Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 11

Families of Children with Attention Deficit

Hyperactivity Disorder: A Review


Lily Hechtman, MD, FRCPC1

Objectives: 1) To review the evidence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and other conditions in
family members (siblings and parents) of children with ADHD and determine the importance of genetic and
environmental factors in this condition. 2) To describe the prospective 10-year follow-up of 65 families with ADHD
children and 43 families of matched normal controls. 3) To review various studies that have looked at parent–child
interactions with ADHD children on and off stimulant medication, and such interactions over time. The paper thus
provides an overview of family pathology and functioning of families of ADHD children over time.
Method: The paper outlines twin, sibling, family and adoption studies with regard to possible genetic and
environmental factors in ADHD. It also presents data of a prospective 10-year follow-up of 65 families with ADHD
children and 43 families of normal controls. This family study evaluated sociocultural factors, child rearing
practices, health of family members and relationships, as well as the parental view of the child’s functioning over
time.
Results: A review of the literature suggests that ADHD has a strong genetic component, but that environmental
factors also play an important role. Families of children with ADHD have more problems than families of normal
controls, but these problems improve as the child with ADHD grows up and leaves home. Families of ADHD
subjects can appreciate positive as well as negative changes in their children over time. Generally, family
interactions with children with ADHD are problematic but improve when the child is on medication and when the
child becomes an adult.
Conclusions: This condition has strong genetic underpinnings; therefore, diagnosing and treating family members
(parents and siblings) as well as the child with ADHD is important in improving parent–child interactions and
better long-term outcome for the child and his or her family.

(Can J Psychiatry 1996;41:350–360)

Key Words: ADHD, families, genetics, follow-up

T his paper addresses the area of families of children with


ADHD in 3 distinct ways. First, it reviews the evidence
of ADHD and other conditions in family members (siblings
with ADHD children on and off stimulant medication, and
such interactions over time.
The paper thus provides an overview of family pathology
and parents) of children with ADHD and determines the and functioning of families of ADHD children over time.
importance of genetic and environmental factors in this con-
dition. Second, it describes the prospective 10-year follow-up Families and the Evidence of Genetic Factors in ADHD
of 65 families with ADHD children and 43 families of
matched normal control children. Third, the paper reviews This section will review twin, sibling, family and adoption
various studies that have looked at parent–child interactions studies in relation to children with ADHD to evaluate the
evidence presented in these studies for a genetic basis in this
condition. Wherever possible, the relative importance of
genetic and/or environmental factors (see Tables I–IV) will
Manuscript received January 1996, revised June 1996. be presented, because often the 2 factors act together in
1
Director of Research, Division of Child Psychiatry, Professor of Pediatrics
and Psychiatry, McGill University; Director of Adolescent Services and
producing symptoms and impairment.
Staff Psychiatrist, Montreal Children’s Hospital, Montreal, Quebec. Studies involving siblings and parents of children with
Address reprint requests to: Lily Hechtman, Department of Psychiatry,
Montreal Children’s Hospital, 4018 Ste Catherine Street West, Montreal, ADHD are important from an etiological perspective in that
QC H3Z 1P2 they suggest possible genetic underpinnings in this condition.
They are also important from a treatment perspective,
Can J Psychiatry, Vol 41, August 1996 however, in that awareness of the genetic factors in ADHD
350
August 1996 Families of Children with ADHD 351

Table I
Evidence of Genetic Factors in ADHD: Twin Studies
Number of
Authors Subjects Results Critique Conclusions
Lopez (1965) MZ - 4 pairs − MZ more concordant than DZ for ADHD - Small number - None possible
DZ - 6 pairs − 4 of DZ opposite-sex pairs
Heffron (1984) MZ - 3 pairs − All concordant for ADHD - Small number − None possible
− No comparisons
Goodman and MZ - 29 pairs − All 13 years − Sufficient number − 50% genetic
Stevenson (1989) DZ - 45 pairs − MZ more alike regarding ADHD symptoms − Looked at genetic and − 50% environmental
than same-sex DZ environmental factors
− Environmental factors operating, eg,
perinatal and family adversity
Gillis and others MZ - 81 pairs − MZ 81% concordant for ADHD − All twins also reading disabled − ADHD is hereditary
(1992); Gilger and DZ - 52 pairs − DZ 29% concordant for ADHD − Statistical regression model used
others (1992)
Stevenson (1992) MZ - 91 pairs − MZ more similar in measures of activity and − Symptoms, not diagnosis, − Genetic factors in attention and
DZ - 105 pairs attention than DZ measured activity
Edelbrock and MZ - 99 pairs − MZ correlations of 0.68, DZ 0.29 for − Wide age range of 4 to 15 years − Genetic factors in attentional
others (1995) DZ - 82 pairs attentional problems − Used only CBCL problems and school competence
− Genetic factors in competence in school and − Multiple regression analysis − Envionmental factors in social
behavioural problems functioning, anxiety, and
− Environmental factors in participation in depression
social relationships, school performance,
anxiety, depression, delinquency

will make clinicians more vigilant about detecting, diagnos- discord, parental criticism, and malaise), and perinatal adver-
ing, and treating the condition in siblings and parents. Such sity (for example, low birth weight). They concluded that
diagnosis and treatment will hopefully improve family func- genetic effects accounted for approximately one-half of the
tioning and parent–child interaction and result in better treat- explainable causes of hyperactivity and inattentiveness, leav-
ment outcome for children with ADHD. ing up to about 50% to be explained by other causes, including
environmental aspects.
Twin Studies
In their extensive study of reading-disabled twins in the
Generally, genetically based disorders should be very Colorado Reading Project, Gillis and others (4) and Gilger
similar in twins and more similar in monozygotic (MZ) and others (5) attempted to diagnose ADHD in the twins by
(identical) than in dizygotic (DZ) (fraternal) twins. Recently, parental responses on the Diagnostic Interview of Children
there have been a number of twin studies that have looked at and Adolescents (DICA) (6). They thus examined 81 MZ and
the concordance of ADHD in twins. 52 same-sex DZ pairs of a reading-disabled sample of twins.
An early study by Lopez (1) compared 4 pairs of MZ males They found that for reading disability, the concordance rate
with 6 pairs of DZ twins. Four of the DZ twins, however, were was 84% for MZ twins and 66% for DZ twins. For ADHD,
opposite-sex pairs in which the male was hyperactive. This the concordance rate was 81% for MZ twins and 29% for DZ
limits the validity of the study. Another small study by twins. The concordance rate for both reading disability and
Heffron and others (2) reported on 3 pairs of MZ twins, all ADHD was 44% for MZ twins and 30% for DZ twins. The
concordant for ADHD. data suggest that both reading disability and ADHD may have
strong though independent genetic components (5).
More recently, Goodman and Stevenson (3) studied 570
thirteen-year-old twins. These authors focused particular at- Gillis and others (4) examined the same group of subjects
tention on 29 MZ and 45 DZ same-sex twin pairs in which at but focused particularly on 37 MZ and 37 DZ same-sex twin
least one twin met criteria for pervasive hyperactivity (that is, pairs of whom one twin had been diagnosed with ADHD via
hyperactivity present in all situations, such as at home and at the DICA. The authors used a basic regression model for
school). MZ twins were more alike than same-sex DZ pairs analysis (a statistical technique) and found that 79% of MZ
on objective measures of attentiveness and on parent and twins and 32% of DZ twins were concordant for ADHD
teacher ratings of hyperactivity (59% versus 33%). In their (P < 0.001). Furthermore, adjustment for IQ or reading
careful study, the authors also explored the possible effects performance differences did not substantially change their
of stereotyping (that is, the tendency to rate identical twins results. The authors thus conclude that the results of this
similarly), adverse family factors (for example, marital analysis suggest that ADHD is highly heritable.
352 The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry Vol 41, No 6

Table II
Sibling Studies
Authors Number of Subjects Results Critique Conclusions
Safer (1973) − 19 full-sibling pairs − 50% (10/19) full siblings − Genetic factors appear
− 22 half-sibling pairs concordant for ADHD important
− 9% (2/22) half-sibling pairs
concordant for ADHD
Borland and Heckman − 22 hyperactive subjects − Mean age of 30 − 20- to 25-year retrospective − As adults, hyperactive
(1976) − 20 brothers − Hyperactive subjects have diagnosis subjects worse than
more emotional and work brothers
problems than brothers − No genetic implications
Feldman and others (1978) − 48 hyperactive subjects − Mean age of 21 − 10- to 12-year retrospective − As adults, hyperactive
− 48 siblings − Hyperactive subjects have diagnosis subjects worse than
more educational, emotional, − Siblings may not be of the siblings
and social problems same sex − No genetic implications
Loney and others (1983) − 22 hyperactive subjects − Age 21 to 23 − Retrospective diagnosis − Hyperactive subjects worse
− 22 brothers − Hyperactive subjects worse − Normal controls than brothers
than brothers in work, social, − National survey, other studies − Brothers worse than normal
and antisocial measures, but not matched or evaluated controls
brothers worse than normal directly − Not clear if genetic or
controls environmental factors
Welner and others (1977) − 53 siblings of − 26% brothers of hyperactive − Siblings of hyperactive
hyperactive subjects subjects had ADHD subjects worse than
− 38 siblings of controls − 9% brothers of controls had siblings of controls
ADHD − Suggest genetic factors as
matched for SES and IQ
Faraone and others (1993) − 140 ADHD − ADHD and their siblings have − No environmental measures − ADHD may be genetic
− 120 controls more school failure and lower
− 303 siblings of ADHD IQ than normal controls and
and others their siblings

One of the largest twin studies is currently being carried environmental influences were detected for participation in
out in Australia (7). The authors plan to screen 3 400 four- to activities, quality of social relationship, performance in
twelve-year-old twin pairs and their siblings and determine school, anxiety and depression, and delinquent behaviour.
the perinatal and developmental history, as well as the inci-
Thus, these twin studies also indicate a greater concor-
dence and concordance of ADHD, conduct disorder (CD),
dance in monozygotic than dizygotic twins for different
and separation anxiety (SA). The study is ongoing and the
components of the syndrome. This supports the hypothesis
results are not yet available.
that there is a genetic component in this condition.
Various studies have suggested that different symptoms of
ADHD are heritable. In an early study, Rutter and others (8) Sibling Studies
reported that monozygotic twin pairs were more similar to Most studies involving siblings of hyperactive subjects
one another than dizygotic twins in psychomotor activity. have used the siblings as a control or comparison group. In
This finding was confirmed by Willerman (9) and Torgensen such studies, hyperactive children usually functioned worse
and Kringlen (10). than their siblings. Thus, Borland and Heckman (13), in a 20-
Stevenson (11), using multiple regression analysis (a sta- to 25-year retrospective study, interviewed 20 men (mean age
tistical technique) on data obtained from 91 pairs of MZ twins 30) who, from childhood medical records, conformed to the
and 105 pairs of same-sex DZ twins, concluded that results diagnostic criteria for hyperactive child syndrome. The
were consistent with a significant genetic contribution to authors compared this group with their brothers (mean age
individual differences in activity level and attention abilities. 28). Generally, they found that neither the hyperactive sub-
jects nor their brothers experienced serious social or psychi-
Recently, Edelbrock and others (12) evaluated 99 MZ and atric problems as adults. The hyperactive subjects, however,
82 same-sex DZ pairs of twins aged 4 to 15 years via the Child had more emotional and work difficulties and lower socio-
Behavior Checklist (CBCL) completed by parents. They economic status.
found a correlation of 0.68 (MZ) and 0.29 (DZ) for attentional
problems. Generally, using multiple regression analysis, they Feldman and others (14) also carried out a 10- to 12-year
found significant genetic influences on competence in school retrospective follow-up study on 48 young adults (mean age
and on all areas of problem behaviour. Significant shared 21) previously diagnosed as hyperactive and compared them
August 1996 Families of Children with ADHD 353

Table III
Family Studies
Authors Number of Subjects Results Critique Conclusions
Morrison and Stewart (1971) − Parents of 59 hyperactive − 20% of hyperactive subjects − Retrospective diagnosis of − Increased rate of
subjects had a parent who was parents via parental reports hyperactive subjects in
− Parents of 48 controls hyperactive as a child parents of hyperactive
vs 5 % of controls subjects
Cantwell (1972) − Parents of 50 hyperactive − 21% of hyperactive subjects − Retrospective diagnosis of − Increased rate of
subjects had a parent who was parents via parental reports hyperactive subjects in
− Parents of 50 controls hyperactive as a child vs parents of hyperactive
2% of controls subjects
Frick and others (1991) − Biological relatives of 177 − Relatives of children with − Retrospective diagnosis via − Increased rate of ADHD in
children with ADHD or CD ADHD increased childhood report of a relative biological relatives of
history of ADHD children with ADHD
Biederman and others (1990, − 457 fist-degree relatives of − 25% of relatives of ADD − Direct structured interview − Increased rate of ADD in
1992) 73 children with ADD, 26 children had ADD vs 5% of evaluations first-degree relatives of
children other conditions, those with other conditions children with ADD
26 normal controls vs 4.6% of normal controls
Faraone and others (1994) − Second-degree relatives of − Relatives of children with − Diagnosis of relatives made − Increased risk of ADHD in
140 children with ADHD, ADHD increased risk for by interview of parents second-degree relatives of
120 normal controls ADHD compared with children with ADHD
second-degree relatives of − Greatest risk for biological
normal controls relative of ADHD parent

with their siblings. Ninety-one percent of individuals with the on Drug Abuse norms. This difference was greater for the
diagnosis of hyperactivity at age 21 were either in school or hyperactive subjects who used more inhalants (glue, cocaine,
working. When compared with their siblings, however, the sedatives, and stimulants) than was the norm in the national
hyperactive subjects had lower educational achievement and survey. There was a trend for the sibling group to use more
lower self-esteem. Some 10% of the individuals with hyper- stimulants than the national survey group. Hyperactive
activity seemed to have significant problems with respect to subjects were more involved in crimes against persons, and
drug use, inactivity, and schizoid personality disorders. Ten this involvement tended to be more severe when compared to
percent of the hyperactive subjects drank alcohol before work that of their brothers. Consequently, the hyperactive subjects
or school compared to none of the sibling controls. Again, had more serious police involvement, with 41% having been
one gets the picture that siblings of hyperactive subjects are convicted or having spent time in jail compared to 5% of their
functioning fairly well and better than the hyperactive indi- brothers. We thus see that though the hyperactive subjects are
viduals. not functioning as well as their brothers, the brothers in turn
In Loney and others’ study (15), she compared 22 pairs of are not functioning as well as a normal control group; they
hyperactive individuals and their brothers, age 21 to 23 years. have more antisocial personality disorder and more alcoholism.
Again, there is a clear pattern that hyperactive subjects do A study comparing siblings of 53 hyperactive subjects
worse than their siblings in a variety of areas; for example, with siblings of 38 matched control subjects was carried out
more hyperactive subjects have significant unemployment, by Welner and others (16). They matched a group of hyper-
impaired interpersonal relationships, and a lack of permanent active subjects and a group of control subjects on sex (all
residence when compared to their brothers. Thus, 45% of the males), race (all white), age (mean age 11 years), and socio-
hyperactive individuals meet antisocial outcomes criteria economic status (roughly middle class). All had IQs of 79 or
(modified by excluding Impaired Interpersonal Relation- above, were free of significant neurological abnormalities
ships) compared with 18% of the brothers and 6% of the and psychosis, and were living with their mother. Welner and
normal control subjects in an extensive depression study. others then compared all the siblings of both groups (hyper-
Even though there was no significant difference in the number
active and controlled) age 6 and older. They found that 26%
of hyperactive subjects and their brothers who were diag-
of the brothers of hyperactive subjects but only 9% of brothers
nosed as definitely and probably alcoholic (27% and 23%,
of control subjects (P < 0.02) had ADHD. In addition, bro-
respectively), this finding is greater than that of a depression
thers of the hyperactive subjects had 3 or more depres-
study, where only about 9% of the controls met alcoholic
sion–anxiety symptoms. Again, no differences were found in
criteria.
the sisters. Interestingly, no differences were found in antiso-
There was also no significant difference with regard to cial behaviour in the brothers and sisters of hyperactive and
nonmedical drug use between hyperactive subjects and their control subjects, though hyperactive subjects themselves
brothers, but both groups differed from the National Survey showed more antisocial behaviour.
354 The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry Vol 41, No 6

More recently, Faraone and others (17) compared psychi- with the much stronger genetic evidence from the twin studies
atric and intellectual assessments of 140 children with ADHD (described previously), sibling studies add further support to
and 120 normal controls with their 303 siblings. The probands the presence of genetic factors in ADHD.
generally were white, non-Hispanic boys. Not surprisingly,
the ADHD children were more likely to have had learning Parental Studies
disabilities, repeated grades, been placed in special classes, Family studies of hyperactive children have been based on
and received academic tutoring. They also performed worse the assumption that a genetic component to hyperactivity will
on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised be reflected in a higher familial rate of the disorder for
(WISC-R). For these ADHD children, comorbid conditions children with ADHD.
such as conduct disorder, major depression, and anxiety dis-
Thus, in an early study, Morrison and Stewart (19) found
order predicted school placement more than school failure or
that 20% of hyperactive children in their study had a parent
WISC-R scores. This comorbidity, however, was not the
who was diagnosed (retrospectively) as hyperactive, com-
main factor as ADHD children without comorbidity had more
pared with 5% of their medical controls. Similarly, Cantwell
school failure and lower WISC-R scores than normal
(20) also found that 21% of hyperactive boys in his sample
controls. Intellectual impairment and school difficulties were
also had a parent who could be classified as being hyperac-
also increased among the siblings of ADHD children.
tive–antisocial in childhood versus 2% of the pediatric clinic
Faraone concludes that this finding lends support to the
controls.
hypothesis that ADHD syndrome is familial.
More recently, Frick and others (21) assessed the history
In another early study, Safer (18) compared the incidence
of childhood behaviour problems in the biological relatives
of ADHD in 19 full- and 22 half-sibling pairs. Each pair had
of 177 outpatient boys, aged 7 to 12 years with ADHD or
been raised together by a common mother. One member of
conduct disorder (CD). The mothers, fathers, and other bio-
this pair was known to have minimal brain dysfunction
logical relatives of children with ADHD were significantly
(MBD), now known as ADHD. Nearly half (10) of the
more likely to have a history of childhood ADHD but no
full-sibling pairs were concordant for ADHD compared to
antisocial behaviour problems or substance use in childhood.
only 2 of the 22 half-sibling pairs. This significant difference
Attention deficit disorder with or without hyperactivity was
between full and half siblings further supports a genetic
similarly associated with a family history of ADHD.
component of this condition.
In a series of studies by Biederman and others (22,23)
In summary, siblings (particularly brothers) of hyperactive
(described in detail later), the authors found that first-degree
subjects function better than the hyperactive subjects but
relatives of ADHD children had higher risks for ADHD
seem to have more problems than siblings of controls (for
compared with relatives of other psychiatrically referred chil-
example, higher rates of hyperactivity and more
dren and relatives of pediatric clinic controls (25.1% versus
depression–anxiety symptoms), although not more antisocial
5.3% versus 4.6%, P < 0.00001), respectively.
behaviour. It remains unclear if these difficulties reflect
familial patterns, genetic influence, or merely the stress of Faraone and others (24) expanded these investigations and
having a same-sex hyperactive sibling. When taken together explored the prevalence of ADHD in second-degree relatives

Table IV
Adoption Studies
Authors Number of Subjects Results Critique Conclusions
Morrison and − Adoptive parents of 35 − Adoptive parents less likely to have been − Retrospective diagnosis − Biological parents of
Stewart (1973) adopted children with hyperactive in childhood than biological from parental reports hyperactive subjects more
hyperactivity parents − No comparison group often hyperactive than
adoptive parents,
therefore genetic
factors supported
Alberts-Corush and − Biological adoptive relatives − Biological relatives do worse on measures − Factors such as IQ and − Some support for genetic
others (1986) of 43 children with ADHD of attention than do adoptive relatives of SES not controlled for factors in ADHD
ADHD children
Cadoret and − N = 283 adoptees aged − Biological parent’s disorder predicted − Biological and
Stewart (1991) 18 to 40 antisocial personality disorder in adult environmental factors,
− Group 1 = Biological parent adoptees only if placed in lower SES home both important
with psychiatric or − Lower SES: no increase in antisocial
behavioural problems personality disorder without biological
− Group 2 = Biological parent parent having criminality/delinquency
with no psychiatric or − Psychiatric problems in adoptive parents
behavioural problems increased aggressivity in adoptees
August 1996 Families of Children with ADHD 355

of 140 ADHD children and 120 normal controls. The infor- normal controls. These findings indicated the importance of
mation was collected from the subjects’ parents. family–genetic risk factors in ADD. In a more recent similar
This assessment process has obvious limitations, however expanded study (23) of 140 probands, 120 normal controls,
the authors found that the second-degree relatives of ADHD and 822 of their first-degree relatives, Biederman showed
children were at increased risk for ADHD compared with the nearly half (49%) of the ADHD subjects had no comorbidity
second-degree relatives of normal control children. These with conduct disorder, major depressive or multiple anxiety
risks were greatest when the second-degree relatives were disorder. Compared to controls, however, ADHD probands
biologically related to an ADHD parent of an ADHD child. were more likely to have these conditions. Similarly, relatives
Consistent with the greater prevalence of ADHD among boys of ADHD probands had a higher risk for ADHD (25% versus
compared with girls, grandfathers were at greater risk than 8%), antisocial disorders, major depressive disorders (26%
grandmothers, and uncles at greater risk than aunts. The versus 9%), substance dependence, and anxiety disorders.
authors thus advocate the usefulness in obtaining information Biederman suggests ADHD and major depressive disorders
from second-degree relatives in evaluating the genetic may show common familial vulnerabilities, that ADHD and
epidemiology of ADHD, as such studies would clarify the conduct disorder may be a distinct subtype, and that ADHD
mechanism of familial transmission. and anxiety disorder are transmitted independently in fami-
lies. He concludes that these results extend previous findings,
Adoption Studies indicating family–genetic influences.
The possible genetic basis of this condition is further Early family studies did not clearly distinguish between
supported by the finding that adoptive relatives of ADHD purely hyperactive children and those who had hyperactivity
children are less likely to have ADHD or associated disorders or ADHD combined with conduct disorder and/or opposi-
than are biological relatives of such children (25). In addition, tional disorder. Recent studies which looked at this comor-
biological relatives of ADHD children perform worse on bidity suggest that children with these combined diagnoses
standardized measures of attention than do adoptive relatives may have significantly different family patterns than children
of ADHD children (26). who have only ADHD.
Other Conditions in Families of Children with ADHD Biederman and his colleagues (29) evaluated first-degree
In addition to increased rates of ADHD in families of relatives of 22 children with ADHD and 29 normal controls.
hyperactive children, other conditions have also been found Fourteen (64%) of the ADHD children also had the diagnosis
with some frequency in these families. Thus, Morrison and of conduct or oppositional disorder. Relatives of this group
Stewart (19) and Cantwell (20) found that biological parents of children had significantly more antisocial disorder, oppo-
of hyperactive children had higher rates of “sociopathy, hys- sitional disorder, antisocial personality disorder, nonbipolar
teria, and alcoholism compared to parents of normal con- depressive disorder, and overanxious disorder compared with
trols.” Morrison (27) also found a higher incidence of relatives of normal controls and hyperactive subjects without
unipolar but not bipolar affective disorder in the combined coexisting conduct or oppositional disorder. Relatives of
second- degree blood relatives of hyperactive children. Re- ADHD children, with and without conduct or oppositional
cently, McCormick (28) found that depression in mothers of disorder, did not differ in their high rates of ADHD (34%
children with ADHD was significantly higher than that found respectively). This was significantly higher than the rate of
in primary care outpatients. Specifically, an incidence of ADHD in relatives of normal controls. Interestingly, the
17.9% for major depression and 20.5% for minor depression incidence of drug and alcohol abuse and dependence did not
was found in mothers of children with ADHD, compared with differ significantly in the 3 groups.
4% to 6% and 6% to 14%, respectively, for primary care
outpatients. Lahey and others (30) also compared parental pathology
in children (6 to 13 years of age) with conduct disorder (N =
The most extensive family studies to date have been car- 37), with ADHD (N = 18), and with both disorders. Parents
ried out by Biederman and his colleagues (22,23). In the first of children with conduct disorder were more likely to abuse
of these studies (22), they compared 73 male ADD probands substances. Mothers of conduct-disordered children were
and 264 of their first-degree relatives to 26 psychiatrically more often depressed and more frequently had the triad of
referred but not ADD children, 101 of their first-degree antisocial personality disorder, substance abuse, and somati-
relatives, 26 normal pediatric clinic controls, and 92 of their zation disorder. In contrast, parents of children with ADHD
first-degree relatives. The authors used blinded interviewers, only did not have any significant disorders; however, fathers
structured psychiatric interviews, and controlled for gender, of children with both conduct disorder and ADHD were more
generation of relative, age of proband, social status, and likely to have a history of aggression, arrest, and imprison-
intactness of the family. Relatives of ADD probands had ment.
higher morbidity risks for ADD (25.1% versus 5% versus
4.6%, P < 0.00001), antisocial disorders (24.3% versus 6.9% Barkley and others (31,32), in their 8-year follow-up of
versus 4.2%, P < 0.00001), and mood disorders (27.1% versus hyperactive children, also collected information on the bio-
13.9% versus 3.6%) than did relatives of psychiatric and logical fathers of these children. They found that fathers of
356 The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry Vol 41, No 6

hyperactive children compared with fathers of normal assessments initially and at 5-year follow-up. The authors
controls had a history of significantly more antisocial acts, thus had an opportunity to evaluate if and how families had
alcohol abuse, police contacts, and arrests. Their job histories changed in these areas with time, and whether these changes
were less stable and they were generally less financially had any relationship to the hyperactive subjects’ functioning.
responsible. The authors concluded that 11% of fathers of This sequential evaluation of families of hyperactive subjects
hyperactive children met DSM-III-R criteria for antisocial constituted a second part of their study.
personality disorder as opposed to 1.6% of fathers of normal Finally, the parents’ view of their children’s functioning
control children (P < 0.05). When the authors examined the at different ages in their development was assessed.
antisocial acts of fathers of children with hyperactivity, with
and without associated conduct disorder, they found that the Subjects
fathers of children with hyperactivity and conduct disorder Sixty-five families of hyperactive subjects being assessed
had more antisocial acts than those with hyperactivity alone. in a 10-year follow-up study (mean age 19) were compared
Fathers of children who were only hyperactive, however, still with 43 families of normal control subjects matched with the
had more antisocial acts than fathers of normal controls. hyperactive subjects for age (range 17 to 24 years), sex, IQ
These studies clearly suggest that the combination of ADHD (all above 85), and socioeconomic status (each group having
and conduct disorder is associated with significant parental equal representation from each status).
pathology.
Structured family interviews by blinded interviewers and
The possible connections between parental pathology, various scales assessing the families and the families’ assess-
childhood ADHD, and its adult outcome were explored in a ment of their child at various ages were completed by the
recent study by Cadoret and Stewart (33). The authors studied interviewers and the families.
283 adoptees, age 18 to 40. The adoptees were divided into
2 groups based on whether or not biological parents showed Results
evidence (from adoption agency records) of psychiatric prob-
lems or behavioural disturbance. In addition to evaluation of Current Assessment of Families of Hyperactive and
biological parents from agency records, direct evaluation of Control Subjects at 10-Year Follow-Up
adoptees and adoptive parents was also carried out. The Sociocultural Factors. In view of the fact that both groups
authors found that the biological parents’ criminality or were matched for socioeconomic status (each group having
delinquency predicted increased antisocial personality disor- an equal representation from each status), most of the
der in adult adoptees only when the adoptee had been placed sociocultural factors reflecting socioeconomic status were, as
in a lower socioeconomic-status (SES) home. The lower SES expected, not significantly different for the families of hyper-
home had no effect on development of adult antisocial per- active and control youngsters. The groups did not differ in
sonality disorder when there was no criminality in the bio- other social factors such as family size, incidence of adoption,
logical background. or being born from a previous marriage. With respect to
sibling order, hyperactive children were significantly more
The importance of the environmental factors was also seen
frequently the eldest (P = 0.01).
in the fact that psychiatric problems in the adoptive home
were associated with aggressivity syndrome, and lower SES Child-Rearing Practices. Of the child-rearing parameters
of the adoptive home increased the incidence of ADHD assessed, including inconsistency, lack of controls, and puni-
syndrome. Aggressivity but not ADHD predicted antisocial tive–authoritative and overprotective styles, only the puni-
personality disorder in adult adoptees. tive–authoritative parameter tended to be more marked in
families of hyperactive subjects (P = 0.07).
This study demonstrates the importance of genetic (bio-
logical parental pathology) and environmental (SES and Health of Family Members. There was no difference in the
pathology of adoptive family) factors in the development of prevalence of physical illness or deaths in the 2 groups of
adult antisocial personality disorder. families, but parents of hyperactive children tended to have
more mental health problems (P = 0.06). Unfortunately, the
types of mental health problems that prevailed were not
Long-Term Follow-Up of Families of Hyperactive
specifically categorized. The severity was categorized on a
Children
5-point scale. “Severe” referred to any condition that required
Hechtman (34) compared 65 families of hyperactive psychiatric hospitalization, psychotic conditions, character
young adult subjects and 43 families of control subjects disorders with multiple offenses, or drug addiction including
matched for socioeconomic status. The mean age of the alcoholism. “Mild” referred to any neurotic condition.
hyperactive and control subjects at this 10-year follow-up Seven of 41 (17%) families of control children received
period was 19 years. In addition to the socioeconomic factors, psychiatric treatment compared with 18 of 65 (28%) families
areas such as child-rearing practices, physical and mental of hyperactive children. When we examined the history of
health of family members, and family relationships were psychiatric symptoms in family members who received no
assessed. The families of hyperactive subjects had had similar treatment, however, we saw that 10 of 41 (24%) control
August 1996 Families of Children with ADHD 357

families had such symptoms compared with 27 of 65 (41%) the latter only if the hyperactive subject had moved out.
hyperactive families. We thus see that the striking difference Generally, families of hyperactive subjects tended to improve
in the mental health of family members between the 2 groups in their functioning with time, even though they did not equal
was not due to a history of severe psychiatric pathology, the functioning of families of matched controls.
which requires treatment, but a reflection of milder symp-
Parental View of Child’s Functioning Over Time. Even
toms, which usually go untreated. Finally, a significantly
though families of hyperactive children see their offspring as
higher proportion of parents of control versus hyperactive
having more difficulties currently than control children, they
children showed good dynamic integration (P = 0.05).
are on the whole not more pessimistic about their present or
Relationships. Marital relationships also tended to be future functioning. Conflicts that remain appear tolerable to
worse in families of hyperactive children (P = 0.09). The both families and the hyperactive young adult. Finally, fami-
2 most significant findings indicated that the emotional cli- lies of hyperactive subjects can, despite many problems with
mate of the home (P = 0.009) and the overall family score their children, still appreciate shifts in both their achieve-
(P = 0.004) were considerably worse in families of hyperac- ments and difficulties at each developmental stage. In view
tive children when compared with those of control children. of the above findings, more concentrated work with the
The emotional climate of the home was defined as the degree families of hyperactive children as part of the comprehensive
of positive versus negative interactions among family mem- treatment of this condition seems highly recommended.
bers (for example, arguments or quiet talks) giving rise to a
general level of tension or tranquillity.
Parent–Child Interactions
Sequential Evaluation of Families of Hyperactive Subjects
Sociocultural Parameters. There appeared to be little In the previous section, we discussed how parents of
change in factors reflecting socioeconomic status during the children with ADHD often have significant mental health
3 assessment periods. Parents’ education and work status problems characterized by higher rates of ADHD, depression,
changed little, although, as one would expect, more mothers antisocial personality disorder, and/or substance abuse. These
were working at the 10-year follow-up period. parental difficulties are often associated with multiple prob-
lems in their ADHD children (for example, comorbidities
Child-Rearing Practices. Child-rearing practices re- such as oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, de-
mained the same over the 3 periods, except for a less pression, and anxiety). It is therefore not surprising that the
punitive–authoritative approach at the 10-year follow-up. parent–child interactions of ADHD children and their parents
Health of Family Members. Families had experienced would be problematic.
more medical illnesses and deaths at 10-year follow-up than Thus, Battle and Lacey (35) showed that mothers of highly
at the time of the initial assessment or 5-year follow-up, active males were critical, disapproving, unaffectionate, and
however the mental health of family members had improved severe in their punishment. This was particularly true in the
by the time of the 10-year follow-up. This had been observed middle-childhood years. None of these maternal behaviours
already 5 years earlier, but was not yet significant at that time. were associated with high activity levels in females.
Relationships. Emotional climate of the home, marital
There have been several studies which have supported the
relationship, and total family score were not significantly
view that mothers of hyperactive children are merely re-
different at the 3 assessments. When we analyzed those
sponding to the type of behaviour and difficulties that their
families where the hyperactive subjects were no longer at
children present and do not cause these problems. Campbell
home, however, we saw that at 5-year follow-up (mean age
(36) compared observational data of 3 groups of mothers and
13) the emotional climate at home was considerably worse at
children while performing easy and difficult tasks. The 3
the initial assessment (mean age 8) or at 10-year follow-up
groups represented reflective, impulsive, and hyperactive
(mean age 19), even though the marital relationship remained
children. The authors found no differences in the
unchanged. This suggests that the emotional climate of the
mother–child interactions among the groups during the easy
home improved when the hyperactive young adult left.
task. During the difficult task, however, mothers of hyperac-
In summary, families of hyperactive children had more tive children made more encouraging comments and pro-
difficulties than those of normal controls. These difficulties vided more suggestions concerning impulse control. There
were mainly in the areas of mental health of family members, was a trend for mothers of the hyperactive children to be more
marital relationships, and, most particularly, the emotional disapproving, but this was not significant. Generally, mothers
climate of the home. The families also tended to use a more of hyperactive children were seen as supportive and not
punitive–authoritative child-rearing approach. In the sequen- punitive. As no differences in mother–child interactions
tial evaluation of families, however, we saw that this punitive existed between the groups during the easy task, the authors
child-rearing tendency decreased at the 10-year follow-up. concluded that the mothers of hyperactive children were
The mental health of family members also improved at the responding to the child and adopting a more controlling style
10-year follow-up, as did the emotional climate in the home; as the child experienced more difficulty.
358 The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry Vol 41, No 6

In a subsequent study (37) that compared the interactions and an age-matched control group in structured-task and
of hyperactive, learning-disabled, and matched normal boys free-play situations. The mothers of hyperactive subjects
and their mothers on the same easy and difficult tasks, the were more directive and negative, and less responsive and
results were similar. Mothers of hyperactive childen provided approving. This was particularly true for the mothers of the
more encouragement, impulse-control directions, and disap- younger hyperactive children in the structured-task situation.
proval when compared to the mothers of the other 2 groups. In a subsequent study, Barkley and Cunningham (41)
Again, the authors concluded that mothers were responding compared mother–child interactions of hyperactive children
to the child’s behaviour. in both drug and placebo states. When the hyperactive chil-
Humphries and others (38) hypothesized that stimulant dren were on drugs, they increased their compliance, and their
medication which modified children’s behaviour would in mothers reduced their commands, and increased their respon-
turn change mothers’ interactions with their children—that siveness. However, the level of responsiveness and interac-
is, make them more positive and less controlling. This would tion between the mothers and their hyperactive sons did not
also support the idea that mothers were primarily responding equal that of the normal control children. Schachar and others
to their children’s hyperactivity. The authors gave hyperac- (42) also showed that when the hyperactive child is medi-
tive children and their mothers a highly structured task to cated, the mothers show increased warmth, increased contact,
perform together under both drug and placebo conditions. and decreased criticism, and the hyperactive child relates
Unlike in Campbell’s studies (36,37) where the mother’s better to siblings and peers.
participation was optional, in Humphries’ studies (38), her The fact that mothers of hyperactive children may get
participation was mandatory. While on medication, hyperac- locked into a negative parenting pattern, which extends to
tive children and their mothers made fewer errors, praised nonhyperactive siblings, was pointed out by Mash and
each other more, and criticized each other less, and mothers Johnston (unpublished observations). The authors compared
gave fewer directions. This change in the nature of the interactions of hyperactive children, their siblings, and their
mother–child interaction, while the child was on stimulants, mothers with those of age-matched normal control children.
led the authors to conclude that mother’s intrusive, control- Results showed that hyperactive children and their siblings
ling, disapproving pattern was a response to the child’s dis- had more social conflict and higher rates of negative behav-
ordered behaviour and not a cause of it, since it modified with iour during play. The conflicts decreased during the mother-
changes in the child’s behaviour. Humphries, however, did supervised situations. Mothers of hyperactive children were
not have a control group with which the hyperactive less responsive, interactive, and rewarding, and more nega-
mother–child dyad could be compared, so the degree of tive than mothers of nonhyperactive children. This was true
normalization achieved by stimulant medication could not be of their interactions with the siblings as well as the hyperac-
assessed. In addition, the authors used a very structured tive children.
laboratory test which may not be relevant to real-life
situations. Few studies have looked at the parenting style of fathers
of hyperactive children. Tallmadge and Barkley (43) found
Both of these shortcomings were addressed and controlled that fathers of hyperactive children, like mothers, were more
by Cunningham and Barkley (39). These authors compared directive than fathers of control children; however, the hyper-
the interactions of hyperactive (off medication) and normal active children were more likely to comply with fathers’ than
boys and their mothers in both free play and task settings. with mothers’ commands.
They found that during free play, mothers of hyperactive
children interacted less with their children. They initiated It is thus not surprising that Mash and Johnston (44) found
fewer contacts, responded less, and encouraged play less. that parents of hyperactive children reported less confidence
When they did interact, they tended to be more controlling, in their parenting knowledge when compared to control chil-
but the children also complied less. In the task setting, again dren. In addition, mothers of hyperactive children reported
mothers of hyperactive children initiated fewer contacts and more stress, social isolation, self-blame, and depression. This
responded less than mothers of control subjects. Mothers of finding was similar to the increased personal psychological
hyperactive children were more controlling and gave more distress reported by mothers of hyperactive adolescents ver-
commands, but even though the children complied less, their sus mothers of normal control subjects who were seen by
mothers rewarded compliance less and tended to be inconsis- Barkley in their 8-year prospective follow-up study (32).
tent in rewarding desired behaviours. The authors concluded Interactions Over Time
that the intrusive, controlling style of the mothers of hyper-
A number of authors have looked at parent–child interac-
active boys, while initially a response to the child’s overac-
tions of hyperactive subjects over time. Campbell and Ewing
tive, impulsive, inattentive style, may further contribute to the
(45) compared interactions from early to late childhood, and
child’s behavioural difficulties.
Barkley and others (32) compared them from childhood to
These findings were almost identical to those of Mash and adolescence. Generally, the authors concluded that findings
Johnston (40), who also compared mother–child interactions with older hyperactive children were less negative and that
in younger and older hyperactive children (off medication) older children were able to sustain compliance better than
August 1996 Families of Children with ADHD 359

younger ones. As seen in Barkley and others’ (32) adolescent regard to treatment, it seems clear that treatment needs to be
follow-up study, however, the interactions, even with older comprehensive. Comprehensive treatment needs to include
hyperactive subjects, are still more problematic when com- medicating the child, treating affected parents, and providing
pared to those of normal control subjects. Observations of parent training and counselling in order to ensure more posi-
mother–adolescent interactions in Barkley’s study found that tive clinical and long-term outcome for these hyperactive
the hyperactive dyad displayed more negative and controlling children and their families.
behaviours and less positive and facilitating behaviours to-
ward each other than the normal dyad. These interaction
patterns were significantly related to similar patterns in Clinical Implications
mother–child interactions observed 8 years earlier. These
difficulties were particularly prominent in situations where • Possible genetic factors in ADHD.
the adolescent had ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder • Affected parents and siblings should be assessed and treated.
(ODD). The authors suggest that the development and • Long-term outcome may improve with better mental health of
maintenance of ODD into adolescence in hyperactive chil- family members.
dren is strongly associated with aggression and negative
parent–child interactions in childhood. Limitations

Generally, one can conclude that the controlling, less • Specific genetic difficulty not determined.
responsive, and more negative parenting style of mothers of • Parents and siblings may decline treatment.
hyperactive children, reported in several studies, is most • Other factors also affect long-term outcome.
probably a consequence and not a cause of the child’s behav-
iour. It is thus also not an intrinsic, predetermined aspect of
the mother’s personality. The evidence that the parenting
style became markedly more positive (less controlling, and References
intrusive, and more rewarding) when the child’s behaviour
improved on stimulant medication shows that it is the child’s 1. Lopez RE. Hyperactivity in twins. Can Psych Assoc Journal 1965;10:421–6.
2. Heffron WA, Martin CA, Welsh RJ. Attention deficit disorders in three pairs of
behaviour which initially influences the mother’s behaviour. monozygotic twins: a case report. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
However, even with stimulant medication, the mother–child 1984;23:299–301.
interaction of hyperactive children, though much improved, 3. Goodman R, Stevenson J. A twin study of hyperactivity II: the aetiological role
of genes, family relationship and perinatal adversity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
does not equal that of normal control children. It seems that 1989;30:691–709.
old habits and patterns, once established, are modifiable but 4. Gillis JJ, Gilger JW, Pennington BF, DeFries JC. Attention deficit disorder in
reading-disabled twins: evidence for a genetic etiology. J Abnorm Child Psychol
difficult to alter entirely; therefore, even if the negative 1992;20:303–15.
mother–child interaction seen in hyperactive children is in- 5. Gilger JW, Pennington BF, DeFries JC. A twin study of the etiology of
itially a response to the child’s overactive, impulsive, inatten- comorbidity. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and dyslexia. J Am Acad
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1992;31:343–8.
tive style, it may affect the child’s future development in 6. Herjanic B, Campbell J, Reich W. Development of a structural psychiatric
terms of self-esteem and social competence and thus exacer- interview for children: agreement between child and parent on individual
symptoms. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1982;10:307–24.
bate his or her handicaps and symptomatology. Interventions
7. Levy F, Hoy D. ADHD in twins and their siblings. Presented at the Society for
(parental counselling, parent training groups) that would fos- Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology. Sarasota FL, 1992.
ter appropriate parenting skills in parents of hyperactive 8. Rutter M, Korn S, Birch HG. Genetic and environmental factors in the
development of “primary reaction patterns.” Br J Soc Clin Psychology
children are therefore crucial. 1963;2:162–73.
9. Willerman L. Activity level and hyperactivity in twins. Child Dev
1973;44:286–93.
Summary and Conclusions 10. Torgensen AM, Kringlen E. Genetic aspects of temperamental differences in
infants. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1978;17:433–44.
In summary, one can see that siblings of hyperactive 11. Stevenson J. Evidence for a genetic etiology in hyperactivity in children. Behav
Genet 1992;22:337–43.
children fare better than the hyperactive children, but worse 12. Edelbrock C, Rende R, Plomin R, Thompson LA. A twin study of competence
than normal control children. The parents of hyperactive and problem behavior of childhood and early adolescence. J Child Psychol
children are more often hyperactive, antisocial, and depressed Psychiatry 1995;36:775–85.
13. Borland BL, Heckman HK. Hyperactive boys and their brothers: a 25-year
when compared to parents of normal control children. Fur- follow-up study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1976;33:669–75.
thermore, the mental health of family members improves with 14. Feldman S, Denhoff E, Denhoff J. The attention disorders and related syndromes:
time, particularly if the hyperactive youngster leaves home. outcome in adolescence and young adult life. In: Stern L, Denhoff E, editors.
Minimal brain dysfunction: a developmental approach. New York NY: Masson
Interactions of hyperactive children with their parents and Publishing; 1979. p 133–48.
siblings are more negative but improve on medication, al- 15. Loney J, Whaley-Klahn MA, Kosier T, and others. Hyperactive boys and their
brothers at 21: predictors of aggressive and antisocial outcomes. In: Van Dusen
though, even on medication, they do not equal those of normal KT, Mednick SA, editors. Prospective studies of crime and delinquency. Boston
control children. Finally, the family studies described in this MA: Kluwer-Nijhoff; 1983. p 181–206.
paper have significant etiological and treatment implications. 16. Welner Z, Welner A, Stewart, MA, Palkes H, Wish E. A controlled study of
siblings of hyperactive children. J Nerv Ment Disease 1977;165:110–17.
Specifically, from the etiological point of view, there is much 17. Faraone SV, Biederman J, Lehman BK, Spencer T, Norman D, Seidman LJ, and
evidence for a familial (genetic) basis for this condition. With others. Intellectual performance and school failure in children with attention
360 The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry Vol 41, No 6

deficit hyperactivity disorder and in their siblings. J Abnorm Psychol 32. Barkley RA, Fischer M, Edelbrock CS, Smallish L. The adolescent outcome of
1993;102:616–23. hyperactive children diagnosed by research criteria III: mother-child interactions,
18. Safer DJ. A familial factor in minimal brain dysfunction. Behav Genet family conflicts and maternal psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
1973;3:175–86. 1991;32:233–55.
19. Morrison JL, Stewart MA. A family study of the hyperactive child syndrome. Biol 33. Cadoret JR, Stewart MA. An adoption study of attention deficit
Psychiatry 1971;3:189–95. hyperactivity/aggression and their relationship to adult antisocial personality.
20. Cantwell DP. Psychiatric illness in the families of hyperactive children. Arch Gen Compr Psychiatry 1991;32:73–82.
Psychiatry 1972;27:414–23. 34. Hechtman L. Families of hyperactives. In: Greenley J, editor. Research in
21. Frick PJ, Lahey B, Benjamin B, Christ MA, Loeber R, and others. History of community and mental health, vol 2. Greenwich CT: JAI Press; 1981. p 275–92.
childhood behavior problems in biological relatives of boys with attention-deficit 35. Battle ES, Lacey B. A context for hyperactivity in children, over time. Child Dev
disorder and conduct disorder. J Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry 1991;20:445–51. 1972;43:757–73.
22. Biederman J, Faraone SV, Keinan K, Knee D, Tsuang MT. Family-genetic and 36. Campbell SB. Mother-child interaction in reflective, impulsive, and hyperactive
psychosocial risk factors in DSM-III attention deficit disorder. J Am Acad Child children. Dev Psychology 1973;8:341–9.
Adolesc Psychiatry 1990;29:526–33. 37. Campbell SB. Mother-child interaction: a comparison of hyperactive, learning
23. Biederman J, Faraone SV, Keinan K, Benjamin J, Krificher B, Moore C, and disabled, and normal boys. Am J Orthopsychiatry 1975;45:51–7.
others. Further evidence for family-genetic risk factors in attention deficit
38. Humphries T, Kinsbourne M, Swanson J. Stimulant effects on cooperation and
hyperactivity disorder: patterns of comorbidity in probands and relatives in
social interaction between hyperactive children and their mothers. J Child Psychol
psychiatrically and pediatrically referred samples. Arch Gen Psychiatry
Psychiatry 1978;19:13–22.
1992;49:728–38.
39. Cunningham CE, Barkley RA. The interactions of normal and hyperactive
24. Faraone SV, Biederman J, Milberger S. An exploratory study of ADHD among
children and their mothers in free play and structured tasks. Child Dev
second-degree relatives of ADHD children. Biol Psychiatry 1994;35:398–402.
1979;50:217–24.
25. Morrison JL, Stewart M. The psychiatric status of legal families of adopted
hyperactives. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1973;28:888–91. 40. Mash EJ, Johnston C. A comparison of the mother-child interactions of younger
26. Alberts-Corush J, Firestone P, Goodman JT. Attention and impulsivity and older hyperactive and normal children. Child Dev 1982;53:1371–81.
characteristics of the biological and adoptive parents of hyperactive and normal 41. Barkley RA, Cunningham CE. The parent-child interactions of hyperactive
control children. Am J Orthopsychiatry 1986;56:413–23. children and their modification by stimulant drugs. In: Knights R, Bakker D,
27. Morrison JL. Adult psychiatric disorders in parents of hyperactive children. Am editors. Treatment of hyperactive and learning disabled children. Baltimore MD:
J Psychiatry 1980;137:825–7. University Park Press; 1980. p 219–36.
28. McCormick LH. Depression in mothers of children with attention deficit 42. Schachar R, Taylor E, Wieselberg M, Thorley G, Rutter M. Changes in family
hyperactivity disorder. Fam Med 1995;27:176–9. function and relationships in children who respond to methylphenidate. J Am Acad
29. Biederman J, Munik K, Knee D. Conduct and oppositional disorders in clinically Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1978;26:728–32.
referred children with attention deficit disorder: a controlled family study. J Am 43. Tallmadge J, Barkley RA. The interactions of hyperactive and normal boys with
Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1987;26:724–7. their fathers and mothers. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1983;11:565–79.
30. Lahey B, Piacentini J, McBurnett M, Stone P, Hartolagen S, Hynd G. 44. Mash EJ, Johnston C. Parental perceptions of child behavior problems, parenting,
Psychopathology in the parents of children with conduct disorder and self-esteem, and mothers’ reported stress in younger and older hyperactives and
hyperactivity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1988;27:163–70. normal children. J Consult Clin Psychol 1983;51:86–99.
31. Barkley RA, Fischer M, Edelbrock CS, and others. The adolescent outcome of 45. Campbell SB, Ewing LJ. Follow-up of hard to manage preschoolers’ adjustment
hyperactive children diagnosed by research criteria: an 8 year prospective at age 9 and predictors of continuing symptoms. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
follow-up study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1990;29:546–57. 1990;31:871–85.

Résumé

Objectifs : 1) Examiner les données relatives au trouble d’hyperactivité avec déficit de l’attention (THADA) et à
d’autres affections chez les membres de la famille (fratrie et parents) d’enfants atteints de THADA et déterminer
l’importance des facteurs génétiques et environnementaux dans le cadre de cette affection. 2) Décrire le suivi
prospectif sur 10 ans de 65 familles d’enfants atteints de THADA et de 43 familles de témoins normaux appariés.
3) Examiner diverses études portant sur les interactions parents-enfants chez des enfants atteints de THADA qui
recevaient périodiquement des stimulants et sur ces interactions au fil du temps. L’article donne donc un aperçu
de la pathologie familiale et du fonctionnement des familles d’enfants atteints de THADA en fonction du temps.
Méthode : L’article décrit des études sur les jumeaux, la fratrie, la famille et l’adoption à l’égard des facteurs
environnementaux et génétiques éventuels du THADA. On y présente aussi les données d’un suivi prospectif sur
10 ans de 65 familles d’enfants atteints de THADA et de 43 familles de témoins normaux. Cette étude familiale a
évalué les facteurs socioculturels, les pratiques d’éducation des enfants, la santé des membres de la famille et
leurs relations mutuelles, ainsi que la perception par les parents du fonctionnement de l’enfant en fonction du
temps.
Résultats : Un examen de la littérature révèle que le THADA comporte une importante composante génétique,
mais que les facteurs environnementaux jouent également un rôle important. Les familles d’enfants atteints de
THADA éprouvent davantage de problèmes que les familles des témoins normaux, mais ces problèmes s’atténuent
au fil de la croissance de l’enfant atteint de THADA et lorsqu’il quitte le domicile familial. Les familles des sujets
atteints de THADA peuvent constater des modifications positives ou négatives chez les enfants en fonction du
temps. En général, les interactions entre les familles et les enfants atteints de THADA sont marquées par des
problèmes, mais elles s’améliorent lorsque l’enfant reçoit des médicaments et lorsqu’il devient adulte.
Conclusion : Cette affection a de profondes ramifications génétiques : par conséquent, le diagnostic et le
traitement des membres de la famille (parents et fratrie), de même que de l’enfant atteint de THADA, sont
importants afin d’améliorer les interactions parents-enfants et les résultats à long terme pour l’enfant et sa famille.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi