Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 5

Behavior Therapy 44 (2013) 213 – 217

www.elsevier.com/locate/bt

COMMENTARY

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Empirical Considerations


Neil A. Rector
University of Toronto

COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL THERAPY (CBT), including


Cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT), including behavior behavior therapy, cognitive therapy, and their
therapy, cognitive therapy, and their integration, has integration, has evolved over the past four decades
evolved over the past four decades to become the most to become the most empirically supported psycho-
empirically supported psychological treatment for a range logical treatment for a range of psychiatric con-
of psychiatric conditions, spanning the preponderance of ditions, spanning the preponderance of Axis I
Axis I disorders, selected Axis II disorders, and a range of disorders, selected Axis II disorders, and a range of
associated clinical-health problems. The evolution of associated clinical-health problems. The evolution
cognitive-behavioral theory and treatment has followed a of cognitive-behavioral theory and treatment has
coherent scientific framework, first introduced in the followed a coherent scientific framework,
cognitive-behavioral modeling and treatment of depres- first introduced in the cognitive-behavioral model-
sion, to include: (a) systematic clinical observations, ing and treatment of depression, to include:
(b) definition and psychometric operationalization of key (a) systematic clinical observations; (b) definition
disorder-specific cognitive, emotional and behavioral and psychometric operationalization of key
constructs, (c) laboratory investigation of operationalized disorder-specific cognitive, emotional and behav-
disorder-specific processes, (d) development of compre- ioral constructs; (c) laboratory investigation of
hensive CBT treatment interventions to target the pro- operationalized disorder-specific processes;
cesses of empirically tested disorder-specific models, (d) development of comprehensive CBT treatment
(e) progression from early noncontrolled clinical outcome interventions to target the processes of empirically
studies to the development of sophisticated, large-scale tested disorder-specific models; (e) progression
randomized controlled trials testing disorder-specific from early noncontrolled clinical outcome studies
CBT interventions, (f) examination of disorder-specific to the development of sophisticated, large-scale
moderators and mediators of change in CBT treatment, randomized controlled trials testing disorder-
and (g) openness to refinements and elaborations based specific CBT interventions; (f) examination of
on empirical updates from experimental and clinical disorder-specific moderators and mediators of
investigations. change in CBT treatment; and (g) openness to
refinements and elaborations based on empirical
updates from experimental and clinical investigations.
Keywords: Acceptance and Commitment Therapy; CBT; A recent testament to the success of this scientific
cognitive therapy; causality; mediational analysis
framework has been its ability to accommodate
theoretical and clinical advancements emerging in
mindfulness-based and acceptance-based treatments
that emphasize skill development in the ability to
pay attention to present-moment experiences in
particular ways that promote openness, acceptance,
Address correspondence to Neil A. Rector, Ph.D., C.Psych.,
Sunnybrook Research Institute & Department of Psychiatry,
and reduced reactivity in ways that are distinct
Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, University of Toronto, 2075 from traditional CBT. Within this area, treatments
Bayview Avenue, Suite F327, Toronto, Ontario, CANADA M4N include Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR;
3M5; e-mail: neil.rector@sunnybrook.ca.
Kabat-Zinn, 1990), Dialectical Behavioral Therapy
0005-7894/44/213–217/$1.00/0
© 2011 Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies. Published by (DBT; Linehan, 1993), Mindfulness-Based Cognitive
Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Therapy (MBCT; Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2002),
214 rector

Meta-Cognitive Therapy (MCT; Wells, 2009), and symptoms, as in CBT, but learning to become
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT; Hayes, mindful and accepting of cognitions and symptoms,
Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999), the latter of which is and pursuing valued behavior.
outlined in the article by Hayes, Levin, Plumb, Although not limiting the ultimate feasibility or
Boulanger, and Pistorello (2013–this issue). A number efficacy of ACT, there is somewhat of a disconnect
of recent reviews have considered the similarities and between the theoretical and philosophical founda-
differences between CBT and ACT in terms of tions of ACT and its clinical strategies. Hayes and
theoretical and clinical approaches (Arch & Craske, colleagues are aware of this as they state, “behav-
2008; Hofmann, 2008; Hofmann & Asmundson, ioral principles are difficult to scale directly into
2008), and there are now three meta-analyses clinical work, and early bold attempts to do so
detailing the clinical efficacy of ACT (Hayes, were long put aside . . . the solution to this
Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006; Öst, 2008; conundrum is to develop clinically useful models of
Powers, Zum, Vording, & Emmelkamp, 2009). This pathology and treatment based on middle-level
literature has addressed conceptual and empirical terms that are not behavioral principles but are
issues pertaining to ACT and its relation to the based on them” (Hayes et al., 2013-this issue). The
broader CBT umbrella. The goal of this commentary lack of correspondence, however, between the
is not to rekindle these debates but rather to address theoretical underpinnings (RFT) and the clinical
conceptual issues that give direction for future approach in ACT raises the important question of
empirical study. whether the theoretical framework of ACT is
As outlined, ACT is a behavioral therapy that parsimonious and whether support for the “middle-
employs acceptance, mindfulness, and cognitive level terms” provides direct or indirect support for the
defusion strategies to enhance psychological flexi- foundational hypotheses of RFT.
bility and harness behavioral change in the direc- Mainstream cognitive-behavioral approaches
tion of valued goals. The theoretical foundation of have been faced with similar challenges in the
ACT is rooted in functional contextualism, “a type adequate operationalization of the theoretical un-
of psychological pragmatism that extends Skinner's derpinnings for certain disorder-specific treatment
radical behaviorism” (Hayes et al., 2013–this issue) interventions and showing the hypothesized rela-
that attempts to integrate cognition and language tion between theory and clinical practice. There
into a behavioral analytic framework. The theory have been, however, notable successes including
from which much of ACT arises, Relational Frame substantial empirical support for the hypothesized
Theory (RFT), focuses on how language impacts cognitive mechanisms in depression: (a) negative
cognition, emotion, and behavior, particularly the cognitive triad (negative view of self, personal
extent to which language gives rise to psychopa- world, and future), (b) negatively biased processing
thology in contexts in which valued behaviors are of stimuli, and (c) dysfunctional beliefs/schemas
restricted or abandoned to cope with cognitive (see Beck, 2008, for review).
activity. ACT is interested in understanding how Notwithstanding the empirical support that has
“psychopathology is caused in large part by the amassed over 40 years, recent research has demon-
tendency to become entangled in cognition, taking strated the need for refinements to the original
thoughts literally and remaining in a problem- model to capture broader aspects of cognitive vul-
solving mode even when that is not helpful” (Hayes nerability. For example, while Beck (1976) first
et al., 2013–this issue). From the ACT perspective, described the importance of distancing or decenter-
this attempt to respond to and control language ing from thoughts to gain a more objective
leads to experiential avoidance, attentional in- perspective on them in order to reduce dysphoric
flexibility, and reduced efforts to pursue valued reactivity, the importance of cognitive reactivity as
behaviors, collectively resulting in a state of an independent vulnerability marker for depres-
psychological inflexibility. Stemming from this sion has been fully appreciated only recently
conceptualization of psychopathology, ACT uses (Fresco, Segal, Buis, & Kennedy, 2007; Scher,
acceptance and mindfulness strategies (i.e., accep- Ingram, & Segal, 2005; Segal et al., 2006). These
tance, defusion, the now, self) and commitment experimental findings have also provided the
and behavior change processes (i.e., values and impetus for the development of novel treatment
committed action) to enhance psychological flex- approaches within a CBT framework that may
ibility. As outlined, “because of its bottom up, more explicitly and completely target cognitive
inductive nature, the ACT model is not a model of reactivity with mindfulness-based strategies to
any specific type of disorder, nor of a set of prevent depressive relapse (Segal et al., 2002).
techniques” (Hayes et al., 2013-this issue). Thus, MBCT employs mindfulness training to teach ways
the goal of ACT is not changing cognitions or of decentering without any explicit attempts to
act:empirical considerations 215

change cognitive content, and this approach has been results from these types of studies would provide
shown to significantly reduce depression relapse in insight into whether the foundational hypotheses
those with recurring (three or more) episodes of ACT, or the correspondence between the
(Teasdale et al., 2000). While initially conceptualized foundational hypotheses and ACT clinical process-
as a depressive relapse intervention, preliminary es, are of special importance.
research suggests that it may also impact on A further philosophical assumption of ACT is that
depressive symptoms in the acute phase of the all behavior is determined by the environmental
disorder, although additional clinical trial work is context with minimal recognition of the internal
required (Hofmann, Sawyer, Witt, & Oh, 2010). determinants of behavior. In an earlier response to
Similar to teaching decentering in MBCT, ACT radical behaviorism or contextualism, David Barlow
teaches defusion strategies to cultivate an open and (1997, p. 447) stated, “but at the very least, it seems
accepting attitude toward thoughts, without explicit to me that biological factors must be integrated into
attempts to change the content of thoughts (or other any comprehensive model of human behavior and
aspects of internal experience). From the ACT human behavior change.” Since then, there have
perspective, becoming entangled with cognition been watershed developments in the study of genetic
reduces opportunity for experiential awareness and contributions to psychopathology, Gene × Environ-
valued action. Examples of defusion techniques ment interactions, and clinical neuroscience. It
include thanking one's mind for a thought, watching would seem necessary for a modern clinical model
thoughts go by as if they were written on leaves of psychopathology to accommodate findings
floating down a stream, and repeating words out pertaining to the role of internal determinants of
loud until only the sound remains (Hayes et al., behavior. This is not only a task for ACT, but for all
2013-this issue). The goal then is not to question or psychological models of psychopathology, including
change thoughts but rather to cultivate mindful mainstream CBT. There have been new and exciting
tolerance and acceptance toward them, thus osten- results emerging from behavioral genetics, clinical
sibly bypassing the ruminative trap of cognition. Skill neuroscience, and cognitive-behavioral theories that
development in defusion and the other ACT clinical show opportunities for integrative programmatic
strategies has been shown in preliminary research to research. Studies have identified a key genetic
reduce features of depression in acute phase in- diathesis (e.g., the presence of the 5-HTTLPR short
terventions (Hayes et al.). allele) in the development of negative cognitions and
In this way, taking depression as an example, processes characteristic of depression (see Beck,
both MBCT and ACT teach new ways of paying 2008, for review), and neuroimaging research points
attention and relating to experiences—in ways that to improvements in neural aspects of affect and
mainstream CBT does not—and show promise to self-regulation in the mood and anxiety disorders
contribute to our effective treatments for depres- following CBT (see Frewen, Dozois, & Lanius, 2008,
sion. While they emerge from distinct theoretical for review). Hayes and colleagues (Fletcher, Schoen-
traditions, they converge in their use of mindfulness dorff, & Hayes, 2010) have recently acknowledged
strategies to change attentional capacities. In the importance of neurobiological features and
MBCT decentering strategies are explicitly devel- outlined a framework for the examination of the
oped to reduce cognitive reactivity, whereas in neuroscience components of ACT (i.e., mindfulness
ACT, defusion techniques are aimed at enhancing meditation) on the mid-level process of ACT (i.e.,
psychological flexibility. If future large-scale em- paying attention). Other non-ACT mindfulness
pirical studies of ACT demonstrate that it can studies have already begun to accumulate, demon-
successfully impact on the mid-level constructs, strating the interesting associations between mind-
such as psychological flexibility, in a way that fulness practice and increased cortical thickness in
mainstream CBT cannot, and these changes are in various parts of the brain (Lazar et al., 2005) and
turn found to mediate treatment response in a way mindfulness training and reductions in neural
that mainstream CBT cannot, then this finding dysphoric reactivity (Farb et al., 2010). Future
would contribute immensely to the scientific basis theory-driven neuroimaging studies of emotion
of ACT as a distinct treatment within CBT. Sim- regulation and the examination of neural changes
ilarly, studies that compare treatment outcomes in traditional CBT and mindfulness and acceptan-
and mediational effects between different mindful- ce-based therapies will contribute to understanding
ness and acceptance interventions for specific the distinction and overlap in etiological models and
disorders, for instance MBCT versus ACT for treatment processes in these approaches.
depression, would provide for a highly specific Beyond the use of neuroimaging approaches to
examination of the shared and distinct components distill differences between traditional CBT and
of these newer treatment approaches. Finally, the mindfulness and acceptance treatments, additional
216 rector

research is required to disentangle the differences instance, while this is addressed explicitly in ACT
between cognitive restructuring and cognitive through discussion and experiential exercises, it
defusion/decentering. Cognitive restructuring, like may be achieved more implicitly in CBT through
acceptance, requires the person to acknowledge clients conducting behavioral exposures and exper-
and deal directly with previously avoided or iments toward life goals and values (Arch &
suppressed cognitive material. Behavioral expo- Craske, 2008). A further assumption of the ACT
sures and behavioral experiments in CBT also aim model is that clients are more interested in pursuing
to facilitate a disengagement from repetitive valued goals rather than reducing the psychological
thinking and a reconnecting with experience in distress associated with the symptoms of clinical
vivo, suggesting a likely impact on maladaptive disorders (i.e., depression and anxiety). This is a
suppression tactics. It has been noted that both worthy idea for empirical testing. In short, the ACT
ACT and CBT require “additional thinking to not emphasis on values should be instructive to CBT
get tied up in thinking” and so both may be at risk investigators to more formally assess what has
of promoting maladaptive suppression in some remained an implicit, largely untested assumption,
cases (Arch & Craske, 2008). Further experimental that reduction of distress and symptoms of psycho-
work is required to assess the distinction between logical disorders naturally convert into improve-
cognitive reappraisal and mindfulness and accep- ments in quality of life. Future studies should aim to
tance emotion-regulation strategies in reducing examine the extent to which reductions in disorder-
suppression tactics and negative affect. In particu- specific symptoms and cognitions mediate improve-
lar, studies examining the timing and sequencing of ments in the person's quality of life at posttreatment
reappraisal and mindfulness and acceptance strat- and follow-up. In CBT, the hypothesized path would
egies could promote a more scientifically informed be from cognitive to symptom change to improve-
integration of these approaches in the treatment of ments in quality of life. In contrast, the hypothesized
specific conditions. Experimental studies could path in ACT would be from change in mid-level
examine the comparison of cognitive restructuring constructs (i.e., psychological flexibility) to improve-
versus cognitive defusion/decentering strategies in ments in quality of life directly, with symptom
negative mood priming designs. In the treatment change providing little or no mediation in outcomes.
context, given that both ACT and CBT incorporate A significant empirical basis is amassing for some
behavioral exposure, study designs comparing stan- of the “mid-level” constructs of ACT, the efficacy of
dard exposure therapy to exposure therapy plus ACT as a psychological treatment, and the purported
ACT components compared to exposure therapy mechanisms by which it works as outlined by Hayes
plus cognitive restructuring would provide an and colleagues (2013-this issue). In terms of treatment
empirical context to delineate the additive benefits efficacy, an initial meta-analysis of ACT in 12
of both treatments beyond the shared behavioral randomized controlled trials (RCTs) showed a
features. Additionally, studies of this nature would mean controlled effect size of d = 0.48 at posttreat-
allow for the examination of specific mediational ment. An independent meta-analysis by Öst (2008)
models while holding the behavioral component with 13 RCTs of ACT reported a mean controlled
constant. Studies are also required to test whether effect size of (Hedges's g) = 0.68. A still more recent
treatment nonresponders to one form of treatment meta-analysis by Powers and colleagues (2009) with
can achieve better clinical outcomes with an alterna- 18 RCTs with 917 patients showed a sustained
tive treatment. For example, a small open study on identical effect size of (Hedges's g) = 0.68. In sum,
social anxiety disorder has shown that patients studies suggest that ACT is superior to wait lists and
achieving partial response to cognitive-behavioral psychological placebos, although it may be the case at
group therapy subsequently benefit from the addi- the present time that ACT is not significantly more
tion of a mindfulness and acceptance-based inter- effective than established treatments, in general, nor
vention (Kocovski, Fleming, & Rector, 2009). superior to control conditions when examined in
In addition to cognitive defusion and mindfulness relation to specific clinical disorders, namely depres-
strategies, an important contribution of the ACT sion and anxiety (Hedges’ g = 0.03) (Powers et al.).
perspective is to highlight the importance of Hayes and colleagues note that Powers et al. enter
addressing values in treatment—the extent to disorder-specific distress as the primary outcome
which the clients have clarity of their goals and variable when it should have been considered
the degree to which their values are personally secondarily in tandem with the goals of ACT.
chosen. The treatment approach of ACT has more However, this objection does pose somewhat of
explicitly recognized the importance of values than a problem for the broader community of CBT
mainstream CBT. Yet, it may be that both researchers who focus on the successful treatment
treatments help clients achieve their life goals—for and relapse prevention of DSM diagnosable
act:empirical considerations 217

disorders. Hayes and colleagues state, “. . . scientific Frewen, P. A., Dozois, D. J. A., & Lanius, R. A. (2008).
politics and the dominance of mainstream measures Neuroimaging studies of psychological interventions for
mood and anxiety disorders: Empirical and methodological
may require ACT researchers to work both sides of review. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 228–246.
that street” (p. x; i.e., inclusion of diagnostic-specific Hayes, S. C., Levin, M. E., Plumb-Vilardaga, J., Villatte, J. L., &
symptom measures and ACT-process measures) and Pistorello, J. (2013). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy
we hope this will occur so that the full contributions and Contextual Behavioural Science: Examining the Progress
of ACT can be understood, assessed, and appreciated. of a Distinctive Model of Behavioural and Cognitive Therapy.
Behavior Therapy, 40, 180–198 (this issue).
To be sure, the careful examination of processes of Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J.
change and outcomes in treatment studies by (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes
ACT investigators is leading to a richer understanding and outcomes. Behavior Research and Therapy, 44, 1–25.
of the importance of such constructs as mindfulness, Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999).
acceptance and psychological flexibility in mediated Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential
approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford Press.
therapeutic outcomes. Hayes and colleagues note Hofmann, S. G. (2008). Common misconceptions about cognitive
ultimately, “whether the model succeeds or fails in mediation of treatment change: A commentary to Longmore
these areas is an empirical matter . . .” (p. x), and it is and Worrell (2007). Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 67–70.
here that we all find common ground. Hofmann, S. G., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2008). Acceptance
In summary, ACT is noted to emerge from a mindfulness-based therapy: New wave or old hat? Clinical
Psychology Review, 28, 1–16.
distinct theoretical and philosophical foundation Hofman, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., & Oh, D. (2010).
from mainstream CBT. Its emerging empirical The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and
basis, focused on clinical variables and processes depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting
that overlap with other empirically supported and Clinical Psychology, 78, 169–183.
mindfulness and acceptance strategies, suggests Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of
your body and mind to face stress, pain and illness. New York:
considerable promise in advancing CBT within the Delacorte.
scientist-practitioner model of psychopathology. Kocovski, N. L., Fleming, J. E., & Rector, N. A. (2009).
Future research will clarify the similarities and Mindfulness and acceptance-based group therapy for social
differences between ACT and mainstream CBT, as anxiety disorder: An open trial. Cognitive and Behavioral
well as the unique aspects among the different Practice, 16, 276–289.
Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C. E., Wasserman, R. H., Gray, J. R.,
mindfulness and acceptance-based treatments. Stud- Greve, D. N., Treadway, M. T., et al. (2005). Meditation
ies of mechanisms and the neurobiological correlates experience is associated with increased cortical thickness.
of change mechanisms within these approaches will Neuroreport, 16, 1893–1897.
continue to contribute to a better understanding of Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive–behavioral treatment of bor-
personal vulnerability and optimal strategies to derline personality disorder. New York: The Guildford Press.
Öst, L. G. (2008). Efficacy of the third wave of behavioral
alleviate human suffering. therapies: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Behavior
Research and Therapy, 46, 296–321.
References Powers, M. B., Zum, M. B., Vording, V. S., & Emmelkamp, P. M.
G. (2009). Acceptance and commitment therapy: A meta-ana-
Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2008). Acceptance and commitment lytic review. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 78, 73–80.
therapy and cognitive behavioral therapy for anxiety disor- Scher, C., Ingram, R., & Segal, Z. (2005). Cognitive reactivity and
ders: Different treatments, similar mechanisms? Clinical vulnerability: Empirical evaluation of construct activation and
Psychology: Science and Practice, 15, 263–279. cognitive diatheses in unipolar depression. Clinical Psychology
Barlow, D. H. (1997). It's yet another empirical question: Review, 25, 487–510.
Commentary on “can contextualism help?”. Behavior Segal, Z. V., Kennedy, S., Gemar, M., Hood, K., Pedersen, R.,
Therapy, 28, 445–448. & Buis, T. (2006). Cognitive reactivity to sad mood
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional provocation and the prediction of depressive relapse.
disorders. New York: International Universities Press. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 749–755.
Beck, A. T. (2008). The evolution of the cognitive model of Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. T. (2002).
depression and its neurobiological correlates. The American Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: A new
Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 969–977. approach to preventing relapse. New York: The Guilford Press.
Farb, N. A. S., Anderson, A. K., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A.,
McKeon, D., & Segal, Z. V. (2010). Minding one's Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse /
emotions: Mindfulness training alters the neural expression recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive
of sadness. Emotion, 10, 25–33. therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68,
Fletcher, L. B., Schoendorff, B., & Hayes, S. C. (2010). 615–623.
Searching for mindfulness in the brain: A process-oriented Wells, A. (2009). Metacognitive therapy for anxiety and
approach to examining the neural correlates of mindfulness. depression. New York: The Guilford Press.
Mindfulness, 1, 41–63.
Fresco, D. M., Segal, Z. V., Buis, T., & Kennedy, S. (2007).
Relationship of post treatment decentering and cognitive R E C E I V E D : July 16, 2010
reactivity to relapse of major depressive disorder. Journal of A C C E P T E D : July 23, 2010
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 447–455. Available online 25 May 2011

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi