Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 11

School Psychology Quarterly

Contribution of Schools to Mental Health and Resilience


in Recently Immigrated Youth
Amanda Venta, Cassandra Bailey, Carla Muñoz, Estrella Godinez, Yessica Colin, Aleyda Arreola,
Anna Abate, Joshua Camins, Monico Rivas, and Sally Lawlace
Online First Publication, July 30, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/spq0000271

CITATION
Venta, A., Bailey, C., Muñoz, C., Godinez, E., Colin, Y., Arreola, A., Abate, A., Camins, J., Rivas, M., &
Lawlace, S. (2018, July 30). Contribution of Schools to Mental Health and Resilience in Recently
Immigrated Youth . School Psychology Quarterly. Advance online publication.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/spq0000271
School Psychology Quarterly
© 2018 American Psychological Association 2018, Vol. 1, No. 999, 000
1045-3830/18/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/spq0000271

Contribution of Schools to Mental Health and Resilience in Recently


Immigrated Youth

Amanda Venta, Cassandra Bailey, Carla Muñoz, Monico Rivas and Sally Lawlace
Estrella Godinez, Yessica Colin, Aleyda Arreola, Liberty High School, Houston, Texas
Anna Abate, and Joshua Camins
Sam Houston State University
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Given the high risk of psychopathology among recently immigrated Central American adolescents, the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

aim of this study was to examine several putative protective factors: parental attachment, peer attach-
ment, and school engagement. Based on prior research with other immigrant groups, parental and peer
attachment were expected to correlate with reduced mental health problems, increased prosocial behav-
ior, and increased resilience. However, the current study sought to add to existing data regarding putative
protective factors by testing the incremental contribution of school engagement over and above existing
support from parents and peers. The present study included 78 recently immigrated adolescents from
Central America who were enrolled at a public high school for recent immigrants. Findings revealed that
school engagement made a significant, positive contribution to mental health and resilience for youth
above and beyond the effects of parental and peer attachment. Specifically, school engagement (i.e.,
subscales Behavioral Engagement, Emotional Disaffection, and Active Behavioral Disaffection)
uniquely contributed to models predicting externalizing psychopathology, prosocial behavior, and
resilience. In sum, the findings of this study preliminarily suggest that fostering school engagement may
have protective effects for recently immigrated youth above and beyond traditional (i.e., peer, family)
supports.

Impact and Implications


The present study included recently immigrated adolescents from Central America, and results
suggested that for this highly vulnerable group, school engagement was related to reduced external-
izing mental health problems and higher prosocial behavior and resilience above and beyond other
forms of social support (e.g., parent, peer attachment). Findings suggest that school engagement may
be an important avenue for future research and intervention given high risk for mental health
problems in this immigrant group and the loss of other forms of social support that often accompany
migration.

Keywords: immigrant, adolescent, school, engagement, attachment

Public debates have surged regarding “exponential growth” in the United States (Rosenblum, 2015), an increase that has been
the population of recently immigrated adolescents from Central called a “humanitarian emergency,” with an estimated 131% in-
America—particularly El Salvador, Guatemala, and Honduras—in crease between 2015 and 2016 (United States Customs and Border
Protection, 2016a). Despite calls to provide mental health care for
high-risk immigrant adolescents and enhance societal supports
(Raphel, 2014), there is a gap between this area of public debate
Amanda Venta, Cassandra Bailey, Carla Muñoz, Estrella Godinez, Yes- and empirical research. Thus, the broad aim of this study was to
sica Colin, Aleyda Arreola, Anna Abate, and Joshua Camins, Department examine the effect of putative protective factors, including parental
of Psychology and Philosophy, Sam Houston State University; Monico attachment, peer attachment, and school engagement, on psycho-
Rivas and Sally Lawlace, Liberty High School, Houston Independent pathology, prosocial behavior, and resilience among Central
School District, Houston, Texas. American minors. The data analytic methods were specifically
This study was funded by Sam Houston State University FAST Grant designed to test whether school engagement makes an incremental
and Enhancement Research Grant and the American Psychological Asso-
contribution to these outcome measures above and beyond other
ciation of Graduate Students.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Amanda forms of social support (i.e., peer and parental attachment).
Venta, Department of Psychology and Philosophy, Sam Houston State This study seeks to address gaps in existing research with
University, Campus Box 2447, Huntsville, TX 77341. E-mail: aventa@ recently immigrated adolescent groups. First, much of the pub-
shsu.edu lished research on immigrant mental health predates unprece-

1
2 VENTA ET AL.

dented increases in violence in Central America and therefore does households are likely to live in areas where 41% of their peers are
not necessarily represent the mental health needs of more recent also from immigrant households, indicating that there are districts
waves of migrants. Increasing violence in Central America is disproportionately attending to the needs of immigrant youth (Car-
recent, with Honduras reporting the highest homicide rate globally amota et al., 2017). Still, native students are likely to attend a
in 2011; 2013 marking the end of a truce between major gang school in which 17% of students are from immigrant backgrounds,
powers in El Salvador; and crime victimization cited as a major suggesting that even school districts in areas that are not densely
reason for Central American migration in 2014 (Hiskey, Córdova, populated by immigrants must address the needs of this demo-
Orcés, & Malone, 2016). Dramatic increases in rates of violence in graphic group (Caramota et al., 2017).
Central America have corresponded to large increases in reports of Investigating the putative protective role of school engagement
psychopathology among recent waves of Central American immi- in this context has important implications. First, any school-based
grants (United States Conference on Catholic Bishops, 2014). supports do not rely on immigrant families as the target of inter-
Second, Central American immigrants are distinct from many vention and therefore bypass many previously identified barriers to
immigrant groups (e.g., Cubans) who have been the subject of intervention like parental hesitation to engage with school person-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

prior research because of both their exposure to violence and, nel; parental language, time, or financial barriers; and parental lack
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

often, undocumented immigration status. Critically, rampant re- of knowledge regarding U.S. academic norms (C. Suárez-Orozco
gional gang and cartel violence in Central America is not recog- et al., 2010). Second, if school engagement can act as a buffer to
nized by current immigration policies as representing persecution mental health problems, it can exert this effect even among youth
or warfare, and adolescents being targeted for gang recruitment or who experience familial separation—which affects roughly 85%
sexual exploitation are not necessarily granted refugee or asylum of recently immigrated Hispanic youth (C. Suárez-Orozco, Todo-
relief from deportation in the United States. Thus, Central Amer- rova, & Louie, 2002) and has been identified as a risk factor for
ican adolescents in the United States face the unique challenges of psychopathology. Third, school engagement among students in the
undocumented status and related discrimination (Potochnick & U.S. public education system can be bolstered by existing staff
Perreira, 2010), making them distinct from many of the displaced (i.e., teachers) with a vested interest in student success rather than
youth included in existing research who received special refugee necessitating funds for hiring mental health professionals. Because
legal and social protections (Fazel, Reed, Panter-Brick, & Stein, immigrant students are bound to attendance by immigration and
2012; Reed, Fazel, Jones, Panter-Brick, & Stein, 2012). visa-related stipulations, schools are a critical avenue to consider
The National Institutes of Health (2011) has noted particular in mitigating risk for mental health problems.
vulnerability among undocumented Hispanic adolescents. Bolster- The aim of this study was to examine the incremental contribu-
ing the view that these immigrant youth will be vulnerable to tion of school engagement, over and above parental and peer
mental health problems is a broader literature indicating that attachment, in relation to psychopathology, prosocial behavior,
immigrants to the United States from countries in which regional and resilience among recently immigrated adolescents from Cen-
violence and economic hardship are present experience high rates tral America. In the approach of this study, we are primarily
of severe emotional and behavioral disturbances (Perreira & Or- guided by Reed and colleagues’ (2012) “conceptual framework to
nelas, 2013; Potochnick & Perreira, 2010). Together, this research understand the ecological and chronological determinants of men-
suggests that Central American adolescents are unlike other im- tal health in forcibly displaced children” (p. 258; see also Fazel et
migrant groups, who paradoxically experience diminished risk of al., 2012). This ecological systems theory model (Bronfenbrenner,
psychopathology, and are instead at high risk (Pair Project, n.d.). 1979) was empirically derived based on research with more than
The influx of adolescents from Central America has dramati- 11,500 youth immigrants, though not one single study focused on
cally increased the number of recently immigrated Hispanic youth recently immigrated youth from Central America. Specifically,
in American public schools, and, based on data suggesting very three of the most well-established protective factors identified by
high rates of psychopathology in this group, schools have become Reed and colleagues (Fazel et al., 2012; Reed et al., 2012) were
key players in their well-being (Chiu, Pong, Mori, & Chow, 2012). selected as candidate protective factors for examination: family
Recently immigrated youth face unique stressors in adapting to support, peer support, and school engagement. Second, the current
and succeeding in traditional secondary schools, which often fail to study utilizes attachment theory, which posits that in early devel-
meet their developmental, pragmatic, and acculturative needs (e.g., opment, the emotional and physical needs of a child, and whether
C. Suárez-Orozco, Onaga, & de Lardemelle, 2010; M. Suárez- or not they are met, inform the development of internal working
Orozco, 2001) due partially to the uniqueness of their circum- models of the self and others that subsequently underlie self-
stances and partially to a broader lack of resources in immigrant reliance and social competence (Bowlby, 1969, 1973). Attachment
communities. Because of the large number of immigrants in sec- theory is used as a broad framework acknowledging that parents
ondary schools and society, the academic trajectory of immigrant play a critical role in mental health, prosocial behavior, and resil-
youth has substantial implications not only for the immigrant ience across the life span, with attachment to peers of growing
community but also for the broader host community as well importance across adolescence (Venta & Sharp, 2015; Venta,
(Masten, Liebkind, & Hernandez, 2012; Motti-Stefanidi & Mas- Shmueli-Goetz, & Sharp, 2014). Finally, the three potential pro-
ten, 2013). Indeed, the Center for Immigration Studies, utilizing tective factors examined in this study fall within National Institute
Census Bureau Data, indicates that, in 2015, one in four public of Mental Health’s (2018) Research Domain Criteria Systems for
school students came from an immigrant household, with even Social Processes and thus warrant interpretation through a social
higher concentrations (i.e., two thirds of students) in parts of capital lens. From this perspective, migration is viewed as leading
Florida, New York, Texas, California, Virginia, and New Jersey to deficits in social capital (e.g., number and quality of social
(Caramota, Griffith, & Zeigler, 2017). Students from immigrant connections), and thus maintenance or enhancement of social
PROTECTIVE EFFECTS OF SCHOOLS FOR IMMIGRANT TEENS 3

relationships has a protective effect, increasing postmigration pos- parent and 53% from another relative like an aunt, uncle, or
itive outcomes (Garcia-Reid, Peterson, & Reid, 2015; C. Suárez- sibling). Of these, 19 did not provide their assent following an
Orozco et al., 2010). In other words, migration interferes with explanation of study procedures, 13 were immigrants from non-
typical adolescent social connections, which places youth at risk Central American countries, and eight revoked consent. Thus, the
for psychopathology after migration. Thus, students with higher present analyses included 78 recently immigrated high school
levels of school engagement postmigration will theoretically ex- students (i.e., freshman to senior) from Central America. The
perience reduced mental health risk related to the compensatory average age of the youth was 19 years (SD ⫽ 2). Participants
social capital role of school relationships. self-reported demographics—59.9% of participants were male,
In sum, the aim of this study was to test whether school with the racial breakdown as follows: 26.9% White, 6.4% Black,
engagement, defined as student perceptions of connection with 6.1% mixed race, and 61.5% marked “other” (e.g., indigenous,
their school, can buffer the gap migration causes in typical forms Moreno/a, Guatemalan, Salvadorian) or chose not to answer. Re-
of social support (i.e., parent and peer attachment). It was hypoth- garding sexual orientation, 91% indicated that they were hetero-
esized that in accordance with ample prior research following from sexual, 2.6% homosexual, 2.6% bisexual, and 3.8% selected
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

attachment theory, higher parent and peer attachment would be “other.” Students’ current academic performance was as follows:
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

associated with reduced psychopathology, increased prosocial be- 16.4% mostly As, 47.9% As and Bs, 12.3% mostly Bs, 15.1% Bs
havior, and increased resilience. Further, we expected that school and Cs, 2.7% mostly Cs, 1.4% mostly Ds, and 4.1% marked “don’t
engagement would make a significant, incremental contribution to know.”
mental health above and beyond these other forms of social sup-
port. Internalizing and externalizing forms of psychopathology
Measures
were examined as outcomes in light of aforementioned research
documenting high, transdiagnostic risk for mental health problems Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA; Gallarin
in recently immigrated youth. The same predictors (i.e., parent & Alonso-Arbiol, 2013). The IPPA was used as both a measure
attachment, peer attachment, and school engagement) were also of parent and peer attachment. An existing Spanish translation,
examined in relation to prosocial behavior and resilience out- previously published by Gallarin and Alonso-Arbiol (2013), was
comes. It is well documented that trait resilience measures predict administered individually to adolescents who selected whether
mental health adaptation following adversity (e.g., Hunter & Chan- they would rather read the items independently or have them read
dler, 1999); likewise, prosocial behavior has been causally linked aloud. Virtually all adolescents selected to have the measure read
to peer acceptance, well-being, and academic achievement (Ca- aloud. The IPPA contains 25 items (each) to assess attachment to
prara, Barbaranelli, Pastorelli, Bandura, & Zimbardo, 2000; Lay- mother, father, and peer attachment. Items are scored on a 5-point
ous, Nelson, Oberle, Schonert-Reichl, & Lyubomirsky, 2012). In Likert scale (ranging from 1 ⫽ almost never or never true to 5 ⫽
this context, identifying potential predictors of resilience and almost always or always true), with high scores indicating higher
prosocial behavior in recently immigrated teens has substantial attachment. Each set of 25 items yields a total score and three
implications for their broader adaptation following migration. subscales: Communication, Alienation, and Trust. Sample items
drawn from the Trust scale include “My father/mother respects my
Method feelings” and “My friends accept me as I am.” From the Commu-
nication scale, sample items are “My father/mother listens to what
Participants I have to say” and “I share my thoughts and feelings with my
friends.” Lastly, from the Alienation scale, sample items include “I
Data for this study represent the first wave of data collection in feel angry with my father/mother” and “I wish I had more friends.”
an ongoing, longitudinal study. Participants were recruited from a The IPPA has demonstrated adequate reliability and validity in
public high school specifically for recently immigrated adolescents Spanish (Gallarin & Alonso-Arbiol, 2013). According to the IPPA
in the Southwestern United States. Data from the school indicate instructions and mirroring prior psychometric studies, adolescents
that the student body is primarily Hispanic (92%) and serves were instructed to answer parental attachment items about their
Grades 9 to 12. The modal number of self-reported years spent mother and father, who may not necessarily be their current or
living in the United States was approximately 2 years at the time temporary guardians. In the current study, internal consistency was
of data collection. The most frequent countries of origin were adequate with Cronbach’s alphas as follows: .79 for mother, .85
reported as El Salvador, Honduras, and Guatemala. The over- for father, and .97 for peer.
whelming majority of students are English Language Learners Engagement versus Disaffection with Learning Scale (Lloyd,
(93%) and receive Free and Reduced Price Lunch (78%); a mi- 2014). The Engagement versus Disaffection with Learning Scale
nority of students earn satisfactory marks on the annual reading was used as a self-report measure of school engagement. The
(STAAR, 5.6% in 2015) and math (STAAR, 50% in 2015) bench- existing Spanish translation was utilized, and adolescents largely
mark exams. Together, these aspects of the school profile indicate elected to have the 33-item measure read aloud to them. Items are
that it is a representative sample of recently immigrated youth in scored on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from not at all true to very
the Southwestern United States, who are predominantly Central true. This questionnaire contains subscales assessing (a) behav-
American and report limited English proficiency, socioeconomic ioral engagement, (b) emotional engagement, (c) behavioral dis-
disadvantage, and serious academic difficulty. affection, (d) emotional disaffection, and (e) active behavioral
All students at the school (approximate enrollment ⫽ 300) were disaffection. Behavioral engagement is defined as behaviors that
invited to participate in the study. Of these, 118 returned signed facilitate learning (e.g., “In class, I work as hard as I can”); the
informed consent provided by their legal guardian (47% from a direct opposite is behavioral disaffection (“In class, I do just
4 VENTA ET AL.

enough to get by”; Lloyd, 2014). Active behavioral disaffection is (i.e., disagree to agree). The English version of the RS has
defined as behavior that is disruptive or restless during learning demonstrated adequate internal consistency, concurrent validity,
(e.g., “When I’m in class, I talk a lot with my classmates”; Lloyd, and construct validity (Wagnild & Young, 1993). Sample items
2014). Emotional engagement is defined as feeling positive emo- include “I usually take things in stride,” “I can get through difficult
tional states during learning (e.g., “When we work on something in times because I’ve experienced difficulty before,” and “I can
class, I feel interested”), whereas emotional disaffection is defined usually look at a situation in a number of ways.” The measure has
as the opposite (e.g., “When I’m doing work in class, I feel been utilized in several studies with adolescents (Black & Ford-
bored”). This measure was initially developed by Skinner, Kin- Gilboe, 2004; Hunter & Chandler, 1999; Rew, Taylor-Seehafer,
dermann, Connell, and Wellborn (2009) without the Active Be- Thomas, & Yockey, 2001) that demonstrate adequate internal
havioral Disaffection scale. The five-scale version utilized in this consistency (alphas ⫽ .72–.91) and convergent validity. For the
study was created and translated into Spanish by Lloyd (2014). In current study, translation and back translation from the English
the current study, Cronbach’s alphas by subscale were as follows: version was undertaken according to the guidelines of Alegria et
.87 for Behavioral Engagement, .65 for Emotional Engagement, al. (2004). In the current study, internal consistency was adequate
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.50 for Behavioral Disaffection, .38 for Emotional Disaffection, with Cronbach’s alpha of .76.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and .79 for Active Behavioral Disaffection. Though alphas were


lower than desired for the Emotional Engagement (five items),
Procedures
Behavioral Disaffection (five items), and Emotional Disaffection
(five items) scales, this was likely a result of the small number of Institutional review board and school district approvals were
items on each scale, as evidenced by adequate interitem correla- sought prior to data collection. A Certificate of Confidentiality
tions (i.e., exceeding .15; Clark & Watson, 1995) on those scales. from the National Institutes of Health was also obtained. Informed
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, consent from the adolescents’ caregivers was collected first via
2001). The SDQ was used in this study as a self-report metric of letters and consent forms sent home with each student. If a com-
internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. The SDQ was pleted consent form was returned, assent from the adolescent was
developed as a population screen for psychopathology and con- obtained in person. A completed consent form included the child’s
tains 25 items. Items are scored on a 3-point Likert scale ranging name, birthdate, parent’s name, and a phone number. Procedures
from not true to certainly true. The measure contains five sub- were carefully designed to protect adolescents and their families.
scales measuring emotional problems, conduct problems, hyper- Specifically, both adolescents and their guardians were provided
activity, peer problems, and prosocial behavior. The Emotional documentation in Spanish and all study procedures were under-
Problems subscale contains items that question about fears, wor- taken in Spanish. Additionally, typical protections regarding con-
ries, misery, nerves, and somatic symptoms; the Conduct Problems fidentiality as well as augmented protections provided by the
subscale inquires about tantrums, obedience, fighting, lying, and Certificate of Confidentiality were detailed to adolescents and their
stealing; the Hyperactivity subscale encompasses restlessness, caregivers. All students were assured that their answers would not
fidgeting, concentration, distractibility, and impulsivity; the Peer be shared with immigration officials—a right protected by the
Problems subscale items include questions about popularity, vic- Certificate of Confidentiality—and would not affect their school
timization, isolation, friendship, and ability to relate to children performance. Indeed, signed consent and assent forms were placed
compared with adults; and the Prosocial Behavior subscale covers in a sealed box and handed over directly to the research team, not
consideration of others, ability to share, kindness to younger school personnel, in order to avoid identifying whether students
children, and helpfulness when other children are distressed and had elected to participate. No school-based incentives were offered
willingness to comfort. The SDQ has been translated and used in in order to avoid the appearance of the study being linked to grades
more than 50 languages (Sharp, Croudace, Goodyer, & Amtmann, or other school-based outcomes. Following written informed con-
2005). It is ideal for use in school settings because it is short, free sent and assent, adolescents were assessed in private by the lead
to use, and can be completed by multiple informants (Goodman, author and/or bilingual graduate student research assistants. Ado-
Renfrew, & Mullick, 2000). The previously published SDQ trans- lescents completed all measures with the one-to-one assistance of
lation to Spanish utilized in this study has demonstrated excellent a trained research assistant. Caregivers were interviewed in Span-
psychometric properties (e.g., Ortuño-Sierra, Fonseca-Pedrero, ish by phone.
Paino, Sastre i Riba, & Muñiz, 2015; total internal consistency ⫽
.75; adequate fit for hypothesized five-factor structure; measure-
Data Analytic Strategy
ment invariance by age and gender). In the current study, internal
consistency of these scales was adequate with Cronbach’s alphas Descriptive statistics and bivariate analyses were conducted to
as follows: .62 for Emotional Problems (five items), .37 for Con- determine bivariate relations between independent (i.e., parental
duct Problems (five items), .56 for Hyperactivity (five items), .38 attachment, peer attachment, and school engagement) and depen-
for Peer Problems (five items), and .46 for Prosocial Behavior dent (i.e., each SDQ subscale and resilience) variables and test
(five items). Alphas were likely suppressed by the small number of whether any covariates (i.e., gender and age) should be added to
items on each scale given adequate interitem correlations (i.e., multivariate analyses as control variables. These covariates were
exceeding .15; Clark & Watson, 1995) on all scales. considered in light of prior research indicating that mental health
Resilience Scale (RS; Wagnild & Young, 1993). The RS risk postmigration is affected by both gender and age (Fazel et al.,
was used in this study as a self-report measure of resilience. The 2012). To examine the incremental contribution of school engage-
RS was developed with a large sample of community-dwelling ment, controlling for parent and peer attachment as well as cova-
young adults and contains 25 items rated on a 7-point Likert scale riates, a series of hierarchical linear regressions with covariates
PROTECTIVE EFFECTS OF SCHOOLS FOR IMMIGRANT TEENS 5

entered at Step 1; mother, father, and peer attachment entered at 5.97, p ⫽ .001, but school engagement variables made a signifi-
Step 2; and school engagement entered at Step 3 were carried out cant incremental contribution in Step 2 (⌬F ⫽ 3.59, p ⫽ .006),
for the various outcome variables. with all variables together accounting for 30% of the variance.
Results of this model are presented in Table 3 and indicate that
Results active behavioral disaffection was significantly positively associ-
ated with hyperactivity, with a moderate effect size.
Correlations between key study variables are presented in Table 1 Using peer problems as the outcome variable, familial and peer
and indicate that various aspects of school engagement were signifi- social support accounted for 16.6% of the variance, F(72) ⫽ 5.79,
cantly associated with psychopathology and prosocial behavior p ⫽ .001, and there was no evidence that school engagement
(SDQ), with significant correlations ranging from .27 to .52 (absolute variables made a significant incremental contribution in Step 2
value). Both behavioral and emotional engagement were significantly (⌬F ⫽ .26, p ⫽ .93).
positively correlated with resilience as well. Age was not significantly With prosocial behavior as the outcome, familial and peer social
associated with any outcome variable. Likewise, independent samples support accounted for a nonsignificant 4.8% of the variance,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

t tests did not provide evidence of significant gender differences in F(72) ⫽ 2.20, p ⫽ .096, yet school engagement variables made a
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

outcome variables. Therefore, neither age nor sex was included as a significant incremental contribution in Step 2 (⌬F ⫽ 2.77, p ⫽
control variable in subsequent analyses, and one step was eliminated .025), with all variables together accounting for 15.6% of the
from the planned models. variance. Results of this model are presented in Table 4 and
To examine the incremental contribution of school engagement, indicate that emotional engagement was significantly positively
above and beyond peer and parent attachment, on psychopathol- associated with prosocial behavior, with a moderate effect size.
ogy, a series of hierarchical linear regressions with mother, father, Finally, to examine the incremental contribution of school en-
and peer attachment entered at Step 1 and school engagement gagement, above and beyond other forms of social support, on
entered at Step 2 were carried out for the various outcome vari- resilience, hierarchical linear regressions with family and peer
ables. Separate models were conducted for each outcome variable attachment entered at Step 1 and school engagement entered at
(i.e., SDQ subscale). Detection tolerance and the variance inflation Step 2 were carried out, with the Resilience scale total score as the
factor (VIF) were used to assess multicollinearity among all pre- outcome variable. Regarding resilience, familial and peer social
dictors; evidence of multicollinearity was not detected with toler- support accounted for a significant 16.1% of the variance, F(72) ⫽
ance greater than .2 and VIF less than 4 in all cases (Aiken & 5.61, p ⫽ .002, yet school engagement variables made a significant
West, 1991; Holmbeck, 2002). incremental contribution in Step 2 (⌬F ⫽ 3.06, p ⫽ .015), with all
With emotional symptoms serving as the outcome variable, variables together accounting for 27% of the variance. Results of
familial and peer attachment accounted for 15% of the variance, this model are presented in Table 5 and indicate that behavioral
F(72) ⫽ 5.37, p ⫽ .002, and school engagement did not make a engagement was significantly positively associated with resilience,
significant incremental contribution in Step 2 (⌬F ⫽ 1.55, p ⫽ with a moderate effect size.
.188).
Regarding conduct problems, familial and peer attachment ac-
Discussion
counted for 14.6% of the variance, F(71) ⫽ 5.09, p ⫽ .003, but
school engagement variables made a significant incremental con- The aim of this study was to examine the incremental contribu-
tribution in Step 2 (⌬F ⫽ 4.105, p ⫽ .003), with all variables tion of school engagement, over and above parent and peer attach-
together accounting for 30.3% of the variance. Results of this ment, in relation to psychopathology, prosocial behavior, and
model are presented in Table 2 and indicate that emotional disaf- resilience among recently immigrated adolescents from Central
fection and active behavioral disaffection were significantly pos- America. Notably, the aim of this study was not to contradict prior
itively associated with conduct problems, with approximately research conducted on immigrant youth (e.g., Fazel et al., 2012;
small to moderate effect sizes. Reed et al., 2012), which shows that social support from family
Examining hyperactivity as the outcome variable, familial and and peers is an important protective factor; instead, we sought to
peer attachment accounted for 17.1% of the variance, F(72) ⫽ examine whether school-based factors made a significant incre-

Table 1
Correlations Between Key Study Variables and Subscales of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire

Key study variables Emotional problems Conduct problems Hyperactivity Peer problems Prosocial behavior Resilience
ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱ ⴱⴱ ⴱⴱ
IPPA Mother total ⫺.40 ⫺.32 ⫺.33 ⫺.32 .03 .31ⴱⴱ
IPPA Father total ⫺.28ⴱ ⫺.24ⴱ ⫺.02 ⫺.34ⴱⴱ .11 .17
IPPA Peer total ⫺.18 ⫺.32ⴱⴱ ⫺.29ⴱ ⫺.32ⴱⴱ .30ⴱⴱ .36ⴱⴱ
School behavioral engagement .11 ⫺.11 ⫺.02 ⫺.01 .32ⴱⴱ .37ⴱⴱⴱ
School emotional engagement ⫺.05 ⫺.19 ⫺.10 ⫺.1 .44ⴱⴱⴱ .38ⴱⴱⴱ
School behavioral disaffection .21 .27ⴱ .27ⴱ .12 ⫺.22 ⫺.16
School emotional disaffection .32ⴱⴱ .52ⴱⴱⴱ .27ⴱ .26ⴱ ⫺.25ⴱ ⫺.15
School active behavioral disaffection .14 .45ⴱⴱⴱ .49ⴱⴱⴱ .15 ⫺.17 ⫺.12
Note. IPPA ⫽ Inventory of Peer and Parent Attachment.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
6 VENTA ET AL.

Table 2 Table 4
Regression Results With Conduct Problems as the Regression Results With Prosocial Behavior as the
Outcome Variable Outcome Variable

Key study variables ␤ t p Key study variables ␤ t p

Step 1 8.38 <.01 Step 1 5.31 <.01


IPPA Mother total ⫺.27 ⫺2.34 .02 IPPA Mother total ⫺.01 ⫺.12 .91
IPPA Father total ⫺.04 ⫺.35 .73 IPPA Father total .00 .01 .99
IPPA Peer total ⫺.26 ⫺2.21 .03 IPPA Peer total .30 2.38 .02
Step 2 2.78 .01 Step 2 1.83 .07
IPPA Mother total ⫺.19 ⫺1.76 .08 IPPA Mother total ⫺.05 ⫺.45 .65
IPPA Father total ⫺.07 ⫺.57 .57 IPPA Father total .02 .14 .89
IPPA Peer total ⫺.09 ⫺.78 .44 IPPA Peer total .20 1.54 .13
Behavioral engagement ⫺.02 ⫺.12 .90 Behavioral engagement .08 .59 .56
Emotional engagement .06 .45 .65 Emotional engagement .36 2.51 .02
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Behavioral disaffection ⫺.03 ⫺.21 .84 Behavioral disaffection .00 .03 .98
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Emotional disaffection .35 2.68 .01 Emotional disaffection ⫺.02 ⫺.16 .88
Active behavioral disaffection .24 2.04 .05 Active behavioral disaffection ⫺.02 ⫺.11 .91
Note. Effect sizes were interpreted as follows: ⫾.1 ⫽ small; ⫾.3 ⫽ Note. Effect sizes were interpreted as follows: ⫾.1 ⫽ small; ⫾.3 ⫽
moderate, and ⫾.5 ⫽ large. p values ⱕ.05 appear in bold. IPPA ⫽ moderate, and ⫾.5 ⫽ large. p values ⱕ.05 appear in bold. IPPA ⫽
Inventory of Peer and Parent Attachment. Inventory of Peer and Parent Attachment.

mental contribution to youth mental health given the aforemen- ciated with all outcome variables examined likely reflects a devel-
tioned practical utility of fostering protective effects in the school opmental process wherein adolescents—particularly the older
setting for immigrant youth. Overall, findings indicated that as- adolescents represented in this sample— become increasingly re-
pects of school engagement play an important role in externalizing liant on peers and romantic partners for emotional support (Venta
behavior problems and may play a role in the development of et al., 2014). Still, it is important to note that low parental attach-
resilience and prosocial behavior. ment was significantly associated with mental health problems in
First, in examining peer and parent attachment, findings broadly this sample despite participants’ age. As the first study, to our
supported prior research conducted in immigrant youth document- knowledge, to assess attachment to parents among recently immi-
ing the importance of these protective factors (e.g., Fazel et al., grated youth in the United States, these findings preliminarily
2012; Reed et al., 2012). Indeed, attachment to mother was sig- suggest that attachment disruption may be an important risk factor
nificantly negatively associated with all forms of psychopathology in this population, perhaps resulting from high rates of parental
and significantly positively associated with resilience (but not separation (C. Suárez-Orozco et al., 2002).
prosocial behavior). Attachment to father was significantly nega- Second, although findings confirmed that attachment to parents
tively associated with all forms of psychopathology other than and peers has a significant relation to all forms of psychopathol-
hyperactivity but neither prosocial behavior nor resilience. Attach- ogy, results also indicated that school engagement uniquely con-
ment to peers was significantly negatively associated with all tributed to these models with regard to externalizing psychopa-
forms of psychopathology and positively linked to prosocial be- thology (i.e., conduct problems and hyperactivity). Specifically,
havior and resilience. The fact that attachment to peers was asso- emotional disaffection and active behavioral disaffection were

Table 3 Table 5
Regression Results With Hyperactivity as the Outcome Variable Regression Results With Resilience as the Outcome Variable

Key study variables ␤ t p Key study variables ␤ t p

Step 1 7.96 <.01 Step 1 2.27 .03


IPPA Mother total ⫺.36 ⫺3.16 <.01 IPPA Mother total .28 2.43 .02
IPPA Father total .22 1.83 .07 IPPA Father total ⫺.06 ⫺.46 .65
IPPA Peer total ⫺.32 ⫺2.72 .01 IPPA Peer total .34 2.86 .01
Step 2 2.76 .01 Step 2 ⫺1.10 .28
IPPA Mother total ⫺.33 ⫺3.03 <.01 IPPA Mother total .31 2.81 .01
IPPA Father total .22 1.87 .07 IPPA Father total .00 ⫺.01 .99
IPPA Peer total ⫺.23 ⫺1.91 .06 IPPA Peer total .27 2.26 .03
Behavioral engagement .08 .61 .54 Behavioral engagement .29 2.28 .03
Emotional engagement .07 .55 .59 Emotional engagement .19 1.44 .16
Behavioral disaffection .19 1.50 .14 Behavioral disaffection ⫺.05 ⫺.34 .73
Emotional disaffection ⫺.05 ⫺.41 .69 Emotional disaffection .18 1.33 .19
Active behavioral disaffection .37 3.13 <.01 Active behavioral disaffection .03 .22 .83
Note. Effect sizes were interpreted as follows: ⫾.1 ⫽ small; ⫾.3 ⫽ Note. Effect sizes were interpreted as follows: ⫾.1 ⫽ small; ⫾.3 ⫽
moderate, and ⫾.5 ⫽ large. p values ⱕ.05 appear in bold. IPPA ⫽ moderate, and ⫾.5 ⫽ large. p values ⱕ.05 appear in bold. IPPA ⫽
Inventory of Peer and Parent Attachment. Inventory of Peer and Parent Attachment.
PROTECTIVE EFFECTS OF SCHOOLS FOR IMMIGRANT TEENS 7

significantly positively associated with conduct problems, and poorer educational outcomes. Critically, the current study suggests
active behavioral disaffection was significantly positively associ- that decreased behavioral (active or otherwise) engagement is a
ated with hyperactivity, with moderate effect sizes. It is important crosscutting risk factor, with implications for both poor academic
to note that active behavioral disaffection is defined as behavior outcomes (e.g., Li & Lerner, 2011) and mental health problems
that is disruptive or restless during learning (e.g., “When I’m in (i.e., conduct problems and hyperactivity), whereas increased emo-
class, I talk a lot with my classmates”; Lloyd, 2014), and thus it is tional and behavioral engagement may be a crosscutting protective
not surprising that the two related constructs were linked in this factor captured in both resilience and prosocial behavior.
study. Emotional disaffection (e.g., “When I’m doing work in The current study did not include explicit measures of teacher
class, I feel bored”) was also associated with conduct problems and connection or attachment, as that has been well-established as a
draws a parallel between emotional disaffection and the more protective factor in previous research. However, that existing re-
blatant, active disaffection typically associated with externalizing search base bolsters the broad conclusions of this study, as emo-
psychopathology. Third, only school engagement variables, and tional engagement is often parameterized as connectedness to
not familial or peer attachment, were significantly associated with teachers (C. Suárez-Orozco et al., 2002). In prior research, sup-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

prosocial behavior, and school engagement played a unique role in portive school relationships have been linked to increased school
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

resilience. Specifically, emotional engagement was significantly engagement more broadly and increased academic performance
positively associated with prosocial behavior, and behavioral en- among recently immigrated youth (C. Suárez-Orozco, Pimentel, &
gagement was significantly positively associated with resilience. Martin, 2009). Existing data suggest that by enhancing a student’s
The fact that these relations were unique (e.g., emotional engage- sense of belonging and support, the adversity associated with
ment was not significantly associated with resilience) in the cur- adapting to a new school context is reduced (Levitt, Guacci-
rent study is surprising in light of the literature linking prosocial Franco, & Levitt, 1994)—a critical factor for recently immigrated
behavior and resilience in youth (e.g., Alvord & Grados, 2005). adolescents who face elevated acculturative stress. Indeed, over a
However, examining the theorized nature of the emotional and 5-year longitudinal study of Hispanic immigrated youth, increased
behavioral dimensions of engagement provides some insight. For relational engagement predicted enhanced cognitive and behav-
instance, Skinner and Pitzer (2012) stated, “the behavioral dimen- ioral engagement at school, which had long-term effects on edu-
sion of engagement includes effort, intensity, persistence, deter- cational attainment (C. Suárez-Orozco et al., 2002). Qualitative
mination, and perseverance in the face of obstacles and difficul- research echoes the conclusion that strong relationships between
ties” (p. 24)—a definition that clearly alludes to resilience in the Hispanic immigrant youth and teachers is linked to behavioral and
face of adversity and may explain why those two constructs were academic success (Conchas, 2001). Critically, teacher support is
linked in the current study (e.g., perhaps because of overlapping uniquely associated with increased school engagement and de-
item content). Likewise, they defined emotional engagement as creased behavior problems, even in the absence of parental support
including enjoyment and enthusiasm—affective states that likely (Rosenfeld, Richman, & Bowen, 2000). Much of this research is
underlie prosocial behavior in school. Unfortunately, the current interpreted through the aforementioned social capital lens, which
study cannot speak to developmental or causal paths between these suggests that teacher–student relationships mitigate social capital
forms of engagement and their apparent correlates (i.e., resilience deficits associated with migration and thus increase positive out-
and prosocial behavior) as a result of the cross-sectional nature of comes (Garcia-Reid et al., 2015; C. Suárez-Orozco et al., 2010).
the data collected. Further, it is outside the scope of the current Taken together with this broader context, the current findings point
study to unpack the conceptual similarities and differences be- to the need for future research examining mechanistic models in
tween the aforementioned constructs. However, given gaps in the which teacher support is examined as a predictor of psychopathol-
available research, both of these limitations are important direction ogy, resilience, and prosocial behavior, with school engagement
for future research aimed at identifying mechanism by which variables tested as mediators. The current study contributes,
school engagement may contribute to youth well-being. importantly, to illustrating the link between school engagement
That school engagement, particularly disaffection across emo- and many of these outcomes, for the first time, in recently
tional and behavioral domains and emotional engagement, relates immigrated youth, although the path from teacher support was
to mental health above and beyond familial and peer attachment not tested.
echoes literature regarding the importance of these variables for Indeed, the current study is unique in documenting that that
the academic success of recent immigrants and extends this liter- school engagement—specifically, emotional engagement, emo-
ature to the mental health and resilience contexts. Indeed, existing tional disaffection and active behavioral disaffection—is a signif-
longitudinal research suggests that school engagement declines icant factor in mental health outcomes as well as the academic
following youth migration (Green, Rhodes, Hirsch, Suárez- outcomes that have been previously evaluated. Moreover, findings
Orozco, & Camic, 2008). Decreased school engagement among indicate that this contribution is above and beyond the contribution
immigrant youth has been widely cited as a key factor in this of traditional forms of social support. Although preliminary be-
academic decline after migration. Indeed, immigrant and Latino cause of the cross-sectional nature of this data, findings suggest
(Li & Lerner, 2011) youth are at a particularly high risk for that schools play a role in mental health problems (in the exter-
decreased school engagement, likely because they experience nalizing domain) as well as prosocial behavior and resilience
alienation and segregation at higher levels than nonimmigrant above and beyond well-established risk and protective effects from
youth (C. Suárez-Orozco, Suárez-Orozco, & Todorova, 2009). In parent and peer attachment. The latter has critical public health
particular, using data from 41 countries, Chiu et al. (2012) dem- relevance given increasing rates of youth migrating from Central
onstrated that immigrant youth experienced decreased emotional America without a guardian, increasing rates of adult parent de-
engagement and weaker relationships with teachers, which led to portation in the United States (United States Border Patrol, United
8 VENTA ET AL.

States Customs and Border Protection, 2016b), and well- is particularly important given that adolescence is a unique period
documented difficulties faced by immigrant youth in integrating of social reorientation (Crone & Dahl, 2012), a window of oppor-
with peers in mainstream classes. tunity for changing attachment strategies resulting from the ado-
lescent’s growing cognitive and emotional freedom (Allen, 2008),
and a particularly vulnerable time for the emergence of psycho-
Discussion of Implications for Practice
pathology (e.g., Weir et al., 2012) and risk taking (e.g., Steinberg,
From policy and intervention standpoints, however, the extent to 2007). The necessity for future research is even more critical
which school engagement can be enhanced in immigrant groups among recent immigrant adolescents given the aforementioned
remains unexamined despite calls to provide care for high-risk risk for declining school engagement after migration and corre-
immigrant adolescents through preventative efforts (Raphel, lated decline in academic achievement and mental health.
2014). Indeed, broad school-based preventative efforts have been
suggested by researchers focusing on recently immigrated youth
Limitations
and include building immigrant-friendly campuses with a multi-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cultural focus, increasing mentoring, and, most often, enhancing The current study is not without limitations. It makes use of a
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the strength of teacher–student relationships (Goh, Wahl, McDon- small sample of recently immigrated youth across a fairly large age
ald, Brissett, & Yoon, 2007; Green et al., 2008; C. Suárez-Orozco band—reflecting public school admittance to high school in the
et al., 2010; C. Suárez-Orozco, Suárez-Orozco, et al., 2009). These state of data collection. The measures utilized were completed in
interventions, theoretically, would enhance school engagement, Spanish because of available research personnel with Spanish
with potential consequences for externalizing psychopathology, fluency, although the participants’ Spanish fluency was not for-
resilience, and prosocial behavior. However, no such programs mally assessed. This omission may have been particularly relevant
have been developed and empirically tested, to our knowledge. considering that, for some Central American immigrants, Spanish
Further, there is a similar dearth of information regarding what is a second language and assessment in a local indigenous lan-
teachers can do to facilitate engagement in adolescent students guage may have been more comfortable for the participants. Fur-
who might otherwise experience disaffection. The findings of the ther, many students stated that they could not read the question-
current study suggest, preliminarily, that enhancing engagement (if naires independently and requested that a graduate student read the
possible) would have positive consequences for mental health, items and response choices out loud to them in a private room.
prosocial behavior, and resilience among recently immigrated Although this level of support allowed the current sample to
youth. However, the broad school engagement literature that in- include otherwise difficult-to-reach participants, it also introduces
cludes metrics of specific teacher behavior is lacking in overall response bias associated with orally answering items. Addition-
number of studies and inclusion of high school students (Hirn & ally, the findings reported make use of cross-sectional data, and
Scott, 2014). Moreover, literature regarding specific teacher be- replication utilizing follow-up data, when available, is needed.
haviors that foster emotional engagement is sparse, with studies
conducted primarily in elementary (Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2002) Conclusions
and middle school students (Matsumura, Slater, & Crosson, 2008;
Voelkl, 1995). Many of these researchers utilize an “extended Notwithstanding limitations, this study examined a sample that
attachment perspective” to conceptualize student–teacher relation- is difficult to recruit, has been the subject of very little empirical
ships, noting that teacher behaviors like sensitivity, closeness, and research, and has a high clinical need. Additionally, the current
reliability put students in an ideal position to explore new frontiers study, although preliminary, has broad implications for other vul-
with a secure base, thereby enhancing academic and mental health nerable groups. Specifically, identifying risk and protective factors
outcomes (Roorda, Koomen, Spilt, & Oort, 2011). A recent meta- for psychopathology in this group (and intervening) may have
analytic study of teacher–student relationships across development implications for American-born Hispanics, which represent a
noted that additional data from secondary school students were growing 16.3% of the U.S. population (Ennis, Rios-Vargas, &
needed, particularly because teacher–student relationships appear Albert, 2011). Finally, the present study may serve as a model for
to grow in their predictive power as students grow older (Roorda future research addressing the mental health needs of other migrant
et al., 2011). This is a critical finding given existing literature groups, a pressing need in light of current global crises of unprec-
noting that school engagement generally decreases in high school edented Middle Eastern and North African youth migration in
(Marks, 2000), amid increases in mental health problems during Europe as well as the United States.
that developmental stage (Weir, Zakama, & Rao, 2012).
The current study highlights the importance of this issue by References
documenting the significant role of school engagement in student
well-being with a sample that is quite a bit older than samples Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
included in previous research. Indeed, it may be that school en- interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
gagement plays a particularly large role for older students who Alegria, M., Vila, D., Woo, M., Canino, G., Takeuchi, D., Vera, M., . . .
Shrout, P. (2004). Cultural relevance and equivalence in the NLAAS
might be graduated or not enrolled in school at all if not for
instrument: Integrating etic and emic in the development of cross-
immigration issues. To date, no study has examined which teacher cultural measures for a psychiatric epidemiology and services study of
behaviors stand to enhance emotional engagement among recent Latinos. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 13,
immigrants, and only a small literature base has examined similar 270 –288. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mpr.181
constructs in adolescents. Future research examining teacher be- Allen, J. P. (2008). The attachment system in adolescence. In J. Cassidy &
haviors that enhance student relational engagement in adolescents P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and
PROTECTIVE EFFECTS OF SCHOOLS FOR IMMIGRANT TEENS 9

clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 419 – 435). New York, NY: Guilford European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 9, 129 –134. http://dx.doi.org/
Press. 10.1007/s007870050008
Alvord, M. K., & Grados, J. J. (2005). Enhancing resilience in children: A Green, G., Rhodes, J., Hirsch, A. H., Suárez-Orozco, C., & Camic, P. M.
proactive approach. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, (2008). Supportive adult relationships and the academic engagement of
36, 238 –245. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.36.3.238 Latin American immigrant youth. Journal of School Psychology, 46,
Black, C., & Ford-Gilboe, M. (2004). Adolescent mothers: Resilience, 393– 412. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2007.07.001
family health work and health-promoting practices. Journal of Advanced Hirn, R. G., & Scott, T. M. (2014). Descriptive analysis of teacher instruc-
Nursing, 48, 351–360. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2648.2004 tional practices and student engagement among adolescents with and
.03204.x without challenging behavior. Education & Treatment of Children, 37,
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York, 589 – 610. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/etc.2014.0037
NY: Basic Books. Hiskey, J. T., Córdova, A., Orcés, D., & Malone, M. F. (2016). Under-
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and standing the Central American refugee crisis: Why they are fleeing and
anger. New York, NY: Basic Books. how U.S. policies are failing to deter them. Washington, DC: American
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experi- Immigration Council. Retrieved from https://www.americanimmigration
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ments by design and nature. Cambridge, Massachusetts/London, Eng- council.org/research/understanding-central-american-refugee-crisis


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

land: Harvard University Press. Holmbeck, G. N. (2002). Post-hoc probing of significant moderational and
Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., Bandura, A., & Zimbardo, mediational effects in studies of pediatric populations. Journal of Pedi-
P. G. (2000). Prosocial foundations of children’s academic achievement. atric Psychology, 27, 87–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/27.1.87
Psychological Science, 11, 302–306. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467- Hunter, A. J., & Chandler, G. E. (1999). Adolescent resilience. Journal of
9280.00260 Nursing Scholarship, 31, 243–247. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1547-
Caramota, S., Griffith, B., & Zeigler, K. (2017). Mapping the impact of 5069.1999.tb00488.x
immigration on public schools. Retrieved from https://cis.org/Report/ Layous, K., Nelson, S. K., Oberle, E., Schonert-Reichl, K. A., & Ly-
Mapping-Impact-Immigration-Public-Schools ubomirsky, S. (2012). Kindness counts: Prompting prosocial behavior in
Chiu, M. M., Pong, S. L., Mori, I., & Chow, B. W. Y. (2012). Immigrant preadolescents boosts peer acceptance and well-being. PLoS ONE, 7,
students’ emotional and cognitive engagement at school: A multilevel e51380. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0051380
analysis of students in 41 countries. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, Levitt, M. J., Guacci-Franco, N., & Levitt, J. L. (1994). Social support and
41, 1409 –1425. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-012-9763-x achievement in childhood and early adolescence: A multicultural study.
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 15, 207–222. http://dx
objective scale development. Psychological Assessment, 7, 309 –319. .doi.org/10.1016/0193-3973(94)90013-2
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.7.3.309 Li, Y., & Lerner, R. M. (2011). Trajectories of school engagement during
Conchas, G. (2001). Structuring failure and success: Understanding the adolescence: Implications for grades, depression, delinquency, and sub-
variability in Latino school engagement. Harvard Educational Review, stance use. Developmental Psychology, 47, 233–247. http://dx.doi.org/
71, 475–505. http://dx.doi.org/10.17763/haer.71.3.280w814v1603473k 10.1037/a0021307
Crone, E. A., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period Lloyd, K. S. (2014). Student engagement: Re-examining behavioral disaf-
of social-affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews fection within the self-system model of motivational development (Un-
Neuroscience, 13, 636 – 650. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3313 published doctoral dissertation). University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa,
Ennis, S. R., Rios-Vargas, M., & Albert, N. G. (2011). The Hispanic AL.
population: 2010 (2010 Census Briefs). Washington, DC: U.S. Census Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional activity: Patterns
Bureau. in the elementary, middle, and high school years. American Educational
Fazel, M., Reed, R. V., Panter-Brick, C., & Stein, A. (2012). Mental health Research Journal, 37, 153–184. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0002
of displaced and refugee children resettled in high-income countries: 8312037001153
Risk and protective factors. The Lancet, 379, 266 –282. http://dx.doi.org/ Masten, A. S., Liebkind, K., & Hernandez, D. J. (Eds.). (2012). Realizing
10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60051-2 the potential of immigrant youth. New York, NY: Cambridge University
Gallarin, M., & Alonso-Arbiol, I. (2013). Dimensionality of the inventory Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139094696
of parent and peer attachment: Evaluation with the Spanish version. The Matsumura, L. C., Slater, S. C., & Crosson, A. (2008). Classroom climate,
Spanish Journal of Psychology, 16, E55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/sjp rigorous instruction and curriculum, and students’ interactions in urban
.2013.47 middle schools. The Elementary School Journal, 108, 293–312. http://
Garcia-Reid, P., Peterson, C. H., & Reid, R. J. (2015). Parent and teacher dx.doi.org/10.1086/528973
support among Latino immigrant youth: Effects on school engagement Motti-Stefanidi, F., & Masten, A. S. (2013). School success and school
and school trouble avoidance. Education and Urban Society, 47, 328 – engagement of immigrant children and adolescents. European Psychol-
343. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013124513495278 ogist, 18, 126 –135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000139
Goh, M., Wahl, K. H., McDonald, J. K., Brissett, A. A., & Yoon, E. (2007). National Institutes of Health. (2011). Health disparities among children
Working with immigrant students in schools: The role of school coun- of immigrants: Proceedings of the National Children’s Study Sym-
selors in building cross-cultural bridges. Journal of Multicultural Coun- posium. Retrieved from https://www1.nichd.nih.gov/publications/
seling and Development, 35, 66 –79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/j.2161- pubs/Documents/HealthDisparitiesChildrenImmig.pdf
1912.2007.tb00050.x National Institute of Mental Health. (2018). Domain: Social Processes.
Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and diffi- Retrieved July 2, 2018, from https://www.nimh.nih.gov/research-
culties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & priorities/rdoc/constructs/social-processes.shtml
Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1337–1345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ Ortuño-Sierra, J., Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Paino, M., Sastre i Riba, S., &
00004583-200111000-00015 Muñiz, J. (2015). Screening mental health problems during adolescence:
Goodman, R., Renfrew, D., & Mullick, M. (2000). Predicting type of Psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the Strengths and
psychiatric disorder from Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire Difficulties Questionnaire. Journal of Adolescence, 38, 49 –56. http://dx
(SDQ) scores in child mental health clinics in London and Dhaka. .doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2014.11.001
10 VENTA ET AL.

Pair Project. (n.d.). Appearing at a master calendar hearing in immigration Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk taking in adolescence: New perspectives from
court. Retrieved from http://www.pairproject.org/resources/MC brain and behavioral science. Current Directions in Psychological Sci-
H%20and%20Pleadings.pdf ence, 16, 55–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00475.x
Perreira, K. M., & Ornelas, I. (2013). Painful passages: Traumatic expe- Suárez-Orozco, C., Onaga, M., & de Lardemelle, C. (2010). Promoting
riences and post-traumatic stress among immigrant Latino adolescents academic engagement among immigrant adolescents through school-
and their primary caregivers. The International Migration Review, 47, family-community collaboration. Professional School Counseling, 14,
976 –1005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/imre.12050 15–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.5330/prsc.14.1.xl6227108g624057
Potochnick, S. R., & Perreira, K. M. (2010). Depression and anxiety among Suárez-Orozco, C., Pimentel, A., & Martin, M. (2009). The significance of
first-generation immigrant Latino youth: Key correlates and implications relationships: Academic engagement and achievement among newcomer
for future research. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 198, 470 – immigrant youth. Teachers College Record, 111, 712–749.
477. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181e4ce24 Suárez-Orozco, C., Suárez-Orozco, M. M., & Todorova, I. (2009). Learn-
Raphel, S. (2014). Eye on Washington migrant children and U.S. policy in ing a new land. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
the twenty-first century. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Suárez-Orozco, C., Todorova, I. L., & Louie, J. (2002). Making up for lost
Nursing, 27, 202–204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jcap.12092 time: The experience of separation and reunification among immigrant
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Reed, R. V., Fazel, M., Jones, L., Panter-Brick, C., & Stein, A. (2012). families. Family Process, 41, 625– 643. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j
.1545-5300.2002.00625.x
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Mental health of displaced and refugee children resettled in low-income


and middle-income countries: Risk and protective factors. The Lancet, Suárez-Orozco, M. (2001). Globalization, immigration, and education: The
379, 250 –265. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60050-0 research agenda. Harvard Educational Review, 71, 345–366. http://dx
Rew, L., Taylor-Seehafer, M., Thomas, N. Y., & Yockey, R. D. (2001). .doi.org/10.17763/haer.71.3.7521rl25282t3637
Correlates of resilience in homeless adolescents. Journal of Nursing United States Conference on Catholic Bishops. (2014). Testimony of Most
Scholarship, 33, 33– 40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1547-5069.2001 Reverend Mark Seitz . . . on unaccompanied children. House Judiciary
.00033.x Committee. Retrieved from http://www.usccb.org/about/migration-
Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Early, D. M., Cox, M. J., Saluja, G., Pianta, R. C., policy/upload/BSeitzfinaltest.pdf
Bradley, R. H., & Payne, C. (2002). Early behavioral attributes and United States Customs and Border Protection. (2016a). United States
teachers’ sensitivity as predictors of competent behavior in the kinder- Border Patrol Southwest family unit subject and unaccompanied alien
garten classroom. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 23, children apprehensions fiscal year 2016. Retrieved from https://www
451– 470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(02)00128-4 .cbp.gov/newsroom/stats/southwest-border-unaccompanied-children/fy-
Roorda, D. L., Koomen, H. M., Spilt, J. L., & Oort, F. J. (2011). The 2016
influence of affective teacher–student relationships on students’ school United States Customs and Border Protection. (2016b). Southwest border
engagement and achievement: A meta-analytic approach. Review of sectors. Retrieved from https://www.cbp.gov/sites/default/files/assets/
Educational Research, 81, 493–529. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/003 documens/2016-Apr/BP%20Southwest%20Border%20Family%20Units
4654311421793 %20and%20UAC%20Apps%20-%20Mar.pdf
Rosenblum, M. (2015). Unaccompanied child migration to the United Venta, A., & Sharp, C. (2015). Mentalizing mediates the relation between
States: The tension between protection and prevention. Retrieved from attachment and peer problems among inpatient adolescents. Journal of
http://www.migrationpolicy.org/sites/default/files/publications/TCM- Infant, Child & Adolescent Psychotherapy, 14, 323–340. http://dx.doi
Protection-UAC.pdf .org/10.1080/15289168.2015.1071997
Rosenfeld, L. B., Richman, J. M., & Bowen, G. L. (2000). Social support Venta, A., Shmueli-Goetz, Y., & Sharp, C. (2014). Assessing attachment in
networks and school outcomes: The centrality of the teacher. Child & adolescence: A psychometric study of the child attachment interview.
Adolescent Social Work Journal, 17, 205–226. http://dx.doi.org/10 Psychological Assessment, 26, 238 –255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
.1023/A:1007535930286 a0034712
Sharp, C., Croudace, T. J., Goodyer, I. M., & Amtmann, D. (2005). The Voelkl, K. E. (1995). School warmth, student participation, and achieve-
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: Predictive validity for help- ment. Journal of Experimental Education, 63, 127–138. http://dx.doi
seeking behaviour from parents over one year. Educational and Child .org/10.1080/00220973.1995.9943817
Psychology, 22, 28 – 44. Wagnild, G. M., & Young, H. M. (1993). Development and psychometric
Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. evaluation of the Resilience Scale. Journal of Nursing Measurement, 1,
(2009). Engagement and disaffection as organizational constructs in the 165–178.
dynamics of motivational development. In K. R. Wenzel & A. Wigfield Weir, J. M., Zakama, A., & Rao, U. (2012). Developmental risk I: De-
(Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 223–245). New York, pression and the developing brain. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric
NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. Clinics of North America, 21, 237–259. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chc
Skinner, E. A., & Pitzer, J. R. (2012). Developmental dynamics of student .2012.01.004
engagement, coping, and everyday resilience. In S. Christenson, A.
Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.), Handbook of research on student engage- Received February 28, 2018
ment (pp. 21– 44). Boston, MA: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978- Revision received June 5, 2018
1-4614-2018-7_2 Accepted June 7, 2018 䡲

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi