Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 6

doi:10.

1093/brain/awm129 Brain (2007), 130, 2117^2122

Increased motor cortex white matter volume


predicts motor impairment in autism
Stewart H. Mostofsky,1,2,3, Melanie P. Burgess1 and Jennifer C. Gidley Larson1
1
Kennedy Krieger Institute, 707 North Broadway, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine,
Departments of 2Neurology and 3Psychiatry, Baltimore, MD, USA
Correspondence to: Stewart H. Mostofsky, MD, Kennedy Krieger Institute, 707 N. Broadway, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA
E-mail: mostofsky@kennedykrieger.org

Careful consideration of motor impairments, such as those documented in autism, can afford valuable
insights into the neurological basis of developmental disorders. Motor signs are highly quantifiable and reprodu-
cible and can serve as markers for deficits in parallel systems important for socialization and communication.

Downloaded from brain.oxfordjournals.org by guest on October 14, 2010


Correlations of motor signs with anatomic MRI (aMRI) measures therefore offer an important means of inves-
tigating brain abnormalities contributing to autism. Prior aMRI studies have revealed increased cerebral volume
in young children with autism, particularly in ‘outer zone’ radiate white matter; however functional correlates
of these findings have not been reported. In this study, we examined whether radiate white matter within
the primary motor cortex would predict impaired motor performance in children with autism. Subjects
included children ages 8^12 years: 20 with autism, 36 typically developing (TD) controls and 20 clinical
controls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Regional tissue volumes were measured
using an automated tissue classification algorithm followed by a semi-automated parcellation method. Motor
performance was assessed using the Physical and Neurologic Examination of Subtle Signs (PANESS),
with higher scores indicating poorer performance. Independent linear regression analyses revealed that for TD
controls there was a significant negative correlation between total PANESS score and primary motor cortex
white matter volume in both the right and left hemispheres, such that increased white matter volume predicted
improved motor skill. In contrast, children with autism showed a robust positive correlation between total
PANESS score and left hemisphere primary motor and premotor white matter volumes, such that increased
white matter volume predicted poorer motor skill. No significant correlations were found for ADHD.
Multivariate regression analyses revealed that the correlation between PANESS score and left motor cortex
white matter volume in children with autism significantly differed from those in both ADHD and TD children.
The correlation in ADHD did not significantly differ from that in TD children. The findings for the first
time demonstrate an association between increasing radiate white matter volume and functional impairment
in children with autism, in this case basic motor skill impairment. The observed association, which appears
specific to autism, may be representative of global patterns of brain abnormality that not only contribute
to motor dysfunction in autism, but also deficits in socialization and communication that define the disorder.

Keywords: autism; white matter; imaging; motor; central coherence

Abbreviations: ADHD ¼ attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; PANESS ¼ Physical and Neurologic Examination
of Subtle Signs; TD ¼ typically developing
Received October 30, 2006. Revised May 9, 2007. Accepted May 10, 2007. Advance Access publication June 15, 2007

Introduction Larson and Mostofsky, 2006) and include difficulties with


Abnormalities on motor examination have often afforded both basic motor control (Jansiewicz et al., 2006) and
valuable insights into developmental disorders of the brain performance of complex motor skills and gestures (Rogers
(Denckla and Roeltgen, 1992). Such abnormalities have et al., 1996; Mostofsky et al., 2006). Increased insight into
been well documented in individuals with autism (Gidley the brain mechanisms underlying autism can be gained

ß The Author (2007). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org
2118 Brain (2007), 130, 2117^2122 S. H. Mostofsky et al.

from careful consideration of these motor signs. Children with autism met Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Motor signs can serve as markers for deficits in parallel Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) criteria. The
brain systems important for control of socialization and diagnosis was confirmed using both the Autism Diagnostic
communication; in a developmental disorder such as Observation Schedule—Generic (ADOS-G) (Lord et al., 2000)
and the Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised (ADI-R) (Lord
autism, procedural learning mechanisms important for
et al., 1994). Children with identifiable causes of autism (e.g.
acquisition of motor skills may also contribute to impaired
Fragile X syndrome) and known neurological disorders, including
development of social and communicative skills (Mostofsky epilepsy, were excluded.
et al., 2000; Ullman, 2004; Walenski et al., 2006). Measures Children in the clinical control group met DSM-IV criteria for
of motor function are more quantifiable and reproducible ADHD. The structured parent interview, Diagnostic Interview for
than are measures of complex social behaviour. Children and Adolescents—Fourth edition (DICA-IV) (Reich
Examination of correlations between motor function and et al., 1997) and ADHD-specific and broad behaviour rating
anatomic magnetic resonance imaging (aMRI) measures of scales [Conners’ Parent Rating Scale—Revised (CPRS-R: Conners
brain structure therefore offers an important means of et al., 1998a) and Teacher Rating Scale—Revised (CTRS-R:
investigating brain abnormalities that contribute to the Conners et al., 1998b)] were used to confirm ADHD diagnosis.
phenotypic features of autism. The CPRS-R and DSM-IV criteria were also used to evaluate
Increased brain volume is the most consistent neuroima- ADHD subtype, with 8 children (one girl) meeting criteria for the

Downloaded from brain.oxfordjournals.org by guest on October 14, 2010


predominantly inattentive subtype and 12 (three girls) meeting
ging finding in children with autism, with prominent
criteria for the combined subtype. Children meeting criteria for
differences during early childhood that are no longer seen
other psychiatric diagnoses based on the DICA-IV, other than
by adolescence (Courchesne et al., 2001; Carper et al., 2002; Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) and simple phobia, were
Hazlett et al., 2005). The increased volume has been excluded from this study. ODD was present in eight of the
principally attributed to larger white matter volumes, subjects and simple phobia was present in two, including one
particularly in outer ‘radiate’ regions (Herbert et al., 2004). subject with both. Children with identified neurological disorders,
For the current study, we examined whether aMRI including epilepsy and tic disorders, were also excluded from
measures of radiate white matter volume within primary participation. In addition, none of the children with ADHD had a
motor cortices would predict motor skill deficits in children history of speech/language disorder or a Reading Disability, and
with autism, hypothesizing that, for children with autism, all had a basic reading standard score of 85 (16th percentile) or
increased white matter volume in the primary motor higher on the Word Reading subtest from the Wechsler Individual
cortex would predict the degree of basic motor skill Achievement Test, First Edition (WIAT; Wechsler, 1992) or
impairment. Furthermore, we hypothesized that this Second Edition (WIAT–II; Wechsler, 2002). Children with ADHD
association would be specific to autism. We examined taking longer-acting medications (i.e. other than stimulants) were
excluded from the study. For children taking stimulant medica-
this by including: (i) a group of typically developing (TD)
tion, parents were requested to withhold medication on the day of
control children and (ii) a clinical control group of children
and the day prior to testing.
with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), a Subjects in the typically developing control group were free of
developmental disorder which, like autism, has been found any neurological, developmental or psychiatric disorders based on
to be associated with impairments in motor execution and responses from the DICA-IV. None of the controls had a history
control (Mostofsky et al., 2001, 2003; Klimkeit et al., 2005; of academic problems requiring school-based intervention services
Klein et al., 2006). or history of defined primary reading or language-based learning
disability and all had a basic reading standard score of 85 (16th
percentile) or higher on the Word Reading subtest from either the
Participants and methods WIAT or WIAT–II.
Subjects Written assent was obtained from all participating children and
Subjects, ages 8–12 years, included 20 high-functioning children written consent was obtained from a parent/guardian under
with autism (3 girls), 36 TD controls (10 girls) and 20 children (4 approval from the Johns Hopkins Medical Institutional Review
girls) with ADHD; children with ADHD were recruited as part of Board.
a separate ongoing study in our laboratory that also included
neuroimaging and motor assessment. All participants had full-
scale IQ (FSIQ) scores greater than 80, except one child with
Motor skill assessment
autism who had a FSIQ of 78 but a verbal IQ of 88, based on
performance on the Wechsler Intellectual Scale for Children Basic motor skills were assessed using a standardized motor
(WISC), 3rd edition (Wechsler, 1991), or the WISC, 4th edition examination for children, the Physical and Neurologic
(Wechsler, 2003). Participants were recruited from outpatient Examination of Subtle Signs (PANESS) (Denckla, 1985). This
clinics at the Kennedy Krieger Institute, local area paediatricians, examination has been used to demonstrate basic motor skill
support groups for families of children with autism and/or ADHD impairments in both children with ADHD (Mostofsky et al., 2003)
(e.g. the Autism Society of America, Children and Adults with and children with autism (Mandelbaum et al., 2006; Jansiewicz
Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder), schools, social/service et al., 2006), and a regression model including PANESS variables
organizations (e.g. Boy and Girl Scouts of America) and from was found to offer a high level of discrimination in distinguishing
advertisements in the community (e.g. postings at libraries). boys with autism from controls (Jansiewicz et al., 2006).
Motor white matter in autism Brain (2007), 130, 2117^2122 2119

Imaging methods autism and children with ADHD (P ¼ 0.4). Chi-square


Fast field echo (FFE) MRI sequences were acquired on a analyses did not reveal any group differences in handedness
1.5 Tesla Philips Gyroscan NT Intera with the following or gender. (See Table 1 for summary of sample
parameters: TR ¼ 35 ms, TE ¼ 6 ms, flip angle ¼ 45 , 256  256, characteristics.)
FOV ¼ 240 mm, slice thickness ¼ 1.5 mm. Images were analysed in There was a significant effect of diagnosis on total
BrainImage using a fuzzy tissue segmentation algorithm (Reiss PANESS score (P50.0001). Post hoc analyses were con-
et al., 1998) and a semi-automated parcellation method for which sistent with published findings (Jansiewicz et al., 2006),
a revised Talairach stereotaxic grid specific for measurement revealing that children with autism show basic motor
in paediatric groups was used to subdivide the cerebrum into skill impairment, with significantly higher total PANESS
lobar and subcortical regions; the frontal lobe was then further scores than TD controls (P50.0001). Children with
subdivided into functionally relevant regions, including the
autism also showed significantly higher total PANESS
primary motor cortex defined by the precentral gyrus (see
Mostofsky et al., 2002 for a more detailed description).
scores than children with ADHD (P ¼ 0.001). Children
with ADHD showed higher total PANESS scores than TD
controls at a level that trended toward significance
Statistical methods (P ¼ 0.08).
Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was used to examine for group There were no significant group differences in TBV

Downloaded from brain.oxfordjournals.org by guest on October 14, 2010


differences in sample characteristics, including FSIQ, age, total (P ¼ 0.6) or in primary motor cortex (total, left and right)
PANESS score, total brain volume (TBV) and motor cortex white white or gray matter volumes, which is consistent with
matter volume. Chi-square analyses were used to examine for previously reported total brain and precentral gyrus
group differences in handedness and gender. findings in children with autism in this age range
Independent linear regression analyses were used to examine for (Courchesne et al., 2001; Herbert et al., 2004).
correlations between motor cortex white matter volume and total
PANESS score in each diagnostic group; these analyses were
conducted both with and without controlling for FSIQ and TBV. Analyses of correlations between motor
Following this, a multivariate regression model was used to cortex white matter volume and PANESS
examine for an interaction effect of diagnosis and primary motor
cortex white matter volume on total PANESS score. The approach
score
allowed us to first test our hypotheses that, for children with Independent linear regression analyses revealed that for
autism, increased white matter volume in the primary motor children with autism there was a positive correlation
cortex would predict the degree of basic motor skill impairment between total PANESS score (with higher score indicating
and subsequent to that, test whether this association would poorer performance) and left primary motor cortex white
significantly differ from that observed in either TD controls or matter volume (R2 ¼ 0.60, P50.0001), such that increased
children with ADHD. white matter volume predicted poorer motor skill (Fig. 1A).
Based on the regression, it was estimated that for every
cubic centimetre increase in left motor cortex white matter
Results volume, there was a 5-point increase in total PANESS score.
Sample characteristics No significant correlation was seen with right primary
The groups did not differ significantly in age (P ¼ 0.7). motor cortex white matter volume.
There was a significant group effect for FSIQ (P ¼ 0.0001). In contrast, for TD controls there was a significant
Post hoc analyses revealed that TD controls showed negative correlation between total PANESS score and
significantly higher FSIQ than both children with autism primary motor cortex white matter volume in the right
(P50.0001) and children with ADHD (P ¼ 0.002); there (R2 ¼ 0.13, P ¼ 0.03) and left hemispheres (R2 ¼ 0.15,
was no significant difference in FSIQ between children with P ¼ 0.02), such that increased white matter volume

Table 1 Results from ANOVA for all groups


Subjects
Autism (n ¼ 20) TD children (n ¼ 36) ADHD (n ¼ 20) Statistics

Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) F-test P-value


Age (years) 10.3 (1.7) 10.5 (1.3) 10.7 (1.3) F(2,75) ¼ 0.34 0.71
FSIQ 104.1 (17.8) 120.2 (11.4) 108.0 (13.5) F(2,75) ¼ 10.2 0.0001
Total PANESS score 33.4 (12.1) 17.6 (8.0) 22.5 (11.5) F(2,75) ¼ 15.5 50.0001
Total brain volume (cm3) 1341.3 (96.9) 1352.2 (107.0) 1321.9 (85.6) F(2,75) ¼ 0.60 0.55
Motor white volume (cm3) 21.0 (2.7) 21.6 (3.5) 20.3 (2.7) F(2,75) ¼ 1.2 0.31

Significant differences between Autism and TD Controls (P50.0001), ADHD and TD controls (P ¼ 0.002).

Significant differences between Autism and TD controls (P50.0001), Autism and ADHD (P ¼ 0.001).
2120 Brain (2007), 130, 2117^2122 S. H. Mostofsky et al.

predicted better motor skill (Fig. 1B). Based on the and that in children with ADHD (P ¼ 0.001). The
regressions, it was estimated that for every cubic centimetre correlation in TD children and that in children with
increase in either right or left motor cortex white ADHD did not significantly differ.
matter volume, there was a 1.5 point decrease in total
PANESS score.
Similar to TD controls, children with ADHD showed a Analyses of correlations of other frontal
negative correlation between total PANESS score and both regional white matter volumes with
left hemisphere (R2 ¼ 0.06, P ¼ 0.3) and right hemisphere PANESS score
(R2 ¼ 0.07, P ¼ 0.3) primary motor cortex white matter To determine whether the findings were specific to the
volumes, although neither of these correlations was primary motor cortex, we examined correlations
statistically significant. Based on the regressions, it was of PANESS scores with other frontal cortical regions,
estimated that for every cubic centimetre increase in either including prefrontal, premotor and anterior cingulate.
right or left motor cortex white matter volume there was a For children with autism, a significant positive correlation
2 point decrease in total PANESS score. was also observed between total PANESS score and left
After covarying for FSIQ and TBV, the findings in both premotor white matter volume (R2 ¼ 0.29, P ¼ 0.02) and
children with autism and TD controls remained significant, this remained significant after controlling for FSIQ and

Downloaded from brain.oxfordjournals.org by guest on October 14, 2010


with correlations that were more robust. For children with TBV (R2 ¼ 0.42, P ¼ 0.05); no correlations were seen for
autism there remained a significant positive correlation right premotor, or either hemisphere prefrontal or anterior
between total PANESS score and left hemisphere white cingulate volumes. For TD children and children with
matter volume (R2 ¼ 0.72, P ¼ 0.0001). For TD controls, ADHD there were no significant correlations between
there remained a significant negative correlation between PANESS scores and white matter volumes in any of the
total PANESS score and primary motor cortex white matter premotor, prefrontal or anterior cingulate regions.
volume in both the right (R2 ¼ 0.30, P ¼ 0.007) and left
(R2 ¼ 0.23, P ¼ 0.04) hemispheres. Correlations in children
with ADHD remained insignificant after controlling for Discussion
FSIQ and TBV. Consistent with our hypothesis, increased left motor and
Multivariate regression analyses including all three premotor white matter volumes were strong predictors of
groups of subjects (autism, ADHD and TD) revealed a poor motor function in children with autism. The findings
significant effect of diagnosis on the correlation between are in direct contrast to those seen in TD children, in
total PANESS score and left motor cortex white matter whom increased white matter volume in both left and right
volume (P50.0001). Separate two-group contrasts revealed motor cortex was associated with improved motor skills.
that the correlation between total PANESS score and left The strength of these observations is evidenced by the fact
motor cortex white matter volume in children with autism that the correlations were significant both with and without
significantly differed from that in TD children (P50.0001) covarying for FSIQ and TBV.

a Autism b TD Children
60 60

50 50
Total PANESS score

Total PANESS score

40 40

30 30

20 20

10 10

0 0
4 6 8 10 12 14 16 4 6 8 10 12 14 16
Left Motor Cortex White Matter Volume (cm3) Left Motor Cortex White Matter Volume (cm3)

Fig. 1 Independent linear regression analyses, covaried for FSIQ and TBV, demonstrating (A) for children with autism there is a robust
positive correlation between total PANESS score and left motor cortex white matter volume (R2 ¼ 0.72, P ¼ 0.0001), such that increased
white matter volume predicts poorer motor skill. (B) In contrast, for typically developing children there is a significant negative
correlation between total PANESS score and left motor cortex white matter volume (R2 ¼ 0.23, P ¼ 0.04), such that increased white
matter volume predicts better motor skill.
Motor white matter in autism Brain (2007), 130, 2117^2122 2121

There were no differences in precentral white matter biological variation in TD controls, suggests that it may be
volume between children with autism and TD children; this a defining biological feature of the disorder.
is consistent with previously published precentral white The observed association between increased white matter
matter findings in this age range (Herbert et al., 2004) and volume and functional impairment may be representative of
the more general observation that increased white matter global patterns of brain abnormality that not only
volume is seen in younger, but not older, children with contribute to motor dysfunction in autism, but also to
autism (Courchesne et al., 2001). Myelination in the deficits in socialization and communication that define the
precentral gyrus reaches maturity around 6 years of age disorder. Investigators have proposed that the pattern of
(Kinney et al., 1988). It may be that for children with impairments associated with autism, as well as some relative
autism, abnormal white matter development in the strengths in perceptual processing, are secondary to
precentral cortex occurs at a young age, contributing to abnormalities in structural and functional connectivity
motor dysfunction; TD children instead show a more (Minshew et al., 1997; Herbert et al., 2004; Happé and
gradual increase in precentral white matter development Frith, 2006). Overgrowth of localized cortical connections
associated with improvement in motor dexterity, so that by and undergrowth of more distant connections between
late childhood no differences in volume are observed. cerebral cortical regions and with subcortical structures
Examination of correlations between motor function and (Herbert et al., 2004; Happé and Frith, 2006) have been

Downloaded from brain.oxfordjournals.org by guest on October 14, 2010


white matter volume in younger children would help to hypothesized to result in impaired complex information
address this hypothesis. processing (Minshew et al., 1997) and ‘weak central
The association of increased motor white matter volumes coherence’ (Shah and Frith, 1993) and may also contribute
and poor motor function appears to be specific to autism. to impaired motor sequence learning necessary for devel-
This association was not observed in the clinical control opment of complex motor skills and social/communicative
group of children with ADHD, another developmental gestures (Mostofsky et al., 2000; Ullman, 2004; Gidley
disorder in which impairments in motor execution and Larson and Mostofsky, 2006; Walenski et al., 2006).
control are a common finding (Mostofsky et al., 2001, The measure of precentral white matter volume used in
2003; Klimkeit et al., 2005; Klein et al., 2006). Furthermore, the present study likely principally comprised localized
the correlation between motor skill performance and left connections; it corresponded to white matter within
motor cortex white matter volume in children with autism the precentral gyrus, with deep subcortical white matter
significantly differed both from that in TD children and explicitly excluded. The findings in the present study
that in children with ADHD. In contrast, the correlation in therefore appear to lend support to the notion that
children with ADHD did not significantly differ from increasing volume of localized cortical connections
that in TD children. The population of children with contributes to functional impairment in children with
ADHD in this study showed higher total PANESS scores autism.
than TD controls at a level that only trended toward
significance. Further evidence for the specificity of the
Acknowledgements
association of increased motor cortex white matter volume
This research was supported by NIH Grants R01 NS048527
with motor skill impairment in autism could come from
(SHM), HD024061 (Mental Retardation and
examining other populations of children with motor
Developmental Disabilities Research Center), M01
difficulties, for instance those with Developmental
RR00052 (Johns Hopkins General Clinical Research
Coordination Disorder, as well as children with features
Center) and grants from the National Alliance for Autism
otherwise related to autism, such as those with specific
Research and Autism Speaks (SHM).
language impairments.
The findings provide insight into the pathophysiology of
References
autism. Autism has been found to be associated with
Carper RA, Moses P, Tigue ZD, Courchesne E. Cerebral lobes in autism:
enlarged cerebral white matter volume, particularly in outer early hyperplasia and abnormal age effects. Neuroimage 2002; 16:
radiate regions (Courchesne et al., 2001; Herbert et al., 1038–51.
2004). Consistent with these neuroimaging findings, post- Casanova MF, van Kooten IA, Switala AE, van Engeland H, Heinsen H,
mortem studies of individuals with autism reveal abnor- Steinbusch HW, et al. Minicolumnar abnormalities in autism. Acta
Neuropathol (Berl). 2006; 112: 287–303.
malities in cortical minicolumns that reflect a bias towards
Conners CK, Sitarenios G, Parker JD, Epstein JN. The revised Conners’
shorter connecting fibers, which are localized to radiate Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion
white matter, at the expense of longer fibers connecting validity. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1998a; 26: 257–68.
distant cortical and cortical-subcortical regions (Casanova Conners CK, Sitarenios G, Parker JD, Epstein JN. Revision and
et al., 2006). The current findings for the first time restandardization of the Conners Teacher Rating Scale (CTRS-R):
factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. J Abnorm Child
demonstrate an association between increasing radiate
Psychol 1998b; 26: 279–91.
white matter volume and functional impairment, in this Courchesne E, Karns CM, Davis HR, Ziccardi R, Carper RA, Tigue ZD,
case basic motor skill impairment. The strength of the et al. Unusual brain growth patterns in early life in patients with autistic
correlation in autism, beyond that observed with normal disorder: an MRI study. Neurology 2001; 57: 245–54.
2122 Brain (2007), 130, 2117^2122 S. H. Mostofsky et al.

Denckla MB. Revised neurological examination for subtle signs. Minshew NJ, Goldstein G, Siegel DJ. Neuropsychologic functioning in
Psychopharmacol Bull 1985; 21: 773–800. autism: profile of a complex information processing disorder. J Int
Denckla MB, Roeltgen DP. Disorders of motor function and control. Neuropsychol Soc 1997; 3: 303–16.
In: Boller F, Grafman J, editors. Handbook of neuro-psychology. Vol. 6. Mostofsky SH, Goldberg MC, Landa RJ, Denckla MB. Evidence for a
Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier Science Publishers; 1992. deficit in procedural learning in children and adolescents with autism:
p. 455–76. Implications for cerebellar contribution. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2000; 6:
Gidley Larson JC, Mostofsky SH. Motor deficits in autism. In: Tuchman R, 752–9.
Rapin I, editors. Autism: a neurological disorder of early brain Mostofsky SH, Lasker AG, Cutting LE, Denckla MB, Zee DS. Oculomotor
development. London: MacKeith Press; 2006. abnormalities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a preliminary
Happé F, Frith U. The weak coherence account: detail-focused cognitive study. Neurology 2001; 57: 423–30.
style in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 2006; 36: 5–25. Mostofsky SH, Cooper KL, Kates WR, Denckla MB, Kaufmann WE.
Hazlett HC, Poe M, Gerig G, Smith RG, Provenzale J, Ross A, et al. Smaller prefrontal and premotor volumes in boys with attention-deficit/
Magnetic resonance imaging and head circumference study of brain size hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 52: 785–94.
in autism: birth through age 2 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62: Mostofsky SH, Newschaffer CJ, Denckla MB. Overflow movements predict
1366–76. impaired response inhibition in children with ADHD. Percept Mot
Herbert MR, Ziegler DA, Makris N, Filipek PA, Kemper TL, Normandin JJ, Skills 2003; 97: 1315–31.
et al. Localization of white matter volume increase in autism and Mostofsky SH, Dubey P, Jerath VK, Jansiewicz EM, Goldberg MC,
developmental language disorder. Ann Neurol 2004; 55: 530–40. Denckla MB. Developmental dyspraxia is not limited to imitation in
Jansiewicz EM, Goldberg MC, Newschaffer CJ, Denckla MB, Landa R, children with autism spectrum disorders. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2006;

Downloaded from brain.oxfordjournals.org by guest on October 14, 2010


Mostofsky SH. Motor signs distinguish children with high functioning 12: 314–26.
autism and Asperger’s syndrome from controls. J Autism Dev Disord Reich W, Welner Z, Herjanic B. The diagnostic interview for children and
2006; 36: 613–21. adolescents-revised. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems; 1997.
Kinney HC, Brody BA, Kloman AS, Gilles FH. Sequence of central nervous Reiss AL, Hennessey JG, Rubin M, Beach L, Abrams MT, Warsofsky IS,
system myelination in human infancy. II. Patterns of myelination in et al. Reliability and validity of an algorithm for fuzzy tissue
autopsied infants. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1988; 47: 217–34. segmentation of MRI. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1998; 22: 471–9.
Klein C, Wendling K, Huettner P, Ruder H, Peper M. Intra-subject Rogers SJ, Bennetto L, McEvoy R, Pennington BF. Imitation and
variability in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry pantomime in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum
2006; 60: 1088–97. disorders. Child Dev 1996; 67: 2060–73.
Klimkeit EI, Mattingley JB, Sheppard DM, Lee P, Bradshaw JL. Motor Shah A, Frith U. Why do autistic individuals show superior performance
preparation, motor execution, attention, and executive functions in on the block design task? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1993; 34: 1351–64.
attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychol Ullman MT. Contributions of memory circuits to language: the
2005; 11: 153–73. declarative/procedural model. Cognition 2004; 92: 231–70.
Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a Walenski M, Tager-Flusberg H, Ullman MT. Language in autism.
revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals In: Moldin SO, Rubenstein JLR, editors. Understanding autism: from
with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord basic neuroscience to treatment. Boca Raton, FL: Taylor and Francis
1994; 24: 659–85. Books; 2006. p. 175–203.
Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EHJr, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, et al. Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children – Third Edition.
The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: a standard San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1991.
measure of social and communication deficits associated with the Wechsler D. Wechsler individual achievement test. San Antonio, TX:
spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2000; 30: 205–23. Psychological Corporation; 1992.
Mandelbaum DE, Stevens M, Rosenberg E, Wiznitzer M, Wechsler D. Wechsler individual achievement test – Second Edition.
Steinschneider M, Filipek P, et al. Sensorimotor performance in San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2001.
school-age children with autism, developmental language disorder, or Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children – Fourth Edition.
low IQ. Dev Med Child Neurol 2006; 48: 33–9. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2003.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi