Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/318404532

Parenting Stress and Development of Late Preterm Infants at 4 Months


Corrected Age

Article  in  Research in Nursing & Health · July 2017


DOI: 10.1002/nur.21809

CITATIONS READS

0 137

1 author:

Muhammad Kashif Mughal


The University of Calgary
20 PUBLICATIONS   64 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Immune Gene Polymorphisms and Resiliency: Linking the bench side and the clinical data View project

Acute Myeloid Leukaemia project View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Muhammad Kashif Mughal on 21 November 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Received: 27 January 2017 | Accepted: 6 June 2017
DOI: 10.1002/nur.21809

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Parenting stress and development of late preterm infants at


4 months corrected age

Muhammad K. Mughal1 | Carla S. Ginn1 | Joyce Magill-Evans2 |


Karen M. Benzies RN, PhD3

1 University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada


2 Department of Occupational Therapy,
ABSTRACT
University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Parenting stress has been linked to child development issues in early preterm infants, but
Canada
less is known about its effects on development in infants born late preterm. We
3 Faculty of Nursing, Department of
Paediatrics, University of Calgary, Calgary, examined relationships between parenting stress of 108 mothers and 108 fathers and
Alberta, Canada,
development of late preterm infants born at 34 0/7 to 36 6/7 weeks gestation. At
Correspondence 4 months corrected age, mothers and fathers completed the Parenting Stress Index (PSI-
Prof. Karen M. Benzies, RN, PhD, Faculty of
3); mothers were primary caregivers in almost all families and completed the Ages and
Nursing, Department of Paediatrics,
University of Calgary, 2500 University Stages Questionnaire (ASQ-2) on child development. Mothers reported significantly
Dr. N.W., Calgary, Alberta T2N 1N4, Canada
more stress than fathers on the PSI-3 Parent Domain. PSI-3 subscale scores from the
Email: benzies@ucalgary.ca
Child Domain were significant predictors of mother-reported infant development as
Funding information
measured by the ASQ-2 in regression models: Reinforces Parent predicted Gross Motor,
Alberta Innovates (formerly Alberta Heritage
Foundation for Medical Research) Mood predicted Communication, and Acceptability predicted Communication, Fine
Motor, Problem Solving, and Personal-Social development scale scores. Experiences of
parenting stress differed for mothers and fathers. Further research is required on
specific dimensions of parenting stress related to development of late preterm infants.

KEYWORDS
birth, developmental stages/events, emotional states/feelings, health promotion/weIlness
behaviors, stress and coping, stress management

1 | INTRODUCTION major organ systems (Sahni & Polin, 2013). Neurological immaturity is
associated with the lack of opportunity for the exponential intra-uterine
In Canada, 7.7% of children are born preterm (Public Health Agency of brain growth and development that occurs in the last 6 to 8 weeks of
Canada, 2013); of these, 75% are born late preterm, between 34 weeks gestation (Baron, Litman, Ahronovich, & Baker, 2012). Compared to their
and 0 days and 36 weeks and 6 days gestation (Ananth, Friedman, & fullterm counterparts, late preterm infants have lower volumes of gray
Gyamfi-Bannerman, 2013; Engle, 2006; Engle, Tomashek, Wallman, & matter (Munakata et al., 2013) and demonstrate less synchrony and
Committee on Fetus and Newborn, 2007). Systematic reviews suggest responsiveness in parent-child interactions (Ludwig, 2007). Similarly, late
that compared to their fullterm counterparts, late preterm infants have preterm infants have less sustained periods of alertness, less vocalizing
a greater risk of short-term morbidity (McGowan, Alderdice, Holmes, & and eye contact, more disorganized sleep-awake cycles, lower levels of
Johnston, 2011; Samra, McGrath, & Wehbe, 2011; Teune et al., 2011). arousal while awake, and fatigue more quickly than term infants (Barratt,
During the birth hospitalization, late preterm infants have greater risk Roach, & Leavitt, 1992). Males born late preterm have increased risk for
of respiratory distress syndrome, apnea, feeding difficulties, hypoglyce- neurodevelopmental delay, later school achievement, and other long-
mia, hyperbilirubinemia, sepsis, and intraventricular hemorrhage (Teune term outcomes than females born late preterm (Boyle et al., 2011; Rose &
et al., 2011), which may be attributed to physiological immaturity of the Engle, 2017). Infants of lower gestational age at birth have greater risk of

Res Nurs Health. 2017;1–10. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/nur © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. | 1


2 | MUGHAL ET AL.

complications than those of higher gestational age at birth (Potijk, de 3.1 | Parenting stress in mothers and fathers of
Winter, Bos, Kerstjens, & Reijneveld, 2015). preterm infants
In addition, parental age (Sutcliffe, Barnes, Belsky, Gardiner, &
Parenting stress may be manifested differently for mothers and fathers of
Melhuish, 2012), and education (Bornstein, Putnick, Suwalsky, & Gini,
children born term and preterm, with different effects on child
2006) can affect late preterm infant development. Mothers of late
development. In one study of parents of 37 children born at full term
preterm infants can experience breastfeeding difficulties (Nagulesapillai,
and 25 children born at 30 to 36 weeks gestation (Benzies et al., 2004),
McDonald, Fenton, Mercader, & Tough, 2013), which can subsequently
there was a positive relationship between maternal-reported parenting
affect infant health and development and increase maternal distress
stress related to children’s distractibility during infancy and conduct
(Brandon et al., 2011). Parenting stress is one aspect of the family
disorder. That is, mothers who reported higher parenting stress related to
environment associated with suboptimal outcomes for children (Coletti
their infants’ distractibility reported more conduct disorder in their children
et al., 2015) and may be particularly important for late preterm infants who
at age 7 years. In contrast, there was a negative relationship between
have greater risk of developmental delays. The aim of this study was to
paternal-reported parenting stress related to children’s distractibility
examine specific dimensions of parenting stress associated with
during infancy and conduct disorder at age 7 years. The authors speculated
development in late preterm infant at 4 months corrected age.
that fathers who were more involved with their infants experienced
greater parenting stress, and that this involvement diminished later
2 | P A R E N T I N G ST R E S S I N TE R M I N F A N T S conduct disorder in their children. Other studies of fathers are limited by
low response rates and limited detail about the dimensions of parenting
Parenting stress is multi-dimensional, can diminish parent-child stress that contributed to infant development (Wigfall, Brannen, Mooney,
interactions, and negatively affect children’s social development & Parutis, 2013; Wong, Roubinov, Gonzales, Dumka, & Millsap, 2013).
(Ashiabi & O’Neal, 2015), with mutually increasing effects (Neece, The purpose of this study was to examine the relationships
Green, & Baker, 2012). Higher stress in the parent domain has been between parenting stress in mothers and fathers who were
linked to: (a) decreased sense of competence as a parent (Slagt, Deković, co-parenting their late preterm infants and child development at
de Haan, van den Akker, & Prinzie, 2012); (b) lack of social support from 4 months corrected age. Our research questions were:
family and friends (Hostinar & Gunnar, 2015); (c) lack of emotional
closeness to the child (Tharner et al., 2012); (d) inability to recognize 1. Does parenting stress differ for mothers and fathers of late preterm
infant’s social cues (Landsem, Handegard, Tunby, Ulvund, & Ronning, infants as measured by the Parenting Stress Index—Third Edition
2014); (e) deteriorating parental health (Treyvaud et al., 2011); (f) (PSI-3) Parent and Child Domain scores?
experiencing the parental role as restricting freedom and identity 2. What is the relationship between parenting stress as measured by
(Hildingsson & Thomas, 2014); (g) depressive symptoms (Dunkel the PSI-3 Parent and Child Domain scores and subscale scores, and
Schetter & Tanner, 2012); and (h) lack of emotional and tangible infant development as measured by the Ages and Stages
support for parenting from the other parent (Raby et al., 2015). Questionnaires—Second Edition (ASQ-2) Domain scores?
Higher parenting stress related to the child includes parental 3. Which PSI-3 subscale scores predict child development on ASQ-2
perceptions of their child as: (a) distractible (Benzies, Harrison, & Magill- Domains?
Evans, 2004); (b) unable to adjust to changes in the environment (Dix & Yan,
2014); (c) displaying behaviors not positively reinforcing parenting (Slagt Bronfenbrenner’s (2005) bioecological model provided the
et al., 2012); (d) placing many demands on the parent (Kim, Mayes, Feldman, theoretical foundation for this study. Bronfenbrenner (1977) empha-
Leckman, & Swain, 2013); (e) unhappy or distressed (Rutherford, Goldberg, sized the need to study children and their families in their natural
Luyten, Bridgett, & Mayes, 2013); and (f) not meeting expectations the settings, considering how they are affected by, and affect, their
parents had for their infant (Spinelli, Poehlmann, & Bolt, 2013). environment through interconnected systems during transitions
across the lifespan. As such, child development is considered a
dynamic process of reciprocal interactions between the child and
3 | P A R E N T I N G ST R E S S AN D P R E T E R M
parents at the microsystem level (Bronfenbrenner, 2005). Parenting
INFANTS
stress can influence these interactions and ultimately influence child
development.
There is much literature regarding early preterm birth (less than 32 weeks
gestation) and parenting stress (Brett, Staniszewska, Newburn, Jones, &
Taylor, 2011; Muller-Nix et al., 2004; Singer, Salvator, Guo, Collin, Lilien, &
4 | METHO D
Baley, 1999; Treyvaud, 2014). However, late preterm infants are a
recently identified population (Engle, 2006), and it is unclear whether the
4.1 | Participants
stress experienced by parents of early preterm infants can be extended to
parents of late preterm infants. Additionally, there is limited research This cross-sectional study was a secondary analysis of baseline data
about the specific dimensions of parenting stress that affect development from a randomized controlled trial (RCT) testing an intervention to
in late preterm infants under 12 months of age (Coletti et al., 2015). improve the quality of fathers’ interactions with their late preterm
MUGHAL ET AL.
| 3

infants (Benzies et al., 2013). The study was approved by two Health normative sample, clinical cut-offs are categorized as: (a) above cut-off
Research Ethics Boards (ID E-21846 and Pro00016692). (typical development); or (b) below cut-off (a score of 2.0 standard
The RCT had three groups: (a) a two-home-visit intervention deviations or more below the mean). Examples of items from the ASQ-2
group; (b) a four-home-visit intervention group; and (c) a comparison Communication and Personal-Social Domains include: (a) Does your
group receiving one home visit with information only. The intervention baby laugh; and (b) Does your baby watch his hands? Cronbach’s α was
consisted of video self-modelling of father-child interactions, with .84, and test-retest reliability was .89 (Squires et al., 1999). The ASQ-2
immediate feedback from trained research assistants regarding was written at a 5th grade reading level and takes 10 to 15 min to
behaviors that fostered child development. For fathers in the two- complete. As recommended by the ASQ-2 developer and others
home-visit group, interventions occurred when the infant was 4 and 6 (Kerstjens, de Winter, Bocca-Tjeertes, Bos, & Reijneveld, 2012), we
months old; for the four-home-visit group, interventions occurred at 4, used corrected age to administer the ASQ-2. For this study, we
5, 6, and 7 months. Fathers in the comparison group were video- predicted child development using ASQ-2 domain raw scores, and
recorded interacting with their infant at age 4 months, with no viewing described the proportions of children in the clinical range using ASQ-2
of the recording or feedback. In overall intervention results, compared categories.
with those in the comparison group, fathers who received either
intervention (two or four visits) had improved interactions with their
4.2.2 | Parenting Stress Index—third edition (PSI-3)
infants.
Inclusion criteria were: (a) first-time, biological parent of a healthy, The PSI-3 is a 120-item, self-reported measure of stress associated
singleton, late preterm infant; (b) age 18 years or older; (c) English-speaking with parenting children from 1 month to 12 years (Abidin, 1995). The
in at least 50% of interactions with infant; (d) cohabiting with infant’s other Parent Domain has seven subscales (Competence, Isolation,
biological parent; and (e) living within 100 km of the university. A total of Attachment, Health, Role Restriction, Depression, and Spouse).
222 participants (111 mothers and 111 fathers) were enrolled from two The Child Domain has six subscales (Distractibility/Hyperactivity,
Canadian cities between December 2008 and June 2011. Adaptability, Reinforces Parent, Demandingness, Mood, and Ac-
At 4 months corrected age, there were 108 mothers and 108 ceptability). Examples of items from the Child Domain Mood
fathers with complete data. Most parents (76%) were European subscale include: (a) My child seems to cry or fuss more often
Canadian, with the next highest ethnicity South Asian (6%). Parents than most children; and (b) I feel that my child is very moody and
were either married (82%) or in a common law relationship (18%). The easily upset. Examples of items from the Child Domain Acceptability
mean gestational age of the late preterm infants was 354/7 weeks and subscale include: (a) My child looks a little different than I expected
ranged from 340/7-366/7 weeks; 57% were boys. and it bothers me at times; and (b) My child does not seem to learn as
On average, mothers were 32 years old (SD = 5.03 years; range 19–46 quickly as most children.
years). More than half (58%) had university education, and the majority were Parents responded on a 5-point scale from 1 (strongly agree) to 5
on parental leave, with less than 5% working outside the home. Canada has a (strongly disagree); items were summed to create total scores for the
government policy supporting paid maternity and parental leave for the first Parent and Child Domains, and each subscale within the domains.
year of a child’s life (https://www.canada.ca/en/services/benefits/ei/ei- Higher scores indicate greater parenting stress. PSI-3 Parent and Child
maternity-parental.html). The majority of mothers (94%) were the primary Domain, and subscale scores ≥ 85th percentile are considered in the
caregivers for their infants, with remaining infants cared for by relatives and/ clinical range. Cronbach’s α for the Parent Domain subscales range from
or friends. On average, fathers were 34 years old (SD = 5.90 years; range .92 to .93, and .90 to .91 for the Child Domain subscales (Abidin, 1995).
19–50 years). About half of the fathers (55%) had university education, and In our study, Cronbach’s α were .92 (Parent Domain) and .91 (Child
the majority (94%) were employed. Domain) for mothers, and .89 and .88, respectively for fathers. Test-
retest reliability ranges from .69 to .91 for the Parent Domain, and .55 to
.82 for the Child Domain (Abidin, 1995). The PSI-3 was written at a 5th
4.2 | Measurement
grade reading level and takes about 20 min to complete.

4.2.1 | Ages and Stages Questionnaires—second


edition (ASQ-2) 4.3 | Procedures
The ASQ-2 is a parent-reported developmental screener to capture Mothers and fathers of late preterm infants were recruited in hospital
risk of delay in children ages 4 to 60 months. The ASQ-2 addresses five at birth. Prior to any intervention, and when their infant was
domains: (a) Communication; (b) Gross Motor; (c) Fine Motor; (d) approaching 4 months corrected age, a questionnaire package was
Problem Solving; and (e) Personal-Social (Squires, Potter, & Bricker, mailed to the family. Mothers and fathers completed the PSI-3
1999). The ASQ-2 consists of 37 age-appropriate items, each separately; mothers completed the ASQ-2 for their infants because in
describing a skill, ability, or behavior to which a parent responds the majority of cases, mothers were the primary caregivers. During a
with three response categories: 10 (yes) to 5 (sometimes) to 0 (not yet). home visit by a research assistant, questionnaires were scanned for
Item scores are summed to create a raw score; a higher raw score completeness and returned to the university research office for data
indicates more optimal child development. Using the raw scores from a entry and cleaning.
4 | MUGHAL ET AL.

4.4 | Data analyses negatively correlated with ASQ-2 domain scores: (a) Reinforces
Parent with ASQ-2 Gross Motor and Personal-Social; (b) Demand-
Data were analyzed separately for mothers and fathers using SPSS
ingness with ASQ-2 Communication; (c) Mood with ASQ-2
version 22.0. The significance level was fixed at p < .05, except for
Communication and Personal-Social; and (d) Acceptability with all
correlation analyses, where the significance was set at p ≤ .01 to
ASQ-2 Domains, except Gross Motor (Table 3).
decrease chances of obtaining spurious results. We calculated
means and standard deviations for the PSI-3 and ASQ-2 scores,
along with frequencies and percentages of scores in the clinical 5.2 | Maternal PSI-3 subscale scores predicting infant
range. development
To compare PSI-3 Parent and Child Domain scores for mothers
We tested five 3-step hierarchical linear regression models to
and fathers, we used independent sample t-tests. To examine
determine predictors of the five ASQ-2 domain scores from among
relationships between ASQ-2 Domain scores and PSI-3 Parent and
the dimensions of parenting stress. In Step 1, we entered infant sex
Child Domain and subscale scores, we used Pearson product-moment
and gestational age as control variables. In Step 2, we added
correlations. To predict child development using PSI-3 scores, we
maternal education and age. In Step 3, we entered the PSI-3 Parent
conducted hierarchical regression models. The variables of infant sex,
Domain subscale scores and the PSI-3 Child Domain subscale
gestational age, mother’s age, and education were controlled for in our
scores that were significantly correlated (p ≤ .01) with ASQ-2
regression models, as they were associated with child development in
scores. All of the models were statistically significant: Communi-
the literature. Household income was not included as a control variable
cation, F (7, 107) = 7.77, p < .001; Gross Motor, F (6, 107) = 3.40,
due to our homogenous sample; 80.2% of participants had income
p = .004; Fine Motor, F (5, 107) = 4.18, p = .002; Problem Solving, F
levels of $80,000 or more.
(5, 107) = 3.29, p = .008; and Personal-Social, F (8, 107) = 5.19,
p < .001 (see Table 5).
5 | RE SULTS

Compared to fathers, mothers reported significantly greater stress on 5.2.1 | ASQ-2 Communication scores
the PSI-3 Parent Domain score, t(216) = 2.66, p = .008. There were no
In combination, infant sex, gestational age, and PSI-3 Child Mood
significant differences between mothers and fathers on any Child
and Acceptability subscale scores explained 31% of the variance in
Domain scores, t(216) = .82, p = .41. Descriptive statistics for the PSI-3
ASQ-2 Communication scores. In the final model, male sex and
Domain and subscale scores for mothers and fathers are presented in
increased gestational age were positively correlated and PSI-3 Mood
Table 1. For mothers, 6–24% of PSI-3 Parent Domain subscale scores
and ASQ-2 Acceptability subscale scores were negatively correlated
and 10–24% of PSI-3 Child Domain subscale scores fell in the clinical
with Communication scores. Mothers of male late preterm infants
range. For fathers, 1–14% of PSI-3 Parent Domain subscale scores and
with older gestational age and mothers who reported greater stress
9–23% of PSI-3 Child Domain subscale scores fell in the clinical range.
related to the infant’s mood (i.e., frequently crying or not content)
Means and standard deviations for the ASQ-2 domain raw scores,
and acceptability (i.e., not possessing physical, intellectual, or
and frequency and percentages of scores below the clinical cut-off are
emotional characteristics matching mothers’ expectations) also
presented in Table 2. Between 4.6% and 13.0% of infants scored below
reported indicators of greater risk of delay in communication
the clinical cut-off.
development.

5.1 | Correlations between parent and child domain


stress scores and infant development 5.2.2 | ASQ-2 Gross Motor
For mothers, there was a significant negative correlation between the The PSI-3 Child Domain Reinforces Parent subscale score alone
PSI-3 Parent Domain scores and infants’ ASQ-2 Personal-Social explained 12% of the variance in the ASQ-2 Gross Motor scores and
Domain scores. There were significant negative correlations between was negatively correlated with it. Mothers who reported greater stress
the PSI-3 Child Domain scores and infants’ ASQ-2 Communication and related to the infant’s reinforcement of her parenting (i.e., not
Personal-Social Domain scores (Table 3). For fathers, there were no experiencing positive interactions) reported indicators of greater risk
significant correlations between the PSI-3 Parent or Child Domain of delay in gross motor development.
scores and any ASQ-2 Domain scores (Table 4). Therefore, no further
analyses of fathers’ PSI-3 subscale scores predicting infant develop-
5.2.3 | ASQ-2 Fine Motor and Problem Solving
ment were conducted.
For mothers, two PSI-3 Parent Domain subscales were The PSI-3 Acceptability subscale score alone explained 13% and 10%
significantly negatively correlated with ASQ-2 domain scores: (a) of the variance in the ASQ-2 Fine Motor and Problem Solving scores,
Competence with ASQ-2 Personal-Social; and (b) Health with ASQ-2 respectively, and both were negatively correlated with stress of
Gross Motor. Four PSI-3 Child subscale scores were significantly acceptability. Mothers who reported greater stress related to her
MUGHAL ET AL.
| 5

TABLE 1 Parenting stress scores for mothers and fathers: Means, standard deviations, and frequency of scores in the clinical range in domains
and subscales
Mothers (n = 108) Fathers (n = 108)

Frequency (%) Frequency (%)


PSI-3 scores M (SD) in clinical range M (SD) in clinical range
Parent domain 114.84 (21.61) 8 (7.4) 107.64 (18.07) 2 (1.9)
Competence 25.11 (5.72) 6 (5.6) 25.23 (5.20) 3 (2.8)
Isolation 12.81 (3.89) 16 (14.8) 12.13 (3.57) 11 (10.2)
Attachment 11.56 (2.72) 9 (8.3) 12.50 (2.97) 15 (13.9)
Health 12.91 (3.19) 26 (24.1) 11.69 (2.70) 10 (9.3)
Role restriction 17.37 (4.13) 10 (9.3) 15.80 (4.07) 5 (4.6)
Depression 18.28 (5.61) 12 (11.1) 15.69 (4.05) 1 (0.9)
Spouse 16.81 (4.36) 18 (16.7) 14.61 (4.03) 6 (5.6)
Child domain 94.44 (17.55) 13 (12.0) 96.34 (16.41) 12 (11.1)
Distractibility 24.25 (4.01) 16 (14.8) 24.70 (3.40) 14 (13.0)
Adaptability 24.31 (5.44) 15 (13.9) 25.53 (5.42) 25 (23.1)
Reinforces parent parent 8.48 (2.41) 11 (10.2) 8.82 (2.67) 15 (13.9)
Demandingness 16.09 (4.74) 12 (11.1) 16.04 (4.28) 10 (9.3)
Mood 9.60 (3.16) 26 (24.1) 9.69 (2.92) 20 (18.5)
Acceptability 11.71 (3.19) 16 (14.8) 11.56 (3.06) 11 (10.2)

PSI-3, Parenting Stress Index-third edition.

infant’s acceptability reported greater risk of delay in fine motor and In summary, maternal PSI-3 subscale scores that were significant
problem solving development. predictors of infant development in the final models were from the
Child Domain of parenting stress. They were Reinforces Parent
(predicted Gross Motor development), Mood (predicted Communica-
5.2.4 | ASQ-2 personal-social tion development), and Acceptability (predicted Communication, Fine
Motor, Problem Solving, and Personal-Social development).
In combination, maternal age, education, and PSI-3 Acceptability
subscale scores explained 24% of the variance in the ASQ-2 Personal-
Social scores. In the final model, maternal age, and Acceptability scores
6 | DISCUSSION
were negatively correlated with ASQ-2 Personal-Social development
scores, and education was positively correlated. Older and less
In this study, mothers experienced greater parenting stress than
educated mothers who reported higher stress related to acceptability
fathers of late preterm infants at 4 months corrected age. Maternal
of their infant also reported indicators of greater risk of delay in
stress related to being a parent (Parent Domain) was significantly
personal-social development.
related to reports of children’s personal-social development. Similarly,
stress related to characteristics of the child (Child Domain) was
significantly related to reports of children’s communication and
TABLE 2 Child development of late preterm infants (N = 108) at
personal-social development. This result is in keeping with Bronfen-
4 months corrected age: means and standard deviations of ASQ-2
domain raw scores and frequency (%) of scores below the clinical brenner’s (1986) identification of stress within the family system as
cutoff contributing to poorer child outcomes. However, fathers’ stress was
Frequency (%) below not significantly related to children’s development in any domain.
ASQ-2 domain M (SD) clinical cutoff
Communication 46.71 (9.96) 10 (9.3)
Gross Motor 52.52 (9.16) 14 (13.0) 6.1 | Parenting stress of mothers and fathers
Fine Motor 47.81 (10.36) 5 (4.6)
Stress levels related to qualities of their child were similar for
Problem Solving 53.56 (7.97) 5 (4.6)
mothers and fathers of late preterm infants, which is not surprising
Personal-Social 50.01 (8.97) 5 (4.6)
given they were considering the same child. However, mothers
ASQ-2,Ages and Stages Questionnaires-second edition. reported significantly more stress than fathers in relationship to their
6 | MUGHAL ET AL.

TABLE 3 Pearson product-moment correlations between mothers’ parenting stress (PSI-3) and Child development (ASQ-2) domain raw scores
ASQ-2 domain scores

PSI-3 scores Communication Gross Motor Fine Motor Problem Solving Personal-Social
Parent domain −.23 −.16 −.09 −.08 −.25*
Competence −.24 −.17 −.03 −.07 −.28*
Isolation −.14 −.07 −.04 −.15 −.13
Attachment −.20 −.07 −.15 −.14 −.20
Health −.17 −.25* −.10 −.11 −.21
Role Restriction −.21 −.19 −.10 −.01 −.17
Depression −.13 −.07 −.06 −.04 −.18
Spouse −.07 .00 −.05 .05 −.11
Child domain −.34** −.13 −.24 −.14 −.28*
Distractibility −.13 .08 −.06 −.06 −.01
Adaptability −.10 −.06 −.18 −.09 −.10
Reinforces Parent −.19 −.30** −.21 −.12 −.35**
Demandingness −.30** −.12 −.14 −.01 −.21
Mood −.48** −.15 −.17 −.12 −.33**
Acceptability −.44** −.14 −.39** −.32** −.45**

PSI-3, Parenting Stress Index-third edition; ASQ-2, Ages and Stages Questionnaires-second edition.
*p ≤ .01
**p ≤ .001

parenting role. Using a short form of the PSI, fathers of late preterm less than mothers’. Tandberg, Sandtrø, Vårdal, & Rønnestad (2013)
infants at 6 months (gestational age 30–36 weeks) experienced reported different sources of stress for mothers and fathers of
significantly more stress than fathers of term infants (Ravn et al., preterm infants at the time of hospital discharge. According to
2012). However, for fathers in our study, Parent Domain stress was Bronfenbrenner (1977), although sources of environmental stress

TABLE 4 Pearson product-moment correlations between fathers’ parenting stress (PSI-3) and mother-reported child development (ASQ-2)
domain raw scores
ASQ-2 domain scores

PSI-3 Scores Communication Gross Motor Fine Motor Problem Solving Personal-Social
Parent domain −.06 −.18 −.11 −.01 −.22
Competence .04 −.01 .12 .15 .04
Isolation −.09 −.14 −.12 −.04 −.18
Attachment −.12 −.12 −.14 −.11 −.23
Health −.07 −.13 −.15 −.11 −.16
Role Restriction −.11 −.16 −.16 .04 −.20
Depression .02 −.12 −.05 .01 −.15
Spouse −.03 −.20 −.12 −.09 −.22
Child domain −.12 −.10 −.11 −.02 −.10
Distractibility .06 −.03 .04 .13 .15
Adaptability −.06 −.15 −.10 −.02 −.08
Reinforces Parent −.14 −.01 −.06 −.03 −.16
Demandingness −.10 −.06 −.10 .03 −.04
Mood −.27* −.10 −.09 −.07 −.19
Acceptability −.12 −.06 −.21 −.14 −.20

PSI-3, Parenting Stress Index-third edition; ASQ-2, Ages and Stages Questionnaires-second edition.
*p ≤ .01
MUGHAL ET AL.
| 7

TABLE 5 Hierarchical multiple regression models predicting infant development from infant and mother characteristics and parenting stress
subscales
Communication Gross Motor Fine Motor Problem-Solving Personal-Social

Predictor variables β Adj. R2


β Adj. R 2
β Adj. R 2
β Adj. R 2
β Adj. R2
Step 1 .08** .00 .00 .02 .00
Infant sex .17 .10 .05 −.11 .01
Gestational age .30** .13 .06 .16 .06
Step 2 .10** .00 .00 .02 .00
Infant sex .18 .09 .06 −.10 .00
Gestational age .31** .12 .05 .16 .04
Age (parent) .18 −.11 .00 .07 −.15
Education −.13 .11 .07 .06 .16
Step 3 .31*** .12** .13** .10** .24***
Infant sex .20* .10 .07 −.09 .04
Gestational age .23** .13 .00 .12 −.01
Age (parent) .08 −.14 −.04 .04 −.21*
Education −.05 .15 .12 .09 .24*
Competence – – – −.05
Health −.15 – – –
Reinforces Parent −.28** – – −.14
Demandingness .09 – – – –
Mood −.35** – – – −.11
Acceptability −.25* – −.41*** −.30** −.32**

Education, 1 = university level or above and 0 = below university level. Infant sex, 1 = male and 0 = female. The symbol (–) denotes the PSI-3 subscale was not
included in the model due to insignificant correlations at the bivariate level.
*p < .05
**p < .01
***p < .001

may differ (such as hospital or home), these interlinking environ- Higher parenting stress related to health was related to poorer
ments (such as workplace and/or social networks of the parent) can gross motor abilities. For mothers, increased stress related to health
affect children’s development. may stem from the physical demands of caring for a late preterm infant.
Late preterm infants may have disorganized sleep-awake cycles
(Barratt et al., 1992), and breast-feeding difficulties (Nagulesapillai
6.2 | Correlations between parent and child domain
et al., 2013). Indeed, 24% of the mothers rated stress related to their
stress and infant development
health in the clinical range; this may result in less energy available to
For mothers, higher stress related to parenting competence was foster their child’s development. Bronfenbrenner and Morris (2006)
correlated with greater risk of delay in personal-social development. A emphasized the importance of external support for new mothers such
lack of perceived competence would be expected for the first-time as relatives, neighbors, churches, and child programs with potential to
parents in our study for numerous reasons, including: (a) feeling that contribute to healthy families and children’s development.
some things were out of their control such as having a late preterm infant Higher parenting stress related to a lack of positive reinforcement
in NICU (Song, Kim, & Ahn, 2015): and (b) feeling that their parenting role from their infant was related to greater risk of delay in gross motor and
was not rewarding (Nakamura et al., 2015). For many first-time mothers, personal-social domains. Higher parenting stress related to mood, or
becoming a parent decreased their feelings of competence and perceptions of high need from their infant, including frequent crying,
increased their stress related to managing day-to-day problems and was related to greater risk of delay in infants’ communication and
coping with more challenges than they experienced before becoming personal-social development. Of note, 24% of mothers and 19% of
mothers (De Caroli & Sagone, 2014). First-time parents may be unaware fathers reported stress related to their infants’ mood that was in the
of how to foster personal-social skills in young infants. Increased clinical range. Positive experiences in early childhood, such as
parenting stress can decrease parenting competence in mothers, reciprocal or “serve and return” interactions with parents, family
whereas parental engagement and confidence increased when mothers members, and primary caregivers, builds healthy brain architecture
view their parenting positively (Berryhill, 2016). (Fox, Levitt, & Nelson, 2010). Yet for late preterm infants, these serve
8 | MUGHAL ET AL.

and return interactions can be difficult for parents when the child is relationships, and health than did fathers, who experienced higher levels
irritable and difficult to soothe. of stress in social isolation (Widarsson et al., 2013).
Higher parenting stress related to the acceptability of their infant, or Limitations of our study include the concurrent assessment of
the lack of alignment of the infant’s characteristics with their expectations, stress and development, as stress levels during earlier periods when
was associated with greater risk of delay in all ASQ-2 Domains except that development was happening were unavailable. Stress may have
Gross Motor. This mismatch of expectations and reality may stem from been much greater earlier in the infant’s life. Some models explained a
characteristics of late preterm infants who are smaller, may have health small proportion of the total variance, leaving a large proportion of the
problems initially, and acquire skills based upon their gestational age rather variance unexplained. Additional variables such as psychosocial factors
than the chronological age. Gray, Edwards, O’Callaghan, & Cuskelly (2012) (e.g., depression), perinatal complications, and breastfeeding rates may
identified greater parenting stress in mothers of preterm compared to have increased the amount of variance explained if available, but we
mothers of term infants, especially in the parent-child dysfunctional could not have included them in the model because of the small
interaction subscale of the PSI-Short Form, a subscale similar to the PSI-3 sample. This sample was highly educated, with high income, and mostly
Acceptability subscale (Abidin, 1995). English-speaking, therefore caution should be taken when generalizing
results to other samples.

6.3 | Maternal subscale scores predicting infant


development 7 | IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE AND
FU TU RE RESEARC H
We investigated the relationships between demographic character-
istics and PSI-3 subscales on child development. For infants,
Experiences of stress differed for mothers and fathers. Therefore,
gestational age was a significant predictor of communication
we need to tailor supports accordingly. Parenting support and
development. This finding is consistent with studies showing an
education, including strategies for stress management, should be
inverse relationship of gestational age with infants’ developmental
offered in-hospital, at the first community health nurse home visit,
outcomes (Kerstjens et al., 2012). Male sex was also a significant
and in post-natal parenting classes for parents of late preterm
predictor of increased risk of delay in communication development. In
infants. It is essential to educate new mothers and fathers about
a similar study (Patra, Greene, Patel, & Meier, 2016), lower language
realistic expectations of parenting a late preterm infant. Late preterm
scores were found in male infants up to the age of 20 months.
infant behaviors include more frequent crying, decreased parent-
For mothers, higher levels of education predicted decreased risk of
child interaction and bonding, and infants not meeting expectations
delay only in personal-social development. Patra et al. (2016) reported
(such as physical or emotional characteristics) in comparison to a
that higher maternal education was the best predictor of positive child
term infant. We recommend further research using longitudinal
development. In our study, older maternal age predicted increased risk
designs and observational measures of infant development and to
of delay in personal-social development. Bornstein et al. (2006) reported
examine, with larger samples, the effects of dimensions of parenting
that older mothers were more perceptive of their term infants’ behaviors
stress on late preterm infant development.
than younger mothers, perceived their infants as more difficult, and/or
had more difficulty adjusting to motherhood. Support from extended
family decreased with increasing maternal age, but older mothers
REFERENCES
communicated more with their infants than younger mothers (Bornstein
Abidin, R. R. (1995). Parenting Stress Index: Professional manual (3rd. ed.).
et al., 2006). In contrast, a study of Australian women in a metropolitan
Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
area, reported that maternal psychological maturity, increased with age, Ananth, C. V., Friedman, A. M., & Gyamfi-Bannerman, C. (2013).
and was associated with positive adaptation to parenthood in the early Epidemiology of moderate preterm, late preterm and early term
months (Camberis, McMahon, Gibson, & Boivin, 2014). delivery. Clinics in Perinatology, 40, 601–610. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.clp.2013.07.001
It is important to consider why fathers’ perceptions of parenting
Ashiabi, G. S., & O’Neal, K. K. (2015). Child social development in context: An
stress were not significantly associated with infant development for this examination of some propositions in Bronfenbrenner’s bioecological theory.
sample of late preterm infants. One explanation is that mothers are often Sage Open, 5(2), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244015590840
the primary caregivers for young infants and fathers’ perceptions may Baron, I. S., Litman, F. R., Ahronovich, M. D., & Baker, R. (2012). Late
preterm birth: A review of medical and neuropsychological childhood
have more indirect relationships to infant development at 4 months
outcomes. Neuropsychology Review, 22, 438–450. https://doi.org/
corrected age. Bronfenbrenner, Alvarez, & Henderson (1984) referred to 10.1007/s11065-012-9210-5
different perceptions of a child related to work status of each parent Barratt, M. S., Roach, M. A., & Leavitt, L. A. (1992). Early channels of mother-
within the family system. If fathers were actively engaged as their child’s infant communication: Preterm and term infants. Journal of Child
primary caregivers, their perceptions of stress and subsequent infant Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 1193–1204. https://doi.org/10.1111/
j.1469-7610.1992.tb00938.x
development may have been different, as found in a previous study
Benzies, K. M., Harrison, M. J., & Magill-Evans, J. (2004). Parenting stress, marital
(Benzies et al., 2004). In a Swedish study of early parenting stress, mothers quality and child behavior problems at age 7 years. Public Health Nursing, 21,
experienced higher levels of stress in competence, role restriction, spousal 111–121. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0737-1209. 2004.021204.x
MUGHAL ET AL.
| 9

Benzies, K. M., Magill-Evans, J., Kurilova, J., Nettel-Aguirre, A., Blahitka, L., Engle, W. A. (2006). A recommendation for the definition of “late preterm” (near-
& Lacaze-Masmonteil, T. (2013). Effects of video-modeling on the term) and the birth weight-gestational age classification system. Seminars in
interaction skills of first-time fathers of late preterm infants. Infants & Perinatology, 30, 2–7. https://doi.org/10.1053/j.semperi.2006.01.007
Young Children, 26, 333–348. https://doi.org/10.1097/IYC.0b01 Engle, W. A., Tomashek, K. M., Wallman, C., & Committee on Fetus and
3e3182a4ed5e Newborn (2007). “Late preterm” infants: A population at risk. Pediatrics,
Berryhill, M. B. (2016). Mothers’ parenting stress and engagement: The 120, 1390–1401. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2007-2952
mediating role of parental competence. Marriage & Family Review, 52, Fox, S. E., Levitt, P., & Nelson, C. A. (2010). How the timing and quality of early
461–480. https://doi.org/10.1080/01494929.2015.1113600 experiences influence the development of brain architecture. Child Develop-
Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Suwalsky, J. T., & Gini, M. (2006). Maternal ment, 81, 28–40. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01380.x
chronological age, prenatal and perinatal history, social support, and Gray, P. H., Edwards, D. M., O’Callaghan, M. J., & Cuskelly, M. (2012).
parenting of infants. Child Development, 77, 875–892. https://doi.org/ Parenting stress in mothers of preterm infants during early infancy. Early
10.1111/j. 1467-8624. 2006.00908.x Human Development, 88, 45–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Boyle, C. A., Boulet, S., Schieve, L. A., Cohen, R. A., Blumberg, S. J., Yeargin- earlhumdev.2011.06.014
Allsopp, M., . . . Kogan, M. D. (2011). Trends in the prevalence of Hildingsson, I., & Thomas, J. (2014). Parental stress in mothers and fathers
developmental disabilities in US children, 1997–2008. Pediatrics, 127, one year after birth. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 32,
1034–1042. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-2989 41–56. https://doi.org/10.1080/02646838.2013.840882
Brandon, D. H., Tully, K. P., Silva, S. G., Malcolm, W. F., Murtha, A. P., Turner, Hostinar, C. E., & Gunnar, M. R. (2015). Social support can buffer against stress
B. S., & Holditch-Davis, D. (2011). Emotional responses of mothers of and shape brain activity. The American Journal of Bioethics Neuroscience,
late-preterm and term infants. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, & 6(3), 34–42. https://doi.org/10.1080/21507740.2015.1047054
Neonatal Nursing, 40, 719–731. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1552– Kerstjens, J. M., de Winter, A. F., Bocca-Tjeertes, I. F., Bos, A. F., &
6909.2011.01290.x Reijneveld, S. A. (2012). Risk of developmental delay increases
Brett, J., Staniszewska, S., Newburn, M., Jones, N., & Taylor, L. (2011). exponentially as gestational age of preterm infants decreases: A cohort
A systematic mapping review of effective interventions for communi- study at age 4 years. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 54,
cating with, supporting and providing information to parents of preterm 1096–1101. https://doi.org/10.1111/j. 1469-8749. 2012.04423.x
infants. BMJ Open, 1, e000023. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen- Kim, P., Mayes, L., Feldman, R., Leckman, J. F., & Swain, J. E. (2013). Early
2010-000023. postpartum parental preoccupation and positive parenting thoughts:
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Lewinian space and ecological substance. Relationship with parent-infant interaction. Infant Mental Health
Journal of Social Issues, 33(4), 199–212. https://doi.org/10.1111/j. Journal, 34, 104–116. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21359
1540-4560. 1977.tb02533.x Landsem, I. P., Handegard, B. H., Tunby, J., Ulvund, S. E., & Ronning, J. A.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human (2014). Early intervention program reduces stress in parents of
development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22, preterms during childhood: A randomized controlled trial. Trials, 15,
723–742. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.22.6.723 387. https://doi.org/10.1186/1745-6215-15-387
Bronfenbrenner, U. (2005). Making human beings human: Bioecological Ludwig, S. M. (2007). Oral feeding and the late preterm infant. Newborn and Infant
perspectives on human development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Nursing Reviews, 7(2), 72–75. https://doi.org/10.1053/j.nainr.2007.05.005.
Bronfenbrenner, U., Alvarez, W. F., & Henderson, C. R., Jr (1984). Working McGowan, J., Alderdice, F., Holmes, V., & Johnston, L. (2011). Early
and watching: Maternal employment status and parents’ perceptions of childhood development of late-preterm infants: A systematic review.
their three-year-old children. Child Development, 55, 1362–1378. Pediatrics, 127, 1111–1124. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-
https://doi.org/10.2307/1130006 2257
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of Muller-Nix, C., Forcada-Guex, M., Pierrehumbert, B., Jaunin, L., Borghini, A.,
human development. In W. Damon, R. M. Lerner & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), & Ansermet, F. (2004). Prematurity, maternal stress and mother-child
Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical models of human development interactions. Early Human Development, 79, 145–158. https://doi.org/
(6th ed., Vol. I, pp. 793–828). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.05.002
Camberis, A.-L., McMahon, C. A., Gibson, F. L., & Boivin, J. (2014). Munakata, S., Okada, T., Okahashi, A., Yoshikawa, K., Usukura, Y.,
Age, psychological maturity, and the transition to motherhood Makimoto, M., . . . Okuhata, Y. (2013). Gray matter volumetric MRI
among English-speaking Australian women in a metropolitan area. differences late-preterm and term infants. Brain and Development, 35,
Developmental Psychology, 50, 2154. https://doi.org/10.1037/a00 10–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2011.12.011
37301 Nagulesapillai, T., McDonald, S. W., Fenton, T. R., Mercader, H. F. G., &
Coletti, M. F., Caravale, B., Gasparini, C., Franco, F., Campi, F., & Dotta, A. Tough, S. C. (2013). Breastfeeding difficulties and exclusivity among late
(2015). One-year neurodevelopmental outcome of very and late preterm and term infants: results from the All Our Babies study.
preterm infants: Risk factors and correlation with maternal stress. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 104, e351–e356.
Infant Behavior Development, 39, 11–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Nakamura, Y., Takeishi, Y., Ito, N., Ito, M., Atogami, F., & Yoshizawa, T.
infbeh.2015.01.003 (2015). Comfort with motherhood in late pregnancy facilitates maternal
De Caroli, M. E., & Sagone, E. (2014). The experience of pre-and post- role attainment in early postpartum. The Tohoku Journal of Experimental
partum in relation to support, stress, and parenthood representation. Medicine, 235(1), 53–59. https://doi.org/10.1620/tjem.235.53
Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 116, 697–702. https://doi.org/ Neece, C. L., Green, S. A., & Baker, B. L. (2012). Parenting stress and child
10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.01.282 behavior problems: A transactional relationship across time. American
Dix, T., & Yan, N. (2014). Mothers’ depressive symptoms and infant Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 117, 48–66.
negative emotionality in the prediction of child adjustment at age 3: https://doi.org/10.1352/1944-7558-117. 1.48
Testing the maternal reactivity and child vulnerability hypotheses. Patra, K., Greene, M. M., Patel, A. L., & Meier, P. (2016). Maternal education
Development and Psychopathology, 26, 111–124. https://doi.org/ level predicts cognitive, language, and motor outcome in preterm
10.1017/s0954579413000898 infants in the second year of life. American Journal of Perinatology, 33,
Dunkel Schetter, C., & Tanner, L. (2012). Anxiety, depression and stress in 738–744. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0036-1572532
pregnancy: Implications for mothers, children, research, and practice. Potijk, M. R., de Winter, A. F., Bos, A. F., Kerstjens, J. M., & Reijneveld, S. A.
Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 25, 141–148. https://doi.org/10.1097/ (2015). Co-occurrence of developmental and behavioural problems in
YCO.0b013e3283503680 moderate to late preterm-born children. Archives of Disease in
10 | MUGHAL ET AL.

Childhood, 101, 217–222. https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2015- Squires, J., Potter, L., & Bricker, D. D. (1999). The ASQ user’s guide: A parent-
308958 completed, child-monitoring system (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Public Health Agency of Canada. (2013). Perinatal health indicators for Sutcliffe, A. G., Barnes, J., Belsky, J., Gardiner, J., & Melhuish, E. (2012). The
Canada 2013: A report from the Canadian Perinatal Surveillance System. health and development of children born to older mothers in the United
Ottawa, ON: Public Health Agency of Canada, Government of Canada. Kingdom: Observational study using longitudinal cohort data. British
Raby, K. L., Lawler, J. M., Shlafer, R. J., Hesemeyer, P. S., Collins, W. A., & Medical Journal, 345, e5116. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.e5116
Sroufe, L. A. (2015). The interpersonal antecedents of supportive Tandberg, B. S., Sandtrø, H. P., Vårdal, M., & Rønnestad, A. (2013). Parents
parenting: A prospective, longitudinal study from infancy to adulthood. of preterm evaluation of stress and nursing support. Journal of Neonatal
Developmental Psychology, 51, 115–123. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Nursing, 19, 317–326. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jnn.2013.01.008
a0038336 Teune, M. J., Bakhuizen, S., Gyamfi Bannerman, C., Opmeer, B. C., van Kaam, A. H.,
Ravn, I. H., Lindemann, R., Smeby, N. A., Bunch, E. H., Sandvik, L., & Smith, L. van Wassenaer, A. G., . . . Mol, B. W. J. (2011). A systematic review of severe
(2012). Stress in fathers of moderately and late preterm infants: A morbidity in infants born late preterm. American Journal of Obstetrics and
randomised controlled trial. Early Child Development and Care, 182, Gynecology, 205, 374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2011.07.015
537–552. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2011.564279 Tharner, A., Luijk, M. P., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg,
Rose, R., & Engle, W. A. (2017). Optimizing care and outcomes for late M. J., Jaddoe, V. W., Hofman, A., . . . Tiemeier, H. (2012). Infant
preterm neonates. Current Treatment Options in Pediatrics, 3(1), 32–43. attachment, parenting stress, and child emotional and behavioral
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40746-017-0074-z problems at age 3 years. Parenting, 12, 261–281. https://doi.org/
Rutherford, H. J. V., Goldberg, B., Luyten, P., Bridgett, D. J., & Mayes, L. C. 10.1080/15295192.2012.709150
(2013). Parental reflective functioning is associated with tolerance of Treyvaud, K. (2014). Parent and family outcomes following very preterm or
infant distress but not general distress: Evidence for a specific very low birth weight birth: A review. Seminars in Fetal and Neonatal
relationship using a simulated baby paradigm. Infant Behavior and Medicine, 19, 131–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2013.10.008
Development, 36, 635–641. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh. Treyvaud, K., Doyle, L. W., Lee, K. J., Roberts, G., Cheong, J. L., Inder, T. E., &
2013.06.008 Anderson, P. J. (2011). Family functioning, burden and parenting stress
Sahni, R., & Polin, R. A. (2013). Physiologic underpinnings for clinical 2 years after very preterm birth. Early Human Development, 87,
problems in moderately preterm and late preterm infants. Clinics in 427–431. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.03.008
Perinatology, 40, 645–663. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clp.2013.07.012 Widarsson, M., Engström, G., Rosenblad, A., Kerstis, B., Edlund, B., &
Samra, H. A., McGrath, J. M., & Wehbe, M. (2011). An integrated review of Lundberg, P. (2013). Parental stress in early parenthood among mothers
developmental outcomes and late-preterm birth. Journal of Obstetric, and fathers in Sweden. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 27,
Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing, 40, 399–411. https://doi.org/ 839–847. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6712.2012.01088.x
10.1111/j.1552–6909.2011.01270.x Wigfall, V. G., Brannen, J., Mooney, A., & Parutis, V. (2013). Finding the right
Singer, L. T., Salvator, A., Guo, S., Collin, M., Lilien, L., & Baley, J. (1999). man: Recruiting fathers in inter-generational families across ethnic
Maternal psychological distress and parenting stress after the birth of a groups. Qualitative Research, 13, 591–607. https://doi.org/10.1177/
very low-birth-weight infant. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical 1468794112446109
Association, 281, 799–805. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.281.9.799 Wong, J. J., Roubinov, D. S., Gonzales, N. A., Dumka, L. E., & Millsap, R. E.
Slagt, M., Deković, M., de Haan, A. D., van den Akker, A. L., & Prinzie, P. (2013). Father enrollment and participation in a parenting intervention:
(2012). Longitudinal associations between mothers’ and fathers’ sense Personal and contextual predictors. Family Process, 52, 440–454.
of competence and children’s externalizing problems: The mediating https://doi.org/10.1111/famp.12024
role of parenting. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1554–1562. https://
doi.org/10.1037/a0027719
Song, J. E., Kim, T., & Ahn, J. A. (2015). A systematic review of psychosocial
interventions for women with postpartum stress. Journal of Obstetric, How to cite this article: Mughal MK, Ginn CS, Magill-Evans
Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing, 44, 183–192. https://doi.org/ J, Benzies KM. Parenting stress and development of late
10.1111/1552-6909.12541
preterm infants at 4 months corrected age. Res Nurs Health.
Spinelli, M., Poehlmann, J., & Bolt, D. (2013). Predictors of parenting stress
trajectories in premature infant-mother dyads. Journal of Family
2017;1–10. https://doi.org/10.1002/nur.21809
Psychology, 27, 873–883. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034652

View publication stats

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi