Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 13

Critical Issues in Dental Education

Clinical Reasoning in Dentistry: A


Conceptual Framework for Dental Education
Shiva Khatami, D.D.S., Cert. Ortho., Ph.D.; Michael I. MacEntee, L.D.S.(I), Dip. Prosth.,
F.R.C.D.(C), Ph.D.; Daniel D. Pratt, Ph.D.; John B. Collins, Ph.D.
Abstract: This study presents a conceptual framework for clinical reasoning by dental students. Using a think-aloud method with
six vignettes, the researchers interviewed eighteen dental students from two stages of training about oral health-related problems
influenced by biopsychosocial factors. Verbatim transcripts of the interviews were analyzed to identify the processes and strate-
gies of clinical reasoning used by the students to produce treatment plans. The process included 1) rituals to collect information;
2) forward and backward reasoning to generate and test clinical hypotheses; 3) pattern recognition from integrated scripts of
knowledge and experience; and 4) decision trees to assess options and outcomes. The process was supplemented by scientific,
conditional, collaborative, narrative, ethical, pragmatic, and part-whole reasoning strategies. Senior students showed a keen
awareness of the contextual determinants of care and emphasized patients’ motivations for treatment. In contrast, junior students
focused more on problems associated with individual teeth as they struggled to integrate the information within each vignette. In
this article, the processes and strategies for reasoning used by both groups of dental students are abstracted and then illustrated by
a model of clinical reasoning that accommodates the complicated contexts in which clinical problems usually arise.
Dr. Khatami is Assistant Professor, Department of Orthodontics, College of Dental Medicine, Nova Southeastern University;
this study was conducted at the University of British Columbia while he was a Ph.D. student there; Dr. MacEntee is Professor
of Prosthodontics and Dental Geriatrics, Oral Health Sciences, University of British Columbia; Dr. Pratt is Professor, Educational
Studies, University of British Columbia; and Dr. Collins is Adjunct Professor, Educational Studies, University of British
Columbia. Direct correspondence and requests for reprints to Dr. Shiva Khatami, Department of Orthodontics, College of
Dental Medicine, Nova Southeastern University, 3200 South University Drive, Fort Lauderdale, FL 33328; 954-262-1896;
shiva.khatami@nova.edu.
Keywords: dental education, dental students, clinical reasoning
Submitted for publication 4/27/11; accepted 11/9/11

D
ental professionals’ competence in clinical they contribute little to the effectiveness of curricular
reasoning is essential for the delivery of oral models and assessment methods in dental education.1
health care. Such competence requires that Existing definitions and models of clinical
the dentist utilize integrated networks of knowledge reasoning tend to be specific to the individual health
to identify and solve clinical problems. Interacting professions and reflect a diversity of clinical profes-
with patients demands competent communications, sions.2-5 Medicine, for example, has focused mainly
critical thinking, professionalism, ethics, and aware- on the process of diagnosis and for the most part falls
ness of the social and cultural context of practice; yet short of describing how physicians plan treatment.5-7
we know very little about when or how these skills In contrast, physiotherapy and occupational therapy
evolve, develop, or become integrated for dental stu- have explored the complicated and interactive pro-
dents during their education. Consequently, dentistry cess those clinicians employ when negotiating and
lacks a robust and comprehensive model of clinical implementing plans.8,9 Higgs and Jones9 recently
reasoning to explain the complicated cognitive and portrayed this reasoning process as a contextual
interactive processes that guide dental clinicians and interactive phenomenon within a multilayered
when identifying, framing, and solving the wide context involving patients, clinicians, and clinical
range of oral health problems. Existing models of problems surrounded by an even larger social, cul-
clinical reasoning in dentistry, as in medicine, con- tural, and global environment.
centrate predominantly on the process of diagnos- Studies of clinical reasoning were originally
ing diseases, but they offer no accommodation for based to a large extent on the observational methods
the more complicated decisions associated with the and psychometric theories of cognitive psychol-
psychosocial determinants of health. Consequently, ogy.7,10 The focus initially was on observable clinical

1116 Journal of Dental Education  ■  Volume 76, Number 9


performance but broadened later to include the cogni- in evidence-based medicine and medical informat-
tive processes underlying clinical performance.10,11 ics to assist rational and evidence-based decisions
This change coincided with the early investigations in clinical practice.11 Recently, medical educators
in medical education of problem-solving skills have favored a conceptual model for the diagnostic
and an emphasis on the “primacy of process over process by incorporating a “dual processing” theory
content”3 by exploring and modelling the reasoning of reasoning.7,23-25 Process 1 resembles pattern rec-
process used by experts. Elstein et al.,3 for example, ognition and forward reasoning that unconsciously
introduced the hypothetico-deductive (H-D) model compares the situation at hand with previous expe-
of solving medical problems whereby the clinician riences. Process 2, in contrast, is highly analytical
acquires cues, generates hypotheses, interprets the and resembles H-D reasoning. The dual processing
cues, and evaluates the hypotheses. Later, more elab- theory explains how illness scripts are triggered and
orate versions of the H-D model were introduced,2,12 processed through a non-analytical approach when
although with little success in explaining differences clinicians encounter a typical presentation of a dis-
between the reasoning processes of novice and expert ease and how they adopt an analytical approach when
clinicians. they encounter an atypical presentation of a disease
Groen and Patel13 sought an alternative ex- for which they cannot find a matching script.23
planation for those differences and suggested that Starting in the 1980s, there was growing inter-
experts apply “forward or data-driven” reasoning est especially among nurses, occupational therapists,
to arrive at a diagnostic hypothesis, while novices and physiotherapists to “interpret” clinical reasoning
apply “backward or hypothesis-driven” reasoning to with an emphasis on the contextual issues influencing
find data that confirm or reject a list of hypotheses diagnostic and treatment decisions.26,27 Interpretive
about the problem. However, it is unclear how the inquiry, for example, was used to explore the nature
novices construct the hypothesis in the first place and of expertise and the role of intuition when clini-
whether or not experts are influenced by a dominant cians encounter clinical problems and the reasoning
hypothesis while they collect and interpret clinical strategies they use to deal with the biopsychosocial
information. About the same time, pattern recognition determinants of health.8,9,28,29
emerged as an explanation for the rapid and efficient Research in clinical reasoning in dentistry has
problem-solving skills of experts, particularly in largely focused on decision theory, particularly to
highly visual material.7,14-16 This idea led further to develop computerized decision support systems.30,31
studies of the structure and application of knowledge Also, H-D models32 and pattern recognition through
in medical problem-solving across levels of expertise, use of a “caries script”33 have offered descriptions of
which led to introductions of schemas,17 prototypes,18 the diagnostic process in dentistry. Results to date
exemplars,19 and illness scripts20 as explanations of suggest that dental experts use “forward reasoning”
the differences between diagnostic reasoning of nov- to produce diagnostic hypotheses from clinical data,
ice and expert physicians.13 Boshuizen and Schmidt21 whereas novices produce the hypotheses before mov-
proposed a theory of medical expertise, suggesting ing “backward” to confirm or reject the hypotheses
three stages of development for organizing and in- from their interpretation of the clinical data.34 How-
tegrating biomedical knowledge for a diagnosis. Ini- ever, the diversity in focus and approach to clinical
tially, the novice builds a “propositional network” to reasoning research within and across health care dis-
explain the pathophysiological mechanisms underly- ciplines poses a challenge to integrate their findings.
ing a disease and then gives the network a diagnostic We do not know that a universal or generic model of
label following further exposure to patients. During clinical reasoning can serve other disciplines, such
the intermediate stage when exposed to additional as dentistry.9,35 This article reports on a study to ad-
clinical experience, the biomedical knowledge be- dress 1) the reasoning strategies used by a group of
comes “encapsulated” within illness scripts, and junior and senior dental students at the University
repeated clinical encounters refine the scripts as the of British Columbia to address the biopsychosocial
clinician evolves from novice to expert. determinants of oral health and 2) the similarities and
Elstein and Schwartz,22 in their review of the differences in clinical reasoning between the junior
cognitive literature, drew attention to the heuristics and senior dental students. The results provide a basis
and biases influencing the diagnostic decisions of for a conceptual framework for the clinical reasoning
physicians. They were encouraged by an interest of dental students.

September 2012  ■  Journal of Dental Education 1117


to reveal a student’s knowledge and awareness of
Methods the psychosocial and ethical factors influencing the
problems, treatment options, prognosis, and context
With approval from the University of British of clinical practice. We incorporated these faculty
Columbia’s Ethical Review Board, we selected as members’ suggestions into the vignettes.
participants nine full-time members of the dental fac- The eighteen students were selected purpose-
ulty from various dental specialties along with eight fully36 with assistance from the associate dean for
dental students at the beginning and ten at the end academic affairs to provide a similar distribution of
of their final year of the predoctoral dental program. participants with below-average, average, and above-
We constructed six situational vignettes2,21 about two average academic standings. Semistructured, tape-
hypothetical patients to represent a realistic simula- recorded individual interviews lasting about one hour
tion of student encounters with clinical problems were conducted by one of the authors (SK) in private
(Figure 1). The vignettes offered information about with each student. At the beginning of the interview,
the two patients to challenge the student participants instructions were given on the “think-aloud” method37
with a range of biopsychosocial and ethical problems. before introducing the first vignette and a request for
The nine faculty members were interviewed thoughts on each scenario from diagnosis to treatment
individually for their opinions on the appropriateness plan. The students were told at the beginning of the
of the vignettes as reflections of the clinical problems interview that diagnostic casts, photographs, and
typically encountered by dental students. They were radiographs for each vignette were available to them
asked if the vignettes were likely to prompt responses for inspection at any time during the interview, but

VIGNETTE 1
A 42-year-old woman attends the university dental clinic. She says, “I want to have new dentures.
My old plate is not stable and is broken. I want to fix my teeth and take care of them.”
PATIENT 1

VIGNETTE 2
After you present a treatment plan, the 42-year-old woman shows concerns about the cost of
treatment. She asks you if you could extract all her teeth and make a set of dentures.
VIGNETTE 3
You are at the end of fourth year, and you need two more crowns to complete your requirements.
The patient is reluctant to pay for the caries-management program and is wondering if you can
just make the removable partial denture without the crowns.
VIGNETTE 4
A 35-year-old man comes to the clinic complaining of pain from broken anterior teeth. He says, “My
front teeth are broken and they are hurting me, but the pain comes and goes. Also, my molar tooth
is broken and I am not able to chew on both sides.”
VIGNETTE 5
You consult your clinical instructor about a white lesion in a patient’s mouth. The instructor believes
PATIENT 2

that the lesion is leukoplakia and it should be monitored for change. You consult a periodontist
about this patient on a different matter, and he notices the white lesion in the patient’s mouth. He
believes that the lesion needs to be biopsied immediately. He rebukes you as a senior student for
ignoring such an important issue and is upset when you tell him in your defense that your clinical
instructor doesn’t agree that a biopsy is needed yet.
VIGNETTE 6
As soon as you tell your instructor about the patient’s smoking habit, he tells the patient to quit
smoking right away because, otherwise, the white lesion in the mouth could become cancerous
and kill him. The patient is quite upset about the way he is treated by your instructor and tells you
that such an attitude would not help him to stop smoking. He asks you to schedule his
appointments on the days when that instructor is not around.

Figure 1. Situational vignettes about two hypothetical patients

1118 Journal of Dental Education  ■  Volume 76, Number 9


only on request. They were prompted, if necessary, understanding its implications. For example, some
to elaborate or clarify explanations, and all explana- were unsure about the reasons for classifying peri-
tions were accepted without comment or judgment. odontal status when “treatment [seems to be] always
Each interview was transcribed verbatim and the same” (JSG), while others reported that the ritual
the texts analyzed by two investigators (SK and MM). was time-consuming and delayed treatment.
Our content analyses identified and coded the spe- Backward (deductive) and forward (induc-
cific steps in the process of discovering, interpreting, tive) reasoning. The students used both backward
and solving problems as explained by each student and forward reasoning to diagnose clinical problems.
participant. With guidance from descriptions by When they observed a white lesion in the mouth, for
Higgs and Jones,9 codes were assigned deductively example, some started with a diagnostic hypothesis
to relevant segments of each transcript. The codes and worked backwards to accept or reject it, as in
were later clustered as broader categories, and themes this comment:
emerged describing the processes and strategies of
[The lesion can be] a hyperkeratosis caused
reasoning used by the participants. We also used a
by trauma . . . [or] lichen planus [which]
bottom-up inductive approach36 to search for new
sometimes has diffuse pattern of light
reasoning processes and strategies. Subsequently,
patches. . . . You might be thinking about any
we summarized the processes and strategies to com-
kind of viral [causes]; however, this doesn’t
pare the junior and senior students. In reporting the
look too much like that. . . . [It could be] a
results, we will identify students by the following
known drug reaction. [So I try to] find out
letters: “JS” is a junior student in the beginning of the
if they’re taking any drugs and see if there’s
fourth year, while “SS” is a senior student at the end
anything that’s causing trauma. . . . Candida
of fourth year. The letters “A,” “B,” “C,” etc. were
[Albicans] is sometimes white. . . . See if it
assigned to students based on the order in which they
rubs off and then leaves erythema behind.
were interviewed, so that, for example, “JSG” is the
. . . [I am] concerned about malignancy, like
seventh junior student interviewed.
a squamous cell carcinoma. (JSC)
Alternatively, other students collected informa-
Results tion and moved forward to a diagnostic hypothesis,
although they occasionally reasoned backward from
an initial diagnosis to form an alternative hypothesis,
Processes of Clinical Reasoning as in this example:
The student participants reasoned their way
through the vignettes by 1) collecting and evalu- First thing would be to ask the patient about
ating information, 2) identifying and interpreting medicine, drugs, smoking, alcohol use. I’d
problems, 3) evaluating options, and 4) diagnos- check the other side [of the mouth] to see if
ing diseases and planning treatment. The passage [the lesion] is bilateral. . . . If you could rub
was not always linear or in need of all four stages. it off . . . it is a Candida infection. . . . I’d
However, the students were usually guided by ritual; look for anything in the mouth that could be
forward or backward reasoning or a combination of irritating those areas. . . . There is always a
the two; pattern recognition and scripts; and deci- concern that it could be a dysplasia or can-
sion analysis. cer. . . . [If] it has a cobblestone appearance,
Ritual. All participants followed the clinical you consider things like Crohn’s disease or
protocol of the dental school in a ritual of obtain- inflammatory bowel disease. (SSD)
ing medical, dental, and social histories; exploring A combination of forward and backward reasoning
the chief complaint; and assessing clinical data and seemed to compensate for a student’s limited expe-
diagnostic aids, such as standard odontograms, rience and was used when dealing with signs and
radiographs, and study models, all of which were symptoms common to several problems.
available only on request. Subsequently, they planned Pattern recognition through scripts. Visual
a sequence of treatment, usually starting with man- and historical cues helped the students to recognize
agement of disease followed by rehabilitation and the patterns of specific diseases, like caries, peri-
maintenance of health. There were participants who odontal diseases, and other mucosal abnormalities.
seemed to follow each step of the ritual without A pattern helped to bypass the process of making

September 2012  ■  Journal of Dental Education 1119


and testing hypotheses and was followed often by is something that I would prefer to refer to
requests for additional clinical information to expand somebody who is more experienced. (SSG)
the differential diagnosis.
None of the students suggested numbers to
Most of the students diagnosed caries from the
quantify the probability or utility value of the options.
color and appearance of lesions and often requested
Decision analysis seemed to be an interpretive activ-
additional information about the tactile feel of the
ity by which the students integrated their knowledge
lesion. Assessing the etiology and risk of caries and
and experience to assess the problem and options for
tooth loss required a more elaborate exploration mov-
intervention and to ensure a reasonable and practical
ing from physical to psychosocial considerations.
outcome.
Generally, the scripts relating to caries consisted of
a network of information about the saliva and oral
bacteria along with details on the patient’s diet, hy- Reasoning Strategies
giene, and socioeconomic status, all of which guided The students used seven reasoning strategies to
a student to a therapeutic decision. interpret and address the problems: scientific, con-
Scripts for periodontal diseases were trig- ditional, collaborative, narrative, ethical, pragmatic,
gered mainly by visual cues about the normal versus and part-whole reasoning.
abnormal appearance of the gingiva around teeth. Scientific reasoning. The students applied the
Measurements of gingival recession, periodontal H-D process of reasoning, which emulates a scientific
pockets, and other numerical data supplemented the method to analyze clinical data and test the diag-
visual cues in the scripts to help classify the severity nostic hypotheses. However, this method was often
of the disease. The classification was used then to supplemented by nonanalytical approaches such as
determine the course of treatment. pattern recognition. We found that the students based
Visual cues helped to diagnose mucosal lesions. their knowledge on theory and experience derived
Malignant lesions, for example, were triggered by a from textbooks and manuals of didactic and clinical
script that included information about the location of courses supplemented by the opinions of instructors
the lesion and cues from the patient’s psychosocial and other students. They were aware of the limitation
background, which influenced prognosis and treat- and uncertainty of scientific evidence to support their
ment. However, in general, all the students struggled decisions because, according to SSG, “[Scientific
with diagnosing mucosal lesions and did not seem evidence] gives me an indication of what sort of
to have developed a reliable diagnostic script for treatment has been done on teeth like this [but] has
oral lesions. it been successful or is it just a five-year recall or has
Decision analysis. Decisions to treat problems it been a ten-year recall? . . . Another thing is: every
usually involved a process of 1) interpreting the prob- patient is different, oral hygiene is different, their
lem, 2) developing options, and 3) evaluating options. habits are different. . . . You have to look into more
A senior student, for example, evaluating options for parameters: how was the study done, what type of
restoring a patient’s mutilated dentition stated: patients . . . what was their guideline for success?”
Conditional reasoning. Conditional reasoning
She doesn’t have any posterior occlusal sup-
involved a historical analysis of the biological, psy-
port and room for her denture. . . . [I would]
chological, and socioeconomic factors that contribute
determine if these teeth are restorable or not
to the initiation and progression of problems. This
and whether we’re going to be using them as
analysis helped the students to explore the source
overdenture abutments or whether we’re just
of problems, envision the problems that could arise
going to be doing a complete upper denture.
in the future, and evaluate the possible outcomes
. . . [I would] determine whether we can
of their selected intervention. Uncertainty about
place any artificial teeth there or not and, if
therapeutic outcomes dominated clinical decisions.
we do, can we open up more space by doing
Biological, psychological, and socioeconomic fac-
an enameloplasty or orthodontics. But that’s
tors were considered routinely by most students. As
pretty hard. . . . You have to present her with
SSJ commented, “You can’t skip caries management
options of bridges, implants. . . . A bridge
altogether because . . . if you crown that tooth before
. . . would be a long span [and] compromise
getting rid of the caries, the caries might spread
the natural dentition. . . . It might look easy
around. . . . There’s just too many problems down
now but it could haunt you later. . . . This
the road.” Another student explained that achiev-

1120 Journal of Dental Education  ■  Volume 76, Number 9


ing the objectives of treatment depended on the a contextual mix of social, cultural, and economic
condition of a patient’s motivation because “it can’t factors, as illustrated in this lament by JSB: “I know
just be me trying to fix the problems” (SSB). Other it’s expensive . . . but the reality is that sometimes
students considered financial implications of future you just can’t afford anything but clearance [of teeth]
problems: for example, “To save these teeth for as and a new denture. . . . It’s really sad that the money
long as possible would be financially a benefit to her. would dictate what kind of treatment you would get,
. . . By 80 [years] she is going to have no bone. . . . but that’s the way the world is I guess. . . . If she re-
So there could be a whole new set of problems and ally, really can’t afford [restorations], the alternative
financial burdens” (SSB). is walking around with no teeth . . . that’s a worse
Collaborative reasoning. All the students re- alternative. So, I probably would end up [extracting
ferred to the significance of interacting with patients the teeth].” Another student, SSG, addressed a similar
and seeking their opinions about treatment decisions dilemma but with a more inductive approach as he
in an effort to reach a shared understanding of the reasoned his way to a final decision: “Do you just
most desirable and feasible treatment outcome, share ignore the patient and say go next door or do you clue
the responsibility of making decisions, and educate this patient . . . like autonomy, this is what they want,
the patient to facilitate decision making or promote [but] can you do it? I consider both sides. . . . I would
a change in behavior. In most situations, the students probably [extract the teeth] because she’s probably
seemed to accept that patients collaborate in the going to go next door and receive the [extractions]
decision and usually have further collaboration with and might not have [received the] informed consent
instructors because there are “just so many things that as maybe I would have provided. So I feel comfort-
can happen” and “so many options” (SSI). able. . . . I would not refuse treatment as long as I
Narrative reasoning. This strategy helped the know it’s an informed decision.”
students understand from the patient’s perspective the Pragmatic reasoning. Although the students
meaning of problems and expectations from treat- tended to follow the rules and requirements of the
ment. Some participants reflected on the assumptions clinic, some clinical problems raised issues relating to
that they bring to this interpretive process and on a the social, economic, or political environment of the
comparison of values and beliefs between themselves health care system. This process called for pragmatic
and their patients. Sometimes, they drew attention to reasoning to help their patients and themselves. To
a problem that they had encountered previously to help patients with financial problems, for example,
help interpret the vignette and to justify a diagnostic some students were disposed to “bending the rules”
decision. of the clinic. SSA commented, “If the patient [is]
Ethical reasoning. The students tended to really, really tight on money . . . I’ll still [provide
tackle ethical problems either by starting from an treatment] for free . . . [and] write on the chart that it
ethical principle and then analyzing the specific case is done . . . but I wouldn’t enter it under a computer
by using that principle or by reasoning their way code. . . . It is an educational facility and we are
through a case toward a principle-based decision. helping poor people.”
The former might be called deductive ethical reason- When problems included interpersonal conflict
ing and the latter inductive ethical reasoning. When between students and their instructors, the students
refusing to extract healthy teeth, for example, one felt vulnerable, clinically inexperienced, politically
student asserted an ethical stance by saying, “I’m at powerless, and fearful of becoming “a target for un-
the point in my life where I’m not going to compro- fair assessments” (JSB). Students in general believed
mise my morals and my values for another person” that “you just don’t argue with anyone” in the clinic
(JSC). Patient autonomy guided other decisions, such (JSB), even if an instructor seems unreasonable or
as “if that’s what she wants after she knows all her wrong. The key to pragmatic reasoning for survival
options . . . you do what [she] wants” (JSH). by many students was to “fly below the radar” and
Ethical decisions typically were made rela- “keep quiet on a lot of things and just let it be” (SSD).
tively quickly and confidently unless competing Part-whole reasoning. Students used several
ethical principles were evident, such as patient reasoning strategies to refocus on various aspects of
autonomy versus practitioner maleficence when a a clinical problem and from one problem to another.
patient wanted healthy teeth extracted, which caused Often, interpreting problems and evaluating options
students much discomfort. Inductive approaches for treatment involved integrating several reasoning
predominated when the ethical problem contained strategies and moving away from a specific problem

September 2012  ■  Journal of Dental Education 1121


to consider the larger context within which the prob- frame of reference that dominated the problem. If
lem occurred. This was most evident when consid- the students interpreted the problem based solely
ering how the needs of an individual tooth relate to on their personal frame of reference, the problem
the dentition as a whole or more generally, in order could be perceived differently from when they tried
to “treat the whole patient, not just the chief com- to look at the problem from the patient’s perspective.
plaint” (SSI). One student summarized this strategy For example, some students believed that it is justifi-
as “you’ve got to remember there’s a person around able to scare a patient about the risk of cancer if the
that mouth as well” (SSE). patient has a precancerous lesion in the mouth and
Generally, clinical reasoning of the students smokes. However, some students looked at smoking
followed a combination of analytical and nonanalyti- as a habit that is difficult to break; therefore, “it’s the
cal reasoning processes. The students adopted and responsibility of a dentist to inform their patients and
integrated the identified reasoning strategies to ad- give them advice on stopping smoking and health
dress the contextual issues influencing their approach advice . . . but it’s not our place to judge or accuse
to problems. In the following section, we describe patients” (SSE).
the contextual aspects of clinical reasoning and the The students referred to the challenge of deal-
interactions of the contexts. ing with patients’ misconceptions about health and
diseases and the need to educate patients about the
Contexts of Clinical Reasoning benefits of certain treatment options to “convince”
patients that the treatment is in their best interest.
The students’ clinical reasoning occurred in a
For example, some students recognized that the
multilayered context including their own and their
preventive approach “doesn’t click-in all that often”
patients’ personal frames of reference. These were
for people who believe that cavities can be simply
influenced by a larger array of social, cultural, politi-
drilled and filled (SSD). Therefore, presenting caries
cal, and economic contexts.
management as “part of the general protocol of dental
Personal frame of reference. Combinations
treatment” rather than as an “add-on” or “money
of knowledge, values, beliefs, and past experiences
grab” achieved a positive response and acceptance
shaped the personal frames of reference for the stu-
from patients (SSD).
dents as explained by SSD: “to solve the problem,
Nature of the problem. The specific nature
you have to address why it happened. If you continue
of some problems influenced the care offered by the
just to patch the holes, you never address why, and
students. For example, most students considered pain,
the patient continues to go down a road where the
bleeding, swelling, or infection to be urgent problems
disease progresses or continues.”
to address before anything else. The impact of each
Some clinical experiences had negative impacts
problem on general health and quality of life also
on students, usually associated procedures with large
influenced the approach to care.
psychosocial characteristics. JSD explained with
The health care environment. All of the
frustration how
students reflected on the environment of the dental
[making a denture] would be opening up a clinic. Some commented how they will approach
can of worms that I wouldn’t want to deal care differently when they graduate. They acknowl-
with. . . . Patients are very finicky when it edged that the context of clinical reasoning extends
comes to their dentures: they expect them to beyond the teaching clinic to the larger health care
be [like] their natural dentition. . . . They get environment. As JSB commented, “It depends on
dentures and they need to get them relined or what your neighborhood is like; if you practice [in a
remade in five years [and] they think you’ve poor neighbourhood], you’re not going to be doing
done something wrong; when in reality, their crowns and bridges and implants. . . . A lot of patients
mouth is changing . . . and even though I try come here, [and] they end up going for suboptimal
ad nauseam to explain things to patients, treatment because they can’t afford anything else.”
they don’t want to hear it, and they only get
frustrated with you, and then you’re carrying Similarities and Differences in
that frustration.
Reasoning
Patient’s frame of reference. The approach to The students’ clinical reasoning differed at
some problems differed depending on the personal several levels within and between groups of students.

1122 Journal of Dental Education  ■  Volume 76, Number 9


The major difference appeared to be the impact of tended towards standard rituals, such as the written
their personal frame of reference for identifying protocol of the clinic, whereas more experienced
and interpreting problems. This interpretive process students were more flexible with their own routines.
determined how the students appraised alternative The juniors moved slowly from the biological to the
treatments within their own preferences. Past expe- psychosocial characteristics of a patient’s problem,
riences helped to recognize the pattern of problems but the seniors could prioritize by exploring patients’
or diagnostic and management scripts although most expectations and motivations and personal context.
students acknowledged their limited experience. It seemed also that the senior students were
Limitations of knowledge and experience influenced more aware of their personal frame of reference, their
their abilities to integrate information into a coher- individual view or philosophy of care, and how that
ent plan, especially among the junior students. JSA influenced their interpretation of problems and their
explained that “It’s hard to put everything together. approach to care. They reflected on their professional
. . . I know bits and pieces here and there, but it’s just responsibilities as dentists and believed that at the end
integrating everything, which hopefully I’ll get better of their predoctoral education they were “almost at
at it this year. . . . There’s so much . . . I need to be able the same level as instructor[s]” (SSF) although with
to integrate everything together. . . . What’s the best some limitations.
option for this patient? At which stage would you do
what? In what sequence? . . . Integrating everything A Conceptual Framework
into a logical, sequential plan as well as evidence or
Figure 2 portrays a conceptual model of clinical
experience for why you’d want to do that—that’s
reasoning in dentistry developed from the findings
what I don’t have at this stage.”
of this study. The multilayered context of clinical
Generally, the students used rituals to collect
reasoning is shown as overlapping ovals to denote
information and plan treatment. The junior students

Figure 2. Conceptual model of clinical reasoning in dentistry

September 2012  ■  Journal of Dental Education 1123


the personal frame of reference of the dentist, the more comprehensive biopsychosocial approach to
frame of reference of the patient, the problem(s), and clinical reasoning for the complex chronic diseases
the larger health care environment. The dentist uses managed by dentistry.
a nonlinear cyclical process to diagnose problems Overall, we found that almost all the students
and plan treatment within the broader context of the combined both analytical and nonanalytical reason-
health care environment and under the influence of ing, depending on the nature of the problems and
separate reasoning strategies that are integrated by their personal experiences. This combination is
the part-whole strategy. consistent with the dual processing model of medical
problem-solving as described by Norman25 and with
the recommendation by medical educators favoring
Discussion a combination to improve the accuracy of diagnostic
decisions.24,39,40 Further studies using standardized
Our research question was based on the premise vignettes, more diverse groups of students, and
that clinical reasoning in dentistry is an integrated dentists with a wide range of expertise are needed
process of diagnosis and treatment planning. There- to consider further the process of clinical reasoning
fore, we paid equal attention to exploring both activi- used in dentistry.
ties. Our findings suggest that dental students rarely Descriptions of scientific reasoning such as the
follow a linear process as they unravel and address H-D reasoning refer mostly to the analytical process
clinical problems (i.e., starting with diagnosis and of diagnosis.27,41 We used this term to denote the
ending with a treatment plan). As the students evalu- strategies used by students to support their reasoning
ate information, nonanalytical approaches such as with any kind of evidence. The limitation of scien-
pattern recognition through illness scripts help those tific evidence was identified by students as a barrier
with more experience to arrive quickly at a diagno- prompting the need for either personal experiences
sis. The diagnostic ritual ensured a comprehensive or the argument of authority to arrive at a decision.
collection and appraisal of information. We left the Nonetheless, despite the growing interest in evidence-
collection of information entirely to the student par- based dentistry and promoting “rational” decision
ticipants without offering information unless it was making of dentists,30 not everyone uses the available
requested, which encouraged them to explain the scientific evidence routinely. Dental students and
rituals they use. Diagnostic rituals were often supple- experienced dentists do not follow Bayesian formu-
mented by a linear process of listing and prioritizing las even when instructed to do so with appropriate
the problems before planning treatment. However, evidence.42 Apparently, they believe that it is difficult
when evaluating alternative treatment options, most to estimate a probability for the presence or prognosis
of the students needed to reevaluate clinical infor- of a disease based on epidemiological data alone.
mation, reassess the accuracy of their diagnosis, and Clearly, reasoning in dentistry extends well beyond a
revisit their treatment plans accordingly. purely scientific and biomedical discipline to involve
The student participants used a mixture of a range of sophisticated practical and experience-
analytical and nonanalytical approaches for diagno- based sociocultural and historical phenomena.43-45
ses. This process necessitated a backward reasoning We found that uncertainty when addressing
to identify specific problems although most of the complex problems leads to conditional reasoning.
students combined forward and backward reasoning For example, evaluating prognosis requires an un-
strategies as they unravelled the problems before derstanding of many factors involved in the initiation
them. Likewise, Crespo et al.34 found that dentists and progression of the condition. The same strategy is
across all levels of expertise used a combination of used by occupational therapists and physiotherapists
forward and backward reasoning in diagnosis. Den- as they anticipate the impact of their interventions.41
tists use scripts to confirm the presence of caries as Furthermore, one of the dominant applications of
described previously.32,33 Dental students also identi- conditional reasoning by the dental students was the
fied pattern recognition and scripts as nonanalytical condition set by the financial status of their patients
reasoning processes for diagnosing caries. Caries on the selection of treatment. These students read-
scripts help to reveal “upstream contributing” fac- ily modified the treatment plan so that it would be
tors such as psychosocial issues that initiate and financially beneficial to the patient.
sustain caries as described by Baelum et al.,38 so it Collaborative reasoning has been found to help
appears that the students were beginning to adopt a clinicians to arrive at a shared understanding of prob-

1124 Journal of Dental Education  ■  Volume 76, Number 9


lems and treatment objectives.9 Communicating with on students needs further exploration as they develop
patients and exploring their perceptions of problems their professional identity and socialize into their
also involves narrative reasoning.8 Since our study professional roles.
did not involve real patients or role-playing with sim- We noticed that part-whole reasoning was the
ulated patients, our descriptions of collaborative and core strategy dominating the reasoning of all the par-
narrative reasoning reflect the comments made about ticipants. With it, the students were able to integrate
the use of those strategies only when a hypothetical problems as they moved in and out of a comfortable
situation demanded such communications. Loftus46 frame of reference for patients and others involved
has stated that communication and use of language in care. The students evaluated problems individually
are the essential aspects of clinical reasoning. Our and in relation to other problems in the larger social,
model illustrates the interaction among clinicians, cultural, political, and financial contexts. Part-whole
patients, and others (e.g., consultant specialists). reasoning helped the participants integrate other strat-
Interactive and interpretive activities are impossible egies of clinical reasoning and supported a compre-
without language; however, the think-aloud method hensive plan to address the biopsychosocial factors
does not replicate the complexity of the “real world” that influence problems across different contexts.
in which clinicians typically interact with problems, The most recent model of clinical reasoning,
patients, and others. introduced by Higgs and Jones,9 highlights the inter-
Consequently, the value of our model will be active and contextual nature of this thought process.
confirmed only when tested during real communi- Higgs and Loftus35 emphasize the need to develop
cations between clinicians and their patients. This discipline-specific models of clinical reasoning for
might be achieved by observational studies using research, practice, and education. The model of
video or other surveillance, although research in clinical reasoning that we have constructed from our
clinical settings is challenging to avoid jeopardizing study illustrates the dynamic interaction of these con-
the privacy of the patient-clinician relationship and texts, largely through the application of part-whole
the Hawthorne effect. reasoning, and reflects the biopsychosocial nature
In our study, the dental students’ ethical reason- of problems and approach to care in dentistry. We
ing followed the top-down, bottom-up, and combined showed how application of conditional and pragmatic
approaches described by Edwards and Delany47 as the reasoning requires a conceptual and pedagogical
students struggled with the ethical dilemmas posed shift from the biology of disease to a larger context
by the vignettes. We found that a deductive approach of patients within society’s health care system. We
to ethical problems resulted in a faster decision, and believe that dental educators and students benefit
students who used this approach seemed more cer- from diverse clinical environments in which students
tain and confident. However, a deductive approach are exposed to a wide range of psychosocial issues
involving two or more ethical principles caused un- influencing oral health.
certainty and confusion. Alternatively, the students Problem-based learning (PBL) is the most
who moved back and forth between inductive and dominant educational method in dentistry with
deductive ethical reasoning could more easily justify claims to improving the problem-solving skills of
their decisions on treatments. dental students by focusing on the development of
These dental students used pragmatic reasoning an H-D reasoning process.49-51 A systematic review
to address the problems inherent in the larger social, by Rochmawati and Wiechula52 found inconclusive
cultural, political, and economic context of practice. evidence regarding the effectiveness of PBL in
Again, financial issues dominated the discourse about improving clinical reasoning. They acknowledged
access to care for some patients. The dental students the diversity of PBL in various educational settings
worried about their personal relationships and teach- and called for further studies to explore the impact
ers, which hindered their opportunities to engage in of PBL on how health professions students reason.
critical thinking or act on their beliefs about social Charlin et al.53 compared the PBL practices within
responsibility to disadvantaged patients. Henzi et al.48 three Canadian medical programs and suggested that
discovered similar issues among students in other problems be used as media for applying and relating
North American dental schools where relationships theoretical knowledge to practice. Others7,24,54 have
with faculty, bureaucracy, and pressures to become recommended that clinical problems be selected
clinically competent occasionally compromised their according to specific educational objectives. We
ethical approach to care. The influence of these issues recommend that dental educators select problems that

September 2012  ■  Journal of Dental Education 1125


integrate reasoning strategies to address the multitude treatment-planning process to identify and evaluate
of biopsychosocial determinants of oral health. alternative approaches that can address the problem.
Dental students could benefit from the concept These interpretive activities are also influenced by the
of “deliberate practice”55 to develop the networks interactions of the clinician with patients and others
of knowledge and experience required for clinical involved with the problem. Often, these interactions
reasoning. This process requires continuous exposure aim to arrive at a shared understanding of the mean-
to problems and aims involving repetition, reflection, ing of the problems and their impact on the patient’s
and feedback. Mylopoulos and Regehr56 argue that quality of life and to determine the objectives of care.
the pedagogical objective of “deliberate practices” We also found that the clinical reasoning of
should emphasize adaptive expertise rather than dental students involves an integration of several
routine or ritualistic approaches to clinical prob- reasoning strategies (scientific, conditional, collab-
lems. This objective requires a greater emphasis on orative, narrative, ethical, pragmatic, and part-whole
reflection and feedback to ensure that the contextual reasoning) to address problems as they surface within
aspects of problems are recognized and that the stu- the larger cultural, social, political, and financial
dents have opportunities to reflect on their reasoning contexts. Using part-whole reasoning strategy helps
and compare it with the reasoning of their peers and a dentist to zoom in and out of problems from a local
more experienced clinicians.57 Perhaps reflection on or specific anatomical level to the larger psychoso-
clinical reasoning could be a routine exercise given cial context. This strategy requires moving back and
to students following encounters with new clinical forth between the problems and alternative treatment
problems, possibly in the context of group discus- options to arrive at a treatment plan that optimizes
sions of real or fictitious patients. Reflections can be interventions, foresees future problems, and is flex-
used as a topic for discussion in group seminars for ible in relation to addressing changes in situation if
students to compare their reasoning to their peers new problems arise.
and mentors and be exposed to alternative frames of
reference and interpretation of problems. REFERENCES
Currently, there are several computer programs 1. Khatami S, MacEntee MI. Evolution of clinical reasoning
available to develop virtual patients in medicine and in dental education. J Dent Educ 2011;75(3):321–8.
dentistry.58,59 They offer templates that could be used 2. Barrows HS, Tamblyn RM. Problem-based learning:
by instructors to incorporate clinical and diagnostic an approach to medical education. New York: Springer
Publishers, 1980.
information about real patients into a virtual environ- 3. Elstein AS, Shulman LS, Sprafka SA. Medical problem
ment. Virtual patients do not offer the opportunity of solving: an analysis of clinical reasoning. Cambridge:
interacting with peers and mentors in face-to-face Harvard University Press, 1978.
seminars, but they provide a cost-effective alternative 4. Kassirer JP, Kopelman RI. Learning clinical reasoning.
for small-group seminars.60 They seem to be at least Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, 1991.
5. Elstein AS, Schwartz A. Clinical reasoning in medicine.
as effective as PBL seminars in achieving educational In: Higgs J, Jones MA, Loftus S, Christensen N, eds.
objectives.59 Further studies are needed to evaluate Clinical reasoning in the health professions. Oxford:
their effectiveness in simulating clinical scenarios. Butterworth-Heinemann, 2008:223–34.
6. Monajemi A, Rikers RM, Schmidt HG. Clinical case
processing: a diagnostic versus a management focus. Med
Conclusions Educ 2007;41(12):1166–72.
7. Norman G. Research in clinical reasoning: past history
and current trends. Med Educ 2005;39(4):418–27.
Our study found that the clinical reasoning
8. Fleming MH, Mattingly C. Giving language to practice.
of dental students involves a nonlinear process of In: Mattingly C, Fleming MH, eds. Clinical reasoning:
diagnosis and treatment planning. The diagnostic forms of inquiry in a therapeutic practice. Philadelphia:
process involves both analytical and nonanalytical F.A. Davis Co., 1994:3–21.
approaches depending on the nature of the problem 9. Higgs J, Jones N. Clinical decision making and multiple
at hand and on the previous experience of the clini- problem spaces. In: Higgs J, Jones MA, Loftus S, Chris-
tensen N, eds. Clinical reasoning in the health professions.
cian confronting the problem. A combination of Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann, 2008:3–17.
knowledge, experiences, values, and beliefs of the 10. Arocha JF, Patel VL. Methods in the study of clinical
clinician forms a personal frame of reference that reasoning. In: Higgs J, Jones MA, Loftus S, Christensen N,
guides an interpretive activity whereby problems are eds. Clinical reasoning in the health professions. Oxford:
identified and prioritized. This frame also directs the Butterworth-Heinemann, 2008:193–204.

1126 Journal of Dental Education  ■  Volume 76, Number 9


11. Patel VL, Kaufman DR, Arocha JF. Emerging paradigms eds. Clinical reasoning in the health professions. Oxford:
of cognition in medical decision making. J Biomed Inform Butterworth-Heinemann, 2008:257–64.
2002;35(1):52–75. 32. Maupome G, Sheiham A. Clinical decision making in re-
12. Gale J, Marsden P. Clinical problem-solving: the begin- storative dentistry: content analysis of diagnostic thinking
ning of the process. Med Educ 1982;16(1):22–6. processes and concurrent concepts used in an educational
13. Groen GJ, Patel VL. The relationship between comprehen- environment. Eur J Dent Educ 2000;4(4):143–52.
sion and reasoning in medical expertise. In: Chi M, Glaser 33. Bader JD, Shugars DA. What do we know about how
R, Farr M, eds. The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: dentists make caries-related treatment decisions? Com-
Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates, 1988:287–310. munity Dent Oral Epidemiol 1997;25(1):97–103.
14. Bleakley A, Farrow R, Gould D, Marshall R. Making 34. Crespo KE, Torres JE, Recio ME. Reasoning process char-
sense of clinical reasoning: judgment and the evidence acteristics in the diagnostic skills of beginner, competent,
of the senses. Med Educ 2003;37(6):544–52. and expert dentists. J Dent Educ 2004;68(12):1235–44.
15. Engel PJH. Tacit knowledge and visual expertise in 35. Higgs J, Loftus S. A place for new research direction in
medical diagnostic reasoning: implications for medical clinical reasoning in the health professions. In: Higgs
education. Med Teach 2008;30(7):184–8. J, Jones MA, Loftus S, Christensen N, eds. Clinical
16. Norman G. From theory to application and back again: reasoning in the health professions. Oxford: Butterworth-
implications of research on medical expertise for psycho- Heinemann, 2008:213–20.
logical theory. Can J Exp Psychol 2005;59(1):35–40. 36. Patton MQ. Qualitative research and evaluation methods.
17. Bordage G. Prototypes and semantic qualifiers: from past Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 2002.
to present. Med Educ 2007;41(12):1117–21. 37. van Someren MW, Barnard YF, Sandberg JAC. The think-
18. Bordage G, Zacks R. The structure of medical knowl- aloud method: a practical guide to modelling cognitive
edge in the memories of medical students and gen- processes. San Diego: Academic Press, 1994.
eral practitioners: categories and prototypes. Med Educ 38. Baelum V, Heidmann J, Nyvad B. Dental caries para-
1984;18(6):406–6. digms in diagnosis and diagnostic research. Eur J Oral
19. Norman GR. The epistemology of clinical reasoning: per- Sci 2006;114(4):263–77.
spectives from philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience. 39. Eva KW, Hatala RM, Leblanc VR, Brooks LR. Teaching
Acad Med 2000;75(10 Suppl):S127–35. from the clinical reasoning literature: combined reasoning
20. Charlin B, Tardif J, Boshuizen HP. Scripts and medi- strategies help novice diagnosticians overcome misleading
cal diagnostic knowledge: theory and applications for information. Med Educ 2007;41(12):1152–8.
clinical reasoning instruction and research. Acad Med 40. Pelaccia T, Tardif J, Triby E, Charlin B. An analysis of
2000;75(2):182–90. clinical reasoning through a recent and comprehensive
21. Boshuizen HPA, Schmidt HG. On the role of biomedical approach: the dual-process theory. Med Educ Online
knowledge in clinical reasoning by experts, intermediates, 2011;16:5890.
and novices. Cognit Sci 1992;16(2):153–84. 41. Chapparo C, Ranka J. Clinical reasoning in occupational
22. Elstein AS, Schwartz A. Clinical problem solving therapy. In: Higgs J, Jones MA, Loftus S, Christensen N,
and diagnostic decision making: selective review eds. Clinical reasoning in the health professions. Oxford:
of the cognitive literature. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed) Butterworth-Heinemann, 2008:265–78.
2002;324(7339):729–32. 42. Chambers DW, Mirchel R, Lundergan W. An investigation
23. Charlin B, Boshuizen HP, Custers EJ, Feltovich PJ. Scripts of dentists’ and dental students’ estimates of diagnostic
and clinical reasoning. Med Educ 2007;41(12):1178–84. probabilities. J Am Dent Assoc 2010;141(6):656–66.
24. Eva KW. What every teacher needs to know about clinical 43. Baelum V, Lopez R. Periodontal epidemiology: towards
reasoning. Med Educ 2005;39(1):98–106. social science or molecular biology? Community Dent
25. Norman G. Dual processing and diagnostic errors. Adv Oral Epidemiol 2004;32(4):239–49.
Health Sci Educ Theory Pract 2009;14(Suppl 1):37–49. 44. Larson D, Loftus S, Higgs J. Understanding knowledge as
26. Loftus S, Smith M. A history of clinical reasoning re- a sociocultural historical phenomenon. In: Higgs J, Jones
search. In: Higgs J, Jones MA, Loftus S, Christensen N, MA, Loftus S, Christensen N, eds. Clinical reasoning in
eds. Clinical reasoning in the health professions. Oxford: the health professions. Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann,
Butterworth-Heinemann, 2008:205–12. 2008:173–80.
27. Fleming MH. The therapist with the three-track mind. Am 45. MacEntee MI. Where science fails prosthodontics. Int J
J Occup Ther 1991;45(11):1007–14. Prosthodont 2007;20(4):377–81.
28. Benner P. From novice to expert: power and excellence 46. Loftus SF. Language in clinical reasoning: using and learn-
in nursing practice. Menlo Park, CA: Addison-Wesley, ing the language of collective clinical decision making.
1984. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Sydney, 2006. At: http://
29. Rew L, Agor W, Emery MR, Harper SC. Intuitive skills in ses.library.usyd.edu.au/handle/2123/1165. Accessed:
crisis management. Nurs Connections 2000;13(3):45–54. April 27, 2011.
30. Matthews DC, Gafni A, Birch S. Preference-based 47. Edwards I, Delany C. Ethical reasoning. In: Higgs J, Jones
measurements in dentistry: a review of the literature and MA, Loftus S, Christensen N, eds. Clinical reasoning in
recommendations for research. Community Dent Health the health professions. Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann,
1999;16(1):5–11. 2008:279–90.
31. Khatami S, MacEntee MI, Loftus S. Clinical reasoning in 48. Henzi D, Davis E, Jasinevicius R, Hendricson W. North
dentistry. In: Higgs J, Jones MA, Loftus S, Christensen N, American dental students’ perspectives about their clinical
education. J Dent Educ 2006;70(4):361–77.

September 2012  ■  Journal of Dental Education 1127


49. Barrows HS. The essentials of problem-based learning. J 56. Mylopoulos M, Regehr G. Cognitive metaphors of exper-
Dent Educ 1998;62(9):630–3. tise and knowledge: prospects and limitations for medical
50. Fincham AG, Shuler CF. The changing face of dental education. Med Educ 2007;41(12):1159–65.
education: the impact of PBL. J Dent Educ 2001;65(5): 57. Ericsson KA. Deliberate practice and acquisition of ex-
406–21. pert performance: a general overview. Acad Emerg Med
51. Susarla SM, Bergman AV, Howell TH, Karimbux NY. 2008;15(11):988–94.
Problem-based learning and research at the Harvard 58. Huwendiek S, Deleng BA, Zary N, Fischer MR, Ruiz JG,
School of Dental Medicine: a ten-year follow-up. J Dent Ellaway R. Towards a typology of virtual patients. Med
Educ 2004;68(1):71–6. Teach 2009;31(8):743–8.
52. Rochmawati E, Wiechula R. Education strategies to foster 59. Poulton T, Conradi E, Kavia S, Round J, Hilton S. The
health professional students’ clinical reasoning skills. Nurs replacement of “paper” cases by interactive online
Health Sci 2010;1–7. virtual patients in problem-based learning. Med Teach
53. Charlin B, Mann K, Hansen P. The many faces of prob- 2009;31(8):752–8.
lem-based learning: a framework for understanding and 60. Huwendiek S, Reichert F, Bosse HM, de Leng BA, van der
comparison. Med Teach 1998;20(4):323–30. Vleuten CP, Haag M, et al. Design principles for virtual
54. Norman GR, Schmidt HG. Effectiveness of problem-based patients: a focus group study among students. Med Educ
learning curricula: theory, practice, and paper darts. Med 2009;43(6):580–8.
Educ 2000;34(9):721–8.
55. Ericsson KA. Deliberate practice and the acquisition
and maintenance of expert performance in medicine and
related domains. Acad Med 2004;79(10 Suppl):S70–81.

1128 Journal of Dental Education  ■  Volume 76, Number 9

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi