Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 11

International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing

Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark. 13: 1–11 (2008)


Published online 12 February 2007 in Wiley InterScience
(www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/nvsm.297

Determinants of charitable donation


intentions: A structural equation
model
Sampath Kumar Ranganathan*, y and Walter H. Henleyy
Fogelman College of Business and Economics, The University of Memphis, USA

 Currently charities have to depend more on individual donors and less on the govern-
ment for funding. Hence, understanding the individual donor and what motivates them
to contribute to charities is something, which has been of increasing interest to nonprofit
marketers. In this article, a path model for the charitable donation process of a religious
individual is developed and tested. The variables that are used in the model are religiosity,
attitude towards helping others (AHO), attitude towards charitable organizations (ACO),
attitude towards the advertisement (Attad) and behavioral intentions (BI). The results
suggest that AHO by itself does not cause BI. Altruistic people need to be targeted with an
appropriate advertisement message. Since religiosity is an important causal variable for
AHO, segmenting and targeting individuals who are religious would be pertinent.
Attempts to build favorable ACO would also be worthwhile.
 Religiosity
 Charitable donation intentions
 Charity advertisements.
 Path model for predicting intentions to donate..
Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

My brothers, what use is it for a man to say he has faith when he does nothing to
show it? Can that faith save him? Suppose a brother or a sister is in rags with not
enough food for the day, and one of you says, ‘Good luck to you, keep yourselves
warm, and have plenty to eat’, but does nothing to supply their bodily needs, what is
the good of that? So with faith; if it does not lead to action, it is in itself a lifeless thing.
(Bible; James 2:14–17).

Introduction
Charity and religion go hand in hand. Major
*Correspondence to: Sampath Kumar Ranganathan, Uni- religions like Christianity, Hinduism, Islam,
versity of Memphis, Fogelman College of Business and Judaism, and Buddhism stress the importance
Economics, The University of Memphis, Memphis, TN of helping the needy. It is a command
38152. E-mail: smgnthn@memphis.edu
y
Sampath Kumar Ranganathan and Walter H. Henley are for Christians to help others bountifully
Doctoral Students of Marketing. and sufficiently (Deuteronomy, 15:7–11).

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., February 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
2 S. K. Ranganathan and W. H. Henley

Christianity has strong commands to the What makes an individual donate to a


faithful about helping others (i.e., The parable charity? Many variables have been cited as
of the Good Samaritan). Even people who reasons for this behavior. Several areas have
are not of the Faith are to be loved and helped been researched including: attitudes toward
(Colossians 3:11 Romans 12:14–20). Comma- charitable organizations (Webb et al., 2000),
nds from God have inspired religious people to altruism (Piliavin and Chang, 1990), religiosity
start hospitals, schools, and charitable organ- (Hoge, 1995), involvement (Chiang, 2003),
izations like Missionaries of Charity to help donor characteristics (Pessemier et al., 1977)
theneedy. Like Christianity major religions of and size of request (Reingen, 1978). Studies
the world like Hinduism and Islam stress the have also focused on personal characteristics
importance of helping the needy. Hindus are and the norms of donors. Rich donors may see
required to remove their sins by donating giving as an obligation that is part of their
Cows, money, and land to the poor. An annual current privileged or posthumous position
charitable donation called as Zakkat is con- (Ostrower, 1995). Moral norms and guilt were
sidered to be a mandatory religious duty of a also found to be related to intentions to give to
Muslim. charities (Moore et al., 1985). In a special issue
Two-thirds of the world’s population has of Journal of Business Research focusing on
various religious affiliations and this is often non-profits, Bennett and Sargeant (2005) stress
used as a plausible rationale for their intentions the need for more studies on individual donors
and behavior (Paloutzian, 1996). Since all and particularly stress studies that develop a
major religions stress the importance of comprehensive model of donor behavior.
charitable actions we would expect religious Hence in this paper we would like to
people to donate generously to charities. investigate charitable giving intentions of
However, the situation is less than heavenly individuals and the potential impact of religion
for many nonprofit firms as there is intense on BI of donations to charity.
competition for charitable donations. Cur-
rently there are more than 1.4 million charities
Conceptual framework
in USA, (Giving USA, 2006) which creates
intense competition for funds (Sargeant, Despite the abundance of consumer research
1999). Thus, the non-profit marketing sector supporting the relationship between attitudes
faces severe competition like any other and behavior, little is known about individuals’
business. Consequently donors have also charitable attitudes (Webb et al., 2000).
begun to feel that non-profit organizations Additionally, businesses have long used seg-
are big businesses (Blum, 2002). Adding to the mentation to identify and target markets to
woes of non-profit organizations is the fact that enhance the impact of the marketing mix.
government, the largest donor is slowly Likewise, effective segmentation can enable
reducing its contribution to non-profits and non-profit marketers to improve their
is demanding that public should contribute marketing strategies in the areas of pro-
instead (Nichols, 2004). Hence non-profits motion, positioning, pricing, and distribution
have to depend mostly upon individual donors. (Schlegelmilch et al., 1992). A few researchers
In 2005, individual donors were the single (e.g., Schlegelmilch, 1998; Harvey, 1990) have
largest source of charitable donations account- attempted to identify demographic, socio-
ing for 76.5% of charitable donations out of a economic, and psychographic variables that
total of $260.28 billion (Giving USA, 2006). influence charitable giving and differentiate
This statistic is very important for non-profit donors from non-donors, but this has proven to
organizations to understand the individual be a difficult task (Webb et al., 2000). The
donor, why he/she patronizes a non-profit heterogeneity of the contributing public,
organization and what spurs him/her to donate charitable organizations and types of gifts are
to a non-profit organization. among the factors contributing to the difficulty

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., February 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
Determinants of charitable donation intentions 3

of differentiating donors from non-donors. Attitude towards helping others and


However, as mentioned earlier, religion is a Altruism
pervasive demographic segment which may be
a significant explanation variable of note. Webb et al. (2000) defines Attitude towards
Helping Others (AHO) as ‘Global and relatively
The ability to measure attitudes toward
charitable giving is important for research enduring evaluations with regard to helping or
assisting other people’. Webb et al. (2000)
scholars, policymakers, and practitioners alike.
who came up with the scale to measure AHO,
Research scholars need measures of attitudes
does not differentiate it from altruism. They
toward charitable giving to develop and
view altruism as a helping motive but also
empirically test theories of donation behavior.
Valid and reliable measures of attitudes point out that attitudes are embedded in
cognitive structures including beliefs, values
influencing charitable giving are important
for policymakers facing tough decisions invol- and other attitudes. In psychology, scholars
differ about definitions of altruism and no
ving the allocation of governmental resources
to social problems, as well as the regulation single acceptable definition of altruism exists
(Piliavin and Chang, 1990). Altruism is defined
and taxation of NPOs. More important, such
as a cognitive activity to help others by Brewer
measures aid non-profit practitioners in their
(2003). Altruism is defined as an attitude by
efforts to develop effective and efficient
Frydman et al. (1995), as a motive by Sober
marketing strategies to attract and retain
(1990), as a helping behavior by Schwartz
donors.
(1970) and as a desire to improve another’s
The extant marketing literature indicates
theimportance of differentiating between condition (Karylowski, 1982). But from the
way altruism is conceived and defined in
AHOand ACO. While a few studies examine-
attitudes toward charitable giving (e.g., literature we find it to be similar to AHO.
Hence, in this study we view AHO and altruism
Pessemier et al., 1977; Burnkrant and Page,
1982; McIntyre et al., 1986) and attitudes as conceptually identical variables.
toward charitable organizations (Schlegel-
milch, 1998; Harvey, 1990), the measures
Hypothesis development
used share several common weaknesses. These
complex constructs have frequently been Helping others is recognized as a universal trait
theorized as single-item measures (Schlegel- stressed by all major religions of the world.
milch, 1998) posing a threat to internal validity Chau et al. (1990) found religiosity to be
and reliability. Studies examining single charity positively correlated with altruism. Church
lack the ability to make generalizations. attendees were found to be more altruistic
(Adams and Lonial, 1984; Schlegelmilch, than non-attendees (Smith et al., 1999). Those
1998), while others are limited by very small who attend church are twice as likely to
sample sizes (Radley and Kennedy, 1995). In volunteer to help others compared to those
addition, cross-disciplinary contributions are who do not attend. (Wilson and Janoski, 1995).
seldom incorporated in studies examining Specific to this research, prior research has
charitable behavior, resulting in a highly shown that religious Christians were found to
fragmented body of knowledge (Schlegelmilch be one of the potential sources for charitable
et al., 1992; Bendapudi et al., 1996). Diverse giving because of the motivation to give for
theories and empirical approaches across religious reasons and out of a sense of duty
disciplines make comparison and synthesis (Slyke et al., 2005). Further several studies
of findings difficult and limit the usefulness of have connected practice of religion, church
prior measures. In order to avoid such pitfalls attendance, and charitable behavior (Hoge,
we have incorporated multiple item scales, 1995; Schneider, 1996). Religious tenants
which have been reported to possess good encourage even the poor to donate to charities
reliability and validity. (Iannacone, 1998).

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., February 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
4 S. K. Ranganathan and W. H. Henley

From literature we can understand that evaluate a charity advertisement with help-
religiosity is closely connected with develop- others appeal more positively. Hence based
ing a positive AHO. Wilkes et al. (1986), view upon the argument of Brunel and Nelson
religiosity to encompass one’s religious beliefs, (2000) we hypothesize that AHO will have a
frequency of worship attendance and per- direct positive impact on Attad.
ceived importance of spiritual values. Thus, we
hypothesize that religiosity encourages people H4: AHO has a direct positive effect on
to have an attitude to help others: Attad.

H1: Religiosity has a direct positive effect Many studies have highlighted the import-
on AHO. ance of advertising in determining behavioral
intentions (BI). Shimp (1981) says that it is
Webb et al. (2000) argues that for individ- important to produce favorable attitudes
uals with a positive AHO, donating to towards advertisements in the minds of
charitable organizations is only one way to customers. Shimp (1981) argues that quality
attain the goal of helping others. Further it is of an advertisement depends upon creating a
argued that ACO is expected to be positively favorable attitude toward the advertisement
correlated with charitable giving and that which in turn has a direct impact on customer
people who are high in ACO will in turn choice behavior. Duncan and Nelson (1985)
make donations to charities. AHO is more argue that attitude towards the advertisement
general in nature while ACO is more specific in has a significant, direct positive relationship
nature. Webb et al. (2000) fit the two variables with BI. Hence based upon these arguments
of AHO and ACO in Eagly and Chaiken’s (1993) we postulate a direct positive relationship
terminology of attitudes toward behaviors and between Attad and intentions to donate. Thus,
attitudes toward targets. AHO fits into the we hypothesize that the following:
general category of attitude toward behavior
and ACO fits into the more specific category of H5: Attad will have a direct positive effect
attitude toward targets. Based upon the BI.
theorizing of Webb et al. (2000) it is thus
hypothesized that ACO is predicted by AHO. There is also evidence suggesting a direct
Hence based upon the above said arguments relationship between altruism (substituted
we hypothesize that AHO has a direct positive here as AHO) and BI. Altruistic reasons like
effect on ACO and ACO has a direct positive ‘wanting to help others’ cause people to
effect on BI. volunteer (Sills, 1957). Altruism was found
to be a strong motive for charitable donations
H2: AHO has a direct positive effect on ACO. (Piliavin and Chang, 1990). Chiang (2003)
reports a direct relationship between altruism
H3: ACO has a direct positive effect on BI. and BI. Hence, we included that direct path in
our model and came to our sixth hypothesis
Brunel and Nelson (2000) indicate that the (Figure 1):
world-view of people is a tool to explain their
evaluations of advertisement appeals. Brunel H6: AHO will have a direct positive effect
and Nelson (2000) did not give a definition of on BI
the variable ‘caring world view,’ but they do
explain it as helping mindset and kindness
towards others, which is similar to our
Experiment
generalization of AHO. People, who have a
caring worldview, helping mindset and kind- We used the one- group- post- test only
ness toward others, tend to more closely research design specified by Shadish et al.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., February 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
Determinants of charitable donation intentions 5

ACO

Religiosity AHO BI

Aad

Figure 1. Theoretically proposed path model.

(2002). This research design has no need for a Measures


control group. We created a fictitious adver-
tisement for a fictitious charity with a small Attitude toward the advertisement was
measured by a four item, 7-point semantic
donation cue of $24 per annum. According to
Bendapudi et al. (1996), the size of the differential scale developed by Holbrook and
Batra (1987). Behavioral intention was
donation request affects charitable donation,
measured by a four-item, 5-point scale of Coyle
so we kept the size of the request at a smaller
and Thorson (2001). Attitude toward charita-
amount of $24 per annum (or $2 per month).
ble organizations was measured by a five item,
Respondents were given a printed advertise-
ment copy of this fictitious charity and were 5-point scale developed by Webb et al., (2000).
AHO was measured by a four item, 5-point
asked to fill out a questionnaire.
scale developed by Webb et al., (2000).
Religiosity was measured by a four item,
5-point agree to disagree religiosity scale of
Participants Wilkes et al. (1986). This scale has been
accepted to be a reliable measure of religiosity
Data was collected from students of a major
(Vitell and Paolillo, 2003). The scale measures
metropolitan university in the mid-southern
religiosity as the level of respondent’s religious
United States. Two hundred and forty surveys
were administered, out of which 26 were commitment.
rejected because students left many responses
blank. A total of 214 surveys were retained for
Assessing the measures
the final analysis. Hair et al. (2006) recom-
mends that for structural equation models with The results of our measurement can be only as
five or fewer constructs and higher item good as our measures and we need to find out
communalities (0.60 or higher), sample size whether our measures are good and corre-
of 100–150 can be considered as adequate. Our spond to the underlying theory. We started
study has five constructs and all communalities testing our measures using factor analysis. The
except one (ACO item C2, commu- 21 items representing the 5 latent variables
nality ¼ 0.41) are above 0.60 mark. Hence were subjected to exploratory (EFA) and
our sample size of 214 can be considered as confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). We ran
adequate. The sample demographics were 49% EFA by using maximum likelihood factor
male, 51% female, 87% Christian, 7% other extraction and oblique rotation. The five
religion, and 5% no religion. This sample factors, which emerged accounted for
reasonably reflected the American religious 59.69% of the variance. All items loaded
demographic standards. The mean age of heavily on the hypothesized a priori factors.
respondents was 25 with a standard deviation All items except one ACO item (Item C2: Much
of 7.02 and a range of 18–54 years. of the money given to charities is wasted) had

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., February 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
6 S. K. Ranganathan and W. H. Henley

Table 1. Reliability and completely standardized loadings: confirmatory factor analysis

Construct Item Item loading t value

Religiosity (a ¼ 0.83) 1. Church attendance 0.70 10.81


2. Spiritual values are important 0.71 10.99
3. If Americans were more religious 0.74 11.59
4. Self perceived religiosity 0.79 12.50
Attitude Towards Advertisement (a ¼ 0.82) 1.Like the ad 0.68 10.39
2.Favorable 0.80 12.99
3.Positive 0.81 13.22
4. Advertisement is good 0.67 10.29
Behavioral Intentions (BI) (a ¼ 0.89) 1. Likely to donate 0.91 16.77
2.Will donate next time 0.87 15.78
3. Will definitely donate 0.90 16.61
4. Will recommend 0.64 10.09
Attitude Towards Helping Others 1.Help others 0.71 11.46
(AHO) (a ¼ 0.87) 2. Helping is important 0.78 13.08
3. People should be charitable 0.90 16.16
4. Help people in need 0.72 11.63
Attitude Towards Charitable Organizations 1. Good causes 0.62 9.03
(ACO) (a ¼ 0.78) 2. Charities waste money 0.49 6.83
3. Positive image on charities 0.77 11.76
4. Charities are successful 0.69 10.45
5. Charities are useful 0.65 9.66
Model statistics:

x2df 179 ¼ 349.7, p ¼ 0.00

NNFI ¼ 0.91, CFI ¼ 0.92, IFI ¼ 0.92, RMR ¼ 0.07

RMSEA ¼ 0.065, 90% Confidence Interval for RMSEA ¼ 0.054 to 0.075

factor loadings of more than 0.6. All items were and found that the measures exhibited
retained after the exploratory factor analysis. convergent and discriminant validities.
Next we proceeded to test internal consist- Harman’s (1967) single factor test was used
ency of our measures. Internal consistency of to test for common method bias. Hence, we
our measures was tested using Cronbach’s conclude that our measures are reliable,
co-efficient alpha. For all the measures coeffi- exhibit convergent and discriminant validity
cient alpha was above 0.7 which suggested and do not exhibit common method bias
that the measures were reliable (Nunnally, (Table 1).
1978) (Table 1 and 2).
The 21 items were subject to confirmatory
Table 2. Mean standard deviation and correlations table
factor analysis. The model fit statistics
suggest that the hypothesized model fits
BI Aad ACO AHO Religion
the data satisfactorily (NNFI ¼ 0.91, CFI ¼
0.92, IFI ¼ 0.92, x2 df 179 ¼ 349.7, p ¼ 0.00,
Attad 0.40
RMR ¼ 0.07, RMSEA ¼ 0.06, 90% Confidence ACO 0.36 0.06
Interval for RMSEA ¼ 0.054 to 0.075). (Hu and AHO 0.36 0.22 0.44
Bentler, 1999). We ran tests for convergent RELIGIOSITY 0.21 0.20 0.22 0.41 1.00
Mean 2.81 4.56 3.70 4.07 3.56
and discriminant validities using the method SD 0.95 1.18 0.57 0.69 0.94
suggested by Anderson and Gerbing (1988)

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., February 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
Determinants of charitable donation intentions 7

Model and hypothesis testing To test for mediation we ran three models
and compared it with the baseline model. In
The hypothesized model was tested using the
Model 1 we did not include the path between
sample covariance matrix as input to Lisrel ACO and BI. In model 2 we did not include the
8.51 (Jöreskog and Sörbom, 2001). With
path between Attad and BI. In model 3 we did
the exception of the x2 statistic, results not include the path between AHO and BI. The
indicate that the model fit was acceptable.
x2 statistics indicate full mediation between
(x2df183 ¼ 354.16 p ¼ 0.00, NNFI ¼ 0.91, CFI ¼
AHO and BI. Compared with the full model,
0.92, IFI ¼ 0.92, RMSEA ¼ 0.06, RMR ¼ 0.07).
~ x2 is not significant at the p < 0.05 level for
(Hu and Bentler, 1999) (Table 3).
model 3. Even if we exclude the path between
The results in Table 3 indicate that the AHO and BI, there is no significant change in
hypothesized linkages among all constructs
statistic or model fit. Thus, we have a strong
with the exception of the path between case for ACO and Attad completely mediating
AHO and BI were statistically significant
the path between AHO and BI.
(t-values  2.0). The antecedent variables The model validated by our study is as
explain around 31% of the variance in the
follows (Figure 2):
dependent variable BI. AHO explains nearly a
quarter of the variance of the variable AHO.
Religiosity explains around 16% of the variance
in the dependent variable AHO and 8% of the
Discussion
variance in the dependent variable Attitude Results from this study indicate that religiosity
towards advertisements. The path between is an important determinant of AHO, ACO,
AHO and BI is not significant. But we have Attad, and BI. Religiosity has a statistically
statistically significant indirect effects between significant indirect relationship with ACO
these variables. Hence we tested whether ACO (Effect Size ¼ 0.2, t ¼ 3.92), Attad (Effect
and attitude towards advertisement mediate Size ¼ 0.11, t ¼ 2.98) and BI (Effect Size ¼ 0.13,
the path between AHO and BI. t ¼ 3.42). This indicates that individuals who

Table 3. Model parameter estimates and goodness-of-fit statistics (n ¼ 214)

Estimates and fit statistics Standardized estimate t-value R2

Direct effects:
Attitude towards advertisement ! BI 0.39 4.94
ACO ! BI 0.30 3.35
AHO ! BI 0.06 0.76 0.31
AHO ! ACO 0.50 5.37 0.25
AHO ! Attitude towards advertisement 0.29 3.49 0.08
Religiosity ! AHO 0.40 4.95 0.16
Indirect effects:
AHO ! BI 0.26 4.13
Religiosity ! BI 0.13 3.42
Religiosity ! Attitude towards ad 0.11 2.98
Religiosity ! ACO 0.20 3.92
Goodness of fit statistics:
x2df183 ¼ 354.16, p ¼ 0.00
NNFI ¼ 0.91
CFI ¼ 0.92
IFI ¼ 0.92
RMSEA ¼ 0.064
RMR ¼ 0.07

Note: t-values  2.0 indicate a significant coefficient. R2 indicates the proportion of variance accounted for in each
dependent construct.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., February 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
8 S. K. Ranganathan and W. H. Henley

ACO

Religiosity AHO BI

Aad

Figure 2. Validated path model.

exhibit high levels of religiosity will be Religiosity is important, but how do we


altruistic, will have positive attitudes towards identify the religious? For marketing prac-
charitable organizations, will evaluate charita- titioners religiosity is a multi-dimensional
ble advertisements more positively and will construct. A major dimensional measure of a
have positive intentions to donate to a person’s religiosity is his/her frequency of
charitable organization. Altruism by itself religious service attendance. Hence charities
doesn’t create BI. People should have a can consider targeting religious individuals on
favorable ACO and Attad. the basis of their frequency of religious service
attendance. Special advertising or direct mar-
keting campaigns can be launched to target
people who regularly visit churches. Managers
Managerial Implications
of charities can consider approaching religious
Religiosity is an important antecedent variable people through church gatherings, sponsoring
for predicting charitable donation intentions. spiritual events and programs and sending
Religiosity directly affects donors to have direct mail to members of religious groups. It
favorable AHO and indirectly affects donors should not be overlooked by practitioners that
to have a positive ACO and Attad. Even though religiosity is considered a universal construct
altruism is an important variable for under- which bridges all religions. Hence a general
standing charitable donation process, altruism media advertisement with a generic religiosity
by itself doesn’t cause BI. Its effect on BI is appeal would be appropriate to reach a broad
completely mediated by ACO and Attad. Hence target market (over 2/3 of the world’s
managers of charitable organizations should population has some religious perspective).
pay attention to the advertisement message. If We believe that our study also has implica-
people have a favorable attitude towards the tions for academic researchers. Since ACO is an
advertisement, they will have positive inten- important determinant of intentions to donate,
tions to donate. Thus designing the advertise- its relationship with other variables like Attad
ment and advertisement message becomes and religiosity can be studied in detail.
important and by logical extension pre-testing Particularly we stress the need to study the
the advertisement before use is recommended. relationship between Attad and ACO. Does a
Our study indicates that as religiosity of an positive Attad cause people to have positive
individual increases, higher is the possibility ACO? Can ACO be built through Attad? Is this
that a person will have a more favorable AHO, relationship mediated or moderated by any
ACO, and intentions to donate. Since religiosity other variable? If this important linkage is
indirectly causes people to have positive studied, charities will benefit by designing
attitude towards charity advertisements, advertisement campaigns to increase attitude
charity advertisements that target religious toward charitable organizations. We recom-
people will be more effective. mend that future researchers can consider

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., February 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
Determinants of charitable donation intentions 9

studying the predictor variables of ACO and Bennett R, Sargeant A. 2005. The nonprofit market-
how to build ACO among non-religious people. ing landscape: guest editors’ introduction to a
Every study has limitations and our study is special section. Journal of Business research
no exception to this rule. Our study was done 58(6): 797–805.
in one geographical area, with one size of Blum DE. 2002. Ties that bind. The Chronicle of
request, one type of charity and with a student Philanthropy 14(11): 7–10.
sample. Most of the respondents were Chris- Brewer GA. 2003. Building social capital: civic
tians. The particular advertisement used was attitudes and behavior of public servants. Jour-
similar to direct marketing and the ability to nal of Public Administration Research and
make generalizations of results across other Theory 13(1): 5.
advertising media should be tested by future Burnkrant R, Page T. 1982. An examination of the
convergent, discriminant and predictive validity of
researchers. We hope that this model will be
Fishbein’s behavioral intention model. Journal of
tested by future studies across different
Marketing Research 19(4): 550–561.
geographical settings, advertising media, reli-
Brunel F, Nelson MR. 2000. Explaining gendered
gions, and donor groups.
responses to ‘help-self’ and ‘help-others’
charity ad appeals: the mediating role of
world views. Journal o f Advertising 29(3):
Biographical notes 15–27.
Chiang KP. 2003. Prediction of monetary donation:
Sampath Kumar Ranganathan is a doctoral
a path analysis, Proceedings of the Annual Meet-
student of Marketing in the Fogelman College
ing of the Association of Collegiate Marketing
of Business and Economics at the University of Educators.
Memphis. He is an ABD and currently is Chau LL, Johnson RC, Bowers JK, Darvill TJ. 1990.
working on his dissertation. His research Intrinsic and extrinsic religiosity as related to
interests are in the fields of Services Marketing, conscience, adjustment, and altruism. Personal-
consumar Behavior, Advertising and Non- ity and Individual Differences 11: 397–
profit Marketing. 400.
Walter H. Henley is a doctoral student of Coyle JR, Thorson E. 2001. The effects of progress-
Marketing in the fogelman College of Business ive levels of interactivity and vividness in web
and Economics at the University of Memphis. marketing sites. Journal of Advertising 30: 65–
He is an ABD and currently is working on his 77.
dissertation. His research interests are in the Duncan CP, Nelson JE. 1985. Effects of humor in a
fields of Social Marketing, Religion, Advertising radio advertising experiment. Journal of Adver-
and Non-profit Marketing. tising 14(2): 33–42.
Eagly AH, Chaiken E. 1993. The Psychology of
Attitudes. Harcourt Bruce Jovanovich: Orlando, FL.
Frydman M, Ledruc L, Hofmans V, Molinier C. 1995.
References
The Development of Altruistic Attitudes,
Adams AT, Lonial CL. 1984. Investigation of giving Enfance, 1, 89–100.
behavior to united way using log-linear modeling Giving USA. 2006. Indianapolis, IN: Retrieved April
and discriminant analysis: an empirical study. July 23rd 2006 from http://www.aafrc.org/
Journal of Marketing Science 12(3): 77–88. press_releases/trustreleases/0606_PR.pdf
Anderson JC, Gerbing DW. 1988. Structural Hair JF, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE, Tatham
equation modeling in practice: a review and RL. 2006. Multivariate Data Analysis, 6th edn.
recommended two-step approach. Psychologi- Pearson Education Inc: Upper Saddle River, N.J.
cal Bulletin 103(May): 411–423. Harman HH. 1967. Modern Factor Analysis. Uni-
Bendapudi N, Singh SN, Bendapudi V. 1996. Enhan- versity of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL.
cing helping behavior: an integrative framework Harvey WJ. 1990. Benefit segmentation for fund
for promotion planning. Journal of Marketing raisers. Journal of the Academy of Marketing
60: 33–49. Science 18(1): 77–86.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., February 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
10 S. K. Ranganathan and W. H. Henley

Hoge DR. 1995. Explanations for current levels of Piliavin JA, Chang HW. 1990. Altruism: a review of
religious giving. New Directions for Philanthro- recent theory and research. Annual Review of
pic Fundraising 7: 51–70. Sociology 16: 27–65.
Holbrook MB, Batra R. 1987. Assessing the role of Radley A, Kennedy M. 1995. Charitable giving by
emotions as mediators of consumer responses to individuals: a study of attitudes and practice.
advertising. Journal of Consumer Research Human Relations 48(6): 685–709.
14(3): 404–420. Reingen PH. 1978. On inducing compliance
Hu L, Bentler PM. 1999. Cutoff criteria for fit with requests. Journal of Consumer Research
indexes in covariance structure analysis: conven- 5 September: 96–102.
tional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Sargeant A. 1999. Charitable giving: towards a
Equation Modeling 6(1): 1–55. model of donor behaviour. Journal of Marketing
Iannacone L. 1998. Introduction to the economics Management 15: 215–238.
of religion. Journal of Economic Literature Schlegelmilch B. 1998. Targeting of fund raising
36(3): 1465–1495. appeals: how to identify donors. European Jour-
Jöreskog KG, Sörbom D. 2001. LISREL 8.51 and nal of Marketing 22(1): 31–40.
PRELIS 2.50. Scientific Software International: Schlegelmilch B, Diamantopoulous A, Love A. 1992.
Chicago. Determinants of charity giving: an interdisciplin-
Karylowski J. 1982. Two types of altruistic ary review of the literature and suggestions for
behavior: doing good to feel good or to make future research. In Marketing Theory and Appli-
the other feel good. In Cooperation and cations, Vol 3. Allen Chris T, et al., (eds). Com-
Helping Behavior, Derlega V, Grzelad J, (eds). bined Proceedings, American Marketing
Academic Press: New York; pp. 397– Association: Chicago, IL; 507–516.
413. Schneider JC. 1996. Philanthropic styles in the
McIntyre P, Barnett MA, Harris RJ, Shanteau J, United States: toward a theory of regional differ-
Skowronski J, Klassen M. 1986. Psychological ences. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quar-
factors influencing decisions to donate terly 25(2): 190–210.
organs. In Advances in Consumer Research, Schwartz SH. 1970. Elicitation of moral and self enhan-
Vol 14. Wallendorf Melanie, Paul Anderson, cing behavior. an experimental study of volunteer-
(eds). Combined Proceedings Association ing to be a bone marrow donor. Journal of
for Consumer Research: Provo, UT; 331– Personality and Social Psychology 37: 283–
334. 293.
Moore EM, Bearden WO, Teel JE. 1985. Use of Shadish WR, Cook DT, Campbell DT. 2002. Exper-
labeling and assertions of dependency in appeals imental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for
for consumer support. Journal of Consumer Generalized Causal Inference. Houghton-Mifflin:
Research 12(1): 90. Boston.
Nichols JE. 2004. Repositioning fundraising in the Shimp TA. 1981. Attitude toward the ad as a
21st century. International Journal of Nonpro- mediator of brand choice. Journal of Advertising
fit and Voluntary Sector Marketing 9(May): 10(2): 9–15.
163–170. Sills D. 1957. The Volunteers. Free Press: Glencoe,
Nunnally JC. 1978. Psychometric Theory, 2nd edn. Ill.
McGraw-Hill: New York. Slyke V, David M, Brooks AC. 2005. Why do people
Ostrower F. 1995. Why the Wealthy Give: The give? new evidence and strategies for nonprofit
Culture of Elite Philanthropy. Princeton Univer- managers. American Review of Public Admin-
sity Press: Princeton, NJ. istration 35(3) September: 199–222.
Pessemier EA, Beamon AC, Hanssen DM. 1977. Smith HL, Fabricatore A, Peyrot M. 1999. Religiosity
Willingness to supply human body parts: some and altruism among African American males.
empirical results. Journal of Consumer Journal of Black Studies 29(4) March: 579–
Research 4: 131–140. 597.
Paloutzian RF. 1996. Invitation to the Psychology Sober E. 1990. What is psychological egoism?
of Religion. Allyn and Bacon: Boston, MA. Behaviorism 17: 89–102.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., February 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm
Determinants of charitable donation intentions 11

Vitell Scott J, Paolillo Joseph GP. 2003. Consumer Wilkes RE, Burnett JJ, Howell RD. 1986. On
ethics: the role of religiosity. Journal of Business the meaning and measurement of religiosity
Ethics 46(2): 151. in consumer research. Journal of the
Webb DJ, Green CL, Brashear TG. 2000. Development Academy of Marketing Science 14(1): 47–
and validation of scales to measure attitudes influ- 50.
encing monetary donations to charitable organiz- Wilson J, Janoski T. 1995. The contribution of
ations. Journal of the Academy of Marketing religion to volunteer work. Sociology of Religion
Science 28(2): 299–309. 56: 137–152.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Nonprofit Volunt. Sect. Mark., February 2008
DOI: 10.1002/nvsm

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi