Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 11

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

1976, Vol. 34, No. 3, 394-404

Test of Attentional and Interpersonal Style


Robert M. Nideffer
University of Rochester
Several studies are reviewed that focus on the development and validation of
the Test of Attentional and Interpersonal Style (TAIS). The TAIS was de-
veloped in an attempt to respond to the criticisms -that assessment procedures
(a) have little predictive utility, (b) fail to lead to the selection of particular
treatment approaches, (c) do not take into account situational variables, and
(d) are not based an a sound theoretical or conceptual base. A summary of
research efforts indicates that the test has good test-retest reliability as well as
some construct and predictive validity. An advantage of the TAIS relative to
other psychological measures is that its test profile describes particular atten-
tional and interpersonal characteristics. These characteristics are operationally
definable, and they permit prediction of performance across a variety of life
situations.

Over the past decade increasing criticism mand for treatment and research productiv-
has been leveled at existing assessment in- ity increase, fewer individuals are willing or
struments. The reliability, validity, and pre- able to spend time developing new assessment
dictive utility of test results have been ques- techniques. Clinical psychologists are finding
itoned by some of the foremost psychologists themselves forced either to get involved in the
in the country (McClelland, 1973; Meehl, delivery of health care services or to engage
1954; Rotter, 1973; Wallace, 1966). One in a research program with more immediate
major concern is that traditional assessment payoff. This is particularly disheartening be-
procedures have been oriented to the mea- cause unless some methods are developed to
surement or inference of needs and/or traits select and evaluate different kinds of treat-
and have not sufficiently taken into account ment approaches to different kinds of clients
the many situational variables that influence and unless some unifying theoretical con-
behavior. Although psychologists are aware structs emerge, clinical psychology cannot
of these shortcomings, they have done little expect to advance as a science.
to correct them. Notwithstanding the difficulties in assessing
Rotter (1973), writing about the future of the environmental, interpersonal, and intra-
clinical psychology^ cites several reasons for personal variables that influence an individu-
the failure to develop more effective assess- al's behavior, some concrete suggestions have
ment devices. The proliferation of treatment been offered. Illustratively, it has been ar-
procedures in psychology, the obvious im- gued that we should define variables more
portance of situational factors in predicting explicitly to provide more structure for cli-
behavior, and the lack of a good unifying ents and sample the behavioral criteria we
theory have resulted in a stimulus jungle. As are attempting to measure. Wallace (1966)
the complexity of the problem and the de- refers to this as assessing the behavioral
competency of the individual in particular
situations. Although this is important, we
The author is indebted to Emory Cowen of the must also be able to generalize lest we end
University of Rochester for his editorial assistance,
to Tessa Bensew, Walter Keller, Dianne Sherwood, up measuring literally thousands of behav-
and Thomas Yock of the University of Rochester, ioral competencies that individuals must have
and to Arthur Wiens of the University of Oregon to function effectively in complex job or life
Medical School for their assistance in data collec- situations.
tion.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Robert M. The concept of personality traits can be
Nideffer, Department of Psychology, University of seen as one way of identifying individual
Rochester, Rochester, New York 14627. characteristics that allows for the prediction
394
TEST OF ATTENTIONAL AND INTERPERSONAL STYLE 395

of behavior across situations. Unfortunately, A requirement for a test's predictive utility


trait concepts, used in the past, have failed is that the variables it presumes to measure
to provide much predictive validity (Mischel, be directly and unambiguously translatable
1968). McClelland (1973) has suggested that to particular interpersonal or environmental
psychologists identify and assess competen- situations. Thus, we should be able to look
cies that are involved in "clusters of life out- at a situation and assess the strength of its
comes." Although he does not use the word demand for "hysterical," "schizophrenic," or
trait, he implies that some of these compe- "self-actualized" behavior. Unfortunately, be-
tencies could be identified as "personality cause most clinical and personality variables
variables" (e.g., patience). McClelland differs have surplus meaning, this is a very difficult
from prior approaches by emphasizing situa- task.
tional analyses that can help to locate gen- Given this way of thinking, a logical first
eralizable competencies. He also identifies step in developing a useful test should be to
interpersonal skills (e.g., communication identify variables which, as McClelland sug-
skills) and "operant thought patterns" (e.g., gests, are important across a variety of life
need achievement) as two prime areas to ex- situations and are as free as possible of am-
amine for generalizability. biguity and surplus meaning. Since most of
The arguments thus far reviewed lead to the situations to which we ultimately want to
the position that assessment devices should predict involve interpersonal interactions, this
be (a) as situation specific as possible (e.g., is an area of obvious importance. Beyond
questions should be phrased to reflect actual the interpersonal area, however, there is a
behavior in particular settings), (b) designed large body of theoretical and empirical litera-
in unambiguous ways (i.e., the examinee ture suggesting that knowledge of an indi-
should know what is being measured and the vidual's attentional processes is critical to
relevance of these measures to his or her life understanding and predicting his or her per-
[Wallace, 1966]), and (c) useful, subject to formance.
evaluation, based on good theoretical con-
structs, and lead to prescriptive statements. Attention and Performance
Meeting these requirements will make it The importance of attention for under-
easier to determine the usefulness of assess- standing and predicting behavior has long
ment devices in selecting and evaluating been emphasized in psychology. Attentional
treatments (Rotter, 1973). Finally, which- constructs have formed the basis of theories
ever competencies, traits, or personality varia- designed to explain phenomena ranging from
bles are measured should be generalizable to schizophrenia (Shakow, 1962) to some al-
clusters of life situations (McClelland, 1973). tered states considered to reflect "higher lev-
els of consciousness" (Nideffer, 1974). It is
The Need for Unambiguous Constructs hard to imagine a variable more central to
The criticisms mentioned, particularly the performance than the ability to direct and
lack of predictive utility associated with most control one's attention (Nideffer, in press).
assessment instruments, underlie the present Attention can be conceptualized on at
attempt to develop a behavioral measure with least two dimensions: breadth of focus and
good predictive utility. Some past difficulties direction. Attention can vary from very nar-
in using traditional assessment devices predic- rowly focused (filtering out a great deal of
tively result from limits in their interpreta- information) to very broad (Cromwell, 1968;
tion (i.e., in applying their implied con- Easterbrook, 1959; Wachtel, 1967). We can
structs). For example, scales labeled hysteria, also attend to external (environmental) or
psychesthenia, and ego strength often mean internal (thoughts, feelings) stimuli (Heil-
different things to different people. Because brun, 1972; Shakow, 1962). Most people
they all typically include so many elements, maintain a balance between these two direc-
it is difficult to make reliable predictions of tions, shifting from one to the other. Figure
a person's behavior from them (Jackson, 1 presents a way of conceptualizing these two
1971). attentional dimensions. At any single time,
396 ROBERT M. NIDEFFER

EXTERNAL
Preoccupied with environmental stimuli.
Responses occur without reflection.
Reinforcement comes from the environ-
ment. Behavior is stimulus-response
pairing at the extreme there is almost a
complete external locus of control

^peractive 2 year old. Color Intellectually bankrupt, concrete.


responses and quick RT s on the Few highly valued objects for
torschach. motivators and reinforcers.
Artists, actors Computer programmer,
Cheerleaders Technical School, Jeweler

BROAD NARROW
When effective integrative how- Persistent, dedicated, focused.
ever, may become overloaded, May be underinclusive, lack
confused by environment. ability to generalize.
Broad internal leads to theoreti- May appear rigid and inflexible
cal analytical thought processes.
Can be overideational.

Humanities and Mathematical theorists,


Social Sciences Sciences, Theoretical
physics.
Philosophy

Overideational Religious Fanatic

INTERNAL
Internal locus of control in that external
controls are not readily apparent. May
be withdrawn, cognitive reinforcers are
most potent. May tune out environment
and have difficulty expressing affect.

FIGURE 1. Theoretical consequences of particular attentional styles.

attention can be described on the basis of velopment, (b) group difference studies, (c)
both its width and direction. construct validity, (d) predictive validity,
Conceptually, it is relatively simple to ap- and (e) a discussion and summary.
ply the attentional dimensions of width and
direction to specific environmental situations.
TEST DEVELOPMENT
Conducting sensitivity training, for example,
demands a broad external focus, whereas The Test of Attentional and Interpersonal
working out a computer program calls for a Style (TAIS) was developed using college
much narrower internal focus. Attentional students as subjects. On the basis of the theo-
competencies, such as controlling width and retical formulations of Shakow (1962), Crom-
direction, should change adaptively as a func- well (1968), and Nideffer (1974), and the
tion of specific environmental demands. clinical observations of MacKinnon and
Knowledge of attentional abilities and en- Michels (1971) and Frank (1961), 17 dif-
vironmental demands should increase the ac- ferent aspects of attentional and interpersonal
curacy of prediction of task success or failure. behavior were identified. Following sugges-
The remainder of this paper provides a tions made by McClelland (1973), Wallace
description of the development and validation (1966), and Jackson (1971), an attempt was
of the Test of Attentional and Interpersonal made to develop a pool of unambiguous items,
Style (TAIS). The presentation is divided reflecting behavioral experiences, in the 17
into the following five sections: (a) test de- areas considered important for predicting
TEST OF ATTENTIONAL AND INTERPERSONAL STYLE 397

performance and making specific treatment Table 2 reports the number of items on
recommendations. each TAIS subscale and the amount of item
Of the 17 TAIS variables, 6 reflect atten- overlap between scales. There is a consider-
tional processes, 2 reflect behavioral and able overlap between two scales measuring
cognitive control, and 9 describe interpersonal effective broad external and internal atten-
style. Table 1 defines the TAIS subscales. tion (BET and BIT) and the one that mea-
An item analysis procedure based on a sures the amount of information an individual
sample of 302 undergraduates was used to processes (INFP).
develop the 17 rationally defined TAIS sub- Table 3 presents the TAIS interscale cor-
scales. Each subscale item was correlated with relations. These range from .01 to .80 with a
its total subscale score, correcting for the median of .28. Although it is conceptually
item's inclusion in the total score. Items that useful to think of amount of information
did not exceed a probability value of .01 processed independently of attentional width,
were dropped. This procedure identified 144 it is unreasonable to assume that these varia-
items belonging to the 17 subscales. bles are, in fact, unrelated. Some of the over-

TABLE 1
THE TEST or ATTENTIONAL AND INTERPERSONAL STYLE (TAIS) SCALES

Scale Abbreviation Description

Broad external attentional focus BET High scores on this scale are obtained by individuals who describe
themselves as being able to effectively integrate many external
stimuli at one time.
Overloaded by external stimuli OET The higher the score, the more individuals make mistakes because
they become confused and overloaded with external stimuli.
High scores indicate that individuals see themselves as able to
Broad internal attentional focus BIT effectively integrate ideas and information from several different
areas. They see themselves as analytical and philosophical.
The higher the score, the more mistakes individuals make because
Overloaded by internal stimuli 01T they confuse themselves by thinking about too many things at
once.
The higher the score, the more effective individuals see themselves
Narrow attentional focus NAR with respect to being able to narrow their attention when they
need to (e.g., to study or read a book).
A high score on this scale indicates that the individuals make
Reduced attentional focus RED mistakes because they narrow their attention too much.
Information processing INFP High scorers tend to process a great deal of stimulus information.
Their perceptual-cognitive worlds are busy.
Behavior control BCON A high score indicates that individuals tend to be somewhat im-
pulsive. In addition, they engaged in behavior that could be
considered antisocial, though not necessarily harmful.
Control scale CON A high score indicates that individuals are in control of most of the
situations, interpersonal and otherwise, they find themselves in.
It also means they attempt to gain control of situations.
Self-esteem SES The higher the score, the more highly the individuals think of
themselves.
Physical orientation P/O A high score indicates the individual participates in and enjoys
competitive athletics.
Obsessive OBS A high score indicates the person has a tendency to ruminate and
worry about one particular thing without any real resolution or
movement.
Extroversion EXT A high score indicates the individual is warm, outgoing, needs to
be with other people, is the life of the party.
Introversion INT A high score indicates the person likes to be alone, enjoys quiet
thoughtful times, and avoids being the center of attention.
Intellectual expression 1EX A high score indicates that individuals express their thoughts and
ideas to other people.
Negative affective expression NAE A high score indicates that individuals express their anger and
negative feelings to others.
Positive affective expression PAE A high score indicates the individuals express their feelings of affec-
tion to others in both physical and verbal ways.
398 ROBERT M. NIDEFFER

TABLE 2
ITEM OVERLAP ON TAIS SUBSCALES

Total no
Scale BET OET BIT OIT NAR RED INFP BCON CON SES P/O O B S EXT INT IEX NAE PAE items

BET — 0 1 0 0 0 4 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 6
OET — 0 0 -1 0 0 2 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 12
BIT — 0 0 -2 6 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 8
OIT — -1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 9
NAR — 4 -2 0 0 0 0 3 0 0 0 0 0 11
RED — -4 0 0 0 0 6 0 0 0 0 0 15
INFP — 0 0 1 0 2 1 0 0 0 0 19
BCON • — 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 3 0 13
CON — 4 4 0 1 0 4 3 0 20
SES — 2 0 3 0 3 1 1 18
P/O — 0 1 0 0 0 0 7
OBS — 0 0 0 0 0 9
EXT — 0 1 0 3 14
INT — -1 0 -1 11
IEX — 1 0 8
NAE — 0 9
PAE — 9
Total no.
items 6 12 8 9 11 15 19 13 20 18 7 9 14 11 8 9 9

Note. TAIS = Test of Attentional and Interpersonal Style.

lapping items on the three scales are as fol- Overlap, where it occurs, obviously acts to
lows: "All I need is a little information and I inflate interscale correlations.
can come up with a large number of ideas" Given item overlap and some high TAIS
(BIT); "Even when I am involved in a game interscale correlations, questions can be raised
or sport my mind is going a mile a minute" about the independence of the 17 variables.
(BIT, INFP); "It is easy for me to focus on Jackson (1971) has suggested one method of
a number of things at the same time" (BET, assessing subscale independence. The mean
INFP); "I am good at quickly analyzing com- correlation (corrected for bias) between sub-
plex situations around me such as how a play scale items and total subscale score is com-
is developing in football or which of four or puted. The remainder of the test items are
five kids started a fight" (BET, INFP). then correlated with total subscale scores to

TABLE 3
TAIS INTERSCALE CORRELATIONS (n =•= 230)

Scale BET OET BIT OIT NAR RED INFP BCON CON SES P/O OBS EXT INT IEX NAE PAE

BET -.26 .52 - . 2 6 .23 -.28 .68 -.15 .51 .43 .29 -.28 .33 -.15 .18 -.11 .35
OET — -.38 .71 - . 6 0 .52 -.22 .63 -.26 -.41 -.22 .39 -.05 .20 -.13 .25 -.13
BIT — -.23 .34 -.48 .80 -.14 .58 .59 .28 -.37 .39 -.10 .47 -.02 .33
OIT AQ
.55 -.16 .54 -.14 -.41 -.13 .36 -.13 .32 -.06 .36 -.21
NAR -.07 .14 -.47 .19 .30 .16 .08 .05 -.07 .14 -.22 .14
RED — -.42 .25 -.28 -.44 -.09 .77 -.16 .21 -.24 .12 -.18
INFP —. .05 .61 .58 .30 -.39 .52 -.16 .45 .02 .46
BCON — .05 -.27 .01 .07 .02 .14 -.01 .55 -.10
CON •— .64 .58 -.27 .51 -.08 .56 .28 .35
SES — .50 -.30 .64 -.36 .54 -.13 .56
P/O — .03 .43 -.15 .07 -.08 .24
OBS — -.10 .12 -.16 .03 -.09
EXT — -.48 .43 -.01 .77
INT — -.11 .17 -.66
IEX — .28 .27
NAE -.09
PAE

Note. TAIS = Test of Attentional and Interpersonal Style.


TEST OF ATTENTIONAL AND INTERPERSONAL STYLE 399
TABLE 4 establish norms for the instrument (Nideffer,
COMPARISON OF IRRELEVANT VERSUS PREDICTED Note 2).
ITEM-TOTAL SCALE CORRELATIONS IN THE
TAIS ITEM POOL There were significant differences between
males and females on five of the 17 TAIS sub-
% of items scales. Males described themselves as (a) bet-
Scale items not scored
mean with correla- ter able to broaden and internally focus at-
Scale No. items correlation tion £ X tention, (b) better able to narrow attention
BET 6 .60 .000 when it is required, (c) more physically ori-
OET 12 .58 .015 ented, (d) more intellectually expressive, and
BIT 8 .55 .000
OIT 9 .59 .007 (e) less expressive of positive affect. These
NAR 11 .46 .007 results are presented in Table 5.
RED 15 .42 .022 The differences between males and females
INFP 19 .40 .016
BCON 13 .45 .000 can be explained on the basis of social learn-
CON 20 .36 .016 ing discrepancies. Men are typically described
SES 18 .44 .015
P/O 7 .71 .000 as more physically oriented, more openly
OBS 9 .45 .000 competitive, and less expressive of positive
EXT 14 .47 .007 affect than females. Thus, differences on these
INT 11 .53 .000
IEX 8 .54 .000 interpersonal scales are not surprising. With
NAE 9 .59 .000 respect to the attentional differences found, it
PAE 9 .55 .000
can be postulated that the need to be in con-
Note. TAIS = Test of Attentional and Interpersonal Style. trol and competitive causes men to make
more use of focused (narrow) and more ana-
lytical (broad-internal) attentional processes.
determine the percentage that correlate with
Such an explanation assumes that attentional
the score as high or higher than the mean
processes can be modified through learning.
for the subscale items. Table 4 presents mean
From a therapeutic point of view, one would
item-scale correlations for 17 TAIS scales
and for each scale indicates the percentage of
nonsubscale items that exceed the mean item-
scale r. The highest percentage of overlap is TABLE 5
only .022 (three items). TAIS BETWEEN-GROUP COMPARISONS

Male vs.female
Test-Retest Reliability introductory
psychology
Police applicants
vs. male college
students students
TAIS test-retest reliabilities based on a 2-
Scale ta 2Qb ta 2Qb
week interval were studied for introductory
psychology students (45 males and 45 fe- BET -1.19 >.20 2.73 <01
OET
males). Correlations ranged from a low of .60 BIT -1.88 <.10 -6.68 <.002
2.64 <.01 1.51 >.10
on the obsessive scale to .93 on physical ori- OIT -.95 >!20 -6.55 <.002
entation, with a median of .83 (Wolfe & Ni- NAR 2.20 <.05 5.42 <.002
deffer, Note 1). RED .82 >.20 -3.14 <.002
INFP -.72 >.20 2.21 <.05
BCON .91 >.20 -5.01 <.002
GROUP DIFFERENCE STUDIES CON .97 >.20 2.50 <.02
SES 1.05 >.20 3.99 <.002
An examination of response differences on P/O 3.02 <.01 3.82 <.002
OBS 0.00 >!80 -.58 >.50
the 17 TAIS subscales was examined by com- EXT -.83 >.20 2.87 <.01
paring the scores of 74 male and 123 female INT 1.45 >.10 -6.32 <.002
introductory psychology students. Students
IEX 2.00 <'.O5 -2.40 <.02
NAE .32 >.50 -3.69 <.002
volunteered to take the TAIS to fulfill a PAE 2.39 <.02 4.80 <.002
course requirement for participation in some
Note. TAIS = Test of Attentional and Interpersonal Style.
experimental studies. The test was admin- * A negative t value indicates that the second group men-
tioned had a higher score on the particular scale than the first
istered to groups of subjects (35 to 50), who group (e.g., male college students scored higher on OET than
police applicants).
were told that the data were being used to b
2Q indicates that the test for significance was two-tailed.
400 ROBERT M. NIDEFFER

hope that this is the case; however, these to select treatment approaches, the TAIS
changes remain to be demonstrated. scale scores must be able to differentiate reli-
Results of a second group difference study ably between individuals and groups of indi-
are also presented in Table 5. Nideffer and viduals. Data just presented suggest that
Wiens (Note 3) compared the scores on the groups who are identifiably different on the
17 TAIS subscales of 60 white male police basis of sex or some occupational and/or atti-
applicants in a large metropolitan city with tudinal variable(s) do respond differently to
those of 97 male college students. The two TAIS items. The question of task relevance of
groups differed significantly on 15 of the 17 these differences depends on both the con-
subscales. Differences that occurred indicated struct and predictive validity of the instru-
police applicants described themselves as more ment.
in control of both attentional and interper-
sonal processes, more extroverted, less intel- CONSTRUCT VALIDITY
lectually expressive, less expressive of nega- Construct validity has been examined by
tive affect, and more expressive of positive correlating TAIS scale scores with the same
affect. individuals' scores on other psychological in-
The preceding findings seem to be logically struments. Nideffer and Wiens (Note 3) cor-
and internally consistent if it is concluded related the TAIS scores for a group of 60
that, as a function of interests and job de- police applicants with scores on the MMPI,
mand, characteristic applicants for police California F Scale, Rotter Internal-External
training should exhibit greater control than (I-E) Scale, Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale
a group of college males. A potential con- (TMAS), Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale
found exists, however, since police applicants (WAIS), and the Maudsley Personality In-
believed the test data would have some influ- ventory (MPI). McPherson and Nideffer
ence on their selection for training. In con- (Note 4) compared scores of 28 female under-
trast, college students had no reason to be- graduates on the TAIS with their scores on
lieve that their lives would be affected by the Personal Orientation Inventory (POI;
their test scores. This difference suggests that Shostrom, 1966) and their scores on the
it is appropriate to speculate about the ex- State-Trait Anxiety Index (Spielberger, Gor-
tent to which police applicants may have such, & Lushene, 1970). Finally, Wolfe and
faked test scores to appear more competent Nideffer (Note 1) correlated the TAIS scores
and effective. Although this question has not of 83 introductory psychology students with
been assessed directly, some indirect evidence scores on the State-Trait Anxiety Index
suggests that faking does not account for the (STAI).
observed group differences. Looking first at the TAIS attentional
A large body of experimental literature ex- scales, Table 6 indicates that the predicted
amines relationships between scores on vari- relationship between attentional processes and
ous personality measures and an individual's level of arousal is reflected in the TAIS at-
tendency to give socially desirable responses tentional subscales. We know, for example,
(Edwards, 1957). The Minnesota Multiphasic that as arousal increases, attention narrows
Personality Inventory (MMPI) K scale has and effective cue utilization decreases (Easter-
been found to correlate between .69 and .81 brook, 1959). Thus, anxious individuals de-
with various measures of social desirability, scribe themselves on the TAIS as making
and high K scores are taken to mean that errors of underinclusion. Anxious individuals
respondents are faking well on the test (Dahl- also tend to direct their attention internally
strom & Welsh, 1963). For the applicant sam- and to become overloaded by their own
ple the mean correlation between MMPI K thoughts and feelings. This tendency is re-
and the 17 TAIS scales was .20, suggesting flected in anxious subjects scoring high on
that faking was minimal. the internal overload scale (OIT) and on
To be useful as a predictor of performance, the obsessive subscale (OBS). Relationships
to select individuals for a particular job, or between scores on the TAIS attentional scales
TEST OF ATTENTIONAL AND INTERPERSONAL STYLE 401

TABLE 6 measures of anxiety deal primarily with bod-


RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN THE TAIS AND ily responses (e.g., "I feel tense/' "I have
MEASURES OF ANXIETY frequent headaches"), whereas the TAIS at-
College Introductory
tentional items reflect cognitive and percep-
students Police psychology tual experiences (e.g., "When people talk to
(Pretest) applicants students
me, I find myself distracted by my own
Scale State Trait TMAS State Trait
thoughts and ideas").
BET .09 -.17 -.38** -.13 -.21 There is some content similarity between
OET .05 .48* .41** .33** .31**
BIT -.49* -.60* -.15 -.19 -.29** several of the TAIS interpersonal scales, the
OIT .03 .43* .58** .38** .39** MMPI K and 5 / scales, the Rotter I-E Scale,
NAR .20 -.40* -.22 -.20 -.26* and the MPI neuroticism and extroversion
RED .38* .53* .35** .55** .54**
INFP -.27 -.31 -.14 -.20 -.28** scales. The TAIS scale correlations that may
BCON .22 .63* .41** .23* .26* be artificially inflated due to the content simi-
CON -.09 -.07 -.01 -.19 -.21
SES -.41* -.57* -.35** -.46** -.55**
larity are interpersonal control, self-esteem,
P/O -.08 .04 -.05 -.13 -.09 physical orientation, extroversion, and ex-
OBS .22 .41* .28* .36** .28** pression of positive and negative affect.
EXT -.30 -.21 -.16 -.31** -.35**
INT .29 .24 .38** .26* .37** Overlap between other tests and the TAIS
IEX -.31 -.26 .07 -.20 -.21 interpersonal scales is limited to one or two
NAE .21 .37* .28* .19 .29**
PAE -.32 -.30 -.31* -.27* -.36** items. In addition, each test listed above
overlaps on several TAIS scales because items
Note, n = 28, 60, and 83 for college students, police appli- cover a much broader range of behaviors. For
cants, and introductory psychology students, respectively.
(Abbreviations: TAIS = Test of Attentional and Interpersonal
Style; TMAS = Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale.)
example, the SI scale on the MMPI is com-
* p < .05. posed of items indicating the individual is
**p < . 0 1 .
self-critical, has low self-esteem, shrinks from
responsibility or a crisis, is sensitive to criti-
and measures of anxiety in these three stud- cism, and withdraws.
ies provide some construct validation for the In conclusion, the attentional scales on
TAIS attentional scales. the TAIS appear to measure an aspect of the
Table 7 presents the relationships found individual that is correlated with other mea-
between the TAIS, the California F Scale, sures of personality and arousal but that is
Rotter I-E Scale, MMPI, and the MPI for conceptually distinct. In contrast, there are
the 60 police applicants in Nideffer and some content similarities between TAIS inter-
Wiens's (Note 3) study. This data, along with personal scales and other conceptually re-
the correlations between the TAIS and the lated personality measures. Such overlap is
POI (McPherson & Nideffer, Note 4), pro- not unwanted if it is assumed that the other
vide some construct validation for several instruments reliably measure the same con-
TAIS interpersonal scales. In particular, structs.
TAIS self-esteem correlates .69 with POI self-
regard, TAIS positive affect expression with PREDICTIVE VALIDITY
POI spontaneity (.42), TAIS introversion Two studies have related TAIS scores to
with neuroticism on the MPI (.36) and with actual behavior measures. Nideffer (in press)
spontaneity on the POI ( - . 6 1 ) , TAIS be- correlated a coach's performance ratings of
havior control with K on the MMPI ( — .32), 10 male swimmers (all performing regularly
and extroversion on the TAIS with extrover- in intercollegiate competition) on an 11-item
sion on the MPI (.64) and with social intro- scale with the swimmers' TAIS attentional
version on the MMPI ( — .46). scale scores. Swimmers scoring high on the
In discussing construct validity of the TAIS scale measuring the tendency to make
TAIS it is important to consider the content errors of underinclusion were rated by the
similarity across the various instruments. coach (a) as choking under pressure (r =
Dealing first with the TAIS attentional scales, .75), (b) as falling apart if they make early
there is very little overlap across tests. The performance errors (r=.59)? (c) as having
o

TABLE 7
TAIS CORRELATIONS WITH OTHER INSTRUMENTS, FOR POLICE APPLICANTS (n = 60)

MPI
MMPI
TAIS Calif Neu- Extro- Psy-
scale F ' I-E L F K Hs D Hy Pd Mf Pa Pt Sc Ma Si roticism version choticism

BET .10 -.25* .08 -.27* .25* -.13 -.23 -.17 -.05 -.39** -.15 -.15 -.01 -.01 -.29* -.30* .11 -.04
OET -.00 .35** -.32* .20 -.24 -.07 .04 -.01 .11 .44** .17 .04 -.16 .06 .09 .49** .03 .13 O
BIT -.05 -.30* .06 —.00 .06 -.13 -.27* .00 -.18 —.11 -.02 -.05 —.11 .22 -.50** -.15 .24 -.06
w
W
OIT .16 .44** -.39** .27* -.35** -.07 .19 -.03 .12 .53** .16 .19 —.00 .13 .19 .56** .02 .01
NAR .13 -.12 .10 -.13 .17 -.09 -.08 -.19 -.03 -.28* -.12 -.13 -.04 -.01 -.09 -.26* .01 .02
S
RED .29* .32* -.35** .16 -.32* -.25* .10 -.28* .22 .21 .07 -.05 -.11 .16 .15 .33** .08 -.02
INFP .06 -.15 .10 -.09 -.02 -.12 -.32* .06 —.21 -.06 .02 -.07 -.14 .28* -.45** — .09 .26* —.11
g
BCON .09 .25* -.32* .18 -.32* -.07 —.00 -.09 .17 .27* .10 .04 -.04 .21 —.04 .50** .15 .12
CON .18 -.03 -.20 .06 -.09 -.22 —.15 -.20 .06 .01 —.01 -.07 -.10 .27* -.51** .03 .51** -.07 g
SES -.02 —.24 .11 -.16 .19 -.15 -.19 -.03 .04 -.14 .04 -.07 -.09 .04 _ # 49** -.32* .38** -.02 5
P/O .22 .09 -.09 -.04 .06 —.12 —.20 -.07 -.04 —.18 -.10 .04 -.03 .18 —.48** -.03 .44** —.14 S
i-rj
OBS .11 .20 -.41** .09 -.18 -.10 .09 -.20 .16 .16 .15 .11 .03 .19 .04 .18 .11 .06 W
EXT .15 .07 -.07 -.02 .19 .03 -.11 .02 .15 .07 .13 -.01 .05 .10 -.46** -.12 .64** —.12
INT -.03 .23 -.34** .25* -.37** -.12 .19 .11 —.09 .52** -.01 .08 -.03 .25* .07 .36** -.27* .01
IEX -.03 -.06 -.27 .26* -.19 -.22 -.12 -.21 .14 .23 .18 -.06 —.10 .22 _.44** .08 .50** .05
NAE .14 .12 -.41** .25* -.30* -.09 .02 -.20 .23 .33** .07 -.02 .02 .30* -.24 .35** .35** -.01
PAE .11 —.03 .23 -.04 .26* -.02 -.11 .03 .05 -.23 .13 -.03 .03 -.07 -.35** -.27* .56** -.06

Note. TAIS = Test of Attentional and Interpersonal Style; Calif. F = California F Test; I-E = Rotter Internal-External Scale; MMPI • Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory;
MPI = Maudsley Personality Inventory.
* p < .05.
**p <.01.
TEST OF ATTENTIONAL AND INTERPERSONAL STYLE 403

to work hard for everything they obtain (r extent that it serves the following functions:
= .66), (d) as becoming worried about one (a) shortening treatment duration through
particular thing and being unable to think of rapid accurate identification of salient areas,
anything else (r = .80). In addition, swim- (b) providing clinicians with a conceptual
mers the coach described as being "very in- framework that enables them to select treat-
consistent ranging from looking very good at ment procedures and facilitates understand-
times to being very poor" tended to become ing of the client's behavior, and (c) improves
overloaded with external (r = .60) and in- communication both ways, between clinicians
ternal stimuli (r = .67), to be unable to inte- and patients. The present study summarizes a
grate thoughts and ideas (r = .63), and to be network of research studies constructing and
unable to effectively narrow attention (r = validating a test measure designed to serve
.63). these functions.
McPherson and Nideffer (Note 4) did a The TAIS consists of 144 items selected
second study examining the relationships be- rationally and grouped into 17 attentional and
tween TAIS scores and performance mea- interpersonal subscales. Each of the 17 sub-
sures at a small, women's liberal arts college. scales was considered on the basis of theo-
In this study the instructor, without knowl- retical formulations and/or clinical observa-
edge of TAIS scores, rated 28 students' be- tions to be potentially useful in predicting
havior (on a 20-item scale) in a vocational behavior across a variety of situations. Data
counseling course. These ratings were corre- collected thus far suggest several conclusions
lated with students' TAIS scores. about the TAIS psychometric properties.
A pattern of significant rs between the in-
structor's ratings and TAIS scores was found. 1. Test-retest reliability for the subscales
These rs clustered into two major groups. ranges from .60 to .93, with a median of .83.
Students judged to contribute to class discus- 2. TAIS items correlate highly (median
sion, assume leadership, express personal opin- .53) within scales and moderately across
ions and not back down, and to seek the in- scales, suggesting that the scales are internally
structor out after class to continue discussions consistent.
described themselves on the TAIS as (a) 3. Group difference studies indicate that
able to integrate ideas from different areas the TAIS will differentiate between subjects
effectively, (b) able to avoid errors of under- differing on some major variable, such as sex.
inclusion, (c) processing large amounts of 4. Correlations between TAIS subscales and
information, (d) in control of interpersonal other psychological measures offer some con-
situations, (e) having high self-esteem, (f) struct validation for the following attentional
being extroverted, and (g) expressive intel- subscales: becoming overloaded with internal
lectually and affectively. stimuli, making errors of underinclusion, and
In contrast to the above, students identi- having an effective broad internal focus. Some
fied by the instructor as easily intimidated, construct validity has also been found for
prone to withdrawal, unable to deal with more the following TAIS interpersonal scales: in-
than one topic at a time, and unable to be- terpersonal control, self-esteem, extroversion,
have in the ways prescribed by the course intellectual expressiveness, and the expression
described themselves on the TAIS as (a) of both positive and negative affect.
having low self-esteem, (b) being out of con- 5. Actual behavioral correlates of TAIS
trol of both their attentional processes and scores were found in two studies. In both
interpersonal situations, (c) processing little instances poor attentional control and the
information, and (d) being introverted. tendency to make errors of underinclusion
were found to be associated with performance
DISCUSSION AND SUMMARY
deficiencies.

If assessment is to be effective and clini- A major potential advantage of the TAIS


cally useful, its procedures must change. when compared with other psychological mea-
Clinical assessment makes best sense to the sures is that its test profile describes specific
404 ROBERT M. NIDEFFER

attentional and interpersonal characteristics. search, Minneapolis: University of Minnesota


These characteristics are operationally de- Press, 1963.
Easterbrook, J. A. The effect of emotion on cue
finable and they permit prediction of per- utilization and the organization of behavior.
formance across a variety of life situations. Psychological Review, 1959, 66, 183-201.
Although the present series of studies shows Edwards, A. The social desirability variable in per-
that the TAIS has reasonable test-retest reli- sonality assessment and research. New York:
Dryden Press, 1957.
ability and some construct and predictive
Frank, J. Persuasion and healing. New York:
validity, the measure has not yet been used Schocken Books, 1961.
to predict performance on an a priori basis Heilbrun, A. B. Style of adaptation to perceived
or to evaluate therapy approaches. Future aversive maternal control and internal scanning
research should attempt to assess the TAIS's behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
chology, 1972, 29, 15-21.
predictive utility for anticipating performance Jackson, D. The dynamics of structured personality
and for selecting intervention techniques. To tests. Psychological Review, 1971, 78, 229-248.
date, informal use of the TAIS in this way MacKinnon, R., & Michels, R. The psychiatric inter-
has been promising. view in clinical practice. Philadelphia: Saunders,
1971.
McClelland, D. Testing for competence rather than
REFERENCE NOTES for intelligence. American Psychologist, 1973, 28,
1. Wolfe, R., & Nideffer, R. M. The use of the 1-14.
TAIS and SAT scores to predict grade point aver- Meehl, P. E. Clinical vs. statistical prediction. Min-
age. Paper presented at the annual meeting of neapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1954.
the Genesee Valley Psychological Association, Mischel, W. Personality and assessment. New York:
Rochester, New York, November 1974. Wiley, 1968.
2. Nideffer, R. M. Test of attentional and inter- Nideffer, R. M. Altered states of consciousness. In
personal style: Interpreter's manual. Unpublished L. Wheeler, R. Goodall, & J. Deese (Eds.), Gen-
manual, University of Rochester, 1974. (Avail- eral psychology. New York: Allyn & Bacon, 1974.
able from Department of Psychology, University Nideffer, R. M. The relationship of attention and
of Rochester, River Station, Rochester, New anxiety to performance. Coach and Athlete, in
York 14627.) press.
Rotter, J. B. The future of clinical psychology.
3. Nideffer, R. M., & Wiens, A. The attentional and
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
interpersonal styles of applicants for police train-
1973, 40, 313-321.
ing. Paper presented at the annual convention of
Shakow, D. Segmental set. Archives of General Psy-
the Western Psychological Association, Sacra-
chiatry, 1962, 6, 1-17.
mento, April 1975.
Shostrom, E. L. Manual for the personal orientation
4. McPherson, J., & Nideffer, R. An assessment of inventory. San Diego, Calif.: Educational and
some new directions in vocational counseling with Industrial Testing Service, 1966.
female college students. Manuscript in preparation, Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E.
1975. STAI manual. Palo Alto, Calif.: Consulting Psy-
REFERENCES chologist Press, 1970.
Wachtel, P. Conceptions of broad and narrow at-
Cromwell, R. L. Stimulus redundancy and schizo- tention. Psychological Bulletin, 1967, 68, 417-429.
phrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, Wallace, J. An abilities conception of personality.
1968, 146, 360-375. American Psychologist, 1966, 21, 132-138.
Dahlstrom, W. G., & Welsh, G. S. An MMPI hand-
book: A guide to use in clinical practice and re- (Received October 30, 1975)

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi