Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 14

Drawings of Emotionally 71

Characterised Figures by
Children from Different
Educational Backgrounds
Esther Burkitt, Martyn Barrett and Alyson Davis

Abstract

Previous research has shown that children a sad figure. Children from the mainstream
systematically alter the size and colour of their schools drew larger figures overall, but educa-
drawings in response to the emotional character tional background did not interact with the
of the figures which they draw. However, these specific emotional character of the figures in
findings have been demonstrated only with chil- producing these scaling changes. However,
dren receiving mainstream Western education. there were differences between the two educa-
This experiment was designed to investigate tional groups in relation to the colours used for
whether children receiving a different kind of the negatively characterised figures. The findings
education also use scaling and colour differen- are discussed in terms of the need to further
tially for depicting figures of contrasting understand the role of the educational system in
emotional significance. 76 children, 44 children mediating children’s depictions of emotional
from mainstream schools (21 boys, 23 girls) and character in their drawings.
32 children from Steiner schools (15 boys, 17
girls) were divided into two age groups, with 38
children in the younger age group (mean age 4
years 7 months) and 38 in the older age group
(mean age 6 years 8 months). All children
completed three drawings of differentially char-
acterised human figures: a neutral, a happy, and

JADE 24.1 ©NSEAD 2005


72 Introduction development may aid their expressive and
Esther Burkitt, Children draw, they like to draw, and there is an communicative development [7], research to
Martyn Barrett
and Alyson Davis inherent enjoyment in the motor activity related date generally suffers from a lack of a systematic
to drawing in early and middle childhood [1]. approach to substantiate such views. Attempts
Parents look at the drawings of their children to have been made to validate the interpretation of
see whether there is a sense of joy or sadness in children’s drawings for insights into the person-
the pictures, people generally regard children’s ality of the child [8], the child’s current mood state
drawings as charming and innocent, and a wide [9], and to glean an understanding of the signifi-
range of emotional terms is generally used for cance drawn topics may have for the child [10].
referring to the contents and characteristics of A more specialised view about the possible
children’s artwork. Whilst it is known that enjoy- clinical-projective use of children’s drawings
ment often goes into the production of children’s began to emerge in the 1940s. This approach
drawings, and that emotional terms are used by was based on the assumption that children
adults to describe these pieces, very little is project their emotions, motives, personalities and
known from a reliable research base about how self-images into their drawings. It was believed
children’s emotions might shape the specific that drawings could be used to assess personal-
contents of their drawings. ity and psychological states, and could be used in
The literature reveals the widely held view that clinical and psychiatric assessment. For example,
the process of producing a piece of artwork Machover [11] introduced a test based on human
involves the operation of more than simply cogni- figure drawing, with the analysis of the drawing
tive and motor-procedural factors [2]. The view being based upon the number, size and shape of
that emotional and communicative components body parts, the orientation of the figure, clothing,
are involved both in drawing production and in background, etc. Conclusions were drawn about
assessing the final drawn product is often used the child’s self-esteem, significant relationships
as the basis for the continued use of drawings in with others and psychosexual development.
a wide range of professional settings [3]. Art ther- This strand of enquiry highlights the possibility
apists work with clients basing therapeutic of interpreting children’s drawings reliably for infor-
discussion around the drawing process and prod- mation about the child, yet, as the related research
uct [4], clinicians are inclined to interpret drawings did not take into account the children’s develop-
for signs of the inner feelings of patients and to mental stage, it is highly possible that the features
generate discussions with vulnerable groups [5], depicted in these drawings were no more than the
and forensic psychologists examine children’s often strange features depicted at classic stages
drawings in order to ascertain whether more of drawing development [12]. Thus, whilst
information is contained within and conveyed by Machover and others working in this tradition
children’s drawings of a witnessed event claimed that their interpretations were validated
compared with their verbal reports [6]. More against clinical experience, other ways of testing
generally, educationalists and parents alike often the reliability and validity of their conclusions are
form impressions as to the well-being and devel- not provided, leaving doubt about the validity of
opment of children, and about children’s feelings projective theories of children’s drawings.
towards objects in their world, on the basis of These early investigations assumed that draw-
their drawings. ings provide a privileged access to the child’s
Although there has been extensive interest in personality or emotional state, but the lack of
the ways in which children’s drawings might be rigour in the research base did little to give a
interpreted for emotional information about the substantial basis to such claims. As Freeman [13]
artist, and the ways in which children’s drawing and others [14] have shown, children’s drawings

JADE 24.1 ©NSEAD 2005


are not simple print-outs of their mental state However, a sequence of studies conducted by 73
representations. Instead, producing a drawing Jolley [19] failed to replicate these findings. This Esther Burkitt,
Martyn Barrett
involves numerous cognitive and motor led Jolly to claim that these effects were unreli- and Alyson Davis
processes and skills by means of which the child able, and that there is no evidence either for the
plans and executes that drawing. operation of a defensive-appetitive mechanism
Indeed, many of the features and curious (where children reduce the size of threatening
distortions which may be present in children’s topics to increase perceived distance from the
drawings have since been shown to be a result of threat, and increase the size of positive topics to
limitations in cognitive planning and motor execu- increase the perception of positivity) or for the use
tion rather than expressions of the child’s of acquired pictorial conventions for the depiction
emotional state or personality. Thus, research in of nice and nasty figures. However, the conflict-
this area needs to account for children’s drawing ing findings in this field were probably due to
ability, level of motor-cognitive development, and methodological differences and limitations, for
emotional attitude towards topics within the example, differences in the research designs
drawing situation. This is indeed a complex task, used, the precise instructions used, and the fail-
yet the complexity of the process of how chil- ure to include independent measures of
dren’s drawings are shaped and interpreted is a children’s positive and negative affect towards
necessary challenge to anyone interested in the drawn figures.
understanding what children are attempting to More recently, Burkitt et al [20] have shown
represent through their drawings, both in func- that reliable phenomena relating to whether chil-
tional and symbolic terms [16]. dren view a figure as being positive or negative do
Systematic research in this field has emerged indeed occur in relationship to the use of both
more recently with a change of focus. figure size and colour. Children have been found
Researchers have begun to look at how topics to consistently increase the size of positively char-
that are significant in children’s lives, and topics acterised stimuli, but to not always decrease the
that have been given emotional descriptions in an size of negatively characterised stimuli [21].
experimental setting, are depicted. This research Children have also been found to vary the use of
has focused on investigating whether there are colour in their drawings in relation to both how
any systematic differences in children’s drawings they feel about the colour and the emotional char-
strategies depending on whether the child views acter of the topics which they are representing
the topic as positive or negative. Fox & Thomas [22]. This work shows the need to know more
[17] and Thomas et al [18] have reported studies about the child (for example, their colour prefer-
suggesting that irrespective of the role of the ences) than simply looking at their drawing to see
cognitive and motor processes and skills, there whether drawing strategies have been altered as
may still be an impact of the child’s emotional a result of an emotional view of the topics.
state upon the size of a drawing. They found that With a few exceptions, there has been a
stimuli which had been described as ‘nasty’ were paucity of research which has systematically
drawn significantly smaller than stimuli which had examined how the drawing strategies children
been described as ‘nice’. They proposed that as use to depict emotion are influenced by the draw-
this effect only occurred with stimuli of men, ing context and by the exact cues provided in the
rather than with apples, the most plausible expla- drawing situation. Whilst the wealth of literature
nation was that the children were potentially exploring children’s perceptions of mood and
scared of the nasty men, and that a defensive- metaphor in drawings contains some investiga-
appetitive mechanism was therefore responsible tions which apply a rigorous and systematic
for the effect. paradigm [23], the effect of the emotional

JADE 24.1 ©NSEAD 2005


74 salience of topics depicted by children and the The possibility that education might have an effect
Esther Burkitt, role of emotional and communicative aspects in specifically upon the use of size in children’s draw-
Martyn Barrett
and Alyson Davis terms of drawing production have been largely ings has received very little attention to date. One
overlooked. This may be due to the fact that draw- exception is a study by Aronsson & Andersson
ings are complexly determined. Yet this only [29], who studied children’s drawings of them-
highlights the need to explore further how selves and their teachers in a classroom setting.
emotional components may act to shape, and The children formed three groups: Tanzanian chil-
appear in, children’s drawings. dren attending traditional schools, children living
In is noteworthy that the more rigorous in refugee settlements, and Swedish children who
research to date that has explored the effects of attended small town schools. They found that the
emotionally describing figures for children to draw children in the more child-centred (Swedish)
has only examined children from mainstream educational system drew larger children than
educational backgrounds. Consequently, the those in the more teacher-centred systems. The
present experiment was designed to assess authors proposed that scaling in children’s draw-
whether similar effects occur amongst children ings indicated social-value systems related to a
who experience contrasting educational systems. traditionality–modernity continuum. These results
are in line with the literature claiming that children
Cultural and educational influences on draw subjectively salient figures larger than less
children’s drawings important figures [30], and indicate that size
Whilst research has been conducted looking into change may also vary as a function of the type of
the effects of the educational system on chil- education received. In order to assess drawing
dren’s human figure drawings [24], most of this behaviour, however, it is important to ascertain
research does not examine whether schooling whether phenomena occur reliably across differ-
encourages differential depictions of emotional ent groups of children. The present study
character. It is known that when art education is assessed the impact of educational system on
not a salient feature in cultures, children’s draw- these drawing strategies, using an experimental
ings are less representational [25]. For example, paradigm which has consistently shown that chil-
Bedouin children’s drawings lack much detail dren in mainstream Western education
[26], yet the term ‘representational’ in this sense systematically alter the size and colour of figures
does not mean a lack of emotional representation which they regard as having contrasting
as there may be a metaphorical representation of emotional characters.
the emotion associated with the figure instead. For the purposes of this study, it was decided
Whilst there are complications for cross- to examine the drawings of children attending
cultural research (for example, whether drawing Steiner schools, which offer an educational
is valued similarly by the cultures under study, and system based on the principles of Rudolf Steiner
whether the given topics occupy a similar status [31]. These children were chosen due to the
across societies) [27], and to a certain extent for contrasting nature of their schooling compared
research examining educational differences as with that received by children in mainstream
well, existing research indicates that formal schools in Western cultures. Art education in the
education is indeed likely to affect the form of chil- Steiner system specifically follows a different
dren’s human figure drawings. This is not curriculum from that used in mainstream
surprising, when it is considered that children schools. Steiner art education, particularly in the
often learn from observation, copying and instruc- early years, is concerned predominately with
tion [28]. The classroom is an ideal forum for such child-centred exploration of colour and forms,
processes to take place. compared with formal Western art instruction

JADE 24.1 ©NSEAD 2005


which emphasises attention to details and visual Table 1: Mean age and age range of children in 75
problem-solving [32]. Steiner children are gradu- each experimental group in years and months Esther Burkitt,
Martyn Barrett
ally introduced to line drawing only after the age and Alyson Davis
of 8.This is not the case in mainstream schools. Experimental Group
Even though the debate continues over how to Mainstream Steiner
combine expression and realism in mainstream (N=44) (N=32)
education, very young children in mainstream
Young Young
schools are still taught to focus on the outlines of
(N=22) (N=16)
the objects which they draw, with an emphasis
M=4y7m M=4y7m
on realism over exploration [33].
Range=4y2m-5y2m Range=4y2m-5y3m
The sharpest contrast between the mainstream
and Steiner educational systems occurs around Old Old
age 6. Steiner children explore colour and form at (N=22) (N=16)
this age, whilst children in mainstream schools are M=6y7m M=6y8m
already being encouraged to focus on realistic Range=6y4m-7y7m Range=6y3m-7y5m
detail instead. Thus, in the present experiment, two
age groups falling either side of this age point were were used by children in both groups. Drawing abil-
used, in order to explore how children’s drawings ity was measured using a five- point Likert scale.
of affectively characterised figures develop under
these two different educational regimes. Procedure
The present study was therefore designed to All children completed one test session in which
explore whether children from these two differ- they were asked to draw three men. All children
ent educational backgrounds would exhibit the drew the baseline uncharacterised drawing first,
same pattern of size change, with positively char- followed by two drawings of differentially char-
acterised figures being drawn larger than acterised men presented in counterbalanced
negatively and neutrally characterised figures. order using the following instructions.
The study also investigated whether these two
groups of children would exhibit a similar use of Baseline drawing
colour for completing their drawings of unchar- I’d like you to draw a man. Use just one of these
acterised, happy and sad men. colours. Draw the whole man as well as you can and
colour him as well as you can.
Method
Participants The first drawing was removed, and a second
76 children were tested. 44 children (21 boys, 23 sheet of A4 blank paper was placed in front of the
girls) formed the mainstream group. The Steiner child. The range of crayons was left in place. The
group consisted of 32 children (15 boys and 17 following happy and sad drawing tasks were
girls) selected from Steiner schools across administered in a counterbalanced order.
England. The age and range of children in each
group is shown in Table 1. Children in each educa- Happy drawing
tional group had either only attended mainstream Now, think of a man who is a very very happy man,
schooling or Steiner schooling within the UK. and who is very pleasant and friendly to everyone.
Draw the man, remembering what a happy person
Materials he is. Use just one of these colours. Draw the whole
10 block crayons (red, orange, yellow, green, blue, man as well as you can and colour him in as well as
purple, pink, white, brown and black) and A4 paper you can.

JADE 24.1 ©NSEAD 2005


76 Table 2: Mean surface area (cm2) and standard deviations (SD) for each drawing
Esther Burkitt, type for each educational group
Martyn Barrett
and Alyson Davis
Drawing Type
Baseline Happy Sad Grand Means
Mainstream 51.05 68.45 52.68 57.39
(N=44) (SD=43.6) (SD=48.99) (SD=42.92) (SD=36.97)
Steiner 26.57 34.55 21.92 27.68
(N=32) (SD=20.28) (SD=25.83) (SD=22.56) (SD=20.53)
Grand Means 40.74 54.18 39.73 44.88
(N=76) (SD=37.54) (SD=43.99) (SD=38.73) (SD=34.29)

The child’s second drawing was removed, leav- Discrepancies were resolved by recounting by
ing the crayons in place, and a new plain piece of both judges, and all drawings were included in
paper was provided. the analysis.

Sad drawing Results: Summary of main findings


Now, think of a man who is a very very sad man, and The data from the mainstream and Steiner
who is very mean and unfriendly to everyone. Draw groups were compared to examine the effects of
the man, remembering what a sad person he is. Use different educational background, whilst
just one of these colours. Draw the whole man as receiving happy and sad affective topic charac-
well as you can and colour him in as well as you can. terisations, and using block crayons to complete
the drawings. All children successfully completed
Drawing ability the range of tasks. Drawing ability was analysed
Children’s drawing ability was measured by class using a 2 (age group) x 2 (educational group) x
teachers using the following five-point scale: (drawing ability) repeated measures ANOVA, and
Please rate (child’s name) level of drawing ability no main or interaction effects were found. This
compared with the average drawing ability of the factor was thus excluded from further analysis.
year group. The data were screened for possible order
effects, and none were found. Thus, order was
Very below average also excluded from the following analyses.
Below average
Average Drawing size for each drawing type and
Above average educational group
Very above average Surface area (see Appendix 1 for statistical
analysis)
Measurements Overall, as shown in Table 2, happy drawings were
Drawing height and width were measured in larger than both the baseline and sad drawings
centimetres from the furthest vertical and hori- from children from both educational backgrounds
zontal points. Surface area was measured using and children in the mainstream group produced
centimetre grid squares. A second judge larger drawings overall than the Steiner group.
measured the surface area of all of the drawings
from each age group. 97% inter-judge reliability
was obtained for the Steiner group’s drawings.

JADE 24.1 ©NSEAD 2005


Table 3: Mean height (cm) and standard deviations (SD) for each drawing type for each age group 77
Esther Burkitt,
Martyn Barrett
Drawing Type Grand Mean and Alyson Davis
Baseline Happy Sad
Mainstream 51.05 68.45 52.68 57.39
(N=44) (SD=43.6) (SD=48.99) (SD=42.92) (SD=36.97)
Steiner 26.57 34.55 21.92 27.68
(N=32) (SD=20.28) (SD=25.83) (SD=22.56) (SD=20.53)
Grand Means 40.74 54.18 39.73 44.88
(N=76) (SD=37.54) (SD=43.99) (SD=38.73) (SD=34.29)

Youngest 15.47 18.02 13.87 15.79


(N=38) (SD=7.32) (SD=7.94) (SD=7.44) (SD=7.14)
Oldest 14.34 15.87 16.02 15.41
(N=38) (SD=5.95) (SD=6.19) (SD=6.99) (SD=5.59)
Grand Means 14.91 16.95 14.95 15.60
(N=76) (SD=6.65) (SD=7.16) (SD=7.25) 6.37

Height (see Appendix 2 for statistical analysis) dimensions in the correspondence analyses
As shown in Table 3, only the youngest group comparing the younger and older children’s draw-
produced happy drawings that were significantly ings. There were also no significant dimensions
taller than the baseline and sad drawings. In addi- in the correspondence analyses comparing the
tion, the youngest age group drew taller drawings mainstream and Steiner children’s baseline and
than the oldest age group for the baseline and happy drawings. However, two significant dimen-
happy drawings. There was one key difference sions were found in the correspondence analysis
between the drawings of children from the two comparing the mainstream and Steiner children’s
educational groups, namely, the mainstream chil- drawings of sad men (c2(10)=25.05, p<0.05;
dren produced taller drawings overall than the c2(7)=18.95, p<0.05). As shown in Figure 1,
Steiner group. green, orange and blue were more closely asso-
ciated with the mainstream children’s sad men
Width (see Appendix 3 for statistical analysis) than with the Steiner children’s sad men, and
It was found that the happy drawings were signif- yellow was more closely associated with the
icantly wider than both the sad and baseline Steiner children’s sad men than the mainstream
drawings. It was also found that children in the children’s sad men.
mainstream group produced wider drawings
overall than children in the Steiner group. Discussion
Overall, the children increased the surface area of
Children’s colour choices for the three the happy men from baseline size but did not
drawing tasks reduce the size of the sad men from the baseline
Children’s responses for each drawing type were figure size. Only the youngest children drew
analysed separately using correspondence analy- happy men taller than sad men. This was found
sis across each educational group and each age for both mainstream and Steiner children, indi-
group (see Appendix 4 for a description of the cating that this pattern of size change may occur
statistical procedure). There were no significant irrespective of educational background. This

JADE 24.1 ©NSEAD 2005


78 group influenced the size of children’s drawings,
Esther Burkitt, but independently from the specific type of affec-
Martyn Barrett
and Alyson Davis tive characterisation describing the figures. The
mainstream children produced larger, taller and
wider figures overall than the Steiner children.
The Steiner schooling system is based on person-
centred and child-centred activities, and on the
basis of Aronsson & Andersson’s findings [37], it
might have been anticipated that this would
study has therefore confirmed that size changes encourage the Steiner children to draw larger
following positive figure characterisation are figures, if size changes are related to the exag-
more consistent than size changes following geration of culturally more salient figures. This
negative figure characterisation. This is in line was not the case, however, suggesting that picto-
with previous research attesting to positively rial conventions of using size to signal the
salient characters being drawn larger than base- different significance of figures is not encouraged
line figures [34]. However, the findings offer no in the Steiner system. Perhaps other drawing
support for research which suggests that nega- strategies, such as colour use, are taught to fulfil
tively salient figures are minimised in children’s that purpose instead. This could be the case in
drawings [35], casting doubt on the claim that that, during the age period under study, an
children operate either on a pictorial convention emphasis on outlining is deliberately omitted
or on the basis of a defensive mechanism [36] from the Steiner system, a strategy which largely
when depicting characters which they hold in a determines drawing size and which possibly
negative regard. resulted in the mainstream children drawing
It is interesting to consider why this pattern of larger figures overall.
scaling effects is found with children who have Another possible explanation for mainstream
received different types of art education. If chil- children producing larger drawings may be due
dren are responding from an acquired pictorial to the fact that mainstream children are encour-
convention, they may have acquired the conven- aged to include details in their work to a much
tion either in the classroom, or from broader greater extent than Steiner children. Even though
cultural and familial influences. Even though chil- supplementary analyses (not reported here)
dren in the present study had been screened for revealed no statistically significant differences
attendance exclusively of mainstream or Steiner between the two educational groups for detail
schooling, this does not rule out the possibility use overall, the mainstream children may have
that children had been taught or learned certain been anticipating the inclusion of more detail than
principles at home, where there is less control they actually included, resulting in larger drawings
over the influences to which children attend. The relative to those from Steiner children. Clearly, a
consistency across educational groups could task-based measure would be needed to assess
also be attributable to influences of belonging to this possibility.
the same general culture, where children are As far as the use of colour was concerned, the
susceptible to similar media images and social correspondence analyses plots revealed that the
norms. It would therefore be interesting to exam- Steiner children’s responses were discriminated
ine whether children attending either traditional from the mainstream group by the colours
or non-traditional schools in non-Western cultures chosen for completion of the sad figures. Yellow
produce a similar pattern of results. discriminated the Steiner children’s drawings
There was some evidence that educational from the mainstream control group, where green,

JADE 24.1 ©NSEAD 2005


orange and blue were instead associated with the References 79
completion of sad drawings. This unusual asso- 1. Thomas, G. V. & Silk, A. M. J. (1990) An Esther Burkitt,
Martyn Barrett
ciation with a light colour for the negative figure Introduction to the Psychology of Children’s and Alyson Davis
differs from previous findings [38], and may be a Drawings. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester
consequence of the difference in educational Wheatsheaf.
background. Children have been shown to asso-
2. For example, Barrett, M. D. & Bridson, A. (1983)
ciate negative emotional qualities with yellow [39]
The Effect of Instruction upon Children’s
as well as positive emotional qualities such as
Drawings, British Journal of Developmental
happiness [40], and it may be that as Steiner chil-
Psychology, Vol 1, pp. 175–8; Cox, M. V. (1992)
dren receive more intensive experience with
Children’s Drawings. Harmondsworth: Penguin;
colours they were more sensitive to the negative
(1993) Children’s Drawings of the Human Figure.
properties of yellow compared with the main-
Hove: Lawrence Earlbaum; Freeman, N. H. (1977)
stream children.
How Children Try to Plan Drawings, in Butterworth,
Overall these findings suggest that different
G. [Ed.] (1977), The Child’s Representation of the
groups of children may well use different drawing
World. New York and London: Plenum Press, pp.
strategies to represent the emotional characters of
3–29; (1995) The Emergence of a Framework
drawn figures. This has implications both for
Theory of Pictorial Reasoning, in Lange-Kuttner, C.
parents and for professionals who use children’s
[Ed.] Drawing and Looking: Theoretical
drawings to gain insight into the child’s feelings
Approaches to Pictorial Representation in Children.
about figures in their lives. This research chal-
Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf,
lenges the assumption that art work is produced in
pp. 135–46.
the same way by all children. From this research, it
is evident that children encode emotional informa- 3. Hammer, E. F. (1958) The Clinical Application
tion through the use of both scaling and colour, yet of Projective Drawings. Springfield, IL: C. C.
the effects which do occur depend, at least in part, Thomas; (1997) Advances in Projective Drawing
on the type of education they are receiving. Interpretation. Springfield, IL: C. C. Thomas;
Although the number of Steiner children who Joiner, T. E., Schmidt, K. L. & Barnett, J. (1996)
were tested in the present study was compara- Size, Detail, and Line Heaviness in Children’s
tively small (the size of the Steiner group being Drawings as Correlates of Emotional distress:
restricted by availability and limited access), the (More) Negative Evidence, Journal of Personality
fact that statistically significant differences never- Assessment, Vol. 67, pp. 127–41; Malchiodi, C. A.
theless emerged shows that the effects which (1998) Understanding Children’s Drawings.
were identified are robust. The findings of this London: Jessica Kingsley.
study therefore show that the type of education
4. For example, McNiff, S. (1992) Art as Medicine:
which is received by children can indeed impact
Creating a Therapy of the Imagination. London:
upon the use of size and colour in their drawings
Piatkus.
of happy and sad men. This implies that other
potential differences in children’s drawings of 5. Joiner, T. E. et al, op. cit.
emotionally significant figures ought to be
6. Butler, S., Gross, J. & Hayne, H. (1995) The
researched, possibly through longitudinal stud-
Effect of Drawing on Memory Performance in
ies, to shed further light on how educational Opposite:
Young Children, Developmental Psychology,
systems, and the personal, domestic and broader Figure 1
Vol 31, pp. 597–608; Colour choices for
cultural factors thought to influence the impact of
the sad men drawn
education, can mediate children’s drawing devel- by the mainstream
opment in this area. and Steiner groups

JADE 24.1 ©NSEAD 2005


80 Gross, J. & Hayne, H. (1998) Drawing Facilitates 13. Freeman, N. H. (1980) Strategies of
Esther Burkitt, Children’s Verbal Reports of Emotionally Laden Representation in Young Children: Analysis of
Martyn Barrett
and Alyson Davis Events, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Spatial Skills and Drawing Processes. London:
Applied, Vol 4, pp. 163–79. Academic Press.

7. For example, McNiff, S. op. cit. 14. For example, Cox, M. V. (1992) op. cit; (1993)
op. cit; Thomas, G. V. & Silk, A. M. J. op. cit.
8. Machover, K. (1949) Personality Projection in
the Drawings of the Human Figure. Springfield, 15. For example, Crook, C. (1984) Factors
IL: C. C. Thomas. Influencing the Use of Transparency in
Children’s Drawings, British Journal of
9. Koppitz, E. M. (1966) Emotional Indicators on
Developmental Psychology, Vol 2, pp. 213–21;
Human Figure Drawings of Shy and Aggressive
(1985) Knowledge and Appearance, in Freeman,
Children, Journal of Clinical Psychology, Vol 22,
N. H. & Cox, M. V. [Eds], Visual Order: The Nature
pp. 466–9; (1968) Psychological Evaluation of
and Development of Pictorial Representation,
Children’s Human Figure Drawings. London:
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,
Grune and Stratton.
pp. 248–64.
10. Craddick, R. (1961) Size of Santa Claus
16. Pufall, P. B. (1997) Framing a Developmental
Drawings as a Function of Time before and after
Psychology of Art, Human Development, Vol 40,
Christmas, Journal of Psychological Studies, Vol
pp. 169–80.
12, pp. 121–5; (1963) Size of Hallowe’en Witch
Drawings prior to, on and after Hallowe’en, 17. Fox, T. & Thomas, G. V. op. cit.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, Vol 16, pp. 235–8;
18. Thomas, G. V. et al, op. cit.
Fox, T. & Thomas, G. V. (1990) Children’s
Drawings of an Anxiety-Eliciting Topic: Effect 19. Jolley, R. P. op. cit.
on Size of the Drawing, British Journal of Clinical
20. Burkitt, E., Barrett, M. & Davis, A. (2003a)
Psychology, Vol 29, pp. 71–81; Jolley, R. P. (1995)
The Effect of Affective Characterisations on
Children’s Production and Perception of Visual
the Use of Colour within Children’s Drawings,
Metaphors for Mood and Emotion in Line
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Drawings and in Art. Unpublished doctoral
Vol 44, pp, 445–55; (2003b) The Effect of Affective
thesis. University of Birmingham, Birmingham;
Characterisations on the Size of Children’s
Sechrest, L. & Wallace, J. (1964) Figure
Drawings, British Journal of Developmental
Drawings and Naturally Occurring Events:
Psychology, Vol 21, pp. 565–84; (2004) The Effect
Journal of Educational Psychology, Vol 55, pp.
of Affective Characterisations on the Use of Size
42–4; Thomas, G. V., Chaigne, E. & Fox, T. J.
and Colour in Drawings Produced by Children in
(1989) Children’s Drawings of Topics Differing
the Absence of a Model, Educational Psychology,
in Significance: Effects on Size of Drawing,
Vol 3, No. 24, pp. 315–45.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
Vol 7, pp. 321–31. 21. Burkitt, E. et al (2003b) op. cit; (2004) op. cit.

11. Machover, K. op. cit. 22. Burkitt, E. et al (2003a) op. cit; (2004) op. cit.

12. Cox, M. V. (1993) op. cit; Kellogg, R. (1969) 23. For example, Jolley, R. P. op. cit; Jolley, R. P.
Analyzing Children’s Art. Palo Alto, CA: Mayfield. & Thomas, G. V. (1995) Children’s Sensitivity
to Metaphorical Expression of Mood in Line
Drawings, British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, Vol 13, pp. 335–46.

JADE 24.1 ©NSEAD 2005


24. For example, Fortes, M. (1940) Children’s 29. Aronsson, K. & Andersson, S. (1996) Social 81
Drawing amongst the Tallensi, Africa, Vol 13, pp. Scaling in Children’s Drawings of Classroom Esther Burkitt,
Martyn Barrett
293–5; (1981) Tallensi Children’s Drawings, in Life: A Cultural Comparative Analysis of and Alyson Davis
Lloyd, B. & Gray, J. [Eds] Universals of Human Children’s Drawings in Africa and Sweden,
Thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
Iwawaki, S. & Vandewiele, M. (1989) The Human Vol 14, pp. 301–14.

Being as Drawn by Japanese Pupils Ages 7–12, 30. Cleeve, H. & Bradbury, R. J. (1992) Children’s
Cashiers de Sociologie Economique et Culturelle, Productions and Judgements of Drawings
Ethnopsychologie, Vol 12, pp. 119–42; Paget, G. W. of People of Different Importance. Poster
(1932) Some Drawings of Men and Women Made presented at the 5th European Conference
by Children of Certain non-European Races, of Developmental Psychology, Seville,
Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, September; Di Leo, J. H. (1973) Children’s
Vol 62, pp. 127–44. Drawings of Diagnostic Aids. New York:
Brunner-Mazel; Fox, T. & Thomas, G. V. op. cit;
25. Cox, M. V. & Bayraktar, R. (1989) A Cross-
Hulse, W. C. (1952) Childhood Conflict
Cultural Study of Children’s Human Figure
Expressed through Family Drawings, Journal
Drawings. Poster presented at the Tenth
of Projective Techniques and Personality
Biennial Conference of the International Society
Assessment, Vol 16, pp. 66–79; Thomas, G. V.
for the Study of Behavioural Development,
et al, op. cit.
University of Jyvaskyla, Finland, July; Dennis, W.
(1960) The Human Figure Drawings of 31. Steiner, R. (1974) The Kingdom of Childhood.
Bedouins, Journal of Social Psychology, London: Rudolf Steiner Press.
Vol 52, pp. 209–19.
32. Huchingson, R. & Huchingson, J. (1993)
26. Dennis, W. op. cit. Waldorf Education as a Program for Gifted
Students, Journal for the Education of the
27. Court, E. (1989) Drawing on Culture:
Gifted, Vol 16, pp. 400–419; Steiner, R. (1950)
The Influence of Culture on Children’s Drawing
The Spiritual Guidance of Man. New York: The
Performance in Rural Kenya, Journal of Art and
Anthroposophic Press; (1974) op. cit; (1985)
Design Education, Vol 8, pp. 65–8, Dennis, W.
An Introduction to Waldorf Education. London:
op. cit; Di Leo, J. H. (1970) Young Children and
Rudolf Steiner Press.
Their Drawings. New York: Brunner-Mazel;
Du Bois, C. (1944) The People of Alor: A Social- 33. Cox, M. V. (1992) op. cit; Congdon, K. G, &
Psychological Study of an East Indian Island. Congdon, D. C. (1989) A Systematic Approach
Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. to Art Education and Mental Health, Visual Arts
Research, Vol 12, pp. 73–9; Jalongo, M. R.
28. Cox, M. V. (1992) op. cit; Ross, D. D. (1982)
(1990) The Child’s Right to the Expressive Arts,
Art for Creative Education: A Basic in
Childhood Education, Vol 66, pp. 195–201;
Elementary Curriculum. Childhood Education,
Ross, D. D. op. cit; Rostan, S. M. (2000) A Study
Vol 59, pp. 22–7; Silverman, J., Winner, E.,
of the Emergence of an Artistic and Creative
Rosenstiel, A. K. & Gardner, H. (1975)
Identity. Poster presented at the 30th Annual
On Training Sensitivity to Painting Styles,
Meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Montreal,
Perception,Vol 4, pp. 373–84; Willats, J. (1977)
Canada, June.
How Children Learn to Draw Realistic Pictures,
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,
Vol 29, pp. 367–82.

JADE 24.1 ©NSEAD 2005


82 34. Aronsson, K. & Andersson, S. op. cit; Burkitt, (p<0.05) paired t-tests on the means in Table 2
Esther Burkitt, E. et al (2003b) op. cit; (2004) op. cit; Cleeve, indicated that happy drawings were larger than
Martyn Barrett
and Alyson Davis H. & Bradbury, R. J. op. cit; Craddick, R. op. cit; both the baseline and sad drawings.
Di Leo, J. H. (1973) op. cit; Fox, T. & Thomas, G. V.
A main effect was also found for educational
op. cit; Hulse, W. C. op. cit; Thomas, G. V. et al,
group (F(1,72)=16.44, p<0.05). This was a large
op. cit.
effect (η2= 0.67) with high observed power (P =
35. Craddick, R. A. op. cit; Fox, T. & Thomas, G. V. 1.00). The means displayed in Table 2 show that
op. cit; Hulse, W. C. op. cit; Sechrest, L. & the mainstream group produced larger drawings
Wallace, J. op. cit; Thomas, G. V. et al, op. cit. overall than the Steiner group. No additional
main or interaction effects were found.
36. Thomas, G. V. et al, op. cit.
Appendix 2
37. Aronsson, K. & Andersson, S. op. cit.
Statistical analysis for drawing height
38. Cf. Burkitt, E. et al (2003a) op. cit; (2004) op. cit. A 2 (age group) x 2 (educational group) x 3
(drawing type) three-way mixed ANOVA was
39. Alschuler, R., & Hattwick, L. A. (1943) Easel
conducted on the height scores, with drawing
Painting as an Index of Personality in pre-School
type entered as the repeated measure, and
Children, Journal of Orthopsychiatry, Vol 13,
independent measures on the other two factors.
pp. 616–25; Hammer, E. F. op. cit.
A main effect was found for drawing type
40. Winston, A. S., Kenyon, B., Stewardson, J. (F(2,144) = 7.50, p<0.01). The observed power
& Lepine, T. (1995) Children’s Sensitivity to was high (P = 0.94) and the effect size was
Expression of Emotion in Drawings, Visual Arts moderate (η2= 0.09). The means are shown in
Research, Vol 21, pp. 1–14. Table 3. Post hoc paired t-tests (p<0.05) showed
that the happy drawings were significantly taller
41. Hammond, S. M. (1988) CORRES 1.0:
than the baseline and sad drawings overall.
A Program for the Correspondence Analysis of
However, an interaction effect between drawing
Contingency Tables on IBM Compatible Micros.
type and age group was also found
Department of Psychology, University of Surrey,
(F(2,144)=7.23, p<0.05). The observed power
Guildford, Surrey; (1993) The Descriptive
was high (P = 0.93) and the effect size was
Analyses of Shared Representations, in
moderate (η2= 0.09). Post hoc paired t-tests
Breakwell, G. & Canter, D. [Eds], Empirical
(p<0.05) on the means in Table 3 showed that
Approaches to Social Representation, Oxford:
only for the youngest group were the happy
Clarendon Press, pp. 205–20.
drawings significantly taller than the baseline and
Appendix 1 sad drawings. Post hoc independent t-tests
Statistical analysis for drawing surface area (p<0.05) showed that for the baseline and
A 2 (age group) x 2 (educational group) x 3 happy drawings, the youngest age group drew
(drawing type) three-way mixed ANOVA was taller drawings than the oldest age group.
conducted on the surface area scores, with
A main effect was also found for educational
drawing type entered as the repeated measure,
group (F(1,72) = 45.15, p<0.01). This effect
and independent measures on the other two
was large (η2= 0.36) with high observed power
factors. A main effect was found for drawing
(P = 1.00). The mainstream group produced taller
type (F(2,144)=7.03, p<0.01). This was a
drawings overall (M = 18.90, SD = 5.77) than the
medium sized effect (η2= 0.09) with relatively
Steiner group (M = 11.06, SD = 3.17). No
high observed power (P = 0.60). Post hoc
additional main or interaction effects were found.

JADE 24.1 ©NSEAD 2005


Appendix 3 83
Statistical analysis for drawing width Esther Burkitt,
Martyn Barrett
A 2 (age group) x 2 (educational group) x 3 and Alyson Davis
(drawing type) three-way mixed ANOVA was
conducted on the width scores, with drawing
type entered as the repeated measure, and
independent measures on the other two
factors. A moderate (η2= 0.06) main effect
with high observed power (P = 0.80) was
found for drawing type (F(2,144) = 4.58,
p<0.05). Post hoc paired t-tests (p<0.05)
revealed that the happy drawings (M = 11.59,
SD = 5.17) were significantly wider than the
sad (M = 10.18, SD = 5.41) and baseline
(M = 10.53, SD = 5.14) drawings.

A main effect was also found for educational


group (F(1,44)=18.35, p<0.01). The effect was
large (η2= 0.20) with high observed power
(P = 0.99). Post hoc independent t-tests on the
means indicated that the mainstream group
produced wider drawings (M = 12.43, SD = 4.56)
overall than the Steiner group (M = 8.48,
SD = 3.53).

Appendix 4: Description of analytical


procedure
Correspondence analysis [41] uses well-
established geometric principles to provide
a pictorial representation of the relationship
between categories of response and groups
of individuals. It permits a multi-dimensional
analysis of categorical data by providing a
plot in which the geometric distance between
the groups and the types of response gives
a direct measure of the relative degree of
association between the groups and the
response types. Each drawing type was
then analysed separately by age group
and educational group.

JADE 24.1 ©NSEAD 2005

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi