Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 23

 

     
     
 
 
 
 
Title:  A  Critical  Examination  of  the  Contributions  of  Edwin  
                       Gordon’s  Music  Learning  Theory  to  the  Music  Education  
                       Profession  
 
Author(s):  Scott  C.  Shuler  
 
Source:  Shuler,  S.  C.  (1991,  Spring  &  Summer).  A  critical  
examination  of  the  contributions  of  Edwin  Gordon’s  music  
learning  theory  to  the  music  education  profession.  The  Quarterly,  
2(1-­‐2),  pp.  37-­‐58.  (Reprinted  with  permission  in  Visions  of  
Research  in  Music  Education,  16(2),  Autumn,  2010).  Retrieved  from  
http://www-­usr.rider.edu/~vrme  

Visions   of   Research   in   Music   Education   is   a   fully   refereed   critical   journal   appearing  


exclusively  on  the  Internet.  Its  publication  is  offered  as  a  public  service  to  the  profession  
by  the  New  Jersey  Music  Educators  Association,  the  state  affiliate  of  MENC:  The  National  
Association   for   Music   Education.   The   publication   of   VRME   is   made   possible   through   the  
facilities   of   Westminster   Choir   College   of   Rider   University   Princeton,   New   Jersey.   Frank  
Abrahams  is  the  senior  editor.  Jason  D.  Vodicka  is  editor  of  the  Quarterly  historical  reprint  
series.   Chad   Keilman   is   the   production   coordinator.   The   Quarterly   Journal   of   Music  
Teaching   and   Learning   is   reprinted   with   permission   of   Richard   Colwell,   who   was   senior  
consulting  editor  of  the  original  series.  
A Critical Examination of the
Contributions of Edwin Gordon's
Music Learning Theory to the
Music Education Profession
By Scott C. Shuler
Music Consultant, State of Connecticut

dwin Gordon opened his early pres- measure designed to help instrumental music

E entations of music learning theory by


stating that "music education is a
profession in search of a discipline." Invari-
teachers match their students with appropri-
ate instruments, because the items were
produced by electronic synthesis rather than
ably, at least a few music educators in the by acoustical instruments. Still others have
audience responded with anger to this alle- questioned the research basis for Gordon's
gation, which they perceived to be an arro- theory of developmental music aptitude.
gant attack on the validity of their profes- Although the controversy surrounding
sional practices. Gordon then explained his many of Gordon's ideas continues, his music
detailed approach to sequencing music
learning, which he called "music learning "Cordon referred to his ideas
theory." Music learning theory clearly
contradicted at least some elements of every as theories} but the mariner in
popular approach to teaching music. Find- which he presented his mate-
ing their cherished beliefs thus challenged,
many music educators responded belliger- rial was anything but tenta-
ently to Gordon's ideas, pressing him for tive. He made frequent refer-
some proof of his theory.
The acrimony surrounding Gordon's ences to the research basis for
presentations soon became so well known his ideas} a base that many
that some music educators attended them for
their entertainment value, just to see the well-read researchers had dif-
sparks fly. Articles and research reports ficulty identifying."
written by Gordon and his students were
rarely, if ever, accepted for publication by
mainstream music education journals. learning theory has been accepted by an
Articles that were critical of his work, on the increasing number of music educators. A
other hand, did reach press. number of public school music departments
Gordon's critics did not limit themselves to have adopted Gordon's learning theory as
attacks on music learning theory. Some the organizational framework for their K-12
music educators criticized the Musical curricula. Many music educators use at least
Aptitude Profile (965), a measure of what one of the several tests of music aptitude,
Gordon now calls "stabilized music aptitude," achievement, and timbre preference that
charging that the test really measured music Gordon has developed.
achievement. More recently, some music Gordon has emerged from his earlier
educators have challenged Gordon's Instru- counterculture status to become a featured
ment Timbre Preference Test 0984a), a presenter at conferences around the world.

Volume 11,Numbers 1& 2 37


The February, 1986, issue of the Music Endowment to arts education in the public
Educators Journal described music learning schools, but also because it recognized the
theory as one of the five major approaches to dearth of satisfactory ways in which to se-
music education, and research articles quence arts instruction.
exploring or using his theories and tests now Criteria for Instructional Method
appear regularly in mainstream journals.
An appropriate method for the classroom
Gordon's concept of developmental music
must fulfill the definition of method and, in
aptitude is often cited by those who support
addition, meet accepted criteria for educa-
the importance of music experiences for
tional effectiveness. Webster defines
young children. The editors of Webster's
"method" as "a procedure or process for
dictionaries have asked Gordon to submit the
attaining an object; as a systematic plan
definition of "audiation" for possible inclu-
followed in presenting material for instruc-
sion in the 1992 edition of Webster's Colle-
tion" 0970, p. 533), Gordon, using more
giate Dictionary.
specific terminology, writes that a method is
The purpose of this article is to examine
"the order in which sequential objectives are
several of Gordon's many contributions to
introduced in a course of study to accom-
the music education profession. To what
plish a comprehensive objective, a goal." He
extent are Gordon's theories and measures
draws a clear distinction between "method,"
radical departures from accepted thought and
a more general term, and "technique," which
practice? How strong is the research evi-
operates on a smaller scale. The latter he
dence to support his ideas? What are the key
defines as "a teaching aid which is used to
questions about Gordon's work that need to
achieve one or more sequential objectives"
be answered? How extensive is Gordon's
0989, p. 28). The above definitions suggest
current influence on the music education
that an appropriate method must meet at
profession? Is that influence likely to expand
least four criteria: It must be systematic,
or diminish- What are the areas in which his
sequential, comprehensive, and effective in
future impact is likely to be the greatest?
accomplishing long-range objectives.
To address these issues, this article will
Leonhard and House add the criteria of
begin by examining the need for method in
validity and retention: "Authentic and lasting
music and possible criteria for an appropriate
musical achievement represents the ultimate
method. It will then provide a summary of
criterion in judging the success of methods of
the kev elements of Gordon's music learning
teaching music" 0972, p. 318). The results
theory' and consider whether that theory
of instruction must be appropriate within the
fulfills the criteria for method. Next, it will
context of the goals of music education, and
then explore the roots of the initial contro-
these results must endure.
versy surrounding Gordon's music learning
Another major criterion in evaluating a
theory, and it will summarize the few studies
method is its effect on student attitudes.
that have investigated the effectiveness of
Leonhard and House define attitudes as
music learning theory. The article will
"general emotionalized reactions for or
conclude with a reflection on Gordon's
against a thing" and note their importance to
general contributions to the music profes-
the learning process: "Attitudes affect the
sion, both in terms of their current impact
efficiency of all learning, since they form a
and their likely future influence.
basic part of an individual's readiness to
The Search for Music Method learn. A student with a negative attitude
In 1985, Frank Hadsell-then chairman of toward music is certain to make little or no
the National Endowment for the Arts-began progress in learning music unless his attitude
to emphasize publicly the need to develop can be changed" 0972, p. 140).
sequential arts instruction as an integral part Numerous factors may have an effect on a
of educational reform (Hadsell, 1985). His student's attitudes toward music and music
position was important not only because it class. Such factors include the student's early
represented a new commitment by the childhood experiences with music, the
attitudes of the student's parents toward

38 The Quarterly
music, and the student's feelings about the another important criterion for the evaluation
music teacher. Method, and even specific of a music method for primary children is the
techniques within a method, can also have a effect of the method on music aptitude.
major influence on the student's attitudes: Existing Music Methods
If a person likes or dislikes one factor in a
Goodlad writes that "children should be
situation, the entire situation may assume
similar coloring. For example, as a result of introduced to the structure of music through
persistently unrewarding experience with sol-fa a carefully planned, sequential curriculum as
syllables in a general music class, a student may rigorous and well-organized as the best math
develop a negative attitude toward the class, curriculum" (1967, p. 2). Music educators
even though he may have found some of his use a wide variety of instructional ap-
class experiences rewarding (Leonard & House,
proaches, often in combination, but have
1972, pp. 140-141).
generally lacked a single comprehensive
Still another criterion in the choice of a
method to organize their curriculum. In his
music method for young children is its effect
book entitled Contemporary Music Education
on the students' developmental music
(1986), Mark discusses eight different ele-
aptitude. Gordon uses the term "develop-
mentary music methods used in the United
mental music aptitude" to refer to those
States: Dalcroze, Orff, Kodaly, Manhattan-
music abilities which, he contends, fluctuate
ville, Learning Sequence (an early term for
during the first several years of a person's life
Gordon's Music Learning Theory), Carabo-
due to formal and informal music experi-
Cone, Suzuki Talent Education, and Compre-
ences (1979, p. 8). The Primary Measures of
hensive Musicianship. The February, 1986,
Music Audiation (PMMA, 1979) and Interme-
special issue of the Music Educator's Journal,
diate Measures of Music Audiation (IMMA,
devoted to the theme "Major Approaches to
1982) are two measures which Gordon
Music Education," identifies Kodaly, Learning
developed for the purpose of assessing
Sequence, Dalcroze, Suzuki, and Orff-
developmental music aptitude.
Schulwerk as the foremost music methods.
In 1977 Schleuter and DeYarman noted the
The approach of the typical music teacher
possibility that formal music instruction might
might best be termed "eclectic." In her
affect the music aptitude of young children:
review of the major approaches used,
In recent years much attention has been
focused upon the learning process and apti- Shehan writes that "there are no universally
tudes of young children. Specifically in music, acceptable methods in music, but rather a
attention has been given to whether or not early variety of adaptations as diversified as the
formal music training influences musical skills and interests of the multitude of music
aptitude and, if so, at what age range such specialists" (1986, p. 31). There is little
training ceases to have an effect upon potential
research evidence to support the effective-
to achieve. Gordon [1971] has stated that
ness of any of these methods (Shuler, 1987).
children's musical aptitude levels stabilize at age
9; however, there is no systematic evidence to If there is a combination most commonly
support this contention 0977, p. 14). used in general music instruction, it is
A typical student's scores on the PMMA or probably a blend of Orff and Kodaly (Mark,
IMMA will fluctuate significantly over time. 1978, p. 104). According to some experts,
During the years since Schleuter and DeYar- this combination has not satisfied the need
man wrote the above paragraph, some for a comprehensive method:
published research evidence (Flohr, 1981; Combining aspects of Orff and Kodaly ap-
McDonald in this issue) has supported the proaches to teaching perhaps typifies the
American music teacher. One criticism of this
view that short-term music instruction may
eclectic approach is that teachers use parts of
have a positive influence on developmental many teaching methods and philosophies but
music aptitude of young students. If music have no logical or "complete" system or method
aptitude can be increased through formal (Peters & Miller, 1982, p. 83).
instruction in the primary grades, then it is Dalcroze, Orff, and Kodaly never intended
likely that one method may have a more their approaches to be adopted as complete
positive effect than another. If so, then methods. The authors of The Eclectic

Volume II, Numbers 1& 2 39


Curriculum in American Music Education contends that music appreciation and liking
point out that the ideas of Dalcroze, Kodaly, can be modeled but not directly taught.
and Orff "were not presented as fully suffi- Without understanding, all but the purely
cient and independent methods ... by their emotional aspects of appreciation are
creators" (Landis & Carder, 1972, p. 2). impossible. Yet, "the more a student under-
Gordon contends that applying the term stands music the more he is able to appreci-
"method" to most popular general music ate music, although he may not necessarily
approaches is inaccurate, for they tend to like all that he understands" 0989a, p. 22).
provide a collection of techniques rather than Music appreciation through understanding
a comprehensive system of sequential is the ultimate goal of music learning theory:
objectives 0989b, pp. 29-30). "Music learning theory is the structuring of
Gordon developed the learning sequence the logical order of sequential objectives
that underlies his music learning theory in which include the music skills and content
order to provide a comprehensive framework that students must learn in order to achieve
for music instruction: the comprehensive objective of music
appreciation" (1989a, p. 29).
When sequential objectives are in logical order,
Audiation
appropriate method is a consequence. Al-
though learning takes place whether method is Music learning theory instruction is intrinsic
appropriate or not, it is most efficient and in approach, in that it focuses on aurally
meaningful when method is appropriate. The perceivable, functional music events. Such
answer to how sequential objectives can be events may be either physically present or
logically ordered to provide for appropriate "audiated." Gordon coined the term "audia-
method can be found in the empirical model of tion" to refer to the process of hearing
learning sequence 0984b, p. 11).
"music Silently, that is, when the sound is not
Gordon's Music Learning Theory physically present" (1989a, p. 7). Walters
The term "music learning theory" refers to expands upon this definition in Readings in
the specific sequential taxonomies for skills Music Learning The01Y, writing that "audia-
and for tonal and rhythm content that tion is the hearing of sounds that are not
Gordon has formulated, as well as to his before the ear at the moment, through recall,
general theories of musical development. prediction, or conception" (Walters &
The latest revisions of Gordon's theories Taggart, 1989, p. 5). Audiation is therefore
appear in his book Learning Sequences in similar to what many musicians have tradi-
Music: Skill, Content, and Patterns (1989a). tionally described as "inner hearing."
What follows is a general summary of the The development of audiation is a central
basic principles of Gordon's approach, focus of Gordon's approach. According to
focusing particularly on the elements to Gordon, "audiation is fundamental to the
which the remainder of this article refers. understanding of music, and thus it is the
Those in search of a more detailed, yet basis of music learning theory" (1989a, p.
concise, summary of music learning theory 22). Audiation is also the prerequisite for a
should consult Holahan's excellent article in sense of tonality and a sense of meter,
Contemporary Music Education (1986). intelligent listening to music, music literacy,
and a theoretical understanding of music.
Understanding as the Purpose of Music
The article in this issue by Trusheim explores
Education
Gordon's general concept of audiation.
The purpose of music education, according
Gordon (1989a) identifies six stages of
to Gordon, is to provide students with
audiation, which may be summarized as
musical understanding through audiation so
follows:
that they can learn to perform and to re-
Stage 1: Unconsciously audiating "short series
spond aesthetically, and to use symbolic of pitches and durations that [were]heard just a
representations of their and others' aesthetic moment earlier in the music"
feelings to the extent that their music apti- Stage 2: Organizing "through audiation the
tudes will allow 0989a, p. 21). Gordon series of pitches and durations into one or more
tonal patterns of essential pitches and one or

40 The Quarterly
more rhythm patterns of essential durations" Skills Taxonomy
Stage 3: "Consciously establishing the tonality The ordering of skills and content during
and the meter of the music"
learning sequence activities is based upon
Stage 4: "Consciously retaining in audiation
tonal patterns and rhythm patterns of essential Gordon's taxonomies (1989a, p. 37). The
pitches and durations that [the listener has! article by Taggart, in this issue, further
already organized in the music" examines those taxonomies.
Stage 5: "Consciously recalling patterns of Gordon's skills taxonomy is as follows:
essential pitches and durations that [the listener DISCRIMINATION LEARNING
has! organized and audiated in other pieces of Aural/Oral
music ... in terms of their similarities to and Verbal Association
differences from those which [the listener is! Partial Synthesis
audiating in the present music" SymbolicAssociation (reading, writing)
Stage 6: "Consciously predicting the next Composite Synthesis (reading, writing)
patterns of essential pitches and durations that INFERENCE LEARNING
[the listener! will be audiating in the music" Generalization (aural/oral, verbal, symbolic)
(Gordon, 1989a, pp. 14-17). Creativity/Improvisation (aural/oral, symbolic)
Theoretical Understanding (aural/oral, verbal,
At each stage of audiation, the preceding symbolic)
stages operate simultaneously. The article in Discrimination and Inference Learning
this issue by Saunders examines the research Discrimination learning is primarily rote
basis for Gordon's stages of audiation. learning, during which the learner develops a
Learning Sequence Activities vocabulary of familiar tonal and rhythm
Gordon writes that "method in the [music patterns. Inference learning is primarily
learning theory] instructional program is conceptual learning, during which the
based upon learning sequence activities" learner gives meaning to unfamiliar patterns
(1989a, p. 209). He recommends that the based on the familiar patterns which he has
first ten minutes of each music class be learned at the discrimination levels. The
devoted to these activities, which consist of discrimination skill levels precede and thus
listening to, audiating, and performing tonal provide the basis for the inference levels.
and rhythm patterns (1989a, p. 210). Per- Aural/Oral. At the aural/oral skill level of
forming during learning sequence activities discrimination learning, students first listen
takes the form of "singing, chanting, and to, then perform individual tonal and rhythm
movement" (1989a, p. 209). The ultimate patterns on neutral syllables.
goals of learning sequence activities are Verbal Association. At the verbal asso-
"listening to conventional literature with ciation skill level, the students learn to
understanding and performing conventional associate "vocabulary names" and "proper
literature with syntactical meaning through names" with the patterns they have learned
audiation" (1989a, p. 210). at the aural/oral level. Vocabulary names are
tonal and rhythm syllables, for which Gordon
Classroom Activities
recommends moveable do solfege and his
Gordon suggests that the remainder of
own system of rhythm syllables, respectively.
each music class be devoted to "classroom
Proper names consist of labels for tonality,
activities," during which the students listen
such as "major" and "minor;" for the tonal
to, sing, and move to rote songs that intro-
function of patterns, such as "tonic" and
duce tonal and rhythm content (1989a, p.
"dominant;" for meter, such as "duple" and
211). During this portion of each class, the
"triple;" and for the rhythmic function of
students also synthesize, generalize from,
patterns, "macro beats" and "micro beats."
and apply to conventional literature the skills
The macro beat is difficult to define in
and content understanding developed during
words, but on a simplistic level might be
learning sequence activities (1989a, p. 53).
described as the large-scale rhythmic pulse-
During classroom activities, the teacher may
generally occurring in pairs-to which a
use such eclectic activities as guided listen-
person might walk or march. The perception
ing, dancing, and the playing of instruments.
of the macro beat is somewhat subjective.

Volume II, Numbers 1 & 2 41


Micro beats are the equal divisions of the theorizes that creativity and improvisation lie
macro beat that determine meter. Thus, a at opposite ends of the same continuum. At
piece is in duple meter when each macro the creativity end of the continuum, there are
beat can be divided into two micro beats. no externally imposed restrictions; at the
At the verbal association skill level, only improvisation end, there are many. Gordon
functional names are used, e.g., those that therefore considers creative behavior to be
refer to aurally perceivable relationships less difficult than improvisation.
among aural phenomena rather than to At the creativity/improvisation oral skill
abstract theoretical or notational concepts. level, the student engages in musical dia-
These names provide the link between the logues with the teacher that involve at least
sound of the patterns, learned by rote at the one unfamiliar pattern, with or without
aural/oral level, and the notation for these verbal association. At the creativity/improvi-
patterns, to be learned later at the symbolic sation symbolic skill level, the student writes
association level. They also contribute to the a set of patterns in response to another set
development of syntax. written or performed by the teacher, either
Partial Synthesis. At the partial synthesis with or without verbal association. Once
skill level, the student hears, audiates, and students attain the creativity/improvisation
applies proper names to series of familiar oral level in both tonal and rhythm learning
patterns. During this stage of development, sequence activities, they may begin combin-
the student also recognizes the syntax of the ing tonal and rhythm elements when creating
patterns, realizing the internal logic of the and improvising during classroom activities.
syllable systems used and beginning to Azzara's article in this journal further
predict in audiation the next patterns. explores Gordon's concept of improvisation.
Symbolic Association. At the symbolic Also of interest is Schilling's article in Read-
association skill level, the student associates ings in Music Learning Theory (Walters &
notation with individual familiar patterns for Taggart, 1989, pp. 227-236).
which he has already learned verbal associa- Theoretical Understanding. At the theo-
tion. The reading subpart of this level, retical understanding skill level, the student
during which the student audiates or per- learns those forms of music labeling and
forms patterns seen in notation, precedes the analysis which are not essential to aural
writing subpart, during which the student understanding, such as the letter names of
notates patterns heard or audiated. notes and the names and mathematical
Composite Synthesis. At the composite definitions of intervals, time signatures, and
synthesis skill level, the student reads and note durations. Gordon stresses that this skill
writes series of familiar patterns. level should be left for last, and that most
Generalization. At the generalization skill important musical behaviors are possible
level, the student begins to infer meaning for without knowledge of music theory.
unfamiliar patterns encountered within sets
Content Sequence
of one or more unfamiliar and familiar
Gordon also provides taxonomic frame-
patterns. In generalization aural activities,
works for sequencing the tonal and rhythm
the student hears two such sets performed
content that students learn as they progress
without verbal association and identifies
whether the sets are the same or different. through the various skills levels in learning
sequence activities. The order of content in-
In generalization oral activities, the student
struction during learning sequence activities
echoes a set on a neutral syllable. At the
is based on the functional classification of
generalization verbal level, the student
tonal (Gordon, 1989a, p. 91) and rhythm
echoes with vocabulary names a set per-
formed on a neutral syllable. In generaliza- (1989a, p. 155) patterns. Tonal patterns
range from two to five notes in length, with a
tion symbolic activities, the student reads a
typical length of three notes. Rhythm
notated set or writes a set which has been
patterns range from two to three macro beats
heard in audiation or dictation.
Creativity/Improvisation. Gordon in length and include at least two notes.

42 Tbe Quarterly
Tonal Content Sequence In the first stage, students gain a vague
The first step in tonal content sequence is impression of the tonality and meter of a
to develop the ability to audiate the resting piece of music that they hear and perform in
tone in both major and minor tonalities. classroom activities. In the second stage, the
Then the student is taught to audiate tonal students study tonal patterns and rhythm
patterns in relation to that resting tone. patterns in that tonality and meter by using a
Gordon recommends the use of moveable do skill in learning sequence activities. The
solfege as the means to develop a sense of tonal patterns and rhythm patterns, however,
tonal syntax and, eventually, tonal literacy. are not necessarily the same as those that
For example, students are taught that when may be found in that piece of music. In the
do is the resting tone, the music is in major third stage, the students are able to interpret
tonality; when la is the resting tone, the the piece of music with syntactical meaning
music is in minor tonality. For this reason, through audiation in additional classroom
tonalities are presented in their relative- activities (1989a, p. 212).
rather than parallel-keys. The first patterns Individualization Through Pattern
that are presented in major and minor Difficulty
tonalities are those classified as having tonic
Gordon's studies of the relative difficulty of
or dominant functions, after which the class
representative patterns in each of the com-
may progress either to other functional
mon modes and meters 0974, 1976, 1978)
classifications within major and minor or to
have enabled him to present tables of tonal
basic patterns in other tonalities.
patterns 0989a, pp. 92-110) and rhythm
Rhythm Content Sequence pa tterns (1989a, pp. 156-166) organized
The first level of rhythm content to be according to difficulty within each functional
mastered by the student in Gordon's rhythm classification. Gordon recommends that the
taxonomy is the ability to audiate a steady easy patterns in a functional classification be
tempo. Then the students learn to audiate taught first, followed by the moderate and,
macro-beat patterns and micro-beat patterns finally, the difficult patterns. Teachers can
in duple and triple meters. The next step is individualize instruction during learning
mastery of patterns incorporating divisions sequence activities by using patterns of
(even divisions of micro beats) and elonga- differing difficulty to accommodate differing
tions (extensions of individual note durations levels of student aptitude and achievement
beyond a single macro beat) in duple and (1989a, p. 235).
triple meters. Then the student may progress
Music Learning Theory and the
to other rhythm patterns in duple and triple
Criteria for Method
meter, or to basic patterns in other meters.
Music learning theory fulfills at least two
Movement Between Levels criteria for appropriate method. It is based
Gordon provides specific rules for moving on taxonomies that are clearly both system-
between skill and content levels 0989a, p. atic and sequential. Music learning theory is
195). The teacher may "bridge," or leap, arguably comprehensive, in that it provides a
from a lower skill level to a higher skill level long-range framework within which instruc-
temporarily during learning sequence tion may be designed to meet the compre-
activities in order to enrich learning, as long hensive goals of a particular music program,
as the class returns afterward to the lower and the means for individualizing instruction
skill level before continuing its stepwise and evaluation. The goals of music learning
progress (1989a, p. 191). theory are also valid in that they are compat-
Coordinating Learning Sequence ible with contemporary music education
and Classroom Activities philosophy. There is some reason to suggest
that music learning theory may be effective
Gordon suggests that teachers coordinate
in fostering student achievement and devel-
learning sequence activities and classroom
opmental aptitude. The effects of music
activities by introducing and developing
learning theory on students' attitudes have
content in three stages.

Volume 11, Numbers 1 & 2 43


not been established through experimental aesthetic goals espoused by many contempo-
research. rary music education philosophers and
Systematic and Sequential approaches these goals in a manner consis-
Learning sequence is both systematic and
tent with this philosophy. According to
sequential. Webster defines the word Leonhard and House, "The primary purpose
of the music education program is to develop
"systematic" as "methodical in procedure or
the aesthetic potential, possessed by every
plan [or] taxonomic" (1970, p. 895). Gor-
human being, to its highest possible level"
don's framework provides detailed skills and
0972, p. 3). These authors advocate an em-
content taxonomies designed to carry the
phasis on the intrinsic value of music 0972,
student in stepwise fashion from very
p. 4), an emphasis which is paralleled by
rudimentary to highly advanced levels of
music learning theory.
tonal and rhythm achievement.
There are, in addition, similarities between
Comprehensive the structures of music learning theory and
Learning sequence is potentially compre- contemporary aesthetic theory. To go into
hensive in that it provides a long-range these parallels in depth is beyond the scope
framework within which instruction may be of this discussion, but a brief summary of the
designed to meet the comprehensive goals of similarities between. Gordon's concept of
a particular music program and the means audiation and two contemporary aestheti-
for individualizing instruction and evaluation. cians' proposed models for aesthetic re-
The taxonomies provided for learning sponse may suffice to illustrate this point. In
sequence activities are logically organized Meyer's books Emotion and Meaning in
and sequence a broad spectrum of tonal and Music (1956) and Music, the Arts, and Ideas
rhythm learning. The guidelines provided (1967), he bridged in part the gap that had
for classroom activities are general enough existed between philosophical aesthetics and
that the teacher has the flexibility during this educational practice by proposing, among
portion of each class to accommodate other other ideas, that it is the prediction through
curricular goals. Even during learning developed syntax of coming musical events
sequence activities, the content of which is and the subsequent fulfillment or disappoint-
focused on tonal and rhythm elements, the ment of that expectation that create affective
teacher models and labels other elements of response 0956, p. 31; 1967, p. 8). Hor-
music such as phrasing, timbre, dynamics, nung's philosophical dissertation (The
form, and style C1989a, p. 47). Furthermore, Development of a Model of the Psychological
learning sequence activities provide the Processes Which Translate Musical Stimuli
teacher with a systematic means to individu- into Affective Experience) builds upon
alize instruction and evaluation. Instruction Meyer's theories, integrating them with
may be tailored to differing ability levels research into such related areas as hearing,
through the use of patterns of varying diffi- perception, and the brain, to develop a
culty, and sequential achievement may be detailed model (Hornung, 1982).
evaluated in relation to specific performance The phenomenon of audiation, as de-
criteria such as those found in the tonal and scribed by Gordon, closely resembles
rhythm register books of the Jump Right In Hornung's holographic images of specific
series (Gordon & Woods, 1984, p. 230). sound stimuli (1982, pp. 172-3). There are
Valid additional parallels between Gordon's
The goals and procedures of any curricu- description of the first four stages of audia-
lum should emanate from the philosophy of tion and Hornung's cyclical model. Stage
the educational institution (Leonhard & one of audiation, in which a short series of
House, 1972, pp. 85-86). The validity of the notes aurally or visually perceived a few
intent and content of any instructional seconds earlier is audiated, is similar to the
approach must therefore be considered from phase of Hornung's model in which sensory
a philosophical standpoint. Learning se- input is rehearsed in short-term memory. In
quence is valid because it shares the intrinsic stages two through four of audiation, the

44 Tbe Quarterly
series of notes is organized into one or more there is insufficient evidence to support
patterns of essential notes, which are then either side of the issue; so one can only
used to establish a tonal and meter context approach the answer based upon reason and
and are retained in audiation. These stages inference from the little evidence that exists.
resemble the phase of Hornung's model There is logic in Gordon's argument that a
during which essential form is extracted from teacher can model a love and appreciation of
what has been perceived. Stage five of music, but that these qualities cannot be
audiation, which involves the recall of taught directly. It seems reasonable, there-
essential note patterns from other pieces of fore, to argue that responsiveness to a
music, is virtually identical to the part of the particular piece of music is rooted in the
process Hornung hypothesizes during which perception and understanding of expressive
holographic mental images are selected and aural detail in that piece. A number of
brought from long-term into short-term prominent music educators and authors
memory for comparison and discrimination (Bessom, Tatarunis, & Forcucci, 1980, p. 24;
(Hornung, 1982, pp. 157, 177, 186). Buggert & Fowler, 1973, p. viii; Hoffer 1983,
Meyer's emphasis on syntactical expecta- p. 296) concur with Gordon's preference for
tion parallels Gordon's emphasis on the role understanding, rather than appreciation, as
of tonal and rhythm syntax rooted in audia- the appropriate focus for music instruction.
tion, and particularly Gordon's sixth (predic- Buggert and Fowler, for example, write that
tion) stage of audiation. These similarities appreciation is a "byproduct of understand-
and others between the music learning ing. Conceivably, disliking a piece of music
theory model and models of contemporary might be as natural a result of understanding
aesthetic theory suggest that Gordon's as learning to enjoy it. Once the student
approach is compatible with the aesthetic understands the music he listens to, he will
rationale for music education and may thus acquire his own set of appreciations."
be considered philosophically valid. If Meyer's theories are correct, a listener
Effective constantly predicts what is going to happen
in a piece of music based on his syntax (or
The effectiveness of music learning theory
lack of syntax) for the style of the piece, and
in facilitating the achievement of valid goals
his response to the music is linked to the
has not been definitively established. How-
accuracy of his predictions. An important
ever, the approach appears to have the
objective in developing musical responsive-
potential for such effectiveness based upon
ness would, accordingly, be to develop
the logic of Gordon's focus on the tonal and
syntax for significant expressive elements.
rhythmic elements of music, the parallels
Hoffer'S review of research in music educa-
between Gordon's approach and accepted
tion has led him to support the importance
learning theory, and research into the
of music syntax as a primary curricular goal.
process of music learning. There is also
He suggests that "the syntax of music is
some experimental evidence that supports
probably the first type of learning that
the effectiveness of music learning theory,
students should acquire in music, because
although that evidence is far from conclusive.
without it the other four areas of learning
Leonhard and House assert that an appro-
won't mean much" 0983, p. 56).
priate, aesthetically based music program
Gordon contends that the most important
"should be dedicated to the development of
expressive syntaxes are tonal and rhythmic.
musical responsiveness and musical under-
There is support for such a view. Numerous
standing" (1972, p. 4). Musical understand-
researchers (Bean, 1939; Broman, 1956;
ing is, as mentioned above, the primary goal
Henkin, 1955, 1957; Hevner, 1935a, 1935b,
of music learning theory. The question,
1936, 1937; Mainwaring, 1933; Ortmann,
therefore, is whether such understanding
1937; Petzold, 1969) have identified the tonal
leads to responsiveness, or whether the
and rhythmic elements of music as funda-
quality of responsiveness can and should be
mental to the musical response. Gardner's
taught separately. This question cannot yet
review of the literature also led him to
be answered on the basis of research, as

Volume II, Numbers 1 & 2 45


conclude that tonal and rhythm processing music learning is also supported by several
are the core components of musical intelli- studies (DeYarman, 1972; Dittemore, 1970;
gence 0983, p. 278). Gordon points out: Jarjisian, 1981, 1983; Macknight, 1975; Miller,
The audiation of both the tonal dimension and 1975; Van Nuys & Weaver, 1943).
the rhythm dimension are fundamental to the Gordon's adaptation of Gagne's conditions
audiation of the harmonic, phrasing, timbre, to music, emphasizing the aural experience
dynamic, form, and style dimensions, because
of music as a prerequisite for the efficient
all other dimensions that we audiate are
superimposed upon our audiation of the tonal development of music literacy, is in keeping
dimension and the rhythm dimension (1984b, p. with a long tradition of music learning
19). theory. Advocacy of the "rote-to-note"
If musical responsiveness results from a process in the United States can be traced
syntactical understanding of the tonal and back to the disciples of Pestalozzi, such as
rhythmic elements of music, then music Elam Ives and Lowell Mason (Keene, 1982),
learning theory may be effective in reaching and has continued in modern times with the
its aesthetic goals. endorsement of such prominent music
Relationship to Contemporary Learn- education philosophers and pedagogues as
ing Theory. Another question that must be Orff (Mark, 1978, p. 87), Shinichi Suzuki
asked when assessing the potential effective- (Mills & Murphy, p. 12), and Leonhard and
ness of music learning theory is whether the House:
model for learning on which it is based is Musical learning entails the development of
consistent with contemporary learning theory many abstract concepts, but concrete experi-
and research. The structure of music learn- ence should precede the abstractions .... The
best sequence moves from aural experience
ing theory is largely rooted in the theories of
with tonal and rhythm patterns to the notation
Gagne, a learning psychologist. Gordon's which represents these patterns (Leonard &
adaptation of Gagne's ideas to music is based House, 1972, p. 305).
in part on music learning theory and re- Questions Raised. On the other hand,
search, and in part on educated speculation. critics of music learning theory have raised
There is a strong resemblance between some legitimate questions that have not been
Gagne's eight conditions of learning, as set satisfactorily answered. First, is it an efficient
forth in his book Conditions of Learning use of limited instructional time to devote a
(965), and Gordon's skills taxonomy. Other portion of each class to learning sequence
Gagne premises are clearly present in the activities? Or, alternatively, is any gain
music learning theory approach, including a accrued through the inclusion of these
focus on observable behavior, avoiding activities outweighed by the effects of the
vague objectives such as "appreciation" while decreased amount of time therefore available
concentrating on measurable performance for other classroom activities? In other
objectives; the identification of objectives that words, do learning sequence activities result
deal with the smallest possible units of in a net gain or a net loss in music achieve-
performance, in this case tonal and rhythm ment? Second, do students trained to
patterns; and the recognition of component perform the isolated tonal and rhythm
or "subordinate" objectives, which Gordon patterns or pattern chains that comprise
calls "sequential" objectives, that lead in learning sequence activities transfer this skill
stepwise fashion to larger-scale objectives, to a setting in which they must deal with
which Gordon calls "comprehensive" objec- music as a multidimensional phenomenon,
tives. Hahn's article in this issue explores i.e., with both the tonal and the rhythm
further the close parallels between Gordon's components occurring simultaneously?
theories and contemporary thought in Third, might Gordon's concern for
educational psychology. audiation, as defined by the student's ability
The research basis for Gordon's emphasis to perform music, unnecessarily limit the
of the tonal and rhythm elements of music student's listening experiences? The size of a
has been discussed above. His choice of person's speaking vocabulary tends to be
patterns as appropriate building blocks for smaller than the vocabulary that the person

46 The Quarterly
understands when listening. Likewise, it is attitudes toward music class and specific
possible that a person's music listening music activities in order to learn whether
vocabulary may exceed, indeed should learning sequence activities have any effect
exceed, that person's performance vocabu- on students' attitudes toward instruction.
lary. Otherwise, a person would have to be Retention and Developmental Aptitude
capable of performing a piece in order to
There is some research evidence regarding
enjoy it during listening.
the effects of music learning theory instruc-
Gordon might respond to the third ques-
tion on developmental music aptitude (see
tion by contending that music learning
McDonald study in this issue), but little
theory supports listening experiences which
research in the area of retention. These are
include unfamiliar patterns, and that building
appropriate topics for future study. Articles
a vocabulary of familiar patterns helps
by Cutietta and Walters in this issue explore
students understand the unfamiliar patterns
further the nature and basis of Gordon's
encountered during listening by giving those
theories regarding music aptitude.
students the basis to infer the meaning of the
unfamiliar patterns. Acceptance of Music Learning Theory
Gordon introduced his music learning
Attitude
theory at a time when existing approaches to
An important area to investigate before
music instruction remained eclectic, un-
adopting Gordon's learning sequence
proven, and generally dominated by Euro-
activities, or any other procedures consisting
pean systems. One might therefore have
largely of drill, as part of the instructional expected American music educators to
process is the potentially negative effect they welcome his approach, which provided
may have on student attitudes. In recom-
detailed guidelines for music instruction.
mending that a ceiling of ten minutes be
However, as mentioned above, the initial
placed upon the amount of time per class
response to music learning theory was mixed
period devoted to learning sequence activi- at best. There were several possible reasons
ties, Gordon acknowledges this concern:
that this response has occurred.
Only under very unusual circumstances should
One reason for the slow acceptance of
more than ten minutes of a class period be
allocated to learning sequence activities, Gordon's approach may have been cultural.
regardless of the length of the class period. Americans have an almost obsessive ten-
Too much time given to learning sequence dency to prefer things European, at least in
activities might make a class period dull 0988, matters of culture. For example, American
p. 210). audiences are notoriously more impressed by
Even if limited to ten minutes, such drill orchestra conductors with foreign-sounding
activities at the beginning of each music names, to the extent that some American-
period may damage student attitudes toward born conductors have cultivated foreign
music class and thus, toward music itself. affectations. The same chauvinism may have
On the other hand, if learning sequence led American music educators to look
activities are effective in fostering achieve- beyond our borders for guidance, turning to
ment, they should have a positive effect on foreign-born pedagogies such as those
student attitudes. Hoffer states that "under- developed by Orff, Dalcroze, Suzuki, and
standing more about music usually contrib- Kodaly. Such an explanation would account
utes to liking it better, and, in turn, positive in part for the extent to which some music
attitudes motivate students to learn a subject educators have uncritically embraced idio-
better" (1983, p. 62). Gordon supports this syncratic aspects of these approaches that are
linkage of achievement and attitude, writing alien, and arguably inappropriate, to Ameri-
that "when learning sequence activities, can culture. Music educators may uncon-
classroom activities, and performance sciously suspect that no American could
activities are found in balance in the instruc- possibly propose a successful system of
tional program ... students are motivated to teaching music, although their spending
learn" C1989a,p. 209), An important topic patterns indicate that they harbor no parallel
for future study will be to measure student reservations about American textbook series.

Volume II, Numbers 1 & 2 47


A second reason for the cool response to music-teacher preparation programs before
Gordon's ideas was the very self-confidence there would be a cadre of teachers who
with which he proselytized for what was, could use music learning theory effectively.
especially at the outset, an untested theory. The fifth reason for resistance to Gordon's
His blunt criticism of many of the cherished ideas was legitimate questioning of his
icons of music pedagogy, such as teaching premises. Although Gordon referred to his
rhythm notation through fractions, was ideas as theories, the manner in which he
unlikely to earn praise from the advocates of presented his material was anything but
such techniques. In keeping with the long tentative. He made frequent reference to the
tradition of confrontation between self-styled research basis for his ideas, a base that many
prophets and the status quo, it was only well-read researchers had difficulty identify-
natural for leaders in music education who ing. The questions that these skeptics raised
felt successful with what they were doing to about Gordon's music learning theory were
question this upstart who exhorted them to therefore justified, for it is appropriate for
repent and see his truth. In fact, as an professionals to scrutinize any set of propos-
author and professor already known to many als before adopting a major shift in approach.
of his peers in the profession, Gordon faced
Research into the Effectiveness of
the classic problem of any prophet seeking
Music Learning Theory
acceptance in his own land. To some
There is limited research available on the
observers, Gordon appeared to relish the
effectiveness of music learning theory.
iconoclast role, for he seemed to seize every
Palmer, Stockton, MacKnight, McDonald, and
opportunity to tweak his opponents.
Shuler have conducted studies either directly
A third reason for Gordon's difficulty in
or indirectly relevant to music learning
convincing music educators of the validity of
theory. Reports based on the studies of
music learning theory was that his audience
McDonald and Shuler appear elsewhere in
often did not understand what he was
this issue, so this article includes only a brief
proposing. Interested teachers who read his
summary of their work. More extensive
books often ran aground on the rocks of his
summaries of the other three studies follow.
dense prose or were overwhelmed by the
newness of his terminology and often- The Palmer Study
complex ideas. Even among Gordon's own Palmer (1974, 1976) sought to compare the
students, it seemed that few had sufficient relative effects on music rhythm reading of
understanding of his ideas to explain them to the Kodaly-based Threshold to Music method
others. Lacking understanding, and faced by to an early version of music learning theory,
a large number of new labels and seemingly as set forth by Gordon in his book Tbe
elaborate rule structures, music educators Psychology of Music Teaching (1971).
often read into Gordon's ideas greater rigidity Palmer's experimental sample consisted of
and more radical changes than he was 136 fourth graders in three Winter Park,
actually proposing. Relatively few seemed to Florida, public elementary schools. The
understand his proposals for what they were: treatment period lasted for 20 weeks. In two
a more detailed revision of the sound-before- of the schools, students were randomly
sign approach to music education, the roots assigned to classes receiving either Kodaly-
of which could be traced back at least as far or Gordon-based instruction, both groups
as Pestalozzi's disciples. being taught by the investigator. The
A fourth reason for the slow acceptance of instruction that the music learning theory
music learning theory was the high level of experimental groups received omitted the
tonal and rhythm skill it demanded from verbal association level of Gordon's skills
teachers. Many practicing music teachers sequence, so that in essence the study
could not sing tonal patterns with accurate compared little more than the Kodaly and
pitch, nor were they adept in the use of Gordon rhythm syllable systems.
moveable do tonal syllables. It appeared that All classes involved in the study were
Significant changes would be needed in intact groups. The experimental classes met

48 Tbe Quarterly
three times a week for 20 minutes each aptitude and meter discrimination achieve-
session. Two control classes in the third ment. Stockton's sample consisted of 52
school were taught by another music special- students who had enrolled in an introductory
ist, and received traditional instruction which music course at Lafayette College in Easton,
included no special instruction in rhythm Pennsylvania. The experimental treatment in
reading. The control classes met once per this study was a rote performance method
week for 45 minutes. derived from Gordon's learning sequence
The criterion measures in this study activities. The control treatment consisted of
consisted of a written and a performance "a lecture-demonstration approach incorpo-
component. The three written measures, rating notation skills without performance"
selected from two standardized achievement (Stockton, 1983, p. 8). Both treatments
tests, were: addressed the aural discrimination of duple,
1) the Meter Discrimination subtest of Colwell's triple, and combined meter.
Music Achievement Test (MAT, 1969) Test 1; All of the students in Stockton's investiga-
2) Subtest b, Rhythm, of MATTest 2, Part 3 tion first took the meter subtest of MAP, then
(Auditory Discrimination in Music); and
were randomly assigned to either an experi-
3) the Rhythm Concepts: Reading Recognition
subtest of Gordon's Iowa Tests qf Musical mental or a control group on the basis of
Literacy (ITML, 1970). their music aptitude. The treatment contin-
The three performance measures, created ued for 12 weeks. During this time each
by the investigator, were: class met for 75 minutes per week, of which
1) Response to Meter, designed to measure the 25 minutes were devoted to either the
ability of the student to synchronize a response experimental or the control treatment. The
to pulses of duple and triple meter musical balance of each class period was devoted to
excerpts;
instruction typical of the course. The
2) Imitation of Rhythmic Patterns, designed to
measure the ability of the student to imitate investigator taught both the experimental and
one- to two-measure rhythm patterns presented the control groups. The experimental group
aurally; and learned 12 four-measure rhythm patterns by
3) Response to Rhythmic Notation. rote, using rhythm syllables. Collectively the
The results of the study were analyzed patterns "represented a variety of typical
with the students' Musical Aptitude Profile or rhythmic devices such as rests, divisions and
MAP (Gordon, 1965) scores used as a elongations of the beat, and anacruses"
covariate. The data yielded a significant 0983, p. 21). The students in the control
difference between the aggregate experimen- group analyzed the meter components of 12
tal groups and the control group on gain recorded musical excerpts with the aid of
scores that favored the experimental groups, notation. These musical excerpts were
regardless of treatment. This result may also selected on the basis of rhythmic variety.
have been attributable to differences in Stockton developed a test of aural meter
teachers, total weekly class time, and/or class discrimination which he administered to the
schedule. There was no significant differ- students at the end of the 12-week instruc-
ence between the two experimental groups tional period. The test presented 50 re-
on gain scores, although the mean improve- corded musical excerpts of 25-30 seconds in
ment of the Gordon group was slightly length. The students were instructed to
higher than that of the Kodaly group. identify the meter of each excerpt as either
The Stockton Study duple, triple, or combined. The data were
Stockton (1983) conducted an investigation analyzed using a two-way analysis of vari-
which involved the use of rhythm learning ance. The experimental group demonstrated
sequence activities with older general music a significantly greater gain in aural meter
students. In this study he compared the discrimination achievement than did the
effects of two different methods of teaching students in the control group.
rhythm on the meter discrimination skills of Stockton concluded that the learning
nonmusic-major college students, and sequence activities approach was superior to
examined the relationship between rhythm the reading and listening approach in

Volume II, Numbers 1 & 2 49


fostering rhythm understanding. Further- The three instrumental music teachers
more, students possessing higher rhythm used in the study were selected from among
aptitude demonstrated a significantly higher five volunteers. The selection criteria were
level of meter discrimination achievement that the teachers (a) traditionally helped
than did those possessing lower rhythm students reach a certain minimum level of
aptitude, regardless of the instructional performance competence by the end of the
treatment used. first year of instruction, (b) had a beginning
Stockton's study is important for several instrumental student dropout record of no
reasons. First of all, he compared the effects greater than ten per cent, and (c) earned a
of a traditional approach to the effects of a score above the nintieth percentile on the
version of music learning theory that was Minnesota Teacher Attitude Inventory
more current than the version used by (MTAI), a scale which has been shown to
Palmer. Secondly, the pattern training in have some usefulness in predicting over-all
Stockton's study consisted of isolated pat- teaching effectiveness. One teacher elected
terns, but during testing the examples were to teach the experimental classes in the
presented within a complete musical context. study, and the other two agreed to teach the
In spite of the transfer thus required, the control classes.
music learning theory approach was found to Students in both groups met for one 30-
be superior. Finally, Stockton found no minute class each week. The treatment
evidence of an interaction between the continued for 32 weeks. MacKnight states
effectiveness of the music learning theory that both groups "covered the same pitches,
treatment and the students' music aptitude. rhythms, meters, keys, tempos, dynamics,
The MacKnight Study and special signs. The only variation in
treatment was in the method and order of
MacKnight (1975) investigated the effects
introducing pitch" (p. 59). Students in the
of a tonal pattern training treatment, very
experimental group learned each new pitch
similar to learning sequence activities, on the
as part of a tonal pattern. Each pattern was
aural and performance achievement of fourth
taught aurally before presenting it in nota-
grade beginning instrumental music students.
tion. The instructional material in this group
One of her secondary questions examined
consisted of melodies that contained the
the interaction between the effects of music
tonal patterns. The melodies were presented
aptitude and pattern training on music
in the same order as the patterns they
achievement. Another secondary question
contained. The control group used a stan-
dealt with the effect of the treatment on
dard method text, Breeze Easy (Kinyon,
student attitudes. Although not explicitly
1959), and allowed the book to dictate the
stated, the study also incorporated an implicit
sequence and manner of presentation for
investigation of rhythm pattern training.
new pitches. Each new pitch was first
The sample for this study consisted initially
introduced in notation at the top of a page,
of 90 fourth grade students in three elemen-
along with its fingering.
tary schools who signed up for beginning
Although MacKnight mentions the fact only
instrumental music. These students were
incidentally, there were also important
stratified on the basis of music aptitude, as
differences in the presentation of rhythm to
reflected by their scores on MAP, and by
the two groups. Students in the experimen-
academic aptitude as measured by their
tal group learned rhythm in phrases, using a
scores on the Large-Thorndike Intelligence
modified version of the Kodaly rhythm
Test (1954). After a trial period of six weeks,
syllables. Pulsation within phrase rhytlun
during which time the students decided
was stressed as the primary organizing
whether they would continue instrumental
element in rhythm. In the control group, on
instruction until the end of the year, the
the other hand, rhythm was taught:
sample was narrowed to 85 students. This
as it numerically relates to a beat. ... For
final sample was then divided into homoge-
example, a [quarter note was) taught as a
neous experimental and control groups of no quarter note having the durational value of one
more than six students each. beat in 4/4 meter. Melodic rhythm [was)

50 The Quarterly
practiced by counting, using a number to data that there was no difference in mean
identify the [quarter note], '1 and' for [two attitude between the two treatment groups.
eighth notes], and '1, 2 and' for the [dotted
MacKnight's investigation had several
quarter followed by an eighth note]. Unlike
strengths, such as her careful teacher selec-
traditional instruction, the control group used
rhythm syllables and were encouraged to sing tion process and the length and control of
assigned melodies with letter names or on a the treatment. Her separate analyses of the
neutral syllable (p. 59). students' scores on the two posttests, on the
These differences in rhythm instruction other hand, implied an assumption that the
were not specifically addressed in Mac- two scores were unrelated, an assumption
Knight's research questions, but may well that seems hard to justify. In fact, aural and
have had a significant effect on the students' performance skills might reasonably be
posttest achievement scores. expected to show a high correlation. An-
The achievement posttests used in Mac- other oversight, alluded to above, was
Knight's investigation were Test 2 of MAT MacKnight's failure to recognize that the
and Form A of the Watkins-Farnum Perform- differences in the rhythm teaching approach
ance Scale (WFPS, 1954). The students' experienced by the experimental versus the
scores were examined for treatment main control groups might have caused at least
effect and for the interaction of music some of the differences that she observed
aptitude and treatment, using a three-factor between the students' scores on the WFPS, a
least-squares design with unequal cells. scale which measures rhythm performance
The WFPS and MAT scores were analyzed and other dimensions, and their scores on
separately. The results showed that the the third part of MAT 2, which requires
experimental group scored significantly students to detect rhythm and tonal errors.
higher in both performance and aural skills. These considerations aside, MacKnight's
The data also revealed a significant effect of study is important for several reasons. First
music aptitude on both posttest scores. of all, the pattern training that constituted her
There were significant interactions between experimental treatment bore a strong resem-
treatment and music aptitude for both blance to learning sequence activities. The
posttest scores, but the makeup of this rhythm component implicit in her treatment
interaction differed between the MAT and differences strengthened that resemblance by
WFPS. For the WFPS the experimental adding the rhythm dimension to the tonal
treatment had a Significant positive effect on dimension which she was explicitly investi-
the scores of lower-aptitude students; gating. Learning sequence activities involve
however, on the MAT the experimental both rhythm and tonal pattern training. The
treatment had a significant positive effect on interaction that she found between the
the scores of higher aptitude students. effects of music aptitude and the treatment
MacKnight created an original measure of on the two dimensions of achievement she
student attitude for use in this study. This measured is in direct contradiction to the
measure was administered before the begin- conclusions drawn by Stockton (see above).
ning and after the end of the 32-week Finally, the time devoted to the pattern
treatment. The test, which she entitled the treatment in MacKnight's study caused no
Student Attitude Questionnaire (SAQ, 1975), apparent effect on student attitudes.
was based on the Osgood criteria for a The McDonald Study
semantic differential scale. The content of
McDonald 0991, in this issue) developed a
this test consisted of bipolar adjective pairs,
method for elementary recorder instruction
placed at opposite ends of a five-point scale,
based on Gordon's learning sequences and
on which the students described their
compared the effectiveness of this method
feelings about each of ten facets of the
with a more traditional method. She exam-
students' instrumental music experience.
ined the effects of two instructional treat-
MacKnight performed no analysis on the data
ments on the students' PMMA scores and on
gathered from the two administrations of
their performance of several brief melodies
SAQ, as it was obvious from the descriptive
on the recorder. The PMMAwas admini-

Volume II, Numbers 1 & 2 51


stered as both a pretest and a posttest. not conclusive. On the other hand, the
The students' mean increase in rhythm and studies conducted by Stockton, MacKnight,
composite PMMA scores was significantly and McDonald indicated that learning
greater for the learning sequence group than sequence activities might facilitate music
for the traditional group. The tonal score of achievement.
the learning sequence group was also higher,
Present and Future Influence of
but the difference was not significant at the
Gordon's Ideas
0.05 level. The recorder performance
measure also revealed significant differences As mentioned above, a number of Gor-
in favor of the learning sequence group in all don's ideas have found increasing accep-
tance, while others remain controversial.
four dimensions measured: melodic, rhyth-
mic, executive skills, and composite score. The terms "audiation" and "developmental
aptitude," for example, have become part of
The Shuler Study the common vocabulary of music educators.
Shuler (1991, in this issue) examined the In contrast, relatively few Orff and Kodaly
effects of learning sequence activities on the educators have abandoned their use of
vocal performance achievement of third pentatonicism and endorsed Gordon's
grade general music students. His primary preference for beginning with complete
goal was to determine whether students major and minor tonal systems. Undergradu-
would transfer discrete tonal and rhythm ate music theory instruction in institutions
skills acquired during learning sequence such as the University of Michigan, Capital
activities into the context of musical perform- University (in Columbus, Ohio), and Pennsyl-
ance, where all of the elements of music vania State University has been reorganized
operate simultaneously. He selected teachers to incorporate many of the principles of
for the study who were competent in the use Gordon's music learning theory. Other
of learning sequence activities. He chose a theorists, such as Brink (1983) and Jones
naturalistic setting, allowing the teachers in (1985), have taken exception to Gordon's
the study to select classroom activities organizational framework for rhythm.
appropriate to their curricula, in order to Miklaszewski is among many who have
determine whether the effects of learning adopted a middle-of-the-road response to
sequence activities were consistent from one Gordon's ideas. In a review of the 1984
teacher and curriculum to another. The edition of Gordon's Learning Sequences in
study continued for almost an entire school Music, Miklaszewski (1986) expressed
year. In the posttest the students performed admiration for several of Gordon's proposals,
two rote songs, and their performances were particularly audiation. On the other hand, he
evaluated by a panel of judges. questioned whether the existing research
The effects of the learning sequence base is adequate to justify the level of
activities treatment were not consistent in specificity and detail Gordon provides when
Shuler's study. No conclusion regarding the proposing concepts such as multiple levels of
effects of learning sequence activities could audiation and the exact number of minutes
be drawn, as a result of an interaction of learning sequence activities that should be
between the treatment and teacher variables. included at the beginning of each class
Both teachers in Shuler's study were clearly period. Miklaszewski is also among many
biased toward the music learning theory music educators who have questioned the
treatment, yet the students of one teacher appropriateness of separating the tonal and
demonstrated greater singing ability when rhythm dimensions of music during learning
they had not engaged in learning sequence sequence activities. Perhaps in response to
activity. The effectiveness of learning such concerns, Gordon provides clearer
sequence activities in fostering vocal per- explanations, more detailed documentation
formance achievement varied depending of his research base, and a clearer identifica-
upon the teacher and/or curriculum. tion of those aspects of his theory that are
These studies suggest that music learning highly speculative in the 1988 edition of
theory may be effective, but the results are Learning Sequences in Music.

52 Tbe Quarterly
Gordon's ideas have already influenced innovation that have, ironically, proven to be
several areas of the music education profes- a liability in the eyes of teachers accustomed
sion. Gordon has taken an increasing to more traditional series. The series pro-
interest in early childhood music education. vides register books that help teachers
While he cannot take sale credit for the sequence tonal and rhythm learning se-
growing emphasis on this area among music quence activities and provide criteria for the
educators, those who argue for early music evaluation of student success. The series
instruction often cite his theory of develop- also provides a wealth of proven songs and
mental music aptitude. Furthermore, in the instructional activities for use during "class-
absence of other published measures, his room activities." The activities are printed on
Primary and Intermediate Measures of Music cards that are coded to coordinate with
Audiation (1979 and 1982 respectively) are learning sequence activities, and include
used by an increasing number of public ideas appropriate for Orff- and Kodaly-based
schools and researchers to measure both instruction, mainstreamed special education
music aptitude and achievement in grades K- students, and other common instructional
4. Hence, his focus on tonal and rhythm situations. Teachers are expected to draw on
audiation has a profound influence both on this material to "roll their own" lessons,
the selection of students for talented pro- selecting and/or designing content and
grams and on the evaluation of music activities appropriate for each class.
instruction, and is therefore likely to have an This attempt to provide teachers with
influence on the nature of instruction as well. quality materials from which they may
Classroom Use of Gordon's Materials custom-design instruction is a laudable step
away from the tyranny of the textbook.
The reasons for the slow acceptance of
Furthermore, it would appear to offer a
music learning theory, described earlier in
reasonable alternative to graded text series.
this article, have continued to limit the extent
Music series are, of necessity, designed to be
to which general music teachers have
used by music teachers who have a certain
applied this approach in their classrooms.
hypothetical amount of instructional time,
Shuler (1987) mentions that he had difficulty
but that time inevitably differs from what is
finding teachers who were skilled in the use
offered in most school districts. Teachers
of music learning theory. The three factors
who have inadequate instructional time find
that seem to be the greatest obstacle for
it impossible to complete graded textbooks.
teachers are the apparent complexity of
The Jump Right In classroom activities cards
music learning theory, the high level of tonal
provide a way for teachers to organize the
and rhythm skill required to teach learning
correct type and amount of material to fit the
sequence activities, and the scarcity of
needs of their particular situation. Indeed,
materials to facilitate the use of music
the card format completely eliminates the
learning theory in the classroom. Students
temptation of the canned lesson plan.
who have studied with Gordon at the
Many teachers have found the Jump Right
University of Iowa, the State University of
In register books cumbersome to use. In
New York at Buffalo, and at Temple Univer-
order to simplify their use, Gordon devel-
sity have carried music learning theory into
oped a "short version" of each register book.
their classrooms, and Gordon maintains an
Some teachers have complained that it takes
active schedule of workshops and inservice
too long for students to complete the lower
presentations. However, the extent to which
skill levels in the register books and begin to
teachers use music learning theory may
learn notation. The series requires teachers
ultimately depend upon the commercial
to use recordings from other series, which
success of published pedagogical materials
creates a problem when those series eventu-
based on Gordon's ideas.
ally go out of print. The rigid textbook
The Jump Right In general music series
adoption criteria of some states, such as
(Gordon & Woods, 1984) has not achieved
Texas, generally do not allow for statewide
widespread adoption. The series incorpo-
adoption of series that venture as far from
rates several well-intentioned attempts at

Volume II, Numbers 1& 2 53


the traditional path as does Jump Right In. listening-and-imitation features of the Suzuki
Perhaps most unfortunately, many music approach, toward an end result that may be
teachers have apparently become accus- more effective than either of those individual
tomed to the ease of moving from page (x) approaches.
to page (x+ 1) in canned series and are not Suzuki. This blending of elements from
interested in "rolling their own" lessons. music learning theory and other approaches
Teachers are understandably reluctant to is a process through which Gordon's ideas
revolutionize both methodology and materi- are having, and are likely to continue to
als at once. In short, the Jump Right In. series have, a strong influence. Some Suzuki
contains many innovative ideas, but will teachers have drawn on Gordon's music
apparently require further revision in order to learning theory to refine their instruction.
become widely used. Byrd's article in this These teachers have begun to focus on
issue explores the potential impact of music audiation, rather than pure imitation, as a
learning theory on general music instruction. means of developing their students' ability to
Ironically, in spite of the fact that music perform their standard pieces. Suzuki
learning theory is widely misunderstood to students who lack aural skills must listen to
be an elementary general music method, seemingly endless repetitions of the Suzuki
Gordon's ideas have found their most recordings in order to learn their music.
successful commercial application-and Suzuki teachers who have incorporated key
therefore their most widespread use-in the elements of music learning theory have
area of instrumental music. Froseth's Indi- found that having students sing and move to
vidualized Instructor (1981) and Comprehen- their pieces makes memorization much more
sive Music Instructor (1984) are two success- rapid, and that using tonal and rhythm
ful beginning instrumental music series that syllables eventually helps students learn to
draw heavily on music learning theory. The read music when notation is introduced.
Jump Right In instrumental series, recently Orff. Gordon may also have an influence
co-authored by Grunow and Gordon on Orff pedagogy. Gordon was invited to
C1989b), is also finding widespread accep- present several sessions at the 1985 national
tance. convention of the American Orff-Schulwerk
The most important reason for the success Association and was well received. Orff
of these instrumental series is the extent to advocates value the creative aspects of
which their authors have adopted practical movement and music-making, but many feel
compromises between the realities of the a need for more structured sequence. Some
traditional instrumental music program and have found part of the answer by adopting
the details of music learning theory, There aspects of Kodaly, but others are finding
are significant differences between the series, useful ideas in music learning theory (see
but they share key common elements. Cernohorsky's article in Readings in Music
Students who use these series first engage in Learning Theory, Walters & Taggart, 1989,
listening, singing, and movement experi- pp. 272-285). For example, some Orff
ences, then develop facility with tonal and teachers share Gordon's reservations about
rhythm syllables in order to provide the aural having students work too long with penta-
foundation for success on their instruments. tonic pitch sets. They agree that experiences
They then play their instruments by ear. with complete major and minor systems are
Only after completing these steps do tl1.e more appropriate for children in our culture
students encounter notation, but the intro- and are more likely to lead to the sense of
duction is not delayed so long that the leading tone and tonal motion that enhances
students are unwilling to read music. Once musical expressiveness.
notated music is introduced many students Some Orff advocates also have joined
quickly develop notational literacy. In a Gordon in questioning whether children are
sense, these authors have developed a truly improvising when playing keyboard
pragmatic blend of the aural/oral-to-syllable instruments from which all bars but those in
levels of music learning theory with the the pentatonic scale have been removed. As

54 The Quarterly
Kratus has pointed out, such activity might designed by others.
better be labeled "exploration" 0990, pp. Music learning theory is also being used
35), These teachers have begun to develop effectively in the traditional ensemble
the students' ability to audiate as a prerequi- rehearsal setting. Shuler describes how
site for true improvisation. music learning theory can be used to refine
Kodaly. Gordon's ideas have already had teaching procedures in the instrumental
some influence on Kodaly pedagogy. Gor- rehearsal (Walters & Taggart, 1989, pp. 208-
don was invited to deliver the keynote 226). Jordan achieved similar results in the
address at the 1981 national meeting of the choral setting (Walters & Taggart, 1989, pp.
Organization of American Kodaly Educators. 168-182). Recognizing the potential contri-
Laurdella Bodolay, formerly director of the bution of music learning theory to the
Kodaly Musical Training Institute, has revised ensemble setting, the American Choral
her use of the Kodaly syllable system as a Directors Association invited Gordon in 1989
result of conversations with Gordon. The to present two sessions at its annual national
traditional Kodaly syllable system is based on meeting.
notation rather than on aural relationships. Challenges and Problems. The fact that
Bodolay now links the syllables to the the content taxonomy of music learning
musical function of rhythm patterns, rather theory is built on characteristics typical of
than to the way the patterns are notated Western art music, such as major and minor
(based on conversation with Bodolay, 1990). tonalities and tonic-dominant harmonic
Other nationally known Kodaly clinicians, relationships, presents a challenge as music
such as John Feierabend, have incorporated educators who deal with the need to teach
an even greater amount of music learning music of other cultures. It may be that such
theory into their pedagogy. For example, music teachers will be able to apply Gor-
Feierabend recommends that teachers have don's skills sequence to musical content
their students echo patterns using syllables drawn from some of those cultures by
before introducing the notation for those identifying a content taxonomy and set of
patterns. To use Gordon's terminology, typical patterns for each musical system. It
Fiereabend is recommending that "verbal seems likely that a vocabulary of pitch and
association" precede "symbolic association." rhythm patterns can be identified for most
(See Feierabend's article in Readings in types of music, and that students could
Music Learning Theory, Walters & Taggart, benefit from listening to and imitating those
1989, pp. 286-300). patterns. (Those interested in exploring
Conversely, Gordon has shown greater cross-cultural applications of music learning
willingness to incorporate ideas from other theory should consult Campbell's book
methods, provided that they are used in a Lessons from the World: A Cross-Cultural
sequence that is consistent with his taxono- Guide to Music Teaching and Learning')
mies. For example, in his lectures, he now Music learning theory presents a potential
acknowledges the potential effectiveness of problem when approaching some avant-
Curwen (often erroneously referred to as garde Western music. Gordon's tonal
Kodaly) hand signs and Orff instrument syllable system for dealing with what is
experiences, techniques he originally criti- traditionally called "atonal" music seems
cized. The pragmatic compromises Gordon awkward. Gordon contends that atonal
made in the Jump Right In instrumental series music is really "multitonal," and that listeners
are further indications of his general evolu- must subjectively audiate a series of tonal
tion toward greater flexibility. Evidence of centers in order to derive meaning from that
his willingness to combine features of music C1989a, pp. 86-87). He suggests that
various approaches may be found in the sightsingers use "intertonal" patterns to
several chapters of Readings in Music connect "two tonal patterns in different
Learning The01Y (Walters & Taggart, 1989) tonalities in the same keyality." He contin-
that address the implications of music ues:
learning theory for pedagogical approaches The first pitch of the intertonal ... pattern is

Volume II, Numbers 1 & 2 55


performed with the appropriate tonal syllable in theory achieves widespread adoption, it is
the tonality and keyality of the first tonal clear that such Gordon contributions as
pattern, bur it is always audiated as "do." The
audiation and developmental aptitude will
second pitch of the interrenal ... pattern is
continue to influence music education.
audiated with the second pitch of the
Intertonal. .. pattern, or its enharmonic. It is Edwin Gordon is one of the most prolific
performed with the appropriate tonal syllable in authors in music education, and one of its
the tonality and keyality of the second tonal most profound thinkers. Whether or not
pattern (p. 118). music educators agree with his ideas, they
Experienced singers often use intervals to should be thankful for his important contri-
sing atonal music. The approach Gordon butions to the debate about music learning.
proposes is more consistent with music References
learning theory, but complicated. Further Bean, K. 1. (1939) An experimental approach to
research is needed to determine the most the reading of music. PsychologicalMono-
efficient way to develop notational audiation graphs, 50,80.
for atonal or multitonal music. Bessom, M. E., Tatarunis, A. M., & Forcucci, S. 1.
(1980) Teaching music in today's secondary
Conclusion schools: A creative approach to contemporary
Considerable research is needed before music education (2nd ed.). New York: Holt,
music educators should abandon traditional Rinehart, and Winston.
Brink, E. (Summer, 1983) A look at Edwin E.
practices and adopt music learning theory as
Gordon's theories. Bulletin of the Council for
the method for teaching music. However,
Research in Music Education, 76, 1-13.
the problems mentioned above notwithstand-
Broman, K. 1. (1956) The effects of subjective
ing, this author finds music learning theory to rhythmic grouping under the influence of
be the most complete and thoughtful de- variable rates. Unpublished doctoral disserta-
scription of music learning now available. tion, Indiana University, Bloomington.
The theory continues to evolve, based on Buggert, R. W., & Fowler, C. B. (1973) 77Jesearch
research findings and the practical experi- for musical understanding. Belmont, CA:
ence of teachers who use it. Many of the Wadsworth Publishing Co., Inc.
finer details are speculative, and some are no Campbell, P. S. (991) Lessonsfrom the world: A
doubt erroneous, but the general process it cross-cultural guide to music teaching and
learning. New York: Schirmer.
describes-moving from aural experience to
Colwell, R. (969) Music achievement tests 1 and
notation to theory-c-is useful and challenging.
2. Chicago: Follett Educational Corporation.
Music learning theory has had some
Cook, W. W., Leeds, C. H., and Callis, R. (1951).
influence on other approaches to music Minnesota teacher attitude inuentory. New
instruction and on the traditional rehearsal York: Psychological Corp.
setting. Until such time as decisive research DeYannan, R. M. (1972) An experimental analysis
evidence surfaces either in support of or in of the development of rhythmic and tonal
contradiction to the fundamental principles ca pabilities of kindergarten and first grade
of music learning theory, its future influence children. Experimental Research in the
may depend upon its success in revising and Psychology of Music: Studies in the Psychology
refining common practice. The development of Music, 8, 1-44.
of appropriate instructional materials would Dittemore, E. E. (1970) An investigation of some
musical capabilities of elementary school
hasten the adoption of music learning theory
students. Experimental Research in the
by classroom teachers, but the ultimate
Psychology of Music: Studies in the Psychology
determinant of its popularity may be the of Music, 6, 1-44.
musical skills of practitioners. Until college Farnum, S. & Watkins, J. (954) The
music theory and teacher preparation Watkins-Farnum performance scale. \'V'inona,
programs insure that music education majors MN: H. Leonard, Inc.
can perform tonal and rhythm patterns, the Flohr, J. W. (1981) Short-term music instruction
use of music learning theory may be limited and young children's developmental music
to an elite group. aptitude. Journal of Research in Music
Regardless of whether music learning Education, 29(3), 219-223.
Froseth, James O. (981) The individualized in-

56 The Quarterly
structor. Chicago: G. 1. A. Publications. study in the components of music. journal
Froseth, James O. (984) The comprehensive of Psychology, 43, 301
music instructor. Chicago: G. 1. A. Publica- Hevner, K. 0935a) Expression in music: A
tions. discussion of experimental studies and
Gagne, R. (1965) The conditions of learning. theories. Psychological Reviews, 47, 186.
New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Hevner, K. (l935b) The affective character of
Gardner, H. (1983) Frames of mind: The theory major and minor modes in music. American
of multiple intelligences .. ew York: Basic journal of Psych 0 log v, 47, 103
Books, Inc. Hevner, K. (1936) Experimental studies of the
Goodlad, J. 1. (1967) Music's place in education. elements of expression in music. American
In M. S. Egbert (Ed.) Creative approaches to journal of Psychology, 48, 246
school music (pp.1-15). Chicago: American Hevner, K. (1937) The affective value of pitch
Music Conference. and tempo in music. Atnerican fournal of
Gordon, E. E. (965) Musical aptitude profile. Psychology, 49, 621
Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Hodsell, Frank. (Fall, 1985) Cited in "Hodsell on
Gordon, E. E. (1970). Iowa tests a/music literacy. the Arts in Education" MhNC Sound post, Vol.
Iowa City: Bureau of Educational Reasearch 2, No. I, p. 10.
and Service, University of Iowa. Holahan, John M. (986) Teaching music
Gordon, E. E. (971) T7.1epsychology 0/ music through music learning theory: The contribu-
teaching. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: tion of Edwin E. Gordon. In Contemporary
Prentice-Hall. Music Education by Michael L. Mark, pp. 152-
Gordon, E. E. (1974) Toward the development of 172. New York: Schirmer Books.
a taxonomy of tonal patterns and rhythm Hoffer, C. R. (1983) Teacbing music in tbe
patterns: Evidence of difficulty level and secondary schools (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA:
growth rate. Experimental Research in the Wadsworth.
Psychology of Music: Studies in the Psychology Hornung, T. M. (1982) The development of a model
of Music, 9, 39-232. of tbe psychological processes which translate
Gordon, E. E. (1976) Tonal and rhythm patterns.' musical stimuli into affective experience
An objective analysis. Albany, NY: State (Doctoral Dissertation, Case Western Reserve
University of New York Press. University, 1982).
Gordon, E. E. (1978) Afactoranalytic description jarjisian, c. S. (1983) The effects of pentatonic
of tonal and rhythm patterns and objective and/or diatonic pitch pattern instruction on
evidence 0/pattern difficulty level and growth the rote-singing achievement of young
rate. Chicago: G.1.A. Publications. children (D.j\'1.A.dissertation, Temple Univer-
Gordon, E. E. (1979) Primary measures of music sity, 1981).
audiation. Chicago: G.1.A. Publications. jarjisian, c. S. (1983) Pitch pattern instruction and
Gordon, E. E. (1982) Intermediate measures of the singing achievement of young children.
music audiation. Chicago: G.I.A. Publica- Psychology of Music, 11, 19-25.
tions. Jones, R. (Summer, 1985) A dialectic analysis of
Gordon, Edwin E. 0984a) Instrument timbre pref- selected contradictions among definitions of
erence test. Chicago: GJ.A. Publications. meter in music. Bulletin of tbe Council on
Gordon, E. E. C1984b) Learning sequences in Research in Music Education, No. 83, 43-56.
music: Skill, content, and patterns. Chicago: Kinyon, John. (959). Breeze Easy. New York:
G.I.A. Publications, Inc. Witmark.
Gordon, E. E., & Woods, D. G. (1984) jump right Keene, J. A. (982) A history ofmusic education
in.' The music curriculum. Chicago: G.I.A. in the United States. Hanover: University
Publications, Inc. Press of New England.
Gordon, E. E. (198%) Learning sequences in Kratus, John. (May, 1990) Structuring me music
music: Skill, content, and patterns. Chicago, curriculum for creative learning. Music
IL: G.1.A. Publications. Educators fournai, Vol. 76, No.9, pp. 33-37.
Grunow, R. and Gordon, E. E. (l989b) jump Landis, B. & Carder, P. (972) The eclectic cur-
Right In: The Instrumental Series. Chicago: riculum in American music education:
G. 1. A. Publications. Contributions of Dalcroze, Kodaly, and 011
Henkin, R. 1. (1955) A factorial study of the com- Washington, D.C.: Music Educators National
ponents of music. Tbejournal ofPsycbology, Conference.
39, 161-181. Leonhard, C. & House, R. W. (1972) Fou ndations
Henkin, R. 1. (1957) A re-evaluation of a factorial and principles of music education. New

Volume II, Numbers 1 & 2 57


York: McGraw-Hill Book Co. Schleuter, S. 1. & DeYarman, R. (1977) Musical
Lorge, Irving & Thorndike, Robert 1. (e. 1954) The aptitude stability among primary school
Lorge-Tborndihe intelligence tests. Boston: children. Bulletin of the Councilfor Research
Houghton Mifflin. in Music Education, 51, 14-22.
MacKnight, C. B. (1975) The effects of tonal Shehan, P. K. (1986) Major approaches to music
pattern training on the performance achieve- education. Music Educators journal. 72 (6),
ment of beginning wind instrumentalists. 26-31.
Experimental Research in the Psychology of Shuler, Scott C. (1987) The effects of Gordon's
Music: Studies in the Psychology of Music, 10, learning sequence activities on music
53-76. achievement. Ann Arbor, MI: University
Mainwaring, J. (1933) Kinesthetic factors in the Microfilms International.
recall of musical experience. British journal Stockton, J. 1. (1983) An experimental study of
of Educational Psychology, 284-307. two approaches to the development of aural
Mark, M. 1. (1978) Contemporary music educa- meter discrimination among students in a
tion. New York: Schirmer Books. college introductory class. Dissertation
Mark, M. 1. (1986) Contemporary music educa- Abstracts International, 43, 718A.
tion. New York: Schirmer Books. Van Nuys, K., & Weaver, H. E. (1943) Memory
Meyer, 1. B. (1956) Emotion and meaning in span and visual pauses in reading rhythms
music. Chicago: The University of Chicago and melodies. Psychological Monographs, 55,
Press. 33-50.
Meyer, 1. B. (1967) Music, the arts, and ideas. Walters, DarrelL., and Cynthia Crump Taggart,
Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. eds,(989) Readings in Music Learning
Miklaszewski, K. (1986) Review of Learning Theory. Chicago: G. 1. A. Publications.
sequences in music: Skill, content, and Webster's seventh new collegiate dictionary. (1970)
patterns and Study guide for Learning se- Springfield, MA: G. & c. Merriam Co.
quences in music: Skill, content, and patterns.
Gordon, E. E. (1984) Chicago: G.1.A.
Publications, Ine. Bulletin of the Councilfor
Research in Music Education, 89, 83-86. Moving?
Miller, P. H. (1975) An experimental analysis of
the development of tonal capabilities of first
grade children. Experimental Research in the
Psychology of Music: Studies in the Psychol-
ogy of Music, 10, 77-97.
Mills, E., & Murphy, T. c. (1973) The Suzuki
concept: An introduction to a successful
methodforearly music education. Berkeley,
CA: Diablo Press.
Ortmann, 0. (1937). Span of vision in note
reading. Yearbook of the Music Educators
National Conference, 88-93.
Palmer, M. H. (1974) The relative effectiveness of
the Richards and the Gordon approaches to Please let us know!
rhythm reading for fourth grade children
Don't miss an issue of The Quarterly
(Ed.D. dissertation, University of Illinois at
because we don't know your new address!
Urbana-Champaign, 1974). Dissertation
Abstracts International, 35, 7266A.
Palmer, M. H. (1976) Relative effectiveness of two Call us at (303) 351-2254
approaches to rhythm reading for or
fourth-grade students. journal of Research in Send a change-of-address postcard to:
Music Education, 24(3), 110-118. University of Northern Colorado
Peters, G. D., & Miller, R. F. (1982) Music School of Music, The Quarterly
teaching and learning New York: Longman 123 Frasier Hall
Ine. Greeley, Colorado 80639
Petzold, R. G. (1969) Auditory perception by We promise that changing your address with The
children. journal of Research in Music Quarterlywill be the easiest part of moving'
Education, 17(1),82-87.

58 The Quarterly

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi