Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 17

Lighting Res. Technol.

2010; 42: 313–329

Field surveys of the effect of lamp spectrum on


the perception of safety and comfort at night
C Knight PhD
Philips Lighting B.V., 5600 JM Eindhoven, The Netherlands

Received 3 February 2010; Revised 2 June 2010; Accepted 5 June 2010

Public lighting improves visibility and provides orientation. It also contributes to


the perception of comfort and safety of people outside after dark. At present, high-
pressure sodium lamps are widely used in street lighting. This is in part due to
their high efficacy and relatively long lifetime (16 000 hours). Their use, however,
comes at the expense of good colour rendering. Recently developed ceramic
metal halide lamps provide many of the advantages of HPS in addition to white
light and better rendering of colours. In this paper, results of research conducted
in three European countries on the effect of lamp spectrum on face recognition
and the perception of safety and comfort outdoors are presented. The results
consistently show that at comparable illuminances, people perceive areas
illuminated with white light to be brighter, safer and more comfortable than the
same neighbourhood illuminated with yellowish light.

1. Introduction take avoiding action if necessary and the


absence of discomfort or disability glare.
Outdoor lighting has several important roles. The average illuminance or luminance, the
In addition to enabling safe movement, light distribution, the glare from the installa-
improving visibility and providing orienta- tion and the spectrum of the light source used
tion, public lighting is increasingly expected can all influence the safe movement, visual
to contribute to the perception of safety and performance and perception of safety and
comfort of people outside after dark. Without comfort of people at night. The CIE1 has
conditions that ensure safe movement, it made recommendations for light levels and
would not be possible for people to walk on glare control for various outdoor lighting
the street, and without conditions that ensure applications. These recommendations do not
a general perception of safety, people might take the effect of lamp spectrum into consid-
choose not to walk on the streets. Factors eration. However, as illustrated by the refer-
contributing to safe movement after dark ences below, there is a growing body of
include visual orientation and the ability to evidence, albeit not entirely consistent, which
detect obstacles on the pavement, which may suggests that the spectrum of the light source
otherwise be a trip hazard. Factors contrib- can have a major effect on visual task
uting to the perception of safety include the performance and the perception of safety
perception of spatial brightness of the area, and comfort at night.
the ability to recognise the expression or faces At present, high-pressure sodium (HPS)
of other road users at a distance sufficient to lamps are widely used in outdoor applications
due to their high efficacy and relatively long
lifetime (16 000 hours). Their use, however,
Address for correspondence: Colette Knight, Philips Lighting
B.V., 5600 JM Eindhoven, The Netherlands. comes at the expense of good colour render-
E-mail: colette.knight@hotmail.com ing (CRI of HPS 25). Recently developed
ß The Chartered Institution of Building Services Engineers 2010 10.1177/1477153510376794

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


314 C Knight

ceramic metal halide (CMH) lamps provide The perception of safety in an area depends
many of the advantages of HPS in addition to on a number of factors that are not related to
having better colour rendering (CRI460) and lighting including the familiarity of the person
a white colour appearance. Related benefits in the area and the crime rate of the area.
of these lamps for residents and pedestrians Nevertheless, research conducted by Boyce
in the areas illuminated by them might et al.8 in New York City and Albany, NY,
include greater ease of facial recognition and suggests that there is a link in the public mind
more accurate colour identification. Indeed, between the perception of safety of an area
an earlier laboratory study conducted by after dark and the perception of brightness of
Raynham and Saksvikronning2 concluded that area. Consequently, there is a need for
that twice the illuminance of HPS is required residential areas to appear appropriately
to achieve the same facial recognition distance brightly illuminated at night to support the
as with white compact fluorescent light perceived safety of people in the area at night.
sources at typical nighttime outdoor light Fotios and Cheal9,10 used brightness rat-
levels. The benefits of white light for facial ings, brightness rankings and brightness
recognition are already taken advantage of in matching to compare the effect of lamp
the British Standard for road lighting, spectrum on the perceived brightness in lab-
BS5489 : 20033,4 as well as the Italian oratory tests. Their results showed that at
Standard, UNI 11248 : 20075. Both of these equal illuminance, lighting from white MH
national standards allow a lower illuminance and compact fluorescent light sources were
to be used in residential areas if the CRI of perceived to be significantly brighter than
the light source used is over 60. from yellowish HPS. Moreover, they found
There is not yet consistent agreement on that at the typical illuminances encountered
the effect of lamp spectrum on facial recog- on urban streets (2–15 lux), the same percep-
nition. Two studies6,7 have reported that light tion of brightness was achieved when the
source type had no significant effect on facial illuminance ratio of MH to HPS was 0.73.
recognition. Rea et al.,6 for example, per- These results were consistent with early lab-
formed a facial recognition test under HPS oratory studies conducted by Rea11 in which
and metal halide (MH) lighting systems. In subjects were asked to adjust the illuminance
their test, one of three confederate pedestrians on a scale model scene illuminated with a
stood behind a partition, while a test partic- HPS source until it matched the brightness of
ipant holding a DVD player with eight digital the same scene illuminated by a MH source.
colour photographs of possible confederate At luminances of 0.1 and 1 cd/m2, the illumi-
pedestrians was requested to start walking nance ratio (MH/HPS) found to achieve an
towards the confederate pedestrian and stop equal perception of brightness was 0.71. This
as soon as he or she could guess who the means that people perceived scenes illumi-
confederate pedestrian was from among the nated with MH light sources to be equally as
eight different pictures. The test participant bright as scenes illuminated with HPS light
then resumed walking and stopped again sources when the measured illuminance was
when he or she was certain. The basis for 29% lower for the MH scene. By contrast, a
judgment was recorded along with the mea- study by Boyce and Bruno12 has suggested
sured distance for the guess and certainty: that there is no difference in the perceived
they did not find any statistically significant brightness of parking lots illuminated with
difference between the MH and HPS lamps MH and HPS at equal illuminance.
for the ‘guess’ or ‘certain’ facial recognition In the more recent field tests conducted by
distances. Rea et al.,6 respondents stood in the middle of
Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


Lamp spectrum, safety and comfort at night 315

a street between two luminaires and com- not the higher perceived safety and brightness
pared the perception of brightness of opposite at equal illuminance of CMH compared to
ends of the street by alternatively looking at HPS reported by several academic studies
the street scenes illuminated by each lumi- done under well-controlled conditions are
naire. Posters with the same painted scenes indeed recognised in actual residential streets.
were located beneath each luminaire. Subjects In other words, this work will enable us to
compared a variety of scenes in which the gauge whether or not the effect of lamp
average vertical illuminance on the poster spectrum is strong enough to be found in
illuminated with HPS varied between 5 lux actual residential streets after dark. Although
and 15 lux, and the average vertical illumi- care was taken to minimise the importance of
nance on the poster in the opposite direction factors not related to the lamp spectrum,
that was illuminated with CMH also varied when doing tests in actual residential streets,
between 5 lux and 15 lux. Subjects were it is practically impossible to control all
given written questionnaires, and for each potential influencing factors such as light
pair of lighting conditions, they were asked to from homes or some variation in the light
make a forced choice for the lighting condi- distribution when different types of lamps are
tion, under which they would feel safer to used in the same luminaires and with the same
walk at night and under which the street and spacing between poles.
surroundings as well as objects in their view In this paper, results of field tests con-
appeared brighter. The test included pairs of ducted in actual urban streets in three differ-
lighting conditions for which the ratios of the ent European countries, namely, the
average vertical illuminance on the poster Netherlands, Spain and the United
illuminated with CMH to the average vertical Kingdom on the effect of lamp spectrum on
illuminance on the poster illuminated with the perception of safety, comfort and face
HPS (CMH/HPS) varied between 0.33 and 3. recognition are presented. Altogether, over
Interpolation of the results suggested that an 300 residents participated in the experiments
illuminance ratio of CMH/HPS of 0.79 was under both HPS and CMH street lighting.
required to create an equal perception of
brightness, and a ratio of 0.66 was required to 2. Lamp technical properties
create an equal perception of safety. These
results reinforce the viability of maintaining The lamps used in the experiments were based
the same perception of safety while reducing on HPS and CMH technologies. The com-
the illuminance when using CMH instead of mercial names for the HPS and CMH lamps
HPS lamps. used together with some properties of the
As evidenced by the references listed above, lamps are listed in Table 1. The lamp spectra
there are a number of laboratory studies and are shown in Figure 1.
field tests done in parking lots or on isolated
streets, which have evaluated the effect of
lamp spectrum on perceived safety and spatial 3. Research methods
brightness in an area. These studies were
generally not carried out in the target appli- Research was conducted in Eindhoven, NL,
cation area, that is, in actual residential streets Navalcarnero, Spain, and St. Helens, UK, by
or with a wide cross section of people IPM International as part of a quantitative
representative of those who might use such study commissioned by Philips Lighting to
an area. One of the objectives of the work evaluate how residents experienced the street
reported here was to determine whether or lighting in their neighbourhoods before and
Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


316 C Knight

after it was changed from SON (CCT average vertical illuminance measured on the
2000K) to CDO 2800K or CDM 4200K, pictures at the position used in the facial
and how this change affected the perception recognition tests was 10 lux under both
of safety, comfort and appreciation of their SON and CDO.
neighbourhoods. In all cases, the existing In the Netherlands and UK, streets were
luminaires were used and only the lamps used on which the luminaires were mounted
were changed. on mast arms (brackets) and optical control
In Spain, architectural post-top luminaires of the light was via the reflector in the
were used in the residential area in which the luminaire. Since CDO and SON lamps have
tests were done. The luminaires consisted of different light center lengths, depending on
diffuse glass bowls through which the light the reflector of the luminaire used, there can
was transmitted. There was a reflector at the be differences in the light distribution of the
top of the luminaire to prevent upward two installations. In the Netherlands, where
emission of light. As a result of the diffuse 100 W SON and 100 W CDO 2800K lamps
transmission, the light distribution was were used, the average horizontal illuminance
unchanged between the SON and CDO measured on the pavement between two poles
lamps. In Spain, where 150W SON and in the test location was 16.5 lux under SON
150W CDO lamps were used, the average and 14 lux with CDO 2800K. The average
horizontal illuminance was 82 lux under vertical illuminance measured on the pictures
SON and 81 lux under CDO, and the at the position used in the facial recognition
tests was 3.3 lux under SON and 1.4 lux
Table 1 Correlated colour temperature (CCT) and colour
under CDO 2800K. This difference in vertical
rendering index (CRI) of HPS and CMH sources used in illuminance is a result of the difference in light
the experiments distribution of the SON and CDO lamps with
Technology Commercial CCT (K) CRI the reflector used.
name As evident from Table 2, the tests in the
HPS SON T 2000 25
UK were conducted after those in the NL and
CMH Master City White 2800 2800 83 in Spain. In addition to evaluating the change
CDO-TT 2800K from (1) SON to CDO 2800K, additional
CMH Master City White 4200 4200 90
CDM-TT 4200K
tests were conducted in neighbouring streets
in St. Helens, UK, where the lighting was

SON CDO 2800K CDO 4200K


HPS technology CMH technology CMH technology
yellow-white light white light white light
power distribution
Relative spectral

400 500 600 700 400 500 600 700 400 500 600 700
Wavelength (nm) Wavelength (nm) Wavelength (nm)

Figure 1 Light spectra of SON (based on HPS technology), CDO 2800K and CDM 4200K (both CDO and CDM are based
on CMH technology)

Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


Lamp spectrum, safety and comfort at night 317

changed from (2) SON to CDM 4200K, and tests at night with minimal risk. The
(3) from CDM 4200K to CDO 2800K and residents were sent or shown a letter inform-
(4) from CDO 2800K to SON. The latter was ing them that tests were being conducted to
done to check whether or not the ratings evaluate the perception of safety of the area
given were due to the lamp spectrum or to the after dark. There was no mention of lighting
fact that residents expect certain changes once or the commissioner (i.e. Philips) in the
the street lighting is changed. The average letters.
horizontal illuminance on the pavement in the The number of different respondents who
four test streets varied between 9.1 lux and participated in the tests in each area is shown
12.7 lux under SON and 8.9 lux and in Table 2. The respondents were recruited
12.6 lux under CDO. With the luminaires from people living in the vicinity but not in
installed in the test streets in the UK, the the actual streets in the experimental area.
difference in the average horizontal and The splits over gender and age groups (below
vertical illuminance was less than 5% between and above 40 years) are shown in Table 3.
SON and CDO installations in a given test Additional recruitment criteria were that
area. respondents could express themselves clearly
The people responsible for public lighting and that they walked or biked outside after
in the respective cities identified possible dark at least three times a week. The colour
locations where the lighting could be changed vision and acuity of respondents was not
according to the research schedule. One tested. However, it should be noted that in the
requirements for the test areas was that comparisons made, the same respondents
they were safe. This was necessary to ensure performed the colour and facial recognition
that the researchers could conduct interviews tests under both the ‘before’ and ‘after’

Table 2 The number of respondents and the timing of evaluations done in three European cities

Location Number of Installed lamps and test dates Evaluations done


different
respondents
Initial Test date Lighting Test date Comparison Subjective
lighting initial after second of visual evaluation
condition condition change condition performance
p p
Eindhoven, NL 55 SON T March 2006 CDO TT April 2006
100 W 2800K 100 W p
56 CDO TT April 2006
2800K 100 W
p p
Navalcarnero, 60 SON T April 2007 CDO ET May 2007
Spain 150 W 2800K 150 W p
60 CDO ET May 2007
2800K 150 W
p p
St. Helens, UK 30 SON T November 2008 CDO TT January 2009
70 W 2800K 70 W p p
33 SON T November 2008 CDM TT January 2009
70 W 4200K 70 W p p
31 CDO TT November 2008 SON T 70 W January 2009
2800K 70 W p p
31 CDM TT November 2008 CDO TT January 2009
4200K 70 W 2800K 70 W

Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


318 C Knight
Table 3 Gender and age groups used in the experiments

Eindhoven, NL (n ¼ 111) Navalcarnero, Spain (n ¼ 120) St. Helens, UK (n ¼ 125) All three countries (n ¼ 356)

# Male # Female # Male # Female # Male # Female # Male # Female

40 17 13 29 33 26 25 72 71
440 54 27 33 25 30 44 117 96

condition, and a ‘within subjects’ statistical research design was not used in the subse-
analysis was done. quent tests done in the UK. Instead, addi-
The test involved individual face-to-face tional lighting changes were investigated. In
interviews during which a detailed question- the tests in the UK, each participant per-
naire was filled in. In addition, objective formed the test under both lighting conditions
measurements of facial recognition were con- in one area and different participants per-
ducted. Each test lasted 45 minutes. The formed the tests in the different areas.
first test of the evening started at least In all three countries, the lighting was
30 minutes after sunset. In the UK and changed soon after the first set of interviews
Netherlands, the last test of the evening was (‘before’ interviews) were completed and the
completed before 10:15 pm. In Spain, the last ‘after’ interviews were started at least 3 weeks
test of the evening was completed before after installation of the new lighting. There
11 pm. was no extra maintenance (e.g. cleaning)
A mixed research design was used in the when the lamps were changed.
Netherlands and Spain. This means that 55 During the face-to-face interviews, the
Dutch respondents and 60 Spanish respon- respondents were asked to
dents participated in the test both under the (1) rate their perception of safety and com-
initial lighting condition as well as after the fort in the test area on a five-point scale
lighting had been changed (the ‘before and (see Table 4 for questions asked),
after’ group), while others only participated in (2) rate the importance of street lighting to
the subjective evaluations after the lighting their perception of safety and comfort,
had been changed (the ‘after only’ group; and
Table 2). This method was used to check if the (3) list the most important aspects of lighting
mean rating of the second lighting (CDO for them and to evaluate the street light-
2800K in both Spain and NL) and compar- ing in the test area against these and other
ison with the previous lighting condition aspects.
(SON in both cases) was different between
Subsequently, the respondents were explic-
the group of people who participated in the itly asked to
test under the initial lighting condition and
the group that only participated in the test (4) make various comparisons using a seven-
under the changed lighting condition. The point scale with respect to the previous
data from the two groups were analysed and lighting condition (see Table 5 for ques-
plotted separately as shown, for example, in tions asked).
Figures 4 and 5. Since, as will be presented in Visual performance was evaluated on the
the results, no significant differences in the basis of the distance to recognise faces and
rating of the second lighting conditions were colours. During the facial recognition test, the
found between the two groups, a mixed researchers stood with their back towards the
Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


Lamp spectrum, safety and comfort at night 319
Table 4 Mean ratings for different lighting conditions in St. Helens

Questions Mean rating

N Condition 1 ! Condition 2 Condition 1 Condition 2 p

How do you feel about this 30 SON ! CDO 2800K 1.93 1.37 50.05
area here? After sunset, do you feel: 33 SON ! CDM 4200K 1.91 1.39 50.05
(1) very comfortable/at 31 CDO 2800K ! SON 1.61 1.68 40.05
ease , (5) very uncomfortable/uneasy? 31 CDM 4200K ! CDO 2800K 1.74 1.65 40.05
Now I would like you to tell 30 SON ! CDO 2800K 2.40 1.67 50.05
me what you think of the lighting in 33 SON ! CDM 4200K 2.61 1.30 50.05
terms of its quality: By quality I mean 31 CDO 2800K ! SON 1.94 2.58 50.05
nice light, good colour. Do you feel that it is: 31 CDM 4200K ! CDO 2800K 1.77 1.94 40.05
(1) very pleasant , (5) very unpleasant
And now I would like to know whether the 30 SON ! CDO 2800K 2.20 1.33 50.05
lighting here makes you feel safe or not. 33 SON ! CDM 4200K 2.06 1.33 50.05
Does it make you feel: (1) very safe) , 31 CDO 2800K ! SON 1.52 2.06 50.05
(5) very unsafe? 31 CDM 4200K ! CDO 2800K 1.65 1.68 40.05
And how do you rate the brightness of the area. 30 SON ! CDO 2800K 3.38 2.90 50.05
For you personally, is it: (1) much too bright , 33 SON ! CDM 4200K 3.34 3.00 50.05
(5) much too dark or (3) just right? 31 CDO 2800K ! SON 3.00 3.61 50.05
31 CDM 4200K ! CDO 2800K 2.97 3.03 40.05

closest pole so that the picture was only chosen under the initial lighting. The vertical
illuminated from the distant neighbouring illuminance at the position of the pictures was
pole. The researchers held pictures with the measured under both lighting conditions.
faces of well-known personalities for the The protocol for the facial recognition test
particular country in front of themselves. was as follows. As shown schematically in
The pictures were printed on non-glossy A4 Figure 2, the test person (P) started walking
paper so that the size of the face was slowly from a distance of 15 m towards the
approximately life sized. A total of eight researcher (R) holding the picture. The test
different pictures were used. The pictures person was instructed to stop and say as soon
were divided into two groups of four pictures. as they were close enough to
Half of the residents were shown one group of
four pictures under the initial lighting, and the
(1) identify the gender of the person on the
picture,
second group of pictures after the lighting had
(2) see the picture well enough to guess the
been changed. The other half of the partici-
identity of the person, and
pants was shown the pictures under the
(3) see the person well enough to be sure of
reverse lighting conditions so that all pictures their identity.
were observed under both lighting conditions.
The order of the four pictures shown was It was stressed that the focus was on seeing
randomised among the different participants. the picture well enough to guess or be sure of
Only the respondents in the ‘before þ after’ the identity of the person on the picture even
group did the facial recognition test, and a if the respondent did not know the person or
within-subject analysis was done to compare remember their name. Both the mean distance
the performance under the different light at which the respondents could guess the
sources. identity of the person as well as when they
The poles used and the position at which said they were sure of the identity of
the researcher stood relative to the pole was the person on the picture is given in Table 6.
Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


320 C Knight
Table 5 Mean ratings for various evaluations comparing the second to the first lighting condition

Question Land N Group Condition 1 Mean Standard Test value ¼ 4,


! error p (2 tailed)
Condition 2 mean

Now I would like to ask you how NL 55 bþa SON ! CDO 2800K 3.28 0.24 50.05
comfortable and pleasant the present 56 a only SON ! CDO 2800K 3.24 0.19 50.05
lighting is in your personal opinion? Spain 60 bþa SON ! CDO 2800K 2.12 0.10 50.05
Compared to the lighting before, is it? 60 a only SON ! CDO 2800K 2.31 0.15 50.05
much more comfortable (1) UK 30 bþa SON ! CDO 2800K 1.77 0.20 50.05
the same (4) much less comfortable (7) 33 bþa SON ! CDM 4200K 1.94 0.17 50.05
31 bþa CDO 2800K 4.06 0.37 40.05
! SON
31 b þ a CDM 4200K 3.45 0.21 50.05
! CDO 2800K
And how about safety? How does NL 55 bþa SON ! CDO 2800K 3.42 0.17 50.05
the present lighting compare with the 56 a only SON ! CDO 2800K 3.25 0.15 50.05
lighting before? Does it make you feel Spain 60 bþa SON ! CDO 2800K 2.41 0.11 50.05
much safer (1) the same (4) 60 a only SON ! CDO 2800K 2.63 0.13 50.05
much less safe (7) UK 30 bþa SON ! CDO 2800K 2.07 0.21 50.05
33 bþa SON ! CDM 4200K 1.97 0.16 50.05
31 bþa CDO 2800K 4.32 0.28 40.05
! SON
31 b þ a CDM 4200K 3.71 0.18 40.05
! CDO 2800K
And what about the brightness NL 55 bþa SON ! CDO 2800K 3.17 0.21 50.05
of the area? Does it look much 56 a only SON ! CDO 2800K 2.76 0.18 50.05
brighter (1) the same (4) Spain 60 bþa SON ! CDO 2800K 2.08 0.96 50.05
much less bright (7) 60 a only SON ! CDO 2800K 2.35 0.12 50.05
UK 30 bþa SON ! CDO 2800K 1.67 0.19 50.05
33 bþa SON ! CDM 4200K 1.85 0.23 50.05
31 bþa CDO 2800K 4.61 0.35 40.05
! SON
31 b þ a CDM 4200K 3.65 0.21 40.05
! CDO 2800K

b þ a ¼ ‘before þ after’ group; a only ¼ ‘after only’ group.

The associated standard error of the mean is


also given.

Pavement
1 2 3 4 5 6 . . . . . . . 15m
4. Results ~150cm
R P
~150cm
4.1 Perception in streets illuminated with SON
Pole Neighbouring pole
and CDO Street -Illuminates picture
In all three countries, the most important R: Position at which researcher stood holding pictures
aspect of street lighting given by the respon- P: Position from which the respondent started walking towards the
dents was the ‘brightness’ of the illuminated researcher.
area. Consistent with studies referenced in the The respondent paused to say when he/she
(1) could identify the gender of the person
‘Introduction’ section, a higher spatial bright- (2) could guess the identity of the person and
ness of the street and pavement contributes to (3) was sure of the identity of the person

a higher perception of safety. Since one of the Figure 2 Schematic of the setup used in the facial
requirements of the areas chosen was that it recognition tests

Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


Lamp spectrum, safety and comfort at night 321

was safe, it is not surprising that independent lighting on their perception of safety when the
of the lighting, people felt relatively safe in all lighting was changed from CDM 4200K to
test areas. CDO 2800K. However, when the lighting was
changed from SON to either CDO 2800K or
As illustrated by the histograms of the
CDM 4200K, the perception of safety, com-
results from the UK, substantially more
fort, brightness and light quality was
people felt very comfortable when the same
improved (p50.05 according to the t-tests).
area was illuminated with CDO 2800K or
CDM 4200K compared to SON (Figure 3).
This trend was also seen in Eindhoven and in
Navalcarnero. The mean and standard error 4.2 Order of lamp change–absolute ratings
of the mean is written next to the plots in As discussed above, when the lighting was
Figure 3 and also by similar plots in later changed from SON to CDO 2800K, the
figures. perception of safety, comfort, brightness and
The respondents were asked the same light quality was statistically significantly
questions under both lighting conditions. improved. When the reverse change was
At the point where they were asked these made and the lighting was changed from
questions under the first lighting condition, CDO 2800K to SON, there was a statistically
there was no discussion that the lighting
significant reduction in the rated light quality,
would be changed, and under the second
brightness of the area and the effect of
lighting condition, there was no mention that
lighting on the perception of safety (p50.05
the lighting had been changed. Table 4
according to t-tests).
summarises how respondents in the UK
After the lighting in Hereford Close had
answered various questions on five-point
scales regarding their perception of comfort been changed from CDO 2800K to SON, the
in the area, the quality of the lighting and the area was evaluated to be ‘too dark’ (Table 4).
effect of the street lighting on their perception The brightness of the area was previously
of safety and brightness of the area. The rated to be ‘just right’ when CDO 2800K
means for the above evaluations are given for lamps at a comparable average horizontal
the situation both before and after the light- illuminance were installed. The respondents
ing had been changed in the different loca- also rated the light quality and their percep-
tions. For each evaluation, a Levene’s test tion of safety in this same street illuminated
was used to access the equality of variances of with SON on average lower than how they
the sample taken before and the sample taken rated it when it was previously illuminated
after the lighting was changed. There were no with CDO 2800K. With this change from
significant differences in the variances found CDO 2800K to SON, the average rating for
for the different evaluations. Therefore, it was the perception of comfort decreased slightly
appropriate to use paired-sample t-tests to under SON. However, in this case, the differ-
determine if the means of the ratings under ence was not statistically significant (p40.05).
the first and second lighting condition are The results indicate that for pedestrians,
different or not. There is a difference when streets illuminated with white light are per-
the corresponding value in probability (p) ceived to be brighter and safer and at least
column in Table 4 is less than 0.05. equal but often more comfortable than the
As seen in Table 4, there were no statisti- same streets with SON at comparable average
cally significant differences according to a illuminance. This effect is found independent
t-test (p40.05) regarding how the same of the order in which SON and CDO street
people rated the area and effect of the street lighting is evaluated.
Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


322 C Knight
Table 6 Mean distances for facial recognition measured in tests done in Eindhoven, Navalcarnero and St. Helens

Condition Horizontal Vertical Distance Distance to p


illuminance on illuminance on to guess identify
pavement (lux) picture (lux) person person
mean  mean  mean  mean 
standard standard standard standard
deviation deviation error mean error mean
(m) (m)

Eindhoven, The Netherlands (55 respondents)


1 SON T 16.5  1.6 3.3  0.6 7.6  0.5 5.4  0.5 50.05
2 CDO TT 2800K 14.1  1.5 1.4  0.4 8.5  0.2 6.6  0.4
Navalcarnero, Spain (60 respondents)
1 SON T 81.2  2.4 10  0.7 9.7  1.9 8.5  0.26 50.05
2 CDO ET 2800K 82.4  2.6 10  0.8 11.1  1.4 10.9  0.21
The Shires and Wedge Avenue, St. Helens, UK (30 respondents)
1 SON T 12.7  2.9 1.6  0.5 10.2  0.6 8.7  0.5 50.05
2 CDO TT 2800K 12.1  3.3 1.6  0.5 11.3  0.4 9.8  0.4
Hereford Close, St. Helens, UK (31 respondents)
1 CDO TT 2800K 10.9  4.5 1.6  0.6 9.1  0.5 7.6  0.5 50.05
2 SON 11.5  3.4 1.6  0.6 10.1  0.5 8.9  0.7
Shropshire Gardens, St. Helens, UK (33 respondents)
1 SON 9.1  2.5 0.6  0.3 7.6  0.3 5.7  0.5 50.05
2 CDM TT 4200K 8.9  2.7 0.6  0.4 9.1  0.5 7.1  0.6
Ledger Road, St. Helens, UK (31 respondents)
1 CDM TT 4200K 12.3  3.0 1.6  0.7 10.5  0.6 9.8  0.5 50.05
2 CDO TT 2800K 12.6  3.4 1.6  0.6 11.9  0.7 11.3  0.5

4.3 Awareness of change CDO 2800K. When triggered to look at the


After answering the questions shown in street lighting, the majority of respondents,
Table 4, the respondents were asked during including those in the ‘after only’ groups in
the second condition if they had noticed any the Netherlands and Spain who had not
recent changes in the test area. As mentioned spontaneously mentioned the street lighting,
in the research methods section, the respon- eventually reported that the colour of the
dents in general did not live in the streets street lights had changed or that brighter
where the lighting had been changed, but in street lights had been installed.
the vicinity. About 50% of the Dutch respon-
dents in the ‘before þ after’ group spontane-
ously mentioned the street lighting had been 4.4 Comparison between initial and changed
changed, as did about 40% in the Dutch ‘after street lighting
only’ group. By comparison, in Spain, 77% Since the street lighting in the Netherlands
of the respondents in the ‘before þ after’ and Spain was changed on a commonly used
group and 50% of the respondents in the connecting road in the residential area, even
‘after only’ group spontaneously mentioned those respondents who did not do the test
that the street lighting had been changed. In under the first condition (i.e. ‘after only’
St. Helens, 85% of the respondents noticed group) were familiar with the test area. In the
the change from SON to CDO and 55% UK, where smaller residential streets were
noticed the change from CDM 4200K to used, all of the respondents did the test under
Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


Lamp spectrum, safety and comfort at night 323
25 SON The Shires & Wedge Ave. SON CDO 2800K
40 ‘Before+After’ 2.12±0.10
19 1.93 ± 0.15
8 3 group n =60
0

Number of respondents
25 CDO 2800K 36
1.37 ± 0.09 11
5
19 8
0
11
0 40
‘After only’ 2.31 ± 0.15
25 SON Shropshire Gardens group n = 60
20 1.91 ± 0.13 32
9 5 5
0 2 2 9 8 1
25 0
Number of respondents

CDO 4200K
1.39 ± 0.08 More comfort Less comfort
Same Can’t say
20 13
Much more A little more A little less Much less
0 comfortable comfortable
comfortable comfortable
25 CDO 2800K Hereford Close 1 3 5 7

17 12 1.61 ± 0.16
Figure 4 Histograms showing how respondents in
0 1 1 Navalcarnero, Spain, compare the perception of comfort
25 SON
1.68 ± 0.12
after the lighting was changed to CDO. The mean and the
standard error of the mean are listed. Question: How
16 12
0 3 comfortable is the current street lighting compared to the
25 street lighting before? (SON ! CDO 2800K, CDO is the
CDO 4200K Ledger Road
‘current’ lighting).
19 1.74 ± 0.10
10
0 2
25 CDO 2800K
1.65 ± 0.10 SON CDO 2800K
18
12 30 3.42±0.17
0 1 ‘Before + After’
group n = 55
Nnumber of respondents

Very Comfortable Uncomfortable Very 26


comfortable Neutral uncomfortable
1 2 3 4 5 8 10 1 2 1 3
0 4
30 ‘After only’ 3.25±0.15
Figure 3 Histograms showing how respondents in St.
Helens, UK, rated the perception of comfort before and group n = 56
after the lighting had been changed. Question: How do 27
you feel about the area here? After sunset, please 3 11 13
1 1
rate how you feel on a five-point scale from very 0
comfortable/very much at ease (1) to very uncomfort- Safer Same Less safe Can’t say
able/very uneasy (5). Much A little A little Much less
safer safer less safe safe
1 3 5 7

Figure 5 Histograms showing how respondents in


the initial as well as the second lighting Eindhoven, NL, compared the perception of safety in
the test neighbourhood after the lighting had been
condition. Thus, the vast majority of respon- changed to CDO. The mean and the standard error of
dents could make an evaluation as to whether the mean are listed. Question: How does the present
or not they felt equally comfortable (or safe, lighting compare with the lighting before? Does it make
you feel much more safe, the same or much less safe?
etc.), or less or more so than before. The (SON ! CDO 2800K, CDO is the ‘present’ lighting).
comparison was done using a seven-point
scale, where ‘no difference’ was assigned a
value of four. A one-sample t-test was used to When specifically asked during the second
check the difference between the mean of the lighting condition to compare the lighting
distribution and the test value ‘4’ (no differ- with the previous condition, the majority of
ence). In Figures 4–6, results of some of the respondents in all three countries rated the
responses are shown graphically, and in CDO street lighting to be equally or more
Table 5, a wide range of data is summarised. comfortable than the SON street lighting.
Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


324 C Knight

30 SON CDO 2800K 1.67 ± 0.19 4.5 Order of lamp change–comparison ratings
16
Consistent with their absolute rating of
11 2 1
0 their perception of safety, comfort and bright-
30 SON CDM 4200K 1.85 ± 0.23 ness under the two lighting conditions
Number of respondents

21 6
(Table 4), when asked to compare, respon-
4 1 1
0 dents said that their perception of safety,
30 CDO 2800K SON 4.61 ± 0.35 comfort and brightness was improved under
11
8
CDO lighting (Table 5). The differences were
3 3 1 4
0
1 statistically significant according to indepen-
CDM 4200K CDO 2800K 3.65 ± 0.21
dent one-sample t-tests, which were done to
30
determine whether or not respondents rated
0
2 4 2 20 1 2 the second condition differently from the first.
As mentioned in the previous section, the
Brighter Same Less bright t-tests used a test value of four, which was the
Much A little A little less Much less value assigned when the respondents did not
brighter brighter bright bright perceive a difference between the second and
1 3 5 7
first condition.
Figure 6 Histograms showing how respondents in In the test street in St. Helens, where the
St. Helens, UK, compared the brightness in the test reverse change was made, namely, the lighting
neighbourhoods after the lighting had been changed as
shown on the plots. The mean and standard error of the was changed from CDO 2800K to SON, some
mean are listed. Question: How does the present lighting respondents reported that their perception of
compare with the lighting before in terms of brightness
and quantity of light?
safety had deteriorated, while the majority
said that they did not notice any change in the
perceived safety in the area. As a result, the
difference in their comparison on their per-
ception of safety was not statistically signif-
This finding was independent of whether or icantly different according to an independent
not the respondents participated in the test one-sample t-test (p40.05). Nevertheless, as
under the initial lighting conditions. Also, noted previously, the average rating of their
those respondents who only participated in perception of safety and brightness was sta-
the test after the SON lighting had been tistically significantly lower in the second
changed to CDO lighting and who recognised lighting condition under SON than under
that the lighting had changed evaluated the the initial lighting condition with CDO 2800K
CDO lighting to be more comfortable (i.e. (Table 4). The fact that this was not trans-
there were no statistically significant differ- lated into a significant difference in the rated
ences between the ‘before þ after’ and the comparison when the CDO lighting was
‘after only’ groups in either the Netherlands offered first and SON offered subsequently
or Spain). might indicate that there is a positive
The most common reasons given for the enhancement in the comparison of the
increased comfort of CDO street lighting were second lighting condition since respondents
related to perception of the respondents that might automatically expect an improvement
they could see more clearly, better and further. when street lighting is changed. Nevertheless,
The reason most often given by the few respon- despite with this ‘expected improvement’,
dents who rated the area to be less comfortable SON is not rated more positively than white
after the change to white light was that the area light even when it is shown as the second
was too brightly lit in their opinion. lighting condition.
Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


Lamp spectrum, safety and comfort at night 325

SON ˜ CDO 2800 SON ˜ CDO 2800


Much less bright Much less safe
Less bright Less safe

A little less bright A little less safe


95% CI

95% CI
Same Same
3.2 3.4
A little brighter A little safer
2.1 2.4
Brighter 2.1
1.7 Safer
Much brighter Much safer
NL UK Spain
NL UK Spain
Country
Country
Figure 7 Comparison of how respondents in the
Netherlands, the UK and Spain rated the spatial bright- Figure 8 Comparison of how respondents in the
ness of the area illuminated with CDO 2800K compared Netherlands, the UK and Spain rated their perception of
to when it was illuminated with SON. The data shown for safety in the area illuminated with CDO 2800K compared
each country are the mean and the associated 95% to when it was illuminated with SON. The data shown for
confidence interval. each country are the mean and the associated 95%
confidence interval.

No difference was found between the according to a Scheffe test for the differences
perceived safety under CDO 2800K and between means). The Scheffe test was used in
CDM 4200K in St. Helens. CDM 4200K this case since the sample sizes in the three
was not evaluated in the Netherlands and countries were not equal. However, there is a
Spain in this test. statistically significant difference between the
The main reason given in all three countries Netherlands and the other two countries
for the increased perception of safety under (p50.05 according to a Scheffe test). In the
white light is related to the perception of Netherlands, the average difference in ratings
higher brightness of the whole area. The of spatial brightness and perceived safety
majority of respondents perceive the area between streets illuminated with SON and
illuminated with CDO street lighting to be CDO were lower than in Spain or the UK.
brighter, even though the measured illumi- The data for all of the countries together
nance was not increased (Table 6). This is was also analysed for differences between
consistent with previous laboratory studies different age (40 and 440 years old) and
referenced in the ‘Introduction’ section. gender groups. As shown in Table 7, no
significant differences were found between the
4.6 Comparisons between countries different age and gender groups of this sample
The results presented up to this point are (p40.05 according to Scheffe tests).
based on the data analysed separately for In summary, in all three countries, respon-
each of the three countries. In Figures 7 and 8, dents generally evaluated the area when
the average rating for the difference in illuminated with CDO street lighting as
perceived spatial brightness and safety after brighter, safer and more comfortable com-
switching the lamps from SON to CDO pared to when it was illuminated with SON.
2800K is compared between the countries. Furthermore, most respondents appreciated
There is no statistical difference on either the increased perception of brightness and
rating between the UK and Spain (p40.05 associated higher perceived safety achieved by
Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


326 C Knight
Table 7 Mean ratings of various evaluations organised according to gender and age group

Respondent N Mean  standard Compared to


group deviation respondent
group

Now I would like to ask you Male 440


how comfortable and pleasant Male 40 23 2.2  0.9 Female 40
the present lighting is in Female 440
your personal opinion? Male 40
Compared to the lighting before, is it? Male 440 52 2.5  1.5 Female 40
– much more comfortable (1) Female 440
– the same (4) Male 40
– much less comfortable (7) Female 40 35 2.2  1.3 Male 440
Female 440
Male 40
Female 440 33 3.0  1.7 Male 440
Female 40
And how about safety? Male 440
How does the present lighting Male 40 23 2.5  1.1 Female 40
compare with the lighting before? Female 440
Does it make you feel Male 40
– much safer (1) Male 440 52 2.8  1.3 Female 40
– the same (4) Female 440
– much less safe (7) Male 40
Female 40 35 2.5  1.3 Male 440
Female 440
Male 40
Female 440 33 2.9  1.1 Male 440
Female 40
And what about the Male 440
brightness of the area? Male 40 23 2.2  0.8 Female 40
Does it look Female 440
– much brighter (1) Male 40
– the same (4) Male 440 52 2.6  1.5 Female 40
– much less bright (7) Female 440
Male 40
Female 40 35 2.1  1.2 Male 440
Female 440
Male 40
Female 440 33 2.8  1.5 Male 440
Female 40

using white CDO street lighting. This was the pictures as well as the distances when they
achieved at the same installed power and said they were sure of the identity are given in
comparable average illuminances. Table 6. Most respondents were able to
identify the gender of the person under all
lighting conditions from their starting posi-
4.7 Facial recognition tion (i.e. from a distance of 15 m).
In each area, respondents in the ‘before þ In Spain and the Netherlands, the distance
after’ group performed facial recognition tests at which residents were sure that they could
under two lighting conditions. The average recognise faces on the picture was increased
distances at which the respondents thought by more than 20% under CDO lighting. In
they could guess the identity of the person on tests conducted in the UK, independent of
Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


Lamp spectrum, safety and comfort at night 327

whether the test was first done under CDO In all test streets used in this work, SON
2800K, CDM 4200K or SON, respondents lamps were replaced with CDO 2800K or
consistently expressed that they felt that their CDM 4200K lamps in the same luminaires or
visibility and ability to see expressions, faces vice versa. In the case of the Spanish test
and details was improved under white CDO location, the average horizontal illuminance
sources. However, this was not consistently on the pavement during the test was 1.5%
reflected in the results from the facial recog- lower with SON than with CDO 2800K.
nition tests done in St. Helens. In Hereford In the UK test streets, it was 2% to 5%
Close, where the test was first done under higher in the case of SON compared to CDO
CDO 2800K and then under SON, the mean 2800K. In the Dutch test location, the average
distance for facial recognition was longer horizontal illuminance was 14% higher with
under SON. It should also be noted that in SON than with CDO 2800K. This was caused
Hereford Close, the mean distance for facial by differences in the light distribution when
recognition under CDO was lower than in using SON and CDO 2800K lamps in the
other test locations in St. Helens where the luminaires used. Nevertheless, at the illumi-
vertical illuminance on the pictures were nances used in all of the test locations,
comparable. The reason for this is unclear. respondents evaluated the spatial brightness
to be higher when the same streets were
illuminated with CDO than when they were
5. Discussion illuminated with SON. As noted in the
section comparing the results from the three
The results presented in this paper are based different countries, the average difference in
on quantitative research exploring the effect ratings of spatial brightness and safety
of lamp spectrum on people’s perception of between SON and CDO were lower in the
street lighting after dark. The results show Netherlands than in the UK or Spain. The
that people experience several benefits in role that the differences in average horizontal
residential streets when white light is used illuminance, light distribution or other factors
instead of yellowish street lighting. In partic- might have played in the difference between
ular, the perceptions of brightness, comfort the ratings in the Netherlands and the other
and safety at comparable average illumi- two countries was not examined further.
nances are significantly enhanced in the The results of the facial recognition tests
same street as judged by respondents in were not consistent. In all test locations, inde-
three European countries who conducted the pendent of whether or not the tests were first
tests in streets before and after the lighting done under SON or CDO, respondents
had been changed between SON and warm described their ability to see faces better under
CMH street lighting. CDO than under SON. However, the faces on
The fact that a significant effect of the light the pictures were often, but not always recog-
source spectrum on the perception of bright- nised from further away under CDO than
ness and safety is not only found under under SON. This inconsistency might point to
controlled laboratory conditions as refer- the complexity of the facial recognition task
enced in the ‘Introduction’ section but also and the need for further studies to clarify the
in actual residential streets underpins its role of different factors, which can influence
relevance in residential street lighting appli- facial recognition. These factors might include
cation. It also reinforces the need to include it glare, light distribution and lamp spectrum
as a parameter in recommendations for res- among others in addition to the vertical or
idential street lighting. semi-cylindrical illuminance.
Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


328 C Knight

The results of these field tests together with use of lower illuminances while maintaining
other published results13,14 illustrate the lim- the perception of spatial brightness and safety
itations of the current practice of using the in an area. The on-going work of CIE TC
photopic luminous efficiency function V() at 4-48 to define the visual effects of spectral
mesopic light levels (i.e. between 0.05 and power distribution under mesopic conditions
5 cd/m2). V() is used to transform the in road lighting with a focus on residential
spectral power distribution of a light source streets and pedestrian areas is also very
into a single measure of the light level relevant in this respect.
(luminance and illuminance). V() character-
ises the spectral sensitivity of foveal cones,
which peak at 555 nm under photopic lighting References
conditions (i.e.45 cd/m2). However, many
1 Commission Internationale de l’Eclairage.
of the light levels encountered on residential Recommendations for the lighting of roads for
streets at night fall within the mesopic range. motor and pedestrian traffic. CIE Publication
At mesopic light levels, both rods and cones 115–1995. Vienna: CIE, 1995.
in the retina may be active. This leads to 2 Raynham P, Saksvikronning T. White light
changes in the spectral sensitivity with chang- and facial recognition. The Lighting Journal
ing light levels since the relative contributions 2003; 68: 29–33.
of rods and cones vary with changing light 3 British Standards Institution (BSI), BS EN
levels in the mesopic region. The peak of the 13201-2:2003. Road Lighting – Part 2:
spectral sensitivity of rods is at 507 nm. Performance Requirements. London: BSI,
2003.
Therefore, for lighting applications at night,
4 Scott A. White light – the UK balance sheet.
the effectiveness of lamps with relatively more The Lighting Journal 2005; 70: 18–20.
short wavelength emissions (e.g. white light 5 Norma Italians, UNI 1124800. Illuminazione
sources compared to yellow light sources) can Stradale, Selezione delle Categarie
be underestimated by the current system of Illuminotechniche. Rome: Norma Italians,
photometry. This is currently being addressed 2007.
by various technical committees within the 6 Rea MS, Bullough JD, Akashi Y. Several
CIE. In particular, CIE TC 1-58 has estab- views of metal halide and high pressure sodium
lished mesopic sensitivity functions, which lighting for outdoor applications. Lighting
can serve as the foundation of a system of Research and Technology 2009; 41: 297–320.
7 Boyce PR, Rea MS. Security lighting: Effects
mesopic photometry based on visual task of illuminance and light source on the capa-
performance (e.g. detection of objects, speed bilities of guards and intruders. Lighting
of detection and identification of the objects). Research and Technology 1990; 22: 57–79.
This system is not expected to correlate well 8 Boyce PR, Eklund NH, Hamilton BJ, Bruno
with visual assessment of brightness in the LD. Perceptions of safety at night in different
mesopic region. However, another technical lighting conditions. Lighting Research and
committee (TC 1-37) is developing a supple- Technology 2000; 32: 79–91.
mentary system of photometry for evaluation 9 Fotios SA, Cheal C. Lighting for subsidi-
of lighting in terms of brightness at all ary streets: Investigation of lamps of
different SPD. Part 1 – visual performance.
illuminances.
Lighting Research and Technology 2007; 39:
The use of a more appropriate system of 215–232.
mesopic photometry will encourage the use of 10 Fotios SA, Cheal C. Lighting for subsidiary
more visually effective light sources for the streets: Investigation of lamps of different
mesopic range. This can contribute to an SPD. Part 2 – brightness. Lighting Research
increased perception of safety or enable the and Technology 2007; 39: 233–252.

Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015


Lamp spectrum, safety and comfort at night 329

11 Rea MS. Essay by invitation. Lighting Design Bodrogi P, Várady G, Szalmas A. Mesopic
and Application 1996; 26: 15–16. visual efficiency IV: A model with relevance
12 Boyce PR, Bruno LD. An evaluation of high to night-time driving and other applications.
pressure sodium and metal halide light sources Lighting Research and Technology 2007; 39:
for parking lot lighting. Journal of the 365–392.
Illuminating Engineering Society 1999; 28: 14 Rea MS, Bullough JD. Making the move
16–32. to a unified system of photometry.
13 Goodman T, Forbes A, Walkey H, Eloholma Lighting Research and Technology 2007;
M, Halonen L, Alferdinck J, Freiding A, 39: 393–408.

Lighting Res. Technol. 2010; 42: 313–329

Downloaded from lrt.sagepub.com at INSTITUTO BIOMECANICA VALENCI on September 4, 2015

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi