Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 8

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2019, 52, 386–393 NUMBER 2 (SPRING)

Reducing adolescent cell phone usage using an interdependent


group contingency
MEGAN E. JONES, R. ALLAN ALLDAY AND ASHANTE’ GIVENS
UNIVERSITY OF KENTUCKY

The purpose of this study was to test the effects of an interdependent group contingency on cell
phone usage in an alternative high school classroom. We used an ABAB reversal design to test
the effects of the contingency on the cell phone usage of the entire class and an individual stu-
dent. Results showed a reduction in the cell phone use of the class and the individual when the
group contingency was in effect, demonstrating a functional relation between the contingency
and student cell phone usage. These findings suggest that group contingencies may be effica-
cious for teachers to use within their classrooms to curb cell phone usage. Further study of this
intervention is warranted to determine its generality.
Key words: adolescents, cell phone usage, group contingency

The use of cell phones in public schools has use (Kuznekoff & Titsworth, 2013). When stu-
increased dramatically and may have both posi- dents are engaged in texting or unapproved
tive and negative aspects in the classroom set- usage of their phone (e.g., Facebook, Twitter),
ting (Thomas & Orthober, 2011). On the plus they are less likely to engage in learning activi-
side, phone applications enable teachers to test ties or perform as well on exams (Junco & Cot-
and poll students through text in order to allow ten, 2012; Kuznekoff & Titsworth, 2013;
automatic grading and immediate feedback Wood et al., 2012). As cell phone usage con-
(Thomas et al., 2011). On the other hand, stu- tinues to increase, school personnel need to
dents can use cell phones to find test answers know how to control it.
on the Internet and also to share with other School policies on cell phone usage vary
students pictures of quizzes and tests (Thomas widely (Obringer & Coffey, 2007), and little
et al., 2011). Similarly, instances of cyber bully- has been written about the most effective
ing can occur due to inappropriate cell phone approaches for curbing inappropriate cell phone
usage, which can perpetuate negative peer use in the classroom. Group contingencies,
interactions (from simple disagreements to
which are typically designed to administer a
physical harm; Patchin & Hinduja, 2006).
consequence to a group based on the behavior
Thomas et al. (2011) found that over 80%
of the individuals within the group (Litow &
of high school students text daily, and nearly
Pumroy, 1975), have been shown to be effec-
half of them admitted to texting hourly. Some
tive in managing a variety of challenging behav-
studies estimate that 64% of teenagers text dur-
ing class, even in schools that restrict cell phone iors, such as disruptive behaviors, negative
social interactions, and poor academic perfor-
Megan Jones and R. Allan Allday, Department of mance (Maggin, Johnson, Chafouleas,
EDSRC, University of Kentucky; Ashante` Givens, Ruberto, & Berggren, 2012). Interdependent
Department of EDP, University of Kentucky.
The authors would like to thank Dr. Brian Bottge for group contingencies are those in which the
his feedback on the manuscript. behavior of all students within a group must
Address correspondence to: Megan E. Jones, EDSRC, meet or exceed a predetermined criteria, and
University of Kentucky 40506-0001, Email: megan.
jones4@uky.edu the reinforcer is delivered to all members of the
doi: 10.1002/jaba.538 group (Groves, & Austin, 2017; Hirsch, Healy,
© 2019 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
386
REDUCING CELL PHONE USAGE 387

Judge, & Lloyd, 2016; Litow & Pum- implemented a cell phone policy restricting stu-
roy, 1975). dents from using phones; however, teachers
Research has shown that interdependent were encouraged to create their own classroom
group contingencies can be an effective way of policy. Anecdotally, teachers voiced concerns
managing behavior with high-school-aged about the high rate of noneducational cell
populations. For example, studies have shown phone use.
that an interdependent group contingency is an The first author, a certified teacher with
effective method for reducing challenging 5 years of experience, implemented the inter-
behavior in a general education high school vention. She observed student cell phone usage
classroom (e.g., Mitchell, Tingstrom, Dufrene, during three separate class sessions to determine
Ford, & Sterling, 2015) and increasing aca- which students used their phones most often.
demic engagement in high school inclusion Then she observed the two students with the
classrooms (e.g., Christ & Christ, 2006). Fur- highest usage for two additional sessions and
thermore, a meta-analysis of studies employing collected duration data on cell phone usage.
group contingencies found up to a two-stan- We selected Veronica, a 14-year-old Caucasian
dard-deviation reduction in disruptive behav- female without an identified disability, as the
iors from baseline levels (Maggin, focus student for individual data collection
Pustejovsky, & Johnson, 2017). The purpose because she used her phone more than the
of this study was to determine if an interdepen- other students.
dent group contingency could reduce the cell All sessions took place in the classroom dur-
phone usage of high school students enrolled in ing the same elective class (Photography) on
an alternative high school. Mondays, Tuesdays, and Thursdays, which was
scheduled for 90 min. Six students and one
teacher participated in the study. The class-
METHOD
room was 2.4 m x 9.1 m and all students sat at
Participants and Setting a rectangular table facing the whiteboard. Prior
A total of six students (three males and three to the intervention, a classroom cell phone pol-
females) between the ages of 14 and 18 partici- icy did not exist. In order to manage excessive
pated in the study. Student ethnic backgrounds cell phone use, classroom observers reported
were varied and included Caucasian (two that the teacher used verbal prompts in a non-
females and two males) and African American systematic manner to reduce cell phone usage
(one male and one female) individuals. One when the students’ technology use became
student had an Individualized Education Pro- excessive or disrupted classroom learning.
gram for emotional/behavioral disorder, and
two students had active 504 Plans. These stu-
dents attended the same elective course in an Response Measurement and Interobserver
urban public alternative high school located in Agreement
the Southeast region of the United States. The Our operational definition for cell phone
school consisted of approximately 170 students usage included any student (a) touching/swiping
with special learning and/or behavioral needs. the screen to activate it, (b) looking at or con-
Nearly 30% of students received special educa- tinuing to manipulate the screen, or (c) using
tion services, and all students were considered two hands to manipulate the phone (texting).
at-risk of dropping out of high school. Class For Veronica, we started the duration timer
sizes ranged from 8 to 20 students. At the time when she touched or swiped the screen and
of the study, the alternative school had not stopped the duration timer when she placed
388 MEGAN E. JONES et al.

the phone either in her pocket or purse, or General Procedures


placed the phone with blank screen on her We employed a reversal design (ABAB) to
desk. Veronica’s data were collected as duration evaluate the effects of the interdependent group
per occurrence and combined in a total dura- contingency on cell phone usage. We deemed
tion for the class period, which is reported. The this design appropriate given its use in evaluat-
teacher measured the duration of Veronica’s ing group contingencies in previous studies
cell phone use with the app Behavior Tracker (e.g., Christ & Christ, 2006; Mitchell
Pro. If any students removed their phones and et al., 2015).
activated the clock feature (i.e., pushed the During both baseline and intervention, the
power button), but did not meet any of the cri- teacher began class by greeting students as they
teria stated above, the action was not consid- entered the room. All students sat at a rectan-
ered cell phone use. gular table with the teacher at the head of the
Both the teacher and a secondary observer table. Prior to student arrival in the classroom,
measured class-wide cell phone usage with a the teacher ensured she had a clean data collec-
paper and pencil, 90-min, partial-interval tion sheet in order to record phone usage. In
recording system. The data sheet included stu- addition, the teacher activated the Behavior
dents’ initials next to empty boxes, which were Tracker Pro app to the necessary screen for
marked “+” for “used phone” or “0” for “did duration data collection. Observations were col-
not use phone.” We calculated percentages of lected in 29 consecutive class meetings exclud-
cell phone usage by dividing the number of ing one day when school was not in session
students who used their phone by the number (i.e., a holiday) and two school-related required
of students present in the classroom. No more functions (e.g., field trip and pep rally). Data
than two students were absent during any ses- collection began after the tardy bell rang and
sion. Students were allowed to go to the rest- ended either when the bell rang to dismiss stu-
room during sessions on request. Students dents to go home (baseline and intervention) or
made this request fewer than 10 times over the when students earned their reward (interven-
course of the study. tion only). During instruction in both condi-
We conducted classroom observations for tions, the teacher either sat at the table with
calculating interobserver agreement (IOA) on the students during group lectures or observed
21% of sessions (17% for baseline phases, 20% students in the room as they practiced skills
and 29% for intervention phases). The teacher delivered in the lesson.
trained a graduate student as a secondary Baseline procedures. During both baseline
observer. To calculate IOA for Veronica, we phases, the teacher did not address cell phone
compared the total duration recorded by each usage with students at the beginning of class.
observer by dividing the shorter duration by The teacher collected data without interfering
the longer duration and multiplying by 100. with or interrupting the students’ normal cell
This resulted in a mean IOA of 95.1% (range, phone usage behaviors. Similar procedures were
82-100%). To calculate IOA for the class, we used during the second phase of baseline. In
conducted a point-by-point comparison of the first session in the return to baseline phase,
individual phone usage. The total number of the teacher announced that the group contin-
agreements was divided by the total number of gency would “not be used for a while.”
agreements plus disagreements, and multiplied Intervention procedures. On the first day of
by 100. IOA was 100% for class-wide cell the intervention, the teacher explained the
phone usage. interdependent group contingency. The teacher
REDUCING CELL PHONE USAGE 389

also informed students that the class could earn What was your reaction when other students
10 min of uninterrupted time on the phone at caused the rest of the class to lose their
the end of class if no one used his/her phone 10 minutes of free time? After the final obser-
during class. They were also told that if any vation, the survey was administered vocally in a
student used his/her phone during class, the group format to all participating students. The
class would not earn the 10 min of time on the teacher asked each question, one at a time, and
phone. Each subsequent day of intervention, in students responded vocally. Their responses
both intervention phases, the teacher would were transcribed by the teacher.
remind students of the reward available at the
end of the period for not using their phones. If
no student accessed his/her cell phone during RESULTS
intervention sessions, the teacher praised the Figure 1 represents Veronica’s total duration
students (e.g., “You guys did not use your of cell phone usage per class. During the initial
phones, so you earned free time to use your baseline condition, Veronica used her cell
phones and talk”), and students received phone an average of 21.2 min (range,
10 min to use the phone at the end of class. 7.5-41.1 min). The introduction of the group
Data were not collected during the 10 min of contingency resulted in an immediate level and
earned cell phone usage. If any student accessed trend change for Veronica. Her mean total
his/her cell phone during intervention sessions, duration of cell phone use during the first
a verbal statement was issued such as, “Cell intervention condition was 0.95 min (range,
phones were used during class, so we will con- 0-5.6 min. Four out of seven data points
tinue class as normal today.” Students were (57%) were at 0 min. Withdrawing the group
then required to complete their normal end of contingency resulted in an immediate and
class routine (i.e., help clean the room and put abrupt increase in cell phone usage for Veronica.
away equipment) while the teacher continued Her mean total duration increased to 39.2 min
instruction and data collection. (range, 24.3-60.8 min) and her usage was more
Treatment fidelity. We assessed treatment variable. Reinstating the group contingency had
fidelity during the intervention conditions, an abrupt impact on her cell phone usage.
which was defined as the teacher (a) informing Veronica’s mean total duration decreased to
students, after the tardy bell but before instruc- 0.06 min (range, 0-0.3 min) in the second
tion, that they could earn 10 minutes of unin- intervention condition. During intervention,
terrupted cell phone time at the end of class if Veronica was able to earn 10 min of uninter-
they did not use their phone during class, and rupted cell phone time. Including these earned
(b) allowing or not allowing 10 min of cell minutes, Veronica’s cell phone use decreased
phone time at the end of class based on the ses- 69% and 80% between baseline and interven-
sion’s data. Treatment fidelity was assessed dur- tion conditions, respectively. Furthermore, there
ing 23% of sessions during the intervention were 100 percent nonoverlapping data (PND)
conditions and reflected 100% fidelity. between each of the conditions for Veronica. All
Social validity. The teacher created a social missing data were due to absences in sessions
validity (Wolf, 1978) survey to examine stu- 9, 10, 11, 21, 23, and 25.
dent perception of the intervention. The survey Figure 2 represents the percentage of the
consisted of three open-ended questions: What class using cell phones. The average percentage
did you think about not having your phones of the class that used cell phones during the ini-
during class time? Did you feel that you could tial baseline was 88% (range, 75-100%). The
focus better during class without your phones? percentage of the class’s use of cell phones
390 MEGAN E. JONES et al.

Figure 1. Veronica’s total duration of cell phone usage, in minutes, per class session.

Figure 2. Percentage of students using cell phones in class.

dropped to an average of 16.5% (range, There was 100 PND between the first inter-
0-50%) following implementation of the group vention condition and return to baseline condi-
contingency. In 5 of 10 (50%) sessions there tion. Reinstating the group contingency
were zero instances of usage. In other words, resulted in an abrupt reduction in cell phone
students received the reinforcer in 50% of ses- usage, with six of the seven (83%) sessions with
sions. Removing the group contingency zero instances of usage. There was one instance
increased the percentage of the class using in the second group contingency condition in
their cell phones to 100% for each session. which Veronica used her phone, while the
REDUCING CELL PHONE USAGE 391

remaining students did not use theirs. Students The teacher in the current study reported
received the reinforcer in 83% of sessions dur- that a majority of participants used cell phones
ing the second intervention condition. There regularly during instructional time prior to
was 100 PND between this and the previous intervention, which was a behavior she wanted
condition. to abate. Group contingencies have proven
Results of the survey revealed that all stu- effective with students exhibiting similar unde-
dents conveyed an unfavorable view of the sireable in-school behaviors (see reviews by Lit-
intervention because they could not access their tle, Akin-Little, & O’Neill, 2015, and Maggin
phones. Students reported that no other class et al., 2017). Group contingencies typically
required such cell phone policies and that they involve establishing clear rules and delivering or
did not enjoy the restriction. All students withholding reinforcers based on targeted
reported an increase in focus during the contin- behavior (Cooper, Heron, & Heward., 2007).
gency phases and they reported that the “free In this study, the teacher provided the rule that
time” with their phone was a powerful rein- if the whole class did not use cell phones dur-
forcer. However, one student noted that, “We ing the period, students would receive uninter-
do not like our phone taken away, but we are rupted phone access for 10 min during “free
able to focus better without our phone.” When time.” The immediate changes observed during
asked about their reactions when another stu- the first sessions of the group contingency may
dent caused the entire class to lose the 10 min be attributable to rule-governed behavior. In
of free time, responses were mixed. Most of the addition, this contingency arranged a whole-
students noted a “frustration” with the peer interval differential reinforcement of other
who used his/her phone and reported forming behavior schedule at the group level, in that
an “intervention” group. This was described as reinforcement (i.e., free access to phone) was
an informal discussion mediated by the stu- available if the target behavior (i.e., looking at
dents in which the group discussed the need to or interreacting with phone) did not occur
avoid using phones during class. They offered throughout an interval. Level changes between
to hold the phone of any student who felt it baseline and intervention suggest that having
was difficult to comply with the directions. uninterrupted access to “free time” on their
phone at the end of class served as a more pow-
erful reinforcer than having interrupted time
DISCUSSION on their phone during class. Students reported
In this study, we examined the effects of an during the social validity survey that there was
interdependent group contingency on the cell a type of peer-driven contingency established
phone usage of a single classroom of high within the context of the intervention. This
school students within an alternative setting. peer contingency included students establishing
For this class, results showed that the group behavioral expectations amongst themselves
contingency reduced student cell phone usage. regarding phone usage. Gaining peer approval
Results support previous studies showing that and/or avoiding peer disapproval could have
group contingencies can be effective at chang- been another variable that impacted the results
ing student behavior (Christ & Christ, 2006; of the group contingency. The peer contin-
Mitchell et al., 2015). Given the potential for gency in combination with the teacher-
inappropriate use of cell phones in schools mediated access-to-phone contingency may
(Kuznekoff & Titsworth, 2013), developing a promote student self-monitoring of their own
simple intervention that can curb this usage behavior (Christ & Christ, 2006; Trevino-
would likely prove helpful. Maack, Kamps, & Wills, 2015).
392 MEGAN E. JONES et al.

Groves and Austin (2018) noted, in their consideration to note in future studies. Third,
social validty measure, that secondary students we did not require students to tell us if they
positively responded to an interdependet group had their cell phones in class each day. Anec-
contingency (i.e., Good Behavior Game). In dotally, one student did not have access to his
their study, the authors focused on earning or phone for 2 days during the intervention phase
losing points based on group behavior. A when his mom took his phone. Otherwise, all
majority of participants reported they enjoyed students were observed to use their cell phones
the intervention and it improved their behavior during “free time” in the intervention phase as
and teamwork. Results of the social validity in the teacher observed cell phones in each stu-
the current study are somewhat contrary to dent’s hand or on the table. All students had
Groves and Austin. Students reported they did their phones on days when baseline was at
not like the intervention. This could be because 100% because they were observed using their
highly valuable tangible reinforcers (i.e., cell phone. On baseline days less than 100%, it is
phones) were restricted, whereas Groves and unknown whether all students had their
Austin did not have such a restriction. Students phones. In future studies, these data should be
in the current study reported they were able to collected. Fourth, it is important to consider
focus better during the intervention, which is that Veronica’s absences were clustered during
not contrary to Groves and Austin’s findings. the treatment phase, as she was absent on five
Furthermore, students engaged in a teamwork- occasions during intervention and one during
like behavior by implementing “intervention baseline. Future research should monitor
groups” to encourage each other. This is a posi- absences, tardiness, and restroom requests dur-
tive finding in that it was not teacher-initiated ing the intervention phase. If student absences
but student-initiated. or tardiness increase due to withdrawing cell
There are a few limitations that merit men- phones it may prove more detrimental to the
tion. First, the use of a 90-min partial interval student and may negatively affect student per-
measure for the entire class was not as sensitive formance. Finally, the social validity measure
to behavior change as a shorter interval might indicated that students did not respond favor-
have been. This type of data collection, while ably to the intervention. For further research
practical for a classroom teacher to implement, on this topic, it may be beneficial to extend the
should be replaced in future studies with a intervention condition and allow students to
more rigorous method (e.g., interval recording choose the intervention over time through pri-
with 1-min or smaller intervals) in order to bet- vate notes instead of group discussion. Even if
ter understand the precise effect of the inter- a student prefers the intervention, it may be
vention. In addition, the partial interval difficult for the students to vocalize the prefer-
recording did not take into account the fre- ence in front of peers; therefore, considering a
quency or duration of cell phone usage for the private social validity measure would be
entire class so a percentage reduction in use prudent.
cannot be determined for the class. Second, we In this study we measured the behavior of
do not know if students used their cell phone one class of students, as well as the detailed
when they were in the restroom. Fewer than behavior of one student in particular, who
10 instances of requests for restroom breaks showed relatively high levels of cell phone usage
occurred across baseline and intervention in class. Future researchers should replicate this
phases. When requested, students were given study using multiple classrooms and additional
less than 5 min to use the restroom and return measures to better understand the generality of
to class, but this may be an important these direct effects as well as the indirect effects.
REDUCING CELL PHONE USAGE 393

An important additional direction for future Applied Behavior Analysis, 8, 341-347. https://doi.
researchers would be to include measures of org/10.1901/jaba.1975.8-341
Little, S. G., Akin-Little, A., & O’Neill, K. (2015). Group
task engagement, as measured through system- contingency interventions with children – 1980-2010:
atic observation and permanent product, when A meta-analysis. Behavior Modification, 39, 322-341.
phones are allowed versus when they are not https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445514554393
allowed in classrooms, to evaluate whether the Maggin, D. M., Johnson, A. H., Chafouleas, S. M.,
students’ reported increase in focus positively Ruberto, L. M., & Berggren, M. (2012). A system-
atic evidence review of school-based group contin-
affects educational objectives. In addition, gency interventions for students with challenging
future researchers should develop and evaluate behavior. Journal of School Psychology, 50, 625-654.
procedures for reducing unapproved usage https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2012.06.001
(e.g., texting, social media) while simulta- Maggin, D. M., Pustejovsky, J. E., & Johnson, A. H.
(2017). A meta-analysis of school-based group con-
neously increasing approved usage (e.g., active tingency interventions for students with challenging
responding through live polling) and consider behaviors: An update. Remedial and Special
evaluating the acceptability of this intervention Education, 38, 353-370. https://doi.10.
1177/0741932517716900
with students and the teachers implementing it
Mitchell, R. R., Tingstrom, D. H., Dufrene, B. A.,
(Groves & Austin, 2018). Ford, W. B., & Sterling, H. E. (2015). The effects of
the good behavior game with general-education high
school students. School Psychology Review, 44, 191-
REFERENCES 207. https://doi.org/10.17105/spr-14-0063.1
Obringer, J. S., & Coffey, K. (2007). Cell phones in
Christ, T. J., & Christ, J. A. (2006). Application of an American high schools: A national survey. The Jour-
interdependent group contingency mediated by an nal of Technology Studies, 33, 41-47. https://doi.
automated feedback device: An intervention across org/10.21061/jots.v33i1.a.6
three high school classrooms. School Psychology
Review, 35, 78-90. Patchin, J. W., & Hinduja, S. (2006). Bullies move beyond
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). the schoolyard: A preliminary look at cyberbullying.
Applied Behavior Analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 4, 148-169. https://
River, NJ: Pearson. doi.org/10.1177/1541204006286288
Groves, E. A. & Austin, J. L. (2017). An evaluation of Thomas, K., & Orthober, C. (2011). Using text-
interdependent and independent group contingencies messaging in the secondary classroom. American Sec-
during the good behavior game. Journal of Applied ondary Education, 39, 55-76.
Behavior Analysis, 50, 552-566. https://doi.org/10. Trevino-Maack, S. I., Kamps, D., & Wills, H. (2015).
1002/jaba.393 A group contingency plus self-management interven-
Groves, E. A. and Austin, J. L. (2018). Does the Good tion targeting at-risk secondary students’ class-work
Behavior Game evoke negative peer pressure? Ana- and active engagement. Remedial and Special
lyses in primary and secondary classrooms. Journal of Education, 36, 347-360. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Applied Behavior Analysis. https://doi.org/10.1002/ 0741932514561865
jaba.513 Wolf, M. M. (1978). Social validity: the case for subjec-
Hirsch, S. E., Healy, S. , Judge, J. P., & Lloyd, J. W. tive measurement or how applied behavior analysis is
(2016). Effects of an interdependent group contin- finding its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
gency on engagement in physical education. Journal 11, 203-214. http://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1978.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49, 975-979. https://doi. 11-203
org/10.1002/jaba.328
Junco, R., & Cotten, S. R. (2012). No A 4 U: The rela- Wood, E., Zivcakova, L., Gentile, P., Archer, K., De
tionship between multitasking and academic perfor- Pasquale, D., & Nosko, A. (2012). Examining the
mance. Computers & Education, 59, 505-514. https:// impact of off-task multi-tasking with technology on
doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2011.12.023 real-time classroom learning. Computers & Education,
Kuznekoff, J. H., & Titsworth, S. (2013). The impact of 58, 365-374. https://doi.10.1016/j.compedu.2011.
mobile phone usage on student learning. Communica- 08.029
tion Education, 62, 233-252. https://doi.org/10.
1080/03634523.2013.767917 Received July 6, 2017
Litow, L., & Pumroy, D. K. (1975). A brief review of Final acceptance April 12, 2018
classroom group-oriented contingencies. Journal of Action Editor, Jeanne Donaldson

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi