Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 17

4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination


John Baldwin
Subject: Critical/Cultural Studies , Gender (Gay, Lesbian, Bisexual and Transgender Studies), Communication and Culture, International/Global
Communication, Interpersonal Communication
Online Publication Date: Jan 2017 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.013.164

Summary and Keywords

Prejudice is a broad social phenomenon and area of research, complicated by the fact that intolerance exists in internal cognitions but is manifest
in symbol usage (verbal, nonverbal, mediated), law and policy, and social and organizational practice. It is based on group identification (i.e.,
perceiving and treating a person or people in terms of outgroup membership); but that outgroup can range from the more commonly known
outgroups based on race, sex/gender, nationality, or sexual orientation to more specific intolerances of others based on political party, fan status,
or membership in some perceived group such as “blonde” or “athlete.” This article begins with the link of culture to prejudice, noting specific
culture-based prejudices of ethnocentrism and xenophobia. It then explores the levels at which prejudice might be manifest, finally arriving at a
specific focus of prejudice—racism; however, what applies to racism may also apply to other intolerances such as sexism, heterosexism, classism,
or ageism.

The discussion and analysis of prejudice becomes complicated when we approach a specific topic like racism, though the tensions surrounding
this phenomenon extend to other intolerances such as sexism or heterosexism. Complications include determining the influences that might lead
to individual racism or an atmosphere of racism, but also include the very definition of what racism is: Is it an individual phenomenon, or does it
refer to an intolerance that is supported by a dominant social structure? Because overt intolerance has become unpopular in many societies,
researchers have explored how racism and sexism might be expressed in subtle terms; others investigate how racism intersects with other forms of
oppression, including those based on sex/gender, sexual orientation, or colonialism; and still others consider how one might express intolerance
“benevolently,” with good intentions though still based on problematic racist or sexist ideologies.

Keywords: culture, racism, discrimination, prejudice, intolerance, sexism, heterosexism, stereotypes, ethnocentrism, xenophobia

Introduction
One of the causes that gave rise to the postmodern revolution in France in 1968 was the failure of modern science and philosophy—liberalism,
social science, reason, and so on—to remedy problems of war, poverty, and intolerance (Rosenau, 1992). As we look around today at the world in
general, or even within specific nations, we continue to see a wide range of prejudice, from the 1994 genocide of Tutsis (and many Hutus) by
Hutus in Rwanda to the mass killing of 70 people, mostly youths, at a Utøyan youth camp in Norway by Anders Behring Breivik. At this writing,
a major refugee problem exists from people fleeing Middle Eastern countries where a strong ISIS influence is leading to the killing of gays,
Christians, and Muslims from rival belief systems. In many European countries, hate groups and right-wing politicians are gaining ground. The
Southern Poverty Law center tracks 1,600 hate groups within the United States (“Hate and Extremism,” . .), classifying 784 that were active in
2014 (“Hate Map,” . .), and the FBI reports nearly 6,000 hate crimes in the United States, with the greatest numbers due to race (48.5%),
religion (17.4%), and sexual orientation (20.8%; FBI, 2014). These statistics reveal some interesting things about intolerance. For example, the
“race”-based hate crimes include crimes based on anti-white sentiment as well as against people of color; and about 61% of hate crimes based on
sexual orientation target gay males.

Both the international events and the statistics relevant to any specific nation prompt difficult questions about intolerance. In a white-dominant
society, can or should we call anti-white crimes by people of color “racist”? If someone commits a hate crime based on sexual orientation, why
are gay men more often the target than lesbians? Would hate crimes in other countries reflect the same axes of difference, or might hate crimes be
based differently? German hate crimes might be based more on ethnicity (e.g., against Turkish immigrants, who by most racial classifications
would be Caucasian). Why do people commit such acts at all?

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 1/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

One mistake we often make is thinking of prejudice and discrimination only in extreme terms such as genocide and hate crimes. In many
countries and cultures, where overt expression of racism (and other intolerances) has become socially unacceptable, intolerances have gone
“underground,” hidden in subtle forms. Further, intolerance can rely upon a wide variety of identity groups, including some that are (supposedly)
biologically based, like racism, or based on other aspects, such as political party, fan status, or membership in some perceived group such as
“blonde” or “athlete.” In sum, we must consider the relationship between different forms of intolerance, including but not limited to prejudice,
racism, and discrimination; but these must always be understood within specific cultural contexts.

Culture and Intolerance


(Re)Defining Culture
As we look to the cultural influence on intolerance, we must first consider the definition of culture. The study of culture has deep roots in
anthropological and linguistic research, especially as seen in the work of Franz Boaz and his students Margaret Mead, Ruth Benedict, and Edward
Sapir, as well as in the early work of Edward Tyler, itself based on earlier traditions of ethology (Darwin) and social evolution (Marx). This work
influenced the work of anthropologist E. T. Hall (Rogers & Hart, 2002) and others who laid the groundwork for the study of intercultural
communication (Leeds-Hurwitz, 1990). Scholars have debated whether culture is a shared mental framework of beliefs, norms for behavior (i.e.,
the expectations for behavior rather than the behaviors themselves), values, and worldview, or whether culture should also include actual
behaviors, texts, and artifacts of a group. In 1952, A. L. Kroeber and Clyde Kluckhohn synthesized over 150 definitions of culture into a single
definition that focuses on “patterns, explicit and implicit, of and for behavior,” along with “ideas and especially their attached values” (p. 181).
These are influenced and created through symbolic behavior, action, and other aspects of the environment (history, geography). The definitional
dimensions of culture described by Kroeber and Kluckhohn explained well many of the definitions of culture up until the 1980s. After that time,
some scholars (especially in communication) began to treat culture more as a set of symbols and meanings. Others framed culture as a process of
constructing social meanings and systems through communication. As people sing, speak, play, tell jokes, and conduct business, they are
constantly (re)creating their culture—both relying upon it and changing it.

More pertinent to the study of intolerance is a new approach to culture that sees culture neither as “suitcase” of things (be those beliefs and values
or texts and artifacts) passed down from one generation to the next nor as a neutral process of mutual symbolic creation through time, but as
having vested power interests that seek to influence what is seen as accepted or normal within a culture. For example, Moon (2002) defines
culture as a “contested zone”:

Thinking about culture as a contested zone helps us understand the struggles of cultural groups and the complexities of cultural life …
If we define culture as a contested zone in which different groups struggle to define issues in their own interests, we must also
recognize that not all groups have equal access to public forums to voice their concerns, perspectives, and the everyday realities of their
lives”

(pp. 15–16).

That is, every cultural manifestation, such as the framing of Australian culture as “individualistic” or saying that “Australian men have such-and-
such characteristics,” highlights what one should not be within that culture and establishes bounds for group-based intolerance.

With this diversity of definitions in mind, one is not sure what to think culture is or should be. Baldwin, Faulkner, Hecht, and Lindsley (2006)
present a series of essays on the definition of culture by authors from six different disciplines (e.g., multicultural education, anthropology, political
science), as well as 313 definitions of culture from an even greater number of disciplines, which they analyze. While they are reluctant to settle on
a single definition of culture, this definition embraces most trends:

The way of life of a group of people, including symbols, values, behaviors, artifacts, and other shared aspects, that continually evolves
as people share messages and is often the result of a struggle between groups who share different perspectives, interests, and power
relationships

(Baldwin, Coleman, González, & Shenoy-Packer, 2014, p. 55).

This definition of culture, like most definitions that take a symbolic, process, or critical approach, does not treat cultures as “nations,” but as
people groups who share symbolic or speech codes, with multiple cultural groups—defined not by demographic constitutions such as race, sex, or
age, but by shared communicative realities—sharing single geographic areas. It is in the creation and defending of cultures—from countries to
local and virtual communities—that intolerance often becomes apparent.

The Role of Culture in Prejudice

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 2/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

Of various schools of thought about the nature and origins of intolerance, only one approach suggests that intolerance is biological or in some way
inherited, and that is sociobiology, or evolutionary theory. This approach suggests that intolerance is based on such things as preservation of the
purity of the gene pool of one’s group, an inherent fear of strangers, or an inherited need for group identity. But even evolutionary theorists cannot
explain all intolerance based on a theory of inherited impulse. Meyer (1987) argues:

Xenophobia and ethnocentrism as extreme forms of this search for identity cannot be attributed to [human] biology … Their very
existence is a result of [human’s] attempts towards understanding the world, and [their] strong affective need to delimit a cosmos of
conspecifics with whom [they] can share interpretations of [their] socially construed world

(p. 93).

Research on intolerance in 90 preindustrial societies suggests that, when there are clearly psychological causes for intergroup conflict, groups
ultimately use communication to create who the enemy is and how one should demonstrate or show intolerance (Ross, 1991). In sum, there is a
strong cultural component determining which intolerances are felt or expressed in a given place or time.

Culture, however one defines it, can affect tolerance. Culture might be a set of values and beliefs, such as the value of loyalty to one’s group,
combined with a belief that people who belong to a particular group have particular characteristics, are unlikeable for some reason, or merit
mistreatment and the application of a different set of standards than we apply to ourselves (Opotow, 1990). If culture is a process, then we might
look at how a culture creates both identity and intolerance through the ongoing structures of language, including word choices (“babe,” “hunk,”
“faggot”), conversational structure (interruptions, etc.), joke- and storytelling, and so on. For example, West and Zimmerman’s (1987) notion of
“doing gender” (i.e., gender as an everyday accomplishment of language) has led to countless studies of gender construction in several nations, as
well as a focus by others on how we also “do race” and other identities. The way that we construct our identities through communication is
inherently linked to how we construct the identities of those in outgroups, as we shall see; but they are also linked to behavior within our group.
Social constructionist approaches to culture thus often become critical in their focus on power relations. Critical approaches look at how cultures,
through communication, architecture, law, literature, education, and so on create a sense of the “other”—and of the self—that constrains us and
pits us against one another in group conflict.

“Culture”-Based Prejudices: Ethnocentrism, Xenophobia


The purpose of this article is primarily to look at racism and discrimination as forms of prejudice; however, these cannot be understood without a
larger understanding of prejudice in general and other forms or types of prejudice. Allport (1979) defines prejudice as an antipathy one has or a
tendency to avoid the other, based on the other person’s group. For Allport, prejudice is a cognitive or psychological phenomenon:

Prejudice is ultimately a problem of personality formation and development; no two cases of prejudice are precisely the same. No
individual would mirror his [or her] group’s attitude unless he [or she] had a personal need, or personal habit, that leads him [or her] to
do so

(p. 41).

Xenophobia
Based on the Greek word that means “fear of strangers,” xenophobia refers to “the fear or hatred of anything that is foreign or outside of one’s
own group, nation, or culture” (Herbst, 1997, p. 235). The idea is frequently applied to a mistrust or dislike (rather than merely fear) of outgroups
or those perceived to be different, especially in national terms. While the Greek translation suggests the psychological component of fear, recent
researchers have treated the concept in behavioral or message terms. Historical research on xenophobia links it to anti-Semitism and, more
recently, to Islamophobia, though it does not have as clear a historical trajectory as ethnocentrism; many more recent studies look at South Africa
as a model nation in attempting to strategically reduce xenophobia. Researchers use a variety of methods to look at xenophobia, depending on
their research assumptions and background disciplines. Rhetorical media research, for example, analyzes how Czech newspapers code anti-Roma
sentiment through subtle terms such as “inadaptable citizens” (nepřízpůsobivý občan, Slavíčková & Zvagulis, 2014, p. 159); and psychological
survey research investigates how, among Southern California students, ethnocentrism is positively associated with both language prejudice and
feelings of being threatened by immigrants (Ura, Preston, & Mearns, 2015).

Van Dijk (1993) notes how groups can use language such as hyperbole of differences to marginalize immigrants, often through appeals to so-
called democratic values. He notes that in some countries, such as in Central Europe, where claims of racism are often forcefully resisted due to
conceptual ties of the term to Hitler’s Holocaust, Ausländerfeindlicheit (fear of foreigners) takes its place, though this fear of foreigners is
frequently aimed at Turks and other (often darker-skinned and religiously different) people who resist adoption of traditional Germanic culture.

Ethnocentrism

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 3/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

Some types of prejudice relate specifically to the larger and more traditional notion of culture (i.e., cultures as nations). Ethnocentrism gained
prominence as an area of research following sociologist Robert Sumner’s 1906 definition of the term as gauging others in reference to one’s own
culture (1975), though other sociologists soon began to distinguish between this notion of “centrality” and the idea of “superiority”—that one’s
culture or group is superior to those of others. If one sees ethnocentrism strictly as a feeling of superiority, nationalism (or school spirit, or
religious loyalty, etc.) might not in and of itself be ethnocentric if it focuses only on being loyal to or highlighting the benefits of one’s own group,
without denigrating others, though some might argue that it is impossible to feel pride in one’s own group without, at some level, disdaining or
thinking less of other groups. The possibility of an ethnocentric bias in research led many early anthropologists to suggest ethnography—spending
extended time within a culture to see things from cultural members’ point of view—as a way to reduce ethnocentrism in research.

A consideration of ethnocentrism has implications for other forms of bias as well, as the factors that predict national cultural ethnocentrism—and
solutions that address it—could apply equally to one’s perception of life within one’s own community. The Hmong-descended people of the
Pacific Northwest in the United States will likely feel that their ways are superior to those of Moroccan- or Guatemalan-descended peoples, as
well as to those of the dominant culture. Auestad (2013) presented a series of essays on the rise of political discourses across the world that
highlighted elements of national security and identity (tradition), as well as the building of cultures of fear by focusing on the negative aspects of
foreigners or those of different religious groups within single countries. Some elements of the U.S. presidential race rhetoric of 2015–2016
exemplified this xenophobic and ethnocentric trend.

Within the field of intercultural communication, at least two lines of research have focused on ethnocentrism. The first is by Jim Neuliep, who,
with colleagues, has revisited the measurement of ethnocentrism in the classic 1950 work by the Frankfurt School, The Authoritarian Personality,
with a new measure of ethnocentrism. After applying the measure to white Americans, Neuliep (2012) continues to test the relationship of
ethnocentrism to other important intercultural variables, such as intercultural anxiety and communication satisfaction. The second is Milton
Bennett’s (1993) consideration of ethnorelativism. In this approach, a range of attitudes reflects either ethnocentrism or ethnorelativism.
Ethnocentric stances include denial (e.g., indifference toward or ignorance of any difference at all), defense (traditional ethnocentrism of
denigrating the culture of the other or feeling one’s own culture is superior, but also in “going native”), and minimization (focusing on similarities
and ignoring differences, by claiming “color blindness,” or focusing on how we are all the same, be that as “God’s children” or in the Marxist
struggle against oppression; 43). As one grows more “ethnorelative,” or accepting of difference, one exhibits one of three stages: acceptance
(being respectful of and even appreciating the value and behavioral differences of others), adaptation (actually adopting behaviors or views of
other groups), or integration (adopting a worldview that transcends any single culture). This approach has gained ground around the world and in
different disciplines, from Finland to Iran, with applications from cultural sensitivity to interreligious tensions.

Prejudice
One of the difficulties of discussing prejudice is the conceptual overlap between terms (e.g., xenophobia conflates with racial or ethnic prejudice;
ethnocentrism might refer to any people group, such as ethnic groups, and not just nations). At the root of our understanding of prejudice is the
very goal of “tolerance.” In fact, the notion of tolerance for diversity may be limited: It is often treated merely as “the application of the same
moral principles and rules, caring and empathy, and feelings of connections to human beings of other perceived groups” (Baldwin & Hecht, 1995,
p. 65). That is, it is similar to Bennett’s (1993) notion of acceptance, of respect for difference, though that respect sometimes (a) occurs at a
difference and (b) sometimes exists in behavioral form only, but is not internalized. Communication of tolerance is a worthwhile pursuit in our
behavior and research; however, we argue that we can go beyond tolerance to appreciation—even to the behavioral and attitudinal integration of
elements of the other culture (Hecht & Baldwin, 1998). There is a danger of such appreciation, as borrowing (e.g., “cultural hybridity”) occurs
within power relations. We are not talking about a dominant group borrowing from subordinate or subaltern groups in a colonizing or folklorizing
way, but about cultural learning and dialogue.

Limited Perspectives of Prejudice


That consideration of tolerance/prejudice should be treated as a dichotomy or a range is only one of the difficulties that has haunted the study and
conceptualization of prejudice. Debates have swirled around the nature of prejudice, the causes of prejudice, and the “locus” of certain prejudices
(such as racism or sexism), among other things. Allport (1979) suggests that prejudice is a “generalized” attitude—that if one is prejudiced, say,
toward Jewish people, she or he will also be prejudiced toward communists, people of color, and so on. It is possible, however, that one might be
prejudiced toward some groups, even in some contexts, but not toward other groups (Baldwin & Hecht, 1995).

The nature of prejudice


Allport (1979) defines prejudice as “an avertive [i.e., avoiding] or hostile attitude toward a person who belongs to a group, simply because he [or
she] belongs to that group, and is therefore presumed to have the objectionable qualities ascribed to the group” (p. 7). By this definition, prejudice
is an aspect of affect, or feeling toward a group, though it is closely related to cognitions, or thoughts about the group, referring to stereotypes.

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 4/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

Also, prejudice is inherently negative, following the primary definition common in modern dictionaries, though a secondary definition includes
any sort of prejudgment based on group belonging, such as prejudice toward one’s own group. Most dictionary definitions follow the attitudinal
approach, though in common usage, people often use the term to refer to things like racism, which carry behavioral and even policy implications
that are not strictly attitudes. By strictest definition, prejudice is an attitude that favors one group over another, based on or related to cognitions,
and both leading to and influenced by behaviors (including communication), texts (e.g., media, rhetoric), and policies (following the notion of
structuration, in which social structures guide social behavior, but social behavior in turn creates and changes social structures).

Causes of prejudice
Allport (1979) recognized a series of influences that impact a particular incident of prejudice, such as police brutality based on racial group/social
class divisions or anti-Islamic bullying in secondary schools around the Western world. These include historical, sociological, situational,
psychodynamic, and phenomenological (i.e., perceptual) influences. But ultimately, for Allport, a social psychologist, prejudice is “a problem of
personality formation and development” (p. 41). For Althusser (1971), a Marxist philosopher, prejudice would likely, in the last instance, be an
issue of economic and social class considerations. Ultimately, a cross-disciplinary perspective is more useful for understanding a complex
phenomenon like prejudice (Hecht & Baldwin, 1998). A broader consideration should consider multiple causes (Baldwin, 1998), including
evolutionary causes, psychological causes (both psychodynamic and perceptual), sociological causes, and rhetorical causes. Communication and
behavior become central in each of these causes, highlighting the need for a communicative understanding of prejudice.

Evolutionary causes, often referred to under the rubric of sociobiology, focus on the way in which prejudice might be an inherited trait, possibly
even genetic (see, e.g., essays in Reynolds, Falger, & Vine, 1987). This approach includes the idea that groups seek to preserve themselves (e.g.,
by preservation of a supposedly pure gene pool or because of fear of the stranger), the ethnocentrism already noted. Behaviors that exclude have a
sense of “naturalness” in that they help a group to survive, and such exclusion of strangers may help to preserve a group’s existence. Some
scholars have criticized this approach as a rationale for conservative politics that create a notion of “us” and “them” as natural and that exclude the
other, often in racial or religious terms, in order to preserve the way of life of a dominant group within a culture or nation.

Psychological explanations of prejudice fall into at least two major divisions. The first, psychodynamic, suggests that prejudice serves as a
mechanism for individuals to meet psychological needs. Thus researchers have long linked it to things such as ambivalence toward parents, rigid
personality structure, and a need for authority (Allport, 1979; Adorno et al., 1950). We see this indirectly through Kenneth Burke’s (1967)
approach to rhetoric in his analysis of Hitler’s campaign against Jewish people as a means to divert negative emotions related to economic and
political difficulties from the mainstream German people to Jews, and in Edward Said’s (2003) Orientalism, which notes how Medieval Europe
cast negative images of lust and vice on Middle Easterners that the Europeans did not see in themselves.

A second aspect of the psychological approach concerns perception or cognition. This contains a range of possible influences on prejudice,
including such things as selective attention, perception, and recall of the negative behavior of outgroup members, or the notion of attributional
biases that impact how we give meanings to the behavior of those of our ingroup and those of outgroups. At the center of many of these
explanations is the notion of categorization of people (i.e., dividing them into cognitive groups such as ingroups and outgroups). Social identity
theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1986) suggests that we cannot think of ourselves apart from the groups to which we belong; we engage in intergroup
comparison as a means to make us feel better about our group; and, if our group does not compare well to a group we admire or must rely on in
some way—often the dominant group—we engage in strategies to reclaim a sense of pride for our group or distance ourselves from it.

Categorization, in social identity theory, is not a form of prejudice—it is simply the mental placing of people (or things, actions, characteristics,
etc.) into mental boxes. However, those boxes are closely related to the stereotypes that cling to groups. Stereotypes are overgeneralizations we
make about groups that we apply to individuals in those groups (Herbst, 1997). Although these stereotypes provide a mental shortcut for
processing information about others, they interfere with our encoding, storage, and recall of information about members of our own group and
other groups (Stephan, 1985). Countless studies of stereotypes suggest that stereotypes, like ethnocentrism, can serve positive ingroup functions,
that they sometimes have at least some basis in an actual behavior or custom (a “kernel of truth”), and that we stereotype both our own group and
other groups. Devine (e.g., Devine & Sharp, 2009) has found that even people who report lower prejudice, if mentally occupied, still rely on
stereotypes, suggesting that everyone is aware of societal stereotypes toward certain groups (e.g., the elderly, athletes, the deaf). It is likely that if
we are on auto-pilot or in a state of mindlessness, we will resort to stereotypes. But individuating people (i.e., taking them out of the group we
perceive them to be in and treating them as individuals; Dovidio, Gaertner, & Kawakami, 2003) may require deliberate cognitive effort.

Group-based, or sociological, approaches, like psychological approaches, are varied. These include Marxist approaches, which are themselves
varied in form (see various essays in Rex & Mason, 1986). Some hold tightly to a “vulgar” vision of Marxism, framing intolerance like racism as
a creation of the elite to divide the working classes and distract them from revolution through “false consciousness.” Few Marxists take such a
severe approach, choosing to see looser relations between capital and the construction of intolerance, but in the “last instance,” seeing intolerance
as linked to social class and economic systems. “Capitalism, colonialism, and patriarchal social systems are frequently identified as producing
inherent race and gender inequalities which, in various ways, serve the needs of the systems they perpetuate” (Knowles & Mercer, 1992, p. 110).
Weberian approaches see a wider variety of classes than workers and elite, with prejudice linked not just to labor forces but to the struggle over

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 5/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

goods, services, and prestige (Gerth & Mills, 1946). Other group-based factors also impact prejudice, such as perceived group competition for
jobs and resources in times of economic upheaval (e.g., the 1970s oil crisis in the United States), known as realistic group conflict (Bobo, 1983);
immigration reasons (refugees versus those seeking economic opportunity, patterns of settlement; Omi & Winant, 1986); and historically
developed class statuses between groups that link immigrants or members of a minority group to a certain class (Wilson, 1978), such as the
Gastarbeiter (guest-worker) Turks in Germany or the Algerian-descended French.

In a classic “chicken-egg” argument about which came first, it is fruitless to debate whether psychology leads to sociological causes or vice versa,
and, in turn, whether these lead to the communicative expression of intolerance, or whether it is the communicative construction of group
identities and intolerance that creates the attitudes (Ruscher, 2001). It is more likely that mental structures and communicative practices co-create
each other, through forms we shall examine in more detail. One possible metaphor for understanding these influences, the impact of historical
situations (such as the longstanding antipathy between Turkish and Greek Cypriots, Broome, 2005), and specific incidents (such as the attack on
the World Trade Towers in New York City in 2001), is as layers building upon one other, or even as a hologram, in which we can imperfectly see
some semblance of a complex prejudice through a single image—an experimental study on racial perceptions and media use, an analysis of an
anti-Irish speech or a pro-nationalist song, or interviews with women who are victims of catcalling (Hecht & Baldwin, 1998). But, as a complete
hologram provides the most faithful image, the most complete view of an intolerance will come through multiple views (e.g., disciplines), using
multiple methods.

Sign In
To
Racism: A Case Study in Prejudice Annotate

Racism as a specific type of prejudice is one of the most hotly discussed and debated sites of intolerance in contemporary times in the United
States and beyond. Even countries that once imagined themselves as “racial democracies” in which racially different people lived side by side
(like Brazil) are now admitting the harsh reality of entrenched and historic racism. Even though many there argue that class, not race, is the
primary social distinction, as racism has become officially illegal, forms of overt racism, from social media to abuse and killing of unarmed
blacks by police continue to receive recent focus in U.S. news.

Racism is a form of intolerance that is based on the supposedly biological distinction of race, but many authors today argue that race is a social
construct, sometimes defined differently from country to country and even over time within a single country. Different authors have outlined the
history of the notion of race in the English language, noting that at different times, it has referred to an ancestral clan (the race of Abraham), to
supposed biological differences, and, more recently, to culture (Banton, 1987; Omi & Winant, 1986). Those who see a biological component
cannot agree on how many races there are and, historically, politics and rhetoric have done as much to construct who belongs in a particular race
as biology (e.g., in the early U.S., the Irish were considered “colored”). In the United States, race was based on racist assumptions, on one having
even a small degree of colored blood in one’s ancestral lineage; in other cultures, race is based strictly on physiological features, regardless of
lineage. Ethnicity, in contrast, is related more to the cultural origins of one’s background or ancestry, sometimes linked to a specific time and
place. To emphasize its social constructedness, many authors bracket “race” with quotation marks.

Who Can Be Racist? The Locus of Racism (and Other Intolerances)

Can minority members be “racist”?


Beyond the nature of race itself, researchers and educators debate the very nature of racism. Some contend that racism is an intolerance based on
the construction of race that is perpetrated and held by the support of the dominant system. For example, Malott and Schaefle (2015) define
racism as “a system of oppression, whereby persons of a dominant racial group (whites in the United States) exercise power or privilege over
those in nondominant groups” (p. 361). According to this argument, only whites can be racist in a white-dominated system (whether that
dominance is by numbers or in political and social power). Others contend that racism is any system of beliefs—“held consciously or
otherwise”—that treats members of a group that is different on supposedly biological grounds as “biologically different than one’s own” (Herbst,
1997, p. 193). By this definition, anyone who sees another race group as inferior would be racist.

The locus of racism: Individual or structural?


This distinction in racism also applies to definitions of sexism or to the delineation between homophobia as a personal dislike or fear of LGBT
individuals and heterosexism as a social structure that reinforces prejudice against them (Nakayama, 1998). The debate is similar to the
definitional debate of prejudice in general—is it something that is strictly an individual trait, or is it something that is socially built into the
structures of society—the laws, the media, the educational system, the church, and so on? Associated with this question is the nature of what
racism is: The “individual-level” definition treats racism as a system of beliefs (i.e., a psychological construct), and the other treats it as a system
of oppression that goes beyond individual psyche and personality to consider racism embedded within social structures. The question of where we
see racism (and other intolerances) is vitally important. Those who see racism and other intolerances as primarily individual-level (stereotypes,

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 6/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

personal dislikes, etc.) tend to address intolerance through training and educational programs in organizations and schools; those who see it as
systemic believe that such approaches ignore larger issues of policy, law, segregation, discrimination, and media/rhetoric that produce and
reproduce racist beliefs or create an environment that makes them grow. We see this tension, for example, in Rattansi’s (1992) discussion of the
debate between multicultural education—an educational solution to tolerance focused on educating about differences—and antiracism, which
addresses political and social structures that propagate and support racism.

Racism: Defined by intent or result?


A related definitional distinction regarding racism concerns whether an intent of harm or exclusion is necessary to define thoughts or actions as
racist. Miles (1989) criticizes earlier notions of racism, largely in that they re-inscribe the notion of race as if it were a concrete reality rather than
a social construction. He weaves together a new approach to racism that begins with discourses that serve to exclude the “other” (based on
supposed biological differences); for Miles, “the concept of racism should refer to the function, rather than the content of the discourses” (p. 49),
allowing racism to include things that may not sound racist but still seek to exclude the other. Miles differentiates racism from racialization, the
categorization of people based on supposed biological differences. He argues against the use of racism and disagrees with a stance that would
have only whites being racist, such that “all ‘white’ people are universally and inevitably sick with racism” (p. 53), as this concept may ignore the
specifics of racism in particular countries, cultures, or circumstances; however, he notes the need to consider institutional racism—racism built
into organizational, legal, and social structures—that does favor whites in many countries. By this, one could speak of racism as something any
person could hold or express, but institutional racism would be reserved for a group that has power in a particular context. Finally, he bases
racism not on the intent of an action, but on the result. He argues that racism is an ideology, based on differentiation, that leads to “exclusionary
practices” (pp. 77–78), such as differential treatment or allocation of resources and opportunities, regardless of one’s intent or even awareness of
the ideological underpinnings of one’s actions. Goldberg (1993) argues that we should allow racism to include either intent or result.

Including resulting exclusionary practice in our definition of racism has implications for redressing or addressing racism. First, it suggests a
limitation in addressing overt racist thoughts and stereotypes only through education, as policies, laws, and social structures foster an environment
for the presence of such thoughts and their communication. Miles (1989) advocates that “strategies for eliminating racism should concentrate less
on trying exclusively to persuade those who articulate racism that they are ‘wrong’ and more on changing those particular economic and political
relations” (p. 82). A second implication is that, even as we seek to address racism through everyday interactions and social media, because racism
is such a charged topic, we will advance our cause little by calling an action, a joke, or a Facebook or Twitter posting “racist.” The poster, holding
a more traditional view of racism as intentionally harmful in some way, will deny racist intent, and a charge of racism will move the discussion
into the original communicator’s attempts to avoid the charge of racism (or sexism, etc.), rather than addressing the specific policy, image, or
statement. Instead, we might discuss and demonstrate through evidence the way that the policy or image excludes others based on race. Without
invoking the “r-word,” we may have a better chance at engaging in dialogues about policies, laws, and communicative behaviors that exclude
others.

Intersectionalities of Racism
As we have begun to notice, one thing that complicates the concept of racism is its overlap with other terms, such as prejudice (with racism being
a subset of prejudice). So, although xenophobia and ethnocentrism are distinct and separate from racism, the “other” within these concepts is
often articulated or perceived in terms of race. A focus on racism and antiracism, unfortunately, often excludes other bases of intolerance that may
be even more prominent within a given area, such as religious intolerance, sexism, or heterosexism. At the same time, it is useful to see how
racism intersects with and sometimes leads to other intolerances, all of which have received much thought in recent years.

Feminism
In some cases, feminists and antiracists have been at odds, proponents of each claiming that their sphere of oppression is the one that merits the
most attention. Feminism is defined as “the belief that men and women are equal and should have equal respect and opportunities in all spheres of
life—personal, social, work, and public” (Wood, 2008, p. 324). Feminist communication research seeks to make the voices of women heard, to
highlight their experiences within the social construction of gender, and “their experiences of oppression and of coping with and resisting that
oppression” (Foss & Foss, 1994, p. 39). Recent feminists consider how patriarchy, or male power or hegemony over the realities and voices of
women, is not something maintained only by men nor is it deliberate. Rather, it is held in place by systems often beyond the awareness of men
and women, and consented to and participated in by women themselves (Zompetti, 2012). Each of these ideas could also apply to racism,
revealing a similarity between sexism and racism. But racism and sexism are also joined in the experiences of women of color, whose specific life
situations are not fully addressed by either antiracist efforts or feminism. Collins (1990), for example, argues that African American women in the
United States live in a site of triple oppression—by race, sex, and class, with these oppressions articulated by both the dominant white community
and within the black community.

Queer theory

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 7/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

Queer theory seeks to challenge the way in which society passes on heterosexuality as the norm. Warner (1991) sees oppression of gays and
lesbians in every aspect of society and in “a wide range of institutions and ideology” (p. 5). But even more so, he feels that the academy’s silence
regarding oppression of sexual identity participates in that oppression. Chávez (2013) supports this claim, noting that at the writing of her article,
no major journal in the National Communication Association had devoted a full issue to queer studies. Again, recent scholars have been looking
at the intersection of race and sexual orientation (Yep, 2013), such as the representations and experiences of older gay male adults, Latina
lesbians, and transgender blacks.

Whiteness studies
Based on the early writings of Richard Dyer (1997) and Ruth Frankenberg (1993), researchers have highlighted the notion of whiteness—a hidden
system of ideology and social structure that maintains whites in a position of advantage—but one that is often invisible to, and yet defended by,
whites (Wander, Nakayama, & Martin, 1999). Whiteness studies call attention to areas of white privilege. “By exposing the ‘invisibility’ of
whiteness, the study of whiteness helps us understand the way that white domination continues” (p. 22). A current search for “whiteness” in a
communication library search engine reveals over 800 articles on the topic. Many of these are media studies on how whiteness is promoted and/or
challenged in a wide variety of texts, including South Park, the Rush Hour movies, The Hunger Games, and Glee. But whiteness is also analyzed
in areas of education, everyday language, and health and organizational communication, as well as in many different countries.

Orientalism/postcolonialism
whiteness studies owe part of their heritage to postcolonialism, which has its own roots in the conceptualization of Orientalism by Edward Said
(2003). Said analyzes European art and literature to reveal the construction of the Arab or Middle Easterner as “other.” He notes how the Western
ideology of the East (referring to the Middle East) folklorizes and sexualizes Middle Easterners, treating them as backward, in a way that justifies
European colonization and paternalism. Thousands of books now deal in some way with Orientalism, and Said’s notion of the “other” has become
a stock theme in how we consider the racial other. For example, though not framed explicitly in Orientalism, James Baldwin’s famous 1955 essay
“Stranger in the Village” talks about the rage of the black man as he confronts white America and the naiveté of whites—a naiveté that they work
hard to preserve (thus relating Baldwin’s ideas to whiteness). When whites arrive in Africa, blacks are astonished:

The white man takes the astonishment as tribute, for he arrives to conquer and to convert the natives, whose inferiority in relation to
himself is not even to be questioned; whereas I, without a thought of conquest, find myself among a people whose culture controls me,
has even, in a sense, created me, people who have cost me more in anguish and rage than they will ever know, who yet do not even
know of my existence … The rage of the disesteemed is personally fruitless, but it is also absolutely inevitable: the rage, so generally
discounted, so little understood even among the people whose daily bread it is, is one of the things that makes history.

Postcolonialism, building upon Orientalism, considers all locations where one nation or people group has colonized another group, considering
the cultural, political, and social ramifications of that colonization and seeking to remedy social ills that it has brought about. Shome and Hegde
(2002) call the approach “interventionist and highly political” (p. 250). Postcolonialism notes how much of the world is forced to work within
thought systems created by the Western world (an effect only magnified through the rise of the internet and globalization). Postcolonial writers are
often interested in issues such as migration of people groups (including diasporic groups); the hybrid (but power-laden) mixture of ideas, artifacts,
and behaviors between cultures; the liminal spaces between cultures; and the imperialism of ideas (Bhabha, 1994). Thus, postcolonialism is
inherently about prejudice and oppression beyond racism, though it also has links to racism specifically, as authors consider the ways that some
have used racial categories to colonize others (e.g., see essays in Nakayama & Halualani, 2010).

Discrimination: Considering the Form(s) of Intolerance


As we have seen, it is difficult to discuss prejudice in general or racism specifically without moving into issues of institutionalized prejudice,
media representations, school and government policies, and so on. In this sense, both prejudice and racism are intricately intertwined with
discrimination. Discrimination specifically refers to “behavior that denies equal treatment to people because of their membership in some group”
(Herbst, 1997, p. 185). It is based on the “beliefs, feelings, fantasies, and motivations of prejudice” (p. 185), but these mental or social concepts
are not in themselves discrimination. Discrimination involves behavior.

Institutional Discrimination
When we think of institutional-level discrimination, many examples come to mind. These include things like not allowing certain groups housing
or refusing other privileges, resources, or opportunities to them. At the writing of this chapter, a popular U.S. media topic is the county clerk, Kim
Davis, who refused to give marriage licenses to gays or lesbians based on her faith, despite a state law that allowed her to do so. The Jim Crowe
laws of the United States, which gave unequal educational and public access rights to blacks and whites is a classic example, with many facilities

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 8/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

being for “whites only.” The website Global Issues (Shah, 2010) details instances of racism and racial discrimination around the world, such as
racism against white farmers in Zimbabwe and discrimination against the Dalits—the “untouchables” in India.

Genocide and ethnic cleansing


At the extreme end of discrimination, we have genocide and ethnic cleansing. For example, around 1915, the Ottomon (Turkish) empire
slaughtered 1.5 million Armenians (75% of the Turkish Armenian population). The Turkish government took Armenian (largely Christian)
children and converted them, giving them to Islamic families. Even today, Turkey defends this “Turkification” of Turkey as a necessary act of war
and has resisted the U.S. and other nations defining it as genocide (Armenian genocide, . .). Other genocides have occurred in Central Europe
(the Holocaust) in the 1930s–1940s, Rwanda in 2003, Cambodia in the 1970s, and the Greek/Pontic genocide of World War I. Extreme
discrimination includes hate crimes and overt hate groups. The introduction of this chapter noted the prevalence of hate crimes and hate groups
within the United States and other nations.

Redlining and racial profiling


In many countries, overt forms of discrimination for many (but seldom all) groups have been outlawed. Institutional discrimination itself may take
forms that are harder to name and prove, such as redlining, the process by which banks give fewer mortgages to people of color, based on the
belief that they are less able to repay loans. Some real estate agents may steer people of color away from rentals in upscale neighborhoods; school
advisers may tell people of color that their children are more suited for trade school rather than college or graduate school. In the United States in
2014–2015, there was a spate of cases surrounding potential police brutality against unarmed black men, leading to the “Black Lives Matter”
movement. There is also racial profiling, such as when police pay more attention to people of color, stopping and/or searching them more
frequently than they do whites (what some people of color call “DWB” or “driving while black”). A growing and complex array of academic
studies examine whether or not profiling exists and, if so, what its nature is (e.g., is it pro-white, or does it depend on the race of the officer?). A
similar phenomenon experienced by many people of color is being followed through stores by security guards, regardless of their attire or
appearance. Notably, some aspects of discrimination, such as redlining, might be done, at least in the minds of the banker, real estate agent, or
high school counselor, without a notion of racial discrimination; but here, Miles’s (1989) notion of racism defined by exclusionary outcome would
classify the behaviors as racist, as they exclude based on supposed biological differences.

Intolerant Communication

Redneck racism/prejudice
Central to our discussion is the way that discrimination and racism can occur through communicative behavior. Brislin (1991) outlined several
forms of discriminatory communication. In addition to hate crimes and ethnic cleansing, he mentions redneck racism—the expression of blatant
intolerance toward someone of another race. He applies these categories to racism, but we can apply them to any group. These might include
jokes, statements (e.g., about the inferiority or backwardness of a group), or slurs or names for people of another group (also called
ethnophaulisms). Conventional wisdom, for example, suggests that there are many more slurs for women then there are for men, and most of
these have some sexual connotation.

Sometimes, the intolerance is slightly veiled though still present, as when we resort to “us/them” language or talk to someone from another group
about “your people.” Brislin’s (1991) notion of arm’s-length prejudice occurs when someone voices tolerance for a group, typically of being
accepting of them in the neighborhood or workplace, but wants to restrict them from closer relationships, such as marrying a family member
(related to Bogardus’s notion of social distance; Allport, 1979). Prejudice might manifest in statements like “She’s very smart for an ‘X’” or “I
have a friend who is a ‘Y,’ and he is very articulate,” since such statements assume that most Xs are not smart and most Ys are not articulate.

Prejudiced colloquialisms
Prejudice also manifests in our use of colloquialisms that play upon a particular aspect of identity or ability, such as calling something “lame” or
“retarded.” Both the harm and use of such phrases has been established. For example, one study found that hearing the phrase “That’s so gay”
made gays and lesbians feel less accepted in the university setting and, to a lesser degree, increased reported health problems. Over 45% of the
participants had heard the word “gay” linked to something “stupid or undesirable” (Hall & LaFrance, 2012, p. 430) ten or more times within the
last year. Hall and LaFrance (2012) find a complex interplay between identity—males’ endorsement of gender identity norms andthe desire to
distance themselves from homosexuality, as well as the social norms around them, and their likelihood to use the expression.

Prejudice built into language


We might well say that intolerance can be embedded in every level of language. In one classic study, men interrupted women much more than
women interrupted men. If women overlapped men, men continued their turn speaking, but if men interrupted women, women yielded their turn
speaking (Zimmerman & West, 1975). Coates’s (2003) analysis of narratives told by men in mixed company (such as around the family dinner

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 9/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

table) notes that men are both the target and subject of most stories, with dinner table discussion typically centering on patriarchal authority.
Research has explored prejudice through verbal and nonverbal behaviors toward people of different ages, people with disabilities, people with
different languages or dialects, and other groups, including much theory and research on how we adjust or do not adjust our behavior toward those
we perceive to be of different groups (communication accommodation theory; Gallois, Ogay, & Giles, 2005) or how minority members must
negotiate their communication with dominant group members because of contexts of power and prejudice (co-cultural theory; Orbe & Spellers,
2005).

Bar-Tal (1990) and Zur (1991) note the way that we use rhetoric to create a sense of others (i.e., to create the identity of the enemy in a way that
then justifies discrimination) resonates with Burke’s (1967) analysis of Hitler’s rhetorical construction of the Jewish people. Collins and Clément
(2012), summarizing research from a special 2007 issue of Journal of Language and Social Psychology on language and discrimination up to the
present, summarize the role of language as it pertains to prejudice:

Language is the primary means through which prejudice can be explicitly and implicitly communicated and is, therefore, a major
contributor to its transmission and maintenance. But language can also play a more rooted and integral role in prejudice: changing
perceptions by distorting the information it carries, focusing attention on social identities, and being a factor in the definition of group
boundaries

(p. 389).

Intolerance gone underground: Subtle forms of prejudice


As early as the mid-1980s, authors began to argue that in Western societies, racism and other forms of intolerance were going underground (i.e.,
aware that the redneck varieties of intolerance were socially unacceptable, people expressed less overt intolerance but continued to show
intolerance through racism in ways that were “subtle” and “everyday”—a new and modern racism). People might express such forms of racism
(and by extension other intolerances) through nonverbal behaviors, such as placing change on the counter instead of in an outgroup member’s
hand, or through subtle sayings and word usages that exclude or put down the other person in some way that is not clearly distinguishable as
prejudice. In the new racism, minority groups are not spoken of as inferior but as “different,” “although in many respects there are ‘deficiencies,’
such as single-parent families, drug abuse, lacking achievement values, and dependence on welfare and affirmative action—‘pathologies’ that
need to be corrected” (van Dijk, 2000, p. 34). Today, researchers and social activists refer to these subtle manifestations of prejudice as
microagressions.

Symbolic racism is similar to subtle racism (Sears & Henry, 2005), though it relates more to political attitudes. Researchers have framed symbolic
racism to include elements of anti-black sentiment hidden by political attitudes (e.g., that affirmative action has gone too far, that blacks are
demanding too much; McConahay, 1986). Political research has a corollary in communication in that often, as whites talk about economic or
political issues, there is at least a mental if not an explicit verbal coding of race or ethnic “othering.” International ownership of business becomes
an issue when Japanese or Chinese companies start buying U.S. businesses, regardless of the large and long-term Dutch and English business
holdings in America; discussions about welfare, gangs, and urban decay are often subtly about race. Similar verbal coding may also hold true with
other identity groups.

Finally, in terms of face-to-face communication, researchers have explored the notion of “benevolent” intolerance. Discussions of things such as
benevolent racism or sexism are often based on a larger notion of benevolent domination, whereby one nation or group seeks to dominate another,
supposedly in its best interests (based on Rudyard Kipling’s notion of the “white man’s burden”). For example, Esposito and Romano (2014)
contrast benevolent racism to other forms of post-U.S.-civil-rights forms of racism, such as laissez-faire racism, symbolic racism, and color-blind
racism. Each might oppose affirmative action, for example, but for different reasons. Laissez-faire would oppose it based on ideas of meritocracy
and free enterprise, blaming blacks themselves for lack of economic progress. Symbolic racism would hold that “the United States is a fair and
equitable society where everyone has ample opportunity to succeed through hard work and talent” (p. 74), and that blacks who use the “race card”
are hypersensitive—they are “too pushy, too demanding, too angry” (McConahay & Hough, 1976, p. 38). Color-blind racism starts with what
seems to be a reasonable assumption, that all people are the same, but then moves to assume that lack of progress of minority members is due to
their personal choices, low work ethic, or lack of ability, and ignores structural support for inequalities.

Benevolent racism has a long history, even into slavery, a time in which some whites felt they were doing blacks a favor by controlling them and
“providing” for them. More recently, it involves a seemingly positive attitude toward blacks that then opposes any social reforms like affirmative
action as belittling blacks and working against their natural progress as citizens (Esposito & Romano, 2014). Benevolent sexism holds the same
basic idea: Rather than sexism being based on anti-woman attitudes, it can also be supported by putting women “on a pedestal,” characterizing
them as “pure creatures who ought to be protected, supported, and adored, and whose love is necessary to make a man complete” (Glick & Fiske,
2001, p. 109). Extensive research has linked such benevolent ideas about women to negative outcomes for them.

Intolerance in the media and on the internet

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 10/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

Finally, many volumes have been written on the issues of stereotypes and intolerance in the media. This includes both social scientific work, such
as the cultivation theory research that analyzes both representation of minorities in the media in different countries and the research that considers
the effects of such representation. It also includes a wide array of critical and cultural analyses from the cultural studies school. Many of these
analyses use the principles discussed—feminism, postcolonialism, critical race theory, whiteness, and so on. They work to demonstrate how the
media systematically ignore, oversimplify, or negatively represent particular groups. One line of research in this field is the focus on the symbolic
annihilation of race (Coleman & Chivers Yochim, 2008), which notes how, unlike stereotypes in the media that focus on the presence of some
characteristic associated with a group, symbolic annihilation also considers “the meanings associated with absence, omission, or even inclusion
that is not so obviously problematic (negative)” (p. 2), in terms of what such absences and seemingly benign images mean.

With the growth of the internet and video gaming, a final area of importance in understanding, researching, and working against prejudice
includes all new media. The internet gives impetus for new research to understand hate groups on the media, flaming (e.g., in comments on video-
hosting websites such as YouTube), and social media. We see examples of the use of social media for racist purposes in the flurry of racist twitters
that followed the crowning of Nina Davuluri, an American of East Indian descent. Research considers both the presence of stereotypes in such
media, as well as their effects.

The potential of communication


Unlike some early critical writers, who felt that media imagery (including new media) only produce and reproduce prevailing (prejudiced)
ideologies, we must also consider the potential of face-to-face, mediated, and new media as places to challenge oppression. In terms of face-to-
face communication, we can work through education to dispel stereotypes. That education can be simply on cultural differences and
accomplishments, though changing cognitions alone may not change deeply felt affective prejudice, and only time (as more tolerant individuals
assume positions of leadership) will lead to changes in discriminatory social structures. This is why some advocate for political education that
addresses both personal and structural prejudice more directly, as well as political action and intervention in media systems.

Many scholars represent interpersonal contact as one of the best ways to address prejudice. Contact theory holds great potential for the planning
of interventions to reduce intergroup tensions, as it describes how interpersonal contact with people from outgroups under the right conditions can
work by changing both attitudes and affect, especially if people can see the other person as both a member of a new group while still recognizing
their original group identity (Dovidio et al., 2003). Thomas Pettigrew (2016) outlines the history of research on authoritarianism (the desire and
support for strong authority structures) and relative deprivation (the feeling that one’s group is disadvantaged in comparison to another group) as
two of the main predictors of intergroup prejudice. He notes how, while personality factors like authoritarianism and cognitive rigidity are related
to greater intolerance and make the likelihood of meaningful intergroup contact more unlikely, even in the presence of these variables, contact
programs can have a positive effect for people with prejudice A meta-analysis of 515 contact studies suggests that contact works specifically by
increasing knowledge of the other group, decreasing anxiety when one is with the other group, and increasing empathy for the other group
(Pettigrew & Tropp, 2008).

In terms of media, we see both a growth in the production of media that challenges and resists stereotypes, rigid gender constructions, and so on,
as well as a growth of grassroots efforts to highlight such oppression. One such effort is the website Fat, Ugly, or Slutty, a site composed of posts
contributed by women who are stereotyped or verbally assaulted by men in video gaming websites, usually when the women have beaten them.
The women are able to post comments made by other players, their own avatars, and even videos that the men sometimes send them. Efforts like
these highlight forms of oppression that occur throughout the internet, but they also highlight the potential of the internet for addressing these
forms of oppression in creative ways.

Conclusions
We have seen throughout this article that culture, prejudice, racism, and discrimination are related in complicated ways. Some people even see the
characteristics of a particular culture (e.g., mainstream America’s conception of male and female beauty, the definition of a “good” education, or
the focus on individualism) as negotiated between people with economic and power interests. Cultures (using the term much more widely than
“nation”) are always ethnocentric, with individuals sometimes being xenophobic. But these forms of intolerance are frequently linked to other
forms of intolerance—religious, racial, ethnic, and otherwise. Prejudice, most technically, is an affect—a desire to avoid someone because of her
or his group, as opposed to stereotypes, which are more cognitive associations with a group—and efforts to reduce prejudice should focus on both
affect and cognition. But intolerance is also clearly linked to higher-order manifestations of prejudice, such as discrimination through legal and
organizational policies, symbolic annihilation of groups in the media, and everyday forms of discrimination, be they overt or subtle. More likely,
communicative and policy forms of prejudice (and their manifest effects in terms of housing, education, job opportunities, and so on) “create”
prejudicial perceptions, which in turn create the conditions of discrimination. Racism serves as an example—but only one of many—of the links
among attitude, communicative action, policy, and social structure. With this complex view in mind, we can see that any attempts to redress or

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 11/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

ameliorate racism or any other intolerance must include not only education, or even merely a wide array of communicative responses (media and
face-to-face), but also efforts at addressing social inequalities at the structural and policy levels.

Further Reading
Chin, J. L. (Ed.). (2010). The psychology of prejudice and discrimination. Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.
Find this resource:

Crenshaw, K. W., Gotanda, N., Pellar, G., & Thomas, K. (Eds.). (1995). Critical race theory: The key writings that formed the movement. New
York: New Press.
Find this resource:

Delgado, R., & Stefanic, J. (Eds.). (1997). Critical White studies: A look behind the mirror. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Find this resource:

Dovidio, J. F. (Ed.). (2010). The SAGE handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination. London: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Fishbein, H. D. (2002). Peer prejudice and discrimination: The origins of prejudice. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Find this resource:

Giles, H., & Watson, B. (Eds.). (2013). The social meanings of language, dialect, and accent: International perspectives on speech styles. New
York: Peter Lang.
Find this resource:

Gioseffi, D. (Ed.). (1993). On prejudice: A global perspective. New York: Anchor.


Find this resource:

Goldberg, D. T. (1993). Racist culture: Philosophy and the politics of meaning. Oxford: Blackwell.
Find this resource:

Harwood, J., & Giles, H. (Eds.). (2005). Intergroup communication: Multiple perspectives. New York: Peter Lang.
Find this resource:

Hecht, M. L. (Ed.). (1998). Communicating prejudice. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.


Find this resource:

Nakayama, T. K., & Martin, J. N. (Eds.). (1999). Whiteness: The communication of social identity. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Nelson, T. (Ed.). (2009). Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination. New York: Psychology Press.
Find this resource:

van Dijk, T. A. (1987). Communicating racism: Ethnic prejudice in thought and talk. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Whillock, R. K., & Slayden, D. (Eds.). (1995). Hate speech. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Yūki, M., & Brewer, M. (Eds.). (2013). Culture and group processes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Find this resource:

References
Adorno, T. W. Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford R. N., with B. Aron, M. H. Levinson, & W. Morrow (Eds.). (1950). The
authoritarian personality. New York: Harper.
Find this resource:

Allport, G. (1979). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.


Find this resource:

Althusser, L. (1971). Lenin philosophy and other essays B. Brewster (Trans.). London: NLB.

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 12/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

Find this resource:

Armenian genocide (n.d.). History.

Auestad, L. (Ed.). (2013). Nationalism and the body politic: Psychoanalysis and the rise of ethnocentrism and xenophobia. London: Karnac
Books.
Find this resource:

Baldwin, J. (1955). Stranger in the village. From Notes of a Native Son (pp. 159–75). (1984 [1955]). Boston: Beacon Press.
Find this resource:

Baldwin, J. R. (1998). Tolerance/intolerance: A historical and multi-disciplinary view of prejudice. In M. L. Hecht (Ed.), Communicating
prejudice (pp. 24–56). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Baldwin, J. R., Coleman, R. R. M., González, A., & Shenoy-Packer, S. (2014). Intercultural communication for everyday life. Malden, MA:
Wiley-Blackwell.
Find this resource:

Baldwin, J. R., Faulkner, S. L., & Hecht, M. L. (2006). A moving target: The illusive definition of culture. In J. R. Baldwin, S. L. Faulkner, M. L.
Hecht, & S. L. Lindsley (Eds.), Redefining culture: Perspectives across the disciplines (pp. 3–26). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Find this resource:

Baldwin, J. R., Faulkner, S. L., Hecht, M. L., & Lindsley, S. L. (Eds.). (2006). Redefining culture: Perspectives across the disciplines. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Find this resource:

Baldwin, J. R., & Hecht, M. L. (1995). The layered perspective of cultural (in)tolerance(s): The roots of a multidisciplinary approach. In R.
Wiseman (Ed.), Intercultural communication theory (pp. 59–91). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Banton, M. (1987). Racial theories. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.


Find this resource:

Bar-Tal, D. (1990). Causes and consequences of deligimitization: Models of conflict and ethnocentrism. Journal of Social Issues, 46, 65–81.
Find this resource:

Bennett, M. J. (1993). Towards ethnorelativism: A developmental model of intercultural sensitivity. In R. M. Paige (Ed.), Education for the
intercultural experience (pp. 21–71). Yarmouth, ME: Intercultural Press.
Find this resource:

Bhabha, H. (1994). The location of culture. London: Routledge.


Find this resource:

Bobo, L. (1983). Whites’ opposition to busing: Symbolic racism or realistic group conflict. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45,
1196–1210.
Find this resource:

Brislin, R. W. (1991). Prejudice in intercultural communication. In L. A. Samovar & R. E. Porter (Eds.), Intercultural communication: A reader
(6th ed., pp. 366–370). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Find this resource:

Broome, B. J. (2005). Building bridges across the Green Line: A guide to intercultural communication in Cyprus. Nicosia, CY: United Nations
Bicommunal Development Program.
Find this resource:

Burke, K. (1967). The philosophy of literary form: Studies in symbolic action (2d ed.). Baton Rouge: Louisiana State University Press.
Find this resource:

Chávez, K. R. (2013). Pushing boundaries: Queer intercultural communication. Journal of International and Intercultural Communication, 6,
83–95.
Find this resource:

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 13/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

Coates, J. (2003). Men talk. Malden, MA: Oxford University Press.


Find this resource:

Coleman, R. M., & Chivers Yochim, E. (2008). The symbolic annihilation of race: A review of the “Blackness” literature. African American
Research Perspectives, 12, 1–10.
Find this resource:

Collins, K. A., & Clément, R. (2012). Language and prejudice: Direct and moderated effects. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 31,
376–396.
Find this resource:

Collins, P. H. (1990). Black feminist thought: Knowledge, consciousness, and the politics of empowerment. Boston: Unwin Hyman.
Find this resource:

Devine, P., G., & Sharp, L. B., (2009). Automaticity and control in stereotyping and prejudice. In T. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice,
stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 61–87). New York: Psychology Press.
Find this resource:

Donald, J., & Rattansi, A. (1992). Introduction. In J. Donald & A. Rattansi (Eds.), “Race,” culture and difference (pp. 1–8). London: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., & Kawakami, K. (2003). Intergroup contact: The past, present, and future. Group Processes & Intergroup
Relations, 6(1), 5–21.
Find this resource:

Dyer, R. (1997). White. New York: Routledge.


Find this resource:

Esposito, L., & Romano, B. (2014). Benevolent racism: Upholding racial inequality in the name of Black empowerment. Western Journal of
Black Studies, 38(2), 69–83.
Find this resource:

FBI. (2014). Latest hate crime statistics report released. FBI.

Foss, K. A., & Foss, S. K. (1994). Personal experience as evidence in feminist scholarship. Western Journal of Communication, 58, 39–43.
Find this resource:

Frankenberg, R. (1993). White women, race matters: The social construction of Whiteness. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Find this resource:

Gallois, C., Ogay, T., & Giles, H. (2005). Communication accommodation theory. In W. B. Gudykunst (Ed.), Theorizing about intercultural
communication (pp. 121–148). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Gerth, H. H., & Mills, C. W. (Eds.). (1946). From Max Weber: Essays in sociology. New York: Oxford University Press.
Find this resource:

Glick, P., & Fiske, S. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and benevolent sexism as complementary justifications for gender inequality.
American Psychologist, 56, 109–118.
Find this resource:

Hall, J., & LaFrance, B. (2012). “That’s gay”: Sexual prejudice, gender identity, norms, and homophobic communication. Communication
Quarterly, 60, 35–58.
Find this resource:

Hate and extremism. (n.d.). Southern Poverty Law Center.

Hate map. (n.d.). Southern Poverty Law Center.

Hecht, M. L., & Baldwin, J. R. (1998). Layers and holograms: A new look at prejudice. M. L. Hecht (Ed.), Communicating prejudice (pp. 57–84).
Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Find this resource:

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 14/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

Herbst, P. H. (1997). The color of words: An encyclopaedic dictionary of ethnic bias in the United States. Yarmouth, ME: Intercultural Press.
Find this resource:

Knowles, C., & Mercer, S. (1992). Feminism and anti-racism: An exploration of the possibilities. In J. Donald & A. Rattansi (Eds.), “Race,”
culture, and difference (pp. 104–125). London: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Kroeber, A. L., & Kluckhohn, C. (1952). Culture: A critical view of concepts and definitions. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Find this resource:

Leeds-Hurwitz, W. (1990). Notes in the history of intercultural communication: The Foreign Service Institute and the mandate for intercultural
training. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 76, 262–281.
Find this resource:

Malott, K. M., & Schaefle, S. (2015). Addressing clients’ experiences of racism: A model for clinical practice. Journal of Counseling and
Development, 93, 361–369.
Find this resource:

McConahay, J. B. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the modern racism scale. In J. S. Dovidio & S. I. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice,
discrimination, and racism (pp. 99–125). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Find this resource:

McConahay, J. B., & Hough, J. C. (1976). Symbolic racism. Journal of Social Issues, 32(2), 23–45.
Find this resource:

Meyer, P. (1987). Ethnocentrism in human social behavior: Some biosociological considerations. In V. Reynolds, V. Falger, & I. Vine (Eds.), The
sociobiology of ethnocentrism: Evolutionary dimensions of xenophobia, discrimination, racism, and nationalism (pp. 81–93). London: Croom
Helms.
Find this resource:

Miles, R. (1989). Racism. London: Routledge.


Find this resource:

Moon, D. G. (2002). Thinking about “culture” in intercultural communication. In J. N. Martin, T. K. Nakayama, & L. A. Flores (Eds.), Readings
in intercultural communication: Experiences and contexts (2d ed., pp. 13–21). Boston: McGraw Hill.
Find this resource:

Nakayama, T. K. (1998). Communication of heterosexism. In M. L. Hecht (Ed.), Communicating prejudice (pp. 112–121). Thousand Oaks, CA:
SAGE.
Find this resource:

Nakayama, T. K., & Halualani, R. T. (Eds.). (2010). The handbook of critical intercultural communication. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
Find this resource:

Neuliep, J. (2012). The relationship among intercultural communication apprehension, ethnocentrism, uncertainty reduction, and
communication satisfaction during initial intercultural interaction: An extension of anxiety and uncertainty management (AUM) theory.
Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 41, 1–16.
Find this resource:

Omi, M., & Winant, H. (1986). Racial formation in the United States: From the 1960s to the 1980s. New York: Routledge.
Find this resource:

Opotow, S. (1990). Moral exclusion and injustice: An introduction. Journal of Social Issues, 46, 1–20.
Find this resource:

Orbe, M. P., & Spellers, R. E. (2005). From the margins to the center: Utilizing co-cultural theory in diverse contexts. In W. B. Gudykunst (Ed.),
Theorizing about intercultural communication (pp. 173–191). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Pettigrew, T. F. (2016). In pursuit of three theories: Authoritarianism, relative deprivation, and intergroup contact. Annual Review of
Psychology, 67, 1–21.
Find this resource:

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 15/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2008). How does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Meta-analytic tests of three mediators. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 922–934.
Find this resource:

Rattansi, A. (1992). Changing the subject? Racism, culture, and education. In J. Donald & A. Rattansi (Eds.). “Race,” culture, and difference (pp.
11–48). London: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Rex, J., & Mason, D. (Eds.). (1986). Theories of race and ethnic relations. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
Find this resource:

Reynolds, V., Falger, V., & Vine, I. (Eds.). (1987). The sociobiology of ethnocentrism: Evolutionary dimensions of xenophobia, discrimination,
and nationalism. London: Croom Helm.
Find this resource:

Rogers, E. M., & Hart, W. B. (2002). The histories of intercultural, international, and development communication. In W. B. Gudykunst & B.
Mody (Eds.), Handbook of international and intercultural communication (3d ed., pp. 1–18). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Rosenau, P. M. (1992). Post-modernism and the social sciences: Insights, inroads, and intrusions. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Find this resource:

Ross, M. H. (1991). The role of evolution in ethnocentric conflict and its management. Journal of Social Issues, 47, 167–185.
Find this resource:

Ruscher, J. B. (2001). Prejudiced communication: A social psychological perspective. New York: Guilford.
Find this resource:

Said, E. W. (2003). Orientalism. New York: Vintage Books.


Find this resource:

Sears, D. O., & Henry, P. J. (2005). Over thirty years later: A contemporary look at symbolic racism. Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology, 37, 95–150.
Find this resource:

Shah, Anup. (Oct 1, 2010). Human rights in various regions. Global issues: Social, political, economic and environmental issues that affect us
all.

Shome, R., & Hegde, R. (2002). Postcolonial approaches to communication: Charting the terrain, engaging the intersections. Communication
Theory, 12, 249–270.
Find this resource:

Slavíčková, T., & Zvagulis, P. (2014). Monitoring anti-minority rhetoric in the Czech print media: A critical discourse analysis. Journal of
Language and Politics, 13 (1), 152–170.
Find this resource:

Stephan, W. (1985). Intergroup relations. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (rev. ed.) (pp. 599–658). Reading,
MA: Addison-Wesley.
Find this resource:

Sumner, G. A. (1975). Folkways and mores. E. Sagarin (Ed.). New York: Shocken.
Find this resource:

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of
intergroup relationships (2d ed.) (pp. 7–24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
Find this resource:

Ura, M., Preston, K. S. J., & Mearns, J. (2015). A measure of prejudice against accented English (MPAAE). Journal of Language & Social
Psychology, 34, 539–563.
Find this resource:

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 16/17
4/16/2019 Culture, Prejudice, Racism, and Discrimination - Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication

van Dijk, T. 2000. New(s) racism: A discourse analytical approach. In S. Cottle (Ed.), Ethnic minorities and the media (pp. 33–49). Milton
Keynes, UK: Open University Press.
Find this resource:

van Dijk, T. A. (1993). Elite discourse and racism. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Wander, P. C., Martin, J. N., & Nakayama, T. K. (1999). Whiteness and beyond: Sociohistorical foundations of Whiteness and contemporary
challenges. In T. K. Nakayama & J. N. Martin (Eds.), Whiteness: The communication of social identity (pp. 13–26). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Warner, M. (1991). Introduction: Fear of a queer plant. Social Text, 29, 3–17.
Find this resource:

West, C., & Zimmerman, D. H. (1987). Doing gender. Gender & Society, 1(2), 125–151.
Find this resource:

Wilson, W. J. (1978). The declining significance of race: Blacks and changing American institutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Find this resource:

Wood, J. (2008). Critical feminist theories: Giving voice and visibility to women’s experiences in interpersonal communication. In L. A. Baxter &
D. O. Braithwaite (Eds.), Engaging theories in interpersonal communication: Multiple perspectives (pp. 323–334).Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Find this resource:

Yep, G. A. (2013). Queering/quaring/kauering/crippin’/transing “other bodies” in intercultural communication. Journal of International


and Intercultural Communication 6, 118–126.
Find this resource:

Zimmerman, D. H., & West, C. (1975). Sex Roles, interruptions, and silences in conversation. In B. Thorne & N. Henley (Eds.), Language and
sex: Difference and dominance (pp. 105–129). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Find this resource:

Zompetti, J. P. (2012). The cultural and communicative dynamics of capital: Gramsci and the impetus for social action. Culture, Theory and
Critique, 53, 365–382.
Find this resource:

Zur, O. (1991). The love of hating: The psychology of enmity. History of European Ideas, 13, 345–369.
Find this resource:

John Baldwin

School of Communication, Illinois State University

PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, COMMUNICATION (oxfordre.com/communication). (c) Oxford University Press USA, 2019. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; c
details see Privacy Policy and Legal Notice).

date: 16 April 2019

oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-164?print 17/17

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi