Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 24

Psychological Bulletin Copyright 1982 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1982, Vol. 91, No. I, 3-26 0033-2909/82/9tO!-0003$00.75

A Self-Presentational View of Social Phenomena


Roy F. Baumeister
Case Western Reserve University

Self-presentation is the use of behavior to communicate some information about


oneself to others. The two main self-presentational motives are to please the
audience and to construct (create, maintain, and modify) one's public self con-
gruent to one's ideal. It is proposed that a wide range of social behavior is
determined or influenced by these self-presentational concerns. Research evidence
is examined to show the relevance of the self-presentational motives to giving
and receiving help, conformity, reactance, attitude expression and change, re-
sponses to evaluations, aggressive behavior, self-serving and counter-defensive
attributional statements, task performance, ingratiation, and emotion.

The present article interprets a series of wards by influencing an audience is mainly


social phenomena as caused or mediated by guided by the audience's criteria of favora-
two main self-presentational motives, the bility and can be conveniently designated as
motive to please the audience and the motive "pleasing the audience." On the other hand,
to construct one's public self. People use when self-presentation is guided by the de-
their social behavior as a means of com- sire to make one's public image equivalent
municating information about (or an image to one's ideal self, it is the ideal self that is
of) themselves to others. Such "self-presen- the criterion of favorability. The attempt to
tation" is aimed at establishing, maintain- make one's public self congruent to one's
ing, or refining an image of the individual ideal self will be designated here as "self-
in the minds of "others (cf, Goffman, 1959; construction."
Jones & Wortman, 1973; Schlenker, 1980). The attempt to please a particular audi-
The intention behind self-presentation need ence is a familiar connotation of "self-pre-
not be conscious, and the impression need sentation." The terms "impression manage-
not be accurate (either objectively or in the ment" and "ingratiation" are well suited to
self-presenter's own view). describe this form of self-presentation, The
There are two main reasons for engaging motivation may be ulterior, such as a par-
in self-presentation. The first is to obtain ticular benefit that the self-presenter wants
rewards. If an audience controls or dispenses from the audience; it may be a desire to be
one's potential rewards, one obtains the re- liked; or it may be the desire to win esteem,
wards by getting the audience to think fa- which may be beneficial in ways and situa-
vorably of oneself. The second reason is as tions that are not yet clearly foreseen. Ac-
a means of, substitute for, or prop to self- cording to Jones and Wortman (1973), self-
fulfillment. It has been claimed that people presentational ingratiation is an attempt to
are motivated to become their ideal selves reduce dependency. Thus, the defining char-
(Cohen, 1959; Rogers & Dymond, 1954). acteristics of audience-pleasing self-presen-
Along with the motivation to become one's tation are that it is an attempt to present
ideal self, there may also be a motivation to oneself "favorably" according to the audi-
convince others that one is like one's ideal ence's values, it is specific to a particular
self. audience, and it is motivated by some desire
Self-presentation designed to gain re- for rewards that the audience controls or
dispenses.
The author wishes to thank S. C. Berglas, D. K. Det- The desire to be one's ideal self gives rise
terman, J. G. Hull, E. E. Jones, and P. A. Parmelee.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Roy F. Bau- to motivations affecting both the private self
meister, Department of Psychology, Case Western Re- and the public self. It causes individuals to
serve University, Cleveland, Ohio 44106. want to believe that they are the way they
ROY F. BAUMEISTER

would like to be. It may also cause individ- cerned with the aggregate sum of other peo-
uals to want an audience to perceive them ple's impressions of him or her.
as being the way they would like to be. To The use of the term self-construction to
some extent, these motives are indepen- denote one kind of self-presentational motive
dent—satisfying one will not necessarily sat- does not imply that the self is created only
isfy the other. Still, there are presumably by impressing others. A "true," "real," or
some relations between the private self-con- "private" self is constructed not by self-pre-
cept and the public image of self. Jones's sentation but through one's choices and per-
(1964) concept of "signification" implies formances. Creating the self is a matter of
that if others perceive one as having a par- self-presentation only insofar as it is con-
ticular trait, this may help one to believe that cerned with establishing and maintaining
one does indeed have that trait. For present one's public self, that is, the image of oneself
purposes, the essential point is that people that exists in the minds of others.
desire to be perceived as congruent to their
ideal self-images. Operationalization
Thus, self-constructive self-presentation
derives from the motive to impress others in The most common procedure for testing
general (as opposed to a particular audience) for self-presentational motives is by com-
with one's good qualities, and these "good paring two situations that are identical in all
qualities" are defined by the self-presenter's respects except that some circumstance is
personal goals and ideals (rather than by the public in one situation but private in the
audience's wants and expectations). It prob- other. Typically, something that happens to
ably is much less susceptible to variation the subject will be known to other people
than is audience-pleasing self-presentation (public condition) or will be known only to
as a function of audience, of dependency on the subject (private condition); or, the sub-
the audience, and of situationally caused ject's behavior will be known to others, or
evaluation apprehension. Cross-situational not. If public awareness makes people change
consistency of self-constructive self-presen- their behavior, it is because they are con-
tation derives from the fact that one's (de- cerned with what their behavior communi-
sired) ideal self is relatively stable and un- cates to others. Similarly, anonymity re-
changing; audience-pleasing self-presentation moves one's concern with self-presentation
needs to be consistent only because of the because one's behavior cannot influence oth-
fear of being perceived as unreliable or hyp- ers' impressions if one is acting anony-
ocritical. mously. Another (although slightly con-
Moreover, the hypothesis of self-construc- founded) indication of self-presentational
tive self-presentation provides a basis for concerns is a change in behavior due to the
understanding audience transfer effects. Au- expectation of future interaction.
dience transfer effects may be defined as Failure of such manipulations to produce
effects in which the self-presentational con- an effect on any given social behavior con-
sequences and implications of a person's in- stitutes nonsupport of the hypothesis that
teraction with one audience lead to changes self-presentational concerns are at work. A
in that person's self-presentation to a second, word of caution, however, is needed. Labo-
different audience. One illustration of au- ratory subjects are generally distrustful of
dience transfer effects is the phenomenon of assurances of "privacy." They typically need
generalized image repair, in which damage extensive assurances of confidentiality in or-
to one's public self-presentation (such as a der to accept an experimental situation as
humiliation or embarrassment) is "repaired" private—which, after all, is quite justified
with a different audience. Thus, people may because it is rarely private in fact. It is often
feel that making a good impression on A necessary to be very thorough and explicit
may offset having made fools of themselves in assuring a subject that some behavior will
in front of B. Such a phenomenon can be be (and will remain) truly private.
understood as self-presentation only if one In addition, the experimenter constitutes
assumes that a person is fundamentally con- a real and important "public" to the subject.
SELF-PRESENTATION

If the subject believes that the experimenter ently altruistic behavior is often influenced
is aware of some fact, then that fact cannot by other factors such as the attractiveness
be termed private or confidential. Thus, for of the person requesting assistance (West
a "private" condition to be effective, the ex- & Brown, 1975) and whether the potential
perimenter must assure the subject that the helper is in a hurry (Darley & Batson, 1973).
circumstances are and will remain known Satow (1975) demonstrated the influence
only to him or her and not to anyone else, of self-presentational concerns on helping
including the experimenter. behavior. Subjects were asked for donations
Self-presentation can be concerned with that would be either public or private (secret
pleasing the audience when different audi- and anonymous). The public donations were
ences elicit different self-presentational be- dramatically greater (indeed, averaging over
havior (that is, behavior that differs in the seven times as large) than the private ones,
image or information it expresses); when the implying that altruistic behavior is at least
behavior varies in proportion to the individ- in part motivated by the desire to be rec-
ual's evaluation apprehension or need for ognized as a charitable and generous person.
approval as situational factors; and when the One of the broadest models of helping was
self-presentation appears to be caused by a proposed by Cialdini, Darby, and Vincent
dependence on the audience. In addition, (1973), who proposed that either a good or
situations that are unusual and unforesee- a bad mood can induce helping. They fo-
able may emphasize audience-pleasing rather cused on the bad moods, and their "negative
than self-constructive self-presentational con- state relief model regards helping as a way
cerns because they may not be relevant to of counteracting a bad mood by making one
the general public image that the person feel good about oneself. Recently, Kenrick,
desires to create. Normally, however, the Baumann, & Cialdini (1979) provided evi-
self-construction motive is relatively con- .dence that publicity is an important ingre-
stant for a given type of situation, and it dient in children's acquisition of this pattern.
explains self-presentations that are not at- They found that bad moods facilitated help-
tributable to other concerns such as pleasing ing behavior in children only when the help-
the audience. In particular, self-presenta- ing would be witnessed by an adult. An ear-
tions that could not be expected to please the lier study by Cialdini and Kenrick (1976)
audience (cf. the sections of this article on found some altruistic behavior under "pri-
reactance and aggression) can be attributed vate" conditions in adults. (One could argue
to self-constructive self-presentational con- that the privacy in that experiment was
cerns. Audience transfer effects are also ev- rather flimsy. Their experimenter repeatedly
idence that self-presentation is concerned discussed the donation with subjects and
with constructing and maintaining the gen- then left them alone for 90 sec to donate.
eral public image of self. The subjects could well have expected the
The following nine sections of this article experimenter to ask them again about their
examine topic areas in social psychology to donations.) Of course, Satow's (1975) find-
ascertain the importance of self-presentation ings indicated that publicity contributes to
in causing or mediating phenomena. altruism in adults, so Cialdini and Kenrick's
results do not disprove the importance of
self-presentation in adult altruism. Kenrick
Self-Presentational Concerns et al. concluded that "the negative state re-
in Specific Behavior lief model and the image repair model are
Altruistic Behavior: Helping and in no way incompatible" (p. 752), which the
Receiving Help present argument echoes.
Gottlieb and Carver (1980) noted that
Truly altruistic behavior would be moti- anonymity was a characteristic of many of
vated by concern for the well-being of others the experimental procedures that have dem-
and, by implication, indifferent to one's own onstrated the so-called bystander effect, in
potential gain and benefit. Research evi- which the presence of a multitude of on-
dence, however, has suggested that appar- lookers at an emergency actually decreases
ROY F. BAUMEISTER

the likelihood that anyone will intervene to constructing his or her public self than aware
aid the victim (Darley & Latane, 1968). of the immediate, short-term demands and
Gottlieb and Carver effectively counteracted expectations of the others who are present.
the bystander effect by eliminating this an- If emergencies are rare, opportunities to
onymity. In their experiment, as in the study do favors are common. Consequently, one
by Darley and Latane (1968), subjects were would expect self-construction concerns to
physically separated and were involved in a be more relevant to doing favors than to
discussion via an intercom system when they bystander intervention. Steele (1975) tele-
heard another subject (actually a tape re- phoned various homemakers to request a fa-
cording) apparently have a seizure and ask vor. For some, the request had been preceded
for help. In this group of physically isolated, by another (ostensibly unrelated) call during
unacquainted people, subjects offered help which the homemaker's reputation in the
more often and more rapidly when they ex- community was described in unfavorable
pected to meet and talk with each other later terms. This latter treatment had the effect
than when they expected to remain anony- of a significant and substantial increase in
mous. This finding implies that self-presen- the subject's willingness to perform the fa-
tational concerns may contribute to a by- vor. Despite the traditional wisdom that flat-
stander's decision about whether to intervene tery is the most effective means of gaining
in an emergency. compliance (cf. Jones & Wortman, 1973),
Schwartz and Gottlieb (1976, 1980) pro- subjects in this study complied more after
vided some evidence that the self-presenta- learning that their public images were bad
tional aspect of bystander intervention may rather than good. In a similar vein, Apsler
be concerned with pleasing the specific au- (1975) found that subjects were more willing
dience that is present (other possible by- to do someone a favor after they had been
standers). Their model held that evaluation publicly embarrassed. Although neither Ap-
apprehension influences the decision of sler nor Steele used an explicit public/pri-
whether to intervene. In support of this, they vate manipulation, their findings can be in-
demonstrated that cues about what others terpreted in terms of self-presentation. They
felt was apropriate behavior in the emer- imply that damage to one's public image
gency strongly influenced helping behavior leads to a motivation to perform a public,
(Schwartz & Gottlieb, 1976; see also Darley, socially desirable action. (Because of the
Teger, & Lewis, 1973). In a subsequent lack of a public/private manipulation, it is
study, Schwartz and Gottlieb (1980) added not clear whether the function of the al-
that when the audience appears to think truistic act is to restore a private mood, a
helping is not appropriate, removal of self- public image, or both.) It does not appear
presentational concerns (by anonymity) can to matter, however, whether the public that
facilitate helping. In that situation, pleasing requests or observes the altruistic act is the
the audience would presumably involve not same one that witnessed the humiliation. If
helping, and the relevance of self-presenta- the helping in the Apsler and the Steele stud-
tional concerns appeared to determine that ies is to be interpreted as self-presentation
behavior. (and the results of Satow and others do attest
The implication—that the self-presenta- to the relevance of self-presentation to help-
tional concern involved in bystander inter- ing behavior), then one must regard these
vention is to please the specific audience— as audience transfer effects. The image that
is consistent with Darley and Latane's (1968) is repaired is not the one held by the specific
discussion of emergencies. They pointed out audience but the individual's generalized
that most people rarely encounter actual public image. Thus, altruistic behavior in
emergencies. As noted earlier, long-term general may relate to concerns with self-pre-
(self-construction) self-presentation goals are sentation as self-construction.
not likely to encompass situations that are Another approach to the study of human
extremely unusual and unforeseen. Conse- generosity is to allow a subject to allocate
quently, a bystander in an emergency is less money among himself or herself and other
likely to be aware of the implications for subjects. Several studies have shown that
SELF-PRESENTATION

such allocations are influenced by whether and quite possibly the restructuring of atti-
they are made publicly or privately (Kidder, tudes by the recipient of help so that the
Bellettirie, & Cohn, 1977; Reis & Gruzen, latter comes to believe that the help was
1976; Shapiro, 1975). In particular, Reis deserved (Gergen, Morse, & Bode, 1974)
and Gruzen (1976) showed that subjects very likely spring from the humiliation (i.e.,
adjusted their allocations not merely ac- the self-presentational damage) experienced
cording to whether someone else would know by the recipient. By asserting that the aid
but also according to who would know. After was deserved, the recipient implies that he
a task performance, the subject was told to or she is not helpless or incompetent and
divide the money in any fashion among him- subsisting on others' handouts but rather is
self or herself and three other subjects who merely someone accepting what is rightfully
had contributed uneven amounts of work to his or hers.
the group's performance. When the subject Tessler and Schwartz (1972) showed that
had to report the allocations to the experi- subjects are more willing to seek help on a
menter, these allocations conformed to the task when the instructions establish a con-
norm of equity (payment proportional to in- text that enables them to save face, that is,
put). When the subject had to report them when failure in general is publicly ascribed
to the other subjects, the allocations con- to external causes. Although their proce-
formed to the norm of equality (equal pay- dures did not distinguish between public and
ment for all, regardless of performance). private concerns, their findings are consis-
When total confidentiality could be pre- tent with the self-presentational argument
sumed, the self-allocation made by the sub- advanced here. Broil, Gross, and Piliavin
ject tended to be disproportionately large. (1974) compared help that was offered with
Thus, norms advocating generous and fair help that was available but had to be re-
treatment of others are most scrupulously quested. They reasoned that asking for help
followed under conditions of public surveil- entailed the greater self-presentational risk
lance. Moreover, the audience-specific na- of humiliation, as when a person needing
ture of these results indicates that pleasing welfare assistance "associates asking for
the audience is a relevant self-presentational help with personal inadequacy [and] feels
concern. It seems possible that self-construc- that the social worker or others will view him
tion is also relevant to such behavior, but or her as incompetent and dependent" (p.
there is as yet no support for that hypothesis. 254), Subjects in their experiment received
Paulhus, Shaffer, and Downing (1977) more help and expressed greater attraction
furnished blood donors with pamphlets that to the helper when the help was offered than
described either the benefits of this act to when it had to be requested.
the donor or how charitable and humani- Finally, the importance of the "rec-
tarian blood donors were. Subjects in the iprocity" norm (Gouldner, 1960) in the psy-
latter condition expressed greater willing- chology of receiving aid may well also in-
ness to donate blood again than did those in volve self-presentation. The central finding
the former condition. This finding can be in this context is that people are more at-
interpreted in a self-presentational context: tracted to the helper when they are able to
People donate blood to get others to perceive reciprocate the help than when they are not
them as good people rather than for their (Gross & Latane, 1974). Why should the
own tangible benefits. Of course, their find- potential to reciprocate make receiving help
ing could also be interpreted in terms of pri- more palatable? Obviously, doing a favor for
vate egotism and self-esteem. one's former benefactor is unimpeachable
Self-presentation is apparently no less im- public evidence that one is not chronically
portant to the recipient of help than to the dependent and incompetent.
helper. Whereas the helper desires to be per- Receiving help is of special interest to self-
ceived as good and altruistic, the recipient presentation research because it appears to
of help desires to avoid being seen as depen- entail a conflict between long-term and
dent and incompetent. The frequent resent- short-term self-presentational goals. To the
ment toward the helper (cf. Gergen, 1974) extent that self-construction is dominant,
ROY F. BAUMEISTER

recipients of help normally desire to present viduation has thus been characterized both
themselves as not being weak, helpless, and by a reduction of inhibitions (Cannavale,
dependent; the immediate ulterior motive of Scarr, & Pepitone, 1970; Festinger, Pepi-
obtaining help, however, is best served by tone, & Newcomb, 1952; Zimbardo, 1970)
presenting oneself as weak, helpless, and and by a reduction of conformity (Maslach,
dependent. The evident importance of the 1974; Singer, Brush & Lublin, 1965).
former motives testifies that self-construc- There is a certain irony in this pattern of
tive self-presentational concerns are indeed findings. The term deindividuation connotes
relevant to the psychology of receiving help. a loss of individuality, and yet deindividua-
Many factors influence the giving and re- tion apparently increases the person's readi-
ceiving of help, including mood (Cialdini, ness to behave in an autonomous fashion,
Darby & Vincent, 1973), the type of request according to his or her own individual incli-
(Cunningham, Steinberg, & Grev, 1980), nations and beliefs rather than in conformity
personality factors (Tessler & Schwartz, with others. The root of this seeming con-
1972), and modeling (Bryan & Test, 1967). tradiction is that deindividuation does not
Some helping does undoubtedly occur in pri- deprive people of their individuality but
vate and anonymous situations (e.g., Hodg- rather of their public identity. The concern
son, Hornstein, & LaKind, 1972; Hornstein, over public identity is apparently an impor-
Masor, Sole, & Heilman, 1971) and thus tant factor in conformity and social inhibi-
has motivations other than the concern with tions.
making a good impression. Still, the impor- Of course, deviants are often censured and
tance of self-presentational motivations punished, whether in subtle or in overt ways
should not be overlooked. By engaging these (Darley & Darley, 1976; Freedman & Doob,
concerns, helping behavior can be increased; 1968; Schachter, 1951). Most people pre-
by respecting them, people can be induced sumably fear social rejection and punish-
to accept help more willingly and more hap- ment, and this fear may well contribute to
pily. their reluctance to go against the group's
expectations when the group is watching.
Conformity, Influence, and Reactance The emphasis here on self-presentational
concerns does not deny the importance of
It has long been known that people con- fears of rejection and punishment. It sug-
form more readily to the opinions and ex- gests only that in these various cases the
pectations of others when these others are person is typically not afraid of some specific
watching than when they are not (Argyle, danger or censure but is more generally
1957; Deutsch & Gerard, 1955). In their afraid of the variety of potential conse-
extension of the Asch (1951) conformity quences of having an unfavorable public
studies, Deutsch and Gerard (1955) com- identity. These potential consequences, of
pared public (face-to-face) and anonymous course, include overt negative sanctions by
situations in which subjects had to express the group as well as possible dislike by in-
judgments after learning that the rest of the dividual members of the group.
group had concurred in a judgment that was Another irony in the relationship between
in fact wrong. The subjects' conformity was publicity and conformity is that although
greater in public than in private situations, people apparently conform and yield to in-
suggesting that it was mediated by self-pre- fluence because of concern with the opinions
sentational concerns. of others, it is not desirable to be recognized
Research on deindividuation has contrib- by others as being conforming or yielding.
uted additional evidence. When individuals Cialdini, Braver, and Lewis (1974) demon-
are deindividuated, their behavior is still in strated that people perceive someone as more
a public setting but their long-term or on- intelligent if he or she is not persuaded than
going self-presentational concerns are irrel- if he or she yields to influence—with one
evant because they feel they are not individ- exception. The person who is doing the per-
ually identifiable, so their reputations will suading perceives the target as more intel-
not be affected by their behavior. Deindi- ligent if the target does yield to persuasion.
SELF-PRESENTATION

Braver, Linder, Corwin, and Cialdini reputation of conforming to whatever indi-


(1977) carried this phenomenon one step vidual expectations and influences happen
further and showed that people are aware to arise.
of these contingencies and will adjust their The conformity in the Asch (1951) and
admissions of yielding to influence so as to in the Deutsch and Gerard (1955) studies
make the optimal impression (of intelli- was apparently conformity to particular ex-
gence). Subjects in this study admitted the pectations rather than to general norms.
most yielding to a persuasive speech when After all, the general norms for appropriate
they were in the presence of the persuader behavior for laboratory subjects are to give
alone. They admitted the least yielding when one's own best answer, not to express a
the persuader was absent but when an ob- wrong one because of fear of embarrass-
server was there, who of course would pre- ment. Thus, the self-presentational concern
sumably view yielding as an indication of that was operative in those situations was
gullibility and lack of intelligence. In the probably concern with pleasing the imme-
presence of both persuader and observer, diate audience. (That interpretation is fur-
subjects admitted an intermediate amount ther supported by Asch's findings that
of yielding/These findings support the view the behavior changed when the audience
that conformity under surveillance is born changed, such as by not being in unanimous
out of concern with maintaining a desirable agreement with the wrong answer.) The is-
public image for oneself rather than out of sue in deindividuation studies was primarily
specific fear or threat of punishment. conformity to general social norms; for ex-
The role of self-presentation in conformity ample, it is not normally appropriate to der-
and influence is thus apparent. In order to ogate one's parents or to use profanity in the
interpret these findings in terms of specific company of strangers (Festinger et al., 1952;
self-presentational concerns, it is necessary Singer et al., 1965). The self-presentational
to distinguish between two kinds of confor- concerns that were eliminated in those stud-
mity. There is conformity to the expectations ies, resulting in uninhibitedness and noncon-
of particular groups or persons, such as the formity, could therefore be of either the au-
expectation that one's spouse will come dience-pleasing or the self-constructive type.
along on visits to one's mother; and there is The concept of deindividuation seems to im-
conformity to general social norms, such as ply that particularly the long-term (self-con-
those regarding appropriate places to exer- structive) concerns are affected, but there
cise bodily functions. Self-presentation that is no proof yet that this is so.
is concerned with pleasing the immediate The Cialdini et al. (1974) and Braver et
audience usually exhibits both kinds of con- -al. (1977) studies of yielding to persuasion
formity because that is what the immediate show the conflict between the two self-pre-
audience usually wants and expects. Nev- sentational concerns over conformity to spe-
ertheless, self-presentation as a means of cific, individual influences. Their procedures
constructing and maintaining the public self included two audiences: the "persuader," as
will tend to show only the second kind of the audience with specific demands and ex-
conformity. It is probably safe to say that pectations, and the "observer," who did not
most people (except perhaps the misan- have such demands and expectations. The
thrope, iconoclast, and psychopath) conform persuader was thus clearly connected with
to most general social norms most of the time the situational, audience-pleasing self-pre-
because it is undesirable to build a reputa- sentational goals, whereas the observer (rep-
tion of doing inappropriate things. It is also resenting observers in general) was more
undesirable, however, to build a reputation connected with the self-constructive self-pre-
of yielding to changing, contradictory, and sentational goals. Yielding to influence in
arbitrary influences; such a reputation por- general makes a bad impression and is there-
trays one as weak, gullible, and unreliable. fore bad for one's general public self, al-
Thus, although it is fine to have the repu- though a specific audience may be pleased
tation of being "proper" and conforming to at someone yielding to its own influence be-
most social norms, it is not fine to have the cause that is flattering and gratifying to the
10 ROY F. BAUMEISTER

audience (Sigall, 1970). In that case, the to the threatener (Deutsch & Krauss, 1960,
audience may view the yielding as indicative 1962; Kelley, 1965) and went on to propose
of being intelligent or open-minded. Still, that the reactance phenomena may be a way
one might expect that if the audience would of signaling one's refusal to accept this in-
view yielding even to its own influence as ferior status. Heilman and Toffler (1976)
yielding (rather than as an intelligent, open- exposed subjects to threatening influence
minded reappraisal of one's beliefs), the self- from an experimental confederate. Subjects
presentational benefits would be lost, and were then given a choice, which should have
yielding would not occur. That brings up the reduced their reactance according to the
issue of reactance. "effectance" view. Reactance, however, was
It was suggested earlier that the view of not reduced unless the confederate had con-
conformity as deriving from self-presenta- sented to allow the subject free choice. The
tional concerns and the view of conformity authors concluded that the reactance they
as deriving from fear of negative sanctions observed was derived from a desire to project
are not entirely incompatible. They do, how- an image of autonomy to the threatening
ever, furnish contradictory predictions re- agent.
garding one type of situation. The more overt Brehm and Mann (1975) disputed the
and threatening the external influence at- self-presentational view of reactance by find-
tempt is, the greater the compliance with it ing no difference between public and private
should be—if conformity springs from fears expressions of reactance. Baer, Hinkle,
of rejection, censure, or other punishment. Smith, and Fenton (1980), however, pro-
In other words, threat-induced conformity posed that there had been a potential re-
should increase as threat increases. Never- sponse bias in Brehm and Mann's procedures
theless, if the primary factor is concern with and conducted a more thorough test of this
one's public image, then conformity should matter. They found reactance in public at-
decrease as overt pressure and threat in- titudes but not in private attitudes, thus sup-
crease. This is because conformity under porting the self-presentational interpreta-
these conditions would make it obvious that tion. Moreover, it had been shown that
one is yielding and conforming, which is bad reactance is diminished if the person has
for one's public image (Cialdini et al., 1974). exercised the freedom shortly prior to its
The empirical evidence supports the latter being threatened (Brehm, 1966; Snyder &
prediction: Increasing threat leads to de- Wicklund, 1976). Baer et al. found that re-
creasing conformity (Worchel & Brehm, actance was diminished only when this prior
1971). exercise of freedom was public and not when
The last finding was derived in the context it was private.
of research on reactance theory (Brehm, Baer et al.'s (1980) results provide strong
1966), according to which individuals seek support for the view of reactance as derived
to assert and maintain their behavioral free- from self-presentational motivations. In their
doms. Therefore, Worchel and Brehm (1971) words, "People are more concerned with
did not interpret their findings in terms of managing the impression of autonomy than
self-presentation but rather in terms of they are with actually maintaining auton-
maintaining freedoms, which they regarded omy" (p. 416).
as an effectance motivation (White, 1959). Thus, evidence supports the view that self-
According to this view, reactance is derived presentational concerns mediate and deter-
from the motivation to achieve and maintain mine how people respond to external pres-
effective control over one's environment. sures and influence. The risk of violating
Recent evidence, however, has questioned norms and public expectations appears to be
the effectance view of reactance and sug- a self-presentational risk, and so people tend
gested that reactance may derive from self- to conform to protect their public images.
presentational motivations. Heilman and Self-presentational concerns (both with
Garner (1975) cited earlier evidence that pleasing a specific audience and constructing
complying with threats implies that one ac- the public self) usually favor conformity to
knowledges having a status that is inferior general social norms of appropriate behav-
SELF-PRESENTATION 11

ior. To the extent that people are inclined agement (self-presentation). Unfortunately,
to break those norms, they appear to be more their argument and subsequent experimental
likely to do so when their self-presentational evidence (Gaes, Kalle, & Tedeschi, 1978)
concerns are removed, such as when the peo- seemed to some dissonance theorists to hold
ple are anonymous or deindividuated, than that cognitive dissonance was something be-
when these concerns are involved. Specific tween a mere response-bias effect and a little
influences of particular persons create po- white lie the subjects used to make the ex-
tential conflict among the two self-presen- perimenter think well of them. This inter-
tational concerns. In order to please the au- pretation created some obstacles to the ac-
dience, one would conform to such influence, ceptance of the impression management
but in order to construct a reputation for view. In particular, Worchel and Cooper
being independent and consistent rather (1979) cited evidence that cognitive disso-
than vacillating and gullible, one would not nance is characterized by physical arousal
conform to (all of) those influences. There- (Zanna & Cooper, 1974; Zanna, Higgins,
fore, conformity to specific influences is most & Tave's, 1976), thus intrapsychic tension,
likely to occur when the self-presenter is to refute the idea that cognitive dissonance
alone with the audience that is trying to exert is nothing more than a quirk of self-presen-
influence and when yielding to influence can tation.
appear to indicate intelligent or open-minded The present argument attempts to rec-
reappraisal of one's position. The attitude of oncile these views. The evidence does indeed
the person exerting influence, or the presence imply that cognitive dissonance is an upset-
of an additional (noninfluencing) audience, ting and motivating state that is character-
however, can cause yielding to seem like ized by physical arousal. This arousal, how-
mere capitulation, which is bad for one's ever, does not derive from any innate inability
(self-constructive) self-presentational con- of the mind to cope with inconsistency.
cerns. Under those circumstances, reactance Rather, it derives from self-presentational
and general resistance to influence occur. In concerns. People fear the negative labels—
reactance, the person becomes willing to dis- such as "hypocrite," "liar," and "two-
please the audience (by refusing to do what faced"—that are attached to persons who
the audience wants) in order to construct the behave in ways that are contrary to what
public image of self as independent and au- they have asserted their feelings to be. In-
tonomous. consistent behavior risks damage to one's
self-presentation, and that is ample reason
Attitude Expression and Change to become upset or aroused. In short, incon-
sistency by itself is not arousing. Telling lies
The term cognitive dissonance implies a is arousing. Lying is particularly arousing
state of tension and conflict within the in- when done by choice for an inadequate or
dividual psyche. The phenomena associated inconsequential reward because that implies
with cognitive dissonance refer to changes a dispositional willingness to lie. Dissonance
in behavior and expressed attitudes that oc- effects thus may arise from the desire to
cur when individuals behave in ways that are maintain a reputation of honesty and con-
contrary to their values and opinions or when sistency (that is, of opinion integrity); such
individuals confront other inconsistencies in a desire belongs in the self-construction cat-
themselves (Festinger, 1957). These phe- egory of self-presentational concerns.
nomena have generally been interpreted as Self-presentation was in fact closely linked
indicating that the human mind is unwilling to cognitive dissonance from the outset. In
or unable to tolerate certain kinds of appar- the best known case study of dissonance,
ent inconsistency. (The research emphasis Festinger, Riecken, and Schachter (1956)
has been on psychological inconsistency studied a small, reclusive group who antic-
rather than on strictly logical inconsistency.) ipated the end of the world on a certain date.
One exception to this view was a radical When the apocalypse failed to materialize,
formulation by Tedeschi, Schlenker, and the group decided that it had saved the
Bonoma (1971) in terms of impression man- world. That alone should have been enough
12 ROY F. BAUMEISTER

to establish cognitive equilibrium, but the volve a conflict between the self-presenta-
group did not stop there. Rather, the pre- tional concerns of pleasing the experimenter
viously reclusive members suddenly began and of constructing the public self. Once the
a campaign to spread the group's beliefs to subject has performed the behavior (thereby
the general public. Thus, frantic self-presen- pleasing the audience), the self-constructive
tational activity was the result of this famous motivations need to be accommodated. Dis-
instance of dissonance. Little attention, how- sonance-reducing behavior may be that ac-
ever, was paid to the implication that if self- commodation.
presentational activity can resolve disso- Carlsmith, Collins, and Helmreich (1966)
nance, perhaps the conflict underlying dis- replicated and extended this procedure to
sonance has to do with self-presentational include conditions in which subjects wrote
concerns. anonymous essays with the same deceptive
The first laboratory demonstration of cog- message. Dissonance effects obtained only
nitive dissonance also involved self-presen- when the subjects gave in person the false
tation. The method used to arouse disso- description of the task to someone else and
nance was to induce subjects to lie to other not when their self-presentations were pro-
subjects for a small reward (Festinger & tected by anonymity.
Carlsmith, 1959). If the subjects were later Helmreich and Collins (1968) produced
accused of being deceptive or hypocritical, strong evidence for the importance of self-
they could presumably justify their behavior presentational concerns in mediating cog-
if the reward were large ($20), but to do that nitive dissonance. Their subjects made coun-
for a small reward implies a dispositional terattitudinal speeches that were recorded,
willingness to deceive. Consequently, the allegedly for use in persuading others in a
subjects had to change their attitudes so that research project. The speeches of half of the
what they said had not been a lie, at least subjects were identified with the subject's
as far as their future public statements were name, face (videotape recording), home-
concerned. town, and departmental major. The speeches
The Festinger and Carlsmith (1959) ex- of the other half were anonymous, recording
periment had two experimenters, one of only the subject's voice (on audiotape) and
whom administered the manipulations while no biographical information. Dissonance ef-
the other collected the dependent measures. fects were observed only in the former con-
The fact that the dissonance effect obtained dition. Thus, again, under conditions of an-
suggests that it may have been an audience onymity (which remove self-presentational
transfer effect (although subjects could sus- concerns), cognitive dissonance did not ap-
pect that different experimenters might later pear.
compare notes, in which case the separation Gaes, Kalle, and Tedeschi (1978) also
of the audiences would be lost). This further found that anonymity removed the cognitive
supports the view that the self-presentational dissonance effect. Moreover, they used a
concern related to cognitive dissonance is "bogus pipeline" (Jones & Sigall, 1971)
one of self-construction. with half of their subjects in the attempt to
Of course, one could ask why a subject force them to introspect and report "true"
consents to perform the counterattitudinal attitudes, and these did not show the usual
behavior in the first place. Certainly, self- dissonance effect even in the "public" iden-
construction would be best served by refus- tification conditions. This strongly supports
ing. Money is not the only reason subjects the view that the appearances of cognitive
consent because many studies (e.g., Zanna dissonance are mediated by self-presenta-
& Cooper, 1974) have not involved paying tional concerns.
the subject for writing the counterattitudinal The relevance of the payment (for coun-
essay. It appears that subtle pressures ex- terattitudinal behavior) to self-presentation
erted on the subject get the subject to com- concerns has been shown by Schlenker, For-
ply. In a sense, the subject complies to please syth, Leary, and Miller (1980). Working
the experimenter. Thus, the decision to en- from the view of attitude change as a tactic
gage in counterattitudinal behavior may in- to protect and enhance one's public identity
SELF-PRESENTATION 13

(Alexander & Knight, 1971; Schlenker & self-presentational concerns from the situa-
Schlenker, 1975; Tedeschi, Schlenker, & tion also remove the phenomena of cognitive
Sonoma, 1971), these authors reasoned that dissonance. This does not necessarily imply
the payment for counter-attitudinal behavior that cognitive dissonance has no intrapsychic
could have either of two public connotations. aspect. Rather, it suggests that the reason
It could function as a testimony to the level people experience dissonance and conse-
of the individual's personal responsibility for quently alter their behavior is that they fear
his or her behavior or it could seem to be a the potential damage to their reputations
bribe. In their experiments, the cognitive that could follow an exposition of their in-
dissonance effect obtained only in the former consistent behavior. This is also consistent
case. When the payment was presented as with the general implication that the self-
a bribe, subjects sought to defend their moral presentational concern that is most relevant
images by indicating that they had not com- to dissonance phenomena is that of self-con-
promised themselves for money, especially struction. It does not seem that all of the
if the payment was large (which makes the dissonance results can be explained on the
appearance of bribery all the more convinc- basis of the subjects' desire to please the
ing). Consequently, the larger "bribes" led experimenter (or anyone else); rather, dis-
subjects to change their attitudes more than sonance may be considered a person's at-
did the smaller ones, presumably to show tempt to adjust his or her general public self
that they had not lied for the money. so as to be compatible with the counterat-
Additional evidence indirectly supports titudinal behavior.
the self-presentational view. Aderman and The preceding does not imply that pleas-
Brehm (1976) had subjects perform coun- ing the audience and other short-term (sit-
terattitudinal behavior and then asked them uational) concerns are irrelevant to the
to recall their initial attitudes. As in an ear- expression of attitude. The self-presenta-
lier study by Bern and McConnell (1970), tional view holds that the precise attitude a
the subjects retrospectively distorted their person expresses on an occasion is a com-
initial attitudes to give the impression that promise between self-construction concerns
they had not changed much. Aderman and (which identify particular positions as the
Brehm, however, also showed that subjects ones the person identifies with in the long
could recall their initial attitudes accurately run) and various self-presentational de-
if they were given sufficient incentive to do mands of the situation. A desire to please a
so. This finding implies that subjects dis- particular audience, such as a potential em-
torted the attitudes they expressed in order ployer or in-law, may cause people to modify
to manage the impression of being consis- the opinions they express. In fact, the atti-
tent, which is the essence of the self-presen- tude expressed may become a pawn in the
tational view. service of making an impression that has lit-
Lepper, Zanna, and Abelson (1970) sug- tle to do with the actual content of the at-
gested that once a person has completed the titude, as research on anticipatory attitude
cognitive mechanism of reducing dissonance, change has suggested.
it is not "reversible," that is, the final atti- Research on attitude change discovered
tude cannot revert back to the initial atti- by accident that subjects who anticipate an
tude. Subsequent work, however, showed attack on their beliefs tend to change these
that dissonance reduction can be reversed if opinions before they are exposed to the at-
the public commitment is removed (Wil- tack (McGuire & Millman, 1965; McGuire
helmy, 1974; Wilhelmy & Duncan, 1974). & Papageorgis, 1962). This finding of an-
This, too, indicates that what binds the per- ticipatory attitude change sparked a line of
son to his or her postdissonance attitude is research studies. It was shown that these
self-presentational concerns because remov- anticipatory changes are apparently not a
ing these concerns causes the attitude to re- result of cognitive inconsistency but rather
vert to its original position. a defense against the possibility of seeming
To summarize the evidence, then, it ap- gullible (Cooper & Jones, 1970). The shifts
pears that any manipulations that remove were shown to be essentially moderating
14 ROY F. BAUMEISTER

shifts toward neutral positions (Cialdini, their public images. Self-esteem is affected
Levy, Herman, & Evenbeck, 1973). As Cial- by an evaluation only if the recipient accepts
dini et al. (1973) pointed out, moderate po- it and revises his or her self-concept accord-
sitions are the easiest to defend, the most ingly. In short, an evaluation necessarily af-
culturally desirable ("open-minded"), and fects one's self-presentational concerns but
the least likely to make enemies. Another does not necessarily affect one's self-esteem.
way of expressing this is that moderate po- The distinction between pleasing the au-
sitions offer self-presentational advantage dience and constructing the public self does
(i.e., the best chance for looking good and not appear to be reflected in the research on
impressing others). Hass and Mann (1976) receiving evaluations. Both concerns are
showed that anticipatory attitude change probably relevant. Evaluations in general tell
vanished when the self-presentational con- a person what his or her public self is like.
cerns were eliminated, supporting the view Evaluations from important audiences tell
that the anticipatory shifts are caused by people whether they are pleasing those au-
self-presentational motivations. Thus, antic- diences.
ipatory attitude change appears to be an- Of course, self-presentation does best to
other phenomenon in which the attitudes begin its work before the evaluation becomes
that persons express become tools designed known. As Jones and Berglas (1978) sug-
to achieve self-presentational advantage. gested, it is much more credible to discredit
Finally, Cooper and Jones (1969) showed an evaluation before one receives it rather
another use of attitude expression in the ser- than after one has heard the bad news.
vice of self-presentation. They confronted Schneider (1969) and Eagly and Acksen
each subject with a confederate who held (1971) explored subjects' responses to eval-
opinions that were identical to those of the uations as a function of whether they ex-
subject but who behaved obnoxiously to the pected further evaluations or not. People
experimenter. Subjects changed their opin- with a preliminary success had best be mod-
ions in order to dissociate themselves pub- est if further evaluation is expected, or else
licly from the obnoxious person. No such the further evaluation could prove highly
shifts were found on private measures of embarrassing. Only if no further evaluation
attitudes. Thus, again, the public expression is expected can people feel free to be very
of attitude was used as a means of conveying positive about themselves. On the other
some image of oneself (that had little or hand, people with a preliminary failure can
nothing to do with the actual attitude) to an only describe themselves favorably if there
audience. is the possibility that a future evaluation
might exonerate them. Schneider's and Eagly
Receiving Evaluations and Acksen's results supported this self-pre-
sentational model.
For a long time, the behavior of a person Schlenker (1975) had subjects describe
after receiving an evaluation was understood themselves to other members of a group,
as mediated by the effects of that evaluation ostensibly as a means of getting acquainted
on his or her self-esteem (e.g., Aronson & prior to task performance. He manipulated
Mettee, 1968; Dinner, Lewkowicz & Cooper, both how well the subject could expect to
1972; Jones & Schneider, 1968; Sigall & perform on the task and whether the per-
Gould, 1977; Walster, Berscheid, Abra- formance results would be public or private.
hams, & Aronson, 1967). Although self-es- When the performance was expected to be
teem may be a factor, evidence has accu- public, the subjects described themselves
mulated stressing the importance of self- consistently with their (manipulated) expec-
presentation in understanding behavior that tations of performance level. Yet when the
follows receiving evaluations. After all, the performance was expected to be anonymous,
literal effect of evaluations is to tell people self-presentations were all positive and self-
what someone else (the evaluator) thinks of enhancing, even if the preliminary feedback
them—thus, to tell them something about had been unfavorable. Thus, only the like-
SELF-PRESENTATION 15

lihood of publicly identifiable, disconfirming a typology of violent people. The three most
evidence will prevent people from presenting common types he identified, which ac-
themselves in a maximally favorable light. counted for more than half of his sample,
Finally, Baumeister and Jones (1978) pro- all reflected some self-presentational moti-
vided clear evidence of the importance of vations. The most common type of violent
self-presentational concerns in reaction to people fear that others (in general) will re-
evaluations by showing that behavior follow- gard them unfavorably, so they respond vi-
ing such an event depends on whether the olently to real or imagined insults or chal-
evaluation was public or confidential. Sub- lenges, even relatively minor ones. A second
jects in that experiment apparently felt con- type appear to seek violent encounters to
strained to be consistent with their reputa- demonstrate their strength, courage, and
tions, as created by (randomly generated) toughness. A third type consist of people who
public evaluations; they did not conform so have social roles or identities that require
closely to the private evaluations. Moreover, aggressive activity, such as leader or strong-
following a private bad evaluation, subjects arm person in a gang. In other words, they
sought to compensate for it by presenting have the reputation of being aggressive, and
themselves favorably on traits unrelated to they need frequent public demonstrations to
the evaluation; again, no such effect followed maintain this reputation. Thus, Toch's find-
a private evaluation. If responses to an eval- ings suggest that a chronic inclination to
uation were mediated by its effects on self- violent behavior is often associated with
esteem rather than by its implications for motivations connected with one's public
self-presentation, no such differences would identity and reputation. Contrary to what
have obtained. one might have expected, these people did
Thus, an evaluation is primarily an event not appear to enjoy violent behavior for its
that concerns self-presentation. Although own sake; rather, they seemed drawn to it
responses to it may indeed be influenced by because of its significance for their public
the recipient's self-esteem, no response to an identities.
evaluation should be interpreted without Violence is rarely an effective means of
considering its self-presentational context, pleasing others. It is therefore not surprising
unless perhaps the recipient is assured of the that the self-presentational concerns re-
evaluation's total and permanent confiden- flected in Toch's categories appear to involve
tiality. self-construction rather than pleasing an au-
dience. Only members of the third category
Aggression (e.g., leaders of street gangs) could be said
to please anyone by being violent, and in
Some theories depict aggression as an ex- their case it is ambiguous whether their vi-
plosion of impulses that build up inside the olence is a means of maintaining their role
psyche, often from instinctual roots (Freud, and identity or of fulfilling the group's ex-
1920; Lorenz, 1966) or from frustration pectations, or both. In the majority of cases
(Dollard, Doob, Miller, Mowrer, & Sears, where self-presentational factors are appar-
1939). Others see aggression as a set of be- ent in aggression, it is likely that the main
havior that is acquired through modeling concern is with constructing the public self
and reinforcement (Bandura, 1973), It is ' rather than pleasing anyone.
hard to see any room for self-presentation It has often been suggested that aggressive
in these theories. behavior in children may be a means of get-
Nevertheless, even if one accepts the view ting attention from a potential audience.
of aggression as an outburst of instinctive or Erikson (1968) carried this line of thought
behavioral patterns, one can still ask why further in his study of juvenile delinquents.
some people appear to have weaker inhibi- He concluded that the motive for violent and
tions and controls concerning aggressiveness antisocial behavior in such persons is often
than do others. Toch (1969) sought to an- to establish that they are not what society
swer that question empirically by producing expects them to be—to establish "negative
16 ROY F. BAUMEISTER

identities," in Erikson's term. Erikson's sug- audience regarded them favorably. Brown's
gestion calls for an expansion of self-presen- results testify to the importance of self-pre-
tational theory. Human self-presentational sentation in revenge behavior: A person will
behavior may seek to deny some image of forgive and forget a hurt much more easily
oneself rather than to affirm such an image. than a humiliation. Again, though, increas-
Again, though, aggression is a means of ing aggression does not appear likely to
("negative") self-construction rather than of please an audience. The vengefulness of
pleasing an audience. Brown's subjects is probably attributable to
Several experimental studies of aggression pride, which belongs to the self-construction
have used self-presentational manipulations. category of self-presentational concerns.
Zimbarbo (1970) showed increased aggres- One study did suggest that aggression may
siveness as one consequence of deindividua- be increased by a desire to please an audi-
tion. This suggests that one factor that re- ence, if the audience clearly seems to endorse
strains people from harmful and sadistic aggressive behavior. (Such an audience is
behavior is the concern that such behavior probably quite unusual, of course.) Borden
will damage their reputations. Removal, by (1975) gave subjects cues that the observer
deindividuation or anonymity, of these self- to an experiment either favored aggression
presentational constraints appears to remove (i.e., was a karate instructor) or disapproved
the inhibitions against aggression as well. of it (i.e., was a pacifist). Subjects were sig-
(Of course, the uninhibiting effects of dein- nificantly more aggressive in front of the
dividuation can pertain to prosocial behav- karate instructor than in front of the pacifist.
ior, too, as suggested by Johnson & Down- This result could alternatively be interpreted
ing, 1979, and by Schwartz & Gottlieb, as the result of cognitive changes produced
1980.) As noted earlier, though, deindivi- by aggressive cues (Berkowitz & Geen,
duation does not distinguish effectively be- 1966). Nevertheless, a cognitive change pro-
tween concerns with pleasing an audience duced by cues would be expected to persist
and with constructing the public self. Re- beyond the removal of the cue, whereas self-
fraining from aggression may therefore de- presentation ends when the audience leaves.
rive from either type of self-presentational Borden's finding that the observed differ-
concern. ences vanished when the observer departed
The deindividuation findings suggest that midway through the experiment thus tends
self-presentational concerns play a role in to support the self-presentational interpre-
the inhibition and prevention of aggression. tation.
Of course, self-presentational concerns can It is probable that there are many factors
increase aggression too, as implied by the at work in aggressive behavior, including
Toch (1969-) findings mentioned earlier. many that do not relate directly to self-pre-
Brown (1968) added a self-presentational sentation. The present argument wishes to
aspect to the Deutsch and Krauss (1960) suggest only that communicative goals can
trucking game paradigm. The subjects were play a role in the production and inhibition
exploited in a first game and then received of aggression. Self-presentation may help to
feedback from an audience that emphasized reduce aggression either through the desire
either their "playing it straight" (and thus to please a specific audience or through the
looking good) or their humiliation by the desire to maintain a favorable reputation for
other (and thus being made to look "like a oneself in general. When self-presentational
sucker"). The subjects then had an oppor- factors increase aggression, it seems usually
tunity to retaliate in a later game, but the to be because of the desire to protect, restore,
retaliation also involved substantial costs to or enhance one's general public self-image.
the subjects themselves. The results showed Still, there is ample room for further re-
that subjects would retaliate despite costs to search on the role of the self-presentational
themselves when they thought they had suf- concerns in aggression. If self-presentation
fered self-presentational damage, much in aggression does indeed derive from con-
moreso than when they believed that the cerns with self-construction, then audience
SELF-PRESENTATION 17

transfer effects should obtain. In other words, external attributions for failure (e.g., Fitch,
research should examine the role of self-pre- 1970; Luginbuhl, Crowe, & Kahan, 1975).
sentation in displaced aggression. This pattern has been labeled that of "self-
In addition, it may be worth noting that serving" attributions because the person ac-
the lone exception to the otherwise dismally cepts credit for success but refuses the blame
nonsupportive record of the "catharsis" hy- for failure. Recently, however, several the-
pothesis involved public circumstances. orists have begun to ask whether the "self
Fromkin, Goldstein, and Brock (1977) found that is thus "served" is the public or the
that frustration-induced aggressiveness could private self (Arkin, Appelman, & Burger,
be reduced if the subjects learned of some 1980; Bradley, 1978; Frey, 1978; Weary,
suffering of the frustrating agent (that is, 1980).
the catharsis effect). This suffering in their Bradley (1978) observed that all research
experiment involved a public censure. The supporting the notions of defensive or self-
subject's frustration was also a public mat- serving attributions involved the subject in
ter, in that it was deliberately and individ- public performance with publicly known
ually caused by another person. Future outcomes. Moreover, the subject's attribu-
research may determine whether self- tions were also public in that the experi-
presentational concerns can facilitate ca- menter was generally expected to examine
tharsis. them, Bradley's conclusion was that "self-
Moreover, "anger" is commonly treated serving attributions may be viewed as public
as a mediating variable in aggression re- self-presentations designed to maximize
search. In laboratory research, anger is often public esteem" (p. 66). Thus, people who
created by having the future aggressive tar- balk at declaring themselves responsible for
get first deliver a bad evaluation to the sub- their recent public failure may be doing so
ject (e.g., Baron & Bell, 1976; Donnerstein, out of face-saving concerns rather than nec-
Donnerstein, & Evans, 1975). It has already essarily expressing their most sincere and
been 'argued that self-presentational con- considered private opinion.
cerns are of central importance in determin- The essence of Bradley's (1978) argument
ing reactions to evaluations. Future research is that performance outcomes have impli-
on aggression should consider the self-pre- cations for one's self-presentation, so pub-
sentational context of the "angering" eval- licly expressed attributions are statements
uation and what the subject may seek to designed to minimize the damage and max-
communicate by an aggressive retaliation for imize the benefit for one's public image. This
such a humiliation. In particular, some par- enabled Bradley to explain the finding of
adigms have the subject receive shocks from "counterdefensive" attributions (Ross, Bier-
a confederate as a punishment for poor per- brauer, & Polly, 1974), in which subjects
formance (thus an evaluation) and later de- accept blame for failure but modestly deny
liver shocks to the same confederate in eval- credit for success (contrary to the usual self-
uation of the confederate's work (e.g., serving pattern). Bradley noted that the sub-
Berkowitz & Geen, 1966). By delivering jects in the Ross et al. (1974) experiment
many shocks to the confederate, the subject expected further evaluations. Citing evi-
can imply that the confederate's work is dence by Schlenker (1975) that the antici-
poor, suggesting that the confederate was pation of public evaluation causes self-pre-
not qualified to evaluate the subject. The sentations to become cautious and modest,
self-presentational damage caused by the Bradley argued that the counterdefensive-
confederate's earlier evaluation is thus dis- ness findings "suggest that when faced with
credited. a future performance where failure is pos-
sible, individuals will make seemingly coun-
Attributions terdefensive a t t r i b u t i o n s . . . in the attempt,
perhaps, to avoid the embarrassment result-
Research has shown that subjects prefer ing from public invalidation of a self-pre-
to make internal attributions for success and sentation that is too positive" (p. 66).
18 ROY F. BAUMEISTER

Arkin, Appelman, and Burger (1980) construction). It is tempting to observe that


tested a hypothesis derived from Bradley's counterdefensiveness in general might be in-
(1978) suggestion that the expectation of compatible with the goals of self-construc-
additional evaluation by others could reverse tion because one does not establish a favor-
the self-serving pattern. They found self- able reputation by blaming oneself for failure
serving attributions among subjects who ex- and denying success. Tetlock's results, how-
pected no further interaction, but there was ever, have only indirect relevance to the issue
a reversal of this pattern among subjects who of self-presentational motivations for coun-
were high in social anxiety and who antici- terdefensiveness, and the other findings do
pated having their performances evaluated not seem to indicate clearly which sort of
by experts. Moreover, they found that bogus self-presentational concern is at work. Pro-
pipeline measures (which are less easily in- tecting oneself against future public evalu-
fluenced by self-presentational concerns than ations (Arkin et al., 1980; Bradley, 1978)
are paper-and-pencil measures) tended to may be a matter of protecting one's general
offset the self-serving attributions. Their re- public image or of protecting a particular
sults supported the self-presentational ex- audience's good impression of oneself.
planation of self-serving attributions. It appears that success and failure have
Additional evidence has supported the effects on self-presentation. When people are
self-presentational explanation of attribu- then asked to allocate responsibility for their
tional statements. Weary (Bradley) herself outcomes, they are in effect being invited to
provided strong support by showing that redefine their self-presentational impact.
they obtained following public but not pri- Such an invitation of course provides a good
vate outcomes (Weary, 1980). Subjects who opportunity either for face-saving (after fail-
received failure feedback made more defen- ure) or for emphasizing one's credit and
sive attributions when an observer and the worth (after success). The evidence suggests
experimenter had monitored their outcomes that the self-serving and counterdefensive
than when no one had paid any attention to patterns of attributional statements should
their outcomes. Thus, the self-serving pat- be understood in that context.
tern appears to depend on publicity.
Frey (1978) showed that subjects' evalu- Task Performance
ations of a test they had taken were affected
by whether they were public or private. In At first thought, one might expect self-
fact, he found that both the publicity of the presentational concerns to be irrelevant to
outcome and the publicity of the subsequent task performances. There is evidence, how-
attributional statement influenced the eval- ever, that in some situations self-presenta-
uations expressed by the subject. The self- tional concerns and motivations can influ-
presentational context (i.e., the audience's ence performance level. Indeed, it is likely
knowledge) thus had a strong effect on the that some kinds of performance pressure are
attributions made by the subject. in fact derived from self-presentational con-
Finally, Tetlock (1980) showed that in the cerns. Pressure may be created by the desire
situations examined by Ross et al. (1974) to for a specific performance level in order to
demonstrate counterdefensiveness, people enhance one's reputation or to please an au-
who make such attributions will in fact be dience.
viewed more favorably than will people who Social facilitation theory (Zajonc, 1965)
make self-serving attributions. Thus, some- holds that the presence of other people em-
times modesty is better for one's self-pre- phasizes the dominant response. Well-learned
sentation than self-aggrandizement is. If behavior is facilitated, whereas learning is
counterdefensiveness is a tactic of self-pre- impaired by the presence of others. Accord-
sentation, it is an effective one. ing to Zajonc, the effects are due to in-
Moreover, if counterdefensiveness is in- creased drive and arousal occasioned by the
deed a self-presentational tactic, Tetlock's presence of others.
(1980) results suggest that it is a means of Obviously, self-presentational concerns are
pleasing the audience (rather than of self- one potential source of this drive or arousal.
SELF-P RES ENTATION 19

Several studies have suggested that the con- exact replications of this effect, however,
cern with being evaluated by the audience have led other researchers to wonder what
is one mediator of "social facilitation" and situational variables can produce such a
have shown that blindfolded or uninterested motivation to confirm an expectancy of fail-
bystanders do not produce social facilitation ure (Brock, Edelman, Edwards, & Schuck,
(Cottrell, Wack, Sekerak, & Rittle, 1968; 1965; Cottrell, 1965; Lowin & Epstein,
Paulus & Murdock, 1971). Henchy and 1965; Ward & Sandvold, 1963). One sug-
Glass (1968) provided additional support for gestion explored by Sigall, Aronson and Van
the "evaluation apprehension" interpreta- Hoose (1970) was that performance would
tion. They found that social facilitation was be poor when success is linked with an un-
stronger when the audience was composed desirable trait. Unfortunately, their proce-
of experts who were there to evaluate the dures failed to distinguish between public
subject's performance than when the audi- and private motivations, so it remained un-
ence was there merely to observe (which, in clear whether the relevant motivations con-
turn, produced more social facilitation than cerned self-presentation or self-concept
in subjects who were alone). They also found maintenance.
social facilitation in subjects who performed Baumeister, Cooper, and Skib (1979)
alone but whose performance was being re- compared public and private awareness of
corded for subsequent evaluation. In the lat- an expectation of poor performance. Sub-
ter condition, social facilitation appeared as jects in their experiment were told that they
a result of anticipated evaluation despite the were expected to perform poorly because of
physical absence of other persons. a personality trait they had. For half of the
Other studies have suggested that some subjects the trait was presented as desirable,
degree of social facilitation can be produced whereas for the other half it was presented
without evaluation apprehension (Haas & as undesirable. Subjects confirmed the ex-
Roberts, 1975; Rittle & Bernard, 1977; Za- pectancy of poor performance only when it
jonc, Heingartner, & Herman, 1969), al- was public and was (publicly) linked to the
though perhaps not as much as when eval- desirable trait. Thus, it appears that self-pre-
uative concerns are relevant (Haas & sentational motivations mediate performing
Roberts, 1975). Still, the evidence indicates poorly in response to an expectancy of poor
that concern with an audience's evaluation performance. Task performance can become
is an important contributor to social facili- a mere tool in the service of self-presenta-
tation. Evaluation apprehension is obviously tional goals.
derived from self-presentational concerns; It could be speculated that the phenom-
the importance of self-presentation in re- enon of learned helplessness is influenced by
sponse to evaluations has already been ar- self-presentation. "Learned helplessness"
gued. Although the mere presence of others describes the pattern that when animals ex-
may produce some drive—due perhaps to perience uncontrollable failure, their subse-
concerns with sex, competition, and the quent performance is inferior (Maier, Selig-
like—it appears that the evaluative presence man, & Solomon, 1969; Overmier & Selig-
of others produces even more drive, which man 1967; Seligman, 1975). When this par-
is probably due to concerns with self-pre- adigm was first applied to humans, however,
sentation, the opposite result was found—humans per-
In most cases, self-presentational concerns formed better after experiencing uncontrol-
motivate a person to perform as well as pos- lable bad outcomes (Roth & Bootzin, 1974;
sible. The question of whether people are Thornton & Jacobs, 1972). Other studies
ever motivated to perform below their best, have suggested that the usual learned help-
however, has also received some attention. lessness pattern can be obtained in humans
Aronson and Carlsmith (1962) suggested (e.g., Roth & Kubal, 1975). It is quite plau-
that people will try to perform poorly when sible, however, that self-presentation plays
they expect poor performance because doing some role in determining whether uncon-
well would create cognitive dissonance. Dif- trollable bad outcomes lead to performance
ficulties in obtaining conceptual and even increments or decrements. The subsequent
20 ROY F. BAUMEISTER

performance may be the person's response searched. Regarding the effects of emotion
to being perceived by others as helpless. By on self-presentation, one can only speculate.
the same token, performance decrements Because of the short-term nature of emotion,
due to working under a label that connotes emotion might be expected to accentuate
inferiority or incompetence ("self-induced the short-term self-presentational concerns.
dependence;" Langer & Benevento, 1978) Thus, emotion might increase people's ten-
may be mediated by the individual's belief dency to do things that will please others
that he or she could (or has) become publicly who are present at the time but be regretted
perceived as inferior or incompetent. Still, later as potentially damaging to their rep-
it is left for future research to determine the utations. Very strong emotions may result
relevance of self-presentational concerns to in behavior that is indifferent to self-presen-
learned helplessness and self-induced depen- tational concerns.
dence. The effects of self-presentation on emo-
tion have received some attention. Ob-
Interpersonal Attraction and Ingratiation viously, to the extent that people care about
Self-presentation is important in winning their public images, they will become elated
the affection of others. Indeed, ingratiation or upset at their self-presentational out-
comes. In discussing the effects of mood on
is defined as "influencing a particular other
person concerning the attractiveness of one's helping, Kenrick, Baumann, and Cialdini
(1979) argued that self-presentational events
personal qualities" (Jones & Wortman,
1973), which is very similar to a definition may be a major source of moods that facil-
of self-presentation. Ingratiation is, of course, itate helping behavior.
There is as yet rather little evidence about
mainly connected with the self-presenta-
tional concern to please a particular audi- the role of self-presentation in producing
ence, not with the attempts to construct a emotion. Nevertheless, one study that has
generally favorable public self. The litera- shown some influence of self-presentational
ture on ingratiation has detailed the strate- behavior on emotion was conducted by Lan-
gies and problems involved in attracting oth- zetta, Cartwright-Smith, and Kleck (1976).
ers (Jones, 1964; Jones & Wortman, 1973). They asked their subjects to suppress or to
Clearly, there is no need for reinterpretation exaggerate the expression of the pain they
felt when receiving electric shocks. Contrary
or argument to establish the role of self-pre-
sentational factors in ingratiation. to the view that suppressing an emotion in-
tensifies it, they found that the degree of
Although the object of ingratiation is de-
emotion—according to physiological mea-
fined in terms of convincing another person
of one's attractive qualities, the decisive issue sures—conformed to the public expression.
This suggests that when a person has a mo-
in an ingratiation attempt often turns out to tive to express a particular emotion (or lack
be convincing the other person about one's
thereof), his or her internal state will co-
motives and intentions (Jones & Wortman,
operate and furnish the desired level of
1973), Ingratiation is thus a special instance
of self-presentation in which one tries to emotion. The public expression can help to
present an attractive image of oneself but at cause the emotional state.
the same time seeks to deny that that is what Although it is common to consider emo-
tion as originating in the private, internal
one is trying to do. Jones and Wortman's
state of the individual and proceeding from
(1973) concept of the "ingratiator's di-
lemma" encompasses a wide variety of cir- there to public expression, Lanzetta et al.'s
cumstances in which self-presentational mo- (1976) findings suggest (but cannot be said
to prove) that the emotion may sometimes
tives must be concealed in order to be
effective. proceed in the reverse direction, from public
expression to internal state. Baumeister and
Cooper (1981) carried this a step further.
Emotion
Their subjects were told that a particular
The relations between self-presentation emotion was expected of them. When ap-
and emotion have not been extensively re- propriate attributional criteria were met, the
SELF-PRESENTATION 21

subjects did appear actually to have that tance, aggression, refusing help, and disso-
emotion. Still, considerably more research nance can be interpreted as showing the
is needed to establish whether and under importance of the self-construction self-pre-
what circumstances an emotion may arise sentational concern when in conflict with the
because it is called for by self-presentational possibility of pleasing the immediate audi-
motivations. ence.
The hypothesis of opposed self-presenta-
Conclusion tional motives helps to explain a variety of
seemingly contradictory sets of findings,
It has been proposed here that self-pre- such as that people yield to influence (as in
sentational concerns and motivations play a conformity studies) and that people refuse
central role in determining conformity and to yield to influence (as in reactance studies).
in determining whether people yield to the The conflict between pleasing the immediate
influence of others or do the opposite of what audience and constructing one's public self
others attempt to induce them to do; in me- can be seen in the choices people make about
diating the expression and change of atti- whether to accept help, whether to yield to
tudes, especially in attitude change following influence and threat, whether to stand by
counterattitudinal behavior; in determining one's counterattitudinal behavior, whether
how people respond to evaluations; in elic- to aggress, and whether to make counter-
iting the self-serving and counterdefensive defensive (self-blaming) attributional state-
patterns in the interpretations that people ments. These choices appear to depend in
express following success and failure; and, part on factors that alter the relative im-
of course, in influencing interpersonal at- portance of the two self-presentational mo-
traction. In addition, self-presentational con- tives.
cerns have been indicated as relevant and Although the distinction between these
sometimes central in producing helping be- two concerns is a promising candidate for
havior and in determining the level of task future self-presentation research, consider-
performance and, possibly, in determining ation must be given to ways in which they
emotion. might resemble each other operationally.
The general notion of self-presentational The absence of audience transfer phenom-
motivation is that people attempt to present ena may not be a reliable indicator of the
themselves as favorably as they can (that is, absence of concerns with self-construction.
safely from disconflrmation; Schlenker, 1975, Certain audiences may have special value or
1980). Most researchers who have discussed meaning for constructing certain identities,
their results in self-presentational terms and other audiences (e.g., an audience com-
have used that concept (e.g., Arkin et al, posed of people who are much younger than
1980). The present article proposed a dis- the self-presenter) may not matter to an in-
tinction between the two main self-presen- dividual, even for his or her general public
tational concerns, pleasing the audience that image. Also, people need friends and are
is present and constructing one's general therefore dependent on others for friendship;
public self. These concerns appear to overlap nevertheless, there is a wide range of social
and have some similar goals. Thus, both identities one can choose and still have
types of self-presentational concerns appear friends, so the dependency alone does not
to promote helping behavior and to promote completely determine the self-presentation.
conformity to general social norms. Other Self-construction concerns help decide which
situations, however, provide a conflict be- of the various possible social identities is
tween the two self-presentational concerns. chosen.
Pleasing an audience may entail conforming Other possible categories of self-presen-
to its expectations, submitting to exploita- tational concerns deserve mention. A woman
tion, yielding to influence, or otherwise ac- who acts as if she is cold and insensitive in
quiescing; such behavior can run contrary order to discourage unwanted romantic at-
to the general public image the person de- tentions is neither constructing an idealized
sires to project. Phenomena such as reac- image of self nor pleasing her audience (the
22 ROY F. BAUMEISTER

suitor). Thus, there are particular ulterior even essentially similar) self-presentational
motives that occasionally require a strategic goals and motivations. The individual's per-
self-presentation that does not derive from sonality structure may help to predict which
either of the two most common self-presen- self-presentational goal is dominant—for
tational concerns. Instead of pleasing the example, protecting oneself from potential
audience, some other manipulative effect on embarrassment as opposed to stressing one's
the audience is sought. Another alternative responsibility for potential success; appear-
category of self-presentation would involve ing to be a "regular type" as opposed to
situations in which it is to the individual's seeming unusual and unique; or seeming to
advantage to present himself or herself as need social support, affection, or love from
honestly and as accurately as possible, such others as opposed to appearing to be a self-
as when discussing one's symptoms with a sufficient and self-reliant loner. Future re-
physician or therapist. search may establish that the presence or
The importance of self-presentational con- absence of certain personality factors may
cerns may well be culturally relative. As has be associated with particular self-presenta-
frequently been observed, a distinguishing tional motives.
feature of our society is that individuals look Finally, the issue of deceptive self-presen-
outside of themselves, rather than looking tation and being an impostor deserves some
inward, to find the criteria of their worth. attention. People who present themselves in
The predominant political and religious ide- a false fashion accept some risk of being
ologies of the West, from Christianity to found out, which would result in at least
Marxism, define the meaning and purpose some humiliation and possibly much worse.
of an individual's life by his or her relation What can cause people to accept such a risk?
to entities that are external to the individual, A study of deception in self-presentation also
whether these be a god, a means of produc- could shed much light on the relation be-
tion, a humanitarian "cause," or whatever. tween self-concept and construction of the
These reflect the basic attitude that a person public self. It makes no sense to engage in
alone has no intrinsic value; rather, the per- deceptive self-presentation as a means of
son is in a permanent state of evaluative bolstering or confirming one's self-concept.
dependence on external entities. Social psychology has long recognized and
Considerable room remains for future re- emphasized the ways in which people seek
search on self-presentation. As noted above, to obtain information from the environment,
the importance of self-presentational moti- such as by social comparison processes
vations in general for several phenomena (Festinger, 1954), by attributions (Heider,
(such as learned helplessness, aggression, 1958; Jones & Davis, 1965; Kelley, 1967),
emotion, and psychological pressure) needs in achievement motivation (Atkinson &
further investigation. The distinction be- Feather, 1966), or in the process of gaining
tween the two major self-presentational goals control over the environment (White, 1959).
also requires more research. Audience trans- Self-presentation theory asserts that much
fer effects require increased systematic study, human behavior can be understood as being
based on comparisons between confidential the result of the individual's attempts to
or private conditions and conditions with communicate information rather than to
multiple audiences. In addition, some inves- seek it.
tigation is needed to establish what situa-
tional factors increase or decrease the im-
portance of the self-presentational concerns, References
both singly and jointly. In other words, what Aderman, D., & Brehm, S. On the recall of initial at-
determines behavior when self-presenta- titudes following counterattitudinal advocacy: An ex-
tional motives conflict with other motives or perimental reexamination. Personality and Social
impulses? The relation of personality factors Psychology Bulletin, 1976, 2, 59-62.
Alexander, C. N., & Knight, G. Situated identities and
to self-presentational motivations also ought social psychological experimentation. Sociometry,
to be explored. At present, it is unwarranted 1971,34, 65-82.
to assume that all people have identical (or Apsler, R. Effects of embarrassment on behavior toward
SELF-PRESENTATION 23

others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Brehm, J. W. A theory of psychological reactance. New
ogy, 1975, 32, 145-153. York: Academic Press, 1966.
Argyle, M. Social pressures in public and private situ- Brehm, J. W., & Mann, M. Effects of importance of
ations. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, freedom and attraction to group membership on in-
1957, 54, 172-175. fluence induced by group pressure. Journal of Per-
Arkin, R., Appelman, A., & Burger, J. Social anxiety, sonality and Social Psychology, 1975, 31, 816-825.
self-presentation, and the self-serving bias in causal Brock, T., Edelman, S., Edwards, D., & Schuck, J.
attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Seven studies of performance expectancy as a deter-
chology, 1980, 38, 23-35. minant of actual performance. Journal of Experi-
Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. Performance expec- mental Social Psychology, 1965, /, 295-310.
tancy as a determinant of actual performance. Jour- Broil, L., Gross, A., & Piliavin, I. Effects of offered and
nal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1962, 65, requested help on help seeking and reactions to being
178-182. helped. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 1974,
Aronson, E., & Mettee, D. Dishonest behavior as a func- 4, 255-258.
tion of differential levels of induced self-esteem. Jour- Brown, B. The effects of need to maintain face on in-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1968, 9, terpersonal bargaining. Journal of Experimental So-
121-127. cial Psychology, 1968, 4, 107-122.
Asch, S. Effects of group pressure upon the modification Bryan, J., & Test, M. Models and helping: Naturalistic
and distortion of judgment. In H. Guetzkow (Ed.), studies in aiding behavior. Journal of Personality and
Groups, Leadership, and Men. Pittsburgh, Penn.: Social Psychology, 1967, 6, 400-407.
Carnegie Press, 1951. Cannavale, R., Scarr, H., & Pepitone, A. Deindividu-
Atkinson, J. W., & Feather, N. T. A theory of achieve- ation in the small group: Further evidence. Journal
ment motivation. New York: Wiley, 1966. of Personality and Social Psychology, 1970, 16, 141-
Baer, R., Hinkle, S., Smith, K., & Fenton, M. Reac- 147.
tance as a function of actual versus project autonomy. Carlsmith, J. M., Collins, B. E., & Helmreich, R. L.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1980, Studies in forced compliance: I. The effect of pressure
38, 416-422. for compliance on attitude change produced by face-
Bandura, A. Aggression: A social learning analysis. to-face role playing and anonymous essay writing.
Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966,
Baron, R. A., & Bell, P. A. Aggression and heat: The 4. 1-13.
influence of ambient temperature, negative affect, and Cialdini, R. B., Braver, S. L., & Lewis, S. K. Attri-
cooling drink on physical aggression. Journal of Per- . butional bias and the easily persuaded other. Journal
sonality and Social Psychology, 1976, 33, 245-255. of Personality and Social Psychology, 1974, .30, 631-
Baumeister, R. F., & Cooper, J. Can the public expec- 637.
tation of emotion cause that emotion? Journal of Cialdini, R. B., Darby, B., & Vincent, J. Transgression
Personality, 1981, 49, 49-59. and altruism: A case of hedonism. Journal of Ex-
Baumeister, R. F,, Cooper, J., & Skib, B. A. Inferior perimental Social Psychology, 1973, 9, 502-516.
performance as a selective response to expectancy: Cialdini, R. B., & Kenrick, D. Altruism as hedonism:
Taking a dive to make a point. Journal of Personality A social development perspective on the relationship
and Social Psychology, 1979, 37, 424-432. of mood state and helping. Journal of Personality
Baumeister, R. F,, & Jones, E. E. When self-presen- and Social Psychology, 1976, 34. 907-914.
tation is constrained by the target's prior knowledge: Cialdini, R. B., Levy, A. S., Herman, C. P., & Evenbeck,
Consistency and compensation. Journal of Person- 5. Attitudinal politics: The strategy of moderation.
ality and Social Psychology, 1978, 36, 608-618. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1973,
Bern, D., & McConnell, H. K. Testing the self-percep- 25, 100-108.
tion explanation of dissonance phenomena: On the Cohen, A. R. Some implications of self-esteem for social
salience of premanipulation attitudes. Journal of Per- influence. In C. Hovland & I. Janis (Eds.), Person-
sonality and Social Psychology, 1970, 14, 23-31. ality and persuasibility. New Haven, Conn.: Yale
Berkowitz, L., & Geen, R. Film violence and cue prop- University Press, 1959.
erties of available targets. Journal of Personality and Cooper, J., & Jones, E. E. Opinion divergence as a strat-
Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 525-530. egy to avoid being miscast. Journal of Personality
Borden, R. J. Witnessed aggression: Influence of an and Social Psychology, 1969, 13, 23-30.
observer's sex and values on aggressive responding. Cooper, J., & Jones, R. A. Self-esteem and consistency
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1975, as determinants of anticipatory opinion change. Jour-
31, 567-573. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1970, 14.
Bradley, G. W. Self-serving biases in the attribution 312-320.
process: A re-examination of the fact or fiction ques- Cottrell, F. Performance expectancy as a determinant
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, of actual performance: A replication with a new de-
1978, 36, 56-71. sign. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Braver, S., Under, D., Corwin, T., & Cialdini, R. B. 1965, 2, 685-691.
Some conditions that affect admissions of attitude Cottrell, N., Wack, D., Sekerak, G., & Rittle, R. Social
change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, facilitation of dominant responses by the presence of
1977, 13, 565-576. an audience and the mere presence of others. Journal
24 ROY F. BAUMEISTER

of Personality and Social Psychology, 1968, 9, 245- Freedman, J. L., & Doob, A. N. Deviancy. New York:
250. Academic Press, 1968.
Cunningham, M., Steinberg, J., & Orev, R. Wanting Freud, A. A general introduction to psycho-analysis.
to and having to help: Separate motivations for pos- New York: Boni & Liveright, 1920.
itive mood and guilt-induced helping. Journal of Per- Frey, D. Reactions to success and failure in public and
sonality and Social Psychology, 1980, 38, 181-192. private conditions. Journal of Experimental Social
Darley, J. M., & Batson, C. D. "From Jerusalem to Psychology, 1978, 14, 172-179.
Jericho:" A study of situational and dispositional vari- Fromkin, H., Goldstein, J., & Brock, T. The role of
ables in helping behavior. Journal of Personality and irrelevant derogation in vicarious aggression cathar-
Social Psychology, 1973,27, 100-108. sis: A field experiment. Journal of Experimental So-
Darley, J. M., & Darley, S. A. Conformity and devia- cial Psychology, 1977, 13, 239-252.
tion. In J. Thibaut, J. Spence, & R. Carson (Eds.), Gaes, G. G., Kalle, R. J., & Tedeschi, J. T. Impression
Contemporary topics in social psychology. Morris- management in the forced compliance situation: Two
town, N.J.: General Learning Press, 1976. studies using the bogus pipeline. Journal of Experi-
Darley, J. M., & Latane, B. Bystander intervention in mental Social Psychology, 1978, 14, 493-510.
emergencies: Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Gergen, K. J. Toward a psychology of receiving help.
Personality and Social Psychology, 1968,5, 377-383. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 1974, 4, 187-
Darley, J., Teger, A., & Lewis, L. Do groups always 193.
inhibit individuals' responses to potential emergen- Gergen, K., Morse, S., & Bode, K. Overpaid or over-
cies? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, worked? Cognitive and behavioral reactions to ineq-
1973, 26, 395-399. uitable rewards. Journal of Applied Social Psychol-
Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. Study of normative and ogy, 1974, 4, 259-274.
informational influence upon individual judgment. Goffman, E. The presentation of self in everyday life.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1955, Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday, Anchor Books, 1959.
51, 629-636. Gottlieb, J., & Carver, C. Anticipation of future inter-
Deutsch, M., & Krauss, R. M. The effect of threat upon action and the bystander effect. Journal of Experi-
interpersonal bargaining. Journal of Abnormal and mental Social Psychology, 1980, 16, 253-260.
Social Psychology, 1960, 61, 181-189. Gouldner, A. The norm of reciprocity: A preliminary
Deutsch, M., & Krauss, R. M. Studies of interpersonal statement. American Sociological Review, 1960, 25,
bargaining. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 1962, 6, 161-178.
52-76. Gross, A., & Latane, J. Receiving help, reciprocation,
Dinner, S. H., Lewkowicz, B. E., &, Cooper, J. Antic- and interpersonal attraction. Journal of Applied So-
ipatory attitude change as a function of self-esteem cial Psychology, 1974, 4, 220-223.
and issue familiarity. Journal of Personality and So- Haas, J., & Roberts, G. C. Effect of evaluative others
cial Psychology, 1972, 24, 407-412. upon learning and performance of a complex motor
Dollard, J., Doob, L., Miller, N., Mowrer, O., & Sears, task. Journal of Motor Behavior, 1975, 7, 81-90.
R. Frustration and aggression. New Haven, Conn.: Hass, G., & Mann, R. Anticipatory belief change: Per-
Yale University Press, 1939. suasion or impression management? Journal of Per-
Donnerstein, E., Donnerstein, M., & Evans, R. Erotic sonality and Social Psychology, 1976, 34, 105-111.
stimuli and aggression: Facilitation of inhibition. Heider, F. The psychology of interpersonal relations.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1975, New York: Wiley, 1958.
32, 237-244. Heilman, M., & Garner, K. Counteracting the boom-
Eagly, A., & Acksen, B. The effect of expecting to be erang: The effects of choice on compliance to threats
evaluated on change toward favorable and unfavor- and promises. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
able information about oneself. Sociometry, 1971, 34, chology, 1975,37, 911-917.
411-422. Heilman, M., & Toffler, B. Reacting to reactance: An
Erikson, E. Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Nor- interpersonal interpretation of the need for freedom.
ton, 1968. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1976,
Festinger, L. A theory of social comparison processes. 12, 519-529.
Human Relations, 1954, 7, 117-140. Helmreich, R., & Collins, B. E. Studies in forced com-
Festinger, L. A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stan- pliance: Commitment and magnitude of inducement
ford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1957. to comply as determinants of opinion change. Journal
Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. Cognitive conse- of Personality and Social Psychology, 1968, 10, 75-
quences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal 81.
and Social Psychology, 1959, 58, 203-210. Henchy, T., & Glass, D. Evaluation apprehension and
Festinger, L., Pepitone, A., & Newcomb, T. Some con- the social facilitation of dominant and subordinate
sequences of deindividuation in a group. Journal of responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1952, 47, 382-389. chology, 1968, 10, 446-454.
Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. When Hodgson, S., Hornstein, H., & LaKind, E. Socially
prophecy fails. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota mediated Zeigarnik effects as a function of sentiment,
Press, 1956. valence, and desire for goal attainment. Journal of
Fitch, G. Effects of self-esteem, perceived performance, Experimental Social Psychology, 1972, 8, 446-456.
and choice on causal attributions. Journal of Per- Hornstein, H., Masor, H., Sole, K., & Heilman, M.
sonality and Social Psychology, 1970, 16, 311-315. Effects of sentiment and completion of a helping act
SELF-PRESENTATION 25

on observer helping. Journal of Personality and So- Campbell & R. Church (Eds.), Punishment. New
cial Psychology, 1971, 17, 107-112. York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1969.
Johnson, R., & Downing, L. Deindividuation and va- Maslach, C. Social and personal bases of deindividua-
lence of cues: Effects on prosocial and antisocial be- tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
havior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- 1974,29,411-425.
ogy, 1979,27, 1532-1538. McGuire, W. J., & Millman.S. Anticipatory belief low-
Jones, E. E. Ingratiation. New York: Appleton-Cen- ering following forewarning of persuasive attack.
tury-Crofts, 1964. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965,
Jones, E. E., & Berglas, S. C. Control of attributions 2, 471-479.
about the self through self-handicapping strategies: McGuire, W. J., & Papageorgis, D. Effectiveness of
The appeal of alcohol and the role of underachieve- forewarning in developing resistance to persuasion.
ment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Public Opinion Quarterly, 1962, 26, 24-34.
1978, < 200-206. Overmier, J. B., & Seligman, M. Effects of inescapable
Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. From acts to dispositions: The shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance learn-
attribution process in person perception. In L. Ber- ing. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy-
kowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy- chology, 1967, 63, 28-33.
chology (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press, 1965. Paulhus, D. L., Shaffer, D. R., & Downing, L. L. Effects
Jones, E. E., & Sigall, H. The bogus pipeline: A new of making blood donor motives salient upon donor
paradigm for measuring affect and attitude. Psycho- retention. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
logical Bulletin, 1971, 76, 349-364. letin, 1977, 3, 99-102.
Jones, E. E., & Wortman, C. Ingratiation: An attri- Paulus, P. B., & Murdock, P. Anticipated evaluation
butiona! approach. Morristown, N.J.: General Learn- and audience presence in the enhancement of domi-
ing Press, 1973. nant responses. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
Jones, S. C., & Schneider, D. J. Certainty of self-ap- chology, 1971, 7, 280-291.
praisal and reactions to evaluations from others. So- Reis, H., & Gruzen, J. On mediating equity, equality,
ciometry, 1968, 31, 395-403. and self-interest: The role of self-presentation in social
Kelley, H. H. Experimental studies of threats in inter- exchange. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
personal negotiations. Journal of Conflict Resolution, ogy, 1976, 12, 487-503.
1965,9,79-105, Rittle, R. H., & Bernard, N. Enhancement of response
rate by mere physical presence of the experimenter.
Kelley, H. H. Attribution theory in social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1977, 3,
In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Mo- 127-130.
tivation (Vol. 15). Lincoln: University of Nebraska
Press, 1967. Rogers, C., & Dymond, R. (Eds,). Psychotherapy and
personality change. Chicago: University of Chicago
Kenrick, D., Baumann, D., & Cialdini, R. B. A step in Press, 1954.
the socialization of altruism as hedonism: Effects of Ross, L., Bierbrauer, G., & Polly, S. Attribution of ed-
negative mood on children's generosity under public ucational outcomes by professional and nonprofes-
and private conditions. Journal of Personality and sional instructors. Journal of Personality and Social
Social Psychology, 1979, 37, 747-755. Psychology, 1974, 29, 253-264.
Kidder, L., Bellettirie, G., & Cohn, E. Secret ambitions Roth, S., & Bootzin, R. Effects of experimentally in-
and public performances: The effects of anonymity duced expectancies of external control: An investi-
on reward allocations made by men and women. Jour- gation of learned helplessness. Journal of Personality
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1977, 13, and Social Psychology, 1974, 29, 253-264.
70-80. Roth, S., & Kubal, L. Effects of noncontingent rein-
Langer, E., & Benevento, A. Self-induced dependence. forcement on tasks of differing importance: Facili-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1978, tation and learned helplessness. Journal of Person-
36, 886-893. ality and Social Psychology, 1975, 32, 680-691.
Lanzetta, J., Cartwright-Smith, J., & Kleck, R. E. Ef- Satow, K. Social approval and helping. Journal of Ex-
fects of nonverbal dissimulation on emotional expe- perimental Social Psychology, 1975, 11, 501-509.
rience and autonomic arousal. Journal of Personality Schachter, S. Deviation, rejection, and communication.
and Social Psychology, 1976, 33, 354-370. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1951,
Lepper, M., Zanna, M. P., & Abelson, R. Cognitive 46, 190-207.
irreversibility in a dissonance reduction situation. Schlenker, B. Self-presentation: Managing the impres-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1970, sion of consistency when reality interferes with self-
16, 191-198. enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Lorenz, K. On aggression. New York: Harcourt, Brace chology, 1975, 32, 1030-1037.
& World, 1966. Schlenker, B. R. Impression management: The self-
Lowin, A., & Epstein, G. Does expectancy determine concept, social identity, and Interpersonal relations.
performance? Journal of Experimental Social Psy- Belmont, Calif.: Brooks/Cole, 1980,
chology, 1965, /, 248-255. Schlenker, B. R., Forsyth, D. R., Leary, M. R., &
Luginbuhl, J., Crowe, D., & Kahan, J. Causal attri- Miller, R. S. A self-presentational analysis of the ef-
butions for success and failure. Journal of Personality fects of incentives on attitude change following coun-
and Social Psychology, 1975, 31, 86-93. terattitudinal behavior. Journal of Personality and
Maier, S. F., Seligman, M. E., & Solomon, R. L. In B. Social Psychology, 1980, 39, 553-577.
26 ROY F. BAUMEISTER

Schlenker, B. R., & Schlenker, P. A. Reactions follow- of prior inescapable/unavoidable stress on intellectual
ing counter-attitudinal behavior which produces pos- performance. Psychonomic Science, 1972, 26, 185-
itive consequences. Journal of Personality and Social 187.
Psychology, 1975, 31, 962-971. Toch, H. Violent men. Chicago: Aldine, 1969.
Schneider, D. Tactical self-presentation after success Walster, E., Berscheid, E., Abrahams, D., & Aronson,
and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- V. Effectiveness of debriefing following deception ex-
chology, 1969, 13, 262-268. periments. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Schwartz, S., & Gottlieb, A. Bystander reactions to a chology, 1967, 6, 371-380.
violent theft: Crime in Jerusalem. Journal of Per- Ward, W., & Sandvold, K. Performance expectancy as
sonality and Social Psychology, 1976, 34, 1188- a determinant of actual performance: A partial rep-
1199. lication. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychol-
Schwartz, S., & Gottlieb, A. Bystander anonymity and ogy, 1963, 67, 293-295.
reactions to emergencies. Journal of Personality and Weary, G. Examination of affect and egotism as me-
Social Psychology, 1980, 39, 418-430. diators of bias in causal attributions. Journal of Per-
Seligman, M. Helplessness. San Francisco: Freeman, sonality and Social Psychology, 1980, 38, 348-357.
1975. West, S., & Brown, T. J. Physical attractiveness, the
Shapiro, E. G. Effect of expectations of future inter- severity of the emergency and helping: A field ex-
action on reward allocation in dyads: Equity or equal- periment and interperson simulation. Journal of Ex-
ity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, perimental Social Psychology, 1975, 11, 531-538.
1975, 37, 873-880. White, R. W. Motivation reconsidered: The concept of
Sigall, H. Effects of competence and consensual vali- competence. Psychological Review, 1959, 66, 297-
dation on a communicator's liking for the audience. 333.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1970, Wilhelmy, R. The role of commitment in cognitive re-
M, 251-258. versibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Sigall, H., Aronson, E., & Van Hoose, T. The coop- chology, 1974, 30, 695-698.
Wilhelniy, R., & Duncan, B. Cognitive reversibility in
erative subject: Myth or reality? Journal of Exper-
dissonance reduction. Journal of Personality and
imental Social Psychology, 1970, 6, 1-10.
Social Psychology, 1974, 29, 806-811.
Sigall, H., & Gould, R. The effects of self-esteem and Worchel, S., & Brehm, J. W. Direct and implied social
evaluator demandingness on effort expenditure. Jour- restoration of freedom. Journal of Personality and
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1977, 35, Social Psychology, 1971, 18, 294-304.
12-20. Worchel, S., & Cooper, J. Understanding social psy-
Singer, J., Brush, C., & Lublin, S. Some aspects of chology. Homewood, 111.: Dorsey, 1979.
deindividuation: Identification and conformity. Jour- Zajonc, R. Social facilitation. Science, 1965, 149, 269-
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1965, /, 274.
356-378. Zajonc, R., Heingartner, A., & Herman, E. Social en-
Snyder, M., & Wicklund, R. Prior exercise of freedom hancement and impairment of performance in the
and reactance. Journal of Experimental Social Psy- cockroach. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 1976, 12, 120-130. chology, 1969, 13, 83-92.
Steele, C. M. Name-calling and compliance. Journal Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. Dissonance and the pill: An
of Personality and Social Psychology, 1975, 31, 361- attribution approach to studying the arousal proper-
369. ties of dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social
Tedeschi, J. T., Schlenker, B. R., & Bonoma, T. V. Psychology, 1974, 29, 703-709.
Cognitive dissonance: Private ratiocination or public Zanna, M. P., Higgins, E. T., & Taves, P. Is dissonance
spectacle? American Psychologist, 1971, 26, 685- phenomenologically aversive? Journal of Experimen-
695. tal Social Psychology, 1976, 12, 530-538.
Tessler, R., & Schwartz, S. Help-seeking, self-esteem, Zimbardo, P. The human choice: Individuation, reason
and achievement motivation: An attributional anal- and order versus deindividuation, impulse, and chaos.
ysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, In W. J. Arnold & D. Levine (Eds.), Nebraska Sym-
1972, 21, 318-326. posium on Motivation (Vol. 17). Lincoln: University
Tetlock, P. Explaining teacher explanations of pupil of Nebraska Press, 1970.
performance: A self-presentation interpretation. So-
cial Psychology Quarterly, 1980, 43, 283-290.
Thornton, J. W., & Jacobs, P. D. The facilitating effects Received April 22, 1981

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi