Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 11

Occipital and parietal lobes

lower bilateral occipital lobe activation than controls Table 4.1.  Simplified summary of some functions of the parietal
lobe and lesions seen following formation of lesions to either the
(Hermann et al., 2007). dominant or nondominant side.

Parietal lobe Side of lesion


Since the masterful volume by Macdonald Critchley Dominant Nondominant
was published in 1953, the parietal lobe has come to be Superior parietal lobule
recognized as being heavily involved in the higher cog- Function
nitive functions of the brain. The parietal lobe receives Spatial-motor Spatial
incoming somatosensory signals, but, unlike the orientation
occipital lobe, is involved in far more than processing Lesion
a single sensory modality. The parietal lobe is integral Aphasia Spatial agnosia
to the perception of external space, body image, and Agnosia Sensory neglect
attention. The complex and fascinating cognitive dis- Astereoagnosia Astereoagnosia
turbances that can occur with parietal lobe lesions may
Agraphesthesia Agraphesthesia
at first be mistaken for hysteria. Information perceived
Dressing apraxia
and elaborated by the parietal lobe is submitted to the
frontal association areas. One may speculate that if the Inferior parietal lobule
information received by the frontal lobes is inaccurate, Ideomotor/ideational apraxia Aprosodia
delusional perception or ideas could develop. Gerstmann syndrome
Bilateral
Functional anatomy Balint syndrome
The parietal lobe underlies the parietal bone of the Movement agnosia
skull. Its anterior border on the lateral aspect is marked
by the central sulcus and its posterior border by the
parieto-occipital fissure (Figures 2.2, 4.1, 4.2 and 5.1). somatotopic map on the postcentral gyrus and has
On the medial aspect it extends inferiorly to the cingu- access to the retinotopic map of the occipital cortex.
late gyrus, anteriorly to the central sulcus, and poster- In order to navigate through space we use maps and
iorly to the parieto-occipital sulcus. The parietal lobe landmarks. An allocentric map uses an external frame
consists of the primary somatosensory (somesthetic) of reference and as with a road map, we identify direc-
cortex (BA 1, BA 2, and BA 3), the superior parietal tion in terms of north, east, south, and west. An ego-
lobule (SPL; BA 5 and BA 7), and the inferior parietal centric map is one defined by our current location as
lobule (IPL ; BA 39 and BA 40). It makes up about a fifth seen on an automobile global-positioning (GPS) dis-
of the total neocortex. play. We define directions in the case of an egocentric
The anterior parietal lobe, made up of the postcen- map as ahead, behind, left and right. The allocentric
tral gyrus (lateral aspect) and posterior paracentral map belongs to the hippocampus (page 177). The ego-
lobule (medial aspect), is concerned with somatosen- centric map appears to be located in the lateral parietal
sory sensations  –touch, pain, temperature, and limb area, more strongly on the right side.
position (proprioception).The posterior parietal lobe The parietal lobe attends to attractive (salient)
consisting of BA 7, BA 39, and BA 40 (Mesulam, 1998), environmental targets and locates these targets in
integrates somatosensory signals with signals from the terms of map coordinates. Input from the temporal
visual, auditory, and vestibular systems (sensorimotor lobe gives it information about a target’s identity and
integration). from past experience, its anticipated weight, texture,
The parietal lobe is important in interacting with and possible value. The parietal lobe formulates motor
the world around us (Table 4.1). It operates on a plans in cooperation with the frontal lobe and subcor-
moment-to-moment basis evaluating and respond- tical structures to generate eye, head, arm, and hand
ing to environmental stimuli in a bottom-up manner. movements (and presumably leg movements) to inter-
Movements are scripted here and may be executed in cept these targets. The motor plans are submitted to the
cooperation with the motor cortex with the permis- frontal lobes including those areas that act as a reposi-
sion of the prefrontal cortex. The parietal lobe contains tory for socially acceptable behavior. The motor plans
40 and/or has access to one or more maps. It has its own will be executed unless deemed socially inappropriate
Parietal lobe

or inhibited by voluntary movements generated inde- timing of small groups of synergistic muscles during
pendently in the motor areas of the frontal lobe. The adjustments to sensory input (Drew et al., 2008).
motor plan formulated by the parietal lobe may range Association fibers from the primary somatosen-
from eye movements used to read text when sitting sory cortex pass through the white matter of the pari-
quietly, to catching a ball in flight while running. In etal lobe and connect the postcentral gyrus with the
response to visual and auditory cues, the parietal lobe somatosensory association areas behind it. These high-
selects and generates speech appropriate to the current er-order association areas, including the superior and
social situation, thus contributing to personality. inferior parietal lobules, integrate touch and conscious
The parietal lobe responds in an almost automatic proprioception with other sensory modalities.
mode to sensory signals and attends to the most salient Rauch et al. (1995) reported that cerebral blood
target (e.g., a red balloon in a sea of blue balloons). This flow increased in the somatosensory cortex, as well as
is described as bottom-up processing. In contrast, it can in the frontal, cingulate, insular, and temporal cortices,
be governed by the frontal lobe to search out for a par- in subjects with simple phobia when they were pro-
ticular target (e.g., trying to spot your mother-in-law as voked (snake, rodent, spider, bees). Subjects reported
passengers disembark a plane). This is top-down pro- that tactile imagery was the most prominent sensory
cessing, is not as automatic as bottom-up processing, aspect of the phobic experience.
and can require considerable mental effort (Buschman
and Miller, 2007; Womelsdorf et al., 2007). Secondary somatosensory cortex (SII) and
the parietal operculum
Primary somatosensory cortex (SI) The portion of the parietal lobe that makes up the upper
The primary somatosensory cortex (SI) occupies the bank of the lateral fissure is the parietal operculum. The
postcentral gyrus (BA 3, BA 1, and BA 2). Projections parietal operculum immediately inferior to SI contains
to the postcentral gyrus include thalamocortical fib- a secondary somatosensory cortex (SII). SII receives
ers from the ventral posteromedial (VPM) and ventral input from SI as well as from VPL and VPM of the thal-
posterolateral (VPL) nuclei of the thalamus (Table 9.1). amus. It occupies much of the parietal operculum and
These nuclei relay somatosensory signals from both extends deep into the lateral fissure. It may overlap and
sides of the face and from the contralateral body, include part of the insula. SII is somatotopically organ-
respectively. Touch and proprioceptive signals project ized but receives input from both sides of the body.
predominantly to BA 1. Pain signals project to BA 3. Four areas of the parietal operculum have been
A somatotopic map of the contralateral body called a described: OP (OPercular) 1–4. OP 1 and OP 2 lie pos-
sensory homunculus exists along the postcentral gyrus teriorly and occupy the inferior part of BA 40. OP 3 and
laterally and extends onto the paracentral lobule medi- OP 4 correspond roughly with BA 43, which lies at the
ally. The leg and genitalia are represented on the medial base of BA 1, BA 2, and BA 3 (SI), and extend anteriorly
aspect of the cortex, with the remainder of the body and to border on the insular cortex. Areas OP 1, OP 3, and
head on the lateral aspect. Corticothalamic projections OP 4 appear to be a part of SII (Eickhoff et al., 2006a).
from the primary somatosensory cortex project back to OP 2 lies deep inside the lateral fissure just posterior to
the VPM and VPL thalamic nuclei. Sereno and Huang the insula. OP 2 is recognized as the primary vestibular
(2006) showed that the superior part of the postcentral area (Eickhoff et al., 2006b).
gyrus was activated in response to puffs of air directed The anatomy of the parietal lobe varies from indi-
to various parts of the face. This appears to be an area vidual to individual, including that of the parietal
that codes for the location of objects near to, or in con- operculum. One variation describes an accessory post-
tact with, the face that might be used in feeding. central gyrus accompanied by a parietal operculum of
A lesion of the primary somatosensory cortex pro- reduced length (Steinmetz et al., 1990). This variation is
duces a temporary loss of sensation over the contralat- suggested to be related to impaired receptive language
eral body. Recovery may be almost complete with time. processing and dyslexia (Kibby et al., 2004). Another
Some loss in muscle control may remain. The parietal variation (Steinmetz type IV) in which the lateral fis-
lobe supplies fibers to the corticospinal tract that pro- sure transitions into the postcentral sulcus appears to
ject to the ventral region of the dorsal horn of the spinal correlate with dyslexia and superior nonverbal pro-
cord. Here they are believed to regulate incoming sen- cessing (Steinmetz et al., 1990; Chiarello et al., 2006;
sory signals. They may also be involved in the level and Craggs et al., 2006). 41
Occipital and parietal lobes

Superior parietal lobule the hand. The anterior SPL (BA 5 and anterior BA 7)
Brodmann’s areas 5 and 7 on the lateral aspect make is involved in evaluating the shape and size of objects
up the superior parietal lobule (Figures 4.1 and 4.2). based on touch (Naito et al., 2008).
BA 7 on the medial aspect is more commonly referred The SPL is concerned with “where” a target is
to as the precuneus and is discussed separately below located (dorsal visual stream) (Figure 6.10). It provides
(Figure 2.2). information about the location of the target including
The SPL receives heavy input from the primary the direction and velocity of movement of that target.
somatosensory cortex. Cortical association fibers con- It can program a plan designed to intercept the tar-
nect it with adjacent cortex, including the occipital get using saccadic eye movements or hand and body
lobe, temporal lobe, and insular lobe, thus providing ­movements. Long association fiber bundles from the
direct access to touch, vision, audition and vestibu- SPL to the frontal cortex allow for the accurate execu-
lar signals. Reciprocal fibers connect the SPL with the tion of the developed plan. The anterior SPL is import-
pulvinar, the anterior cingulate gyrus, and the lateral ant in the perception of object shape based on feedback
thalamic nuclei (Chapter 9). Pyramidal cells found in of fine finger movements. The right anterior SPL attends
the SPL contribute heavily to fibers that project to the to the object being explored while the left anterior SPL
brainstem and to the spinal cord. Efferent fibers from maintains information about object shape in working
the SPL also project to motor control centers such as memory (Stoeckel et al., 2004).
the red nucleus, basal ganglia, superior colliculus, and The SPL is part of a dorsal network that functions
pontine tegmentum. Long association bundles connect in spatial attention and includes the frontal lobe. The
the SPL with the frontal lobe (Figure 4.5). Commissural right SPL appears to be an especially important com-
connections through the corpus callosum intercon- ponent of this network (Abdullaev and Posner, 2005;
nect the left and right SPLs. Corbetta et al., 2005). Neurons representing various
The right (nondominant) SPL is part of the poster- objects compete for representation and it appears that
ior attention system. It is critical in selecting one stimu- a top-down mechanism exists to bias the final selection
lus location among many. It also disengages and shifts (Bisley and Goldberg, 2003). That is, other areas such
attention to a new target when appropriate (Posner and as the prefrontal cortex, can alert the SPL to narrow
Dahaene, 1994; Chapter 12). The right side attends to its search to attend to a particular target. Both sides
stimuli in both visual fields and accounts for the fact play a role in shifts in attention during this process
that neglect is more severe following right parietal (Behrmann et al., 2004).
damage (Posner and Petersen, 1990). Norepinephrine Lesions in the left (dominant) SPL can produce
input to the right parietal region is greater than to the dysphasia and agnosias. The dysphasic patient speaks
left, and norepinephrine primes the cortical neurons slowly, makes many grammatical errors, and may be
during times of heightened arousal to react to novel mistakenly labeled uncooperative or confused. A
stimuli (Tucker and Williamson, 1984). lesion bordering the postcentral gyrus can produce
The SPL integrates the sensation of touch and pro- tactile agnosia, in which the patient cannot recall the
prioception with vision as well as with audition, mark- name of an object by touch alone. With eyes closed a
ing it as a multimodal integrative area. It is especially patient with astereoagnosia is unable to name a famil-
important in planning and executing visually guided iar object held in his or her hand based on the weight
reaching. It is activated during tactile exploration of and three-dimensional characteristics of the object. A
objects and body part localization (Binkofski et al., number or letter written on the patient’s skin will not
1999, 2001; Felician et al., 2004), and visuomotor track- be recognized by touch following a lesion in the super-
ing (Grafton et al., 1992), as well as when imagining ior parietal lobule (agraphesthesia).
rotary hand movements (Wolbers et al., 2003). It is also
activated during attentional switching, i.e., when vis- Precuneus
ual attention is switched between targets (Rees et al., The precuneus is the medial aspect of the SPL repre-
2002). The parietal lobe is concerned with selecting sented by BA 7. It is rarely damaged due to strokes or
and attending to a specific target located on the skin trauma and its function has been revealed only recently
or in the nearby extrapersonal space. Anterior superior by imaging studies. The neurons are not uniform in
parietal association area (BA 5) provides the ability to shape throughout the precuneus. The anterior por-
42 appreciate the weight and texture of an object held in tion is characterized by larger neurons and the smaller
Parietal lobe

Clinical vignette the frontal lobe, including the premotor and supple-
A 65-year-old right-handed man had progressive dif- mentary motor areas, as well as the dorsolateral and
ficulty locating items in space or orienting himself in ventromedial prefrontal areas. Intermediate between
familiar surrounding. He behaved as if blind, unable to the precuneus and prefrontal lobe in terms of signal
either look at or reach for objects in his environment, processing is the posterior cingulate and the retrosple-
such as the buttons on his clothes or utensils for eat- nial cortices (BA 30) (Figure 12.1). The retrosplenial
ing. When presented with complex scenes, he could cortex has reciprocal connections with the precuneus
not recognize more than one item at a time (simul- as well as with the medial temporal lobe including the
tanagnosia). He could not identify two adjacent but hippocampus. The precuneus has connections with
unlinked drawings, large letters made up of smaller
the dorsal thalamus including the pulvinar. Brainstem
ones, or fragmented pictures. When commanded to
connections with structures such as the pretectal area,
move his eyes to specific visual objects in his periph-
eral fields, he could not do so (oculomotor apraxia). superior colliculus, and reticular formation as well as
When attempting to reach out and touch objects in the frontal eye fields suggest a role in eye movement
his peripheral fields with either arm, he would entirely control (Leichnetz, 2001; Parvizi et al., 2006). The pre-
miss them (optic ataxia).In addition to the visuospatial cuneus is recognized as being involved in four general
deficits of Balint syndrome, the patient had other functions:  consciousness, body movements in space,
impairments. Despite the absence of motor weak- self-awareness, episodic memory retrieval, and visuo-
ness, he was unable to brush his teeth or wave good- spatial imagery.
bye with his left upper extremity on verbal command
(ideomotor apraxia). His attempts at performing these Consciousness
praxis tasks resulted in grotesque motor movements Along with the IPL and the ventromedial, dorsomedial
of his left upper extremity. He also had a slow, rigid prefrontal, and retrosplenial cortices, the precuneus
gait (parkinsonism), abnormal posturing of his right
is highly metabolically active during the resting state
hand and neck (dystonia), and spontaneous jerking of
his extremities (myoclonus). Single photon emission (Alkire et al., 2008). The precuneus and the retros-
tomography imaging showed decreased perfusion plenial cortex and posterior cingulate gyrus make up
in both parietal regions (Figure 4.7). This patient’s ill- the “posteromedial cortex.” The precuneus is the most
ness was consistent with corticobasal degeneration, active of these areas consuming about 35% more glu-
a disorder that includes cortical deficits such as Balint cose (Gusnard and Raichle, 2001). The precuneus
syndrome and ideomotor apraxia, and basal ganglia is believed to be tonically active during the resting,
deficits, such as parkinsonism and dystonia. waking state. It continuously gathers and processes
information about the world within and around us.
posterior portion is populated with smaller neurons, It receives visual input from the dorsal visual stream
suggesting a regional difference in the function of the so is constantly monitoring the peripheral visual field.
precuneus (Zilles et al., 2003). The precuneus receives It can choose at any time to shift attention to a novel
multimodal sensory input from the lateral parietal attractive target unless inhibited by the frontal lobe.
cortex including the superior and inferior lobules as Joint activation of the precuneus and prefrontal cortex
well as from the cortex within the intraparietal sul- may underlie a state of reflective self-awareness, and
cus (IPS). It has strong reciprocal connections with activity correlates with mind-wandering (Kjaer and

Figure 4.7.  This scan is a three-


­dimensional computerized recon-
struction of the patient’s single photon
emission ­tomography images. The left
hemisphere is on the left side. There are
prominent bilateral areas of decreased
perfusion in both parietal lobes con-
sistent with his Balint syndrome and
ideomotor apraxia. (Reprinted with per-
mission from Mendez, 2000.)

43
Occipital and parietal lobes

Lou, 2000; Kjaer, et al., 2002). Both these areas show the precuneus where they are relived and elaborated.
significant deactivation during states of altered con- It is believed to gather and integrate past information
sciousness including sleep, hypnosis, dreaming, and regarding the self and external world especially in the
persistent vegetative state (Maquet et al., 1997, 1999; realm of spatial tasks (Gündel et al., 2001; Lou et al.,
Laureys et al., 1999; Rainville et al., 1999; Hobson et 2004). It provides self-representation, alertness, and
al., 2000; Maquet, 2000). Along with the posterior cin- “the internal mentation processes of self-conscious-
gulate gyrus, the precuneus becomes progressively ness” (Cavanna, 2007).
deactivated as anesthetic-induced sedation progresses
(Alkire et al., 1999; Fiset et al., 1999). It is also one of Self-awareness
the first areas to resume activity when consciousness Self-awareness includes the recognition of self-own-
is regained (Laureys et al., 2004, 2006). The precuneus ership and that “I am the initiator of the action and
becomes less active during goal-directed cognitive or thus that I am causally involved in production of that
perceptual tasks, suggesting it is selectively reducing action” (Gallagher, 2000). Self-awareness allows one
awareness of potentially distracting environmental fac- to realize that someone else may be the initiator of
tors (Gusnard and Raichle, 2001). Its activity is reduced action in appropriate situations. Action attributed to
in normal aging but more so in patients with dementia another produced activity in the right IPL suggesting
(Lustig et al., 2003). that it monitors multimodal sensory signals repre-
senting movements of both the self and others in an
Body movements in space
allocentric frame of reference (Farrer and Frith, 2002).
The precuneus is activated when preparing to make a The precuneus appears to play a role in self-related
movement or when executing a movement in space, tasks whether involving spatial orientation, episodic
especially movements involving pointing, reach- memory, or social judgments. Along with the posterior
ing, and saccades. It is activated when an individual cingulate, the precuneus is activated when processing
just imagines making a movement (Hanakawa, et al., intentions related to the self (Vogeley and Fink, 2003;
2003). It also appears to play a key role in attending to a den Ouden et al. 2005).
target and shifting attention from one target to another, The precuneus is believed to be part of a network
even if no movement is made (Beauchamp et al., 2001; involved in theory-of-mind processing. Activation of
Simon et al., 2002). the precuneus and posterior cingulate (BA 31) occurs
The precuneus is part of a network that elaborates in situations involving deception and cooperation. It
information about maps to help locate one’s self on these appears to function in a broad sense related to per-
maps. It can operate using retinotopic coordinates or spective taking as well as attribution, and the pro-
head-centered coordinates. When imagining moving cessing of emotions and intentions in this situation
through an environment with obstacles the precuneus is (Lissek et al., 2008). It shows strong activation when
activated bilaterally along with the right lateral parietal making judgments that require empathy in social situ-
cortex and left supplementary motor cortex (Malouin ations (Farrow, et al., 2001; Ruby and Decety, 2001).
et al., 2003). It has been suggested that the precuneus In studies, the left precuneus was preferentially acti-
acts as the “mind’s eye” in these situations, assessing the vated when attributing emotions and intentions to the
environment and choosing a navigable route through actions of others (Ochsner et al., 2004; Abraham et al.,
it (Burgess et al., 2001). In this role it may activate vis- 2008).
ual images associated with remembered words, objects,
and specific autobiographical events as part of episodic
memory recall of the meaning of specific environmental Intraparietal sulcus
landmarks. The anterior precuneus appears to be asso- The IPS (or intraparietal fissure) lies on the lateral
ciated with attention and active recall (visual imagery) aspect, separates the superior from the inferior par-
whereas the posterior precuneus is more selectively ietal lobule, and contains the intraparietal cortex.
activated during the successful recall of specific events The shape and course of the sulcus are variable but in
(Cavanna and Trimble, 2006). general it extends from the postcentral sulcus to the
occipital lobe. The IPS becomes deeper and its cor-
Episodic memory retrieval tex more extensive as it approaches the occipital lobe
44 Evidence suggests that memories of personally expe- (Figure 4.8). The more anterior parts in the human are
rienced events (episodic memory) are retrieved to very similar in location to those of the monkey whereas
Parietal lobe

Intraparietal sulcus Figure 4.8.  The intraparietal sulcus


extends from the postcentral sulcus to the
occipital lobe. It is the border between
the superior and inferior parietal lobules.
The sectional views show the depth and
­complexity of the sulcus as well as the
Lateral fissure
extent of the medial and lateral banks.
Calcarine fissure

Intraparietal sulcus
Central sulcus
Lateral fissure

those closer to the occipital lobe are more variable. stream alerts the dorsal visual stream to create a motor
The cortex that makes up the medial and lateral banks program in the IPS and surrounding region to look at
within the sulcus has been described as the intrapari- and grasp the desired package (“see and seize”).
etal area. It has been subdivided into 17 subregions in
the monkey (Lewis and Van Essen, 2000a). The lateral, Lateral intraparietal area (parietal saccade region)
medial, anterior, ventral, and posterior intraparietal The lateral intraparietal area is one of the most stud-
subregions are recognized in the human. The anterior ied subregions in the monkey, in which it makes up
IPS is more concerned with somatosensory process- the lateral bank of the sulcus near the occipital lobe. In
ing whereas the posterior IPS processes visual signals. the human the lateral intraparietal area lies close to the
Neurons in the medial bank are more responsive to occipital lobe but appears to be more medially located
arm movements, and lateral bank neurons are more in the IPS (Koyama et al., 2004). The lateral intrapa-
responsive to eye movements. rietal area receives information regarding objects in
Each area of the IPS functions in response to more the contralateral hemifield. Object location, direction,
than one sensory modality making the cortex of the and velocity are registered, using signals from the vis-
IPS a multimodal integrative area. The IPS acts to ual, auditory, and somatosensory systems (Andersen,
focus attention in space to salient stimuli and espe- 1997; Andersen and Buneo, 2003). It also monitors
cially threat-related stimuli. Fearful faces bias atten- current eye position with respect to the head and head
tion toward the threat-related location and increase position and with respect to the body as well as the
the gain of face-related signals in the occipital cortex. head’s orientation to gravity (i.e., vestibular sense).
The timing of these events suggests that negative emo- The lateral intraparietal area determines the relative
tional stimuli can focus attention on a specific loca- importance of the object (salience) and then programs
tion through mechanisms in the IPS (Pourtois and and executes a saccade to that object (Ipata et al., 2006).
Vuilleumier, 2006). This is a bottom-up control of attention as the atten-
Information carried by the dorsal visual stream tion is determined by the attractiveness of the target
defines target location. Information carried by the (Buschman and Miller, 2007).
ventral visual stream defines target identification. Top-
down signals from the frontal lobe can act to preselect Medial intraparietal area (parietal reach region)
targets. An individual in a grocery aisle records the A region of the medial bank of the IPS near the
location of all items on the nearby shelf using the dorsal occipital lobe and extending laterally onto the sur-
visual stream. The frontal lobe maintains in working face of the superior parietal lobule is described as
memory the specific brand from the ­shopping list. The the “parietal reach region” (Connolly et al., 2003).
ventral visual stream matches the shopping list brand It operates in a manner similar to saccade program- 45
with the package label on the shelf. The ventral visual ming but is responsible for planning and executing
Occipital and parietal lobes

visually guided reaching movements of the upper to monitor a personal zone of safety surrounding the
limb. Success of the movement is greater if the target body (Graziano and Cooke, 2005).
is located on the fovea. If the target is in the periph-
Posterior intraparietal area (three-dimensional analysis)
ery (e.g., reaching for a cup of coffee while reading
The posterior intraparietal area in the monkey occupies
the paper) a larger region of parieto-occipital cor-
the lateral bank of the IPS close to the occipital lobe and
tex is activated including the precuneus. Continual
posterior to the lateral intraparietal area. It receives
adjustments are made while reaching for an object
input from visual areas V3 and V4 and is involved
in the periphery, suggesting an “automatic pilot”
in the analysis of the features of three-dimensional
since the individual is unaware of the adjustments
(3D) objects including texture features (Tsutsui et al.,
(Himmelbach et al., 2006).
2002). Results indicate that this area is responsible for
Anterior intraparietal area (parietal reach and grasp region) the short-term memory of object surface features that
The anterior intraparietal area is described as the allows the changing view of an object to be remem-
grasp region and occupies the anterior lateral bank bered long enough to compare current with past views.
of the sulcus (Grol et al., 2007). Neurons of the anter- Binocular and monocular depth cues and other infor-
ior area are active during fixation and manipulation mation from both the dorsal and ventral visual streams
of objects (Buxbaum et al., 2003). It contains neurons are integrated in this region. Signals from the posterior
that are sensitive to size, shape, and orientation of intraparietal region are forwarded to the anterior intra-
objects to be grasped. The objects may be viewed or parietal region to code for finger shaping and grasping
remembered and the area also monitors hand pos- movements (Grefkes and Fink, 2005).
ition and movement (Tunik et al., 2005). The anter- Lesions involving the IPS may cause neglect or extinc-
ior intraparietal area projects to the ventral premotor tion affecting more than one modality. Since the area is
cortex to provide signals for finger and arm move- head-centered, lesions in this area may account for symp-
ments involved in reaching and grasping. Pa and toms seen in visuospatial neglect (Vallar et al., 2003).
Hickok (2007) showed that a region of the anterior In comparison with normally developing controls,
intraparietal area was activated by pianists making IPS depth in children with Asperger syndrome has
covert (imagined) playing movements while listening been reported to be greater. The depth of the IPS cor-
to music, suggesting this is also a region of auditory- relates with age and IQ (Nordahl et al., 2007). The gray
manual integration. matter density is less in the left IPS in children with
dyscalculia (Isaacs et al., 2001). Molko et al. (2004)
Ventral intraparietal area (navigation in space) showed that the right IPS was shallower and tended
The ventral intraparietal area lies deep within the IPS to be shorter in girls with Turner syndrome. This cor-
and is believed to contain a crude somatotopic map related with difficulty during calculation tasks and
with most of the map devoted to the head. It receives visuospatial processing.
input from higher order visual processing areas includ-
ing the middle temporal area (MT) and the middle Inferior parietal lobule
superior temporal area (MST). It also receives input The inferior parietal lobule (IPL) corresponds with the
from motor, somatosensory, auditory, and vestibu- supramarginal gyrus (BA 40) and the angular gyrus
lar areas (Lewis and Van Essen, 2000b). It appears to (BA 39; Figure 4.2). Further subdivisions have been
code for self and object motion and coordinates eye suggested. Based on its histological and magnetic res-
and head movements. Neurons in the ventral intrapa- onance imaging features, the supramarginal gyrus (BA
rietal area are sensitive to the direction and velocity of 40) can be divided into five areas and the angular gyrus
moving visual targets as well as current head position, (BA 39) into two (Zilles et al., 2003; Caspers et al., 2006).
velocity, and acceleration. Other neurons in the same Considerable variability has been found between spec-
area are active during pursuit eye movements (Schlack imens, as well as left-right differences (asymmetry).
et al., 2003). Signals from visual, vestibular, and audi- The individual variability is believed to be related to
tory cortices also contribute to the ventral intrapari- the fact that this is one of the last regions of the cortex
etal area, emphasizing its importance as a multimodal to mature. The final configuration may be influenced
integrative region (Schlack et al., 2002). It plans motor by experience (Caspers et al., 2006). Like the SPL, the
responses to navigate through nearby space while IPL has reciprocal connections with the pulvinar and
46
avoiding obstacles (Bremmer, 2005). It may function the lateral thalamic nuclei. Short association fibers
Parietal lobe

connect it with nearby occipital and temporal lobes, as the right IPL is larger than the left (Frederikse et al.,
well as the SPL and precuneus. Long association fib- 1999; Chen et al., 2007). One subdivision of the supra-
ers link the IPL with the frontal cortex, including the marginal gyrus (area PFcm) which makes up part of
frontal eye fields. its inferior aspect, accounts for most of the gender
The IPL receives signals representing the sensation difference (Caspers et al., 2008). Weaker leftward
of touch, proprioception, and vision and integrates asymmetry has been reported for patients with bor-
these signals in order to determine the identity of a tar- derline personality disorder. Psychotic symptoms and
get (Aguirre and D’Esposito, 1997). The strategic loca- schizoid personality traits of these patients were cor-
tion of the angular gyrus between the occipital lobe and related with larger right IPL size suggesting a right-
Wernicke’s speech area results in it being “the region sided neurodevelopmental deficit (Irle et al., 2005,
which turns written language into spoken language 2007). In another study, an increase over controls was
and vice versa …” (Geschwind, 1965). observed in the gray matter of the right supramarginal
The IPL has been described as where “all the facts gyrus, which was specific for deficits in social commu-
can be stored and retrieved” (Bear, 1983). It has also nication and interaction, and repetitive, stereotyped
been described as “an association area of association behavior in children with autism spectrum disorder
areas” (Geschwind, 1965). Remembering (retrieval (Brieber et al., 2007).
of specific content) and knowing (perception that The supramarginal gyrus of the left IPL works in
processed information is from the past) activates the cooperation with the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
IPL as well as portions of the intraparietal sulcus. in support of working memory. Collette et al. (2007)
Remembering also activates the anterior fusiform found that these two areas became activated dur-
gyrus bilaterally, which processes visual information ing working memory in control subjects but failed to
related to objects (Wheeler and Buckner, 2004). In one show activation in patients with posttraumatic stress
study, the left IPL, extrastriate body area, premotor disorder. This failure in activation appears to reflect a
cortex, and supplementary motor were activated when reduction in working memory updating rather than in
the viewer saw body movements that were within the working memory maintenance operations. The reduc-
range of motor capability of the viewer (i.e., capable of tion in working memory updating is suggested to relate
imitation) (Blakemore and Decety, 2001). The IPL plays to the difficulty in concentrating and remembering
an important role when the self takes the perspective of reported by patients with posttraumatic stress disorder
others (Ruby and Decety, 2001) and may be part of a (Moores et al., 2008).
“concern mechanism” (Decety and Chaminade, 2003).
The IPL functions to encode and retrieve a motor
sequence. It is proposed that different areas within the
Behavioral considerations
IPL encode different types of sequences. The angular Schizophrenia
gyrus is particularly involved in evaluating visual mes- Attention deficits are recognized in patients with
sages and selecting an appropriate motor sequence in schizophrenia (Laurens et al., 2005). Since the dorsal
response (Ruby et al., 2002). visual stream serves areas of the parietal lobe import-
The IPL and the supramarginal gyrus in particu- ant in attention it has been hypothesized that a defect
lar are activated in skill learning (right side) and tool in this system may underlie attention deficits in this
use (left side). It is hypothesized that the supramarginal population (Laycock et al., 2007). However, there
gyrus stores information about limb and hand posi- appears to be limited evidence to support this hypoth-
tions based on previous experience. Retrieval of a pre- esis (Skottun and Skoyles, 2008).
viously formed motor memory may be important in Volume reductions were reported for the IPL in
elaborating the skill and adapting it to a new situation patients with schizophrenia (Schlaepfer et al., 1994;
(Seidler and Noll, 2008; Vingerhoets, 2008). Goldstein et al., 1999) and in all parietal subregions
Although the left and right IPL are not signifi- in a group of 53 patients with schizophrenia and
cantly different in size, asymmetries have been found schizotypal personality disorder (Zhou, et al., 2007).
when sex is included in the analysis. Total IPL volume Delusions of passivity in individuals with schizo-
is greater in males than females and the left IPL is lar- phrenia are associated with hyperactivity coupled
ger in men than women (i.e., leftward asymmetry). with reductions in gray matter volume of the infer-
The asymmetry is less marked in women in whom ior parietal lobe (Dankert et al., 2004; Maruff et al., 47
Occipital and parietal lobes

2005). Male but not female patients with schizophre- Attention


nia have been found to show a reversal of the normal Three attention networks have been described, all of
left greater than right angular gyrus (Frederikse et al., which involve the parietal cortex. The default network
2000; Niznikiewicz et al., 2000). These regions sup- described above is active during resting states (see
port language and may help explain the language and page 43). It is believed that the default network sup-
thought disorders found in schizophrenia (Shenton ports internally directed, mind-wandering processing
et al., 2001). Whalley et al. (2004) found that during that includes spontaneous and introspective thoughts,
a verbal task subjects at high risk for schizophrenia remembering one’s role in past events, or planning one’s
showed increased activation in the left IPL compared future (Raichle et al., 2001; Fransson and Marrelec,
with controls. It was suggested that the overactivation 2008). When either of the other two attention networks
of the IPL is a compensatory action related to attention (dorsal and ventral) is activated, the default network is
to task and preparation of a suitable response. Subjects suppressed.
at risk for schizophrenia who showed isolated symp- Corbetta et al. (2000) reported that the dorsal atten-
toms, reported difficulties in focusing attention and tion network is activated when presented with import-
exhibited a state-related overactivation of the intra- ant external stimuli in a bottom-up manner or when
parietal sulcus. In another study, gray matter volume the subject is asked to voluntarily direct attention to a
was observed to be reduced in the precuneus in men specific cue in a top-down manner. The structures of
with schizophrenia accompanied by small regions of the dorsal attention network become activated bilat-
increased gray matter in the right IPL (Shapleske et al., erally and include the cortex of the intraparietal sulcus
2002). The posterior parietal lobe and precuneus have and the junction of the precentral and superior frontal
been implicated in distinguishing between self and sulcus (frontal eye field).
others (Meltzoff and Decety, 2003). The ventral attention network consists of the right
The IPL is part of frontal-limbic-temporal-parietal temporoparietal junction and the right ventral frontal
network involved in schizophrenia (Torrey, 2007). cortex. When attention is focused, the dorsal network
Reduced asymmetry has been reported for individuals is suppressed to prevent attention to distracting stim-
with schizophrenia, with smaller left and larger right uli (Todd et al., 2005). The ventral attention network is
IPLs than controls (Frederikse et al., 2000; Niznikiewicz thought to act in concert with signals from the dorsal
et al., 2000; Nierenberg et al., 2005). The smaller size of attention network to provide an interrupt or reset sig-
the left temporoparietal cortex including the left supra- nal coincident with shifting attention to a new exter-
marginal gyrus correlates with the severity of auditory nal target (“Is that my phone ringing?”) or to a new
hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia (Gaser thought (“Did I remember to turn off the stove?”). It
et al., 2004). may be active in reorienting when the individual has
A group of men with schizophrenia and antisocial no ongoing task; however in most situations it reacts
personality disorder were compared based on a history and supports reorientation in response to important
of violent behavior with healthy controls, using fMRI. novel stimuli selected by the dorsal system (Corbetta
Men with schizophrenia and a history of violent behav- et al., 2008).
ior showed reduced activation in the precuneus, right
IPL, left frontal gyrus, and anterior cingulate gyrus.
Spatial neglect
Men with antisocial personality disorder also showed Spatial neglect is considered a visual attention dis-
reduced activation in the precuneus, left frontal gyrus, order. Patients do not recognize the opposite side of
and anterior cingulate gyrus. The reduced activation their body and will not dress it (dressing apraxia).
of the right IPL in the subjects with schizophrenia Patients are often unaware of the deficit (anosogno-
was most strongly associated with ratings of violence. sia). Inability to correctly copy a simple drawn sym-
It was hypothesized that the combined involvement metrical figure (constructional apraxia) is common.
of the right IPL and frontal cortex reflected impaired The right IPL at the temporoparietal junction trad-
executive control (Kumari et al., 2006). Episodic mem- itionally has been implicated (Mort et al., 2003). It has
ory, associated with the precuneus, is reported to be been suggested that there is a map of the contralateral
one of the most severely impaired neuropsychological body and peripersonal space in the left IPL, but both an
elements in schizophrenia (Reichenberg and Harvey, ipsilateral and contralateral map in the right IPL. Loss
48 2007). of the map found in the left hemisphere has little or no
Parietal lobe

effect because of a redundant right hemifield map in contralesional visual field with either hand. Their
the right hemisphere. A lesion in the right hemisphere efforts are inaccurate when using peripheral vision
results in a loss of the map of the left side. It is theo- (dorsal visual stream), but accuracy is normal when
rized that since the brain cannot locate objects on the using foveal vision (ventral visual stream). Pointing
opposite side following a nondominant parietal lobe errors improve with time in a way that suggests that
lesion, the objects cannot be attended to and therefore recovery involves potential interaction between the
are ignored. damaged dorsal visual stream and intact ventral vis-
Spatial neglect can occur following damage to ual stream (Himmelbach and Karnath, 2005; Karnath
several areas including subcortical sites (caudate and Perenin, 2005). The dorsal and ventral streams do
nucleus, putamen, and pulvinar), the frontal lobe, not work independently since patients with parietal
and the superior temporal gyrus. Most frequently, lesions are usually not successful in completing all
however, the IPL and temporoparietal junction in visuospatial tasks using intact ventral streams alone
particular, are involved, including the white matter (Ellison and Cowey, 2007).
deep to this cortex (Mort et al., 2003; Thiebaut de
Schotten et al., 2005). Neglect of personal space cor- Apraxia
relates with lesions of the supramarginal and post- Apraxia is the inability to executed skilled, learned
central gyri with some involvement of the posterior motor acts despite preservation of motor and sensory
superior temporal gyrus (Committeri et al., 2007). A systems, comprehension, cooperation, and coordin-
lesion restricted to the posterior superior temporal ation. Ideomotor apraxia is impaired performance in
gyrus may also result in spatial neglect (Karnath et spite of preservation of sensory, motor, and language
al., 2001). Lesions involving white matter pathways function (Heilman and Rothi, 2003). The patient can-
between the temporoparietal region and the frontal not perform a meaningful movement of a limb when
cortex have been implicated in hemineglect but do not requested. Many authors include the inability to imi-
necessarily cause neglect. However, damage to these tate gestures as part of ideomotor apraxia.
pathways or to the connections between the dorsal Ideational apraxia is seen as the impaired ability to
and ventral attention networks may impair shifting carry out the appropriate sequential actions of a multi-
from one environment target to another (Doricchi et step, complex task. It is a disruption of the space–time
al., 2008). Since the ventral attention network is local- movement plan or of its proper activation. The patient
ized to the right hemisphere, its loss may inhibit an cannot form a plan to carry out movements in a proper
attempt to direct attention to the contralesional side sequence. Ideational apraxia is associated with lesions
(He et al., 2007). of the left parietal lobe alone or in combination with
Damage to the ventral (temporal) visual stream, temporal and frontal lesions.
concerned with detailed object information, leads to The left parietal lobe is activated when carrying out
allocentric impairment. Damage to the dorsal (pari- meaningful limb movements and ideomotor apraxia is
etofrontal) visual stream results in egocentric deficits seen following a lesion of the left parietal lobe. Since
(Grimsen et al., 2008). Interestingly, caloric vestibular many meaningful movements involve the use of tools,
stimulation has been reported to temporarily improve knowledge of the location of tool use is helpful in under-
a number of elements of sensory neglect including standing apraxia and patient rehabilitation (Wheaton,
anosognosia for left hemiplegia (Vallar et al., 2005; 2007). Specific areas associated with apraxia include
Rode et al., 1998; Bottini et al., 2005). It is proposed the cortex of and around the left IPS and the left middle
that anosognosia of hemiplegia is due to a loss of motor frontal gyrus (Haaland et al., 2000). Right side lesions
planning ability (Vallar et al., 2003). can also result in apraxia as well as a lesion of the anter-
ior corpus callosum (Leiguarda and Marsden, 2000;
Optic ataxia Petreska et al., 2007). A model of praxis proposes that
Optic ataxia can be seen following a lesion on either the left inferior parietal lobe is an integrative area that
side involving the dorsal visual processing stream in brings together information from the dorsal and ven-
the posterior superior parietal lobule. It has been seen tral visual streams. It is an area that processes represen-
following lesions in or near the medial IPS (Pisella tations of body part positions and generates plans for
et al., 2000; Roy et al., 2004). Patients are impaired limb movements (Buxbaum et al., 2007). The frontal
in the ability to reach for and grasp objects in the lobe is responsible for gesture production. 49
Occipital and parietal lobes

Gerstmann syndrome Individuals with borderline personality disorder


Pantomime recognition (recognition of common ges- have been shown to have reduced size of the parietal
tures) may be lost following damage to the dominant lobes. The right precuneus has been shown to exhibit
IPL. A lesion involving the angular gyrus (BA 39) may reduced resting glucose metabolism and reduced size
produce part or all of Gerstmann syndrome:  in individuals with borderline personality disorder
• Left–right confusion. (Left–right confusion (Lange et al., 2005; Irle et al., 2007). The right postcen-
among neurologically intact adults is seen in 9% of tral gyrus was shown to be larger in individuals with
men and 18% of women.) a comorbid diagnosis of dissociative amnesia or dis-
• Finger agnosia –difficulty in naming fingers. sociative identity disorder. Individuals with borderline
personality disorder have also been reported to have
• Dysgraphia –difficulty with writing.
elevated pain thresholds coupled with reduced activity
• Dyscalculia –difficulty with numbers.
in the precuneus in response to pain (Schmahl et al.,
2006). Irle et al. (2007) suggested that the smaller pre-
Balint syndrome cuneus may relate to symptoms of depersonalization.
The increased postcentral gyrus has been speculated to
The patient with Balint syndrome is unable to view the
relate to the dissociative amnesia or dissociative iden-
visual field as a whole and is fixed on only one part;
tity disorder reflecting severe childhood abuse (Lange
a form of tunnel vision (simultanagnosia). Bilateral
et al., 2005; Irle et al., 2007).
damage to the posterior IPL, often including adjacent
Parietal lobe seizures can produce bizarre and tran-
occipital cortex, may produce Balint syndrome: 
sient symptoms that can be confusing to both patients
• Optic apraxia –eyes tend to remain fixed (stuck) and clinicians. Feelings of paresthesia, numbness,
on a visual target, although spontaneous eye heat, or cold have been described. These feelings can
movements are unaffected. begin locally and spread to other contiguous body
• Optic ataxia –a deficit in using visual guidance to parts. Seizures beginning more posteriorly can cause
grasp an object. pronounced distortion of body image. Limbs may feel
• Simultanagnosia –seeing only the components heavier or feel as if they disappear. Even more bizarre,
of a visual object; unable to see the object as a patients have reported feeling that someone is stand-
whole. ing close by or the appearance of a phantom third
Bilateral damage to the occipitoparietal area, often limb. Patients with newly diagnosed schizophrenia are
extending deep enough to involve the precuneus is the reported to show greater sulcal enlargement in the par-
most common cause of this disorder (Raichle et al., ietal lobe (Rubin et al., 1993).
2001). The similarities between hysteria and parietal
lobe disease should again be stressed. Patients with
parietal lobe lesions may show marked inconsistency
Other considerations in task performances, such that he or she may suc-
A lesion involving the nondominant IPL may produce ceed in a task that moments before appeared to be
a deficit in processing the nonsyntactic component of impossible.
language (aprosodia). In this situation, patients fail to
appreciate aspects of a verbal message that are con-
veyed by the tone, loudness, and timing of the words
Select bibliography
Critchley, M.The Parietal Lobes. (London: Edward Arnold,
(i.e., emotional tone).
1953.)
Van Essen et al. (2006) found that subjects with
Hecanen, H., and Albert, M.L. Human Neuropsychology.
William syndrome showed reduced asymmetry of
(New York: Wiley, 1978.)
the lateral fissure and no asymmetry in the superior
Hyvarinen, J. The Parietal Cortex of Monkey and Man. (New
temporal sulcus compared with controls. The authors York: Springer-Verlag, 1982.)
suggested that in light of the relatively intact language
Lishman, W.A. Organic Psychiatry (2nd ed.). (Boston:
skills and heightened emotional responses to music  Blackwell Scientific, 1987.)
in William syndrome subjects, the abnormalities in
Milner, A.D., and Goodale, M.A. The Visual Brain in Action
cortical folding might reflect altered cortical circuitry (2nd ed.). Oxford Psychology Series. (New York: Oxford
50 rather than a simple size reduction. University Press, 2006.)

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi