Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 10

Cognitive, Visual, Auditory, and Emotional Factors That

Affect Participation in Older Adults

Monica S. Perlmutter, Anjali Bhorade, Mae Gordon,


Holly H. Hollingsworth, M. Carolyn Baum

KEY WORDS OBJECTIVE. To determine whether changes in hearing, cognition, depression, and vision affect daily life
 cognition disorders participation and whether screening tests that identify problems could be used in the home.

 depression METHOD. Interviewers assessed presence of medical conditions, social class, distance acuity, cognition,
hearing, depression, and participation using valid screening tools. Participation scores were subgrouped
 hearing disorders
according to negative or positive results. Multiple regression analysis determined association of screening
 home care services
tests with participation.
 human activities
RESULTS. Eighty-eight older adults, ages 62–90, participated. Positive screening tests were found in
 mass screening 43% of participants for distance acuity, 9% for cognitive problems, 8% for depression, and 15% for
 vision disorders hearing loss. Relationships were found among age, cognition, depression, and vision and participation.
CONCLUSION. Mild levels of decreased vision, depression, and decreased cognition are associated with
lower participation. These conditions can be screened by occupational therapists and managed to help older
adults remain active and maintain their health.

Perlmutter, M. S., Bhorade, A., Gordon, M., Hollingsworth, H. H., & Baum, M. C. (2010). Cognitive, visual, auditory, and
emotional factors that affect participation in older adults. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 64, 570–579.
doi: 10.5014/ajot.2010.09089

Monica S. Perlmutter, MA, OTR/L, is Instructor of


Occupational Therapy and Ophthalmology, Program in
Occupational Therapy, Washington University School of
A common fear people have as they age is losing the ability to participate in
necessary and desired activities. Many older adults remain active in their
family and community lives. At the same time, a growing population of older
Medicine, 660 South Euclid, Box 8505, St. Louis, MO
63110; perlmutterm@wusm.wustl.edu adults will need to use social services and medical systems to manage their chronic
diseases and resulting disabilities (Beck & Stuck, 1996; Fried & Guralnik,
Anjali Bhorade, MD, is Assistant Professor of 1997). Older adults are the fastest growing segment of the U.S. population
Ophthalmology, Department of Ophthalmology and Visual
today. It has been reported that as many as 40% of people ³ age 65 experience
Sciences, Washington University School of Medicine,
St. Louis, MO. difficulty performing their usual activities (Rubin, Roche, Prasada-Rao, &
Fried, 1994). Manton, Corder, and Stallard (1997) estimated that by 2050, the
Mae Gordon, PhD, is Professor of Ophthalmology and number of older people with one or more disabilities will triple; this poses
Biostatistics, Department of Ophthalmology and Visual
Sciences, Washington University School of Medicine,
a major challenge for older adults, their families, and society.
St. Louis, MO. Everyday instrumental, leisure, and social activities help people sustain their
health as they engage in activities that allow them to remain involved with their
Holly H. Hollingsworth, PhD, is Research Associate
families, communities, and society (Glass, Seeman, Herzog, Kahn, & Berkman,
Professor of Occupational Therapy, Washington University
School of Medicine, Program in Occupational Therapy, 1995; Marsiske, Klumb, & Baltes, 1997; Wilson et al., 2002). As the number
St. Louis, MO. of older adults increases, it is vital that society anticipate the resources needed to
support older adults’ performance. Understanding the difference between
M. Carolyn Baum, PhD, OTR/L, is Professor of
successful and unsuccessful aging and how the normal conditions of aging can
Occupational Therapy, Program in Occupational Therapy,
and Professor of Neurology, Department of Neurology, be managed to retain older adults in a successful state will be important (Rowe
Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO. & Kahn, 1997). We must also increase our efforts to understand the psycho-
logical, physiological, social, and environmental factors associated with the
decline in engagement in activity and occupational performance. We chose to
address participation, which is defined by the World Health Organization

570 July/August 2010, Volume 64, Number 4


(2001) as involvement in a life situation. With this un- functioning and synapse structure that manifest as in-
derstanding, it will be possible to design programs and creased reaction time and slowed processing of in-
services to maximize older adults’ participation in family formation (Hooyman & Kiyak, 2005). In addition,
and community life. chronic illness has been linked to cognitive decline among
Depression and the loss of vision, hearing, and cog- older adults; this accelerated cognitive loss is one of the key
nitive abilities are common in older adults. Reidy et al. risk factors for decreased occupational performance among
(1998) conducted a community-based study and found community-residing elders (Stuck et al., 1999).
that bilateral vision impairment occurs in 30% of adults The aging process can involve many stressful events;
³ age 65. Several common eye conditions, such as macular one’s ability to alleviate stress, learn from experience, and
degeneration, glaucoma, and cataract, may result in vi- obtain a sense of meaning and fulfillment greatly influ-
sion loss significant enough to affect older people’s oc- ences psychological well-being in older adulthood (Foster
cupational performance. Of community-dwelling older et al., 2008). Psychological disorders are common among
adults, 8% have macular degeneration (Reidy et al., community-dwelling older adults, ranging from 15% to
1998), which causes central vision loss and affects activ- 25% (Hooyman & Kiyak, 2005). Depression alone or in
ities such as reading, shopping, community mobility, and combination with other chronic diseases can lead to
participation in leisure activities (Lamoureux, Hassell, & a shortened active life expectancy (Reynolds, Haley, &
Keeffe, 2004). Open-angle glaucoma, which primarily Kozlenko, 2008). Lack of identification and treatment of
affects peripheral vision, occurs in 3% to 8% of older psychological conditions is a key related issue and is
adults living in the community (Campbell, Crews, problematic for many reasons. For example, some symp-
Moriarty, Azck, & Blackman, 1999; Reidy et al., 1998). toms of depression, such as changes in sleeping and eating
Approximately 20.5 million (17.2%) of Americans > age patterns and loss of interest in activities, may be associ-
40 have cataracts in one or both eyes, with the total ated with normal aging (Hooyman & Kiyak, 2005).
number expected to increase to 30.1 million by 2020 Another factor is that mental health services may not be
(Eye Diseases Prevalence Research Group, 2004). Cata- covered by Medicare or Medicaid (Norris, Molinari, &
racts can be managed with surgery, but if left untreated Rosowsky, 1998); therefore, older adults seek treatment
they can cause blurred vision, difficulty with color rec- by primary care physicians, who may be more likely to
ognition, problems with glare, and loss of contrast sen- prescribe medication than to recommend psychosocial
sitivity (Colby, 2008). approaches to therapy (Wetherall, 1998). Given the
Hearing loss is another common chronic condition prevalence of these impairments, it is imperative that
and has been reported at a level of 33.2% to 87% of older occupational therapists identify problems that threaten
adults living in the community (Campbell et al., 1999; older adults’ occupational performance.
Cruickshanks et al., 1998; Keller, Morton, Thomas, & The objective of this descriptive pilot study was to
Potter, 1999; Strawbridge, Wallhagen, Seema, & Kaplan, answer the following research questions:
2000). Hearing loss has the potential to disrupt com- 1. What are the influences on participation of common
munication with others (Hooyman & Kiyak, 2005) and conditions faced by older adults, such as depression
to affect participation in activities such as talking on the and changes in hearing, cognition, and vision?
telephone (Stuck et al., 1999). To compensate for a single 2. Can screening tests that identify problems, such as
sensory impairment, elders will depend on their intact depression and changes in hearing, cognition, and
senses. Unfortunately, older adults with dual sensory vision, be used in the home?
impairments have more difficulty compensating. Dual
sensory losses have been found in 8.6% of older adults
(Campbell et al., 1999), and another study (Bergman & Method
Rosenhall, 2001) found that 23% of people > age 81 have
some degree of dual sensory impairment. Participants and Recruitment
Even in the absence of neurological disease, older adults The institutional review board of Washington University
may experience a variety of age-related central nervous School of Medicine approved the protocol. Ninety-six
system changes that have the potential to cause cognitive community-dwelling adults were recruited from three
decline (Foster, Perlmutter, & Baum, 2008). Decreased sources. A systematic proportional sample of community-
executive functioning is thought to occur as a result of loss dwelling elders was recruited from the 2000, 2001, and
of frontal cortex (Grigsby, Kaye, & Robbins, 1995). Other 2002 membership lists of the Older Adults Services
changes in the brain result in changes in neurotransmitter and Information System (OASIS), an arts-, humanity-,

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 571


and health-oriented volunteer organization. Every fifth Roth, 1968; Katzman et al., 1983). The Hearing Screening
name on the list of OASIS members residing in three Test assesses the capacity for hearing a combination of
target zip code areas in metropolitan St. Louis was selected high- and low-pitched sounds used in everyday conversa-
from each yearly membership list. To be included, the tion without the benefit of lip reading (Scheuerle, 2000).
participants had to live in the community, be ³ age We tested binocular distance visual acuity with the
55, and be willing to participate. Selected members were Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS)
mailed an invitation to participate in the study and were chart developed by Ferris, Kassoff, Bresnick, and Bailey
contacted by telephone. We attempted to contact 636 (1982) to standardize the measurement of distance visual
people (96, Year 1; 386, Year 2; and 154, Year 3). We were acuity. Rather than testing each eye individually, we
unable to contact a high proportion of the target sample tested binocular vision to better reflect functional vision
because of out-of-date contact information and people not (Azen et al., 2002). Visual acuity was tested initially with
being at home. A total of 81 people were contacted by the participants’ usual distance correction under ambient
telephone, 74 of whom signed a consent form and com- household lighting and again under standardized lighting
pleted the assessment. We selected a sample of 20 people similar to an ophthalmic clinical setting. Standard light-
recently discharged to home from the stroke service at ing levels were achieved with the use of a gooseneck-style
Barnes–Jewish Hospital, St. Louis, Missouri; 14 were in- floor lamp. Household lighting was measured with the
cluded. To enrich the sample with people managing a visual Luna-Pro digital light meter (Model No. 4022; Gossen
condition, we enrolled 8 patients diagnosed with age-related GmbH, Erlangen, Germany).
macular degeneration. These participants were living in- We used the Geriatric Depression Scale–Short Form
dependently in the community and were recruited consec- (Alden, Austin, & Sturgeon, 1989; Sheikh & Yesavage,
utively by ophthalmologists at the Barnes Retina Institute. 1986), a 15-item version of a 30-item scale, to assess
presence of depression. The Activity Card Sort (ACS;
Measures Baum, 1995; Baum & Edwards, 2008) assesses an in-
Participants completed a comprehensive assessment battery dividual’s participation in instrumental, leisure, and so-
that included screening tools to capture cognition, de- cial activities and provides a profile of the extent to which
pression, audition, and a standardized measure of distance activities relevant to the individual have been retained,
acuity as well as a measure of participation to determine the given up, or performed less often.
older adults’ current level of engagement in instrumental,
leisure, and social activities. The Person–Environment– Procedure
Occupational Performance (PEOP) Measurement Model Occupational therapy graduate students participated in
(Baum & Christiansen, 2005) provided the rationale for a 1-wk training course to learn to conduct interviews and
the selection of the measurements of the sensory, affective, functional testing. The occupational therapy students also
and cognitive problems faced by older adults in the context had 2 additional days of training on the vision measures by
of their everyday lives. Data from environment and occu- Anne Coleman, Department of Ophthalmology, Uni-
pation measures were also collected and will be the focus of versity of California, Los Angeles, who provided in-
future articles. Specific variables include cognition, hearing, struction and supervision as the students tested normal
vision, depression, and activity participation. Measures volunteers from Washington University and community
used to describe the sample and to collect data for each volunteers. In-house training at the university was fol-
variable are described briefly; psychometric properties and lowed by a day of training in the St. Louis community,
scoring information are included in Table 1. which consisted of performing the battery of assessments
We used a modified version of the Physical Health with older adults in their own homes. Interrater reliability
portion of the Older Adult Resources and Services (OARS; was established for the vision measure on the sample of
Fillenbaum, 1988) Multidimensional Functional Assess- Washington University community volunteers (intraclass
ment Questionnaire and Services Supplement to gather correlation coefficient 5 .92). Older adults who gave
information regarding the study participants’ medical verbal consent by telephone for study participation were
conditions. The Hollingshead Index of Social Position scheduled for a home visit. After verbal consent, potential
was used to measure social class and further describe the participants received letters that confirmed the date and
sample (Hollingshead & Redlich, 1957). time of appointment and included contact information
To screen for memory and concentration deficits, for research staff and photographs of the evaluators.
we used the Short Blessed Test (or Short Orientation– During the home visit, study participation was de-
Memory–Concentration Test; Blessed, Tomlinson, & scribed, and written consent was obtained by the

572 July/August 2010, Volume 64, Number 4


Table 1. Measures Used to Assess Participation, Vision, Depression, Hearing, Social Class, Health Conditions, and Cognition
Instrument Purpose and Psychometrics Scoring
ACS (Baum, 1995; Assesses an individual’s participation in instrumental, Yields a profile of activities that have been retained or
Baum & Edwards, 2008) leisure, and social activities. The content, construct, given up or are now being done with less frequency
and predictive validity have been established (Katz, since age 60. A total score of 80 is possible with a
Karpin, Lak, Furman, & Hartman-Maeir, 2003; range of 0–80.
Sachs & Josman, 2003). The ACS has high internal
consistency for instrumental activities of daily living
and social activities and moderate internal
consistency for low and high physical leisure
activities (Katz et al., 2003).
ETDRS chart (Ferris et al., 1982; Measure of distance visual acuity. Test–retest reliability Positive screening value of 20/40 or worse is indicative
Lovie-Kitchin, 1988) is .98; concurrent validities with the Flom, Snellen, of declining visual function (Salive et al., 1994).
and Illiterate E charts are .91, .94, and .96,
respectively.
Geriatric Depression Study participants answer 15 yes–no questions about Range of possible scores is 0–15. A score of ³5
Scale–Short Form (Alden et al., feelings, interests, activities, and hopes. Reliability, indicates possible depression.
1989; Brink et al., 1985; validity, and sensitivity–specificity studies have been
Sheikh & Yesavage, 1986) conducted on the original 30-item version (Sheikh &
Yesavage, 1986), and the short form distinguishes
depressed from nondepressed study participants
(r 5 .84, p < .001)
Hearing Screening Test Assesses capacity for hearing a combination of high- Score of £4 on a scale ranging from 0 (worst)
(Scheuerle, 2000) and low-pitched sounds used in everyday to 5 (best) is indicative of possible hearing loss.
conversation without the benefit of lip reading.
Hollingshead Index of Social Uses self-reported information regarding the head of Social class scale ranges from 1–5, with 5 being the
Position (Hollingshead & household’s occupation and the participant’s level of highest.
Redlich, 1957) education to determine position on social class scale.
OARS Multidimensional Functional Gathers information regarding medical conditions Participants indicate conditions they have and the
Assessment Questionnaire of the study participants. Concurrent validity, degree to which the conditions bother them.
and Services Supplement–Physical Spearman rank-order correlation coefficient 5 0.82;
Health (Fillenbaum, 1988; interrater reliability, complete agreement for 74% of
Fillenbaum & Smyer, 1981) items; intraclass correlation coefficient 5 .662.
Short Blessed Test (or Short Consists of six items assessing memory, orientation, Range of possible scores is 0–28; the higher the score is,
Orientation–Memory–Concentration and concentration and is a reliable and valid screening the greater is the cognitive impairment. A score of ³8
Test; Blessed et al., 1968; tool for detection of dementia in community and long- is considered indicative of possible cognitive impairment.
Katzman et al., 1983) term care populations. Concurrent validity was established
with the Mental Status Questionnaire (r 5 .945).
Note. ACS 5 Activity Card Sort; ETDRS 5 Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study; OARS 5 Older Adult Resources and Services.

evaluator. Demographic information was collected, and participation, we also computed an effect size for vision,
the assessments were administered. Trained occupational cognition, depression, hearing, and age differences. A
therapy graduate students collected data for this pilot multiple regression analysis was performed to assess the
study over a period of 3 years as part of their master’s contribution of vision, cognition, depression, hearing,
degree projects. To understand the impact of these mild gender, and age to the variability of participation.
impairments, 8 study participants of the original 96 were
excluded from the analysis dataset because they had sig-
Results
nificant vision loss; more than two positive screening
values on cognitive, depressive, or auditory tests; or both. The participants ranged in age from 62 to 90 yr; the mean
age was 74.3 (Table 2). The participants were primarily
female (64%); self-identified White (96%); and married
Data Analysis (58%) and represented a uniform distribution of social
We calculated descriptive statistics including means, status.
standard deviations, and range of scores for each de- Table 3 reports the means, standard deviations, and
mographic and outcome variable. Correlation coefficients ranges for the ETDRS distance acuity chart, Short
were used to calculate relationships between variables. Blessed Test, Geriatric Depression Scale, Hearing Screen-
Student’s paired t tests were conducted to compare ac- ing Test, and age. In addition, mean activity participation
tivity participation for negative and positive findings for scores, as measured by the ACS, are stratified by classifi-
vision, cognition, depression, hearing, and age. To mea- cation on the screening tests as negative or positive. A
sure the magnitude of the standardized differences of participant could screen positive on more than one test, so

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 573


Table 2. Demographic Characteristics of Study Participants vision was limited on the basis of their eye exam. Binocular
(N 5 88) distance visual acuity was assessed with the participant’s
Characteristic n (%) M (SD), range usual correction (95% wore glasses). The mean distance
Age 74.3 (7.2), 62–90 visual acuity value was 52.0 or approximately 20/25 dis-
Gender tance equivalent. Among the 35 participants with a posi-
Female 56 (64)
tive screening value of £20/40 for binocular distance visual
Male 32 (36)
Ethnicity
acuity tested under ambient household light, the mean
White 84 (96) ACS score was 38.4 (standard deviation [SD] 5 10.8)
African-American 3 (3) compared with 48.8 (SD 5 10.7) among those with
Other 1 (1) a normal screening value for vision (p < .0001). We
Marital status computed a moderate effect size correlation of .43 for this
Married 51 (58)
difference.
Single (never married) 6 (7)
Single, divorced, widowed 31 (35)
Few participants had a positive auditory screening,
Socioeconomic status but 15% (n 5 13) could not repeat all of the sounds
1 Lower 14 (16) when tested. Among the 13 participants with a positive
2 25 (28) screening value for hearing, the mean ACS score was 44.5
3 Middle 25 (28) (SD 5 7.5) compared with 44.5 (SD 5 12.5) among
4 15 (17)
participants with a negative screening value (p 5 .80),
5 Upper 6 (7)
Missing 3 (3)
showing no difference. A small effect size correlation for
this difference was computed to be .03.
Note. M 5 mean; SD 5 standard deviation.
In the sample of older adults, only 9% (n 5 8) had
the total number of participants with positive screening cognitive limitations when tested, whereas 25% (n 5 22)
tests is >100%. Of 88 participants, 35 had a positive indicated on the modified Duke Medical Index that they
screening value for one impairment, and 9 had a positive had memory problems. Among the 8 participants with
screening value for two impairments. a positive screening value for cognition, the mean ACS score
Of the participants, 40% (n 5 35) had a positive was 38.7 (SD 5 13.4) compared with 45.3 (SD 5 11.6) for
vision screening score, yet only 15% (n 5 13) self-reported those with a normal screening value (p 5 .14). The small
on the Physical Health portion of the OARS that their effect size correlation for this difference was .16.

Table 3. Comparison of Mean Activity Participation Scores Stratified by Screening Status and Age
Activity Card Sort
Measure n (%) M (SD) Range
Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study (binocular distance acuity) 52.0 (8.4) 28–66
Negative (better than 20/40) 53 (60) 48.8 (10.7)
Positive (220/40 or worse) 35 (40) 38.4 (10.8)
t(86) 5 24.44, p < .0001, r 5 .43
Short Blessed Test 2.7 (7.2) 0–16
Negative (<8) 80 (91) 45.3 (11.6)
Positive (³8) 8 (9) 38.7 (13.4)
t(86) 5 1.49, p 5 .14, r 5 .16
Geriatric Depression Scale 1.5 (0.6) 0–12
Negative (<5) 81 (92) 45.3 (11.9)
Positive (>5) 7 (8) 37.4 (9.1)
t(86) 5 1.70, p 5 .09, r 5 .18
Hearing Screening Test 4.8 (3.3) 0–5
Negative (5) 75 (85) 44.5 (12.5)
Positive (£4) 13 (15) 44.5 (7.5)
t(86) 5 2.26, p 5 .80, r 5 .03
Age 74.3 (7.2) 62–90
Younger (<75 years) 48 (55) 48.8 (11.4)
Older (³75 years) 40 (45) 39.7 (10.6)
t(86) 5 3.33, p 5 .001, r 5 .34
Note. N 5 88. M 5 mean; SD 5 standard deviation; r 5 effect size correlation.

574 July/August 2010, Volume 64, Number 4


Table 4. Pearson Correlation Coefficients of Key Variables (N 5 88)
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. Gender —
2. Age .02 —
3. Hearing screening test 2.05 2.16 —
4. Short Blessed Test 2.17 .18* .07 —
5. Geriatric Depression Scale–Short Form 2.05 .12 .02 .02 —
6. Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study .02 2.45** .01 2.14 2.13 —
7. Activity Card Sort 2.09 2.45** 2.01 2.31** 2.39** .51** —
*p < .05. *p < .01.

Of the participants, 8% (n 5 7) had a positive The measures used in the data analysis were selected
screening value of ³5 on the Geriatric Depression Scale, from a much larger 2.5-hr battery of assessments used in
and 5% (n 5 4) reported mental health problems. a research project. The measures reported in this article
Among the 7 participants with a positive screening value required approximately 25 min to complete. Participants
for depression, the mean ACS score was 37.4 (SD 5 9.1) were receptive to the entire assessment process. All measures
compared with 45.3 (SD 5 11.9) for those with a nega- are available free of cost in the public domain and can easily
tive screening value (p 5 .09). The small effect size be used in the home setting or included in a routine initial
correlation was computed as .18. Older participants en- assessment to determine factors that influence design of
gaged in fewer activities (mean 5 39.7, SD 5 10.6) than rehabilitation programs.
younger participants (mean 5 48.8, SD 5 11.4, p <
.001). The moderate effect size correlation for this dif-
ference was .34 (Table 3). Discussion
Table 4 reports Pearson correlation coefficients be-
The PEOP Measurement Model (Baum & Christiansen,
tween gender, age, Hearing Screening Test, Short Blessed
2005) proved to be a valuable structure for guiding the
Test scores, Geriatric Depression Scale–Short Form rat-
assessment of sensory, affective, and cognitive problems
ing, ETDRS binocular distance visual acuity scores ob-
older adults face in their everyday lives and guided us to
tained under ambient light, and ACS participation scores.
address the physical and social environments as well.
The highest intercorrelations with the ACS were found
Pearson (2000) supported this notion, stating that a con-
for binocular distance visual acuity (r 5 .51), age (r 5
ceptual framework “provides the frame of reference for
2.45), Short Blessed Test scores (r 5 2.31), and Geriatric
understanding the variables studied, for selecting the in-
Depression Scale scores (r 5 2.39).
struments used to collect information and for interpreting
Table 5 reports a multiple regression analysis of the
results” (p. 22). By using the PEOP model, we were able
relationship of screening assessment scores (vision, de-
to create an approach to assessment that allowed us to
pression, hearing, and cognition) and demographic fac-
detect changes in daily activities in a community-residing
tors (age, gender) to activity participation. Factors that
sample of older adults and determined that vision, cognition,
significantly contributed to participation included age,
and depression had a significant impact on participation.
depression, cognition, and vision. Hearing and gender
The findings of this pilot study suggest that partici-
were not associated with ACS participation scores.
pation changes as people age. As would be expected,
younger participants were more active (engaging in 48.8 of
Table 5. Summary of Multiple Regression Analysis for Variables 80 activities) than older participants (engaging in 39.7 of
Predicting Active Participation (N 5 88) 80 activities). However, the problems of decreased vision,
Step and Variable B Standard Error b decreased cognition, and depression are limiting factors
1. Gender 23.76 1.20 2.15 beyond age and must be considered to foster health and
2. Age 20.35 0.15 2.21* participation in older adults. The importance of a com-
3. Hearing 20.55 1.52 2.03 prehensive screening procedure to identify factors that
4. Cognition 20.89 0.30 2.25* may limit participation is supported by the results. People
5. Depression 21.87 0.47 2.32*
at risk for restricted participation need to be identified
6. Distant vision 0.47 0.13 .34*
because many of these age-related conditions may be
Note. R 5 .48, F(6, 81) 5 12.54, p < .0001. B 5 unstandardized coefficients;
2

b 5 standardized coefficients. improved with proper medical care; in addition, activity


*p < .05. plays an important role in maintaining health and

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 575


avoiding excess disabilities. We discuss each of the con- of failing health was linked to depressive symptoms and
tributing factors in further detail. that replacing these activities with suitable alternatives
A distance visual acuity level in the range of 20/40 to resulted in psychological well-being comparable to that
20/70 was a positive screening test for mild visual im- of healthier adults. Depression must be detected early, di-
pairment. We found a statistically significant decrease in agnosed accurately, and managed (Ormel et al., 1998). In
participation at this seemingly adequate visual acuity level. addition to referring clients to their primary physician to
These findings are surprising considering that 20/40 visual address depressive symptoms, occupational therapists are
acuity is considered sufficient to possess an unrestricted well positioned to adapt activities of interest or assist
driver’s license in many states. The 1999–2002 National clients in choosing meaningful replacement activities.
Health and Nutritional Examination Survey (Vitale, The tests selected to screen for distance acuity, cog-
Cotch, Sperduto, & Ellwein, 2006) found that 83% of nition, hearing, depression, and activity participation were
the visual impairment, defined as distance visual acuity of sensitive to mild problems that affected performance. The
20/50 or worse, could be improved to 20/40 or better ETDRS distance acuity chart (Ferris et al., 1982), Short
with refractive correction. Small changes, such as getting Blessed Test (Blessed et al., 1968; Katzman et al., 1983),
new glasses or increasing levels of luminance in the home, Hearing Screening Test (Scheuerle, 2000), Geriatric
may increase visual acuity. In addition, adding contrast Depression Screen–Short Form (Alden et al., 1989;
can increase the visibility and safety of the environment. Sheikh & Yesavage, 1986), and ACS (Baum, 1995; Baum
This finding suggests that low-cost public health in- & Edwards, 2008) proved to be easy to administer and
tervention could have an important impact on quality of applicable for use in home and community settings.
life and function. Rather than rely on self-report of these problems, we
Surprisingly, we found no significant difference in found actual screening for deficits to be important. Actual
participation levels between the study participants who measurement of vision, cognition, hearing, depression,
had a positive Hearing Screening Test (n 5 13, 15%) and and participation is important because older adults may
those with a normal screening value. The small number under- or overreport problems or attribute changes to the
of participants with a positive Hearing Screening Test normal aging process. In addition, changes may be so
limited the statistical power to detect differences. In ad- gradual that they are not noted by the individual or his or
dition, it is possible that the Hearing Screening Test her family members.
(Scheuerle, 2000) is not sensitive enough to detect mild Administration of these tests as part of routine oc-
hearing losses or that mild hearing loss may not have an cupational therapy plays an important role in supporting
impact on instrumental activities of daily living or leisure successful aging in two ways: identifying impairments that
activities that are performed alone, thus participation can limit participation in older adults and addressing these
levels would not be affected. Further investigation is impairments through occupational therapy intervention
warranted. or referring older adults to their primary care physician
In our sample, age, depression, cognition, and vision and other health care professionals.
were related to participation. As people aged, they were The limitations of our study include possible selection
less active. In addition, depression and decreased cognition bias because people who agreed to participate may have
negatively affected participation. People with minor vision a higher level of participation than is truly present in the
impairment (20/40) experienced reduced participation. community. The small sample size of positive screening
These results were surprising and of particular interest tests for hearing, cognition, and depression may not ac-
in light of low vision’s being defined as 20/70 or worse. curately reflect the effects these sensory modalities have on
These findings suggest the importance of occupational participation. Future studies including a larger, more rep-
therapists’ administering screening tools to identify these resentative sample size may provide a better understanding
issues that may affect what people need and want to do. of whether such associations exist. The population used in
The negative relationship indicates that as depression this study was predominantly White and resided in
scores increase, activity participation decreases. Similar a community-dwelling neighborhood. These participants
findings have been noted by Williamson (1998), who were relatively healthy and had fairly good visual acuity.
concluded that stressful life events experienced by older The results of this study may not generalize to other
adults affect psychological well-being “largely (and some- ethnicities, older adults in nursing homes, or people
times, only) to the extent to which they restrict the ability to with increased comorbidities or substantially decreased
engage in routine activities” (p. 327). Benyamini and vision. The etiology of decreased vision in this report was
Lomranz (2004) found that giving up activities because not documented. Further assessment of whether various

576 July/August 2010, Volume 64, Number 4


ocular disorders affect participation differently will be (Baum, Perlmutter, & Edwards, 2000). Occupational
important to determine as well. Last, the cross-sectional therapists are in a prime position to identify impairments
nature of our study makes it difficult to attribute a cause- that will affect the occupational therapy assessment and
and-effect relationship among vision, depression, and intervention process and to connect clients with services
participation. and other health care professionals to enhance occupa-
The cause-and-effect relationship of these variables tional performance and well-being. s
and their impact on occupation and participation are not
known. We pose the following interesting questions that
could guide future research in the area of productive Acknowledgments
aging. This project was possible because of the efforts of occu-
1. Does decreased vision limit participation and ulti- pational therapy graduate students at Washington Uni-
mately result in depression? versity in St. Louis’s Program in Occupational Therapy.
2. Do people who self-identify cognitive and sensory We acknowledge the contributions and support of Michael
changes limit their activities? Kass, MD, and Anne Coleman, MD. Preliminary results
3. Does depression limit participation and cause further of this study were presented at the Association for Re-
decline in participation when vision is limited? search in Vision and Ophthalmology Annual Conference,
4. Is it possible that people with depression and limited Ft. Lauderdale, Florida, May 2005.
vision restrict participation and, therefore, decrease
engagement in experiences, resulting in cognitive
loss? References
These questions can be addressed with large samples Alden, D., Austin, C., & Sturgeon, R. (1989). A correlation
of people with a broader range of sensory and cognitive between the Geriatric Depression Scale long and short
impairment, as well as more diverse ethnic and socio- forms. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Scien-
economic representation. An ideal situation would be to ces and Social Sciences, 44B, 124–125.
have a longitudinal cohort study to test the hypothesis Azen, S. P., Varma, R., Preston-Martin, S., Ying-Lai, M.,
Globe, D., & Hahn, S., for Los Angeles Latino Eye Study
using a structured equation model.
Group. (2002). Binocular visual acuity summation and
A broader issue is the evolution of our understanding inhibition in an ocular epidemiological study: The Los
of the importance of participation and its relationship to Angeles Latino Eye Study. Investigations in Ophthalmology
health and well-being. Inherent in understanding suc- and Visual Science, 43, 1742–1748.
cessful aging is the need to determine how changes as- Baum, C. M. (1995). The contribution of occupation to func-
sociated with aging affect social engagement, leisure tion in persons with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Occu-
participation, and activities instrumental to community pation Science: Australia, 2, 59–67.
Baum, C. M., & Christiansen, C. H. (2005). Person–environ-
independence. This study is a beginning step in describing
ment–occupational performance: An occupation-based frame-
how person, environmental, and occupational factors
work for practice. In C. H. Christiansen, C. M. Baum, & J.
contribute to the participation of older adults. Eventually, Bass-Haugen (Eds.), Occupational therapy: Performance, partic-
such work can inform policy, inform the practice of ipation, and well-being (pp. 244–266). Thorofare, NJ: Slack.
medicine and rehabilitation, and provide evidence for Baum, C. M., & Edwards, D. (2008). Activity Card Sort (2nd
innovative programs for older adults. ed.). Bethesda, MD: AOTA Press.
Factors such as cognition, vision and other sensory Baum, C. M., Perlmutter, M., & Edwards, D. (2000, Sum-
mer). Measuring function in Alzheimer’s disease. Alz-
modalities, and depression dictate, in part, a person’s
heimer’s Quarterly, pp. 44–61.
capacity for doing and influence his or her desire and Beck, J. C., & Stuck, A. E. (1996). Preventing disability: Be-
need to engage in occupation (Foster et al., 2008) and yond the black box. JAMA, 276, 1756–1757.
should be routinely assessed. Knowledge of the presence Benyamini, Y., & Lomranz, J. (2004). The relationship be-
of these impairments can be incorporated into client- tween activity restriction and replacement with depressive
centered assessment and intervention plans. Occupational symptoms among older adults. Psychology and Aging, 19,
therapists should be aware that even seemingly minimal 362–366.
Bergman, B., & Rosenhall, U. (2001). Vision and hearing in
losses of vision and cognition and mild depression can
old age. Scandinavian Audiology, 30, 255–263.
have negative ramifications on participation and poten- Blessed, G., Tomlinson, B. E., & Roth, M. (1968). The asso-
tially on quality of life and that excess disabilities can ciation between quantitative measures of dementia and of
occur when a person has untreated vision or hearing loss senile changes in cerebral grey matter of elderly partici-
or has unrecognized cognitive impairment or depression pants. British Journal of Psychiatry, 114, 797–811.

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 577


Brink, T. L., Curran, P., Door, M. L., Janson, E., McNulty, memory–concentration test of cognitive impairment.
U., & Messina, M. (1985). Geriatric Depression Scale American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 734–739.
reliability: Order, examiner and reminiscence effects. Clin- Keller, B. K., Morton, J. L., Thomas, V. S., & Potter, J. F.
ical Gerontologist, 3(4), 57–59. (1999). The effect of visual and hearing impairments on
Campbell, V., Crews, J., Moriarty, D., Azck, M., & Blackman, functional status. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society,
D. (1999). Surveillance for sensory impairment, activity 47, 1319–1325.
limitation and health related quality of life among older Lamoureux, E. L., Hassell, B. A., & Keeffe, J. E. (2004). The
adults—United States, 1993–1997. MMWR Surveillance determinants of participation in activities of daily living in
Summaries, 48, 131–156. people with impaired vision. American Journal of Ophthal-
Colby, K. (2008). Eye disorders: Cataract. Retrieved January 3, mology, 137, 265–270.
2009, from www.merck.com/mmpe/sec09/ch104/ch104a. Lovie-Kitchin, J. E. (1988). Validity and reliability of visual
html?qt5cataract&;alt5sh#sec09-ch104-ch104a–512 acuity measurements. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics,
Cruickshanks, K. J., Wiley, T. L., Tweed, T. S., Klein, B. E., 8, 363–370.
Mares-Perlman, J. A., & Nondahl, D. M. (1998). Preva- Manton, K. G., Corder, L., & Stallard, E. (1997). Chronic
lence of hearing loss in older adults in Beaver Dam, Wis- disability trends in the elderly United States populations:
consin: The epidemiology of hearing loss study. American 1982–1984. Proceedings of the National Academy of Scien-
Journal of Epidemiology, 148, 879–886. ces, USA. Medical Sciences, 94, 2593–2598.
Eye Diseases Prevalence Research Group, National Eye Insti- Marsiske, M., Klumb, P., & Baltes, M. M. (1997). Everyday
tute. (2004). Citations and abstracts from April 2004 Ar- activity patterns and sensory functioning in old age. Psy-
chives of Ophthalmology: Prevalence of cataract and chology and Aging, 12, 444–457.
pseudophakia/aphakia among adults in the United States Norris, M. P., Molinari, V., & Rosowsky, E. (1998). Providing
[Abstract]. Retrieved January 3, 2009, from www.nei. mental health care to older adults: Unraveling the maze of
nih.gov/eyedata/pbd6.asp Medicare and managed care. Psychotherapy: Theory, Re-
Ferris, F. L., Kassoff, A., Bresnick, G. H., & Bailey, I. (1982). search, Practice, Training, 35, 490–497.
New visual acuity charts for clinical research. American Ormel, J., Kempen, G. I. J. M., Deeg, D. J. H., Brilman, E. I.,
Journal of Ophthalmology, 94, 91–96. van Sonderen, E., & Relyveld, J. (1998). Functioning,
Fillenbaum, G. G. (1988). Multidimensional functional assess- well-being, and health perceptions in late middle-aged
ment of older adults: The Duke Older Americans Resource and older people: Comparing effects of depressive symp-
and Services procedures. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. toms and chronic medical conditions. Journal of the Amer-
Fillenbaum, G. G., & Smyer, M. (1981). The development, ican Geriatrics Society, 46(1), 39–48.
validity and reliability of the OARS Multidimensional Pearson, V. I. (2000). Assessment of function in older adults.
Functional Assessment Questionnaire. Journal of Gerontol- In R. L. Kane & R. A. Kane (Eds.), Assessing older persons:
ogy, 36, 428–434. Measures, meaning, and practical applications (pp. 17–48).
Foster, E., Perlmutter, M., & Baum, C. M. (2008). Evaluating New York: Oxford University Press.
occupational performance of older adults. In S. Coppola, Reidy, A., Minassian, D. C., Vafidis, G., Joseph, J., Farrow, S.,
S. J. Elliott, & P. E. Toto (Eds.), Strategies to advance ger- Wu, J., et al. (1998). Prevalence of serious eye disease and
ontology excellence: Promoting best practice in occupational visual impairment in a north London population: Popu-
therapy (pp. 349–382). Bethesda, MD: AOTA Press. lation based, cross sectional study. British Medical Journal,
Fried, L. P., & Guralnik, J. M. (1997). Disability in older 326, 1623–1646.
adults: Evidence regarding significance, etiology, and risk. Reynolds, S. L., Haley, W. E., & Kozlenko, N. (2008). The
Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 45, 92–100. impact of depressive symptoms and chronic diseases on
Glass, T. A., Seeman, T. E., Herzog, A. R., Kahn, R., & active life expectancy in older Americans. American Jour-
Berkman, L. F. (1995). Change in productivity in late nal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 16, 425–432.
adulthood: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Journals Rowe, J. W., & Kahn, R. L. (1997). Successful aging. Geron-
of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social tologist, 37, 433–440.
Sciences, 50B, 65–76. Rubin, G. S., Roche, K. B., Prasada-Rao, P., & Fried, L. P.
Grigsby, K., Kaye, K., & Robbins, K. (1995). Behavioral distur- (1994). Visual impairment and disability in older adults.
bance and impairment of executive functions among the Optometry and Visual Science, 71, 750–760.
elderly. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 21, 167–177. Sachs, D., & Josman, N. (2003). The Activity Card Sort: A
Hollingshead, A. B., & Redlich, F. C. (1957). Social class and factor analysis. OTJR: Occupation, Participation and
mental illness: A community study. New York: Wiley. Health, 23(4), 165–176.
Hooyman, N. R., & Kiyak, H. A. (2005). Social gerontology: A Salive, M. E., Guralnik, J., Glynn, R. J., Christen, W., Wallace,
multidisciplinary perspective. Boston: Pearson Education. R. B., & Ostfeld, A. M. (1994). Association of visual
Katz, N., Karpin, H., Lak, A., Furman, T., & Hartman-Maeir, impairment with mobility and physical function. Journal
A. (2003). Participation in occupational performance: Re- of the American Geriatric Society, 42(3), 287–292.
liability and validity of the Activity Card Sort. OTJR: Scheuerle, J. (2000). Hearing and aging. Educational Gerontol-
Occupation, Participation and Health, 23, 1–9. ogy, 26, 237–247.
Katzman, R., Brown, T., Fuld, P., Peck, A., Schecter, R., & Sheikh, J. I., & Yesavage, J. A. (1986). Geriatric Depression
Schimmel, H. (1983). Validation of a short orientation– Scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of

578 July/August 2010, Volume 64, Number 4


a shorter version. In T. L. Brink (Ed.), Clinical gerontology: ment in the United States, 1999–2002. Ophthalmology,
A guide to assessment and intervention (pp. 165–173). New 113, 2163–2170.
York: Haworth Press. Wetherall, J. L. (1998). Treatment of anxiety in older adults. Psy-
Strawbridge, W. J., Wallhagen, J. I., Seema, S. J., & Kaplan, chotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 35, 444–458.
G. A. (2000). Negative consequences of hearing impair- Williamson, G. M. (1998). The central role of restricted normal
ment in old age: A longitudinal analysis. Gerontologist, 49, activities in adjustment to illness and disability: A model of
320–326. depressed affect. Rehabilitation Psychology, 43, 327–347.
Stuck, A., Walthert, J., Nikolaus, T., Bula, C., Hohmann, C., & Wilson, R. S., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Barnes, L. L., Schneider,
Beck, J. (1999). Risk factors for functional status decline in J. A., Bienias, J. L., Evans, D. A., et al. (2002). Participa-
community-living elderly people: A systematic literature re- tion in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of inci-
view. Social Science and Medicine, 48, 445–469. dent Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA, 287, 742–748.
Vitale, S., Cotch, M. F., Sperduto, R., & Ellwein, L. (2006). World Health Organization. (2001). International classification
Costs of refractive correction of distance vision impair- of functioning, disability and health. Geneva: Author.

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 579

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi