Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 13

REVIEWS

THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE


OF REMEMBERING
Randy L. Buckner and Mark E. Wheeler
Remembering draws on a diverse array of cognitive processes to construct a representation that
is experienced as a copy of the original past. The results of brain-imaging, neuropsychological and
physiological studies indicate that distinct neocortical regions might interact with medial temporal
lobe structures to reinstate a memory. Frontal regions mediate the strategic retrieval attempt and
monitor its outcome, with dissociated frontal regions making functionally separate contributions
to retrieval. Parietal and frontal regions might supply a signal that information is old during the
process of retrieval, allowing us to perceive that reconstructed representations are memories,
rather than the products of new stimuli in the environment. Domain-specific cortical regions are
reactivated during vivid remembering and contribute to the contents of a memory. Here, we
describe how these regions interact to orchestrate an act of remembering.

Remembering the location of a parked car or reminisc- support the processes associated with retrieval
ing about a recently attended symphony event are both attempts? Clues have come from studies of patients
perceptions of the past that often include rich sensory with brain lesions. Patients with frontal cortical lesions
and contextual details of the original episode. For much often show retrieval difficulties, in particular when the
of the past century, remembering was considered taboo specific context (or source) of an episode must be
for scientific exploration because of unease about remembered or when minimal cues are provided to aid
exploring subjective phenomena. Progress in cognitive retrieval7,10–17. Gershberg and Shimamaura10, for exam-
psychology, neuropsychology and, more recently, brain- ple, asked patients with frontal lesions repeatedly to
imaging research, has provided experimental tools for study and recall lists of words or pictures, and found
the objective investigation of remembering, and provid- that the patients were significantly impaired at free
ed a means to link cognitive-level description with recall. Moreover, examination of the individual order-
underlying neural processes. Recent findings indicate ing of the recalled items indicated that frontal patients
that acts of remembering separate into component were not using subjective organizational strategies. That
processes that are subserved by dissociable brain regions. is, whereas healthy control subjects had a tendency to
In this review, we consider evidence for these separate consistently group certain words together across
neural components and how they might combine to retrieval trials (for example,‘spoon’ and ‘plate’), frontal
orchestrate an act of remembering. Our discussion is patients did so to a lesser degree, in essence recalling the
framed in terms of the strategic aspects of attempting to words in a more random fashion.
Howard Hughes Medical
Institute at Washington remember and the products of a memory attempt (for Another clue from neuropsychogical studies is
University, Department of more expansive models of retrieval, see REFS 1–9). from patients who confabulate during remembering
Psychology, One Brookings by falsely recalling details of a memory (what
Drive, Campus Box 1125, St Retrieval attempt Moscovitch7 calls “honest lying”). For example, during
Louis, Missouri 63130, USA.
Correspondence to R.L.B.
A familiar face or scene might spontaneously trigger a an interview, one patient with widespread frontal
e-mail: memory, but most acts of remembering begin with a damage7 was asked how long he had been married. He
rbuckner@artsci.wustl.edu goal-directed attempt to remember. How does the brain answered: “About four months.” Then asked how

624 | SEPTEMBER 2001 | VOLUME 2 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


© 2001 Macmillan Magazines Ltd
REVIEWS

The basic finding of frontal activation has been


generalized to retrieval of sounds, pictures and faces,
and during MEMORY TESTS of cued recall, free recall and
simple recognition23–30,182 (for reviews, see REFS 31–36).
A considerable challenge has been to specify the pro-
cessing contributions of these frontal regions to
remembering. Some broad principles have emerged,
Posterior: Anterior:
including the finding that multiple, distinct regions of
Frontal region property BA 44/6 BA 10
frontal cortex show functional dissociation during
Selective to remembering No Somewhat
Material dependent Yes No remembering31 (FIG. 1).
Tracks amount of effort Yes No In particular, posterior regions of frontal cortex near
Tracks retrieval success No Sometimes left-lateralized BA 44/6, and more ventrally near BA
Temporal profile Early Late
Sensitive to task Somewhat Very
45/47, have a general role in elaborating on verbal
information that includes, but extends beyond, tasks
Figure 1 | Multiple, functionally distinct frontal regions involving remembering, as described for the cued recall
are active during retrieval attempt and monitoring. The task above. Demb and colleagues37 found activation of
top figure shows two broad regions of frontal cortex that are
these frontal regions during a task in which subjects
associated with retrieval attempt during a prototypical verbal
retrieval task, with colour representing distinct functional classified words as representing either abstract (‘free-
properties, as outlined in the table below. Further functional dom’) or concrete (‘anvil’) entities. Tasks involving
subdivisions of left frontal cortex have been made that are elaborate word generation, word classification, verbal
not represented in the figure, including a prominent working memory and intentional memorization of ver-
distinction between more dorsal–posterior regions near bal material all activate these regions38–42. Remembering,
Brodmann area (BA) 44/6 and more ventral–anterior regions
HAEMODYNAMIC IMAGING
in many contexts, seems to tap into these processes. In a
near BA 45/47 (REFS 23,179,180).
METHODS similar manner, posterior right-lateralized regions of
Techniques used to measure frontal cortex become preferentially activated during
neural activity by monitoring many children he had, he responded,“Four … Not bad retrieval of non-verbal information43–45.
changes in regional blood flow.
for four months”, and later,“They’re adopted.” All four Posterior frontal participation during remember-
Positron emission tomography
(PET) measures blood flow children were his own, and he had been actively ing tracks the amount of cognitive effort exerted dur-
directly. Functional magnetic involved in their lives for the previous 30 years. On stan- ing a retrieval attempt. As an act of remembering is
resonance imaging (fMRI) dard tests of memory, another patient suffering right made more difficult by reducing the strength of the
measures oxygen concentration frontal damage repeatedly endorsed new items incor- original study episode, posterior frontal regions are
in the blood that relates to blood
flow. PET and fMRI have good
rectly as having been studied earlier18,19. This behaviour- required to a greater extent22,46,47. In one study46, sub-
spatial resolution but relatively al pattern is opposite to that typically observed in jects studied words under conditions of either highly
poor temporal resolution. memory loss, in which studied items are usually for- effective or minimal encoding that promoted high and
gotten, and indicates difficulties in implementing an low levels of retrieval, respectively. Activity in left-lat-
BRODMANN AREAS
appropriate retrieval strategy. From these findings, it is eralized posterior frontal cortex was strongest when
(BA). Korbinian Brodmann
(1868–1918) was an anatomist possible to speculate that frontal cortex makes a contri- subjects attempted to retrieve those words studied
who divided the cerebral cortex bution to retrieval that includes implementation and under minimal encoding conditions, tracking the
into numbered subdivisions monitoring during the retrieval attempt. amount of time (effort) that the retrieval trials
based on cell arrangements, HAEMODYNAMIC BRAIN-IMAGING STUDIES of memory required. Posterior frontal regions are also activated
types and staining properties
(for example, the dorsolateral
retrieval using both positron emission tomography independently of whether or not remembering is suc-
prefrontal cortex contains (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging cessful48,49. We can tentatively conclude that posterior
subdivisions, including BA 44, (fMRI) have almost ubiquitously shown activated frontal regions provide general processing resources
BA 45, BA 47 and others). regions in frontal cortex, confirming suggestions of their for the strategic elaboration required during a retrieval
Modern derivatives of his maps
participation from neuropsychological studies. Other attempt. This role is similar to what has often been
are commonly used as the
reference system for discussion regions of cortex have also been prominently activated termed ‘working memory’ or, in this context, ‘working
of brain-imaging findings. but, for our purposes, frontal regions are highlighted. A with memory’ (for a discussion of this issue, see REF. 50).
prototypical series of studies was carried out by Buckner To the degree that a retrieval attempt is engaged, these
MEMORY TESTS
et al.20 and Squire et al.21 using cued recall (see also REF. 22). posterior frontal regions will be recruited. As the
Formats used to test explicit
retrieval in the laboratory vary
Before imaging, subjects studied words such as ‘couple’, retrieval attempt becomes more difficult, they will be
in relation to how much ‘string’ and ‘abstain’. During imaging, the beginnings of recruited more extensively.
information is provided to aid the words were presented (‘cou’, ‘str’ and ‘abs’) and the Anterior frontal-polar cortex (near BA 10, often
retrieval. In free recall, items are subjects were instructed to recall the study words that right-lateralized) is also reliably activated during a
recalled in an open fashion
completed the stems. Activity changes occurred during retrieval attempt, but its role differs from that of posteri-
(“Recall the words from the
list.”). In cued-recall, item-by- recall in multiple frontal regions, including posterior left or frontal cortex in several ways (FIG. 1). Relative to poste-
item aids are given as cues frontal cortex, extending into dorsolateral prefrontal cor- rior frontal cortex, frontal-polar cortex is more selective
(“Recall the word that began tex and anterior frontal-polar cortex, near BRODMANN AREA for tasks that tap remembering, and is not always active
with cou.”). In recognition, the (BA) 10. Anterior frontal-polar activation also showed during elaborate verbal or non-verbal processing tasks51
full item is given and the test is
to decide whether the item was
selectivity — it was present during the recall task but (see also REFS 52,53). Moreover, frontal-polar cortex does
studied (“Was the word ketchup absent during additional conditions that required word not show increased activity as individual retrieval
presented earlier?”). completion without remembering. attempts become more effortful46 (but see REF. 21), but

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 2 | SEPTEMBER 2001 | 6 2 5


© 2001 Macmillan Magazines Ltd
REVIEWS

a Training Test Retrieval success


The goal of a retrieval attempt is to reconstruct a per-
ception of the past. If we assume that the structures dis-
KETCHUP
New
HOUSE
cussed above relate in some way to strategic aspects of a
retrieval attempt, other brain regions probably support
information about whether retrieval has been successful
and the episode-specific contents of a memory.
Old Structures in the medial temporal lobes have been
repeatedly implicated in retrieving recently learned facts
b and events61–63. In some studies, if a lesion is limited to
KETCHUP
the hippocampus, the ability to remember episodes
seems to be selectively impaired, with a preserved ability
to retrieve general facts64. Brain-imaging studies have
intermittently shown hippocampal activation during
c retrieval (for reviews see REFS 36,65–67), and have corre-
lated hippocampal activation with the level of retrieval
success68. Recently, a study by Eldridge et al.69 has indi-
cated selective activation of the hippocampus when
subjects report a distinct memory for an earlier episode,
as opposed to having a vague sense of familiarity (but
see REFS 28,70). On the basis of these kinds of observation
and animal studies of hippocampal lesions, several
0 600 Time (ms) models of retrieval have proposed that structures in the
medial temporal lobes rapidly bind neural representa-
tions associated with an experience to each other during
Figure 2 | Parietal cortex is associated with retrieval success. a | The basic experimental
paradigm used to study retrieval success. Subjects are asked to recognize previously studied old
memory formation, and then, for a period of time fol-
and new words. Those items correctly remembered as old are directly compared with rejected lowing acquisition, function to reinstate those represen-
new items to determine correlates of retrieval success. b | Functional magnetic resonance tations during retrieval71–74. Because a detailed account
imaging (fMRI) data from REF. 49 show differential activation associated with success over multiple of hippocampal participation in retrieval has recently
brain regions, prominently including left parietal cortex (arrow). c | Event-related potential (ERP) been published71, we focus this review on regions outside
studies have identified a related effect, showing a rapid positive waveform that develops over left the medial temporal lobes.
parietal sites during remembered old items (red) as compared with new items (green). Reprinted
with permission from REF. 3 © 2000 Elsevier Science. The correspondence between the fMRI and
One open question has been whether there are neural
ERP findings is intriguing, but should be entertained cautiously as their properties differ in certain changes in neocortex that signal successful retrieval of
regards (for example, contrast REF. 76 with REF. 181). mnemonic information. During an act of remembering,
we are usually aware that items being recalled are from
the past and do not arise solely from our immediate sur-
could be modulated by the subject’s expectations54. roundings or de novo from our imagination. In experi-
Activity in frontal-polar cortex generalizes across mental settings, such neural changes might provide infor-
retrieval of verbal and non-verbal information, tending mation that is useful for deciding whether a presented
to be right-lateralized even for verbal material31,34,43. item is old or new on a recognition test. Habib and
Finally, frontal-polar cortex shows atypically long Lepage75 explored neocortical changes associated with
responses in EVENT-RELATED FMRI studies48,55,56 that might retrieval success in a meta-analysis of PET data from five
have correlates in brain electrical activity as measured by studies. Across all included studies, measurements were
57,58
ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY (EEG) . made during blocks of old items, as compared with new
These counterintuitive properties have stumped any items. Items were presented visually or aurally, and were
simple interpretation. One possibility is that frontal- either words or pictures. Results showed a network of
polar cortex provides ongoing monitoring during the brain areas, including left parietal cortex, medial parietal
attempt to retrieve29,55,57,59. Alternatively, anterior cortex (near cuneus) and left anterior frontal cortex, near
EVENT-RELATED FMRI frontal-polar cortex might contribute to the high-level the frontal-polar region discussed above, that responded
A variant of functional magnetic
resonance imaging (fMRI)
monitoring required when a task demands multiple more to the blocks of old items.
methods that allows neural sub-goal processes. That is, certain kinds of task require Event-related fMRI studies have extended these obser-
correlates of individual trials or dynamic navigation between multiple goals52. vations by allowing individual items correctly identified
classes of trials to be isolated and Remembering might inherently rely on this form of as old (hits) to be compared with items correctly identi-
compared.
processing, with the rememberer continuously navigat- fied as new. This comparison directly targets processes
ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY
ing between information provided by cues present in associated with retrieval success. Several fMRI stud-
(EEG). A technique used to the environment and representations constructed from ies28,49,55,76,77 have identified the same basic network as
measure neural activity by memory (see also REFS 7,60). reported by Habib and Lepage75, and particularly high-
monitoring electrical signals In summary, the above results indicate that frontal lighted the role of left parietal cortex (FIG. 2). Moreover,
from the brain that reach the
scalp. EEG has good temporal
regions participate in strategic aspects of retrieval attempt activity in this network has been shown to predict, on
resolution but relatively poor with specific, dissociated regions making distinct average, whether a subject will correctly identify an old
spatial resolution. contributions. item on a recognition test78, and is sometimes present for

626 | SEPTEMBER 2001 | VOLUME 2 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


© 2001 Macmillan Magazines Ltd
REVIEWS

old items, even when a recognition decision is not ories varied depending on the region of cortex stimulat-
required76,79. Event-related potential (ERP) studies ed. Regions of superior and middle temporal lobes were
comparing remembered old items with new items have associated with auditory memories (“I hear singing …
shown a relatively fast (onset ~400 ms) positive wave- Yes, it is White Christmas”) whereas regions of more
form over left parietal cortex that is present for remem- posterior temporal and occipital lobes were associated
bered items and might parallel certain properties of the with visual memories (“… I saw someone coming
fMRI findings80 (for review, see REF. 58) (FIG.2). Across toward me as if he were going to hit me”). Although cer-
these studies, words, pictures and sounds have been tain aspects of these early studies have been re-exam-
used as stimuli, implying that the network participates ined102,103, they provided initial evidence for the idea that
generally in retrieval success. So, its participation in cortical regions associated with sensory processing are
retrieval is either not dependent on the episode-specific also associated with memory processes.
contents of a memory, or signals the retrieval of Crucial insights into brain regions supporting mem-
abstract forms of information that generalize across ory representation have come from studies of mental
numerous retrieval contexts. imagery. In a typical imagery study, subjects are asked to
These collective findings indicate a relatively fast signal imagine what an object or place looks like from their
in left parietal and associated cortex that correlates with general knowledge (for example,“Picture an elephant in
retrieval success. One speculation is that activity in these your mind” or “Picture the letters in the word HOUSE”).
cortical regions informs a person that something is from In other studies, participants are asked to construct
the past. It will be important to determine, in future stud- images based on specific, recently learned stimuli, paral-
ies, whether these cortical correlates of retrieval success leling in many ways an act of remembering. For these
are dependent on intact medial temporal structures and reasons, findings from studies putatively targeting
how findings integrate across methods. For example, an imagery relate closely to those exploring remembering of
open question surrounds whether lesions affecting these content-specific information. Furthermore, behavioural
left parietal regions that are associated with retrieval suc- analyses of imagery tasks have long indicated similarities
cess lead to changes in memory function. between imagery-based retrieval and stimulus-based
perception104–106 (see also REF. 87).
Retrieval content Assessments of visual mental imagery ability in
The identification of cortical regions that provide a patients with damage to visual cortex support the possi-
general signal associated with retrieval success raises bility that brain regions involved in perception are also
the separate question of how the brain represents the used during imagery and remembering107–111. Patients
episode-specific contents of a memory. Recollective with deficits in perceiving certain stimulus properties,
experience can include, for example, the face of a such as colour, form or spatial location, can also have
recently introduced person, the sound of his or her deficits in their ability to imagine that information when
voice, and the topic of the conversation. Recent theo- given verbal instructions108,110,111. These differing stimulus
ries have indicated that regions controlling the strate- properties are probably represented in different regions
gic aspects of retrieval, such as those discussed earlier, of visual cortex. For instance, Farah et al.109 showed that
are distinct from those that represent the remembered bilateral damage to temporal–occipital cortex resulted in
information81–84 (see also REF. 85), but the general con- decreased ability to imagine specific object features such
cept of top–down control is not new1. There is consid- as colour and size, but preserved imagery for spatial fea-
erable evidence from a variety of experimental ap- tures such as mental rotation and scanning. These two
proaches that certain regions of the brain that process components of visual information are processed in sepa-
incoming (bottom–up) perceptual information are rate (but highly interconnected) visual processing
also involved in representing that information during streams, with visual object information processed pri-
remembering84,86–96. We refer to this process as ‘reacti- marily in ventral occipital and temporal cortex, and spa-
vation’, similar to what William James1 called ‘re-exci- tial information processed primarily in dorsal occipital
tation’, as information associated during memory for- and parietal cortex112,113.
mation is reactivated during retrieval (see also REF. 97). However, the extent to which sensory regions sub-
The notion of reactivation also shares similarities with serve both perceptual and retrieval processes is not
the more recent cognitive theoretical framework of entirely clear from the neuropsychology literature.
transfer-appropriate processing, which postulates that Several individuals with brain damage have been
memory performance is influenced by the overlap described, who show impaired visual mental imagery,
between the specific processes engaged during memory but relatively preserved stimulus-based perceptual pro-
formation and retrieval98–100. cessing114. The opposite dissociation has also been
Early evidence that sensory regions are associated observed. Bartolomeo et al.115 describe a woman with
with memory retrieval was obtained by Wilder Penfield bilateral temporal–occipital lesions presenting a variety
and Phanor Perot101. Penfield electrically stimulated of visual processing impairments, including agnosia
regions of exposed cortex in awake human patients (the inability to recognize objects) and prosopagnosia
undergoing surgery for epilepsy, and found that stimu- (the inability to recognize faces), but who could imag-
lation of regions of occipital and temporal cortex would ine both objects and faces. This patient, remarkably,
sometimes elicit memories (as verified by the patient or could draw objects from memory, but failed to perceive
by witness), and that the sensory modality of the mem- their identity when presented with them at a later time

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 2 | SEPTEMBER 2001 | 6 2 7


© 2001 Macmillan Magazines Ltd
REVIEWS

Training Strong evidence for reactivation of sensory-specific


cortex during retrieval comes from studies using PET
and fMRI. Many studies in which subjects retrieve visu-
DOG al information result in activity increases in occipital
and temporal cortex88,95,122–136, whereas retrieval of audi-
tory information results in increased activity in superior
Test
and middle temporal cortex91,95,96,137,138 (see also REF. 139).
Wheeler et al.95 asked subjects to study a set of picture
and sound items extensively over several days, then test-
DOG + ed them on a SOURCE MEMORY TASK in which the subjects
were told to vividly recall the items and indicate
whether they had been studied as pictures or sounds. A
subset of cortical regions that were selectively activated
a b during perception of pictures and sounds were also reac-
tivated during retrieval of the same forms of informa-
tion (FIG. 3). A left-lateralized region along the fusiform
gyrus was associated with both perception and retrieval
of picture information, whereas bilateral superior tem-
poral regions were associated with both perception and
retrieval of sounds. A recent PET study showed that
c d reactivation might extend to the motor system140.
Subjects remembered study episodes in which physical
actions were performed. During retrieval of these
action-associated events, activation increased in regions
Figure 3 | Subsets of regions activated during perception
are reactivated during vivid remembering. A paradigm
of the motor system. In addition, many of these studies
used to explore modality-specific reactivation. Subjects study report involvement of frontal and/or parietal regions
words paired with either sounds or pictures (example shows a during retrieval, lending support to the idea that the
picture trial). At test, subjects are asked to remember whether regions involved in retrieval attempt interact with sen-
the words had been previously associated with pictures or sory and motor regions that are reactivated depending
sounds,encouraging retrieval of vivid, modality-specific on specific memory content.
memories. Brain images based on functional magnetic
Evidence for cortical representation of retrieval con-
resonance imaging (fMRI) show regions of the brain active
during perception of pictures (a) and sounds (b), and tent comes from SINGLE-UNIT RECORDINGS during paired-
subsequently during retrieval of the same pictures (c) and associate retrieval tasks in monkeys90,94,141,142 and imagery
sounds (d) from memory. Perception resulted in increased recall tasks in humans89. During paired-associate recall,
activity in visual cortex (from calcarine to fusiform gyrus) for one stimulus is arbitrarily made to predict another.
pictures, and from auditory cortex (near Heschl’s gyrus to Because single-unit responses in certain visual areas can
middle temporal gyrus) for sounds. Retrieval of pictures from
be highly selective for specific visual stimuli, single-unit
memory was associated with reactivation of visual cortex near
fusiform gyrus, whereas retrieval of sounds was associated with
recordings can be characterized in terms of their respon-
bilateral superior temporal gyrus near secondary auditory siveness to presented stimuli versus those absent but
regions. These results indicate that certain regions of sensory associated through learning. Sakai and Miyashita94 (see
cortex associated with perception are differentially reactivated also REF. 90) tested PAIRED-ASSOCIATE RECALL, and found ‘pair-
during retrieval of that information. Reproduced with permission recall’ neurons in inferior temporal cortex, the activity of
from REF. 95 © 2000 National Academy of Sciences, USA. which increased in the absence of their optimal stimuli if
SOURCE MEMORY TEST cues were presented that had been associated with their
A form of explicit retrieval test
(see also REFS 115–118). These findings indicate that per- optimal stimuli during learning (FIG. 4). Although not all
in which a specific attribute of
the study episode is queried ceptual and retrieval systems do not completely overlap, studies have found such effects143,144, these results indi-
(“Was the dog studied as a but do not address the extent to which they might share cate a mechanism, at the level of individual cell ensem-
sound or picture?”). certain levels of neural representation. bles, that might contribute to the representation of
EEG studies of retrieval tasks also support a reactiva- specific visual context during remembering.
SINGLE-UNIT RECORDING
A method used to measure the
tion hypothesis86,119–121. These studies indicate that neural How specific is controlled reactivation during
activity of individual neurons in activity in posterior regions of the brain increases when remembering? Human fMRI studies are particularly
awake, behaving animals. This people remember detailed perceptual information. For appropriate for this question, because they can simulta-
method has excellent spatial and example, Rösler et al.121 asked subjects to learn associa- neously survey indirect correlates of neuronal activity
temporal resolution, but can
tions between pictures and spatial locations, pictures and across multiple, distributed cortical regions in a sensory
only survey activity over small
numbers of neurons. colours, and pairs of words. Subsequent EEG recordings modality. Several recent fMRI studies in humans indi-
during memory tests showed maximal responses over cate a relatively high degree of specificity during reacti-
PAIRED-ASSOCIATE RECALL left frontal regions during word retrieval, parietal regions vation. Ishai and colleagues127 identified separate
A form of retrieval test in which during spatial location retrieval, and occipital and tem- regions of ventral visual cortex showing category-pref-
item pairs are studied
(“dog–cat”). At test, one member
poral regions during colour retrieval. These results indi- erential fMRI activity increases in response to faces,
of the pair is given to cue cate that different regions were reactivated depending on houses and chairs during perception. Recall on the basis
retrieval of the other (“dog”). the type of information retrieved. of imagining these objects produced significant increases

628 | SEPTEMBER 2001 | VOLUME 2 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


© 2001 Macmillan Magazines Ltd
REVIEWS

retrieved information) and differences in individual abili-


Cue optimal ties might account for the discrepant results123,128,147.
Roland and Gulyás92, and Hebb148, have suggested that
40 it is sometimes unnecessary to recruit early sensory
regions to recall information represented at later stages of
Control the sensory processing hierarchy. That is, to remember
0
what a dog looks like, it might be sufficient for retrieval
0 320 2320
Time from cue onset (ms) processes to reactivate late visual regions in which neu-
ronal activity codes the object representations of the dog,
and not earlier regions in which activity codes more
Paired associate
primitive visual attributes. This hypothesis is appealing,
10
because it suggests efficiency in the systems subserving
retrieval content. Reactivation processes cascade back-
Control
0 wards through sensory processing areas as is required to
TE 0 320 2320
represent the level of sensory detail in a memory, much as
Time from cue onset (ms) qualitatively different regions can be preferentially recruit-
ed as indicated by the studies discussed earlier127,134 (but
Figure 4 | Paired-associate recall tasks in monkeys might indicate single-unit correlates see REFS 128,149). It is also possible that increases in activity
of reactivation. The left panel shows diagrams of the ventral surface of the monkey brain showing in visual and auditory cortex during retrieval tasks are
area TE of the temporal lobe, in which recordings were made (red). On the right are recordings from modulated, to some degree, by attentional shifts in base-
a neuron that responds preferentially to a specific stimulus. The bottom graph shows the response line neural activity122,150–156 that interact with processes
in this neuron when a non-optimal stimulus is presented that had been paired, during learning, with associated with the retrieval of specific stimuli.
the cue-optimal stimulus. The neuron’s activity does not show an initial response to the non-
optimal cue, but does show a slowly developing response to the retrieved stimulus that might
Research into the neural mechanisms underlying
reflect a neuronal correlate of reactivation for the paired-associate. Adapted with permission from retrieval content faces a final, difficult challenge.
REF. 90 © 2001 American Association for the Advancement of Science. Memory content extends beyond simple sensory details
of an original episode to include abstracted forms of
representation, such as verbally mediated thoughts,
in activity across these regions. Importantly, the regions emotional content and even a sense of the personal per-
most associated with perception of one category were spective of the rememberer. Prominent theories have
also most associated with imagery from that category noted that these abstract forms of representation are
(FIG. 5). In another study, O’Craven and Kanwisher134 central to the experience of remembering1,4. Unfor-
found a similar dissociation using faces and buildings. tunately, we are largely unaware of their neural bases
Recalling the images of famous faces (cued by their (but see REFS 35,157–159). Nonetheless, the basic princi-
names) reactivated a portion of fusiform gyrus that was ples learned from the above discussions of how sensory
preferentially associated with perceiving faces, whereas details are remembered might extend to other forms of
recalling familiar buildings reactivated a portion of the information. That is, sensory systems present the most
medial temporal lobes, the parahippocampal gyrus, that approachable targets for initial exploration of retrieval
was associated with perceiving those same buildings. On content, because their properties are relatively well
a trial-by-trial basis, activity in fusiform and parahippo- understood. However, their contributions to remem-
campal gyri could often predict whether the subject was bering probably represent only a fraction of the overall
imagining a face or a building. Kreiman et al.89 showed distributed network reactivated during retrieval.
similar predictability for the specific visual content of a Frontal and temporal regions might reactivate to sup-
memory based on recordings from individual neurons in port the verbal and verbally mediated semantic con-
humans undergoing brain surgery. tents of memories, the amygdala might participate in
The extent to which retrieval signals traverse the visual the emotional content160, and so on. Collectively, these
system during remembering, from later to earlier process- widely distributed representations reactivated during
ing areas, is also a topic of debate. Results from several remembering might convey the vividness and richness
studies have indicated that reactivation effects can reach as that is experienced.
far back as calcarine cortex, at or near primary visual cor-
tex (the earliest processing stage in the cortical visual sys- Integration during remembering
tem)123,128,130,131,135,145,146. One fMRI study, in which subjects From the perspective of the rememberer, processes
imagined the appearance of visual objects they might associated with retrieval attempt and their various
encounter while walking though their hometowns, even products are probably experienced as one integrated
indicated reactivation of the thalamic region supplying memory4. The discussions above focus on manipula-
inputs to primary visual cortex (the lateral geniculate tions that pull apart component processes of remem-
nucleus) during visual recall123. On the other side of the bering, and provide us with some insight into their dis-
debate, many studies indicate that retrieval of detailed tinct properties. A remaining challenge is to understand
visual30,88,95,124,125,127,132,133 and auditory91,95,96,137 information how the component parts orchestrate an entire act of
can take place without robust activity in early sensory cor- remembering. It seems likely that the separate processes
tex. Experimental methodology (for example, choice of discussed earlier, and their neural mechanisms, act
control task), task demands (for example, detail of the interdependently during retrieval. In this regard, a

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 2 | SEPTEMBER 2001 | 6 2 9


© 2001 Macmillan Magazines Ltd
REVIEWS

a b Houses

Faces

Chairs

Figure 5 | Regions of visual cortex that respond preferentially to different kinds of objects during perception show
similar preference during imagery of those objects. a | Bilateral regions of ventral temporal cortex show modulation of activity
during perception of houses, faces and chairs. Within this general region, certain regions show preferential responses for the
different object classes. Perception of houses is differentially associated with increased activity in medial fusiform gyrus (green),
faces with lateral fusiform gyrus (red) and chairs with inferior temporal gyrus (blue). b | Modulation of activity during visual imagery
of houses (green), faces (red) and chairs (blue) is associated with regions preferentially activated during perception of each stimulus
form. Such specificity during imagery indicates that regions specialized for processing certain types of information during
perception might also, to some extent, be involved in reconstructing that information during retrieval. Reproduced with permission
from REF. 127 © 2000 Elsevier Science.

tentative model can be constructed on the basis of neural basis of remembering could arise, given the avail-
available data that describes how an act of remembering able data. Extending this tentative model, it is also possi-
might proceed. ble to speculate on how certain closely related cognitive
During successful remembering, top–down modu- experiences could differ from remembering.
lation from frontal cortex probably interacts with pos- An important distinction in cognitive theories of
terior neural representations of environmental cues to memory retrieval is between remembering and know-
trigger reactivation of the cortical networks that repre- ing161,162, or the related distinction between RECOLLEC-
163,164
sent a memory. The medial temporal lobe probably is TION AND FAMILIARITY (see also REF. 5). Decisions
required for certain forms of reactivation, perhaps about whether something is old or new are not always
through its parallel role in the rapid initial binding of supported by a fully developed recollective experi-
information into new cortical networks. As informa- ence. Behaviourally, the distinction is often tested in
tion reflecting mnemonic representations is realized, the ‘remember–know’ procedure, in which subjects
cortical networks involving parietal and frontal regions are given the opportunity to indicate whether a cor-
contribute to a general signal indicating that informa- rectly identified old item is simply known to be old, or
tion is old. Concurrently, reactivation of the domain- whether specific details associated with the item’s
specific contents of a memory draw on later stages of original presentation can be remembered162. In mem-
sensory processing that also encode such information ory tests, items are often correctly identified as being
during sensory and imagery processing. For example, old because of a vague sense of familiarity. This cogni-
visual regions in inferior temporal cortex will be reac- tive distinction might capture the degree to which
tivated to support the visual contents of the memory. retrieval content has been achieved during the process
Other regions, as yet poorly understood, support of retrieval — a distinction that yields qualitatively
abstract and verbal forms of retrieval content. It is different retrieval experiences. In situations in which
RECOLLECTION AND also likely that frontal cortex participates in the ongo- parietal and frontal regions signal that a perception is
FAMILIARITY
ing evaluation of the emerging products of the from the past, concurrent with reactivation of senso-
Theoretical memory processes
that are believed to contribute to retrieval attempt, and that the above processes are ry-specific and other cortex associated with retrieval
explicit retrieval. Familiarity extended, depending on the successes and goals of the content, the experience will be recollection. When
refers to the general sense that retrieval event. The operations of these interactive brain regions signal that a perception is from the past
something is familiar (old). processes are phenomenologically experienced as with minimal reactivation of cortex supporting re-
Recollection refers to retrieval of
specific details and the context
remembering. trieval content, the experience will be devoid of the
associated with an earlier The above model is incomplete and unlikely to be richness of a fully formed memory and experienced as
episode. correct in all details, but nonetheless illustrates how the familiarity.

630 | SEPTEMBER 2001 | VOLUME 2 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


© 2001 Macmillan Magazines Ltd
REVIEWS

Conclusions and future directions episodes in a manner similar to how selective attention
Cognitive neuroscientific exploration into remembering modulates perception of externally presented stimuli.
is just beginning, and goes forward with an array of Slow-wave EEG recordings57 and mixed-model event-
methods that can link neural systems to the cognitive related fMRI designs168 provide new tools that can sepa-
phenomenon associated with remembering. We have rate the neural correlates of ongoing mode-related
provided a summary of recent findings that seem to processes from transient neural changes associated with
indicate: first, specific dissociable regions of frontal cor- individual retrieval events. As another example, it is wide-
tex are involved in strategic aspects of retrieval attempt ly believed that acts of remembering unfold over time,
and monitoring; second, parietal and frontal regions with dynamic temporal interactions between brain
provide a general signal of retrieval success, perhaps regions having a central role. Glimpses into the temporal
indicating that information is old; and last, regions orchestration of retrieval processes have come from stud-
within sensory cortex reactivate to provide memory ies using EEG80,169,170 and single-unit recordings across
content during remembering. The picture is still rather multiple brain regions90. In addition, network analysis of
murky and the data are incomplete. brain-imaging data has indicated interactions between
Several large gaps in understanding remain. One regions during remembering136,171,172. Methods that com-
important future direction for research is to target how the bine techniques, such as methods based on electrical
neocortical processes described above interact with medial activity (magnetoencephalography and EEG) and
temporal regions that are associated with memory forma- haemodynamic methods (fMRI and PET)173–175, provide
tion and retrieval. For example, to what degree do neocor- considerable potential for widespread characterization of
tical correlates of retrieval success depend on the integrity the dynamic processes associated with remembering. A
of the medial temporal lobe? If they do, how do medial recent study has shown that it is possible simultaneously
temporal structures enable these neocortical signals to be to record single-unit activity and fMRI responses in mon-
associated with retrieval success? Answers to these ques- keys, providing another powerful tool for combining
tions might be clinically useful.Alzheimer’s disease can be methods with different spatial and temporal properties176.
predicted on the basis of structural changes within the Second, theories of remembering have often been
medial temporal lobe165,166, and many believe that func- explored in relative isolation from other fields of cogni-
tional changes precede the gross structural changes by tive neuroscience. To fully understand the contribution
several years. One strategy to detect the earliest stages of of a brain region or set of brain regions to remember-
dementia has been to measure medial temporal activity ing, it will be important to integrate studies across sub-
during memory processes using brain-imaging meth- ject areas. An obvious example relates to retrieval con-
ods167. However, structures within the medial temporal tent. Numerous studies have explored imagery and
lobe are relatively small and have been a difficult challenge perception. It would seem parsimonious that the role of
for imaging. If specific neocortical correlates of remem- a brain region in more than one kind of task derives
bering depend on the integrity of medial temporal lobe from a common process that is utilized across tasks.
function, these correlates might provide powerful mark- That is, understanding a brain region’s role in visual
ers for medial temporal lobe function, and their measure- perception might clarify its contribution to remember-
ment might predict the progression of dementia. ing. A similar idea applies to those brain regions dis-
Significant future progress is also likely to increase the cussed in terms of strategic aspects of retrieval.
sophistication of the link between neural correlates and Contributions of frontal cortex are not selective to
cognitive theories of remembering. Two distinct gaps can remembering38,42,43,177,178. By fully understanding how
be identified in this area. First, ideas about the component frontal regions guide executive control processes across
cognitive processes involved in remembering have multiple kinds of task, such as those classically defined
advanced beyond simple distinctions between retrieval as working memory tasks, we will gain insight into their
attempt and retrieval content1–9, and the methods that fundamental processing contributions and how these
can distinguish between these more detailed processes are processes control remembering.
just now being developed. In this regard, we can expect to
see significant progress as new methods are adopted. For
example, many theories of remembering propose that Links
adopting a preparatory cognitive state serves as a founda- DATABASE LINKS Alzheimer’s disease
tion for individual recollective experiences — what has MIT ENCYCLOPEDIA OF COGNITIVE SCIENCES Positron
often been termed ‘retrieval mode’4. Retrieval mode emission tomography | Magnetic resonance imaging |
might operate in the context of remembering past Wilder Penfield | Imagery

1. James, W. The Principles of Psychology (Henry Holt and Co., 4. Tulving, E. Elements of Episodic Memory (Oxford Univ. (eds Roediger III, H. L. & Craik, F. I. M.) 73–92
New York, 1890). Press, New York, 1983). (Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc., Hillsdale, New Jersey,
Although an older text, this classic is still the seminal A comprehensive cognitive framework for 1989).
text of psychology and should be read. Chapters 16 on understanding processes that are associated with 7. Moscovitch, M. in Varieties of Memory and Consciousness:
memory and 18 on imagination set the foundation for remembering. Essays in Honour of Endel Tulving (eds Roediger III, H. L. &
the modern cognitive neuroscience of remembering. 5. Gillund, G. & Shiffrin, R. M. A retrieval model for Craik, F. I. M.) 133–160 (Lawrance Erlbaum Assoc., Hillsdale,
2. Johnson, M. K. MEM: mechanisms of recollection. J. Cogn. both recognition and recall. Psychol. Rev. 91, 1–67 New Jersey, 1989).
Neurosci. 4, 268–280 (1992). (1984). 8. Mayes, A. R. in Memory: Systems, Process, or Function?
3. Rugg, M. D. & Wilding, E. L. Retrieval processing and 6. Ratcliff, R. & McKoon, G. in Varieties of Memory and (eds Foster, J. K. & Jelicic, M.) 130–161 (Oxford Univ. Press,
episodic memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 108–115 (2000). Consciousness: Essays in Honour of Endel Tulving Oxford, 1999).

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 2 | SEPTEMBER 2001 | 6 3 1


© 2001 Macmillan Magazines Ltd
REVIEWS

9. Burgess, P. W. & Shallice, T. Confabulation and the control A review of imaging studies of memory retrieval that functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval
of recollection. Memory 4, 359–411 (1996). highlights the dissociation between posterior and processes. Brain 121, 1985–2002 (1998).
10. Gershberg, F. B. & Shimamura, A. P. Impaired use of anterior left frontal regions associated with verbal This study shows that anterior frontal regions might
organizational strategies in free recall following frontal lobe retrieval, and between left and right posterior regions be sensitive to subject expectations during retrieval
damage. Neuropsychologia 33, 1305–1333 (1995). associated with verbal and non-verbal retrieval, tasks, and therefore indicates that their role extends
11. Incisa Della Rocchetta, A. & Milner, B. Strategic search and respectively. to high-level monitoring and/or initiation of retrieval
retrieval inhibition: the role of the frontal lobes. 32. Fletcher, P. C., Frith, C. D. & Rugg, M. D. The functional strategies.
Neuropsychologia 31, 503–524 (1993). neuroanatomy of episodic memory. Trends Neurosci. 20, 55. Henson, R. N., Rugg, M. D., Shallice, T. & Dolan, R. J.
12. Janowsky, J. S., Shimamura, A. P. & Squire, L. R. Memory 213–218 (1997). Confidence in recognition memory for words: dissociating
and metamemory: comparisons between patients with 33. Nyberg, L. Mapping episodic memory. Behav. Brain Res. right prefrontal roles in episodic retrieval. J. Cogn. Neurosci.
frontal lobe lesions and amnesic patients. Psychobiology 90, 107–114 (1998). 12, 913–923 (2000).
17, 3–11 (1989). 34. Tulving, E., Kapur, S., Craik, F. I. M., Moscovitch, M. & 56. Schacter, D. L., Buckner, R. L., Koutstaal, W., Dale, A. M. &
13. Jetter, W., Poser, U., Freeman, J. R. B. & Markowitsch, H. J. Houle, S. Hemispheric encoding/retrieval asymmetry in Rosen, B. R. Late onset of anterior prefrontal activity during
A verbal long term memory deficit in frontal lobe damaged episodic memory: positron emission tomography findings. true and false recognition: an event-related fMRI study.
patients. Cortex 22, 229–242 (1986). Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 91, 2016–2020 (1994). Neuroimage 6, 259–269 (1997).
14. Milner, B., Petrides, M. & Smith, M. L. Frontal lobes and the A landmark paper for the field because it highlighted 57. Düzel, E. et al. Task-related and item-related brain processes
temporal organization of memory. Hum. Neurobiol. 4, frontal contributions to memory processes including of memory retrieval. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 96,
137–142 (1985). retrieval. 1794–1799 (1999).
A seminal analysis of memory impairments in patients 35. Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T. & Tulving, E. Toward a theory of 58. Rugg, M. D. & Allan, K. in The Oxford Handbook of Memory
with frontal lobe lesions. Patients were found to be episodic memory: the frontal lobes and autonoetic (eds Tulving, E. & Craik, F. I. M.) 521–537 (Oxford Univ.
impaired on complex retrieval tasks involving consciousness. Psychol. Bull. 121, 331–354 (1997). Press, New York, 2000).
judgements of order and their frequency of 36. Desgranges, B., Baron, J. C. & Eustache, F. The functional 59. Ranganath, C., Johnson, M. K. & D’Esposito, M. Left anterior
occurrence. neuroanatomy of episodic memory: the role of the frontal prefrontal activation increases with demands to recall specific
15. Schacter, D. L. Memory, amnesia, and frontal lobe dysfunction. lobes, the hippocampal formation, and other areas. perceptual information. J. Neurosci. 20, RC108 (2000).
Psychobiology 15, 21–36 (1987). Neuroimage 8, 198–213 (1998). 60. Shallice, T. & Burgess, P. W. Deficits in strategy application
16. Shimamura, A. P., Janowsky, J. S. & Squire, L. R. in Frontal 37. Demb, J. B. et al. Semantic encoding and retrieval in the left following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain 114, 727–741
Lobe Function and Dysfunction (eds Levin, H., Eisenberg, inferior prefrontal cortex: a functional MRI study of task (1991).
H. M. & Benton, A. L.) 173–195 (Oxford Univ. Press, New difficulty and process specificity. J. Neurosci. 15, 61. Cohen, N. J. & Eichenbaum, H. Memory, Amnesia, and the
York, 1991). 5870–5878 (1995). Hippocampal System (MIT Press, Cambridge,
17. Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T. & Tulving, E. Frontal lobe 38. Gabrieli, J. D. E., Poldrack, R. A. & Desmond, D. E. The role Massachusetts, 1993).
damage produces episodic memory impairment. J. Int. of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory. Proc. Natl 62. Corkin, S. Lasting consequences of bilateral medial
Neuropsychol. Soc. 1, 525–536 (1995). Acad. Sci. USA 95, 906–913 (1998). temporal lobectomy: clinical course and experimental
A meta-analysis of 32 separate studies that examined 39. Kapur, S. et al. The neural correlates of intentional findings in H. M. Sem. Neurol. 4, 249–259 (1984).
whether patients with frontal lesions show memory learning of verbal materials: a PET study in humans. 63. Squire, L. R. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis
impairments. Contrary to conventional wisdom at the Cogn. Brain Res. 4, 243–249 (1996). from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol.
time, this extensive analysis showed that patients 40. Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., Posner, M. I., Mintun, M. & Rev. 99, 195–231 (1992).
with frontal lesions exhibited memory difficulties even Raichle, M. E. Positron emission tomographic studies of the An extremely thorough review of data that support a
for tests based on simple recognition. The size of the processing of single words. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 153–170 role for the medial temporal lobes in declarative
impairment was greatest for recall tests. (1989). memory function.
18. Schacter, D. L., Curran, T., Galluccio, L., Milberg, W. P. & 41. Wise, R. et al. Distribution of cortical neural networks 64. Vargha-Khadem, F. et al. Differential effects of early
Bates, J. F. False recognition and the right frontal lobe: a involved in word comprehension and word retrieval. Brain hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory.
case study. Neuropsychologia 34, 793–808 (1996). 114, 1803–1817 (1991). Science 277, 376–380 (1997).
19. Curran, T., Schacter, D. L., Norman, K. A. & Galluccio, L. 42. Braver, T. S. et al. Direct comparison of prefrontal cortex 65. Nyberg, L. & Cabeza, R. in The Oxford Handbook of
False recognition after a right frontal lobe infarction: memory regions engaged by working and long-term memory tasks. Memory (eds Tulving, E. & Craik, F. I. M.) 501–519 (Oxford
for general and specific information. Neuropsychologia 35, Neuroimage 14, 48–59 (2001). Univ. Press, New York, 2000).
1035–1049 (1997). 43. McDermott, K. B., Buckner, R. L., Petersen, S. E., Kelley, 66. Schacter, D. L. & Wagner, A. D. Medial temporal lobe
20. Buckner, R. L. et al. Functional anatomical studies of explicit W. M. & Sanders, A. L. Set- and code-specific activation in activations in fMRI and PET studies of episodic encoding
and implicit memory retrieval tasks. J. Neurosci. 15, 12–29 the frontal cortex: an fMRI study of encoding and retrieval of and retrieval. Hippocampus 9, 7–24 (1999).
(1995). faces and words. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 631–640 (1999). A comprehensive review of neuroimaging findings
21. Squire, L. R. et al. Activation of the hippocampus in normal 44. Wagner, A. D. et al. Material-specific lateralization of that detect activation of the hippocampus during
humans: a functional anatomical study of memory. Proc. prefrontal activation during episodic encoding and retrieval. memory retrieval.
Natl Acad. Sci. USA 89, 1837–1841 (1992). Neuroreport 9, 3711–3717 (1998). 67. Cabeza, R. & Nyberg, L. Imaging cognition II: an empirical
22. Schacter, D. L., Alpert, N. M., Savage, C. R., Rauch, S. L. & 45. Lee, A. C., Robbins, T. W., Pickard, J. D. & Owen, A. M. review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12,
Albert, M. S. Conscious recollection and the human Asymmetric frontal activation during episodic memory: the 1–47 (2000).
hippocampal formation: evidence from positron emission effects of stimulus type on encoding and retrieval. 68. Nyberg, L., McIntosh, A. R., Houle, S., Nilsson, L.-G. &
tomography. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93, 321–325 (1996). Neuropsychologia 38, 677–692 (2000). Tulving, E. Activation of medial temporal structures during
23. Petrides, M., Alivisatos, B. & Evans, A. C. Functional 46. Buckner, R. L., Koutstaal, W., Schacter, D. L., Wagner, A. D. episodic memory retrieval. Nature 380, 715–717 (1996).
activation of the human ventrolateral frontal cortex during & Rosen, B. R. Functional-anatomic study of episodic 69. Eldridge, L. L., Knowlton, B. J., Furmanski, C. S.,
mnemonic retrieval of verbal information. Proc. Natl Acad. retrieval using fMRI. I. Retrieval effort versus retrieval Bookheimer, S. Y. & Engel, S. A. Remembering episodes:
Sci. USA 92, 5803–5807 (1995). success. Neuroimage 7, 151–162 (1998). a selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval.
24. Andreasen, N. C. et al. Short-term and long-term verbal 47. Nolde, S. F., Johnson, M. K. & Raye, C. L. The role of Nature Neurosci. 3, 1149–1152 (2000).
memory: a positron emission tomography study. Proc. Natl prefrontal cortex during tests of episodic memory. Trends 70. Stark, C. E. & Squire, L. R. Functional magnetic
Acad. Sci. USA 92, 5111–5115 (1995). Cogn. Sci. 2, 399–406 (1998). resonance imaging (fMRI) activity in the hippocampal region
25. Fletcher, P. C. et al. Brain systems for encoding and retrieval 48. Buckner, R. L. et al. Functional-anatomic study of episodic during recognition memory. J. Neurosci. 20, 7776–7781
of auditory–verbal memory: an in vivo study in humans. retrieval. II. Selective averaging of event-related fMRI trials to (2000).
Brain 118, 401–416 (1995). test the retrieval success hypothesis. Neuroimage 7, 71. Eichenbaum, H. A cortical–hippocampal system for
26. Haxby, J. V. et al. Face encoding and recognition in the 163–175 (1998). declarative memory. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 1, 41–50
human brain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93, 922–927 49. Konishi, S., Wheeler, M. E., Donaldson, D. I. & Buckner, (2000).
(1996). R. L. Neural correlates of episodic retrieval success. 72. McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L. & O’Reilly, R. C. Why
27. Tulving, E. et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of retrieval in Neuroimage 12, 276–286 (2000). there are complementary learning systems in the
episodic memory: auditory sentence recognition. Proc. Natl An event-related fMRI study that shows clear hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the success and
Acad. Sci. USA 91, 2012–2015 (1994). correlates of retrieval success. failures of connectionist models of learning and memory.
28. Henson, R. N., Rugg, M. D., Shallice, T., Josephs, O. & 50. Moscovitch, M. Memory and working-with-memory: a Psychol. Rev. 102, 419–457 (1995).
Dolan, R. J. Recollection and familiarity in recognition component process model based on modules and central 73. O’Reilly, R. C. & Rudy, J. W. Computational principles of
memory: an event-related functional magnetic resonance systems. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 4, 257–267 (1992). learning in the neocortex and hippocampus. Hippocampus
imaging study. J. Neurosci. 19, 3962–3972 (1999). An insightful theoretical discussion of cognitive 10, 389–397 (2000).
An event-related fMRI study that shows clear neuroscience findings, relating strategic aspects of 74. Treves, A. & Rolls, E. T. Computational analysis of the role of
correlates of successful retrieval. This study is long-term retrieval to the online (working memory) the hippocampus in memory. Hippocampus 4, 374–391
particularly noteworthy in that several previous demands that support these processes. (1994).
studies failed to find such effects, probably because 51. Buckner, R. L. & Tulving, E. in Handbook of Neuropsychology 75. Habib, R. & Lepage, M. in Memory, Consciousness, and the
of methodological limitations. Vol. 10 (eds Boller, F. & Grafman, J.) 439–466 (Elsevier, Brain (ed. Tulving, E.) 265–277 (Psychology, Philadelphia,
29. Nyberg, L. et al. Functional brain maps of retrieval mode and Amsterdam, 1995). Pennsylvania, 1999).
recovery of episodic information. Neuroreport 7, 249–252 52. Koechlin, E., Basso, G., Pietrini, P., Panzer, S. & Grafman, J. 76. Donaldson, D. I., Petersen, S. E. & Buckner, R. L.
(1995). The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human Dissociating memory retrieval processes using fMRI:
30. Roland, P. E. & Gulyás, B. Visual memory, visual imagery, cognition. Nature 399, 148–151 (1999). evidence that priming does not support recognition memory.
and visual recognition of large field patterns by the human 53. MacLeod, A. K., Buckner, R. L., Miezin, F. M., Petersen, Neuron (in the press).
brain: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography. S. E. & Raichle, M. E. Right anterior prefrontal cortex 77. McDermott, K. B., Jones, T. C., Petersen, S. E., Lageman,
Cereb. Cortex 5, 79–93 (1995). activation during semantic monitoring and working S. K. & Roediger III, H. L. Retrieval success is accompanied
31. Buckner, R. L. Beyond HERA: contributions of specific memory. Neuroimage 7, 41–48 (1998). by enhanced activation in anterior prefrontal cortex during
prefrontal brain areas to long-term memory retrieval. 54. Wagner, A. D., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H. & Gabrieli, recognition memory: an event-related fMRI study. J. Cogn.
Psychon. Bull. Rev. 3, 149–158 (1996). J. D. E. Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory: Neurosci. 12, 965–976 (2000).

632 | SEPTEMBER 2001 | VOLUME 2 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


© 2001 Macmillan Magazines Ltd
REVIEWS

78. Sanders, A. L., Wheeler, M. E. & Buckner, R. L. Episodic 101. Penfield, W. & Perot, P. The brain’s record of auditory and 126. Goldenberg, G. et al. Regional cerebral blood flow patterns
recognition modulates frontal and parietal cortex activity. visual experience. Brain 86, 595–696 (1963). in visual imagery. Neuropsychologia 27, 641–664 (1989).
J. Cogn. Neurosci. (Suppl.), 50A (2000). 102. Bancaud, J., Brunet-Bourgin, F., Chauvel, P. & Halgren, E. 127. Ishai, A., Ungerleider, L. G. & Haxby, J. V. Distributed neural
79. Koutstaal, W. et al. Perceptual specificity in visual object Anatomical origin of déjà vu and vivid ‘memories’ in human systems for the generation of visual images. Neuron 28,
priming: functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain 117, 71–90 (1994). 979–990 (2000).
for a laterality difference in fusiform cortex. 103. Halgren, E., Walter, R. D., Cherlow, D. G. & Crandall, P. H. 128. Klein, I., Paradis, A.-L., Poline, J.-B., Kosslyn, S. M. &
Neuropsychologia 39, 184–199 (2001). Mental phenomena evoked by electrical stimulation of the Le Bihan, D. Transient activity in the human calcarine cortex
80. Wilding, E. L. & Rugg, M. D. An event-related potential study human hippocampal formation and amygdala. Brain 101, during visual-mental imagery: an event-related fMRI study.
of recognition memory with and without retrieval of source. 83–117 (1978). J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12 (Suppl. 2), 15–23 (2000).
Brain 119, 889–905 (1996). 104. Shepard, R. N. The mental image. Am. Psychol. February, 129. Köhler, S., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Houle, S. &
A defining paper for ERP studies of memory retrieval. 125–137 (1978). McIntosh, A. R. Networks of domain-specific and general
Dissociated components are identified that participate A thoughtful discussion of how memory images might regions involved in episodic memory for spatial location and
in early and late components of remembering. be represented within cognitive systems. object identity. Neuropsychologia 36, 129–142 (1998).
81. Damasio, A. R. Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: a 105. Paivio, A. Mental Representations: a Dual Coding Approach 130. Kosslyn, S. M. et al. Visual mental imagery activates
systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 1986). topographically organized visual cortex: PET investigations.
and recognition. Cognition 33, 25–62 (1989). 106. Kosslyn, S. M. Image and Mind (Harvard Univ. Press, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 263–287 (1993).
82. Fuster, J. M. The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1980). 131. Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., Kim, I. J. & Alpert, N. M.
and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe 107. DeRenzi, E. & Spinnler, H. Impaired performance on color Topographical representations of mental images in primary
(Lippincott–Raven, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, 1997). tasks in patients with hemispheric lesions. Cortex 3, visual cortex. Nature 378, 496–498 (1995).
83. Hasegawa, I., Fukushima, T., Ihara, T. & Miyashita, Y. 194–217 (1967). 132. Mellet, E. et al. Functional anatomy of spatial mental imagery
Callosal window between prefrontal cortices: cognitive 108. Farah, M. J. in Handbook of Neuropsychology Vol. 2 (eds generated from verbal instructions. J. Neurosci. 16,
interaction to retrieve long-term memory. Science 281, Boller, F. & Grafman, J.) 395–413 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1989). 6504–6512 (1996).
814–818 (1998). A comprehensive review of neuropsychological case 133. Mellet, E. et al. Functional anatomy of high-resolution visual
84. Kosslyn, S. M. Image and Brain: the Resolution of the Imagery studies and other data methods, suggesting that mental imagery. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 98–109 (2000).
Debate (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1994). imagery relies on separable subsystems that provide 134. O’Craven, K. M. & Kanwisher, N. Mental imagery of faces
A comprehensive investigation of the cognitive distinct forms of information during imagery and and places activates corresponding stimulus-specific brain
neuroscience of visual mental imagery and its relation remembering. regions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 1013–1023 (2000).
to visual perception. 109. Farah, M. J., Hammond, K. M., Levine, D. N. & Calvanio, R. A clear example of domain specificity in reactivation
85. Mesulam, M.-M. Large-scale neurocognitive networks and Visual and spatial mental imagery: dissociable systems of of retrieval content. Fusiform and parahippocampal
distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. representation. Cogn. Psychol. 20, 439–462 (1988). regions that show preferential activation during
Ann. Neurol. 28, 597–613 (1990). 110. Farah, M. J., Levine, D. N. & Calvanio, R. A case study of perception of faces and buildings, show similar
86. Fallgatter, A. J., Mueller, T. J. & Strik, W. K. Neurophysiological mental imagery deficit. Brain Cogn 8, 147–164 (1988). preferential activity during image-based retrieval of
correlates of mental imagery in different sensory modalities. 111. Levine, D. N., Warach, J. & Farah, M. J. Two visual systems faces and buildings.
Int. J. Psychophysiol. 25, 145–153 (1997). in mental imagery: dissociation of ‘What’ and ‘Where’ in 135. Owen, A. M., Milner, B., Petrides, M. & Evans, A. C. Memory
87. Farah, M. J. Psychophysical evidence for a shared imagery disorders are due to bilateral cerebral lesions. for object features versus memory for object location: a
representational medium for mental images and percepts. Neurology 35, 1010–1018 (1985). positon-emission tomography study of encoding and
J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 114, 91–103 (1985). 112. Felleman, D. J. & Van Essen, D. C. Distributed hierarchical retrieval processes. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93,
88. Knauff, M., Kassubek, J., Mulack, T. & Greenlee, M. W. processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex 1, 9212–9217 (1996).
Cortical activation evoked by visual mental imagery as 1–47 (1991). 136. McIntosh, A. R., Cabeza, R. E. & Lobaugh, N. J. Analysis of
measured by fMRI. Neuroreport 11, 3957–3962 (2000). A paper detailing a wealth of information on the neural interactions explains the activation of occipital cortex by
89. Kreiman, G., Koch, C. & Fried, I. Imagery neurons in the functional hierarchical organization of visual, auditory, an auditory stimulus. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 2790–2796 (1998).
human brain. Nature 408, 357–361 (2000). somatosensory and motor cortex in the macaque 137. Halpern, A. R. & Zatorre, R. J. When that tune runs through
A study of single-unit properties during retrieval in monkey. your head: a PET investigation of auditory imagery for
humans undergoing brain surgery. The results 113. Ungerleider, L. G. & Mishkin, M. in Analysis of Visual Behavior familiar melodies. Cereb. Cortex 9, 697–704 (1999).
indicate that specific neurons in the medial temporal (eds Ingle, D. G., Goodale, M. A. & Mansfield, R. J. Q.) 138. Rosen, A. C., Vaidya, C. & Gabrieli, J. D. E. Reactivation of
lobe selectively respond to the remembered stimulus. 549–586 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1982). encoding context during memory retrieval: an fMRI study.
90. Naya, Y., Yoshida, M. & Miyashita, Y. Backward spreading of 114. Farah, M. J. The neurological basis of mental imagery: a Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 30, 812 (2000).
memory-retrieval signal in the primate temporal cortex. componential analysis. Cognition 18, 245–272 (1984). 139. Martin, A., Haxby, J. V., Lalonde, F. M., Wiggs, C. L. &
Science 291, 661–664 (2001). 115. Bartolomeo, P. et al. Multiple-domain dissociation between Ungerleider, L. G. Discrete cortical regions associated with
91. Nyberg, L., Habib, R., McIntosh, A. R. & Tulving, E. impaired visual perception and preserved mental imagery in knowledge of color and knowledge of action. Science 270,
Reactivation of encoding-related brain activity during a patient with bilateral extrastriate lesions. Neuropsychologia 102–105 (1995).
memory retrieval. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 36, 239–249 (1998). 140. Nyberg, L. et al. Reactivation of motor brain areas during
11120–11124 (2000). 116. Berhrmann, M., Winocur, G. & Moscovitch, M. Dissociation explicit memory for actions. Neuroimage 14, 521–528 (2001).
A demonstration, using PET, that auditory areas are between mental imagery and object recognition in a brain- 141. Naya, Y., Sakai, K. & Miyashita, Y. Activity of primate
reactivated during retrieval of auditory information. damaged patient. Nature 359, 636–637 (1992). inferotemporal neurons related to a sought target in pair-
This study also shows that such reactivation might A case study of a patient (C.K.), who shows a association task. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93,
occur independently of whether subjects explicitly remarkable dissociation between visual perception and 2664–2669 (1996).
attempt to retrieve such information. imagery abilities. C.K. can draw objects from memory 142. Erickson, C. A. & Desimone, R. Responses of macaque
92. Roland, P. E. & Gulyás, B. Visual imagery and visual but cannot identify his own drawings at a later time. perirhinal neurons during and after visual stimulus
representation. Trends Neurosci. 17, 281–287 (1994). 117. Servos, P. & Goodale, M. A. Preserved visual imagery in visual association learning. J. Neurosci. 19, 10404–10416 (1999).
93. Rösler, F., Heil, M. & Glowalla, U. Monitoring retrieval from form agnosia. Neuropsychologia 33, 1383–1394 (1995). 143. Gochin, P. M., Colombo, M., Dorfman, G. A., Gerstein, G. L.
long-term memory by slow event-related brain potentials. 118. Jankowiak, J., Kinsbourne, M., Shalev, R. S. & Bachman, & Gross, C. G. Neural ensemble coding in inferior temporal
Psychophysiology 30, 170–182 (1993). D. I. Preserved visual imagery and categorization in a case cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 71, 2325–2337 (1994).
94. Sakai, K. & Miyashita, Y. Neural organization for the long-term of associative visual agnosia. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 4, 144. Sobotka, S. & Ringo, J. L. Investigation of long term
memory of paired associates. Nature 354, 152–155 (1991). 119–131 (1992). recognition and association memory in unit responses from
This seminal study reports the existence of ‘pair- 119. Farah, M. J., Péronnet, F., Gonon, M. A. & Giard, M. H. inferotemporal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 96, 28–38 (1993).
recall’ neurons in inferotemporal cortex of the Electrophysiological evidence for a shared representational 145. Le Bihan, D. et al. Activation of human primary visual cortex
monkey, in which activity correlates with learned medium for visual images and visual percepts. J. Exp. during visual recall: a magnetic resonance imaging study.
stimulus associates. Psychol. Gen. 117, 248–257 (1988). Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 90, 11802–11805 (1993).
95. Wheeler, M. E., Petersen, S. E. & Buckner, R. L. Memory’s 120. Ranganath, C. & Paller, K. A. Neural correlates of memory 146. Kosslyn, S. M. et al. The role of area 17 in visual imagery:
echo: vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific retrieval and evaluation. Cogn. Brain Res. 9, 209–222 (2000). convergent evidence from PET and rTMS. Science 284,
cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 11125–11129 (2000). 121. Rösler, F., Heil, M. & Hennighausen, E. Distinct cortical 167–170 (1999).
An indication, using event-related fMRI, that auditory activation patterns during long-term memory retrieval of verbal, 147. Sakai, K. & Miyashita, Y. Visual imagery: an interaction
and visual areas are reactivated during retrieval of spatial, and color information. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 7, 51–65 between memory retrieval and focal attention. Trends
auditory and visual information, respectively. (1995). Neurosci. 17, 287–289 (1994).
96. Zatorre, R. J., Halpern, A. R., Perry, D. W., Meyer, E. & Evans, An ERP study showing differences during the retrieval 148. Hebb, D. O. Concerning imagery. Psychol. Rev. 75,
A. C. Hearing in the mind’s ear: a PET investigation of musical of distinct forms of information. The paper uses a clever 466–477 (1968).
imagery and perception. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 29–46 (1996). item-association paradigm to derive these results. 149. Thompson, W. L., Kosslyn, S. M., Sukel, K. E. & Alpert,
97. Semon, R. The Mneme (George Allen & Unwin, London, 122. Buckner, R. L., Raichle, M. E., Miezin, F. M. & Petersen, S. E. N. M. Mental imagery of high- and low-resolution gratings
1921). Functional anatomic studies of memory retrieval for auditory activates area 17. Neuroimage 14, 454–464 (2001).
98. Blaxton, T. A. Investigating dissociations among memory words and visual pictures. J. Neurosci. 16, 6219–6235 (1996). 150. Kawashima, R., O’Sullivan, B. T. & Roland, P. E. Positron-
measures: support for a transfer-appropriate processing 123. Chen, W. et al. Human primary visual cortex and lateral emission tomography studies of cross-modality inhibition in
framework. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn 15, geniculate nucleus activation during visual imagery. selective attention tasks: closing the ‘mind’s eye’. Proc. Natl
657–668 (1989). Neuroreport 9, 3669–3674 (1998). Acad. Sci. USA 92, 5969–5972 (1995).
99. Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. P. & Franks, J. J. Levels of 124. D’Esposito, M. et al. A functional MRI study of mental image 151. Corbetta, M., Miezin, F. M., Dobmeyer, S., Shulman, G. L. &
processing versus transfer appropriate processing. J. Verb. generation. Neuropsychologia 35, 725–730 (1997). Petersen, S. E. Selective and divided attention during visual
Learn. Verb. Behav. 16, 519–533 (1977). 125. Goebel, R., Khorram-Sefat, D., Muckli, L., Hacker, H. & discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional
100. Roediger, H. L. I., Weldon, M. S. & Challis, B. H. in Varieties Singer, W. The constructive nature of vision: direct evidence anatomy by positron emission tomography. J. Neurosci. 11,
of Memory and Consciousness: Essays in Honour of Endel from functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of 2383–2402 (1991).
Tulving (eds Roediger III, H. L. & Craik, F. I. M.) 3–41 apparent motion and motion imagery. Eur. J. Neurosci. 10, Among the first PET studies of attention. This article
(Lawrance Erlbaum Assoc., Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1989). 1563–1573 (1998). extensively covers a series of selective-attention

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 2 | SEPTEMBER 2001 | 6 3 3


© 2001 Macmillan Magazines Ltd
REVIEWS

conditions that show domain-specific modulation of 164. Yonelinas, A. P. Components of episodic memory: the 174. Heinze, H. J. et al. Combined spatial and temporal imaging
visual areas. contribution of recollection and familiarity. Phil. Trans. R. of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans.
152. Kanwisher, N. & Wojciulik, E. Visual attention: insights from Soc. Lond. B (in the press). Nature 372, 543–546 (1994).
brain imaging. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 1, 91–100 (2000). A review of behavioural, neuropsychological and 175. Snyder, A. Z., Abdullaev, Y. G., Posner, M. I. & Raichle, M. E.
153. Kastner, S., Pinsk, M. A., De Weerd, P., Desimone, R. & neuroimaging results indicating that recollection and Scalp electrical potentials reflect regional cerebral blood flow
Ungerleider, L. G. Increased activity in human visual cortex familiarity make distinct contributions to retrieval. responses during processing of written words. Proc. Natl
during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. 165. Jack, C. R. J. et al. Prediction of AD with MRI-based Acad. Sci. USA 92, 1689–1693 (1995).
Neuron 22, 751–761 (1999). hippocampal volume in mild cognitive impairment. 176. Logothetis, N. K., Pauls, J., Augath, M., Trinath, T. &
154. Shulman, G. L. et al. Areas involved in encoding and Neurology 52, 1397–1403 (1999). Oeltermann, A. Neurophysiological investigation of the basis
applying directional expectations to moving objects. 166. Jack, C. R. J. et al. Rates of hippocampal atrophy correlate of the fMRI signal. Nature 412, 150–157 (2001).
J. Neurosci. 19, 9480–9496 (1999). with change in clinical status in aging and AD. Neurology 55, 177. Duncan, J. & Owen, A. M. Common regions of the human
155. Shulman, G. L. et al. Top–down modulation of early visual 484–489 (2000). frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends
cortex. Cereb. Cortex 7, 193–206 (1997). 167. Bookheimer, S. Y. et al. Patterns of brain activation in people Neurosci. 23, 475–483 (2000).
156. Gandhi, S. P., Heeger, D. J. & Boynton, G. M. Spatial at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. N. Engl. J. Med. 343, 178. Buckner, R. L. & Koutstaal, W. Functional neuroimaging
attention affects brain activity in human primary visual 502–503 (2000). studies of encoding, priming, and explicit memory retrieval.
cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 96, 3314–3319 (1999). 168. Donaldson, D. I., Petersen, S. E., Ollinger, J. M. & Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 891–898 (1998).
157. Levine, B. et al. Episodic memory and the self in a case of Buckner, R. L. Dissociating state and item components of 179. Buckner, R. L., Raichle, M. E. & Petersen, S. E. Dissociation
isolated retrograde amnesia. Brain 121, 1951–1973 (1998). recognition memory using fMRI. Neuroimage 13, 129–142 of human prefrontal cortical areas across different speech
158. Nyberg, L. et al. General and specific brain regions involved (2001). production tasks and gender groups. J. Neurophysiol. 74,
in encoding and retrieval of events: what, where, and when. 169. Allan, K., Wilding, E. L. & Rugg, M. D. Electrophysiological 2163–2173 (1995).
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93, 11280–11285 (1996). evidence for dissociable processes contributing to 180. Poldrack, R. A. et al. Functional specialization for semantic
159. Cabeza, R. et al. Brain regions differentially involved in recollection. Acta Psychol. 98, 231–252 (1998). and phonological processing in the left inferior prefrontal
remembering what and when: a PET study. Neuron 19, 170. Senkfor, A. J. & Van Petten, C. Who said what? An event- cortex. Neuroimage 10, 15–35 (1999).
863–870 (1997). related potential investigation of source and item memory. 181. Rugg, M. D. et al. Dissociation of the neural correlates
160. Phelps, E. A. et al. Activation of the left amygdala to a J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn 24, 1005–1025 (1998). of implicit and explicit memory. Nature 392, 595–598
cognitive representation of fear. Nature Neurosci. 4, 171. Nyberg, L. et al. Large scale neurocognitive networks (1998).
437–441 (2001). underlying episodic memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 182. Rugg, M.D. et al. Differential activation of the prefrontal
161. Gardiner, J. M. & Richardson-Klavehn, A. in The Oxford 163–173 (2000). cortex in successful and unsuccessful memory retrieval.
Handbook of Memory (eds Tulving, E. & Craik, F. I. M.) 172. Köhler, S., McIntosh, A. R., Moscovitch, M. & Winocur, G. Brain 119, 2073–2083 (1996).
229–244 (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 2000). Functional interactions between the medial temporal lobes
162. Tulving, E. Memory and consciousness. Can. Psychol. 26, and posterior neocortex related to episodic memory Acknowledgements
1–12 (1985). retrieval. Cereb. Cortex 8, 451–461 (1998). We thank L. Jacoby and H. Roediger for pointing us to relevant
163. Kelley, C. M. & Jacoby, L. L. in The Oxford Handbook of 173. Dale, A. M. et al. Dynamic statistical parametric mapping: literature. The Howard Hughes Medical Institute, the James S.
Memory (eds Tulving, E. & Craik, F. I. M.) 215–228 (Oxford combining fMRI and MEG for high-resolution imaging of McDonnell Foundation, the Alzheimer’s Association of America,
Univ. Press, New York, 2000). cortical activity. Neuron 26, 55–67 (2000). and the National Institutes of Health provided support.

634 | SEPTEMBER 2001 | VOLUME 2 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


© 2001 Macmillan Magazines Ltd
Copyright of Nature Reviews Neuroscience is the property of Nature Publishing Group and its content may not
be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi