (2000). The neurological basis of developmen- ence and perception can lead to an increased risk of
tal dyslexia: An overview and working hypothesis. that individual also feeling dysphoric (Kistner et al.
Brain, 123, 2373–2399. 1999). One’s self-perception concerning peer
Pennington, B. F. (1999). Toward an integrated under-
acceptance is actually a crucial determining factor
standing of dyslexia: Genetic, neurological, and cogni-
tive mechanisms. Development and Psychopathology, in determining whether youth develop dysphoria in
11, 629–654. later years; and this phenomenon can take place
Ramus, F. S., Rosen, S., Dakin, S., Day, B., Castellote, J., even if a particular youth’s peers actually do accept
White, S., et al. (2003). Theories of developmental
them (Demir and Urberg 2004). The key element in
dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case study of dys-
lexic adults. Brain, 126, 841–865. feelings of dysphoria is self-perception, as dyspho-
Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Very early language deficits in ria itself often is comprised of a self-report of
dyslexic children. Child Development, 61, 1728–1743. depression. During adolescence, there is an inten-
Scerri, T. S., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2010). Genetics of
sified emphasis on peer acceptance or rejection;
developmental dyslexia. European Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 19, 179–197. this component of an adolescent’s experience can
Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2005). Dyslexia effectively establish and sustain that person’s sense
(specific reading disability). Biological Psychiatry, 57, of self-worth. Hence, it proves unsurprising that
1301–1309.
such perceptions could have a lingering impact on
an adult’s potential to develop dysphoria that can
lead to more severe symptoms. Even during
adolescence, the presence of high levels of self-
Dysphoria devaluative dysphoric experiences increase the
liability for persistence of a first-episode of major
Roger J. R. Levesque depressive disorder (Park et al. 2005).
Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA

Cross-References
Dysphoria can be defined as a self-report of feel-
ings of depression in an individual (Kistner et al. ▶ Depressive Symptoms
1999). Similar types of feelings could be
described as dejection, disaffection, dysthymia,
unhappiness, dissatisfaction with one’s life or References
self (or part of one’s self), or generally low self-
esteem. Importantly, dysphoria refers only to a Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., & Pfäfflin, F. (2010). The DSM
diagnostic criteria for gender identity disorder in ado-
condition of mood. It can be experienced as part lescents and adults. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39,
of typical development as well as atypcial devel- 499–513.
opment. Dysphoria is a component of several psy- Demir, M., & Urberg, K. A. (2004). Friendship and adjust-
chiatric disorders, including anxiety disorders and ment among adolescents. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 88, 68–82.
mood disorders as well as gender identity disor- Kistner, J., Balthazor, M., Risi, S., & Burton, C. (1999).
der, which is sometimes called gender dysphoria Predicting dysphoria in adolescence from actual and
(see, e.g., Cohen-Kettenis and Pfäfflin 2010). perceived peer acceptance in childhood. Journal of
The study of dysphoria during the adolescent Clinical Child Psychology, 28, 94–104.
Park, R. J., Goodyear, I. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (2005). Self-
period tends to focus on depressive symptoms and devaluative dysphoric experience and the prediction of
the role peers play in their experience. Adolescents persistent first-episode major depressive disorder in
exhibit a tendency to experience feelings of adolescents. Psychological Medicine, 35, 539–548.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi