Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 90

FOOD CRAVINGS: THE ROLE OF BODY MASS INDEX,

GENDER, AND AFFECT

Bridget L. Tribout, B.A., M.S.(R)

A Dissertation Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate


School of Saint Louis University in Partial
Fulfillment of the Requirements for the
Degree of Doctor of Philosophy

2007

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
UMI Number: 3324229

INFORMATION TO USERS

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy
submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and
photographs, print bleed-through, substandard margins, and improper
alignment can adversely affect reproduction.
In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript
and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized
copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

UMI
UMI Microform 3324229
Copyright 2008 by ProQuest LLC.
All rights reserved. This microform edition is protected against
unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code.

ProQuest LLC
789 E. Eisenhower Parkway
PO Box 1346
Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
FOOD CRAVINGS: THE ROLE OF BODY MASS INDEX,

GENDER, AND AFFECT

Bridget L. Tribout, B.A., M.S.(R)

An Abstract Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate


School of Saint Louis University in Partial
Fulfillment of the Requirements for the
Degree of Doctor of Philosophy

2007

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Abstract

Food cravings, defined as an intense desire to eat a specific food, constitute an

important and often neglected area of research. Research suggests that food cravings

may be associated with gender, body mass index (BMI), and affect and that the

strength of the association between food cravings and affect may vary according to

BMI and gender. Unfortunately, the food craving literature lacks studies that have

systematically examined these associations with instruments with known

psychometric properties. Therefore, a sample of 121 faculty and staff recruited from

a Midwest university participated in a study to test the following hypotheses: 1)

increasing BMI is associated with increases in food cravings, 2) women experience

more food cravings than men, 3) increasing negative affect is associated with

increases in food cravings, 4) BMI moderates the relationship between negative affect

and food cravings such that the relationship between negative affect and food

cravings is expected to be stronger among persons with higher BMIs, and 5) gender

moderates the relationship between negative affect and food cravings such that the

relationship between negative affect and food cravings is expected to be stronger

among women.

Results showed that food cravings were associated with BMI, negative affect,

and gender. For women, increases in BMI as well as increases in negative affect were

associated with increases in food cravings. Women reported greater food cravings

than men, even when menstrual phase was statistically controlled. In addition, the

relationship between negative affect and food cravings appeared to be stronger for

women than for men and for women with higher BMI’s than for women with lower

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
BMI’s. This study was among the first to systematically examine individual

differences in food cravings and associated characteristics, thereby paving the way for

future mechanistic and longitudinal studies. Although not all hypotheses were fully

supported, results suggest that negative affect and food cravings may be related to

weight control problems among women and that food cravings may be related to

weight control problems among men.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
COMMITTEE IN CHARGE OF CANDIDACY:

Assistant Professor Jillon S. Vander Wal


Chairperson and Advisor

Professor Eddie M. Clark

Professor Michael J. Ross

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
TABLE OF CONTENTS

List of Tables........................................................................................................................... iv

CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION.................... 1

CHAPTER 2: REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE


Challenges with Food Craving Research....................................................... 2
Food Cravings and Eating Pathology....................................................... 3
Food Cravings and Body Mass Index.................................... 4
Food Cravings and Affect............................................................................... 6
Food Cravings and Gender............................................................................. 7
Objectives of the Present Study...................................................................... 8

CHAPTER 3: METHODOLOGY
Participants......................................................................................................11
Measures.........................................................................................................11
Procedures........................ 16

CHAPTER 4: RESULTS
Introduction......................................... 21
Food Cravings and BMI................................................................................ 25
Food Cravings and Gender........................................................................... 31
Food Cravings and Negative Affect: The Role of BMI.............................. 33
Food Cravings and Negative Affect: The Role of G ender.........................48

CHAPTER 5: DISCUSSION
Objectives...................................................................................................... 55
Food Cravings and BMI.................................................................................55
Food Cravings and Gender........................................................................... 56
Food Cravings and Negative Affect: The Role of BMI........................ 57
Food Cravings and Negative Affect: The Role of Gender.........................58
Strengths of the Study................. 59
Limitations of the Study................................................................................ 60
Directions for Future Research.....................................................................60
Conclusions.................................................................................................... 61

APPENDICES
Appendix A: Demographic Questionnaire................................................... 62
Appendix B: Validation Questions...............................................................63
Appendix C: Food Craving Inventory......................................................... 64
Appendix D: Eating Inventory...................................................................... 65
Appendix E: Trait Version Food Cravings Questionnaire..........................70
Appendix F: Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Expanded F orm .....74
Appendix G: Short Affect Intensity Scale................................................... 75
Appendix H: Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale......... 76

ii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
REFERENCES....................................................................................................................... 77

Vita Auctoris........................................................................................................................... 80

iii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
LISTS OF TABLES

Table 1: Sample Characteristics.......................................................................................22

Table 2A: Descriptive Statistics for Measures of Food Cravings ..........................23

Table 2B: Descriptive Statistics for Measures of Affect.................................... 24

Table 3A: Partial Correlations between BMI and Measures of Food Cravings for
both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating.............................................26

Table 3B: Partial Correlations between BMI and Measures of Food Cravings for
Men, Controlling for Time since E atin g .......................................................... 26

Table 3C: Partial Correlations between BMI and Measures of Food Cravings for
Women, Controlling for Time since Eating, and Menses Phase ................... 27

Table 4A: Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) for BMI and Measures of Food
Cravings, for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating......................29

Table 4B: Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) for BMI and Measures of Food
Cravings, for Men, Controlling for Time since Eating.................................... 30

Table 4C: Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) for BMI and Measures of Food
Cravings, for Women, Controlling for Time since Eating,
and Menses Phase....................................................................... 30

Table 5: Point by Serial Correlations between Gender and Measures of Food


Cravings for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating.......................31

Table 6: Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) for Gender and Measures of Food


Cravings, for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating......................33

Table 7A: Partial Correlations between Affect and Measures of Food Cravings for
both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating.............................................36

Table 7B: Partial Correlations between Affect and Measures of Food Cravings for
Men, Controlling for Time since Eating........................................................... 36

Table 7C: Partial Correlations between Affect and Measures of Food Cravings for
Women, Controlling for Time since Eating, and Menses Phase.....................37

Table 8A: Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (CESD) for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating............ 39

iv

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 8B. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (PANAS) for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating 40

Table 8C. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (AIM) for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating.............. 41

Table 8D. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (CESD) for Men, Controlling for Time since Eating..........................42

Table 8E: Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (PANAS) for Men, Controlling for Time since Eating........................43

Table 8F: Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (AIM) for Men, Controlling for Time since Eating............................... 44

Table 8G: Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (CESD) for Women, Controlling for Time since Eating and Menses
Phase................................................................................ 45

Table 8H: Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (PANAS) for Women, Controlling for Time since Eating and Menses
Phase.................................................................................................................... 46

Table 81: Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (AIM) for Women, Controlling for Time since Eating and Menses
Phase..................................................................................................................... 47

Table 9A: Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, Gender, and Negative
Affect (CESD) for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating............ 49

Table 9B: Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, Gender, and Negative
Affect (PANAS) for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating.........50

Table 9C: Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, Gender, and Negative
Affect (AIM) for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating............... 51

Table 9D: Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, Gender, and Negative
Affect (CESD) comparing Gender effects, Controlling for Time since Eating
for Males and Time since Eating and Menses Phase for Females.................. 52

Table 9E: Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, Gender, and Negative
Affect (PANAS) comparing Gender effects...................................................... 53

Table 9F: Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, Gender, and Negative
Affect (AIM) comparing Gender effects........................................................... 54

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION

Food cravings, defined as an intense desire to eat a specific food, constitute an

important and often neglected area of research. Research on food cravings has been

hampered by the lack of a universal definition as well as by the use of multiple measures

for which psychometric information may be weak or unavailable. Although patterns of

association among food cravings and body mass index (BMI), gender, and affect have

been reported in the literature, these patterns are evidenced only after assembling reports

from various literatures employing an assortment of instruments and operational

definitions of food cravings. The present study will systematically examine these

associations across a set of food craving measures with known psychometric properties.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER 2: REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE

Challenges with Food Craving Research

Conducting food craving research has been problematic due to the lack of a

universal definition (Weingarten & Elston, 1990). For example, Weingarten and Elston

(1991) defined food cravings in their Craving Questionnaire as “an intense desire to eat a

specific food” (p. 174). Gendall, Joyce, Sullivan, and Bulik (1998) defined a food craving

as “a strong motivation to go out of ones’ way (if necessary) to obtain a specifically

desired food” (p. 353). Hill, Weaver, and Blundell (1991) defined food cravings as “a

strong urge to eat a particular food” (p. 188), and White Whisenhunt, Williamson,

Greenway, and Netemeyer (2002) defined food cravings in their research as “an intense

desire to consume a particular food (or food type) that is difficult to resist” (p. 109). A

common discrepancy among food craving definitions is the use of ambiguous references

regarding the intensity of the craving such as, “go out of one’s way,” “strong urge,” or

not indicating an intensity level at all (Cepeda-Benito, Fernandez, & Moreno, 2003;

Gendall et al., 1998; Lafay et al., 2001).

Varying definitions of food cravings have been found to significantly influence

prevalence rates. For example, Gendall and colleagues (1997) found that when

participants were asked if they had ever experienced a food craving, 58% said yes;

however, fewer participants, as low as 4%, reported experiencing cravings when they

were presented with more detailed definitions such as, have you experienced a food

craving that was “moderate” to “strong” in intensity, or have you experienced an “intense

craving” and experienced “anxiety or discomfort when abstaining from eating the craved

food” (p. 67). Although food craving definitions vary, there are some similarities. The

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
majority of food craving definitions in the literature indicate that food cravings involve a

desire to consume a particular type of food. The present study will use Weingarten and

Elston’s (1991) definition of food cravings, “an intense desire to eat a specific food”

(p.174).

The lack of a universal definition of food cravings makes the objective of

measuring this construct a difficult task. Food cravings are often measured using self-

report questionnaires. These self-report instruments vary in the way they measure food

cravings; for example, some use multiple item instruments with Likert scales and others

use visual analogue scales (VAS) to quantify and describe participants’ food cravings. In

addition, researchers often develop their own food craving measures and/or have their

participants keep cravings journals (Gendall et al., 1998; Hill et al., 1991). The reliability

o f these measures varies as well. Several of the multi-item questionnaires, such as the

Food Craving Inventory (FCI; White et al., 2002) and the Eating Inventory (El; Stunkard

& Messick, 1985), have been found to have adequate internal consistency and test-retest

reliability. However, other measures, such as the Craving Questionnaire (Harvey, Wing,

& Mullen, 1993) and the Conceptual Food Craving Scale (Hill et al., 1991), are lacking

psychometric data on their reliability and validity. The lack of a universal definition of

food cravings, coupled with various methods of measurement, hamper further study of

this construct.

Food Cravings and Eating Pathology

Despite these research challenges, studies suggest that most individuals

experience food cravings (Parker, Kamel, & Zellner, 2003); however, some research

suggests that individuals with eating pathology may experience food cravings differently

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
than those without pathological eating behaviors. For example, food cravings have been

found to be antecedents to binge eating for individuals with bulimia nervosa and binge

eating disorder (Gendall et al., 1998; White & Grilo, 2005). In a study investigating cue-

elicited food cravings, researchers found a significant association between cue-elicited

food cravings and binge eating behaviors (Sobik, Hutchison, & Craighead, 2005). Sobik

and colleagues (2005) found that as female participants’ cravings increased after the food

cue, so did the frequency of binge eating behaviors they reported. Research has also

revealed that individuals who binge eat are more likely to experience food cravings when

they are not hungry compared to people who do not engage in binge eating behaviors

(Mussell, et al., 1996; Waters, Hill, & Waller, 2001). For example, Waters et al. (2001)

revealed that participants were more likely to report binge eating after experiencing a

food craving when they were more tense but less hungry. However, Sobik et al. (2005)

reported that female participants with more tension had lower BMIs. They suggested that

this finding may imply that women who experience more tension in the presence of food

may engage in more restrictive eating and therefore have lower BMIs. The role of tension

in the relationship between eating pathology and food cravings is unclear. Furthermore,

Carter and colleagues (1996) found that women with bulimia nervosa felt less confident

in their ability to resist indulging in a food craving than women who did not have bulimia

nervosa.

Food Cravings and Body Mass Index

In addition to pathological eating behaviors, research suggests that there may be

an association between experiencing food cravings and having a higher BMI. Sobik et al.

(2005) suggest that the association between food cravings and BMI may differ for men

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
and women. In their study, they found that female participants reported experiencing

more food cravings and having higher BMIs than male participants. Lafay et al. (2001)

discovered that female cravers’ total daily energy intake on average was 110 kcals higher

than female non-cravers. As mentioned earlier, Sobik et al. (2005) discovered that

decreases in cravings were related to both lower BMIs and to lower levels of binge eating

behavior. Moreover, Gendall and colleagues (1998) revealed that food cravers who binge

eat were heavier as well as more likely to meet criteria for bulimia nervosa than cravers

who did not binge. Furthermore, it appears that those with higher BMI’s may crave

certain types of foods such as salty foods, especially those with high flavor intensity

(Rodin, Mancuso, Granger, & Nelbach, 1991).

Research suggests that dieting may also influence the relationship between BMI

and food cravings. For instance, Pelchat (1997) found that dieters had significantly higher

BMIs than non-dieters and reported experiencing more food cravings; however, when

BMI was used as a covariate, differences in food cravings between dieters and non­

dieters become non-significant. Harvey et al. (1993) conducted a study investigating

obese participants with type two diabetes, who were either placed on a low calorie diet

(LCD) or on a very low calorie diet (VLCD). Participants placed on the VLCD compared

to those on the LCD reported a greater decrease in food cravings for the categories of

low-fat protein foods and for complex carbohydrates. Furthermore, there was a tendency

for those on the VLCD to lose more weight than participants assigned to the LCD.

Finally, Delahanty, Meigs, Hayden, Williamson and Nathan (2002) found, in a

population of individuals who were overweight or obese and had diabetes, that binge

eating severity, food cravings, and dietary restraint explained 15% of the variance in

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
BMI. Moreover, when a stepwise regression model was adjusted for age, sex, race-

ethnicity, and diabetic center from which the participants came, binge eating severity and

food cravings remained significant and the effect of dietary restraint was reduced,

suggesting that food cravings account for a significant amount of variance in BMI above

and beyond eating pathology.

Food Cravings and Affect

Researchers have also discovered an association between food cravings and

mood. Research participants with higher levels of psychological distress have been

found to experience more food cravings than research participants reporting lower levels

of psychological distress (Gendall, Sullivan, Joyce, Fear, & Bulik, 1997; Gendall et al.,

1998; Ghiz & Chrisler, 1995). For example, Hill and colleagues (1991) found that cravers

in their study had higher rates of boredom and anxiety and that they were more likely to

report a dysphoric mood prior to experiencing a food craving than were non-cravers.

Gendall et al. (1997) revealed that as cravers reported an increase in the strength and

intensity of their cravings, they also reported experiencing higher levels of anxiety if they

were unable to eat the craved food. In a study investigating self-identified chocolate

addicts, results indicated that those who experienced strong cravings for chocolate had

significantly higher rates of depression, had higher rates of body shape dissatisfaction,

and reported more disordered eating symptomatology than those without strong cravings

for chocolate (Macdiarmid & Hetherington, 1995). Some investigators have also found

that during certain phases of the menstrual cycle, women experience higher rates of food

cravings and negative mood (Dye, Warner, & Bancroft, 1995). For instance, Dye et al.

(1995) discovered that food craving ratings for the premenstrual phase were higher than

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
the postmenstrual phase for all levels of depression reported (i.e. none, mild, moderate,

severe); however, female participants with the most severe ratings of depression had the

highest mean ratings of food cravings for all phases of the menstrual cycle. Furthermore,

Dye and colleagues investigated the effects of relationship satisfaction and stress levels

on women’s food cravings. Their results indicated that women who experienced high

levels of stress and who were involved in an unhappy relationship reported the highest

levels of food cravings. However, women who reported being “very happy” in their

relationship reported the lowest level of food cravings despite their level of stress. It

appears that relationship satisfaction and/or stress may moderate the effects of stress on

women’s food cravings.

Food Cravings and Gender

The experience of food cravings appears to differ between the sexes. Women

have been found to experience food cravings almost twice as often as men (Cepeda-

Benito et al., 2003; Lafay et al., 2001; Pelchat, 1997; Weingarten & Elston, 1991). This

may partially be due to the high rate of women who report experiencing food cravings

during the pre-menstrual phase of their menstrual cycle (Dye et al., 1995). In North

America, women more often than men report craving sweets, in particular chocolate

(Zellner et al., 1999); however, this finding has not been found universally among all

women. Moreover, Lafay et al. (2001) found that the feeling associated with food

cravings most frequently quoted for men (40%) was relaxation and for women (40%) was

tiredness. In addition, women more often than men reported indulging in their cravings

when they were bored or depressed and when they were alone (Lafay et al.* 2001).

Furthermore, some researchers have found a relationship between food cravings and BMI

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
only for women but not for men (Sobik et al., 2005). As mentioned earlier, Lafay et al.

(2001) found that female cravers had higher total daily energy intakes than female non-

cravers. They also discovered that both female and male cravers consumed more simple

sugars and that they derived a higher proportion of their energy intake from between-

meal snacks. Moreover, both male (75.8%) and female (67.6%) cravers reported that their

food cravings were related to a “desire” and not a “need.” In addition, Lafay and

colleagues (2001) revealed that men reported being able to resist cravings more easily

than women. However, it is important to note that more women than men in their study

reported dieting and therefore they may have experienced more cravings due to hunger.

Additionally, because many commonly craved foods (i.e., sweet and savory) are

forbidden on most diets, the women may have craved a greater range of foods.

Furthermore, although men reported feeling more confident in their ability to resist a

craving, both the male and female cravers indicated that they indulged in their cravings

50% of the time (Lafay et al., 2001).

Objectives of the Present Study

The intent of the current study was to examine the relationships between food

cravings, BMI, gender, and affect. Although associations among food cravings and BMI,

gender, and affect have been reported in the literature, these reports have been assembled

from various literatures employing an assortment of instruments and operational

definitions of food cravings. No study has systematically examined these associations

across a single set of food craving measures. Some researchers have suggested that there

is a relationship between food cravings and individuals’ BMIs. Specifically, some

investigators have found that those with higher BMIs tend to experience more food

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
cravings (Gendall et al., 1998). The first objective of this study was to determine the

extent to which food cravings and BMI were associated and to determine whether

persons who were obese had more food cravings than did persons who were overweight

or average weight.

In addition to the association between food cravings and BMI, researchers have

also found an association between food cravings and gender. It appears that men and

women may experience and respond to food cravings differently (Lafay et al., 2001). The

second objective was to measure the strength of the association between food cravings

and gender and to determine whether men and women differed in their food cravings.

Researchers have also found a relationship between food cravings, BMI, and

affect. Investigators have found that individuals with higher BMIs tend to experience

more food cravings (Gendall et al., 1998). Therefore, the third objective was to determine

the strength of the relationship between negative affect and food cravings and to

determine the extent to which BMI moderates this relationship. Furthermore, researchers

have discovered that women, more often than men, tend to associate negative moods with

experiencing food cravings (Lafay et al., 2001). Therefore, the fourth objective was to

determine the extent to which gender moderates the relationship between negative affect

and food cravings.

For the third and fourth objectives, the intent of the study was to investigate the

relationships among the above stated variables utilizing food craving variables that

matched the study’s operational definition of the term, “an intense desire to eat a specific

food” Weingarten and Elston (1991) (p.174). In addition, objectives three and four

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
included an exploratory investigation of an intense desire to eat, including planning food

consumption, preoccupation with food, and the physical state of hunger.

10

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER 3: METHODOLOGY

Participants

Participants were recruited from the faculty and staff population at Saint Louis

University via email. A total of one hundred and eight-five participants completed the

survey; however, 64 did not meet inclusion criteria, which resulted in a final sample size

o f 121 participants. Involvement in the study was voluntary and participants had the

option of being enrolled in a lottery to win one of two $50 gift certificates for gasoline.

Participants interested in enrolling in the lottery were instructed to email their name to

foodcravingresearcher@vahoo.com. This ensured that participants’ names would not be

associated with their responses to the survey. To participate in the study, participants had

to be between the ages of 18 and 65 and could not endorse any of the following: being

pregnant or breastfeeding, gaining or losing 10 or more pounds in the past 60 days, a

medication change within the past 60 days, diagnosis of a major systemic disease (e.g.

heart failure, cancer, autoimmune disease), uncontrolled diabetes, uncontrolled hyper- or

hypothyroidism, history of drug or alcohol abuse, or history of an eating disorder.

Measures

Demographic Questionnaire (Appendix A). On the Demographic Questionnaire,

various demographic characteristics of the participants were gathered including gender,

race, age, and highest level of education completed. In addition, participants were asked

about their dieting status as well as their current height and weight so that body mass

index (BMI; kg/m2) could be calculated. Because menstrual cycles have been found to be

associated with food cravings, women were asked to complete the survey during the two

weeks following the completion of their menses.

11

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Validation Questions (Appendix B). A validation questionnaire was used to ask

participants to what extent they complied with the investigator’s instructions to not eat

two hours before completing the questionnaires. A validation question asked, “When did

you begin completing this survey?” The validation question had four answers from which

participants could choose, including, (a) 1 hour after completing a meal, (b) 2 hours after

completing a meal, (c) 3 hours after completing a meal, and (d) I did not pay attention to

the time o f my food intake prior to completing the survey. Participants who reported

answer choice “d” were not included in analyses. A second validation question asked

female participants the extent to which they followed the directions to complete the

survey during the two weeks following the completion of their menses. The validation

question had four answers from which participants could choose, including, (a) during

the first week following the completion o f my menses, (b) during the second week

following the completion o f my menses, (c) during the third week following the

completion o f my menses, and (d) I did not pay attention to the duration o f time that

lapsed since the completion o f my menses prior to completing the survey.

Food Craving Inventory (Appendix C). The Food Craving Inventory (FCI) was

developed by White, Whisenhunt, Williamson, Greenway, and Netemeyer (2002) to

measure specific food cravings. Craving was defined by White et. al. (2002) as a strong

desire for particular foods that is difficult to resist. This self-report questionnaire consists

of 28 items measuring the frequency of cravings for specific foods. The FCI consists of

four factors or subscales that measure food cravings for high fats, carbohydrates/starches,

sweets, and fast food fats, as well as a total score. There are eight items on the high-fats,

sweets, and carbohydrates/starches scales and four items on the fast-food fats scale.

12

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Responses are provided on 5-point Likert scales that range from 1, never, to 5,

always/almost every day. The FCI is scored by generating means for each factor and the

total score is the mean of all items completed. Previous research indicates that the

internal consistencies for these factors range from .76 (fast food fats) to .93 (high fats;

White et al, 2002). In addition, White et al. (2002) reported that the two week test-retest

reliability for the subscales and total score were moderate ranging from .79

(carbohydrates/starches) to .91 (high fats).

Eating Inventory (Appendix D). Stunkard and Messick (1985) created the Eating

Inventory to assess dimensions of eating behavior. The Eating Inventory is a two-part

questionnaire with 36 true-false items in Part I, and 15 items with responses provided on

several different types of rating scales in Part II. The Eating Inventory has three factors:

(a) Cognitive Restraint o f Eating (21 items), (b) Disinhibition (16 items), and (c) Hunger

(14 items). Stunkard and Messick (1985) reported that the internal consistencies for

Factor I ranged from .79 to .93, for Factor II from .84 to .91, and for Factor III from .83

to .87.

Trait Version Food Cravings Questionnaire (Appendix E). The Trait Version of

the Food Craving Questionnaire (FCQ-T) is a 39-item self-report questionnaire that was

developed by Cepeda-Benito, Gleaves, Williams, and Erath (2000) to measure trait-based

food cravings. Responses are recorded on 6-point Likert scales ranging from 1, never/not

applicable, to 6, always. The FCQ-T measures 9 dimensions of food cravings found in

the research literature including: (a) an intention and plan to eat, (b) expectations of

receiving positive emotional rewards from eating, (c) anticipation of negative feelings

dissipating as a result of eating, (d) potential loss of control over food intake if food is

13

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
eaten, (e) perseverate about food, (f) physical states of craving, (g) feelings possibly

experienced prior or during food cravings or eating, (h) potential cues in the environment

that activate food cravings, and (i) the experience of remorse after undergoing a food

craving or giving into a food craving (Cepeda-Benito, et al., 2000, p. 154).

Cepeda-Benito et al. (2000) reported that the internal consistencies of the subscales

ranged from .81 to .94. In a sample of 109 adults, the FCQ-T three-week test-retest

reliability values ranged from .54 to .86 (Vander Wal, Johnston, & Dhurandhar, 2007).

Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Expanded Form (PANAS-X; Appendix F).

The PANAS-X was developed by Watson and Clark (1994) to measure two dimensions

of affect (i.e. positive and negative) as well as 11 specific affects: Fear, Sadness, Guilt,

Hostility, Shyness, Fatigue, Surprise, Joviality, Self-Assurance, Attentiveness, and

Serenity. This 60-item self-report measure consists of thirteen scales, one for each affect

dimension and one for each of the 11 specific affects. Responses are made on a 5-point

Likert scale ranging from 1, very slightly or not at all, to 5, extremely. In eleven studies

using both college and community samples, the median alpha reliabilities for the 11

specific affect scales were found to be moderate, ranging from .76 (Serenity) to .93

(Joviality; Watson & Clark, 1994).

Short Affect Intensity Scale (Appendix G). The Short Affect Intensity Scale was

developed by Geuens and De Pelsmacker (2002) to measure three areas of affect

including Positive Intensity (i.e., strong feelings of happiness, elation), Negative

Affectivity (i.e., negative emotions such as the feelings of anxiety or tension), and

Serenity (i.e., describing positive affect such as calm or being relaxed). The Short Affect

Intensity Scale is a brief version of the original 40-item Affect Intensity Measure (AIM),

14

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
developed in 1987 by Larsen and Diener. Correlational analyses were conducted between

the original and short version of the scale with three participant samples and the

correlations ranged from .93 to .94. The Short Affect Intensity Scale has 20 items and

consists of 3 scales, one for each of the affect dimensions. Responses are made on a 6-

point Likert scale ranging from 1 ,1 never feel like that, to 6 ,1 always feel like that. In a

study with 880 participants, consisting of college students and a community sample, the

alpha reliabilities for the total score and three affect scales where found to be moderate

ranging from .74 (Positive Intensity) to .88 (Total Score; Geuens & De Pelsmacker,

2002).

Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CESD; Appendix H). The

CESD is a 20-item self-report scale that was developed at the Center for Epidemiological

Studies in 1971 (Radloff, 1977) to measure depressive symptomatology in the general

population. The items on the scale survey somatic complaints, mood, motor functioning,

and interactions with others (Eaton, Muntaner, Smith, Tien, & Ybarra, 2004). The

response values are on a 4-point Likert scale, with response options ranging from 1,

rarely or none o f the time, to 4, most or all o f the time. Scores range from 0 to 60, with

higher scores indicating greater depressive symptomatology. People with scores of 16 or

greater are likely to have depression. Among community samples, internal consistency

estimates have ranged from .80 to .90; test-retest reliability, ranging from two weeks to

one year, has been reported to be between .40 and .70 (Devins et al., 1988; Radloff,

1977).

15

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Procedures

Participants for the study were recruited through email. The email provided them

with a cover letter and recruitment statement about the study as well as a web link for

completing the online survey. The recruitment statement provided information about the

research project, explained that the study was about food cravings, stated that

involvement in the study was voluntary, and made requests for faculty and staff

participation. The recruitment statement also instructed individuals that to participate in

the study, participants must be between the ages of 18 and 65 and must not endorse any

of the following: being pregnant or breastfeeding, gaining or losing 10 or more pounds in

the past 60 days, a medication change within the past 60 days, diagnosis of a major

systemic disease (e.g. heart failure, cancer, autoimmune disease), uncontrolled diabetes,

uncontrolled hyper- or hypothyroidism, history of drug or alcohol abuse, or history of an

eating disorder. In addition, the email explained procedures for completing the online

survey, explained that responses to the survey were anonymous, and identified potential

risks that participants might endure from partaking in the study. This email also

instructed participants to complete the survey two to three hours after they completed a

meal and that snacks should not be consumed between meal completion and completion

of the survey. Female participants were instructed to complete the survey two weeks after

the completion of their menses.

All research surveys were provided online through Test Pilot. Measures provided

on the online survey network were presented in the following order: Food Craving

Inventory, Short Affect Intensity Scale, Eating Inventory, Positive and Negative Affect

Schedule-Expanded Form, Trait Version Food Craving Questionnaire, Center for

16

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale, Demographic Questionnaire, and then the

Validation Question. The measures were placed in this order to prevent participants from

becoming uninterested in completing the surveys due to repeated questions about the

same construct. A randomized or counter-balanced presentation was not possible in the

on-line format.

Results of participants’ responses to the questionnaires were loaded into a SPSS

database created by the primary investigator. Participants were given two weeks to

complete the online survey and to submit it electronically. In addition, they were

instructed to complete the survey without assistance from other individuals and to

complete the measures in the order provided.

Data Management. At the time of data collection, the individual surveys were

reviewed to ensure accuracy and completeness. An electronic copy of the data was stored

on the primary investigator’s personal computer and was password protected. No hard

copy o f the data exists. In addition, no identifying information, such as URLs, were

collected.

Preliminary Analyses. The data were sent by the online survey company to an

SPSS spreadsheet. Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS version 11.5 with a

significance level of p < .05. Descriptive statistics, including means, standard deviations,

skewness, and kurtosis were computed for total scores and subscale scores in order to

describe the data, screen for outliers, check scoring, and ensure that the data did not

violate the assumptions of multivariate statistical techniques. Transformations were

employed as necessary.

17

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Proposed Analyses. To test the first hypothesis that increasing BMI is associated

with increases in food cravings, Pearson and partial correlations were conducted. To

further explore the relationship between BMI and food cravings, a one-way analysis of

covariance (ANCOVA) was used to determine the extent to which obese persons had

greater food cravings than overweight or average weight persons.

To test the second hypothesis that gender is associated with food cravings,

Pearson and partial correlations were conducted. To further explore the relationship

between food cravings and gender, a one-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was

used to determine the extent to which men and women experienced food cravings

differently.

To test the third hypothesis that negative affect is associated with specific food

cravings and multiple characteristics of food cravings, including planning food

consumption, preoccupation with food, and hunger, Pearson and partial correlations were

conducted. In the instance that negative affect and food cravings were significantly

related, a hierarchical regression, controlling for relevant covariates, was conducted to

determine the extent to which BMI moderated the relationship between negative affect

and food cravings using the methodology of Baron and Kenny (1986). Negative affect

was entered into the first step, BMI was entered into the second step, and the interaction

term between negative affect and BMI was entered into the third step. To the extent that

the interaction between negative affect and BMI was significant, the relationship between

negative affect and food cravings was found to be moderated by BMI.

In addition, a hierarchical regression, controlling for relevant covariates, was

conducted to determine the extent to which gender moderated the relationship between

18

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
affect and food cravings. Based on Baron and Kenny’s (1986) methodology for analyzing

dichotomous moderators, food cravings were regressed on negative affect separately for

women and men and the difference in the unstandardized regression coefficients was

tested. A significant difference suggested that gender served as a moderator.

Power Analyses. A power analysis was conducted to determine the requisite

number of subjects. A review of the literature was conducted to generate estimates of

effect size. The most power intensive proposed analysis was the test of moderation. In

this analysis negative affect, BMI, and their interaction term were used to predict food

cravings. A review of the literature showed that negative affect accounts for about 20

percent o f the variance in food craving frequency and intensity (Hill et al., 1991). This

review also showed that BMI accounts for between 3 and 9 percent of the variance in

food craving frequency and intensity (Hill et al., 1991). Assuming a desired power level

of .80, an alpha level of p < .05, that negative affect would account for approximately 20

percent of variance, and that BMI would account for approximately 4 percent of the

variance, a sample size o f 190 participants was found to ensure sufficient power to detect

an interaction effect that accounted for as little as 3 percent of the variance.

In the second moderation analysis, negative affect, gender, and their interaction

term were used to predict food cravings. As no studies had yet examined gender

differences on continuous measures of food cravings, a power analysis was not possible.

However, the assumptions underlying the percent of variance explained by BMI seemed

to provide a reasonable staring point.

To ensure that the sample size was sufficient to detect differences in food

cravings between men and women, a second analysis was conducted. Based on findings

19

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
from Weingarten and Elston (1991), showing that 97% of women versus 68% of men

reported food cravings, it was determined that a sample size of 30 per group would be

sufficient to detect a significant gender difference with a power of .86 and an alpha p <

.05. Power calculations were conducted with the use of SamplePower2.

20

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER 4: RESULTS

Introduction

A total of one hundred and eighty-five participants completed the survey. O f the

185 who participated, 64 were excluded from analyses due to not meeting inclusion

criteria for the study, which resulted in a final sample size of 121 participants. Of the 64

participants who were excluded, one person did not indicate gender, five men and five

women reported not paying attention to the time that transpired between the last time they

ate and when they completed the survey, and 53 women indicated not paying attention to

the stage of their menses at the time they completed the survey. Table 1 shows the

demographic characteristics of the sample which consisted largely of middle aged,

Caucasian women, with at least a four year degree or greater, and who indicated they

were not actively participating in an organized weight loss program. Overall, the majority

of the participants (57.9%) were in the average weight range and approximately 26%

and 15% o f the sample were overweight or obese, respectively.

21

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 1

Sample Characteristics (N = 121)

Variable N (%) or M (SD )

Age 36.7 (9.58)


Gender
Female 96 (79.3)
Male 25 (20.7)

Race
Native American 1 ( 0 .8 )
Asian 2 (1 .7 )
African American 4 (3.3)
Caucasian 105 ( 8 6 .8 )
Hispanic 7 (5.8)
Other 1 ( 0 .8 )

Education
GED/High School Diploma 2 (1 .7 )
1 to 2 years o f college 11(9.1)
3 to 4 years o f college 36 (29.7)
Masters 36 (29.8)
Doctorate or Professional Degree 36 (29.8)
Height (inches) 66.24 (3.37)
Weight (lbs.) 157.73 (38.24)
BMI 25.21 (5.06)
Average weight 70 (57.9)
Overweight 32 (26.4)
Obese 18(14.9)
Weight loss program
Yes 5(4.1)
No 116(95.9)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Descriptive statistics for each measure are presented in Tables 2A and 2B. For the

PANAS-X, two of its subscales, fear and guilt, were found to be positively skewed and

therefore transformations were completed. On average, participants appeared to crave

sweet foods more than other types of food. In addition participants endorsed greater

positive than negative affect.

Table 2 A

Descriptive Statistics for Measures o f Food Cravings

Variable Mean SD Range Alpha

FCI
(1) High Fat 1.57 0.43 1.5 .65
(2) Sweets 2.50 0.73 3.25 .81
(3) Carbohydrates 1.85 0.62 2.63 .79
(4) Fast Food Fats 2.33 0.76 3.00 .69
(5) Total 2.83 0.45 2 .1 1 .87
FCQ-T
( 1 ) Plan 8 .2 1 2.96 13 .81
(2) Positive 12.09 3.59 16 .78
(3) Negative 6.69 2.63 13 .82
(4) Control 13.65 6 .1 0 28 .93
(5) Thoughts 14.04 6.04 35 .93
( 6 ) Hunger 11.34 3.23 16 .67
(7) Emotions 10.40 4.55 19 .93
( 8 ) Environment 12.37 4.19 18 .82
(9) Guilt 7.22 3.14 13 .84
El
(1) Cognitive 8.59 3.56 15 .69
(2) Disinhibition 5.70 2.97 12 .67
(3) Hunger 4.76 3.13 13 .77

Note\ FCI = Food Craving Inventory; FCQ-T = Trait Version o f the Food Craving Questionnaire; El =
Eating Inventory.

23

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 2B
Descriptive Statistics for Measures o f Affect

Variable Mean SD Range Alpha

AIM
(1) Positive 26.60 6.61 37 .91
(2) Negative 2 1 .1 2 S.21 26 .91
(3) Serenity 23.04 4.42 23 .8 8

PANAS-X
(1) Positive 30.59 5.73 31 .8 6

(2) Negative 16.60 5.23 28 .84


(3) Fear 2 .8 6 0.44 1.91 .78
(4) Hostility 10.72 3.53 18 .79
(5) Guilt 2.62 0.58 2.27 .8 8

(6 ) Sad 8.43 3.72 19 .90


(7) Jovial 22.99 5.51 27 .91
( 8 ) Assured 14.67 4.09 22 .81
(9) Attentive 13.78 2.41 12 .79
(10) Shy 5.27 1.95 10 .78
(11) Fatigue 11.03 3.58 16 .89
(12) Serenity 9.36 2.29 11 .77
(13) Surprise 4.86 1.65 6 .67
CESD 8.57 7.98 39 .91

Note: AIM = Short Affect Intensity Scale; PANAS-X = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-expanded
form; CESD= Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Food Cravings and BMI

To test the first hypothesis that increasing BMI is associated with increases in

food cravings, correlational analyses were conducted. To control for participants’

responses regarding how soon after food consumption they completed the survey, which

varied from 1 to 3 hours, and womens’ reports of menstrual stage at the time of survey

completion, which varied from 1 to 3 weeks after menses, partial correlations using the

above responses as covariates were conducted. See Tables 3A-3C.

Table 3A presents partial correlations for both genders combined, controlling for

the time interval between food consumption and survey completion. Correlation analyses

revealed that increases in BMI were significantly correlated with an increase in food

cravings for high fat foods, (r = .35, p < .001). In addition, correlations indicated that

increases in BMI were significantly correlated with an increase in feelings of guilt related

to cravings and/or indulging in the craved food(s), (r = .29,p < .01). However, when

partial correlations were conducted separately for each gender, controlling for time of

food consumption for men, and controlling for time of food consumption and menstrual

stage for women, significant correlations between BMI and high fat food cravings and

between BMI and feelings of guilt related to food cravings were only found for female

participants, (r = .41, p < .001; r = .36,/? < .001), respectively. See Tables 3B and 3C.

25

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 3A. Partial Correlations between BMI and Measures of Food Cravings for both
Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating.__________________________________
Scale Subscales BMI
FCI
High Fats 35***
Sweets -.14
Carbohydrates -.12
Fast Food Fats .07
Total .03
FCQ-T
Plan .03
Positive .09
Negative .13
Lack of control .18
Thoughts -.08
Hunger -.05
Emotions .05
Environment .18
Guilt 29* *
Note\ FCI= Food Craving Inventory; FCQ-T= Trait Version o f the Food Craving Questionnaire.
**p <.01; *** p < .001

Table 3B. Partial Correlations between BMI and Measures of Food Cravings for Men,
Controlling for Time since Eating.
Scale Subscales BMI
FCI
High Fats .04
Sweets -.29
Carbohydrates -.23
Fast Food Fats .32
Total .25
FCQ-T
Plan .04
Positive -.13
Negative .05
Lack of control .20
Thoughts .19
Hunger -.35
Emotions .28
Environment .30
Guilt .09
Note: FCI= Food Craving Inventory; FCQ-T= Trait Version o f the Food Craving Questionnaire.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 3C. Partial Correlations between BMI and Measures of Food Cravings for Women,
Controlling for Time since Eating, and Menstrual Phase.___________________________
Scale Subscales BMI
FCI
High Fats 41 ***
Sweets -.05
Carbohydrates -.13
Fast Food Fats .16
Total .03
FCQ-T
Plan .08
Positive .19
Negative .21
Lack of control .15
Thoughts -.11
Hunger .00
Emotions .12
Environment .14
Guilt .36***
Note: FCI= Food Craving Inventory; FCQ-T= Trait Version o f the Food Craving Questionnaire. ***/?<.001

To further explore the extent to which obese persons had greater food cravings

than overweight or average weight persons, one-way analyses of covariance

(ANCOVA’s) were computed. The ANCOVA’s were used to examine the effect of BMI

on food cravings, with time of food consumption prior to survey completion and stage of

womens’ menstrual cycle at survey completion being treated as covariates. Univariate

analyses o f covariance were conducted for each of the dependent measures of food

cravings. See Table’s 4A-4C.

Table 4A reveals the results of the ANCOVA's for both genders combined,

controlling for the time interval between food consumption and survey completion. A

significant main effect for BMI was found for high fat foods, F(2, 115) = 9.60, p < .001.

Bonferroni post hoc comparisons tests using adjusted means indicated that overweight

and obese individuals experienced greater cravings for high fat foods than average weight

persons (p < .01). A significant main effect for BMI was also found for the lack of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
control subscale of the FCQ-T, which measures possible lack of control over eating if

food is eaten, F(2, 116) = 3.15,/? < .05. However, the Bonferroni pos hoc comparisons

tests were non-significant. Lastly, a significant main effect of BMI was found for the

guilt subscale of the FCQ-T, which measures feelings of guilt related to having food

cravings and/or indulging in food(s) craved, F(2,114) = 5.22, p < .01. Bonferroni post

hoc comparisons tests using adjusted means indicated that obese individuals experienced

greater feelings of guilt associated with food cravings than average weight persons (p <

.01 ).

Tables 4B and 4C present ANCOVA's for hypothesis one for each gender

separately. No significant main effects were found for the ANCOVA’s conducted on the

male sample. For the female sample, when time interval between eating and food

consumption and menstrual phase were used as covariates, a significant main effect for

BMI was found for high fat food cravings, F(2, 90) = 10.90,/? < .001. Bonferroni post

hoc comparisons tests using adjusted means indicated that overweight (/? < .01) and obese

(p < .001) women experienced greater cravings for high fat foods than average weight

women. No other significant main effects were found for BMI and specific food cravings.

A significant main effect for BMI was found for the positive trait subscale of the

FCQ-T, which measures anticipation of positive reinforcement that may result from

eating, F(2, 91) = 5.12,p < .03. Bonferroni post hoc comparisons tests using adjusted

means indicated that obese women experienced more anticipation of positive

reinforcement from eating than average weight women (p < .05). A significant main

effect for BMI was also found for the negative trait subscale of the FCQ-T, which

measures anticipation of relief from negative states as a result of eating, F(2, 91) = 4.22,

28

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
p < .02. Bonferroni post hoc comparisons tests using adjusted means indicated that obese

women reported more anticipation of relief from negative feeling states than average

weight women (p < .05). Lastly, a significant main effect for BMI was found for the guilt

trait subscale of the FCQ-T, F(2, 89) = 6.42,/? < .002. Bonferroni post hoc comparisons

tests using adjusted means indicated that obese women experienced more feelings of guilt

related to their food cravings and/or indulging in their cravings than average weight

women (p < .01).

Table 4A. Analysis o f Covariance (ANCOVA) for BMI and Measures o f Food Cravings, for both Genders,
Controlling for Time since Eating.________________ ________________________________________________
Scale Subscale Average Overweight Obese F
M ±SE M +SE M ±SE
FCI
High Fats 1.43 ± 0.05 1.73 ± 0 .07 1.80 ± 0 .09 9.60***
Sweets 2.52 ± 0.09 2.61 ± 0.13 2.23 ± 0 .17 1.61
Carbohydrates 1.87 ± 0 .07 1.92 ±0.11 1.68 ± 0 .1 4 0.92
Fast Food Fats 2.27 ± 0.09 2.44 ± 0 .14 2.42 ± 0 .18 0.67
Total 2.81 ± 0.05 2.94 ± 0.08 2.77 ± 0.11 1 .2 2
FCQ-T
Plan 8.13 ± 0 .36 8.01 ± 0.53 8.89 ± 0.70 0.58
Positive 11.75 ± 0.43 12.19 ± 0 .64 13.23 ± 0.85 1 .2 0
Negative 6.29 ± 0.31 7.01 ± 0.46 7.67 ± 0.61 2.37
Control 12.48 ± 0.72 15.18 ± 1.07 15.44 ± 1.42 3.15*
Thoughts 14.35 ± 0.74 13.99 ± 1.08 12.94 ± 1.44 0.38
Hunger 11.26 ± 0 .39 11.81 ± 0 .58 10.80 ± 0 .76 0.61
Emotions 10.10 ± 0.55 10.54 ±0.81 11.23 ± 1.08 0.46
Environment 11.62 ± 0 .50 13.22 ± 0.74 13.72 ± 0 .98 2.70
Guilt 6.53 ± 0.37 7.69 ± 0.54 9.00 ± 0.72 5.22**
Note. FCI= Food Craving Inventory; FCQ-T = Trait version o f the Food Craving Questionnaire.
* p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.

29

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 4B. Analysis o f Covariance (ANCOVA) for BMI and Measures o f Food Cravings, for Men,
Controlling for Time since Eating.
Scale Subscale Average Overweight Obese F
M +SE M ±SE M ±SE
FCI
High Fats 1 .5 3 1 0 .2 2 1 .8 4 1 0 .1 7 1 .6 5 1 0 .2 4 0.56
Sweets 2.21 ± 0 .3 7 2 .4 8 1 0 .2 8 1.4910.41 1.98
Carbohydrates 1 .8 7 1 0 .2 9 2.08 1 0 . 2 2 1.51 ± 0.31 1.06
Fast Food Fats 2 .4 1 1 0 .3 5 2 .7 4 1 0 .2 7 1 .7 6 1 0 .3 8 2 .1 2
Total 2.75 1 0 .2 4 3 .0 2 1 0 .1 8 2.41 ± 0 .26 1.74
FCQ-T
Plan 6.23 1 0.95 7 .5 8 1 0 .7 2 6 .6 8 1 1.04 0.59
Positive 1 0 .9411.74 1 1 .5 6 1 1 .3 2 9 .5 3 1 1 .9 0 0.38
Negative 4 .3 4 1 1.33 7.3 2 1 1 .0 1 5 .1 7 1 1 .4 5 1.53
Control 1 2 .4012.37 1 4 .9 6 1 1 .8 0 1 5 .5 4 1 2 .5 9 0.49
Thoughts 10 .2 8 1 1 .5 7 1 3 .1311.57 1 3 .1 0 1 2 .2 6 0.63
Hunger 14 .1 8 1 1 .4 0 1 1 .3710.98 9 .4 8 1 1.44 2.95
Emotions 5 .7 1 1 1 .3 5 8 .0 4 1 1 .0 2 8 .1 0 1 1.47 1.04
Environment 10 .8 0 1 1 .8 9 12.7911.43 1 4 .1 8 1 2 .0 6 0.78
Guilt 5 .8 0 1 0 .8 9 6 .5 4 1 0 .6 7 6.78 1 0 .9 6 0.34
Note. FCF= Food Craving Inventory; FCQ-T = Trait version o f the Food Craving Questionnaire.

Table 4C. Analysis o f Covariance (ANCOVA) for BMI and Measures o f Food Cravings, for Women,
Controlling for Time since Eating, and Menstrual Phase.
Scale Subscale Average Overweight Obese F
M l SE M ±SE M +SE
FCI
High Fats 1.4 2 1 0 .0 4 1 .6 9 1 0 .0 8 1 .8 4 1 0 .1 0 10.90***
Sweets 2 .5 6 1 0 .0 8 2 .6 8 1 0 .1 4 2 .5 3 1 0 .1 7 0.32
Carbohydrates 1 .8 6 1 0 .0 7 1.8610.13 1 .7 3 1 0 .1 6 0.26
Fast Food Fats 2 .2 1 1 0 .0 9 2 .4 1 1 0 .1 6 2 .6 2 1 0 .1 9 2 .1 2
Total 2.81 ± 0.05 2.93 1 0 .0 9 2 .9 0 1 0 .1 1 0.89
FCQ-T
Plan 8 .3 6 1 0 .3 9 8 .1 9 1 0 .6 9 9 .7 6 1 0 .8 5 1.26
Positive 11.8810.43 12.43 1 0 .7 6 1 4 .6 9 1 0 .9 4 3.73*
Negative 6 .4 9 1 0 .3 0 6.93 1 0.53 8.58 1 0 .6 7 4.22*
Control 12.53 1 0 .7 9 1 5 .1211.40 1 5 .4911.73 2 .1 0
Thoughts 14.7810.81 14 .5911.42 12.901 1.76 0.48
Hunger 11.0410.41 11 .8 4 1 0 .7 4 1 1 .3 5 1 0 .8 9 0.46
Emotions 1 0 .6310.58 11.9111.03 1 2 .4 6 1 1 .2 7 1 .2 1
Environment 11.71 ±0.53 1 3 .5010.94 1 3.551 1.16 2 .0 2
Guilt 6 .6 5 1 0 .4 1 8.21 ±0.71 9.92 1 0.88 6.42**
Note. FCI= Food Craving Inventory; FCQ-T = Trait version o f the Food Craving Questionnaire.
* p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Food Cravings and Gender

To test the second hypothesis, that gender is associated with food cravings,

correlational analyses were conducted. To control for participants’ responses regarding

how soon after food consumption they completed the survey, point by serial correlations

using the above responses as covariates were conducted. Table 5 presents point by serial

correlations for both genders, controlling for the time interval between food consumption

and survey completion. Correlation analyses revealed that being male was significantly

correlated with an increase in food cravings for high fat foods, (r = -.18,/? < .05). In

addition, correlation analyses indicated that being female was significantly correlated

with an increase in food cravings for sweets, (r = .22,/? < .05). Furthermore, correlations

indicated that being female was also significantly correlated with an increase in

experiencing emotions during or before food cravings or eating, (r = .30, p < .01).

Table 5. Point by Serial Correlations between Gender and Measures of Food Cravings for
both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating. ______________________________
Scale Subscales BMI
FCI
High Fats -.18*
Sweets .22*
Carbohydrates -.04
Fast Food Fats -.06
Total .03
FCQ-T
Plan .16
Positive .14
Negative .10
Lack of control -.10
Thoughts .12
Hunger -.07
Emotions .30**
Environment -.04
Guilt .10
Note. FCI= Food Craving Inventory; FCQ-T = Trait version o f the Food Craving Questionnaire.
*p < .05; **p < .01

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
To further explore the relationship between gender and food cravings, one-way

analyses of covariance (ANCOVA’s) were computed. The ANCOVA’s examined the

effect o f gender on food cravings, with time of food consumption prior to survey

completion being treated as a covariate. Univariate analyses of covariance were

conducted for each of the dependent measures of food cravings. See Table 6.

A significant main effect for gender was found for high fat foods, F( 1,117) =

4.44, p < .05. Bonferroni post hoc comparisons tests using adjusted means indicated that

male participants experienced greater cravings for high fat foods than female participants

(p < .05). A significant main effect for gender was also found for sweet foods, F( 1,117)

= 5.65, p < .05. Bonferroni post hoc comparisons tests using adjusted means indicated

that women reported greater cravings for sweets than men, (p < .05). Furthermore, a

significant main effect for gender was found for the plan subscale of the FCQ-T, which

measures individuals’ intention and planning to consume foods, F( 1,118) = 4.61,/? < .05.

Bonferroni post hoc comparisons tests using adjusted means revealed that women

experienced more intentions and planning to consume foods than men, (p < .05). Lastly, a

significant main effect of gender was found for the emotion subscale of the FCQ-T,

which measures emotions that may be experienced before or during food cravings or

eating, F(l, 117) = 13.03,/? < .001. Bonferroni post hoc comparisons tests using adjusted

means indicated that women reported experiencing more emotions before or during food

cravings or eating than men, (p < .001).

32

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 6. Analysis o f Covariance (ANCOVA) for Gender and Measures of Food Cravings,
for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating.____________________
Scale Subscale Male Female F
M ±SE M ±SE
FCI
High Fats 1.73 ±0.08 1.53 ±0.04 4.44*
Sweets 2.20 + 0.14 2.57 ± 0.07 5.65*
Carbohydrates 1.90 +0.12 1.83 ±0.06 0.23
Fast Food Fats 2.43 ±0.15 2.31 ±0.08 0.47
Total 2.81 ±0.09 2.84 ± 0.05 0.07
FCQ-T
Plan 7.09 ± 0.59 8.50 ±0.30 4.61*
Positive 11.03 ±0.72 12.37 ±0.37 2.76
Negative 6.13 ±0.52 6.84 ± 0.27 1.49
Control 14.31 ± 1.23 13.48 ±0.63 0.36
Thoughts 12.27 ± 1.21 14.51 ±0.62 2.73
Hunger 11.81 ±0.66 11.22 ±0.33 0.65
Emotions 7.59 ±0.87 11.14 ±0.45 13.03***
Environment 12.46 ± 0.85 12.34 ±0.43 0.01
Guilt 6.44 ± 0.63 7.43 ± 0.32 1.93
Note. FCI= Food Craving Inventory; FCQ-T = Trait version o f the Food Craving Questionnaire.
* p < .05; *** p < .001.

Food Cravings and Negative Affect: The Role of BMI

To test the third hypothesis, that negative affect is associated with food cravings,

correlations were conducted. To control for participants’ responses regarding how soon

after food consumption they completed the survey, and women’s reports of menstrual

stage at the time of survey completion, partial correlations using the above responses as

covariates were conducted. See Tables 7A-7C.

Table 7A presents partial correlations for both genders combined, controlling for

the time interval between food consumption and survey completion. Correlation analyses

revealed that increases in positive affect were significantly correlated with an increase in

food cravings, (r = .36, p < .001). Increases in positive affect were correlated with

increases in specific food cravings for high fat foods, (r = .31 ,P < .01), carbohydrates, (r

= •34, p < .01), and fast food fats, (r = .27, p < .01). Increases in negative affect were also

33

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
correlated with increases in food cravings, (r = .24, p < .05), specifically for sweets, (r =

.22, p < .05) and fast food fats, (r = .34, p < .01).

In addition, correlations indicated that increases in positive affect were

significantly correlated with multiple subscales of the FCQ-T, including an intention or

plan to consume food, anticipation of positive reinforcement from eating, thoughts or

preoccupation with food, hunger, and environmental reactivity (r’s ranging from .27 to

.36). See Table 7A. Furthermore, correlations revealed that increases in negative affect

were significantly correlated with multiple subscales of the FCQ-T, including plans or

intentions to consume food, anticipation of positive reinforcement from eating, hunger,

feelings o f guilt related to cravings and/or indulging in the craved food(s), and feeling a

possible lack of control if food is eaten (r’s ranging from .21 to .32). See Table 7A.

Table 7B presents partial correlations for men only, controlling for the time

interval between food consumption and survey completion. Correlations revealed that

increases in positive affect were significantly correlated with increases in food cravings

for men, (r = .63,/? < .01), specifically for high fat foods, (r = .53,/? < .05), sweets, (r =

.58,/? < .05), and fast food fats, (r = .63,/? < .01). In addition, increases in negative affect

were found to be correlated with decreases in anticipation of positive reinforcement from

eating, (r - -.53,p < .05), and hunger, (r = -.52, p < .05).

Table 7C presents partial correlations for women only, controlling for the time

interval between food consumption and survey completion and for menstrual stage.

Correlational analyses revealed that increases in positive affect were significantly

correlated with increases in food cravings, specifically for high fat foods and

carbohydrates (r’s from .23 to .30). See Table 1C. Correlations also showed that

34

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
increases in negative affect were significantly correlated with increases in food cravings,

specifically for high fat foods, sweets, carbohydrates and fast food fats (r’s from .25 to

.34). See Table 1C.

In addition, increases in positive affect were significantly correlated with multiple

subscales of the FCQ-T, including plan or intention to consume food, preoccupation with

food, hunger, environmental reactivity, and feelings of guilt associated with craving

and/or indulging in craved foods (r’s from .23 to .33). See Table 1C. Moreover,

correlations showed that increases in negative affect were significantly correlated with

multiple subscales of the FCQ-T, including plan or intention to consume food,

anticipation of positive reinforcement from eating, anticipation of relief from negative

affect from eating, possible lack of control when eating, hunger, experiencing emotions

before or during food cravings or eating, environmental reactivity, and feelings of guilt

associated with experiencing food cravings and/or indulging in craved foods (r’s from .26

to .39). See Table 1C.

35

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 7A. Partial Correlations between Affect and Measures of Food Cravings for both Genders,
Controlling for Time since Eating.
Scale Subscale CESD PANAS- PANAS- AIM- AIM-
positive negative positive negative
FCI
High Fats .15 .1 2 -.06 31* * .15
Sweets .1 0 .09 .07 .2 0 .2 2 *
Carbohydrates .18 .16 .14 .34** .07
Fast Food Fats .15 - .0 2 ,1 2 .27** .34**
Total .18 .13 .1 1
3^ * * * .24*
FCQ-T
Plan .25* .04 .14 .23* .26*
Positive .05 .06 -.05 .2 1 * .2 2 *
Negative .18 -.13 - .0 0 .14 .08
Lack o f .25* - .1 2 .2 1 * .13 .18
control
Thoughts .08 .04 .0 1 .26* .13
Hunger .07 .14 .05 .32** .23*
Emotions .17 -.16 .09 -.08 .14
Environment .18 -.07 .17 .26* .14
Guilt .06 .0 2 .0 1 .2 0 .30**
Note. FCI== Food Craving Inventory; FCQ-T = Trait version o f the Food Craving Questionnaire; CESD =
Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-
Expanded Form; AIM = Short Affect Intensity Scale. *p < .05; **p <.0 1 ; ***/?< .0 0 1

Table 7B. Partial Correlations between Affect and Measures o f Food Cravings for Men, Controlling for
Time since Eating. ______________________________________________________________________
Scale Subscale CESD PANAS- PANAS- AIM- AIM-
positive negative positive negative
FCI
High Fats - .0 1 .30 -.35 .53* -.05
Sweets -.07 .31 -.33 .58* -.18
Carbohydrates .15 .41 -.1 0 .43 .16
Fast Food Fats .15 .39 -.08 .63** .35
Total .04 .42 -.26 .63** .03
FCQ-T
Plan - .0 1 .39 -.16 .2 2 .09
Positive -.35 .2 2 -.53* .35 -.36
Negative - .1 2 .13 -.33 .31 - .2 2
Lack o f .03 .08 -.04 .17 .17
control
Thoughts -.03 .2 0 -.36 .25 -.40
Hunger -.41 .35 -.52* .42 -.18
Emotions -.0 0 -.08 -.18 .03 -.27
Environment -.05 .0 2 -.09 .35 -.04
Guilt .04 -.05 .08 -.18 .2 0
Note. FCI= Food Craving Inventory; FCQ-T = Trait version o f the Food Craving Questionnaire; CESD =
Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-
Expanded Form; AIM = Short Affect Intensity Scale. *p < .05; **p <.01

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 1C. Partial Correlations between Affect and Measures o f Food Cravings for Women, Controlling for
Time since Eating, and Menstrual Phase._____________________ _________________________________ _
Scale Subscale CESD PANAS- PANAS- AIM- AIM-
positive negative positive negative
FCI
High Fats .2 0 .03 .0 2 .23* .25*
Sweets .26* - .0 0 .34** .06 .30**
Carbohydrates .23 .04 .24* .30* .06
Fast Food Fats .2 1 -.18 .15 .14 .34**
Total .32** -.04 .32** .25* .32**
FCQ-T
Plan .36** - .0 1 .27* .25* .26*
Positive .24* -.0 0 .17 .16 .32**
Negative .34** -.23 .15 .09 .13
Lack of .30** -.19 .29* .13 .23
control
Thoughts .14 -.0 1 .16 .26* .2 2
Hunger .2 0 .08 .23* .33** .39**
Emotions .27* -.15 .24* -.07 .16
Environment .26* -.1 2 .25* .23* .2 0
Guilt .09 .04 .05 .26* .31**
Note. FCI= Food Craving Inventory; FCQ-T = Trait version o f the Food Craving Questionnaire; CESD =
Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-
Expanded Form; AIM = Short Affect Intensity Scale. *p < .05; **p <.01

To further test the third hypothesis, multiple regression analyses were conducted

to determine the extent to which BMI moderated the relationship between various

measures of negative affect and food cravings. To control for time since eating, this

variable was entered into the model first followed by negative affect, BMI, and the

interaction term. This sequence was conducted for analyses performed on both genders

and men only. To control for time since eating and menstrual phase for women, these

variables were entered in to the model first (time interval) and second (menstrual phase)

followed by negative affect, BMI, and the interaction term. Independent variables were

centered.

Results of the analyses for both genders and men alone controlling for the time

interval between food consumption and survey completion showed that BMI did not

moderate the relationship between negative affect and food cravings. These findings

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
suggest that the magnitude of the relationship between negative affect and food cravings

is the same across various levels of BMI. See Tables 8A-8F. Results of the analyses for

women, controlling for the time interval between food consumption and survey

completion and menstrual phase, revealed that BMI significantly moderated the

relationship between negative affect and food cravings for high fat foods. This finding

suggests that the relationship between negative affect and food cravings and BMI was

stronger among women with higher BMI’s (r = .21, p < .09) than among women with

lower BMI’s (r = .02, p < .88); however, the post hoc analyses were non-significant.

None o f the other hypothesized moderation effects were statistically significant (See

Tables 8G-8I).

38

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 8A. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (CESD) for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating._____________
DV IV b se P AR2 t
Food cravings
Time since eating -.1 0 .06 -:i 6 .04 -1.72
Negative affect-CESD .0 2 .0 0 .29 .08 3 11**
BMI -.0 0 .0 1 -.04 .0 0 -0.46
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 - .0 1 .0 1 -0 . 1 0
Food Cravings for High Fat Foods
Time since eating -.07 .06 - .1 1 .0 1 -1.23
Negative affect-CESD .0 1 .0 0 .1 1 .0 1 1.28
BMI .03 .0 1 .38 .13 4 2 1 ***
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .14 .0 2 1.53
Food Cravings for Sweets
Time since eating -.14 .1 0 -.13 .03 -1.36
Negative affect-CESD .0 2 .0 1 ,24 .06 2.61**
BMI - .0 2 .0 1 -A l .0 2 -1.87
Interaction Term - .0 0 .0 0 -.08 .0 1 -0 . 8 6
Food Cravings for Carbohydrates
Time since eating -.16 .08 -.18 .05 -1.90
Negative affect-CESD .0 2 .0 1 .26 .06 2.81**
BMI -.0 1 .0 1 -.13 .0 2 -1.38
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 - .0 1 .0 0 -0.07
Food Cravings for Fast Food Fats
Time since eating .0 2 .1 1 .0 2 .0 0 0.19
Negative affect-CESD .0 2 .0 1 .2 1 .04 2.16*
BMI .0 1 .0 1 .05 .0 0 0.52
Interaction Term -5.66 .0 0 -.0 0 .0 0 -0.03
Plan and intent to consume food
Time since eating -.09 .41 - .0 2 .0 0 -0.19
Negative affect-CESD .1 0 .04 .27 .07 2.89**
BMI .0 2 .05 .03 .0 0 0.32
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 1 .06 .0 0 0.61
Thoughts or preoccupation with food
Time since eating 1.15 .80 .14 .0 1 1.44
Negative affect-CESD .1 1 .07 .16 .0 2 1 .6 8
BMI -.09 .1 0 -.09 .0 1 -.89
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 1 .03 .0 0 .35
Hunger
Time since eating -.46 .45 - .1 0 .0 1 - 1 .0 1
Negative affect-CESD .03 .04 .08 .0 1 0 .8 6
BMI -.03 .06 -.05 .0 0 -0.48
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 1 .04 .0 0 0.41
Note. CESD = Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale. ** p < .01.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 8B. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (PANAS) for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating.____________
DV IV b se P AR2 t
Food cravings
Time since eating -.15 .06 -.2 1 .04 -2.33
Negative affect-PANAS .0 1 .0 1 .16 .0 2 1.69
BMI - .0 0 .0 1 -.0 2 .0 0 -0.16
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .0 1 .0 0 0.07
Food cravings for high fat foods
Time since eating -.09 .06 -.14 .0 2 -.159
Negative affect-PANAS - .0 0 .0 1 -.05 .0 0 -0.59
BMI .03 .0 1 .34 .1 0 3.65***
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .08 .0 1 0.90
Food cravings for sweets
Time since eating -.17 .1 0 -.16 .03 -1.69
Negative affect-PANAS .0 2 .0 1 .14 .0 2 1.55
BMI - .0 2 .0 1 -.1 2 .0 1 -1.30
Interaction Term -.0 0 .0 0 -.06 .0 0 -0.63
Food cravings for carbohydrates
Time since eating -.23 .09 -.24 .06 -2 .6 8 **
Negative affect-PANAS .0 2 .0 1 .14 .0 2 1.55
BMI - .0 1 .0 1 - .1 1 .0 1 -1.13
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .05 .0 0 0.59
Food cravings for fast food fats
Time since eating -.07 .1 1 -.06 .0 0 -0.63
Negative affect-PANAS .03 .0 1 .17 .03 1.89
BMI .0 1 .0 1 .07 .0 1 0.70
Interaction Term -.0 0 .0 0 -.04 .0 0 -0.41
Plan and intent to consume food
Time since eating -.41 .41 -.09 .0 1 -0.99
Negative affect-PANAS .1 2 .05 .2 1 .04 2.29*
BMI .0 2 .06 .04 .0 0 0.38
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 1 .0 2 .0 0 0.25
Thoughts or preoccupation with food
Time since eating .67 .8 8 .07 .0 0 0.76
Negative affect-PANAS .13 .1 1 .1 1 .0 1 1.19
BMI - .1 2 .1 2 -.1 0 .0 1 -0.98
Interaction Term .0 1 .0 2 .03 .0 0 0.32
Hunger
Time since eating -.80 .47 -.16 .03 -1.70
Negative affect-PANAS .03 .06 .04 .0 0 0.43
BMI -.04 .06 -.06 .0 0 -0.60
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 1 .0 2 .0 0 0.18
Note. PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Expanded Form. *p < .05.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 8C. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (AIM) for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating.______________
DV IV b se P AR2 t
Food cravings
Time since eating -.09 .06 -.14 .04 -1.61
Negative affect-AIM .03 .0 1 .35 .1 2 3 94 ***
BMI .0 0 .0 1 - .0 0 .0 0 -0.05
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .0 1 .0 0 0.08
Food cravings for high fat foods
Time since eating -.06 .06 -.1 0 .0 1 -1.16
Negative affect-AIM .0 1 .0 1 .18 .03 2 .0 2
*
BMI .03 .0 1 .36 .13 4 j9***
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .03 .0 0 0.38
Food cravings for sweets
Time since eating - .1 1 .09 -.1 0 .03 -1.14
Negative affect-AIM .05 .0 1 .33 .1 1 3.77***
BMI -.0 2 .0 1 -.14 .0 2 -1.57
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .0 1 .0 0 -0.07
Food cravings for carbohydrates
Time since eating -.17 .08 -.19 .05 -2.04*
Negative affect-AIM .0 2 .0 1 .2 1 .04 2.33*
BMI -.0 1 .0 1 -.09 .0 1 -0.98
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 - .0 2 .0 0 -0 . 2 2
Food cravings for fast food fats
Time since eating .03 .1 0 .03 .0 0 0.34
Negative affect-AIM .05 .0 1 .36 .14 4.10***
BMI .0 1 .0 1 .07 .0 0 0.83
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .07 .0 0 0.78
Plan and intent to consume food
Time since eating -.09 .39 - .0 2 .0 1 -0.23
Negative affect-AIM .2 1 .05 .37 .13 4.19***
BMI .0 2 .05 .04 .0 0 0.50
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 1 - .0 0 .0 0 -0.04
Thoughts or preoccupation with food
Time since eating 1.09 .82 .1 2 .0 1 1.33
Negative affect-AIM .35 .1 0 .31 .08 3.35**
BMI - .1 0 .1 1 -.08 .0 1 -0.95
Interaction Term - .0 2 .0 2 -.1 1 .0 1 -1.17
Hunger
Time since eating -.45 .43 -.1 0 .0 2 -1.06
Negative affect-AIM .2 0 .05 .33 .1 0 3.57**
BMI - .0 1 .05 - .0 1 .0 0 -0.13
Interaction Term - .0 1 .0 1 -.06 .0 0 -0.69
Note. AIM = Short Affect Intensity Scale. ** p < .01; ***p< .001.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 8D. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (CESD) for Men, Controlling for Time since Eating.____________________
DV IV b se 0 AR2 t
Food cravings
Time since eating -.09 .25 - .1 0 .0 2 -0.38
Negative affect-CESD .0 1 .0 2 .14 .0 2 0.51
BMI -.04 .03 -.32 .1 1 -1.37
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .03 .0 0 0.13
Food cravings for high fat foods
Time since eating -.03 .23 -.04 .0 1 -0.15
Negative affect-CESD .0 1 .0 2 .1 0 .0 0 0.36
BMI - .0 0 .03 -.03 .0 0 -0 . 1 2
Interaction Term - .0 0 .0 0 -.1 0 .0 1 -0.35
Food cravings for sweets
Time since eating -.37 .40 -.23 .05 -0.92
Negative affect-CESD .0 0 .03 .0 1 .0 0 0.03
BMI -.07 .04 -.34 .1 2 -1.48
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 1 .05 .0 0 0.18
Food cravings for carbohydrates
Time since eating - .2 1 .28 -.18 .05 -0.75
Negative affect-CESD .0 2 .0 2 .25 .08 1 .0 0
BMI -.04 .03 -.29 .09 -1.29
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .08 .0 0 0.30
Food cravings for fast food fats
Time since eating .69 .33 .46 .17 2.13*
Negative affect-CESD .0 1 .0 2 .15 .0 2 0.65
BMI -.07 .04 -.40 .16 -1.99
Interaction Term - .0 0 .0 1 -.04 .0 0 -0.19
Plan and intent to consume food
Time since eating -1.07 .92 -.29 .05 -1.16
Negative affect-CESD - .0 2 .07 -.07 .0 0 -0.27
BMI .04 .1 0 .09 .0 0 0.37
Interaction Term .0 2 .0 2 .29 .06 1.06
Thoughts or preoccupation with food
Time since eating -1.27 2.07 -.15 .0 0 -0.61
Negative affect-CESD -.06 .15 -.1 1 .0 0 -0.43
BMI .24 .24 .24 .03 1 .0 1
Interaction Term .05 .03 .37 .1 0 1.37
Hunger
Time since eating -2.14 1.31 -.36 .04 -1.63
Negative affect-CESD -.19 .1 0 -.46 .09 -1.97
BMI -.24 .15 -.33 .14 -1.60
Interaction Term .03 .0 2 .30 .07 1.29
Note. CESD = Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 8E. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (PANAS) for Men, Controlling for Time since Eating.___________________
DV IV b se P AR2 t
Food cravings
Time since eating -.29 .24 -.28 .03 - 1 .2 0
Negative affect-PANAS - .0 2 .0 2 - .2 1 .0 1 -0.94
BMI -.06 .03 -.42 .1 1 -1.90
Interaction Term .0 1 .0 1 .33 .08 1.33
Food cravings for high fat foods
Time since eating -.23 .2 1 -.27 .05 - 1 .1 2
Negative affect-PANAS - .0 2 .0 2 -.25 .03 -1.07
BMI -.03 .03 -.27 .04 -1.15
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 1 .23 .04 0 .8 8
Food cravings for sweets
Time since eating -.47 .39 -.28 .05 - 1 .2 1
Negative affect-PANAS -.04 .03 -.25 .04 - 1 .1 1
BMI -.08 .05 -.35 .09 -1.53
Interaction Term .0 1 .0 1 .19 .03 0.75
Food cravings for carbohydrates
Time since eating -.45 .27 -.37 .04 -1.69
Negative affect-PANAS - .0 1 .0 2 - .1 1 .0 0 -0.53
BMI -.08 .04 -.47 .1 2 -2 .2 2 *
Interaction Term .0 1 .0 1 .45 .15 1.91
Food cravings for fast food fats
Time since eating .30 .38 .19 .07 0.80
Negative affect-PANAS -.0 1 .03 -.05 .0 0 -0.23
BMI -.09 .05 -.38 .1 1 -1.65
Interaction Term .0 1 .0 1 .16 .0 2 0.64
Plan and intent to consume food
Time since eating -1.47 .92 -.39 .07 -1.60
Negative affect-PANAS - .0 1 .08 -.0 2 .0 0 -0.09
BMI -.06 .13 - .1 2 .0 0 -0.52
Interaction Term .03 .0 2 .34 .09 1.33
Thoughts or preoccupation with food
Time since eating -.49 2.03 -.06 .0 0 -0.24
Negative affect-PANAS - .2 1 .18 -.27 .03 -1.14
BMI .14 .28 .1 2 .04 0.52
Interaction Term .07 .06 .34 .09 1.33
Hunger
Time since eating - 1 .2 1 1.14 - .2 0 .04 -1.06
Negative affect-PANAS -.33 .1 1 -.57 .16 -3.03**
BMI -.51 .16 -.59 .23 -3.24**
Interaction Term .05 .03 .37 .09 0.36
Note. PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Expanded Form. **p < .01.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 8F. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (AIM) for Men, Controlling for Time since Eating._____________________
DV IV b se P AR2 t
Food cravings
Time since eating -.15 .2 2 -.15 .0 2 -0.70
Negative affect-AIM .0 2 .03 .1 1 .04 0.48
BMI -.0 1 .07 -.05 .03 -0.09
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 1 .17 .0 0 0.32
Food cravings for high fat foods
Time since eating - .1 2 .2 0 -.13 .0 2 -0.59
Negative affect-AIM .0 1 .03 .1 1 .0 1 0.45
BMI .0 2 .06 .16 .0 1 0.30
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 1 .09 .0 0 0.16
Food cravings for sweets
Time since eating -.43 .34 -.26 .07 -1.25
Negative affect-AIM - .0 0 .05 - .0 2 .0 1 -0.07
BMI -.03 .1 0 -.17 .07 -0.34
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 1 .14 .0 0 0.27
Food cravings for carbohydrates
Time since eating - .2 2 .26 -.19 .03 -0.85
Negative affect-AIM .0 2 .04 .1 2 .04 0.49
BMI -.03 .08 -.23 .03 -0.44
Interaction Term - .0 0 .0 1 -.05 .0 0 -0.09
Food cravings for fast food fats
Time since eating .46 .30 .29 .09 1.50
Negative affect-AIM .06 .05 .31 .15 1.43
BMI .0 1 .09 .06 .03 0 .1 2
Interaction Term .0 1 .0 1 .27 .0 1 0.58
Plan and intent to consume food
Time since eating -1.19 .80 -.30 .08 -1.49
Negative affect-AIM .2 1 .1 2 .39 .08 1.75
BMI .17 .24 .34 .05 0.70
Interaction Term .0 1 .03 .1 0 .0 0 0 .2 0
Thoughts or preoccupation with food
Time since eating -.1 0 1.82 - .0 1 .0 0 -0.05
Negative affect-AIM - .0 1 .27 - .0 1 .0 1 -0.04
BMI .60 .54 .59 .04 1 .1 2
Interaction Term .06 .08 .41 .03 0.78
Hunger
Time since eating -.54 1 .2 1 -.09 .03 -0.45
Negative affect-AIM -.37 .2 1 -.43 .0 1 -1.76
BMI -.40 .34 -.55 .24 -1.16
Interaction Term .0 0 .05 .03 .0 0 0.07
Note: AIM = Short Affect Intensity Scale.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 8G. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (CESD) for Women, Controlling for Time since Eating and Menstrual Phase.
DV IV b se P AR2 t
Food cravings
Time since eating -.1 0 .05 -.17 .06 -1.82
Menses Phase .19 .05 .35 .1 0 3.83***
Negative affect-CESD .0 2 .0 0 .37 .13 3 91 * * *
BMI .0 0 .0 1 .0 2 .0 0 0.27
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .03 .0 0 0.29
Food cravings for high fat foods
Time since eating -.07 .05 - .1 2 .0 2 -1.28
Menses Phase .16 .05 .30 .08 3.35**
Negative affect-CESD .0 1 .0 0 .19 .0 2 2.09*
BMI .04 .0 1 .45 .16 4 9 5 ***
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .25 .06 2 .6 6 **
Food cravings for sweets
Time since eating -.07 .09 -.08 .0 2 -0.77
Menses Phase .26 .08 .30 .07 3.04**
Negative affect-CESD .03 .0 1 .34 .1 2 3.44**
BMI -.0 1 .0 1 -.07 .0 0 -0.74
Interaction Term - .0 0 .0 0 -.05 .0 0 -0.48
Food cravings for carbohydrates
Time since eating -.16 .09 -.19 .06 -1.90
Menses Phase .19 .08 .23 .04 2.34*
Negative affect-CESD .0 2 .0 1 .27 .07 2.63**
BMI - .0 2 .0 1 -.14 .0 2 -1.39
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 -.0 1 .0 0 -0.14
Food cravings for fast food fats
Time since eating -.09 .1 0 -.09 .0 2 -0.90
Menses Phase .19 .1 0 .2 0 .03 1.99*
Negative affect-CESD .0 2 .0 1 .27 .07 2.54*
BMI .0 2 .0 1 .15 .0 2 1.42
Interaction Term 8 .1 2 .0 0 .0 0 .0 0 0.04
Plan and intent to consume food
Time since eating .2 0 .46 .05 .0 0 0.44
Menses Phase .54 .42 .13 .0 1 1.30
Negative affect-CESD .13 .04 .34 .1 0 3.26**
BMI .05 .06 .08 .0 0 0.77
Interaction Term .0 1 .0 1 .09 .0 1 0.89
Thoughts or preoccupation with food
Time since eating 1.60 .8 8 .19 .0 2 1.82
Menses Phase 1.49 .80 .19 .03 1.87
Negative affect-CESD .15 .08 .2 0 .04 1.91
BMI -.14 .1 2 -.1 2 .0 2 -1.15
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 2 .03 .0 0 0.28
Hunger
Time since eating -.35 .48 -.08 .0 1 -.73
Menses Phase -.1 1 .44 -.03 .0 0 -.24
Negative affect-CESD .08 .04 .2 0 .03 1.83
BMI .0 0 .07 .0 0 .0 0 0 .0 2
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 1 .07 .0 0 0.58
Note. CESD = Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale. **p < .01; ***p< .001.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 8H. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (PANAS) for Women, Controlling for Time since Eating and Menstrual Phase.
DV IV b se P AR2 t
Food cravings
Time since eating -.14 .06 -.2 2 .05 -2.37*
Menses Phase .2 0 .05 .35 .1 1
2 7 7 ***
Negative affect-PANAS .03 .0 1 .32 .09 3.35**
BMI .0 0 .0 1 .06 .0 0 0.63
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .05 .0 0 0.47
Food cravings for high fat foods
Time since eating -.09 .05 -.14 .0 2 -157
Menses Phase .13 .05 .25 .08 2.71**
Negative affect-PANAS .0 0 .0 1 .03 .0 0 0.35
BMI .44 .15 4 4 9 ***
.03 .0 1
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .15 .0 2 1.51
Food cravings for sweets
Time since eating -.1 0 .09 - .1 1 .0 2 -1.14
Menses Phase .29 .08 .33 .09 3.51**
Negative affect-PANAS .05 .0 1 .38 .14 2 92 * * *
BMI - .0 0 .0 1 -.03 .0 0 -0.33
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .2 1 .0 0 0 .2 1
Food cravings for carbohydrates
Time since eating -.23 .09 -.25 .07 -2.58*
Menses Phase .2 0 .08 .24 .04 2.42*
Negative affect-PANAS .0 2 .0 1 .2 0 .04 2 .0 0 *
BMI - .0 1 .0 1 -.1 0 .0 1 -0.92
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .03 .0 0 0.32
Food cravings for fast food fats
Time since eating -.15 .1 1 -.14 .0 2 -1.43
Menses Phase .19 .1 0 .19 .03 1.90
Negative affect-PANAS .03 .0 1 .23 .05 2.28*
BMI .0 2 .0 2 -.03 .03 1.42
Interaction Term -.0 0 .0 0 .0 0 -0.32
Plan and intent to consume food
Time since eating -.14 .46 -.03 .0 0 -0.29
Menses Phase .41 .42 .1 0 .0 1 0.97
Negative affect-PANAS .19 .06 .31 .08 2.97**
BMI .06 .07 .1 0 .0 1 0.96
Interaction Term .0 1 .0 1 .07 .0 0 0.62
Thoughts or preoccupation with food
Time since eating 1.03 .98 .1 1 .0 1 1.05
Menses Phase .98 .89 .1 1 .0 1 1 .1 0
Negative affect-PANAS .27 .13 .2 1 .05 2.03*
BMI -.15 .14 - .1 2 .0 2 -1.05
Interaction Term .0 1 .03 .0 2 .0 0 0 .2 1
Hunger
Time since eating -.70 .50 -.15 .03 -1.39
Menses Phase -.15 .46 -.03 .0 0 -0.33
Negative affect-PANAS .1 2 .07 .19 .03 1.73
BMI .0 2 .07 .03 .0 0 0.31
Interaction Term .0 1 .0 1 .08 .0 1 0.73
Note. PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Expanded Form. *p< .05.

46

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 81. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, BMI, and Negative
Affect (AIM) for Women, Controlling for Time since Eating and Menstrual Phase.
DV IV b se P AR2 t
Food cravings
Time since eating -.09 .05 -.15 .05 -.163
Menses Phase .16 .05 .29 .1 1 3.28**
Negative affect-AIM .03 .0 1 .40 .15 4 27***

BMI .0 0 .0 1 -.0 0 .0 0 -.0 2


Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 -.0 2 .0 0 -.23
Food cravings for high fat foods
Time since eating -.07 .05 -.1 2 .0 2 -1.29
Menses Phase .13 .05 .25 .09 2.81**
Negative affect-AIM .0 2 .0 1 .2 1 .06 2.30*
BMI .03 .0 1 .36 .13 3.70***
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .04 .0 0 0.38
Food cravings for sweets
Time since eating -.03 .09 -.03 .0 1 -0.37
Menses Phase .2 1 .08 .25 .08 2.65**
Negative affect-AIM .05 .0 1 .37 .13 3.89***
BMI - .0 1 .0 1 -.08 .0 1 -0.80
Interaction Term - .0 0 .0 0 -.09 .0 1 -0.89
Food cravings for carbohydrates
Time since eating -.17 .08 -.2 0 .07 -2 .0 2 *
Menses Phase .15 .07 .2 0 .05 2.06*
Negative affect-AIM .03 .0 1 .26 .06 2 .6 6 **
BMI - .0 2 .0 1 -.14 .0 2 -1.34
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .0 2 .0 0 0.16
Food cravings for fast food fats
Time since eating -.07 .1 0 -.07 .0 2 -0.73
Menses Phase .14 .09 .14 .04 1.52
Negative affect-AIM .05 .0 1 .37 .14 3.85***
BMI .0 1 .0 1 .09 .0 1 0 .8 6
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 0 .04 .0 0 0.42
Plan and intent to consume food
Time since eating .19 .44 .04 .0 0 0.43
Menses Phase .1 2 .40 .03 .0 0 0.31
Negative affect-AIM .2 0 .06 .35 .1 2 3.37**
BMI .0 2 .06 .03 .0 0 0.32
Interaction Term -4.94 .0 1 .0 0 .0 0 0 .0 0
Thoughts or preoccupation with food
Time since eating 1.60 .92 .17 .0 1 1.73
Menses Phase .42 .83 .05 .0 0 0.51
Negative affect-AIM .41 .1 2 .34 .1 0 3.35**
BMI -.18 .13 -.14 .03 -1.33
Interaction Term - .0 1 .0 2 -.06 .0 0 -0.53
Hunger
Time since eating -.35 .43 -.08 .0 2 -0.81
Menses Phase -.44 .39 -.1 1 .0 0 -1.13
Negative affect-AIM .28 .06 .47 .2 1 4.85***
BMI .0 0 .06 .0 0 .0 0 0 .0 1
Interaction Term - .0 1 .0 1 -.08 .0 1 -0.83
Note: AIM = Short Affect Intensity Scale. **p< .01; ***p < .001.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Food Cravings and Negative Affect: The Role of Gender

To test the fourth hypothesis, multiple regression analyses were conducted to

determine the extent to which gender moderated the relationship between various

measures of negative affect and food cravings. To control for time since eating, this

variable was entered into the model first followed by negative affect, gender, and the

interaction term. This sequence was conducted for analyses performed on both genders;

multiple regressions were then conducted separately for each gender. Multiple

regressions for men controlled for time since eating by entering this covariate into the

model first followed by negative affect. Multiple regressions for women controlled for

time since eating and menstrual phase by entering those variables into the model first

followed by negative affect. Following Baron and Kenny's (1986) recommendations on

how to test for moderation with a dichotomous moderator, gender specific multiple

regression standardized regression coefficients were compared using a one-way t-test.

Independent variables were centered.

Results of the analyses for both genders, controlling for the time interval between

food consumption and survey completion, showed that gender did moderate the

relationship between negative affect and food cravings for sweets. Gender also moderated

the relationship between negative affect and hunger. See Tables 9A-9C. These findings

suggests that the relationship between negative affect and food cravings for sweets, as

well as the relationship between negative affect and hunger are stronger for women than

men; however, the post hoc analyses comparing the unstandardized regression

coefficients were non-significant. See Table 9D-9F.

48

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 9A. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, Gender, and Negative
Affect (CESD) for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating._______________
DV IV b se P AR2 t
Food cravings
Time since eating -.09 .06 -.14 .03 -1.54
Negative affect-CESD .0 0 .0 1 .09 .07 0.46
Gender .0 1 .1 0 .0 1 .0 0 0.07
Interaction Term .0 1 .0 1 .2 0 .0 1 1 .1 2
Food cravings for high fat foods
Time since eating -.07 .06 - .1 2 .0 1 -1.23
Negative affect-CESD .0 0 .0 1 .06 .0 1 0.29
Gender -.2 1 .1 0 -.2 0 .04 -2.08*
Interaction Term .0 0 .0 1 .06 .0 0 0.31
Food cravings for sweets
Time since eating -.1 0 .1 0 -.1 0 .0 2 -1.06
Negative affect-CESD .0 0 .0 2 .03 .05 0.16
Gender .38 .17 .2 1 .04 2.27*
Interaction Term .0 2 .0 2 .23 .0 1 1.27
Food cravings for carbohydrates
Time since eating -.15 .08 -.17 .04 -1.79
Negative affect-CESD .0 1 .0 1 .17 .05 0.90
Gender -.1 2 .14 -.08 .0 1 -0 . 8 6
Interaction Term .0 1 .0 2 .07 .0 0 0.38
Food cravings for fast food fats
Time since eating .0 2 .1 0 .0 2 .0 0 0.19
Negative affect-CESD 8.48 .0 2 .0 0 .04 0 .0 0
Gender -.13 .18 -.07 .0 0 -0.74
Interaction Term .0 2 .0 2 .23 .0 1 1 .2 0
Plan and intent to consume food
Time since eating - .0 0 .39 .0 0 .0 0 -0 . 0 0
Negative affect-CESD .03 .07 .08 .07 0.45
Gender 1.36 .67 .19 .03 2.04*
Interaction Term .09 .08 .2 2 .0 1 1.19
Thoughts or preoccupation with food
Time since eating 1.15 .79 .14 .0 1 1.46
Negative affect-CESD .07 .13 .1 0 .0 2 0.52
Gender 1.54 1.31 .1 1 .0 1 1.18
Interaction Term .06 .15 .0 1 .0 0 0.41
Hunger
Time since eating -.59 .44 -.13 .0 1 -1.33
Negative affect-CESD - .1 0 .07 -.26 .0 1 -1.38
Gender -.69 .74 -.09 .0 1 -0.94
Interaction Term .17 .08 .37 .04 2.06*
Note. CESD = Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale. *p< .05

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 9B. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, Gender, and Negative
Affect (PANAS) for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating.______________
DV IV b se P AR2 t
Food cravings
Time since eating - .1 2 .06 -.18 .04 -1.94
Negative affect-PANAS - .0 1 .0 1 - .1 1 .0 2 -0 . 6 6
Gender .03 .1 1 .03 .0 0 0.33
Interaction Term .03 .0 2 .32 .03 1 .8 8
Food cravings for high fat foods
Time since eating -.09 .06 -.14 .0 2 -1.53
Negative affect-PANAS - .0 2 .0 1 - .2 0 .0 0 - 1 .1 2
Gender -.18 .1 0 -.17 .03 -1.81
Interaction Term .0 1 .0 2 .13 .0 0 0.77
Food cravings for sweets
Time since eating -.1 1 .1 0 - .1 1 .0 2 -1.19
Negative affect-PANAS -.03 .0 2 -.23 .0 2 -1.35
Gender .33 .16 .18 .04 2.04*
Interaction Term .08 .03 .47 .06 2.78**
Food cravings for carbohydrates
Time since eating -.2 1 .09 - .2 2 .05 -2.38*
Negative affect-PANAS .0 0 .0 2 .03 .0 2 0.18
Gender -.04 .15 -.03 .0 0 -0.29
Interaction Term .0 1 .0 2 .13 .0 0 0.74
Food cravings for fast food fats
Time since eating -.05 .1 1 -.04 .0 0 -0.47
Negative affect-PANAS .0 1 .03 .06 .03 0.35
Gender -.05 .18 -.03 .0 0 -0.31
Interaction Term .0 2 .03 .13 .0 0 0.74
Plan and intent to consume food
Time since eating -.28 .40 -.06 .0 1 -0.70
Negative affect-PANAS .0 1 .1 0 .0 2 .04 0.09
Gender 1.38 .6 8 .19 .04 2.05*
Interaction Term .17 .1 1 .25 .0 2 1.45
Thoughts or preoccupation with food
Time since eating .84 .85 .09 .0 0 0.99
Negative affect-PANAS -.16 .2 0 -.14 .0 1 -0.79
Gender 2.03 1.41 .13 .0 2 1.44
Interaction Term .43 .24 .31 .03 1.76
Hunger
Time since eating -.6 6 .45 -.13 .03 -1.46
Negative affect-PANAS -.23 .1 1 -.36 .0 0 -2.03*
Gender -.70 .77 -.08 .0 0 -0.91
Interaction Term .34 .13 .46 .06 2.60*
Note. PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Expanded Form. *p< .05; **p< .01

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 9C. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, Gender, and Negative
Affect (AIM) for both Genders, Controlling for Time since Eating._________________
DV IV b se P AR2 t
Food cravings
Time since eating -.09 .06 -.13 .03 -1.52
Negative affect-AIM .03 .02 .34 .13 1.45
Gender -.08 .12 -.08 .01 -0.71
Interaction Term .00 .02 .04 .00 0.19
Food cravings for high fat foods
Time since eating -.06 .06 -.10 .01 -1.09
Negative affect-AIM .01 .02 .11 .03 0.44
Gender -.23 .11 -.22 .06 -2.02*
Interaction Term .01 .02 .14 .00 0.59
Food cravings for sweets
Time since eating -.09 .09 -.08 .02 -0.95
Negative affect-AIM .02 .03 .17 .12 0.71
Gender .29 .19 .17 .01 1.54
Interaction Term .02 .04 .15 .00 0.64
Food cravings for carbohydrates
Time since eating -.16 .08 -.18 .04 -2.00*
Negative affect-AIM .03 .03 .31 .05 1.25
Gender -.21 .16 -.14 .01 -1.26
Interaction Term -.00 .03 -.04 .00 -0.18
Food cravings for fast food fats
Time since eating .02 .10 .01 .00 0.18
Negative affect-AIM .09 .03 .61 .14 2.58*
Gender -.42 .20 -.23 .03 -2.15*
Interaction Term -.03 .04 -.18 .00 -0.82
Plan and intent to consume food
Time since eating -.05 .38 -.01 .01 -0.13
Negative affect-AIM .14 .13 .25 .13 1.07
Gender .87 .77 .12 .01 1.13
Interaction Term .06 .14 .09 .00 0.39
Thoughts or preoccupation with food
Time since eating 1.15 .80 .13 .01 1.43
Negative affect-AIM -.12 .28 -.11 .08 -0.44
Gender 2.49 1.61 .17 .00 1.54
Interaction Term .50 .30 .39 .02 1.66
Hunger
Time since eating -.41 .40 -.09 .02 -1.02
Negative affect-AIM -.10 .15 -.17 .10 -0.67
Gender -.42 .82 -.05 .03 -0.51
Interaction Term .37 .16 .55 .04 2.26*
Note: AIM = Short Affect Intensity Scale. *p< .05

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 9D. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, Gender, and Negative
Affect (CESD) comparing Gender effects, Controlling for Time since Eating for Men and
Time since Eating and Menstrual Phase for Women._________ ____________________
DV IV b se P AR2 tw h
Food cravings for Sweets
Men
Time since eating -.21 .35 -.15 .02 -0.60
CESD -.0000813 .02 -.00 .00 -0.00
Women
Time since eating -.07 .09 -.07 .02 -0.76
Menses Phase .26 .08 .30 .07 3.09**
CESD .03 .01 .35 .12 3.63*** 1.48
Hunger
Men
Time since eating -1.60 1.21 -.30 .03 -1.33
CESD -.13 .09 -.34 .10 -1.47
Women
Time since eating -.40 .47 -.09 .01 -0.84
Menses Phase -.14 .44 -.03 .00 -0.32
CESD .07 .04 .19 .03 1.76 1.61
Note. CESD = Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale. * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.

52

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 9E. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, Gender, and Negative
Affect (PANAS) comparing Gender effects, Controlling for Time since Eating for Men
and Time since Eating and Menses Phase for Women.__________________________
DV IV b se P AR2 tw h
Food cravings for Sweets
Men
Time since eating -.17 .33 -.12 .02 -0.53
PANAS -.03 .03 -.20 .04 -0.90
Women
Time since eating -.10 .09 -.11 .02 -1.18
Menses Phase .28 .08 .33 .09 3.52**
PANAS .05 .01 .37 .14 4.02*** 1.56
Plan and intent to consume food
Men
Time since eating -.96 .74 -.28 .07 -1.29
PANAS .02 .08 .07 .00 0.31
Women
Time since eating -.14 .46 -.03 .00 -0.30
Menses Phase .44 .42 .11 .01 1.05
PANAS .18 .06 .29 .08 2.88** -0.47
Thoughts or preoccupation with
food
Men
Time since eating .05 1.68 .01 .00 0.03
PANAS -.14 .17 -.19 .03 -0.83
Women
Time since eating .99 .97 .10 .01 1.01
Menses Phase .88 .89 .10 .01 0.99
PANAS .28 .13 .22 .05 2.09* 0.84
Hunger
Men
Time since eating -.36 1.16 -.06 .03 -0.31
PANAS -.24 .12 -.42 .16 -1.97
Women
Time since eating -.72 .49 -.15 .03 -1.46
Menses Phase -.16 .46 -.04 .00 -0.35
PANAS .11 .07 .17 .03 1.62 1.23
Note. PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Expanded Form. * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p <
.0 0 1 .

53

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 9F. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions for Food Cravings, Gender, and Negative
Affect (AIM) comparing Gender effects, Controlling for Time since Eating for Men and
Time since Eating and Menses Phase for Women.__________________________
DV IV b se P A ft tw tb
Thoughts or preoccupation
with food
Men
Time since eating -.40 1.56 -.05 .00 -0.26
AIM -.10 .24 -.09 .01 -0.42
Women
Time since eating 1.51 .92 .16 .01 1.64
Menses Phase .35 .83 .04 .00 0.42
AIM .38 .12 .32 .10 3.15** 0.81
Hunger
Men
Time since eating -.76 1.13 -.15 .03 -0.68
AIM -.09 .18 -.11 .01 -0.50
Women
Time since eating -.34 .43 -.08 .02 -0.80
Menses Phase -.43 .38 -.10 .00 -1.12
AIM .27 .06 .46 .21 4 92*** 1.15
Note. AIM = Short Affect Intensity Scale. * p < .05; **/?< .01; *** p < .001.

54

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER 5: DISCUSSION

Objectives

The current study examined the associations between food cravings, BMI, gender,

and negative affect. The objectives were to investigate whether increases in BMI were

associated with increases in food cravings, if men and women experienced food cravings

differently, and if increases in negative affect were associated with increases in food

cravings. Furthermore, BMI and gender were both explored as potential moderators of

the relationship between negative affect and food cravings.

Food Cravings and BMI

For the first hypothesis, that increases in BMI were associated with increases in

food cravings, results suggested that the relationship between increases in BMI and

increases in food cravings were specifically related to increases in food cravings for high

fat foods. In addition, individuals with higher BMI’s endorsed multiple traits related to

food cravings including anticipation of an increase in positive or a decrease in negative

emotional states, as well as an increase in feeling guilty related to experiencing or

indulging in a food craving. However, the above findings were only supported for women

and not men, a finding consistent with previous research suggesting a relationship

between increases in BMI and increases in food cravings only for women and not for

men (Sobik et al., 2005).

Among women, increases in BMI may be associated with increases in food

cravings for high fat foods because some research suggests that eating fatty foods

actually increases cravings for more fatty foods. The body does not interpret fat as an

immediate energy source and therefore the brain does not turn off hunger signals (Holt,

55

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Miller, Petocz, & Farmakalidis, 1995). This cycle of fatty food consumption and

increased cravings for more fatty food may contribute to problems with weight gain and

obesity.

Furthermore, results suggested that women with higher BMI’s experience greater

anticipation of an increase in positive or a decrease in negative emotional states, as well

as an increase in feeling guilty related to experiencing or indulging in a food craving.

These findings are consistent with current food craving literature, which has found that

women with emotional difficulties, including pathological eating behaviors, often report

greater food cravings and that these women often also have higher BMI’s (Gendall et al.,

1998; White & Grilo, 2005).

Gender differences in these findings may reflect differences in statistical power,

due to the small sample of men (« = 25), rather than an actual difference between men

and women regarding the relationship between BMI and food cravings. In fact, several

of the correlations between BMI and food cravings for men, although not statistically

significant, were sizable, with increased BMI associated with decreased cravings for

sweets and carbohydrates, yet increased cravings for fast food fats. Higher BMI was also

non-significantly associated with less hunger and greater endorsement of eating to

alleviate negative emotions and in response to environmental cues.

Food Cravings and Gender

The second hypothesis, that gender is associated with food cravings, was

supported. Results indicated that men and women experience food cravings differently.

Findings were consistent with the current literature in that women were found to

experience more food cravings than men, women reported higher rates of food cravings

56

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
for sweets, and men reported more cravings for foods other than sweets, in particular,

cravings for high fat foods (Cepeda-Benito et al., 2003; Zellner et al., 1999). It has been

suggested that women may be more likely to experience food cravings due to the

premenstrual phase of their menstrual cycle and to pregnancy (Dye et al., 1995). A

contribution of the current study is that pregnancy and menstrual phase were controlled

by conducting covariate analyses, which indicated that significant gender differences in

food cravings were still found.

Furthermore, women reported experiencing greater levels of affectivity before and

during a food craving than men. This finding is consistent with the current literature,

which has found that women more often than men have reported experiencing more

distressing emotions before and after food cravings (Lafay et al., 2001). These gender

differences may be influenced by several factors. For instance, women have a higher

prevalence rate of both eating and mood disorders, as well as experiencing more social

pressures regarding maintenance of a certain weight and exhibiting limited food

consumption.

Food Cravings and Negative Affect: The Role of BMI

Findings for the third hypothesis, that negative affect is associated with food

cravings, revealed a relationship between increases in negative affect and increases in

food cravings. However, once again this was only supported for women and not for men.

Some findings suggested that the relationship between negative affect and food cravings

may be the opposite for men, with decreases in negative affect being associated with

increases in food cravings. Indeed, previous research suggests that women more often

. 57

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
associate negative affect with food cravings, while men more often associate food

cravings with neutral and/or positive affect (Lafay et al., 2001).

Moreover, further investigation of the third hypothesis, that BMI moderates the

relationship between negative affect and food cravings, was partially supported by the

current data. The findings suggested that BMI moderated the relationship between food

cravings for high fat foods and negative affect for women but not for men. That is, the

relationship between negative affect and food cravings was stronger among women with

higher BMPs than among women with lower BMPs, but on only one measure of

negative affect and not for affective intensity. This trend suggests that interventions for

negative affect and coping with food cravings could be important components for weight

loss programs for women. Future studies may benefit from utilizing a larger sample of

both men and women, which would not only increase statistical power but would also

increase the variation in BMI among participants.

Food Cravings and Negative Affect: The Role of Gender

The fourth hypothesis, that gender moderates the relationship between negative

affect and food cravings, was also partially supported by the current data. Results

indicated that gender moderated the relationship between food cravings for sweets and

negative affect, and that gender moderated the relationship between hunger and negative

affect; however, analyses comparing differences between men and women were non­

significant. Although these analyses were non-significant, it is important to note that

when comparing the unstandardized regression coefficients, the females’ coefficients

were consistently larger than males'. This pattern suggests that the relationship between

food cravings and negative affect may be stronger for women.

58

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The suggested relationship between food cravings for sweets and negative affect

among women aligns well with current research, which has found that women more often

than men crave sweets and they more often associate negative affect with food cravings

(Lafay et al., 2001; Zellner et al., 1999). Furthermore, the relationship between hunger

and negative affect may be attributable to the greater likelihood of dieting and experience

of subsequent hunger among women than among men. Due to the small number of

participants who endorsed being in a weight loss program (n= 5), it was not possible to

use dieting status in any of the analyses. Although a few reported being in a weight loss

program, this number does not account for the possible number of individuals who may

have been dieting without professional assistance. The current literature has revealed

mixed findings on the relationship between dieting and food cravings (Delahanty et al.,

2002; Harvey et al., 1993; Pelchat, 1997).

Strengths of the Study

The current study has multiple strengths; most notably it is the fist study to

systematically examine the associations between food cravings, BMI, gender, and

negative affect across a set of food craving measures with known psychometric

properties. Use o f standardized measures facilitates replication and cross study

comparisons. Furthermore, although previous researchers have found associations

between food cravings and negative affect, this is the first study to investigate moderators

of the relationship between food cravings and negative affect. Moreover, this study

explored the effects of BMI, gender, and affect on specific and non-specific food

cravings. In addition, the current study controlled for female menstrual phase, which is a

variable that has been found to influence food cravings research. By controlling for

59

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
women’s menstrual phases, it was possible to show that a significant difference between

men and women’s food cravings exists, which cannot be fully explained by biological

differences between the sexes. Furthermore, the study attempted to control for the effects

of food consumption on results by using participants’ reported time intervals between

food consumption and survey completion as a covariate in all analyses.

Limitations of the Study

One of the main limitations of this study is that data were collected based on self-

report measures. Self-report measures are vulnerable to the provision of inaccurate

information due to memory errors or a need for socially desirable responding. Due to the

homogenous sample used, the current findings cannot be generalized to a more diverse

population. Furthermore, the number of male participants (n = 25) was comparatively low

relative to the number of female participants (n = 96). Results of the power analysis

suggested that this number was sufficient to examine gender differences in food cravings,

but may have been insufficient to detect potential moderation effects particularly with

regard to BMI and gender. Moreover, due to the small number of participants who

endorsed being involved in a weight loss program, the influence of dieting could not be

statistically controlled.

Directions for Future Research

Although not all of the hypotheses of this study were fully supported, the findings

do suggest that a relationship exists between food cravings, BMI, gender, and negative

affect. Replication of the present findings with a larger sample size and with men would

be beneficial. Continued food cravings research utilizing a systematic approach would

increase the research communities’ knowledge about food cravings and would facilitate

60

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
the ability to compare findings across studies. Longitudinal research is needed to

determine whether increases in food cravings are associated with prospective increases in

BMI. If such a relationship exists, identification of strategies to curb food cravings may

be of importance in the prevention and treatment of overweight and obesity. Longitudinal

work is also needed to determine the role of negative and positive affect in the

development of food cravings. For instance, does the induction of negative affect

promote the development of food cravings or do individuals feel dysphoric for having

indulged in cravings? Should negative affect promote food cravings, interventions for

weight loss may need to incorporate interventions for negative affectivity.

Conclusions

In conclusion, the current study revealed that food cravings were associated with

BMI, negative affect, and gender. For women, increases in BMI as well as increases in

negative affect were associated with increases in food cravings. Women reported greater

food cravings than men, even when menstrual phase was statistically controlled. In

addition, the relationship between negative affect and food cravings may be stronger for

women than for men and for women with higher BMI’s than for women with lower

BMI’s. Although not all hypotheses were fully supported, the current study contributed to

the literature by showing that food cravings, BMI, gender, and negative affect are related,

utilizing systematic research methods that consisted of an operational definition of food

cravings and measures with known psychometric properties. Longitudinal support of

these relationships would suggest that interventions for weight gain and obesity should

address both negative affect and food cravings among women and food cravings among

men.

61

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
APPENDIX A: DEMOGRAPHIC QUESTIONARRIE

Demographic Questionnaire

1- Age:___________

2. Gender (please circle one): Female Male

3. What cultural or ethic group do you identify with?


(Please Circle one):
a.) American Indian or Alaska Native
b.) Asian
c.) Black or African American
d.) Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander
e.) White-Hispanic origin
f.) White-Non Hispanic origin
g.) Other: (please specify)____________

4. What is the highest level of education you have completed?


(Please circle one):
a.) Some High School
b.) GED/High School Diploma
c.) 1 year of college
d.) 2 years o f college/Associates degree
e.) 3 years of college
f.) 4 years of college/Bachelor Degree
g.) Masters
h.) Doctorate or Professional Degree

5. Height:________ Weight:___________

6. Are you currently participating in a weight loss program (e.g. Weight Watchers, Jenny
Craig)
Yes No

62

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
APPENDIX B: VALIDATION QUESTIONS

Follow-up Questions

1. When did you begin completing this survey?

(a) 1 hour after completing a meal


(b) 2 hours after completing a meal
(c) 3 hours after completing a meal
(d) I did not pay attention to the time or my food intake prior to completing the survey

2. How soon after the completion of your menses did you complete the survey?

(a) during the first week following the completion of my menses


(b) during the second week following the completion of my menses
(c) during the third week following the completion of my menses
(d) I did not pay attention to the duration of time that lapsed since the completion of my
menses prior to completing the survey.

63

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
APPENDIX C: FOOD CRAVING INVENTORY

Food Craving Inventory

A craving is defined as an intense desire to consume a particular food (or food type) that
is difficult to resist.

Over the past month, how often have you experienced a craving fo r the food?

l=never 2=rarely 3=sometimes 4=often 5=always/almost everyday

Fried Chicken
Sausage
Gravy
Fried fish
Bacon
Com bread
Hot dog
Steak
Brownies
Cookies
Candy
Chocolate
Donuts
_ _ Cake
Cinnamon rolls
Ice cream
Rolls
_ _ Pancakes or waffles
Biscuits
___ Sandwich bread
Rice
Baked potato
Pasta
Cereal
Hamburger
___ French fries
Chips
Pizza

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
APPENDIX D: EATING INVENTORY

Part 1
Directions: Please respond to each of the statements below by circling “T” for True c
“F” for False.

T F 1. When I smell a sizzling steak or see a juicy piece of meat, I find it


very difficult to keep from eating, even if I have just finished a meal.

T F 2. I usually eat too much at social occasions, like parties and picnics.

T F 3. I am usually so hungry that I eat more than three times a day.

T F 4. When I have eaten my quota of calories, I am usually good about not


eating any more.

T F 5. Dieting is so hard for me because I just get too hungry.

T F 6. I deliberately take small helpings as a means of controlling my


weight.

T F 7. Sometimes things just taste so good that I keep on eating when I am


no longer hungry.

T F 8. Since I am often hungry, I sometimes wish that while I am eating, an


expert would tell me that I have had enough or that I can have
something more to eat.

T F 9. When I feel anxious, I find myself eating.

T F 10. Life is too short to worry about dieting.

T F 11. Since my weight goes up and down, I have gone on reducing diets
more than once.

T F 12. I often feel so hungry that I just have to eat something.

T F 13. When I am with someone who is overeating, I usually overeat too.

T F 14. I have a pretty good idea of the number of calories in common food.

65

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
T F 15. Sometimes when I start eating, I just can’t seem to stop.

T F 16. It is not difficult for me to leave something on my plate.

T F 17. At certain times of the day, I get hungry because I have gotten used to
eating things.

T F 18. While on a diet, if I eat food that is not allowed, I consciously eat less
for a period of time to make up for it.

T F 19. Being with someone who is eating often makes me hungry enough to
eat also.

T F 20. When I feel blue, I often overeat.

T F 21. I enjoy eating too much to spoil it by counting calories or watching


my weight.

T F 22. When I see a real delicacy, I often get so hungry that I have to eat
right away.

T F 23. I often stop eating when I am not really full as a conscious means of
limiting the amount that I eat.

T F 24. I get so hungry that my stomach often seems like a bottomless pit.

T F 25. My weight has hardly changed at all in the last ten years.

T F 26. I am always hungry so it is hard for me to stop eating before I finish


the food on my plate.

T F 27. When I feel lonely, I console myself by eating.

T F 28. I consciously hold back at meals in order not to gain weight.

T F 29. I sometimes get hungry late in the evening or at night.

T F 30. I eat anything I want, any time I want.

T F 31. Without even thinking about it, I take a long time to eat.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
T F 32. I count calories as a conscious means of controlling my weight.

T F 33. I do not eat some foods because they make me fat.

T F 34. I am always hungry enough to eat at any time.

T F 35. I pay a great deal of attention to changes in my figure.

T F 36. While on a diet, if I eat a food that is not allowed, I often then
splurge and eat other high calorie foods.

Part 2
Directions: Please answer the following questions by circling the number above the
response that is appropriate to you.

37. How often are you dieting in a conscious effort to control you weight?

1 2 3 4
rarely sometimes usually always

38. Would a weight fluctuation of 5 lbs. affect the way you live your life?

1 2 3 4
not at all slightly moderately very much

39. How often are you hungry?

1 2 3 4
only at sometimes often almost
meal times between between always
meals meals

40. Do your feelings of guilt about overeating help you to control your food
intake?

1 2 3 4
not at all slightly often always

67

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
41. How difficult would it be for you to stop eating halfway thought dinner and
not eat for the next four hours?

1 2 3 4
easy slightly moderately very
difficult difficult

42. How conscious are you of what you are eating?

1 2 3 4
not at all slightly moderately extremely

43. How frequently do you avoid “stocking up” on tempting foods?

1 2 3 4
almost never seldom usually almost always

44. How likely are you to shop for low calorie foods?

1 2 3 4
unlikely slightly moderately very
likely likely likely

45. Do you eat sensibly in front of others and splurge alone?

1 2 3 4
never rarely often always

46. How likely are you to consciously eat slowly in order to cut down on how much
you eat?

1 2 3 4
unlikely slightly moderately very
likely likely likely

47. How frequently do you skip dessert because you are no longer hungry?

1 2 3 4
almost seldom at least almost every
never once a week day

68

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
48. How likely are you to consciously eat less than what you want?

1 2 3 4
unlikely slightly moderately very
likely likely likely

49. Do you go on eating binges though you are not hungry?

1 2 3 4
never rarely sometimes at least once
a week

50. On a scale of 0-5, where 0 means no restraint in eating (eating whatever you
want, whenever you want it) and 5 means total restraint (constantly limiting
food intake and never “giving in”), what number would you give yourself?

0
eat whatever you want, whenever you want it
1
usually eat whatever you want, whenever you want it
2
often eat whatever you want, whenever you want it
3
often limit food intake, but often “give in”
4
usually limit food intake, rarely “give in”
5
constantly limiting food intake, never “giving in”

51. To what extent does this statement describe your eating behavior? “I start
dieting in the morning, but because of any number of things that happen
during the day, by evening I have given up and eat what I want, promising
myself to start dieting again tomorrow”.

1 2 3 4
not like me a little pretty good describes me perfectly
like me description
of me

69

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
APPENDIX E: TRAIT VERSION FOOD CRAVINGS QUESTIONNAIRE

FCQ-T

1. Being with someone who is eating often makes me hungry.


Never/Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

2. When I crave something, I know I won’t be able to stop eating once I start.
Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

3. If I eat what I am craving, I often lose control and eat too much.
Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

4. I hate it when I give in to cravings.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

5. Food cravings invariably make me think of ways to get what I want to eat.
Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

6. I feel like I have food on my mind all the time.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

7. I often feel guilty for craving certain foods.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

8. I find myself preoccupied with food.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

9. I eat to feel better.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

10. Sometimes, eating makes things seem just perfect.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
11. Thinking about my favorite foods makes my mouth water.
Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

12. I crave foods when my stomach is empty.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

13. I feel as if my body asks me for certain foods.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

14. I get so hungry that my stomach seems like a bottomless pit.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

15. Eating what I crave makes me feel better.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

16. When I satisfy a craving I feel less depressed.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

17. When I eat what I am craving I feel guilty about myself.


Never/Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

18. Whenever I have cravings, I find myself making plans to eat.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

19. Eating calms me down.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

20. I crave foods when I feel bored, angry, or sad.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

21. I feel less anxious after I eat.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

22. If I get what I am craving I cannot stop myself from eating it.
Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

71

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
23. When I crave certain foods, I usually try to eat them as soon as I can.
Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

24. When I eat what I crave I feel great.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

25. I have no will power to resist my food cravings.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

26. Once I start eating I have trouble stopping.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

27. I can’t stop thinking about eating no matter how hard I try.
Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

28. I spend a lot of time thinking about whatever it is I will eat next.
Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

29. If I give in to a food craving, all control is lost.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

30. When I’m stressed out, I crave food.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

31. I daydream about food.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

32. Whenever I have a food craving, I keep on thinking about eating until I actually
eat the food.
Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

33. If I am craving something, thoughts of eating it consume me.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

72

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
34. My emotions often make me want to eat.
Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

35. Whenever I go to a buffet I end up eating more than what I needed.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

36. It is hard for me to resist thetemptation to eat appetizing foods that are in my
reach.
Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

37. When I am with someone who is overeating, I usually overeat too.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

38. When I eat food, I feel comforted.


Never /Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

39. I crave foods when I’m upset.


Never/Not Applicable Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

73

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
APPENDIX F: POSTIVE AND NEGATIVE AFFECT SCHEDULE-EXPANDED
FORM

PANAS-X
This scale consists of a number of words and phrases that describe different feelings and
emotions. Read each item and then mark the appropriate answer in the space next to that
word. Indicate to what extent you have felt this way during the past two weeks. Use the
following scale to record your answers:

1 2
very slightly a little moderately quite a bit extremely
or not at all

cheerful sad _ active angry at self


disgusted calm _ guilty enthusiastic
attentive afraid joyful downhearted
bashful tired _ nervous sheepish
sluggish amazed _ lonely distressed
daring shaky _ sleepy blameworthy
surprised happy _ excited determined
strong timid _ hostile frightened
scornful alone _ proud astonished
relaxed alert jittery interested
irritable upset _ lively loathing
delighted angry _ ashamed confident
inspired bold _ at ease energetic
fearless blue _ scared concentrating
disgusted shy drowsy dissatisfied
with self with self

74

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
APPENDIX G: SHORT AFFECT INTENSITY SCALE

The Short Affect Intensity Scale

Directions: The following questions refer to the emotional reactions to typical life-events.
Please indicate how YOU react to these events by placing a number from the following
scale in the blank space preceding each item. Please base your answers on how YOU
react, not on how you think others react or how you think a person should react.

Almost Almost
Never Never Occasionally Usually Always Always

1 2 3 4 5 6

When I feel happy it is a strong type of exuberance.


My happy moods are so strong that I feel like I’m “in heaven”.
If I complete a task I thought was impossible, I am ecstatic.
Sad movies deeply touch me.
When I’m happy it’s a feeling of being untroubled and content rather than
being zestful and aroused.
When I talk in front of a group for the first time my voice gets shaky and
my heart races.
When I’m feeling well it’s easy for me to go from being in a good mood to
being really joyful.
When I’m happy I feel like I’m bursting with joy.
When I’m happy I feel very energetic.
When I succeed at something, my reaction is calm contentment.
When I do something wrong I have strong feelings of shame and guilt.
When things are going good I feel “on top of the world”.
When I know I have done something very well, I feel relaxed and content
rather than excited and elated.
When I do feel anxiety it is normally very strong.
When I feel happiness, it is a quiet type of contentment.
When I’m happy I bubble over with energy.
When I feel guilty, this emotion is quite strong.
I would characterize my happy moods as closer to contentment than to
joy.
When I am nervous I get shaky all over.
When I am happy the feeling is more like contentment and inner calm than
one of exhilaration and excitement.

75

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
APPENDIX H: CENTER FOR EPIDEMIOLOGICAL STUDIES DEPRESSION
SCALE

CESD

Using the scale below, indicate the number which best describes how often you felt or
behaved this way DURING THE PAST TWO WEEKS

1- Rarely or none of the time


2- Some or little of the time
3- Occasionally or a moderate amount of time
4- Most or all of the time

I was bothered by things that usually don’t bother me.


I did not feel like eating; my appetite was poor.
I felt that I could not shake off the blues even with help from my family.
I felt that I was just as good as other people.
I had trouble keeping my mind on what I was doing.
I felt depressed.
I felt that everything I did was an effort.
I felt hopeful about the future.
I thought my life had been a failure.
I felt fearful.
My sleep was restless.
I was happy.
I talked less than usual.
I felt lonely.
People were unfriendly.
I enjoyed life.
I had crying spells.
I felt sad.
I felt that people disliked me.
I could not “get going”.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
REFERENCES

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in


social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations.
Journal o f Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-1182.

Carter, F., Bulik, C., Lawson, R., Sullivan, P., & Wilson, S. (1996). Effect of mood and
food cues on body image in women with bulimia and controls. International
Journal o f Eating Disorders, 20(1), 65-76.

Cepeda-Benito, A., Fernandez, M., & Moreno, S. (2003). Relationship of gender and
eating disorder symptoms to reported cravings for food: Construct validation of
state and trait questionnaires in Spanish. Appetite, 40, 47-54.

Cepeda-Benito, A., Gleaves, D., Williams, T., & Erath, S. (2000). The development and
validation of the State and Trait Food-Cravings Questionnaires. Behavior
Therapy, 31, 151-173.

Delahanty, L., Meigs, J., Hayden, D., Williamson, D., & Nathan, D. (2002).
Psychological and behavioral correlates of baseline BMI in the Diabetes
Prevention Program (DPP). Diabetes Care, 25(11), 1992-1998.

Devins, G., Orme, C., Costello, C., Binik, Y., Frizzell, B., Stam, H., & Pullin, W. (1988).
Measuring depressive symptoms in illness populations: Psychometric properties
o f the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D), Psychology &
Health, 2 , 139-156.

Dye, L., Warner, P., & Bancroft, J. (1995). Food craving during the menstrual cycle and
its relationship to stress, happiness of relationship and depression: A preliminary
enquiry. Journal o f Affective Disorders, 34, 157-164.

Eaton, W., Muntaner, C., Smith, C., Tien, A., & Ybarra, M. (2004). Center for
Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale: Review and revision (CESD and CESD-
R). In Mark E. Maruish (Ed), The use ofpsychological testing fo r treatment
planning and outcomes assessment: Volume 3: Instruments fo r adults (3rd ed.),
(pp. 363-377). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

Flint, A., Raben, A., Blundell, J., & Astrup, A. (2000). Reproducibility, power and
validity of visual analogue scales in assessment of appetite sensations in single
test meal studies. International Journal o f Obesity, 24, 38-48.

Gendall, K., Joyce, P., & Sullivan, P. (1997). Impact of definition on prevalence of food
cravings in a random sample of young women. Appetite, 28, 63-72.

Gendall, K., Joyce, P., Sullivan, P., & Bulik, C. (1998). Food cravers: Characteristics
of those who binge. International Journal o f Eating Disorders, 23(4), 353-360.

77

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Gendall, K., Sullivan, P., Joyce, P., Fear, J., & Bulik, C. (1997). Psychopathology and
personality of young women who experience food cravings. Addictive Behaviors,
22(2), 545-555.

Geuens, M., & Des Pelsmacker, P. (2002). Developing a short affect intensity scale.
Psychological Reports, 91(2), 657-670.

Ghiz, L., & Chrisler, J. (1995). Compulsive eating, obsessive thoughts of food, and their
relation to assertiveness and depression in women. Journal o f Clinical
Psychology, 51(4), 491-499.

Harvey, J., Wing, R., & Mullen, M. (1993). Effects on food cravings of a very low
calorie diet or a balanced, low calorie diet. Appetite, 21, 105-115.

Hill, A., Weaver, C., & Blundell, J. (1991). Food craving, dietary restraint and mood.
Appetite, 17, 187-197.

Holt, S.H., Miller, J.C., Petocz, P., & Farmakalidis, E. (1995). A satiety index of common
foods. European Journal o f Clinical Nutrition, 49, 675-690.

Lafay, L., Thomas, F., Mennen, L., Charles, M., Eschwege, E., Borys, J., & Basdevant,
A. (2001). Gender differences in the relations between food cravings and mood in
an adult community: Results from the Fleurbaix Laventi Ville Sante study.
International Journal o f Eating Disorders, 29(2), 195-204.

Larsen, R.J., & Diener, E. (1987). Affect intensity as an individual difference


characteristic: A review. Journal o f Research in Personality, 21, 1-39.

Macdiarmid, J., & Hetherington, M. (1995). Mood modulation by food: An exploration


of affect and cravings in ‘chocolate addicts’. British Journal o f Clinical
Psychology, 3 4 ,129-138.

Mussell, M., Mitchell, J., de Zwaan, M., Crosby, R., Seim, H., & Crow, S. (1996).
Clinical characteristics associated with binge eating in obese females: A
descriptive study. International Journal o f Obesity & Related Metabolic
Disorders: Journal o f the International Association fo r the Study o f Obesity,
20(4), 324-331.

Parker, S., Kamel, N., & Zellner, D. (2003). Food craving patterns in Egypt:
Comparisons with North America and Spain. Appetite, 40, 193-195.

Pelchat, M. (1997). Food cravings in young and elderly adults. Appetite, 28, 103-113.

Radloff, L.S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self report depression scale for research in the
general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401.

78

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Rodin, J., Mancuso, J., Granger, J., & Nelbach, E. (1991). Food cravings in relation to
body mass index, restraint and estradiol levels: A repeated measures study in
healthy women. Appetite, 17, 177-185.

Sobik, L., Hutchison, K., & Craighead, L. (2005). Cue-elicited craving for food: A fresh
approach to the study of binge eating. Appetite, 4 4 ,253-261.

Stunkard, A., & Messick, S. (1985). The Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire to measure
dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. Journal o f Psychosomatic Research,
29(1), 71-83.

Yander Wal, J.S., Johnston, K.A., & Dhurandhar, N.V. (2007). Psychometric properties
of the State and Trait Food Cravings Questionnaires among overweight and obese
persons. Eating Behaviors, 8, 211-223.

Waters, A., Hill, A., & Waller, G. (2001). Bulimics’ responses to food cravings: Is binge-
eating a product o f hunger or emotional state? Behaviour Research and Therapy,
39, 877-886.

Watson, D., & Clark, L.A. (1994). Manual for the Positive and Negative Affect
Schedule-expanded form. University of Iowa.

Weingarten, H., & Elston, D. (1990). The phenomenology of food cravings. Appetite, 15,
231-246.

Weingarten, H., & Elston, D. (1991). Food cravings in a college population. Appetite, 17,
167-175.

White, M.A., & Grilo, C.M. (2005). Psychometric properties of the Food Craving
Inventory among obese patients with binge eating disorder. Eating Behaviors, 6,
239-245.

White, M.A., Whisenhunt, B., Williamson, D., Greenway, F., & Netemeyer, R. (2002).
Development and validation of the Food-Craving Inventory. Obesity Research,
10(2), 107-114.

Zellner, D. A., Garriga-Trillo, A., Rohm, E., Centeno, S., & Parker, S. (1999). Food
liking and craving: A cross cultural approach. Appetite, 33, 61-70.

79

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
VITA AUCTORIS

Bridget Tribout majored in psychology at the University of Connecticut where

she received a Bachelor of Arts in May of 1999. After graduation she went on to work for

the Yale Child Study Center for two years. During her stay at the Child Study Center she

worked as a teacher assistant in the Yale child psychiatric hospital and as a research

assistant for several programs at Yale, including the National Center for Children

Exposed to Violence and the Child Development Community Policing Program. Mrs.

Tribout then went on to pursue her Master of Science and Doctor of Philosophy in

clinical psychology at Saint Louis University. After receiving her Master of Science

Research degree and upon completion of the academic requirements for her doctorate she

was accepted to an APA accredited predoctoral internship at the Central Arkansas

Veterans Healthcare System. Her current research interests are food cravings, health

psychology, Post Traumatic Stress Disorder, and military and law enforcement

psychology.

80

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi