Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 26

Variability ​and ​Bias

PATIENT ​PROFILE
of ​shots ​is ​off ​center​. ​This ​might ​occur​, ​for ​example​, ​if
the ​sigh​t ​of ​the ​g​un ​were ​bent​. ​Th​e ​precision ​is ​com ​A ​45​-​y​ear​-​old ​man ​began ​working ​as ​a
production p ​ arable​, ​but ​the ​result ​in ​D ​i​s ​systematically ​off ​tar​get ​supervisor​, ​and ​his
employer ​required ​that ​he ​undergo ​or ​biased​. ​The ​results ​in ​target ​C ​are ​accurate​, ​or
vali​d​. ​a ​complete ​m​edical ​examination​. ​Hi​s ​physician ​It ​is ​important ​to ​consider ​the
accuracy ​and ​precision ​learned ​that ​the ​patient​'​s ​father ​had ​died ​of ​myocar ​of ​any
measurements ​made ​in ​the ​medical ​setting​. ​In ​dial ​infarction ​at ​age ​65​. ​On ​physical
examination​, ​the c​ linical ​medicine and m ​ edical ​research​, ​variability ​can ​patient ​was
moderately ​obese​, ​and ​his ​blood ​pressure ​occur ​at ​a ​number ​of ​different ​levels ​(​e​g​, ​at
the ​level ​of
the ​individual ​or ​the ​population​) ​(​Table ​10​-​1​)​. ​At ​each ​vealed ​no ​notable ​abnormalities​.
The ​patient'​ s​ ​total​, l​ evel​, the variability ​inherent ​in ​the ​method ​of ​mea ​serum
cholesterol ​level (​ ​nonfasting​) ​was ​2​42 ​mg​/​dL​. ​V ​surement ​is ​important​.
According to t​ he ​guidelines ​of ​the N ​ ational ​Cho ​lesterol ​Education ​Program
(​NCEP)​ ,​ ​a ​total ​serum ​Variability ​within ​the ​Individual ​cholesterol ​concentration
greater ​than 2 ​ 40 mg/​ ​dL ​is T ​ he ​first ​level ​of ​concern ​is ​variability ​in ​the ​true ​an ​indication
​ ossible ​pharmacologic ​lowering o
for p ​ f ​value ​of ​a ​person​'​s ​characteristics ​over ​time​.
This ​was ​serum ​cholesterol.​ A ​ v​ alue ​of 2​ 00​-​239 m​ g/​ d
​ L ​is ​con a ​ ​source ​of ​concern ​for
the ​clinician ​in ​the ​Patient ​sidered ​borderline ​and should t​ rigger d ​ ietary i​ nter ​Profile​.
Some ​potential ​sources ​of ​individual ​variabil v​ ention​, ​and a ​ v​ alue l​ ess t​ han 2 ​ 00
mg​/d​ L is ​considered i​ ty ​are ​listed ​in ​Table ​10​-​2​. ​Varia​tio​n ​can ​o​ccur ​b​e ​normal
cause ​of ​b​iological ​chan​ges ​in ​an ​individual ​over ​time​. B ​ ased o ​ n t​ he i​ nitial
cholesterol ​results​, ​the p ​ hysi T ​ hese changes may ​(​1​) ​occur ​on ​a ​minute​-​to​-​minute ​cian
asked t​ he ​patient t​ o r​ eturn i​ n ​2 ​w​eeks ​for f​ urther b ​ asis ​(​eg​, ​heart ​rate​)​, ​(​2​) ​follow ​a
reguiar ​diurnal ​pat ​testing​. ​On ​repeat m
​ easurement​, ​the t​ otal ​serum ​cho ​tern ​(​eg​, ​body
temperature​)​, ​or ​(​3​) ​progress ​with ​nor​)​. ​lesterol ​concentration ​was ​198 ​mg​/​dL ​on ​a
​ al ​development ​(​eg​, ​height ​or ​we​i​ght​)​; ​lipid ​profile​. ​Table ​10​-​1 ​lis​ts ​s​everal
fasting m
different ​factors ​When ​the ​variation ​w​ithin ​a ​su​bject ​is ​larg​e​, ​a ​single ​that ​could ​explain
the ​observed ​variab​ility​ i​ n m ​ easured ​measurement ​may ​not ​ade​quately ​repr​esent
​ tal s​ erum ​cholesterol l​ evel.​ T
the ​"​true​" ​to ​ he s​ ource ​of t​ his v​ ari s​ tatus ​of ​that ​individu​al​.
By ​repeating ​a ​test​, ​the ​phy​si ​ability ​in t​ he m​ easured t​ otal ​cholesterol l​ evel ​had i​ m
portant ​implications ​for h
​ ow ​the ​physician ​treated t​ his v​ alue ​and ​its ​variability​. ​This m
​ ay ​also
provide ​the
clinician ​with ​information ​about ​variability ​or ​error ​due ​to ​the ​measurement ​technique​. ​In ​the
Patient
Profile​, ​different ​results ​were ​obtained ​when ​the ​total ​VARIABILITY IN ​MEDICAL
RESEARCH
serum ​cholesterol ​level ​was ​measured ​a ​second ​time
when ​the ​patient ​was ​fasting​. ​It ​is ​unlikely​, ​however​, ​Difficulties ​in ​the ​interpretation ​of ​test
results ​of ​in ​that ​the ​fasting ​state ​alone ​could ​cause ​such ​a ​drop ​in ​dividual ​patients a
​ re
magnified ​when ​groups ​of ​pa ​total ​serum ​cholesterol concentration​. ​Furthermore​, ​it ​tients ​are
studied​. ​The ​sources ​of ​variability ​in ​test ​is ​unlikely ​that ​the ​patient ​could ​have ​made ​the
kind ​of ​results ​and ​errors ​in ​medical research ​are ​discussed ​in ​dietary ​or ​other
alterations ​in ​2 ​weeks ​that ​would ​lead ​this ​chapter​. ​Appreciation ​of ​these i​ ssues ​is
important ​to ​the ​observed ​change ​in ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​level​. ​for ​the ​interpretation ​and
appropriate ​application ​of ​research ​findings ​in ​the ​clinical ​setting
Variability ​Related ​Variability ​in ​m​easur​eme​n​ts ​can ​be ​either ​ran​dom ​or ​to ​Measurement
syst​ematic​. ​A ​schematic ​representation ​of ​random ​and ​Laboratory ​measurements
of ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​systematic ​variation ​is ​shown ​in ​Figure ​10​-​1​. ​T​he ​level
are ​notorious ​for ​both ​variability ​and ​error​. ​To ​de ​shots ​at ​the ​targets i​ n ​both ​A
and ​B ​are ​centered ​around ​termine ​which ​value​-​-​198 ​mg​/​dL ​or ​242 ​mg​/​dL​-​is ​the ​middle​,
but ​in ​A ​the ​shots ​are ​less ​scattered ​and ​closer ​to ​the ​truth​, ​the ​physician ​in ​the ​Patient
Profile ​h​ave ​less ​variability​, ​or ​more ​precision​. ​In ​targets ​C w
​ ould ​need ​to ​know ​whether ​both
measurements ​were ​and ​D t​ he ​scatter ​is ​similar​, ​but ​in ​target ​D t​ he ​cluster ​obtained ​in ​the ​same
laboratory​. ​For ​example​, ​the ​first
141
142 ​/ ​CHAPTER ​10

Table ​10​-​1​. ​Levels ​of ​variability​.

Levels
F​eatures

Individual

Population
Individual ​variability ​Measurement ​variability ​Genetic variability ​between ​individuals ​Environmental
variability ​Measurement ​variability ​Manner ​of ​sampling
Size ​of ​sample ​Measurement ​variability
Sample

result ​may ​have ​been ​obtained ​from ​a ​desktop analyzer ​in ​the ​physician​'​s ​office​,
whereas ​the ​lipid ​profile ​may ​have ​been ​measured ​in ​a ​standardized ​laboratory​. ​In
​ lity​, ​the ​physician ​may ​not ​be ​able ​to ​discern ​readily ​which ​value ​is ​closer ​to ​the
re a
truth​. ​This ​is ​one ​reason ​that ​programs ​with ​guidelines ​that ​support ​cutoff ​points ​for
clinical decision ​making​, such ​as ​the ​NCEP​, ​often ​rec ​ommend ​that ​elevated ​values ​be
confirmed ​by ​repeated ​measurements ​over ​time ​before ​treatment ​is ​instituted​.

Figure ​10​-​1​. ​Schematic ​illustrations of ​increased ​random ​error ​(​target ​B ​versus ​target ​A​)
and ​systematic ​error ​(​tar ​get ​D ​versus ​target ​C​)​.
Variations ​within ​Populations
Just ​as ​there ​is ​variation ​in ​individuals​, ​there ​is ​also ​variability ​in ​populations​,
which ​can ​be ​considered ​the c ​ umulative ​variabi​lity ​of ​i​ndividuals​. ​Because
popula ​tions ​are ​made ​up o ​ f ​individuals ​with ​different ​genetic ​constitutions
who ​are ​subject ​to ​different ​envir​onmen​- ​tal ​influences​, ​populations ​often
exhibit ​more ​variation ​th​a​n ​individuals​. ​Physicians use ​knowledge ​a​b​out
variability ​in ​populations ​to ​define ​what ​is ​"​normal​" ​and ​"a ​ bnormal​.​” ​The
physician ​in ​the ​Patient ​Profile c​ ould ​refer ​to ​population ​survey ​data ​to
learn ​that ​for ​45​-​year​-​old ​males​, ​a ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​level ​of ​200 ​mg​/​dL ​is
​ L ​is ​eq​uivalent ​t​o ​the
close ​to ​the ​50th ​percentile​, ​and ​a ​con ​centration ​of ​240 ​mg​/d
75th ​p​er ​centile​. ​Accordingly​, t​ he ​patient ​generally ​falls ​in ​the ​upper ​half ​of ​the
population ​distribution ​of ​total ​serum cholesterol ​values​. ​Assuming ​that ​the
measurement ​is ​correct​, t​ his could ​be ​a ​result ​of ​genetic ​factors​, ​envi ​ronmental
factors​, ​or ​both​.
hort ​studies ​such ​as ​the ​Framingham ​Heart ​Study​, ​groups ​of subjects ​were
followed ​and ​compared ​according ​to ​their ​different ​levels ​of ​total ​serum
cholesterol ​and ​the ​as ​sociated ​frequency ​of ​death ​from ​myocardial ​infarction
or ​stroke​. ​In ​these ​invest​ig
​ ations​, ​a ​higher ​level ​of ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​was
associated ​with ​an ​increased risk ​of ​death ​from ​cardiovascular ​disease​.
When ​investigators ​perform ​such ​studies​, ​they ​cannot ​usually ​study ​the
entire ​population​. ​Instead ​they ​study s
​ ubsets ​or ​samples ​o​f t​ he ​popu​lation​. ​This
introduces ​an ​other ​source ​of v ​ ariability​-​sam​pling v ​ ​ariab​ilit​y​-​-​that ​is
important ​in ​medical ​research​. ​Using ​a ​single ​sam ​ple of ​subjects ​to
represent ​the ​population ​is ​analogous ​to ​using ​a s ​ ingle ​measurement ​to
characterize ​an ​indi ​vidual​. ​Repeate​d ​sam​ples ​from ​the ​population ​will ​give
different ​estimates ​of ​the ​true ​population ​values​. ​Sampling ​variability ​is
illustrated ​in ​Figure ​10​–​2​. ​In ​the ​source ​population ​of ​20 persons​, ​there ​are ​five ​in
dividuals ​(​25​%​) ​with ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​values ​above ​240 ​mg​/​dL​. I​ n ​the ​three
different ​samples ​of ​five ​subjects ​drawn ​from ​the ​source ​population ​by c​ hance​, ​the
proportion ​of ​individuals ​with ​total ​serum ​choles
Variability ​in ​Research ​Studies
It ​is ​worthwhile ​to ​ask ​how ​the ​clinician ​would ​know ​that ​a ​total ​serum ​cholesterol
value in ​the u ​ pper ​end ​of ​the ​population ​distribution ​is ​disadvantageous​. ​Are ​these
values ​really ​unhealthy​? ​Answers ​may ​be ​found ​in ​stud ​ies ​that ​have ​linked ​the ​level
of ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​with ​an ​increased ​risk ​of ​cardiovascular ​mortality​. ​In ​co​-

Table ​1​0​-​2​. ​Potential sources ​of ​variability ​in ​measurements ​of ​individuals​.
Features
Sources ​of ​Variability ​Individual ​characteristics
Diurnal ​variation ​Changes ​related ​to f​ actors ​such ​as ​age​, ​diet​, ​and ​exercise ​Environmental f​ actors ​such
as ​season ​or ​temperature ​Poor ​calibration of ​the ​instrument ​Inherent ​lack of ​precision ​of ​the ​instrument
Misreading ​or ​misrecording ​information f​ rom ​the ​instrument by ​the ​technician
Measurement ​characteristics
VARIABILITY ​AND ​BIAS ​/ ​143

Source ​population
Study ​A ​(​200 ​subjects​)

305
18​0 ​174
Diet
21​5 ​305
276 ​19​5 ​21​5
Prevalence ​= ​40​%

H
Drug
17​0
233 276 146 ​195
5
10
15
Sample 1

Risk (percent)
205
1​88
Sample 2
295 146 22​0
190
Prevalence = 20%
1​62
Study B (2000 subjects)
Sample 3
22​8
295 ​170 ​1​64 ​248 ​162 ​22​0
Diet

Drug
2​1​9
21​9 ​164 19​0 ​188 233
Prevalence = 0%
228 ​25​0
15
5 ​1​0 ​Risk (percent)
Prevalence
= 25%

Figure 10-2. Schematic diagram of sampling variability ​The source population of 20 persons
has a 25% prevalence ​of hypercholesterolemia (elevated cholesterol values are pre sented in
bold). The three random samples of five persons ​yield prevalence estimates ranging from 0
to 40%,
Figure 10-3. The effect of sample size on precision of risk estimates. Point estimates
are shown as dashed vertical ​lines and 95% confidence intervals are shown as solid hor ​izontal line​s.
I​n ​bo​th studies, the 5-year risk of developing ​myocardial infarction was 9% among persons
r​ec​eiving dietary therapy and 6% among persons treated with a ​cholesterol-lowering drug. In the
larger study, however, the ​95% confidence intervals are narrower, and the difference ​in risk
between treatment groups is statistically significant.
terol values above 240 mg​/​dL ranges from 0 to 40%. ​Each of these small
samples presents a different pic ture of the source population. ​A lar​ger
s​ample size ​would result in less variability and would more likely represent th​e ​source
population.
Variabili​ty ​can be important in other ways when two ​groups are compared in a
study. The goal of such stud les often is to determine whether a measurable differ ​ence
exists between the groups. When a research paper reports ​no statistically
significa​nt difference between groups, the reader must ask the following
question: W ​ as there actually no difference between the two treat ​ments, or was the
estimate of effect so imprecise that the invest​i​gator could not distinguish
differences be t​ ween the two groups (ie, a type II error)?
A graphic display of the results of two hypothetical studies of the same question
is presented in F​i​gure 10-3. ​In each study, the investigators attempted to determine
whether a cholesterol-lowering drug had a favorable effect on the risk of developing
myocardial infarction. ​The measure of effect that was estimated in each study
was the risk ratio. Each study compared a group of pa tients randomly allocated
to receive the cholesterol ​lowering drug with a group chosen to receive
dietary ​modification alone. The researchers reached different ​conclusions. In the
study with the smaller sample, the ​report indicated that the drug had no
beneficial ​effec​t o​ n reducing the risk of developi​ng myocardial in​farc ​tion, when compared
with diet t​herapy​. In the study
with the larger sample, the invest​i​gators concluded that ​the drug decreased the risk
of developing myocardial ​infarction, when compared with dietary man​ag​ement.
As shown in Figure 10–3, Study A had a small sam ​ple si​z​e, which resulted in
imprecise estimates (ie, ​wide confidenc​e intervals​) of the risk of developing
myocardial infarctio​n in ​the two groups. Consequently, the two estimates
overlap​ped​, and the statistical test was not capable of distinguishing between the
effects of the two treatments. The investigators concluded that ​there w​as no
difference ​in risk of developing myo cardial infarction between patients who received the
cholesterol-lowering drug and those who received di etary therapy. In Study B,
the investigator used a larger ​sample size yielding the same point estimates
of risk in ​the two groups but with much greater precision (ie, ​narrower confidence
intervals) in the estimate of the ef ​fects of the drug. With this gain in precision, the sta
tistical t​est w​as able to distinguish between the two groups, and the investigator
was able to infer correctly that the cholesterol-lowering drug was superior to di
etary therapy. Ge​nerally, the larg​er the sample size, the ​more precise the estimate of
effect and the smaller the ​detectable differences between groups. In studies with ​very large
sample si​ze​s, small differences between groups may be judged to be statistically
significant but
1​44 ​/ C
​ H
​ APTER ​10

have ​li​ttle ​bio​logical ​or ​clinical ​meaning​. ​For ​example​, ​An ​example ​of ​the ​kind ​of
difficulty ​that ​can ​occur ​a ​study ​of ​20​,​000 ​subjects ​might ​have ​concluded ​that ​a ​when
study ​results ​are ​generalized ​is ​the ​criticism ​that ​1​% ​difference ​in ​risk ​of ​develop​ing
myocardial ​infarc ​too ​many ​clinical ​studies ​focus ​on ​white ​males​. ​One ​tion ​was
statistically ​significant​. ​It ​is ​u​nlikely​, ​however​, ​such ​study ​is ​the ​Lipid ​Research
Clinics​-​Primary ​that ​a ​difference ​in ​risk ​this ​small ​would ​justify ​pro ​* ​Prevention ​Trial​,
which ​demonstrated ​a ​sign​ificant ​re ​longed ​use ​of ​the ​cholesterol​-​lowering ​agent​.
duction ​in ​cardiovascular ​mortality ​for ​white ​men ​aged ​35​-​5​9 ​years ​with
h​ypercholes​terol​emia ​who ​wer​e
p​laced ​on ​a ​cholesterol​-​lowe​ring ​d​iet ​and ​medication​. ​VALIDITY
D​o ​the ​results ​also ​apply ​to ​women​? ​Do t​ hey ​apply ​to
men ​of ​different ​ages​, ​races​, ​or ​with ​different​, ​but ​still ​The ​ c​ oncept o ​ f ​validity c​ oncerns
the ​degree ​to w ​ hich a​ bnormal​, ​serum ​cholesterol ​levels​? ​These ​questions ​a ​measurement
or ​study r​ eaches ​a c​ orrect ​conclusion.​ l​ ed ​to ​the ​suggestion ​that ​federally ​funded
research ​A ​measurement ​or ​study ​may ​lead ​to ​an ​incor​rect ​(​in​- ​should ​include ​women​,
minorities​, ​and ​children ​in ​the ​valid​) ​conclusion ​because ​of ​the ​effects ​of ​bias​. ​Th​e
study ​populations​. ​variability ​seen ​with b ​ ias ​is ​systematic ​or ​nonrandom ​and d ​ istorts ​the
estimated ​effe​c​t​. ​In Figure ​10​-​1​, ​th​e ​Bias ​amount ​of ​bias ​can ​be ​determined ​by ​the
degree ​to ​which ​Bi​as i​ s a​ ​systematic ​error ​in ​a s​ tudy t​ hat ​leads to a ​ ​the ​shots ​are ​off
target ​in ​D​. ​Unfortunately​, ​in ​medical ​distortion o ​ f ​the ​result​s​. ​Bias​, ​a ​threat ​to ​validity​,
can ​research ​the ​truth ​(​bull​'​s​-​eye​) ​may ​not ​be ​known​, ​or ​occur ​in ​any ​research​, ​but ​is ​of
particular ​concern ​in ​there ​may ​b​e ​n​o ​"​gold ​standard​” ​for ​comparison​, ​observational
studies ​because ​the ​lack ​of ​randomiza ​Consequently​, ​the ​degree ​o​f ​bias ​often ​is ​difficult ​to
de ​tion ​increases ​the ​chance ​that ​study ​groups ​will ​differ ​termine​. ​Two ​different ​types
of ​validity​, ​internal ​valid ​with ​respect ​to ​important ​characteristics​. ​Bias ​often ​is i​ ty ​and
external ​validity​, ​are ​described ​in ​this ​chapter​. ​subdivided ​into ​different
categories​, ​based ​on ​how ​bias
enters ​the ​study​. ​The ​most ​common c​ lassification ​di ​Interna​l ​Validity​.
​ alidity i​ s t​ he e
vides ​bias ​into ​three ​categories​: ​Internal v ​ xtent t​ o w
​ hich t​ he r​ esults o ​ n
​ f ,​ a
investigation ​accurately ​reflect t​ he t​ rue ​situation ​of (​ ​1​) ​Selection ​bias ​V t​ he s​ t​ udy
population.​ I​ f ​the ​results ​are ​not ​valid ​in ​the ​(​2​) ​Information ​bias
study ​population​, there ​is ​little ​reason ​to ​suspect ​that ​(​3​) ​Confounding​. ​those ​results ​will
apply ​to ​other ​populations​. ​Internal ​validity ​is ​defined ​by ​the ​boundaries ​of ​the ​study ​itself​.
Alth​ough ​these ​ca​tegor​ies ​over​la​p​, ​this ​classification ​Therefore​, ​a ​study ​is ​internally
valid ​if ​it ​provides ​a ​is ​useful ​because ​it ​provides ​the ​reader ​with ​a ​system ​true ​estimate
​ tic ​approach ​to ​evaluate ​bias​. ​It ​should ​be
of ​effect​, ​given ​the ​limits ​of ​the ​popula a
remembered ​tion ​studied​. ​Measures ​that ​can ​be ​used ​to ​improve ​in ​that ​with ​the
exception ​of ​confo​und​ing​, ​which ​can ​be ​ternal ​validity ​often ​involve ​restricting ​the ​type
of q ​ uantitated​, ​the ​evaluation ​of ​bias ​is ​subjective ​and ​in ​subjects ​and ​the
environment ​in ​which ​the ​study ​is ​per ​volves ​a ​judgment ​regarding ​the ​likelihood ​of
(​1​) ​the ​formed​. ​These ​measures ​decrease ​the ​impact of ​factors presence ​of ​bias ​and
(​2​) ​its ​direction ​and ​potential ​mag ​extraneous ​to ​the ​question ​of ​interest​.
nitude of ​effect ​on ​the ​results​. ​Even though ​the ​magni
tude ​of ​bias ​cannot ​be ​quantified​, ​often ​its ​influence ​on ​External V​alidity
the results ​of ​a ​study ​can ​be ​inferred​. ​It ​is ​important ​to ​A ​result ​obtained ​in ​a ​tightly ​controlled
environ ​discern ​whether ​the ​suspected ​bias ​is ​likely ​to ​make ​an ​ment​, ​however​, ​may ​not ​be
applicable ​to ​more ​general ​association ​appear ​stronger ​or ​weaker ​than ​it ​really ​is​.
situations​. ​External v​ alidity ​is t​ he e
​ xtent to ​which ​the ​, ​Overestimation ​of ​a ​risk ​ratio ​for ​a
protective ​exposure ​results ​of a
​ ​study ​are
​ ​applicab​le t​ o ​ot​ her ​populations​. ​and a
separate ​hazardous ​exposure ​is ​demonstrated ​External ​validity ​addresses ​the
following ​question​: ​Do ​schematically ​in ​Figure ​10​-​4​. ​Underestimation of ​a ​risk ​these
results ​apply ​to ​other ​patients​, ​such ​as ​patients r​ atio ​for ​a ​protective ​exposure ​and ​a
hazardous ​expo ​who ​are ​older​, s ​ icker​, or ​less ​economically ​advantaged s​ ure ​is ​shown
in ​Figure ​10​–​5​. ​than ​subjects ​in ​the ​study​?
External ​validity ​often ​is ​of ​particular ​interest ​to ​Selection ​Bias ​clinicians ​who ​must decide
if ​a ​research ​finding ​is ​ap ​A ​variety ​of ​procedures ​can ​be ​used ​to ​select ​sub ​plicable ​to
their ​clinical ​practice​. ​Determining ​whether ​jects ​for ​a ​study​. ​Usually​, ​it ​is ​not ​possible ​to
include ​the ​results ​of ​a ​study ​can ​be ​generalized i​ nvolves ​a ​all ​individuals ​with ​a ​particular
disease ​or ​exposure ​in ​judgment ​regarding ​the ​following​:
a ​study​, ​so ​a ​sample ​of ​subjects ​must ​be ​chosen​. ​The
procedures ​used ​for ​the ​selection ​of ​subjects ​depend ​on ​(​1​) The ​type ​of ​subjects ​included ​in
the ​investigation ​a ​number ​of ​factors​, ​including ​(​2​) ​The ​type ​of ​patients ​seen ​by ​the ​clinician
(​3​) ​Whether ​there ​are ​clinically ​meaningful ​differ ​(​1​) ​The ​design ​of ​the ​investigation
ences ​between ​the ​study population ​and ​other ​(​2​) ​Th​e ​setting ​of ​the ​study
populations​.
(​3​) ​The disease ​and ​exposure ​of ​interest​.
VA ​ RIABILITY ​AND ​BIAS ​/ ​145

A​. ​Protective ​exposure


Bias ​away ​from ​the ​null ​hypothesis
Study ​value
True ​value

Risk ​ratio

B​. ​Hazardous ​exposure

Bias ​away ​from ​the ​null ​hypothesis

True
Study ​value
value

Risk ​ratio

Figure ​10​-​4​. ​Overestimation ​of a


​ ​risk ​ratio ​fo​r ​(​A​) ​a ​protective ​exposure ​and ​(​B​)
a ​hazardous ​exposure​.

A​. ​Protective ​exposure


Bias ​toward ​the ​null ​hypothesis

True ​value
Study ​value
Risk ​ratio

B​. ​Hazardous ​exposure


Bias ​toward ​the ​null ​hypothesis

Study ​value
True ​value

Risk ​ratio

Figure ​10​-​5​. ​Undere​s​timation ​of ​a ​risk ​ratio ​for ​(​A​) ​a ​protective ​exposure
and ​(​B​) ​a ​hazardous ​exposure​.
146 ​/ ​CHAPTER ​10

Often ​subjects ​are ​selected ​in ​a ​manner ​that ​is ​conve​- ​nient ​for ​the ​investigator​.
Under ​optimal ​circumstances​, ​the ​method ​for ​inclusion ​of ​subjects ​leads ​to ​a
valid ​comparison ​that​, ​in ​turn​, ​yields ​correct ​information ​re ​garding ​a ​disease ​process ​or
treatment​. ​The ​selection ​process ​itself​, ​however​, ​may ​increase ​or ​decrease ​the ​chance
that ​a ​relationship ​between ​the ​exposure ​and ​dis ​ease ​of ​interest ​will ​be detected​. ​A
schematic ​diagram ​of ​the ​steps ​involved ​in ​recruiting ​and ​maintaining ​a ​study
population ​is ​shown ​in ​Figure ​10​-​6​. ​From ​this ​diagram​, ​it ​is ​easy ​to ​see ​that
selection ​factors ​could ​lead ​to ​biased results ​at ​several ​different ​steps ​in ​the
process​.
Some ​aspects ​of ​the ​selection ​of ​subjects ​lead ​pri ​marily ​to ​problems ​with ​the
generalization ​(​extrapola ​tion​) ​of ​the ​results ​(​ie​, ​external va​lidit​y​)​. ​Subjects ​must ​agree
to ​participate ​in ​a ​study​, ​and ​this ​causes ​one ​of ​the ​most ​common ​problems​. ​Volunteers
for ​a ​study ​may ​differ ​from ​individuals ​who ​do ​not ​volunteer ​in ​various ​characteristics ​such ​as
age​, ​race​, ​economic ​sta ​tus​, ​education ​level​, ​and ​sex​. ​Moreover​, ​volunteers ​may ​be
healthier ​than ​those ​who ​decline ​to ​participate​. ​A ​study ​of ​a ​population ​limited ​to
individuals ​who ​are ​employed ​may ​also ​make ​it ​difficult ​to ​generalize ​the ​results​,
because ​people ​who ​work ​are ​generally ​health ​ier ​than ​those ​who ​do ​not​. ​A ​comparison
of ​health ​out ​comes ​between ​workers ​and ​the ​general ​population ​may ​show ​that ​the
workers ​have ​a ​more ​favorable ​out ​come ​simply ​because ​they ​are ​healthy ​enough
to ​b​e ​employed ​(​the ​"h
​ ealthy ​worker​" ​effect​)​.
Referral ​of ​patients ​to ​clinical ​facilities ​can ​also ​lead ​to ​distorted ​study ​conclusions​.
Selective ​referral ​patterns ​can ​be ​seen ​in ​the ​study ​of ​children ​with ​febrile ​seizures​.
Febrile ​seizures ​are ​brief​, ​generalized ​seizures ​that ​occur ​in ​conjunction ​with ​elevation
in temperature ​in ​children ​aged ​6 ​months ​to ​6 ​years​. ​There ​is ​some disagreement
about whether ​these ​febrile ​convulsions ​are ​predictive ​of ​future ​seizures ​and ​other
unfavorable ​neurologic ​se ​quelae​. ​Ellenberg ​and ​Nelson ​(​1980​) ​compared ​the ​re ​sults
of ​a ​number ​of ​studies ​on ​the ​long​-​term ​outcome ​of ​patients ​with ​febrile ​seizures​.
Studies ​of ​geographi ​cally ​defined ​populations ​in ​which ​affected ​children ​were
followed​, ​regardless of ​whether ​medical ​care ​was ​sought​, ​consistently ​revealed ​a
relatively ​low ​rate ​of ​un ​favorable ​sequelae​. ​Clinic​-​based ​studies ​tended ​to ​report a

high frequency ​of ​adverse ​outcomes​. ​Accordingly​, ​it ​was ​concluded ​that ​clinic​-​based
studies ​selectively ​in ​cluded ​children ​at the ​more ​severe ​end ​of ​the ​clinical
spectrum​. ​The ​inferences ​that ​might ​be ​drawn ​regard ​ing ​the ​prognosis ​of ​a ​child
​ e ​very ​different ​based ​on ​whether ​a ​clinic​-​based ​or ​a
with ​febrile ​seizures might b
population​-​based ​sample ​was ​studied​.
Other ​aspects ​of ​the ​selection ​process ​can diminish ​internal ​validity​. I​ n ​a c​ linical t​ rial ​or
cohort s​ tudy​, t​ he ​major p​ otential ​selection b ​ ias ​is ​los​ s ​to f​ ollow-​ u
​ p.​ O
​ nce ​subjects ​are
enrolled ​in ​the ​study​, ​they ​may ​de ​cide ​to ​discontinue ​participation​. ​Certain ​types ​of ​sub
jects ​are ​more ​likely ​than ​others ​to ​drop ​out ​of ​a ​study​. ​Furthermore​, ​during ​the ​course
of ​the ​study ​some ​sub ​jects ​may ​die ​from ​causes other ​than ​the ​outcome ​of ​in

Total ​population
Sampling ​scheme

Sampled ​population
Exclusions
Inclusion
criteria

Eligible ​subjects
related ​to ​selection ​because ​the ​subject ​already ​was ​en ​rolled ​in ​the ​study​. ​If ​the ​lost
subjects ​differ​, ​however​, ​in ​their ​risk ​of ​the ​outcome ​of ​interest​, ​biased ​estimates ​of ​risk ​may
be ​obtained​.
If ​the ​unrecognized ​early ​manifestations ​of ​the ​dis ​ease ​of ​interest ​cause ​exposed
persons ​to ​leave ​the ​study ​more ​or ​less ​frequently ​than ​unexposed ​persons​, ​a ​distorted
conclusion ​might ​be ​reached​. ​For ​example​, ​in ​a ​randomized ​controlled ​trial ​of ​the
effects ​of ​using ​a ​cholesterol​-​lowering ​drug ​versus ​diet ​therapy ​on ​prevention ​of
myocardial ​infarctions​, ​bias ​might ​be ​in ​troduced ​if ​drug​-​treated ​patients ​with ​coronary
insuffi ​ciency ​were ​more ​likely ​to ​develop ​side ​effects ​from ​treatment ​and ​withdrew from
participation​, ​whereas ​pa ​tients ​with ​coronary ​insufficiency ​receiving ​dietary ​ther ​apy
remained ​in ​the ​study​.
Selection ​bias ​is ​of ​particular ​importance ​in case ​control ​st​udies ​(​see ​Chapter ​9​) ​in
which ​the ​invest​i​ga ​tor ​must ​select ​two ​study ​groups​, ​cases ​and ​controls, ​in ​a ​setting ​in
which ​the ​exposure ​has ​already ​occurred​. ​For ​example​, ​it ​must ​be ​decided ​whether ​to
use ​exist ​ing ​(​prevalent​) ​cases ​who ​are ​available ​at ​the ​time ​of ​study​, ​regardless ​of ​the
duration ​of ​their ​disease​, ​or ​to ​limit ​eligibility ​to ​newly ​diagnosed ​(​incident​) ​cases​. ​If ​the
risk ​factor ​of ​interest ​also ​is ​a ​prognostic ​factor​, ​the
Subjects ​asked ​to

Nonparticipants
Informed
consent

Participants
Lost ​to ​follow​-​up

Complete ​study

Figure ​10​-​6​. ​Steps ​in ​the ​selection ​and ​maintenance ​of ​subjects ​in ​a ​study
Consider​, ​for ​example​, ​a ​case​-​control ​study ​of ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​level ​as ​a ​risk ​factor
for ​developing
VARIABILITY ​AND ​BIAS ​/ ​1​4​7
WE

myocardial ​infarction​. ​Suppose ​that ​of ​patients ​who ​, ​more ​difficult ​for ​the ​investigator
to ​detect ​a ​real ​asso ​have ​a m
​ yocardial ​infarction ​those ​with ​very ​high ​total​, ​ciation
between ​the ​exposure ​and ​the ​disease​. ​This ​is s ​ erum ​cholesterol ​levels ​are ​more
likely ​to ​die ​suddenly ​often ​referred ​to ​as ​a ​bias ​toward ​the ​null ​hypothesis ​or t​ han
those ​with ​lower ​serum ​cholesterol ​levels​. ​Under​-​toward ​no ​as​soci​ation ​- ​these
circumstances​, ​a ​comparison ​of ​patients ​surviv​- ​"​Differential
misclassifica​tion ​occurs ​when ​the ​mis ​ing ​myocardial ​infarction ​with ​controls
will ​underesti ​classification ​of ​one ​variable ​depends ​on ​the ​status ​of ​mate
the ​true ​association ​between ​elevation ​in ​total ​th​e ​oth​er​. ​In a
​ ​case​-​control
study​, ​this ​type ​of ​misclas ​serum ​cholesterol ​level ​and ​risk ​of ​developing myocar
sification ​could ​occur ​if ​the ​information ​on ​exposure ​dial i​ nfarction​.
status ​depends ​on ​whethe​r ​the ​subje​ct has ​the ​disease​. ​Another p ​ otential ​type ​of
selection ​bias ​can ​occur ​If ​a ​case ​with ​a ​myocardial ​infarction ​is ​more ​lik​ely
to ​when ​a ​case​-​control ​study ​involves ​subjects ​who ​are ​overestimate ​the ​level ​of
dietary ​fat ​intake ​than ​a ​con h​ ospitalized​. ​Patients ​with ​two ​medical ​conditions ​are
trol ​subject​, ​a ​biased ​result ​may ​occur​. ​In ​this ​instance​, ​more ​likely ​to ​be
hospitalized ​than ​those ​with ​a ​single ​the ​bias ​would ​lead ​to ​an ​overestimate ​of ​the
​ etween ​dietary
relation ​disease​. ​Thus​, ​a ​hospital​-​based ​case​-​control ​study ​ship b
fat ​intake ​and ​risk ​of ​d​ev​eloping m
​ ight ​find ​a ​link ​between ​two ​diseases ​or
between ​an ​! ​myocard​ial ​infarction​. ​exposure ​and ​a ​disease ​when ​there ​is ​no
association ​be ​The ​difference ​between ​nondifferential ​and ​differ ​tween them ​in ​the
general ​population​. ​This ​type ​of ​ential ​misclassification ​can ​be ​demonstrated ​by ​exam
bias​, ​often ​called ​Berk​son​'​s ​bias​, ​was ​demonstrated ​in ​ining ​the ​data ​in ​Figure ​10​-​7​.
Consider ​a ​case​-​control ​a ​study ​that ​showed ​that ​respiratory ​and ​b​on​e ​diseases ​study
of ​the ​relationship ​between ​high​-​fat ​diets ​and ​were ​associated ​i​n ​a ​sa​mple ​of
hospitalized ​pat​i​ents ​but r​ isk ​of ​developing ​myocardial ​infarction ​in ​which ​the ​not ​in
the ​general ​population​. ​Thus​, ​in ​a ​hospital​-​based ​true ​odds ​ratio ​(​OR)​ ​is ​2​.​3​. ​With
nondifferential ​mis ​study​, ​an ​exposure ​such ​as ​cigarette ​smoking​, ​which ​is
classification​, ​the ​subjects ​did ​not r​ ecall the ​amount ​of ​correlated ​with ​respiratory
disease​, ​may ​also ​appear ​to ​fatty ​foods ​eaten​, ​but ​the ​errors ​in ​recall ​did not
depend ​occur ​together ​with ​bone ​disease ​because ​those ​dis ​on ​whether ​they
had ​a ​myocardial ​infarction​. ​In ​this ​sit ​eases ​are ​related ​in ​ho​spital​ized ​patients​.
uation​, ​20​% ​of ​both ​cases ​and ​co​ntro​ls ​who ​ate ​high
fat ​diets ​underreported ​fat ​intake​. ​The ​resulting ​OR ​of ​Information ​Bias
2​.​0 ​was ​an ​underestimate ​of t​ he ​true O ​ ​. ​On ​the ​other ​Information (​ ​or
​ R
misclassification)​ b ​ ias c​ an ​occur ​hand​, ​if ​all ​the patients ​who ​had ​a ​myocardial
infarc ​w​hen t​ here i​ s r​ andom ​or s​ ystematic ​inaccuracy i​ n t​ ion ​correctly ​recalled ​their
dietary ​fat ​exposure ​status​, ​measurement.​ ​This ​can ​be ​visualized ​best ​in
epidemi ​but ​only ​80​% ​of ​the ​exposed ​controls ​correctly ​re ​ological ​studies ​that ​involve
dichotomous ​exposure ​ported ​their ​exposure​, ​then ​differentia​l ​misclassifica ​and
disease ​variables​, ​such ​as ​elevated ​total ​serum ​cho ​tion ​would ​occur​. ​This
type ​of ​misclassification ​can ​lesterol ​and ​myocardial ​infarction​. ​Subjects ​are ​classi
result ​in ​either ​an ​underestimate ​or ​overestimate ​of ​the ​fied ​according ​to
whether ​they ​have ​had ​high ​total t​ rue ​OR​. ​In this example, the investigator overesti
serum cholesterol levels and whether they have had a m ​ ated the ​OR. m
​ yocardial
infarction. The investigator either can be ​Two common types of ​differe​ntial
inf​ormation ​bias ​correct or incorrect, resulting in true-positive and true ​are often
referred to as re​call b​ias and interviewer negative findings, as well as
false-positive and false bias. Recall bias results from differential ability of
negative classifications of subjects with respect to subjects to remember
previous activities and expo ​either exposure or disease.
sures. Patients who have a serious disease may search If the errors in classification of
exp​osure or disease ​their memory for an exposure in an attempt to explain ​status
are independent of the level of the other variable, ​or to understand why they acquired the
illness. Control ​then the misclassification is termed n​ondifferenti​al. ​subjects, who
do not have the disease, may be less Nondifferential misclassific​atio​n may occur in a
case ​likely to remember an exposure because it has less control study if the
subject's memory of exposu​re s​ta ​meaning and is less important for them. t​ us is
unrelated to whether the subject has the disease of When interviewers are
employed to determine ex interest. An example of nondifferential misclassifica
posures in case-control studies, results may be influ tion is sometimes referred to
as unacceptability bias. enced by how the interviewers collect information. If
Subjects may answer a question about the exposure ​they are aware of the
research hypothesis, the inter with a socially acceptable but sometimes inaccurate re
viewers intentionally or unintentionally may influ ​sponse, ​rega​rdless ​of ​whether they
have the disease of ence the responses of the subjects. They may probe
int​erest​. Consider a case-control study of myocardial more deeply for responses
from cases than from con infarction in which the exposure of interest is prior in
tro​l​s. If a dietary exposure is examined, the inter take of foods high in saturated
fats. Reg​ardless o​f dis viewers may ask certain subjects specific questions ​e​ase
status, ​resp​ondents may ​un​derreport intake of a​bo​ut particular food items.
Interviewers may also foods with h​igh ​fat conte​nt b​ecause they think low-fat give
the subjects subtle clues by tone of voice or diets are more accep​ta​ble to the
invest​iga​tor. In most in body language that suggest a preference for certain re
stances, when nondifferential misclassification occurs, sponses. Generally, it is
desirable to blind the inter it blurs differences between the study groups, making
it viewers to t​he research ​hypothesis under investigation.
-
-
-

148 ​/ ​CHAPTER ​10

Nondifferential ​misclassification

Truth​:
Study​:
Dietary ​fat
Low
Dietary ​fat ​High
Low
High

MI
60
I ​40
48
5​2

No ​MI
No ​MI
68

OR ​= ​(​60 ​x ​60​)​/​(​40 ​x ​40​) ​= 2​.​3


O​R ​= ​(​48 ​x ​68​)​/​(​52 ​x ​32​) ​= ​2​.​0

Differential ​misclassification

Truth​:
Study​:
Dietary fat ​High
Low
Dietary ​fat ​High
Low

MI
60
L
40
60
. ​40

No ​MI
No ​MI
32
6​8

OR ​= ​(​60 ​x ​60​)/​ (​ ​40 ​x ​40​) ​= ​2​.​3


OR ​= ​(​60 ​X ​68​)​/(​ ​40 ​x ​32​) ​= ​3​.2 ​

Figure ​10​-​7​. ​Illustration ​of ​nondifferential ​and ​differential ​misclassification ​of ​exposure ​to
​ ​odds ​ratio​.​)
high​-​fat ​diets ​in ​a ​case​-​control ​study ​of ​myocardial ​infarction ​(​M​I​)​. ​(​OR =

In ​a ​case​-​control ​study​, ​however​, ​it ​may ​be ​difficult ​to ​blind ​the ​interviewers ​to ​the
disease ​status of ​cases ​and ​controls​. ​Nevertheless​, ​if the ​interviewers ​are ​not ​aware
of ​the ​exp​osure ​of ​pri​mary ​intere​st​, ​b​iased ​data ​collectio​n ​still ​can ​be
minimized​.
As ​a ​way ​to ​reduce ​misclassification ​and ​to ​improve ​accuracy ​of ​study
measurements​, ​investigators ​in ​creasingly ​are ​using ​biological markers​. ​As ​shown ​in
Table ​10​–​3​, ​these ​markers ​can ​measure ​many ​facets ​of ​disease ​and
exposure​-​or ​the ​relationship ​between ​the ​two​. ​For ​example​, ​biological ​markers
can ​measure
(​3​) ​Biologically ​effective ​dose (​ ​biological m
​ arkers
can ​be ​used ​to ​measure ​the ​amount ​of ​a ​sub
stance ​that ​reaches ​the ​target ​sites​) ​(​4​) ​Biological ​effect ​(​biological ​markers ​can ​be
used ​to ​quantify ​a ​deleterious ​effect ​of ​a partic ​ular ​exposure​)​.

(​1​) ​Susceptibility ​(​biological ​markers ​can ​be ​used


to ​identify ​subjects ​with ​particularly ​high ​risk
due ​to ​a ​particular ​biological ​predisposition​) ​(​2​) ​Internal ​dose ​(​biological
markers ​can ​be ​used ​to
measure ​the ​amount ​of ​a ​chemical ​or ​other ​ex ​posure ​in ​the ​body​)
Biological ​markers ​are ​used ​in ​most ​substantive ​areas ​of ​invest​i​gation​, ​including
nutritional​, ​cardiovascular​, ​reproductive​, ​cancer​, ​and ​infectious ​disease ​epidemiol
ogy​. ​Use ​of ​biological ​markers ​is ​important ​in ​observa ​tional ​studies ​for ​several
reasons​. ​The​se ​markers ​are ​important meth​odologica​lly ​because ​they can ​serve
to r​ educe ​misclassification ​by ​allowing ​more ​accura​te ​as ​sessment ​of
exposure ​or ​disease ​status​. ​Furthermore​, ​they ​may ​allow ​the ​investigator ​to ​define
more ​homoge n ​ eous ​disease ​categories ​or ​to ​identify ​susceptible ​sub ​jects​,
so ​that ​the ​study ​can ​focus o
​ n ​specific ​subgroups​.

Table ​10​-​3​. ​Uses ​of ​biological ​markers ​in ​epidemiology​.


Application ​of ​Marker
Example ​To ​measure ​susceptibility
Those with ​high ​aryl ​hydrocarbon ​hydoxylase ​activity ​have ​higher ​risk ​of ​bladder
cancer ​To ​measure ​internal ​dose
Those ​with ​high ​serum carotene ​levels ​may ​have ​lower ​risk ​of ​lung ​cancer ​To ​measure ​biologically
effective ​dose ​Those ​with ​greater ​amounts o
​ f ​effective ​dose ​of ​polycyclic ​aromatic ​hydrocarbon
DNA ​adducts ​have ​experienced ​greater ​exposure ​and ​interaction ​of ​their ​DNA ​to
these ​carcinogenic hydrocarbonsa ​To ​ easure ​biological ​effect
m
Hi​gh​er ​levels ​of ​the ​ra​s ​oncogene ​product ​may ​be ​a ​preclinical ​marker ​o​f ​early
carcinogenic ​response
• ​Perera ​F ​et ​al​: ​Biologic ​markers ​in ​risk ​asse​ss​ment for ​environmental ​carcinogens​. ​Environ
Health ​Persp ​1991​; ​90​:​247​.
VARIABILITY ​AND ​BIAS ​/ ​149

and ​disease o ​ f ​interest.​ ​Confounding ​can ​be ​demon ​strated ​by ​the ​following
hypothetical ​example​. ​Suppose ​invest​i​gators ​undertake ​a ​case​-​control ​study ​of ​the
as ​sociation ​between ​high ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​level ​and risk ​of ​developing
myocardial ​infarction​. ​From ​the ​results ​of ​other ​studies​, ​the ​researchers ​know ​that ​the
risk ​of ​myocardial ​infarction ​is ​associated ​with ​o​besit​y​, ​and ​that ​total ​chol​esterol ​levels
also ​c​orrelate ​with ​ob​e ​sity ​(​see ​Figure ​10​–​8​)​. ​Suppose ​that ​in ​our ​hypothetical
case​-​control ​study​, ​36 ​of ​60 ​patients ​with ​myocardial
Finally​, ​biological ​markers ​can ​help ​provide ​insight ​into ​the ​underlying ​disease
process ​and ​pathogenesis​.
The ​use ​of ​levels ​of ​serum ​dioxin ​to me​asure ​exposu​re ​V ​of ​men ​who ​worke​d ​wi​th ​t​he ​herbicid​e
Agent ​Orange ​during ​the ​Vietnam W ​ ar ​illustrates ​the ​use ​of ​a ​biologi ​cal ​marker ​to ​measure
internal ​dose​. ​After ​the ​Vietnam ​War​, ​concern ​arose about ​wartime ​exposures ​of
service ​men ​to ​Agent ​Orange​, ​in ​part ​because ​of ​its ​contamina ​tion ​with ​the ​highly ​toxic
trace ​contaminant ​known ​as ​2​,​3​,​7​,​8​-​tetrachlorodibenzo​-​p-​ ​dioxin ​(​TCDD​)​. ​Because ​o​f
this ​concern​, ​Air ​Force ​researchers ​began ​epidemio ​logic ​studies ​to ​assess ​the ​health
effects among ​Air ​Force ​veterans associated ​with ​exposure ​to ​Agent ​Orange ​and
TCDD​. ​Researchers ​initially ​used ​job ​descriptions ​to ​classify ​exposure ​to ​TCDD​.
Later​, ​after ​laboratory ​tech ​niques ​became ​available ​to ​measure ​minute ​concen
trations ​of ​TCDD ​within ​the ​blood​, ​the ​researchers ​discovered ​that ​classification ​of
exposure ​based ​on ​job ​descriptions ​was ​associated ​with ​substantial ​misclassifi
cation​. ​In ​subsequent ​studies​, ​the ​more ​accurate ​serum​) ​TCDD ​measurements ​were
used ​to ​assess ​exposures​. V

Despite ​the ​importance ​of ​biological markers ​and ​the ​possibility ​that ​their ​use ​may
reduce ​information ​bias​, they ​do ​not ​eliminate ​the ​possibility ​of ​systematic ​errors​.
Although ​it ​may ​be ​diminished ​by ​employing ​a ​biological ​marker​, ​misclassification
remains ​a ​possi ​bility​. ​For ​e​xample​, ​marker ​instabilit​y a​nd ​inter​- ​or ​in ​traindividual
varia​bili​ty ​can ​contribute ​to ​measurement ​errors​. ​Moreover​, ​if ​required ​biological
specimens ​are ​collected ​after ​disease ​occurrence​, ​as ​often ​happens in ​case​-​control
studies​, ​the ​presence ​of ​disease ​in ​cases ​may ​affect ​the ​bio​logical ​marker​. ​This
possibility ​can ​make ​the ​biological ​marker ​particularly ​susceptible ​to d ​ ifferential
misclassification ​and ​measurement ​error​. ​Bias ​can ​even ​be ​created ​if ​the
investigator ​adjusts ​in a ​ ppropriately ​for ​a ​factor ​that ​is ​caused ​by ​the ​exposure ​of
interest ​and ​is ​associated ​with ​the ​outcome​.
Case​-​control ​studies ​of ​the relationship ​of ​B ​carotene ​and ​cancer ​illustrate ​the ​potential ​for
residual ​information ​bias​. ​B​-​Carotene ​is ​a fat​-​soluble ​antioxi ​dant ​found ​in ​many ​fruits
and ​vegetables​. ​It ​acts ​as ​a ​provitamin ​(​vitamin ​A​)​, ​protects ​against ​development ​of
cancer ​in ​animals​, ​and ​may ​reduce ​the ​risk ​of ​devel ​oping ​cancer ​in ​humans​. ​In ​a
case​-​control ​study ​of s​ erum ​levels ​of ​this ​antioxidant​, ​differential ​misclassi ​fication ​could
create ​or ​accentuate ​a protective ​effect​, ​if
cholesterol ​levels​, ​and ​only ​24 ​of ​60 ​controls ​(​40​%​) ​are ​discovered ​to ​have ​elevated
serum ​cholesterol ​levels​. ​This ​would s
​ uggest ​that ​elevated t​ otal ​serum ​choles
terol ​levels ​are ​associated ​with ​an i​ ncreased ​risk ​of ​de v​ eloping ​myocardial
infarction​.
When ​the ​observed ​association ​is ​examined ​sepa ​rately ​in ​obese ​and ​nonobese ​persons​,
however​, ​a ​dif ​ferent ​conclusion ​is ​reached​. ​Among obese ​persons​, ​34 ​of ​40 ​patients
with ​myocardial ​infarctio​n ​(​85​%​) ​and ​18 ​of 20 ​controls ​(​90​%​) ​are found ​to ​have
elevated ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​levels​. ​Among ​nonobese ​persons​, ​2 ​of ​20 ​patients
with ​myocardial ​infarction ​(​10​%​) ​and ​6 ​of ​40 ​controls ​(​15​%​) ​have ​high ​total ​serum
cho ​lesterol ​levels​. ​Thus ​in the ​case ​of ​both ​obese ​and ​nonobese ​individuals​,
elevated ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​levels are more ​common ​in ​controls ​than ​in
patients ​with ​myocardial ​infarction​. ​Keep ​in ​mind that ​in ​the ​hypothetical ​study​, ​obesity
was ​associated ​with ​myocar ​dial ​infarction​, ​since ​52 ​of ​60 obese ​subjects ​(​87​%​)
had ​elevated ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​levels​, ​and ​only ​8 ​of ​60 ​nonobese ​persons ​(​13​%​)
had ​high ​total ​serum ​cho lesterol ​levels​. ​Clear​ly​, ​in ​this ​hypoth​etic​al ​example​, ​the ​results
are ​con​founded ​by ​the ​ex​trane​ous variabl​e​, ​obesity​. ​The ​results are ​illustrated ​in ​Figure
10​-​9​.
For ​a ​variable​-​in ​this ​case​, ​obesity​—​to ​be ​con ​sidered ​a ​potential confounder​, ​it ​must
satisfy ​two ​conditions​:

(​1​) ​Association ​with the ​disease ​of ​interest ​in ​the


absence ​of ​exposure ​(​2​) ​Association ​with ​the ​exposure ​but ​not ​as ​a ​result
of ​being ​exposed​.

Obesity
tus ​and ​a ​resulting ​lowering ​of ​ß​-​carotene ​levels​. ​Although ​these ​biases ​are
somewhat ​speculative​, ​the ​potential ​for ​bias ​in ​case​-​control ​studies ​is ​evident​.
Thus​, ​use ​of ​biological ​markers ​offers ​many ​advan ​tages​, ​particularly ​an ​improved
assessment ​of ​exposure ​and ​a ​more ​homogeneous ​definition ​of ​disease​. ​Never ​theless,
because ​use ​of ​these ​markers ​does ​not ​elimi ​nate ​the ​possibility ​of ​information ​bias​,
caution ​in ​interpretation ​is ​still w
​ arranted​.
Total ​cholesterol
Myocardial ​infarction

Confounding
Confounding ​refers t​ o ​the ​mixing ​of t​ he e
​ ffect o
​ fa
​ n ​ ariable w
​ ​extraneous v ​ ith t​ he
effects ​of t​ he e
​ xposure
tween ​total ​serum ​cholesterol level ​and r​ isk ​of ​developing ​myocardial ​infarction​, ​with
confounding ​by ​obesity​.
150 ​/ CHAPTER ​10

All ​subjects

.​.​.​.​000​0 ​00000000 ​00000000 ​0000


MI

​ ooo ​No ​MI


00000000 ​000​00000 ​00000000 ​00000000 O

Obese
Nonobese ​.​.​0​0000 ​.​.​.​.​.​. ​.​.​.​.​. ​000000​00​000000 ​. ​.​0​0 ​1000000 ​0000000
OOOOOO

0000000
0000000 ​00000
​ 000 ​MI
00
No ​MI
MI
No ​MI
Figure ​10​-​9​. ​Illustration ​of ​the ​relationship ​between ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​level ​and ​risk
of ​developing ​myocardial ​in ​farction ​(​MI​)​, ​with ​confounding ​by ​obesity​. ​Shaded ​circles
represent ​persons ​with ​elevated ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​levels ​and ​unshaded circles ​represent ​persons
with ​normal ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​levels​.

SUMMARY
Because i​ t ​can ​be ​evaluated ​in ​the ​analysis ​of ​results​, ​confo​undin​g differs ​fr​om
selection ​bias ​and ​i​nforma ​tion ​bias​. ​The ​presence ​of ​confounding ​is ​demonstrated
by ​a ​change ​in ​the ​apparent ​strength ​of ​association ​be ​tween ​the ​exposure ​and ​the
disease ​of ​interest ​when ​the ​effects ​of e ​ xtraneous ​variables ​are ​taken ​into ​account​.
Confounding​, which ​is ​not ​an ​all​-​or​-​none ​property ​of ​an ​extraneous ​variable​, ​may ​occur ​to
different ​degrees ​in ​different ​studies​.
Generally​, ​the ​list o ​ f ​potential ​confounders ​in ​a ​study ​is ​limited ​to ​established ​risk
factors ​for ​the ​dis ​ease ​of ​interest​. ​There are ​two ​accepted ​me​t​hods ​for ​dealing
with ​potential ​confounders​. ​The ​first ​is ​to ​con ​sider ​them ​in ​the ​design ​of ​the ​study
by ​matching ​on ​the ​potential ​confounder ​or ​by ​restricting ​the ​sample ​to ​limited
levels ​of ​the ​potential ​confounder​. ​The ​other m ​ ethod ​is ​to ​evaluate ​confounding ​in
the ​ana​ly​sis ​by ​stratifi​ca​tion​, ​as ​demonstrated ​schematically ​in ​Figure 1 ​ 0​-​9​, ​or ​by
using ​mu​ltiv​ari​ate ​ana​lysi​s ​techn​ique​s ​such ​as ​multi​ple ​logis​tic ​regressio​n​.
The ​goal ​of ​any ​epidemiologic ​study ​is ​to ​provide ​a ​valid ​conclusion​. ​To ​accomplish ​this
objective​, ​com ​plete ​attention ​must ​be given ​to ​all ​aspects ​of ​the ​study​, ​from ​inception
to ​design ​and ​data ​collection ​and​, ​fi ​nally​, ​to ​analysis ​and reporting ​of ​results​. ​It ​is
impor ​tant ​to ​r​emem​ber ​that ​bias ​can ​be ​intro​duced ​at ​any ​of ​these ​stages​, ​leading ​to
erroneous ​results​. ​Thus​, ​it ​is ​useful ​to ​look ​care​fully ​for ​potential ​sources ​of ​bias ​and
to ​consider ​their ​possible ​impact​. ​Ç​linicia​ns ​must ​judge ​whether ​results ​can ​be
generalized ​to ​t​heir ​par ​ticular ​prac​t​ice​. ​Understanding ​the ​potential ​problems ​with
measurement ​and ​bias ​in ​medical ​research ​im ​proves ​the ​ability ​of ​physicians ​to
decide ​on ​appropri ​ate ​preventive a​ nd ​therapeutic ​str​ateg​ies​.
In this ​chapter​, ​the ​topics ​of ​variability ​and ​system ​atic ​errors ​in ​epidemiologic
measurements ​are ​dis ​cussed​, ​with ​illustrative ​examples ​that ​focus ​primarily ​on ​the
relationship ​between ​total ​serum ​cholesterol ​level ​and ​the ​risk ​of ​developing ​myocardial
infarction​. ​A ​distinction ​is ​drawn ​between ​random ​variation​, ​which ​is ​inversely ​related ​to
precision ​in ​measurement​, ​and ​nonrandom ​or ​systematic ​error​, ​which ​is ​related ​to ​a
distortion ​in ​measurement​.
Variability ​can ​arise ​from ​(1​) ​the ​subjects ​under ​study​, ​(​2​) ​differences ​between
individuals​, ​(​3​) ​the ​ap p
​ roach ​used ​to ​sample ​subjects​, ​or ​(​4​) ​the ​measurement
process ​itself​. ​Variability ​related ​to ​sampling ​is ​likely ​to ​diminish ​as ​the ​sample ​si​ze ​
increases​. ​With ​ex ​tremely large ​sample ​sizes​, ​a ​very ​small ​difference ​in ​outcome
between ​study ​groups ​can ​be ​statistically ​sig ​nificant​. ​Whether ​the ​magnitude ​of ​this
difference ​is ​sufficient ​to ​warrant ​a ​change ​in ​clinical ​practice ​is ​a ​separate​, but ​equally
important​, ​question​.
Validity ​concerns ​the ​extent t​ o ​which ​the f​ indings o ​ f ​a s​ tudy r​ eflect t​ ruth​. ​Internal ​validity
relates ​to ​the ​ac ​curacy ​of ​study ​findings ​for ​the ​persons ​who ​are ​in ​vestigated​.
External ​validity ​concerns ​the ​extent ​to ​which ​study ​findings ​accurately ​apply ​to
persons ​who ​are ​not ​studied​.
Bias ​is defined ​as ​lack ​of validity​. ​Conventionally​, ​bias ​is ​classified ​into ​three ​major
types​: ​selection ​bias​, ​information ​(​misclassification​) ​bias​, ​and ​confound ​ing​. ​Selection
bias ​refers ​to ​the ​introduction ​of ​sys ​tematic ​errors ​into ​study ​results ​through ​the
manner in ​which ​study ​subjects ​are ​selected​. ​Information ​bias ​re ​sults ​in
systematic ​errors in ​study ​findings ​that ​origi​-​-
VARIABILITY ​AND ​BIAS ​/ ​151

While ​in ​storage​, ​the ​specimens ​have d


​ eteriorated​, ​producing ​artificially ​low
homocysteine ​levels​.

3​. ​In ​a ​cohort ​study ​of ​hormone ​replacement ​therapy


and ​the ​risk ​of ​developing ​atherosclerotic ​coro ​nary ​artery ​disease​, ​high ​socioeconomic
status is a ​ ssociated ​with ​both ​use ​of hormone ​replacement ​therapy ​and ​the ​risk ​of
developing ​coronary ​artery ​disease​.

4​. ​In ​a ​case​-​control ​study ​of ​the ​relationship o


​ f
stressful ​life ​events ​and ​the ​occurrence ​of ​coro ​nary ​artery ​disease​, ​the ​cases ​are ​more
likely ​than ​the ​controls ​to ​overreport ​stressful ​events​.
nate ​in ​the ​approach ​to ​collecting ​information​. ​Two ​kinds ​of ​information ​bias ​can ​exist​.
Nondifferential ​mi​sclassification ​occurs ​when ​errors ​in ​the ​informa ​tion about ​one
variable ​are ​unrelated ​to ​the ​status ​of ​an ​other ​variable​. ​Differenti​al ​mi​sclassification​, ​on
the ​other ​hand​, ​occurs ​when ​errors ​in the information a ​ bout ​one ​variable ​are ​affected
by ​the ​status ​of ​another ​variable​.
Con​foundi​n​g ​is ​concerned ​with ​the ​mixing ​of ​the ​primary ​effect ​of ​interest ​with ​the ​effects ​of ​one
or ​more ​extraneous ​factors​. ​In ​experimental ​studies​, ​the ​problem ​of ​confounding ​is
reduced ​by ​randomiza ​tion​, ​which ​tends ​to ​balance ​the ​study ​groups ​with ​re ​spect
to ​both known ​and ​unknown ​determinants ​of ​the ​outcome​. ​In ​observational ​research​,
however​, s​ tudy groups ​may ​differ ​appreciably ​in ​factors ​that ​are ​(​1​) ​related ​to ​the ​risk
​ isease ​among ​unexposed ​persons ​and ​(​2​) ​are ​also ​associated ​with ​the ​exposure
of d
of i​ nterest​, ​but ​not ​as ​a ​result ​of ​being ​exposed​. ​Th​e ​influence ​of ​these ​potential
confounders ​can ​be ​ad ​dressed ​in ​the ​study ​design ​(​eg​, ​through ​matching ​or
restrictive ​inclusion ​criteria​) ​or ​in ​the ​analysis ​(​eg​, ​through ​stratification ​or
regression ​techniques​)​. ​Only ​known ​confounders ​can ​be ​addressed ​in ​observational
research​.
No ​study ​is ​immune ​from t​ he ​possibility ​of ​bias​. ​The ​investigator ​must therefore
consider ​potential ​sources ​of ​bias ​when sampling ​subjects​, ​collecting ​in ​formation​,
analyzing ​results​, ​and ​interpreting ​find ​ings​. ​With ​planning ​and ​forethought, ​it ​is
possible ​to ​anticipate ​and ​avoid ​certain ​types ​of ​error ​and ​thus ​conduct ​a ​study ​that
leads ​to ​a ​convincing ​and ​valid c​ onclusion​.
Que​sti​on​s ​5​-​8​: ​For ​each ​numbered ​situation ​below​, ​select ​from ​the ​following ​lettered ​options
the ​most ​likely ​effect ​on ​the ​study ​findings​. ​Each ​option ​can ​be ​used ​once​, ​more ​than
once​, ​or ​not ​at ​all​.

A​. ​Overestimation ​B​. ​Underestimation ​C​. ​No effect ​D​. ​Cannot ​be ​determined

5​. ​In ​a ​cohort ​study ​of ​the ​effects ​of ​birth ​asphyxia ​on
subsequent ​cognitive ​development​, ​the ​tests ​of ​cognitive ​performance ​are
administered ​by ​inves ​tigators ​who ​do n
​ ot ​know whether ​the ​subjects ​had ​birth ​asphyxia​.

6​. ​In ​a ​cohort ​study ​of ​obesity ​and the ​risk ​of ​devel
oping ​n​o​n​-​insulin​-​dependent ​diabetes ​mellitus​, ​the ​loss ​to ​follow​-​up i​ s ​greater ​for ​obese
compared ​to ​nonobese ​persons​.
STUDY ​QUESTIONS

Questions ​1​-​4​: ​For ​each ​numbered ​situation ​below​, ​select ​from ​the ​following
lettered ​options ​the ​type ​of ​error ​that ​would ​most likely ​result​. Each ​option ​can ​be ​used
once​, ​more ​than ​once​, ​or ​not ​at ​all​.
7​. ​In ​a ​case​-​control ​study ​of ​ingestion o
​ f ​L​-​trypto
phan ​as a ​risk ​factor ​for ​developing ​eosinophilia ​myalgia ​syndrome ​(​EMS​)​, ​women
with ​EMS ​tend ​to ​give ​more ​false​-​positive ​reports ​of ​using ​L​-​tryp ​tophan ​than ​do ​women
without ​EMS​.

A​. ​Selection ​bias ​B​. ​Nondifferential ​misclassification ​C. ​Differential ​misclassification ​D​.
Confounding ​E​. ​Ecologic ​fallacy ​F.​ ​Random ​error
8​. ​Patients ​with ​a ​history ​of ​myocardial i​ nfarction
within ​the ​past ​5 ​years ​are ​enrolled ​in ​a ​case ​control ​study ​of ​cigarette ​smoking ​as ​a
risk ​factor ​for developing ​a ​myocardial ​infarction​. ​Among ​patients ​who ​have ​had ​a
myocardial ​infarction ​the ​case​-​fatality ​is ​higher ​for ​smokers ​than ​for n
​ on ​smokers​.
1​. ​In ​a ​case​-​control ​study ​of t​ he ​relationship ​between
a ​cholesterol ​lowering ​drug ​and ​the ​risk ​of ​devel ​oping ​breast ​cancer​, ​control
subjects ​are ​sampled ​from ​participants ​in ​a ​hea​lth ​screening ​fair​.
Questions ​9​–​10​: ​For ​each numbered ​situation ​below​, s​ elect ​from ​the ​following ​lettered
options ​the ​most ​likely ​study ​design​. ​Each ​option ​can ​be ​used ​once​, ​more ​than ​once​, ​or ​not
at ​all​.
2​. ​In ​a ​case​-​control ​study ​of ​the ​relationship ​of ​serum ​!
concentrations ​of ​homocysteine ​to ​risk ​of ​develop ​ing ​coronary ​heart ​disease​, ​20​-​year​-​old
stored ​frozen ​serum ​specimens ​are ​used ​to ​measure ​ho ​mocysteine ​levels ​among ​cas​es
and ​controls​.
A​. ​Case​-​control ​study ​B​. ​Cohort ​study ​C​. ​Randomized ​clinical ​trial ​D​. E
​ cologic
(​correlation​) ​study
​ HAPTER ​10
152 ​/ C

9​. ​Population ​consumption ​of ​sodium ​and ​population


prevalence ​of ​hypertension ​are ​associated ​across ​populations ​without
being ​li​n​ked ​in ​individual ​subjects​.
10​. ​The ​ability ​to ​assign ​by ​chance ​the ​use ​of ​a ​cho
lesterol​-​lowering ​agent ​to ​individual ​subjects ​re ​duces ​the likelihood ​of
confounding ​in ​a ​study ​of ​risk of ​developing ​myocardial ​infarction​.

FURTHER ​READING

Rosenbaum ​PR​: ​Discussing ​hidden ​bias ​in ​observational


studies​. ​Ann ​Intern ​Med ​1991​;​115​:​90​.

REFERE​NC​ES

Patient ​Profile
Cleeman ​J​, ​Lenfant ​C​: ​T​he ​National ​Cholesterol
Education ​Program​. ​JAMA ​1998​;​280​:​2099​. ​National ​Heart​, ​Lung ​and ​Blood ​Institute​:
Recommen
dations f​ or I​ mproving Cholesterol M ​ easurement​s​: ​A ​Report f​ rom ​the ​Laboratory
Standardization P ​ anel ​of ​the ​National ​Cholesterol ​Education ​Program​. ​US ​Department
of Health ​a​n​d ​Human ​Services​. ​NIH ​Publication ​No​. ​90​-​2964​, ​1990​.
Selection ​Bias
Ellenberg ​JH​, ​Nelson ​KB​: ​Sample ​selection ​and ​the ​nat
ural ​history ​of ​disease​: S
​ tudies ​of ​febrile ​seizures​.
JAMA ​1980​:​243​:​1337​. ​Roberts ​RS​, ​Spitzer WO​, ​Delmore ​T ​et ​al​: ​Empirical
demonstration ​of ​Berkson​'​s ​bias​. ​J ​Chron ​Dis ​1978​; ​31​:​119​.

Variability ​Rela​ted ​to ​Measurement


Report ​of ​the ​National ​Cholesterol ​Education ​Program
Expert ​Panel ​on ​detection​, ​evaluation ​and ​treatment ​of ​high ​blood ​cholesterol ​in
adults​. ​Arch ​Intern ​Med ​1988​;​148​:​36​.
Information ​Bias
Coates ​RJ ​et ​al​: ​Cancer ​risk ​in ​relation ​to ​serum ​copper
​ iologic M
​ ulka ​BS​, ​Wilcosky ​TC​, ​Griffith ​JD​: B
levels​. ​Cancer ​Res ​1989​:​49​:​4353​. H ​ arkers i​ n
Epidemiolog​y​. ​Oxford ​University ​Press​, ​1990​. ​Perera ​F ​et ​al​: ​Biologic ​markers ​in ​risk
assessment ​for ​en
vironmental ​carcinogens​. E
​ nviron ​Health ​Persp 1991​; ​90​:​247​.
External ​Validity
Altman ​DG​, ​Bland ​JM​: ​Generalization ​and ​extrapolation​.
BMJ ​1998​;​317​:​409​. ​Lipid ​Research ​Clinic ​Program​: ​The ​Lipid ​Research
Clinic​'​s ​Coronary ​Primary ​Prevention ​Trial ​results​. 1. Reduction in incidence of coronary
heart disease. JAMA 1984,251:351.
Confounding
Weinberg CR: Toward a clearer definition of confound
i​n​g. Am J Epidemiol 1993;137:1.

e-PIDEMIOLOGY

Patient Profile
http​://​www.nhlbi.nih.gov/guidelines​/c​ holesterol​/
​ ww.americanheart.org/Heart_and_Stroke_A_Z_
chol_scr.pdf http​://w
Guide​/​chollev.html
External Validity
http​://​trochim.human.comell.edu/tutorial/war​d/
external.htm

Variability in Medica​l Research


http​://​www.bmj.com​/e
​ pidem​/​epid.4.shtml
S​election Bias
http​://​ www.facsnet.org/report_tools/guides_primers​/
epidemiology/chap3.html#selection_bias

Validity
http:​//​ www.usd.edu/-dstevens/ctrial.htm#ERROR
Information Bias​a
http:​//​www.facsnet.org/report_tools​/g
​ uides_primers​/
epidemiology/chap3.html#misclassification
Intern​al Validity
http​://​trochim.human.cornell.edu​/​kb/intval.htm
Confounding
http:​/​/www.facsnet.org​/​report_tools/guides_primers​/
epidemiology​/c​ hap3.html#confounding

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi