Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 8

Article

Smooth Pursuit Eye-Tracking Impairment


in Childhood-Onset Psychotic Disorders

Sanjiv Kumra, M.D., F.R.C.P. Objective: Although childhood-onset Results: Subjects with childhood-onset
schizophrenia is relatively rare, a sizable schizophrenia had qualitatively poorer
group of children with severe emotional eye tracking, higher root mean square er-
Alexandra Sporn, M.D.
disturbances have transient psychotic ror, lower gain, and a greater frequency
symptoms that fall outside of current syn- of catch-up saccades than healthy chil-
Daniel W. Hommer, M.D. dren. Subjects with psychotic disorder not
drome boundaries. The relationship of this
otherwise specified also had qualitatively
Robert Nicolson, M.D. group of children to those with childhood-
poorer eye tracking, higher root mean
onset schizophrenia and other childhood
square error, and lower gain than healthy
psychiatric disorders is unclear. In this
Gunvant Thaker, M.D. children, but saccade frequency did not
study, the authors compared smooth pur- differ significantly.
suit eye tracking, a biological trait marker
Elizabeth Israel, B.A. Conclusions: Children with childhood-
associated with schizophrenia, of children
onset schizophrenia exhibit a pattern of
and adolescents with psychotic disorder
Marge Lenane, M.S.W. eye-tracking dysfunction similar to that
not otherwise specified to that of children
reported for adult patients. Similar abnor-
with childhood-onset schizophrenia and
Jeffrey Bedwell, B.S. malities were seen in the subjects with
healthy comparison subjects. psychotic disorder not otherwise specified
Leslie K. Jacobsen, M.D. Method: By means of infrared oculogra- except that they did not exhibit a greater
frequency of catch-up saccades. Prospec-
phy, smooth pursuit eye movements dur-
tive longitudinal neurobiological and clin-
Peter Gochman, B.A. ing a 17°/second visual pursuit task were
ical follow-up studies of both groups are
quantitatively and qualitatively com-
currently underway to further validate
Judith L. Rapoport, M.D. pared in 55 young adolescents (29 with
the distinction between these two disor-
childhood-onset schizophrenia and 26 ders. Also, family studies are planned to
with psychotic disorder not otherwise establish whether eye-tracking dysfunc-
specified) and their respective indepen- tion represents a trait- or state-related
dent healthy comparison groups (a total phenomenon in subjects with psychotic
of 38 healthy subjects). disorder not otherwise specified.

(Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1291–1298)

A cross all of medicine, the study of individuals with an


early onset of a multifactorial disorder has been an impor-
hood-onset schizophrenia had other disorders (3). Within
this group, a relatively large number of children with severe
tant approach used to minimize heterogeneity and possi- emotional disturbances reported brief, intermittent psy-
bly identify unique or more salient risk factors. Since 1990, chotic symptoms that did not meet the DSM-III-R/DSM-IV
a study of childhood-onset schizophrenia (onset of psy- duration or severity criteria for schizophrenia.
chotic symptoms by age 12) has been ongoing at the Na- Children with atypical or subclinical psychotic symp-
tional Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) (1). These clinical toms have been well recognized but difficult to classify (4,
and neurobiological studies have provided support for the 5). Because of the clinical heterogeneity of this group, a
continuity of childhood-onset schizophrenia with later- subgroup from the larger population of children with
onset schizophrenia (2). The fact that childhood-onset psychotic disorder not otherwise specified, provisionally
schizophrenia is often chronic and refractory and that it labeled “multidimensionally impaired,” was studied as a
may be associated with higher rates of familial “schizo- parallel group to the childhood-onset schizophrenia
phrenia spectrum disorders” has led to the speculation group (3, 6, 7). Children from this psychotic disorder not
that this form of the disorder may represent a particularly otherwise specified subgroup shared a number of phe-
severe variant with a stronger genetic predisposition (2). nomenological similarities with the childhood-onset
Perhaps because of its relative rarity, childhood-onset schizophrenia patients, including prepsychotic linguistic
schizophrenia is frequently misdiagnosed (3). As part of our deficits (6), higher rate of schizophrenic spectrum disor-
search for subjects with childhood-onset schizophrenia, it ders in first-degree relatives (6), a generalized pattern of
became apparent from chart review or in-person examina- cognitive deficits (8), and quantitative structural brain ab-
tions that most patients with a referring diagnosis of child- normalities (e.g., enlarged lateral ventricles and smaller

Am J Psychiatry 158:8, August 2001 1291


EYE-TRACKING IMPAIRMENT IN CHILDHOOD PSYCHOSIS

total cerebral volume) seen at initial magnetic resonance Method


imaging (MRI) scan (9). In contrast, a 2–8 year prospective
clinical follow-up study of 27 of these subjects with this Subjects
psychotic disorder not otherwise specified subtype The recruitment and diagnostic procedures have been de-
scribed previously (3). These are summarized briefly herein, and
showed that psychotic symptoms had been transient and
the methods for the eye-tracking procedure are given in more de-
that none had progressed to schizophrenia (7). Moreover, tail. All subjects and their parents gave informed assent/consent
at follow-up about 50% of these subjects met criteria for before participation. This protocol was approved by the NIMH In-
an affective disorder (7). ternal Review Board.
Eye-tracking dysfunction characterized by a disturbance Male and female patients age 6 to 18 diagnosed with schizo-
phrenia whose onset of psychotic symptoms occurred by age 12
in the smooth pursuit system has been found in 21%–85% were recruited through national professional and patient advo-
of schizophrenic patients and in 8% of the normal popula- cacy organizations for participation in an inpatient trial of an
tion (10, 11). Similar eye-tracking dysfunction has also atypical neuroleptic. A premorbid IQ of less than 70 and the pres-
been observed in 19%–34% of the biological first-degree ence of active medical or neurological disorders were exclusion-
relatives of schizophrenic patients (11–14) and in children ary. A primary diagnosis of pervasive developmental disorder,
dissociative disorder, mood disorder, conduct disorder, or per-
of schizophrenic patients (15). Studies of psychotic pa- sonality disorder was also exclusionary.
tients have yielded inconsistent results about whether From 950 referrals, over 600 charts were reviewed, and 200 pa-
smooth pursuit eye-tracking dysfunction is specific to tients and their families were seen in person. Admission to the
schizophrenia (11, 12, 14) or if it is associated with other study followed a detailed screening examination, which included
nonaffective psychoses (11, 16–18). These discrepant find- a review of old charts and clinical and structural interviews with
parents and child.
ings may reflect problems with sample size or varying diag-
nostic criteria or methodology across studies (11). Childhood-Onset Schizophrenia Subjects
In a preliminary report, the oculomotor performance of Seventy children and adolescents were diagnosed at the
our first 17 patients with childhood-onset schizophrenia screening examination with childhood-onset schizophrenia, but
six patients did not participate because of clinical reasons (e.g.,
(onset of psychotic symptoms before age 12) was charac-
loss of residential placement, parental reluctance). Of the 64 re-
terized by significantly lower gain (ratio of eye velocity to maining subjects, 34 underwent the eye-tracking task, but three
target velocity), higher root mean square error (extent to patients were subsequently excluded because of poor tracings re-
which eyes do not reproduce target motion), lower per- sulting from either machine failure or excessive head motion. The
centage of total task time engaged in tracking the target, records for two additional patients were excluded from this anal-
ysis because of inattention or lack of motivation during the task
and a greater frequency of anticipatory saccades than
determined by visual inspection of the tracing. Thus, data for 29
healthy comparison subjects (19). In contrast, there were subjects with childhood-onset schizophrenia are included in this
no significant differences in gain scores or any saccade report. To optimize performance, subjects were tested during the
frequency between subjects with attention deficit hyper- last week of an atypical neuroleptic trial. Of these 29 patients, 15
activity disorder (ADHD) and healthy comparison sub- were receiving medications known to affect eye-tracking perfor-
mance such as clozapine or a benzodiazepine (10, 21, 22). The re-
jects, although the ADHD subjects had higher root mean
maining patients were being treated with olanzapine or a typical
square error. This study was limited, however, by the use of neuroleptic.
two different recording systems and the lack of a qualita-
tive rating system to characterize smooth pursuit eye Psychotic Disorder Not Otherwise Specified Subjects
tracking. More recently, similar abnormalities were found Thirty-one children who were referred with a diagnosis of
childhood-onset schizophrenia for an inpatient trial of an atypi-
in a separate group of 10 childhood-onset schizophrenia
cal neuroleptic were felt to fall under DSM-III-R or DSM-IV crite-
subjects for whom higher rates of familial eye-tracking ab- ria for psychotic disorder not otherwise specified and were la-
normalities were also seen (20). beled by our group as being “multidimensionally impaired” (3).
In this report we focus on one specific neurobiological Based on 71 consecutive referrals to the NIMH childhood-onset
trait—eye-tracking dysfunction—as part of a program ex- schizophrenia study, this multidimensionally impaired subgroup
was reliably diagnosed (kappa=0.81) according to empirically
amining the neurobiological characteristics associated derived criteria (3) and was distinguishable from children with
with childhood-onset schizophrenia and psychotic disor- overlapping clinical presentations (e.g., ADHD, bipolar disorder,
der not otherwise specified to help differentiate them nonautistic forms of pervasive developmental disorder). This
from each other. The present study compared the oculo- psychotic disorder not otherwise specified subgroup reported
motor performance of an expanded cohort of 29 subjects brief hallucinations and delusions that occurred a few times a
month, typically under stress, and showed no evidence of thought
with childhood-onset schizophrenia to that of 20 healthy disorder (3). Although their psychotic symptoms were ego-dys-
comparison subjects similar in age distribution and gen- tonic and impaired their functioning, these intermittent prob-
der proportion. In addition, the smooth pursuit eye track- lems were less the focus of concern than were their dramatic
ing of a subgroup of 26 subjects with psychotic disorder mood outbursts and periodic aggression, which had necessitated
not otherwise specified was compared to a separate group frequent psychiatric hospitalizations. These children lacked the
striking and pervasive disturbances in cognition, defective emo-
of 18 healthy subjects also matched for age and gender to tional rapport, bizarre hallucinations, and delusions that charac-
see if the between-group differences resembled those terized the subjects with childhood-onset schizophrenia. They
seen with childhood-onset schizophrenia. were socially deficient but eagerly sought social interactions and

1292 Am J Psychiatry 158:8, August 2001


KUMRA, SPORN, HOMMER, ET AL.

were distressed by their frequent peer rejection. Although the dis- while the procedure was described again. During this description,
torted reality and affective lability of this group suggested border- subjects were instructed to keep their eyes on the target and fol-
line or schizotypal personality disorder, none of the children low it as closely as possible.
given a diagnosis of psychotic disorder not otherwise specified Eye tracking was recorded during a smooth pursuit task in
met criteria for these disorders (3, 6). which subjects followed a target moving horizontally across the
Five children with psychotic disorder not otherwise specified screen at a constant velocity of 17°/second. The target stopped
did not participate in this study because of clinical considerations 15° to the left and right of center and remained stationary for ap-
(previous head trauma [N=1], age below 8 at time of testing [N=1]) proximately 1 second at the end of each ramp. The constant hori-
or poor tracings resulting from machine failure or excessive head zontal velocity combined with fixation points at the end of each
motion (N=3). ramp created a trapezoidal waveform of target and eye motions.
Smooth pursuit eye-tracking data were obtained for 26 sub- The fixation points between each ramp were used for further cal-
jects with psychotic disorder not otherwise specified (22 male, ibration before data analysis.
four female). The testing for the patients was completed during Each trial consisted of 10 full ramps with a partial ramp before
the first 2 weeks of their inpatient admission while they were on a the first full ramp and following the last full ramp. The two half-
stable medication regimen. Of the psychotic disorder not other- ramps were excluded from the analysis, leaving 10 ramps for
wise specified patients, two were receiving medications (lithium quantitative and qualitative analysis.
or carbamazepine) known to affect eye-tracking performance
(10) as part of their medication regimen. Eye Movement Analysis
Eye movement data were analyzed with pattern-recognition
Healthy Volunteers computer software described elsewhere (28, 29). The raw data
Healthy comparison subjects for both patient groups were se- consisted of eye- and target-position values for each millisecond
lected from an ongoing NIMH study of brain development in chil- of recorded tracking, which could be displayed on a computer
dren and adolescents (23). Any physical or neurological disorder, a screen as a plot of eye position versus time. Since the initiation of
lifetime history of any psychiatric disorder, or a history of a major smooth pursuit is quite different from its maintenance, we ex-
psychiatric disorder in first-degree relatives was exclusionary. For cluded the 250 msec preceding or following changes in target di-
this carefully screened group approximately one in six contacts rection from the analysis. Sequential manipulations were per-
were accepted for the study. From a pool of 38 healthy volunteers formed on the remaining data to isolate saccades from true
who agreed to participate in this study, 18 subjects were selected pursuit and artifacts. First, for each millisecond the velocity of the
as comparison subjects for the psychotic disorder not otherwise tracking was calculated by subtracting the position value at 5
specified group, and 20 subjects were selected as comparison sub- msec before the given point from the position value at 5 msec af-
jects for the childhood-onset schizophrenia group on the basis of ter the given point and dividing the result by 10 msec. The next
age and gender. All volunteers were seen as outpatients. step was identifying the saccades. This was done by using an au-
tomated procedure that labeled as saccades local peaks in eye ve-
Behavioral, Cognitive, and MRI Assessments locity that were at least twice the variance of the local velocity.
From the literature, the following clinical and neurobiological This approach allows reliable identification of saccades as small
measures were available for correlative analyses with eye-track- as 0.4° even during smooth pursuit. Presaccadic and postsaccadic
ing variables: age at study (24), full-scale IQ (25), and clinical rat- position errors were determined 5 msec before the beginning and
ings of symptom severity from the Scale for the Assessment of 5 msec after the end of the saccade, respectively. High-velocity in-
Negative Symptoms (26, 27). Exploratory analyses of neuropsy- tervals with a peak velocity greater than 850°/second or those that
chological data (tests that measured six cognitive domains: were immediately followed by another high-velocity interval in
speeded visual-motor processing and attention, abstraction-flex- the opposite direction were discarded as artifacts. Both phases of
ibility, verbal intelligence and language, spatial organization, ver- any biphasic pattern were discarded, since preliminary studies
bal learning, and visual learning) (8) and eye-tracking measures (in which subjects purposely blinked) indicated that this pattern
were also planned for this study. represents eye blinks. Following this computer analysis, each file
was checked by the raters for missed blinks or artifacts or inap-
Eye-Tracking Procedure propriately labeled artifacts. Those areas determined to be head
Eye movements were recorded in a quiet, darkened room with movement or moments of inattention were manually labeled as
an infrared limbus detection eye-tracking device (Eye Trac Model artifacts before the final computer calculations were completed.
210, Applied Science Laboratories, Waltham, Mass.). Subjects All intervals of eye tracking not meeting the above criteria for be-
wore goggles with infrared sources and sensors mounted on the ing high-velocity interval or artifact were considered to be
inside. The sensors require no mechanical calibration, making smooth pursuit eye movement. Since we were interested in mea-
the device convenient for use with subjects. This device has a re- suring smooth pursuit eye movement only when actively pursued
sponse time constant of 4 msec, an accuracy of better than 0.25 of targets were properly visualized by the fovea, we excluded from
visual angle, and a range of 15° to the left and right of center. Elec- analysis intervals in which smooth pursuit eye movement veloc-
tronic calibration was accomplished by having the subject fixate ity was less than 5°/second or smooth pursuit eye movement po-
on a series of sequentially presented white square targets (each sition error (eye position subtracted from target position) was
subtending a visual angle of less than 2.5°) displayed on a black greater than 2.5°, which is the radius of the fovea in humans.
background for 2 seconds at 7.5° intervals horizontally across the Quantitative eye movement variables and saccade classifi-
video monitor. This calibration procedure was run at the begin- cation system. Quantitative variables were defined as follows.
ning of each eye movement task. The subjects were seated 57 cm Smooth pursuit eye movement gain was defined as the ratio of
away from a computer monitor on which a small bright white tar- eye velocity to target velocity of each separate smooth pursuit eye
get was displayed against a black background. The subjects’ movement interval, averaged across intervals. Saccades were ob-
heads were secured by a biteplate. Analog output of the infrared tained by tabulating the number of all intervals considered to be
tracking device was sampled at 1000 Hz by using an 8-bit analog- saccades according to the criteria stated above (i.e., high-velocity
to-digital converter and was stored for subsequent analysis. intervals with simple uniphasic pattern). In addition, saccades
Before each trial, the subjects were given a verbal description were subclassified as catch-up and anticipatory saccades by their
of the task followed by a demonstration on the computer monitor direction in relation to target motion and presaccadic and post-

Am J Psychiatry 158:8, August 2001 1293


EYE-TRACKING IMPAIRMENT IN CHILDHOOD PSYCHOSIS

TABLE 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Patients With Childhood-Onset Schizophrenia, Psychotic Disorder
Not Otherwise Specified, and Respective Healthy Comparison Groups
Childhood-Onset Schizophrenia Psychotic Disorder Not Otherwise Specified
Patient Comparison Patient Comparison
Group Group Group Group
Characteristic (N=29) (N=20) Analysis (N=26) (N=18) Analysis
N % N % χ2 df p N % N % χ2 df p

Gender 0.1 1 0.90 1.0 1 0.45


Male 16 55.2 11 55.0 22 84.6 13 72.2
Female 13 44.8 9 45.0 4 15.4 5 27.8
Ethnicity 1.7 3 0.60 2.4 3 0.51
Caucasian 16 55.2 13 65.0 21 80.8 14 77.7
African American 5 17.2 4 20.0 1 3.8 2 11.1
Hispanic 3 10.3 2 10.0 2 7.7 0 0.0
Other 5 17.2 1 5.0 2 7.7 2 11.1

Mean SD Mean SD t df p Mean SD Mean SD t df p

Age (years) 14.6 2.5 14.6 2.3 0.1 47 0.90 13.7 3.5 13.1 3.4 0.6 42 0.50
Vocabulary score (WISC-R)a 7.3 3.7 11.2 2.1 3.2 35 0.003 7.8 3.1 15.8 2.5 4.9 28 <0.001
Block design score (WISC-R)b 7.3 3.8 12.1 2.9 3.8 38 0.001 8.4 9.2 16.0 2.4 3.5 28 0.002
Reading decoding scorec 90.6 15.8 — — 83.5 24.3 — —
Age at onset of psychosis (years) 10.3 1.8 7.6 1.9
Length of neuroleptic treatment (months) 25.0 16.6 25.8 23.0
Length of psychiatric hospitalization (months) 5.5 10.9 5.4 11.8
BPRS score 40.6 10.7 — — 31.1 6.9 — —
a Schizophrenic patients, N=27; schizophrenic comparison group, N=10; comparison group for the subjects with psychotic disorder not oth-
erwise specified, N=4.
b N=11 and N=4 for the comparison groups of the subjects with schizophrenia and psychotic disorder not otherwise specified, respectively.
c From the Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement (33); for the schizophrenic patients, N=27.

saccadic position errors. Saccades in the direction of the target comparison subjects, t tests or chi-square analyses were used de-
motion that started and ended behind the target, and saccades pending on data distribution and type. Distributions for all eye-
that ended ahead of the target but with at least two-thirds of their tracking variables were examined within each group for signifi-
saccadic amplitude-moving gaze toward the target, were classi- cant skew, kurtosis, and heterogeneity of variance. Because the
fied as catch-up saccades. Saccades in the direction of target mo- data for three of six eye-tracking variables from both the psy-
tion that ended ahead of the target and were followed by a 50 chotic disorder not otherwise specified patients and the healthy
msec interval of eye velocity of less than 5°/second were classified volunteers were not normally distributed, data were analyzed by
as anticipatory saccades. Anticipatory saccades that were greater using the Mann-Whitney U test.
than 4° were subclassified as large anticipatory saccades. Saccade Spearman rank correlation coefficients, with partial correction
rates were determined by dividing the number of saccades by the for age, tested relationships between eye-tracking measures that
pursuit duration. differed significantly from those of healthy volunteers and clinical
Root mean square error, which provides a global measure of measures that might be related to eye-tracking performance.
the extent to which the eyes do not reproduce target motion (30),
was calculated by squaring the difference between eye position
and target position for all intervals of nonartifactual pursuit
Results
tracking, and then obtaining the average of the square root of As seen in Table 1, the patients with childhood-onset
these numbers. Split-half reliability (intraclass correlation coeffi-
schizophrenia and psychotic disorder not otherwise spec-
cient [ICC]) for quantitative variables was high (ICC=0.75–0.96).
ified performed more poorly on the WISC-R vocabulary
Qualitative eye movement analysis. Each ramp was scored
and block design subtests than did the healthy volunteers.
qualitatively by one of the authors (R.N.) blind to patient identity
on a scale of 1 (best) to 5 (worst) (31). Two of the authors (R.N., Because there were no significant group differences in age
A.S.) rated a subset of this sample (N=10) with high reliability or gender distribution between the two patient groups
(ICC=0.98). The average of 10 ramps was determined for each and their respective healthy volunteers, comparative anal-
subject and used as the primary qualitative measure of eye-track- yses are unadjusted.
ing dysfunction. Additionally, subjects with a mean score of 2.5 or
greater were classified as having abnormal eye tracking. Qualita- Eye-Tracking Performance
tive ratings for all 93 subjects (26 with psychotic disorder not oth-
erwise specified, 29 with childhood-onset schizophrenia, 38 Significantly poorer qualitative ratings were seen among
healthy volunteers) were highly correlated with gain (r=0.83, df= the childhood-onset schizophrenia patients than in the
91, p<0.001) and with root mean square error (r=0.77, df=91, p= healthy volunteers (Table 2). Significantly more subjects
0.0001). with childhood-onset schizophrenia (79.3%, N=23 of 29)
Data Analyses than the healthy volunteers (20.0%, N=4 of 20) were found
Analyses were performed with SPSS statistical analysis soft-
to have eye-tracking dysfunction (χ2=14.3, df=1, p<0.01).
ware (32). To examine demographic differences between psy- The qualitative eye-tracking scores were also significantly
chotic disorder not otherwise specified patients and healthy worse in the patients with psychotic disorder not otherwise

1294 Am J Psychiatry 158:8, August 2001


KUMRA, SPORN, HOMMER, ET AL.

specified than in the healthy volunteers. Significantly more schizophrenia subjects relative to healthy comparison
subjects with psychotic disorder not otherwise specified subjects (20). Together, these data support a hypothesis
(65.4%, N=17 of 26) than healthy volunteers (11.1%, N=2 of that childhood-onset schizophrenia is related to later-on-
18) had eye-tracking dysfunction (χ2=12.77, df=1, p<0.001). set schizophrenia and thus may be a manifestation of the
For the 17°/second visual pursuit task, the subjects same disease process.
with childhood-onset schizophrenia and those with psy- The main purpose of this study was to investigate
chotic disorder not otherwise specified had significantly whether the pattern of smooth pursuit eye tracking in sub-
higher root mean square error than their respective jects with psychotic disorder not otherwise specified re-
healthy comparison groups. Significant differences in sembled that in a group with narrowly defined childhood-
gain were also noted, with gain being lower in both pa- onset schizophrenia. Patients with psychotic disorder not
tient groups (Table 2). otherwise specified were found to have poor global track-
After removing the 15 patients who were being treated ing performance as marked by significantly worse qualita-
with clozapine or a benzodiazepine, root mean square er- tive ratings, higher root mean square error, and lower gain
ror remained significantly higher in the childhood-onset but no difference in frequency of catch-up saccades rela-
schizophrenia patients relative to the healthy comparison tive to healthy comparison subjects. These data along with
volunteers (U=37.0, df=32, p<0.001). Between-group sig- a similar pattern of abnormalities observed in subjects
nificant differences for the other eye-tracking characteris- with either psychotic disorder not otherwise specified or
tics also held: gain (U=72.5, df=32, p<0.02), qualitative childhood-onset schizophrenia (prepsychotic linguistic
score (U=14.0, df=32, p<0.001), saccade rate (U=72.0, df= deficits, family history, neuropsychological test perfor-
32, p<0.02), and rate of catch-up saccades (U=82.0, df=32, mance, and quantitative brain MRI data) support a hy-
p<0.05). For the subjects with psychotic disorder not other- pothesis that the two disorders may share some risk factors
wise specified, after removing the two patients who were (6).
receiving lithium or carbamazepine, between-group sig- Because much remains to be known about the patho-
nificant differences in root mean square error, qualitative physiology of childhood-onset schizophrenia, it is difficult
score, and gain remained (U=96.0, df=40, p=0.002; U=63.0, to fully define the relationship between psychotic disorder
df=40, p<0.001; and U=111.2, df=40, p=0.008, respectively). not otherwise specified and childhood-onset schizophre-
nia. Our initial clinical distinction, however, does appear
Relationship Between Eye Tracking, Age, to have prognostic importance. The 2- to 8-year clinical
and Clinical Measures follow-up data indicate that psychotic disorder not other-
For the entire group of healthy comparison subjects (N= wise specified does not progress onto schizophrenia but
38), age was not significantly correlated with root mean may instead develop into affective disorders (7). Further-
square error (rs =–0.24, p=0.15), gain (r s=0.26, p=0.11), more, this psychotic disorder not otherwise specified sub-
qualitative score (rs =–0.15, p=0.37), or rate of all saccades group also differed from the childhood-onset schizophre-
(rs=–0.12, p=0.49). There was a significant correlation be- nia group with respect to follow-up progression of brain
tween age and the rate of catch-up saccades (rs=–0.33, p= MRI abnormalities. In childhood-onset schizophrenia,
0.04). For both patient groups, after correction for multi- there is progressive loss of regional cortical gray during ad-
ple comparisons, there were no significant correlations olescence (43, 44), whereas no significant loss of frontal or
between any eye-tracking measure and age, IQ, neuropsy- other regional gray volume was seen in the subjects with
chological test performance, or severity of positive or neg- psychotic disorder not otherwise specified (45). These
ative symptoms. data would argue for a separation of subjects with psy-
chotic disorder not otherwise specified from future ge-
netic analyses of childhood-onset schizophrenia.
Discussion
It is theoretically possible that the pattern of neurobio-
To our knowledge, this is the first comparative study of logical abnormalities that we observed in this psychotic
smooth pursuit eye tracking in pediatric patients with disorder not otherwise specified subgroup might be seen
contrasting childhood-onset psychotic disorders. As ex- in a broad range of children and adolescents with severe
pected, the childhood-onset schizophrenia patients ex- emotional disturbances. Other patient groups, such as
hibited a pattern of eye-tracking dysfunction character- adult bipolar patients (17, 18), have been reported to have
ized by poorer qualitative scores, lower gain, higher root smooth pursuit tracking dysfunction. However, the bio-
mean square error, and a greater frequency of catch-up logical relatives of bipolar patients have not been shown to
saccades than age-matched healthy volunteers. A similar have eye-tracking dysfunction (11), suggesting that glo-
pattern of eye-tracking dysfunction has been consistently bally assessed eye-tracking dysfunction in affective disor-
reported in studies of adult schizophrenic patients unse- ders may be state-, rather than trait-, related (46). Our pre-
lected for age at onset (11, 29, 34–42). Recently, similar liminary data suggest that some measures of eye-tracking
findings have also been reported (although with small an- dysfunction are also seen in the biological first-degree
ticipatory saccades) for a cohort of 10 childhood-onset relatives of childhood-onset schizophrenia patients (47).

Am J Psychiatry 158:8, August 2001 1295


EYE-TRACKING IMPAIRMENT IN CHILDHOOD PSYCHOSIS

TABLE 2. Characteristics of Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements of Patients With Childhood-Onset Schizophrenia, Psychotic
Disorder Not Otherwise Specified, and Respective Healthy Comparison Groups
Subjects With Childhood-Onset Schizophrenia Subjects
Patient Group (N=29) Comparison Group (N=20) Analysis Patient Group (N=26)
Mann Whitney
Measure Mean SD Range Mean SD Range U (df=47) p Mean SD Range
Qualitative scorea 3.3 0.9 1.5–5.0 1.7 0.7 1.1–3.2 55.0 <0.001 3.0 0.9 1.4–4.4
Gainb 0.69 0.23 0.26–1.06 0.87 0.13 0.63–1.02 148.0 0.004 0.73 0.16 0.44–0.96
Root mean square errorc 4.13 2.04 1.30–9.37 2.12 0.17 0.85–4.59 97.0 <0.001 4.11 2.46 1.14–10.65
Frequency (saccades/second)
All saccades 3.60 1.18 1.81–6.38 2.86 0.84 1.60–4.50 183.0 0.03 3.37 1.34 1.55–7.61
Catch-up saccadesd 1.59 0.75 0.40–3.69 1.10 0.42 0.48–2.06 165.0 0.01 1.35 0.64 0.36–2.99
Anticipatory saccadese 1.28 0.56 0.29–2.21 1.32 0.54 0.42–2.43 287.0 0.95 1.29 0.50 0.45–2.60
Large anticipatory saccadesf 0.49 0.32 0.00–1.32 0.42 0.42 0.00–1.38 224.0 0.18 0.60 0.40 0.00–1.41
a Rating of smooth pursuit eye movement (1=best, 5=worst); subjects with ratings of 2.5 or greater were classified as having eye tracking dys-
function.
b Ratio of eye velocity to target velocity of each separate smooth pursuit eye movement interval, averaged across intervals.
c Global measure of extent to which eyes did not reproduce target motion.
d Saccades in the direction of the target motion that started and ended behind the target or ended ahead of the target but with at least two-
thirds of the amplitude-moving gaze heading toward the target.
e Saccades in the direction of the target motion that ended ahead of the target and were followed by a 50-msec interval of eye velocity less
than 5°/second.
f Anticipatory saccades greater than 4°.

Because eye tracking in younger children is harder to tients will be necessary to define more homogeneous sub-
record and therefore has greater variability (24, 48), the groups of patients.
psychotic disorder not otherwise specified group is being The data from our study should be considered in light of
followed longitudinally to establish the stability of the other methodologic issues. The children with childhood-
study findings. Also, family studies of the psychotic disor- onset schizophrenia and psychotic disorder not otherwise
der not otherwise specified group are currently underway specified both differed from their healthy comparison
to address the question as to whether eye-tracking dys- groups in terms of IQ and attentional disturbances. The
function in these patients may represent a state- or trait- discrepancy in IQ between patients and normal subjects
related phenomenon. may not be a confounding factor, since in normal children
Various lines of evidence suggest that the eye-tracking no consistent relationship between IQ and eye movement
dysfunction of schizophrenia patients may arise from an variables has been found (24). Also, the lower IQ scores of
underlying dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex. The iden- the patients may not be reflective of their true abilities
tification of eye-tracking dysfunction in subjects with based on previous IQ testing (8). Thus, we did not covary
childhood-onset schizophrenia and their families may be IQ (or its surrogate, vocabulary) in primary analyses.
of use for future genetic studies as a potential alternative A major shortcoming is that a monitor condition was not
phenotype for childhood-onset schizophrenia (46). Such included in this protocol, which may have enhanced atten-
an alternative phenotype could increase the statistical tion and provided a more conservative estimate of the sub-
power of genetic analyses in schizophrenic pedigrees by jects’ optimal performance (15). In a study of children and
increasing the number of affected subjects. It is also possi- adolescents, attention facilitation effectively normalized
ble that eye-tracking dysfunction may represent a biologi- the high anticipatory saccade rate observed in the off-
cal deficit in schizophrenia that could be more closely re- spring of schizophrenic probands compared to commu-
lated to a single gene effect than schizophrenia itself (46). nity control subjects (15). However the primary results of
Between 7 to 15 years of age, the performance of the the study (i.e., global eye-tracking measure) remained un-
smooth pursuit system continues to develop in healthy changed such that children and adolescents who were off-
children (24, 48, 49). As seen in previous studies, the corre- spring of depressed parents could not be distinguished
lation between age and smooth pursuit system variables from offspring of schizophrenic parents (15). In the present
accounts for at most 20% of the variance (24), which may study, it is unlikely that the eye-tracking dysfunctions in
reflect differential rates of brain maturation in healthy patients could be solely attributed to attentional distur-
children. In this study, age effects were dealt with by creat- bances. First, there was no significant correlation between
ing two healthy comparison groups matched in gender gain or root mean square error with performance on tests
and age to each respective patient group. of speeded visual-motor processing and attention (e.g.,
Because children described as having “multiplex devel- Trailmaking A and B and average of the coding and digit
opmental disorder” (5, 50) or borderline disorder (4) bear symbol subtests of the WISC-R/WAIS-R). Second, all pa-
some clinical resemblance to our psychotic disorder not tients were carefully supervised during the task to ensure
otherwise specified group, cross-center replications of that they were engaged in smooth pursuit and were in-
these findings and the inclusion of medication-naive pa- structed not to jump ahead of the target.

1296 Am J Psychiatry 158:8, August 2001


KUMRA, SPORN, HOMMER, ET AL.

References
1. McKenna K, Gordon CT, Rapoport JL: Childhood-onset schizo-
With Psychotic Disorder Not Otherwise Specified phrenia: timely neurobiological research. J Am Acad Child Ad-
Comparison Group (N=18) Analysis olesc Psychiatry 1994; 33:771–781
Mann Whitney 2. Nicolson R, Rapoport JL: Childhood-onset schizophrenia: rare
Mean SD Range U (df=42) p but worth studying. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 46:1418–1428
1.7 0.6 1.1–3.0 67.0 <0.001 3. McKenna K, Gordon CT, Lenane M, Kaysen D, Fahey K, Rapo-
0.85 1.35 0.60–1.02 128.0 0.01 port JL: Looking for childhood-onset schizophrenia: the first 71
2.18 1.06 0.79–5.11 111.0 0.003 cases screened. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1994; 33:
636–644
3.74 1.16 1.87–6.32 174.0 0.15 4. Petti TA, Vela RM: Borderline disorders of childhood: an over-
1.42 0.63 0.38–2.80 225.0 0.83 view. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1990; 29:327–337
1.53 0.64 0.57–2.80 189.0 0.28
5. Towbin KE, Dykens EM, Pearson GS, Cohen DJ: Conceptualizing
0.52 0.50 0.00–1.63 197.0 0.38
“borderline syndrome of childhood” and “childhood schizo-
phrenia” as a developmental disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
Psychiatry 1993; 32:775–782
6. Kumra S, Jacobsen LK, Lenane M, Zahn TP, Wiggs E, Alagha-
band-Rad J, Castellanos FX, Frazier JA, McKenna K, Gordon CT,
Smith A, Hamburger SD, Rapoport JL: “Multidimensionally im-
paired disorder”: is it a variant of very early-onset schizophre-
nia? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1998; 37:91–99
7. Nicolson R, Lenane M, Brookner F, Gochman P, Kumra S,
Spechler L, Giedd J, Thaler G, Wudarsky M, Rapoport J: Children
We chose to test patients while they were on stable
and adolescents with psychotic disorder not otherwise speci-
medication regimens to enhance compliance and opti- fied: a two to eight year follow-up. Compr Psychiatry (in press)
mize performance (51). The weight of evidence suggests 8. Kumra S, Wiggs E, Bedwell J, Smith AK, Arling E, Albus K, Ham-
that typical neuroleptics and antidepressants do not burger SD, McKenna K, Jacobsen LK, Rapoport JL, Asarnow RF:
cause eye-tracking dysfunction in patients but that cloza- Neuropsychological deficits in pediatric patients with child-
hood-onset schizophrenia and psychotic disorder not other-
pine, sedatives, anticonvulsants, and other psychotropic
wise specified. Schizophr Res 2000; 42:135–144
medications (e.g., lithium carbonate) can influence eye- 9. Kumra S, Giedd JN, Vaituzis AC, Jacobsen LK, McKenna K, Bed-
tracking performance (10, 11, 52). Clozapine has been well J, Hamburger S, Nelson JE, Lenane M, Rapoport JL: Child-
found to reduce gain scores and to significantly increase hood-onset psychotic disorders: magnetic resonance imaging
the frequency of catch-up saccades (22). However, when of volumetric differences in brain structure. Am J Psychiatry
2000; 157:1467–1474
we removed the subjects with childhood-onset schizo-
10. Levy DL, Holzman PS, Matthysse S, Mendell NR: Eye tracking
phrenia receiving clozapine the findings of this study dysfunction and schizophrenia: a critical perspective.
were unchanged. Schizophr Bull 1993; 19:461–536
In summary, given our evidence that subjects with psy- 11. Levy DL, Holzman PS, Matthysse S, Mendell N: Eye tracking and
chotic disorder not otherwise specified do not progress schizophrenia: a selective review. Schizophr Bull 1994; 20:47–62
12. Holzman PS, Proctor LR, Levy DL, Yasillo NJ, Meltzer HY, Hurt
onto schizophrenia but instead may develop affective dis-
SW: Eye-tracking dysfunctions in schizophrenic patients and
orders, these findings are comparable with at least some their relatives. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1974; 31:143–151
studies of eye tracking in bipolar disorder, although here 13. Grove WM, Lebow BS, Clementz BA, Cerri A, Medus C, Iacono
results are inconsistent (17, 18). The relative nonspecific- WG: Familial prevalence and coaggregation of schizotypy indi-
ity of these eye movement findings in children and adoles- cators: a multitrait family study. J Abnorm Psychol 1991; 100:
cents and other differences in clinical and biological mea- 115–121
14. Iacono WG, Moreau M, Beiser M, Fleming F, Lin T: Smooth-pur-
sures do support separation of subjects with psychotic
suit eye tracking in first-episode psychotic patients and their
disorder not otherwise specified from future genetic stud- relatives. J Abnorm Psychol 1992; 101:104–116
ies of early-onset schizophrenia. 15. Rosenberg DR, Sweeney JA, Squires-Wheller E, Keshavan M,
Cornblatt B, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L: Eye-tracking dysfunction in
Received April 21, 2000; revision received Jan. 17, 2001; accepted offspring from the New York High-Risk Project: diagnostic speci-
Jan. 30, 2001. From the NIMH Child Psychiatry Branch; the Labora- ficity and the role of attention. Psychiatry Res 1997; 66:121–130
tory of Clinical Studies, National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alco- 16. van den Bosch RJ, Rozendaal N, Mol JM: Symptom correlates of
holism, Bethesda, Md.; and Yale University School of Medicine, New eye tracking dysfunction. Biol Psychiatry 1987; 22:919–921
Haven, Conn. Address reprint requests to Dr. Rapoport, NIMH Child 17. Friedman L, Abel LA, Jesberger JA, Malki A, Meltzer HY: Sac-
Psychiatry Branch, Building 10, Rm. 3N202, 10 Center Dr., MSC 1600,
cadic intrusions into smooth pursuit in-patients with schizo-
Bethesda, MD 20892-1600; rapoport@helix.nih.gov (e-mail).
phrenia or affective disorder and normal controls. Biol Psychi-
The authors would like to thank for their assistance the NIMH Clin-
atry 1992; 31:1110–1118
ical Center staff, including Claudia Briguglio, R.N., M.S.N.; the nurses
and staff of the Child Psychiatry Inpatient Unit at NIMH; and the cli- 18. Tien A, Ross D, Pearson G, Strauss M: Eye movements and psy-
nicians who referred patients to this study. Drs. C.T. Gordon and Kath- chopathology in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. J Nerv
leen McKenna provided clinical care for some of these patients and Ment Dis 1996; 184:331–338
encouraged the further study of psychotic disorder not otherwise 19. Jacobsen LK, Hong WL, Hommer DW, Hamburger SD, Castell-
specified as a diagnostic subtype. anos FX, Frazier JA, Giedd JN, Gordon CT, Karp BI, McKenna K,
Rapoport JL: Smooth pursuit eye movements in childhood on-

Am J Psychiatry 158:8, August 2001 1297


EYE-TRACKING IMPAIRMENT IN CHILDHOOD PSYCHOSIS

set schizophrenia: comparison with ADHD and normal con- 37. Litman RE, Torrey EF, Hommer DW, Radant AR, Pickar D, Wein-
trols. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 40:1144–1154 berger DR: A quantitative analysis of smooth pursuit eye track-
20. Ross RG, Olincy A, Harris JG, Radant A, Hawkins M, Adler LE, ing in monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia. Arch
Freedman R: Evidence for bilineal inheritance of physiological Gen Psychiatry 1997; 54:417–426
indicators of risk in childhood-onset schizophrenia. Am J Med 38. Radant A, Claypoole K, Wingerson D, Cowley D, Roy-Byrne P:
Genet 1999; 88:188–199 Relationships between neuropsychological and oculomotor
21. Friedman L, Jesberger JA, Meltzer HY: Effect of typical antipsy- measures in schizophrenia patients and normal controls. Biol
chotic medications and clozapine on smooth pursuit perfor- Psychiatry 1997; 42:797–805
mance in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 1992; 41: 39. Friedman L, Jesberger J, Siecer L, Thompson P, Mohs P, Meltzer
25–36 HY: Smooth pursuit performance in-patients with affective dis-
22. Litman RE, Hommer DW, Radant A, Clem T, Pickar D: Quantitative orders or schizophrenia and normal controls. Psychol Med
effects of typical and atypical neuroleptics on smooth pursuit eye 1995; 25:387–403
tracking in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1994; 12:107–120 40. Sweeney JA, Clementz BA, Escobar MD, Li S, Pauler DK, Haas G:
23. Giedd JN, Snell JW, Lange N, Rajapaske JC, Casey BJ, Kozuch PL, Mixture analysis of pursuit eye-tracking dysfunction in schizo-
Vaituzis AC, Vauss YC, Hamburger SD, Kaysen D, Rapoport JL: phrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1993; 34:331–340
Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of human brain de- 41. Clementz BA, McDowell JE, Zistook S: Saccadic system function-
velopment: ages 4–18. Cereb Cortex 1996; 6:551–560 ing among schizophrenia patients and their first-degree bio-
24. Ross RG, Radant AD, Hommer DW: A developmental study of logical relatives. J Abnorm Psychol 1994; 103:277–287
smooth pursuit eye movements in normal children from 7 to 42. Clementz BA, Iacono WG, Grove WM: The construct validity of
15 years of age. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1993; 32: root-mean-square error for quantifying smooth-pursuit eye
783–791 tracking abnormalities in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1996;
25. Katsanis J, Iacono WG: Clinical, neuropsychological, and brain 39:448–450
structural correlates of smooth-pursuit eye tracking perfor- 43. Giedd JN, Jeffries NO, Blumenthal J, Castellanos FX, Vaituzis AC,
mance in chronic schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 1991; 100: Fernandez T, Hamburger SD, Liu H, Nelson J, Bedwell J, Tran L,
526–534 Lenane M, Nicolson R, Rapoport JL: Childhood-onset schizo-
26. Ross DE, Thaker G, Buchanan RW, Kirkpatrick B, Lahti A, Medoff phrenia: progressive brain changes during adolescence. Biol
D, Bartko JJ, Goodman J, Tien A: Eye tracking disorder in schizo- Psychiatry 1999; 46:892–898
phrenia is characterized by specific ocular motor defects and is 44. Rapoport J, Giedd J, Blumenthal J, Jeffries W, Nicolson R, Bed-
associated with the deficit syndrome. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 42: well J, Lenance M, Fernandez J, Zijedenbox A, Paus T, Evans A:
781–796 Progressive cortical changes during adolescence in childhood
27. Siever LJ, Friedman L, Moskowitz J, Mitropoulou V, Keefe R, onset schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:649–654
Roitman SL, Merhige D, Trestman R, Silverman J, Mohs R: Eye 45. Rapoport J, Castellanos XF, Giedd J: Normal and abnormal
movement impairment and schizotypal psychopathology. Am brain development in childhood and adolescence, in Abstracts
J Psychiatry 1994; 151:1209–1215 of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. Nash-
28. Nickoloff SE, Radant AD, Reichler R, Hommer DW: Smooth pur- ville, Tenn, ACNP, 2000, p 33
suit and saccadic eye movements and neurological soft signs in 46. Katsanis J, Iacono WG, Harris M: Visual fixation and smooth
obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res 1991; 38:173–185 pursuit eye movement abnormalities in patients with schizo-
29. Radant A, Hommer D: A quantitative analysis of saccades and phrenia and their relatives. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci
smooth pursuit during visual pursuit tracking. Schizophr Res 1995; 7:197–206
1992; 6:225–235 47. Rapoport JL, Inoff-Germain G: Update of childhood-onset
30. Iacono WG, Lykken DT: Electro-oculographic recording and schizophrenia. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2000; 2:410–415
scoring of smooth pursuit and saccadic eye tracking: a para- 48. Ross RG, Hommer D, Radant A, Roath M, Freedman R: Early ex-
metric study using monozygotic twins. Psychophysiology 1979; pression of smooth-pursuit eye movement abnormalities in
16:94–107 children of schizophrenic parents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psy-
31. Shagass C, Amadeo M, Overton DA: Eye-tracking performance chiatry 1996; 35:941–949
in psychiatric patients. Biol Psychiatry 1974; 9:245–260 49. Katsanis J, Iacono WG, Harris M: Development of oculomotor
32. Norusis MJ: SPSS for Windows: Base System User’s Guide and functioning in preadolescence, adolescence, and adulthood.
Advanced Statistics, Release 6.1. Chicago, SPSS, 1994 Psychophysiology 1998; 35:64–72
33. Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL: Kaufman Test of Educational Achieve- 50. Cohen DJ, Towbin KE, Mayes L, Volkmar FR: Developmental
ment. Circle Pines, Minn, American Guidance Service, 1985 psychopathology of multiplex developmental disorder, in De-
34. Abel L, Friedman J, Jesberger JA, Malki A, Meltzer H: Quantita- velopmental Follow-Up: Concepts, Genres, Domain and Meth-
tive assessment of smooth pursuit gain and catch-up saccades ods. Edited by Friedman SL, Haywood HC. Orlando, Fla, Aca-
in schizophrenia and affective disorders. Biol Psychiatry 1991; demic Press, 1994, pp 155–179
29:1063–1072 51. Ross DE, Thaker G, Buchanan RW, Lahti A, Conley RR, Medoff D:
35. Cegalis JA, Sweeney JA: Eye movement in schizophrenia: a Specific measures account for most of the variance in qualita-
quantitative analysis. Biol Psychiatry 1979; 14:13–26 tive ratings for smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophre-
36. Clementz BA, McDowell JE: Smooth pursuit in schizophrenia nia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:184–186
research: abnormalities of open- and closed-loop responses. 52. Sharpe JA, Morrow MJ: Cerebral hemispheric smooth pursuit
Psychophysiology 1994; 31:79–86 disorders. Acta Neurol Belg 1991; 91:81–96

1298 Am J Psychiatry 158:8, August 2001

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi