Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 8

Early Childhood Educ J

DOI 10.1007/s10643-014-0644-7

Effects of Toys on the Play Quality of Preschool Children:


Influence of Gender, Ethnicity, and Socioeconomic Status
Jeffrey Trawick-Smith • Jennifer Wolff •

Marley Koschel • Jamie Vallarelli

Ó Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Abstract This study examined the effects of nine toys on Introduction


the play of 60 3- and 4-year-old children in culturally
diverse preschool classrooms. The toys, which varied in Play is critical for the social and intellectual development
their features and intended uses, were selected from a list of young children (Vygotsky 1976). Several elements of
of those that were nominated by teachers and parents as play are related to learning in preschool classrooms. Play
being developmentally beneficial. Each toy was video activities that include make believe (Smilansky 1968), self-
recorded for 240 h during free play time in four different regulation (Bodrova and Leong 2007; Elias and Burke
classrooms. Researchers coded 828 two-minute segments 2002), problem solving (Ginsberg 2006), creative expres-
of children’s play with these toys using a play quality with sion (Lloyd and Howe 2003), and social interaction (Ho-
toys (PQT) rating instrument developed in a previous wes and Matheson 1992) have been associated with
investigation. Toys were found to vary significantly in their positive child outcomes. Play with these elements has been
impact on play quality. PQT scores were also found to vary referred to as ‘‘high quality,’’ because of its educational
for each toy depending on the gender, socioeconomic sta- benefits (Bergen 2001).
tus, and ethnicity of the child playing with it, and the length Previous research has examined a variety of factors that
of time it was available in the classroom. Implications for influence the quality of play in preschools (Trawick-Smith
selecting toys for classrooms and observing children’s play 1990, 1992, 1993; Trawick-Smith and Dzuirgot 2010a, b).
with them are presented. Yet there has been scant research on the effects of toys on
play, even though 90 % of young children’s play involves
Keywords Play  Toys  Preschool  Ethnicity  toys (Tizzard et al. 1976). Teachers have little empirical
Socioeconomic status  Gender evidence, then, to guide the important decisions they make
about which toys to provide in their classrooms.
The purpose of this study was to examine two interre-
lated questions about toys in preschool: (1) To what degree
do toys affect the quality of young children’s play? (2)
How do the effects of toys vary according to the novelty of
the toy or the gender, socioeconomic status (SES), and
J. Trawick-Smith (&)  J. Wolff  M. Koschel  J. Vallarelli
ethnicity of children playing with them?
Center for Early Childhood Education, CECE 216, Eastern
Connecticut State University, Willimantic, CT 06226, USA
e-mail: trawick@easternct.edu
J. Wolff Theoretical Foundations: What Are Toys and Why Are
e-mail: wolffje@my.easternct.edu They Important?
M. Koschel
e-mail: koschelm@my.easternct.edu We define a toy as any object—realistic or not, commercial
J. Vallarelli or natural—that children use in their play. In some parts of
e-mail: vallarellij@my.easternct.edu the world, sticks, stones, and mud are common toys. In the

123
Early Childhood Educ J

U.S., many children play with commercially-manufactured et al. 2009; Wolfgang et al. 2001). These building materials
materials. In the present study, we adhere to Brian Sutton- were found to prompt numerous symbolic activities in play
Smith’s (1986, 2009) classic theory that the toys children and the complexity of building was found to predict
choose are directly related to their culture. Toy preferences reading and mathematics abilities later in school.
and use will reflect the histories, world views, values, and Some toy research has compared the toy preferences of
gender role assignments of a child’s culture and family. For boys and girls. (Campenni 1999; Cherney et al. 2004;
this reason, the present investigation examines toys that Miller 1987). In these investigations, girls were found to
have been selected by parents and teachers—not special- prefer ‘‘female-stereotypic toys’’—dolls or dress up
ists. We compare toy effects on children of both genders clothes—whereas boys were more likely to choose ‘‘male-
and of diverse cultural and socioeconomic backgrounds. stereotypic toys,’’ such as toy cars and construction
Another underlying assumption of this study is that toys materials. Little research has been done on the impact of
provide two vital functions in play. First, they inspire, toys for children from diverse cultural or socioeconomic
maintain, and enrich play (Pellegrini and Bjorklund 2004). backgrounds. Ethnographic studies of children from
For example, a firefighter’s hat motivates children to ini- around the world indicate that toy use varies, depending
tiate in a new play theme; the introduction of a pretend fire on cultural traditions and resources available to families
hose extends and elaborates the play episode. Toys also (Whiting and Edwards 1988). In one study, children in
function to draw peers together into a shared play theme, Puerto Rico were found to prefer playing with blocks, toy
by helping them establish joint attention—a circumstance cars, musical instruments, and miniature people (Trawick-
when children all focus on the same object or activity Smith 2010). In another, African American children were
(Mundy and Newell 2007). In the present study, we found to be more ‘‘people oriented’’ and less ‘‘object
examine how toys influence the duration and complexity of oriented’’ in their play, using fewer toys and including
play activities and how well they inspire social interaction. more siblings and peers in their activities (Hale-Benson
1986).

Previous Toy Research


The Current Study
A small number of early studies focused on the impact of
toys on peer interactions. Blocks and pretend play props The current study was designed to address gaps in previous
were found to inspire more frequent and cooperative social toy research. Early studies have focused narrowly on the
activity, whereas ‘‘cognitively oriented’’ toys, such as influence of toys on a single aspect of play—social inter-
puzzles, were found to prompt solitary play (Hendrickson action, symbolic activity, or play preferences. In the pres-
et al. 1981; Ivory and McCollum 1999; Rettig et al. 1993; ent investigation we examined toy effects on play more
Rubin 1977). In contrast, Hughes and Carter (2002) found broadly, including all the features of play that have been
no differences between these social and solitary play found to enhance learning and development. We used a
materials in their effects on children’s social interactions measurement system to analyze the quality of play with
(2002). toys that captures children’s levels of thinking and problem
Other investigations explored the impact of toys on the solving, creative expression, social interaction and verbal-
symbolic level of children’s play (McLoyd 1983; Trawick- ization, and self-directed behavior. Since play is influenced
Smith 1990, 1993). In these studies, researchers asked: by culture and SES (Goncu et al. 2000; McLoyd 1986;
How often do children transform specific toys into things Riojas-Cortéz 2001; Trawick-Smith 2010), we measured
that are imaginary? The effects of highly realistic toys— how toys vary in their impact, depending on the back-
pretend play props, toy cars, or miniature people—were grounds of children playing with them; we also looked at
compared with nonrealistic ones—blocks, rubber shapes, how toy effects differ for boys and girls.
or paper towel rolls. The findings of these studies were Most previous research has been conducted in labora-
uniform: Younger preschoolers were found to perform their tory settings where only one or several toys were made
most frequent symbolic transformations with realistic toys. available to children at a time. We studied toys in authentic
Older preschoolers engaged in periodic symbolic actions preschool classrooms during naturalistic free play. Many
with non-realistic items, so long as realistic props were also prior studies examine common preschool toys—blocks or
available. Children age five and older engaged in highly pretend play props, in particular. Findings nearly always
symbolic play with nonrealistic objects, even when realistic confirm the benefits of these classic materials. In the
toys were not present. Other researchers examined the present investigation, we examined the effects of less-
symbolic level of children’s play with two common pre- studied toys—those that were nominated by teachers and
school construction toys—blocks and LEGOs (Hanline parents.

123
Early Childhood Educ J

Table 1 Toys studied and their descriptions and intended uses Child Participants
Toy Description/intended use
Toys were studied as they were being used by 60 3- and
Bristle blocks A set of multicolored plastic blocks covered with 4-year-old children (M = 48.8 months), who were of
tiny bristles that allow them to affix to one diverse backgrounds (34 were Latino, 19 Euro-American, 4
another to build objects and shapes
African American, 2 Jamaican, 1 Filipino; 32 female, 28
Duplo bricks A set of interlocking blocks in several colors that
can be used to build structures, vehicles, and male), and were enrolled in four classrooms of a preschool
other objects program located on a small university campus. SES of par-
Lincoln logs A set of small wooden logs and roof pieces that ticipants was determined by family eligibility for free or
are notched so they fit together to build reduced lunch; 22 child participants were eligible for free
structures lunch (low SES), 12 for reduced-price lunch (mid-low SES),
Measure up! A set of twelve multicolored nesting cups with and 26 did not qualify for this subsidy (middle SES). Each of
Cups numbers and animal images that can be used for
sorting, counting, stacking, and other play
the four classrooms had a teacher, (two with master’s
activities degrees, two with bachelor’s degrees), a teacher associate
Rainbow people A set of people-shaped, multicolored wooden (three had bachelor’s degrees, one an associate’s degree),
pieces that can be sorted, stacked next to or on and two assistant teachers (three with bachelor’s degrees,
top of each other, or used to carry out pretend five with no degree). All but two of these professionals were
play themes
of Euro-American background; 1 was Puerto Rican, 1 Ira-
Castle bucket set A set of sand toys that include a bucket that can
nian. The center is nationally accredited with a full day, play-
be used as a castle mold, a spinning sand/water
mill, a watering can, a shovel, a rake, and a based curriculum. This study was reviewed and approved by
scoop the institutional review board of the authors’ university. A
Shape, model A set of toys to be used with sculpting material, consent letter was distributed to and signed by a parent or
and mold such as Play-DohTM, including wooden legal guardian of each child participant. This included per-
stamping cubes, patterned rolling pins, and a
mission for the investigators to video record and analyze
wooden rolling cutting tool
children’s classroom behaviors.
Tree blocks A set of natural wooden blocks made from
reclaimed tree branches that can be stacked and
balanced to create various structures Procedures
Wooden train set A wooden train set that includes tracks and train
cars that can be used to pretend and to construct Each of the nine toys was video recorded in four classrooms,
various track configurations on 12 separate 20-min periods during free play time. A toy
was placed in one of the classrooms and recorded for 20 min
on the first, second, and fourth day that it was available to
children. (No recording was done on the third day, but it was
Method still provided during free play). After 4 days in one class-
room, the toy was moved into another classroom and
Toys Studied recorded for 20 min on each of 3 days. This continued until
each toy had been recorded in all four classrooms and a total
We invited nominations of toys for our study from parents of 240 min of play with it were captured. The play of all
and teachers through announcements at conferences, on a children who chose to play with these toys during these
website, and through an early childhood listserv. We observation periods was recorded. Recordings were made
requested the names of ‘‘indoor toys,’’—excluding com- using hidden cameras and microphones in the classrooms
puter applications, video, and books—that these adults that could be controlled from a remote site. Toys were placed
considered to be developmentally beneficial. From the first in the same type of play center in each classroom, as agreed
18 toys that were nominated, nine were chosen for study. upon by teachers prior to the beginning of the study.
The other nine toys were screened out by a selection Teachers followed a uniform process for briefly introducing
committee, comprised of a teacher, a parent, a researcher, each toy on the first day it was available and limiting their
and an administrator, because of concerns about safety or interactions with the toy to when children needed help.
suitability for use in a preschool classroom. The toys that
were chosen for the study are presented in Table 1. As Measures
shown in the table, the toys varied according to the type of
play they were intended to promote; some are common in Each toy was scored during each two-minute sequence of
preschools, some are not. recorded video using a play quality with toys instrument

123
Early Childhood Educ J

(PQT) developed in a previous study (Trawick-Smith et al. depending on how often children chose to play with them.
2010). The instrument was an eight-item, five-point So these frequencies provided a measure of how often each
observation rubric on which researchers scored the quality toy was chosen by children. Next, mean total scores on the
of children’s play with the toy during each of these two PQT instrument were computed for each toy. These were
minute clips. Using this tool, researchers assigned a score calculated by averaging each of the eight subscores for
of 1 (lowest coding) to 5 (highest) for each video segment each toy across all of the two minute segments in which it
for each of the following behaviors: (1) Thinking and was recorded. These mean scores provided a measure of
learning behaviors, such as studying and exploring objects, the quality of play with the toys that was independent from
displaying facial expressions of concentration, or com- how often they was used. Analysis of variance was used to
menting on new concepts or discoveries. (2) Problem examine differences in mean play quality scores across
solving behaviors, such as attempting to overcome chal- toys and categories of children who were playing with
lenging dilemmas or trying to complete difficult tasks. (3) them.
Curiosity and inquiry behaviors, such as asking questions
about the toy, showing facial expressions of puzzlement or
fascination, or engaging in exploratory behaviors. (4) Results
Sustained interest, such as persisting in play with the toy
with minimal distraction. (5) Creative expression, includ- Across all toys, 828 two-minute recorded play segments
ing articulation of unique ideas or using the toy in novel were analyzed. The frequency of segments examined for
ways. (6) Symbolic transformations, such as using of the each toy are presented in Table 2. As shown in the table,
toy to represent something completely different, engaging the frequency of recorded segments varied from 32 (for
in pretend play, or enacting imaginary actions or situations. Rainbow People) to 184 (for the Shape, Model, and Mold
(7) Interacting, communicating, and collaborating with set). These frequencies serve as a rough measure of how
peers, including conversations and cooperative levels of often each toy was chosen by children. Means and standard
play. (8) Autonomous play with the toy, including the deviations of overall play quality scores for the toys are
independent use of the toy without adult assistance or also presented in Table 2. Conducting an analysis of var-
expressions of frustration. The mean score across these iance, we found significant differences in score across the
eight items and all 20 min segments comprised the overall nine toys, F (8, 4,512) = 27.26, p \ .001. Tukey post hoc
PQT score for each toy. tests revealed the major sources of these differences: Two
In previous research, interrater reliability coefficients for toys scored significantly higher than all others on the PQT.
the instrument were found to range from .81 to .89. A For Duplo Bricks, mean differences with all other toys
validity study indicated that items on the tool were inter- ranged from .24 to 1.03, all statistically significant at the
related and discriminated among toys in a way that was .05 level. For Rainbow People, these mean differences
consistent with previous research (Trawick-Smith et al. ranged from .28 to 1.07, also statistically significant,
2010). In the current study, 50 % of recorded video was p \ .05. Another toy, Tree Blocks, scored significantly
independently coded by a second researcher to evaluate lower than all other toys, with mean differences ranging
interrater reliability. Reliability coefficients for the two from -.27 to -1.07, p \ .05 for all comparisons. For all
raters were high (r ranged from .78 to .92 across items, other pairs of toys, mean differences varied from positive
with a mean of .87). to negative, but none were significant. So, two toys were
If more than one child was playing with a toy in a two more likely to inspire high quality play; one was less likely
minute segment (a very common circumstance), a separate to do so.
set of scores was assigned to the toy for each child who was Additional analyses showed that toys’ scores did not
using it. So, a video segment for a toy could be coded vary significantly across classrooms. Too, no significant
several times—once for each child who was playing with differences in play quality were found for toys when used
it. This allowed us to examine the impact of toys on by children of different ages, reflecting the narrow age
individual children of diverse backgrounds and to assign range of participants studied. A number of significant
distinct scores for a toy, even when it was used in very interaction effects were discovered between toys and other
different ways by children during the same observation. child characteristics. A 2 (gender) 9 9 (toys) interaction
The units of analysis for this study were the coded two- was found, F (8, 3,286) = 8.301, p \ .001. Table 2 pre-
minute video segments for each of the nine toys. We first sents means and standard deviations on play quality scores
tallied the frequencies of two minute segments for each for toys when being used by children of each gender, based
toy. Across all toys, 828 of these segments were counted. on 376 coded video segments for girls and 452 for boys. As
Since toys were studied in naturalistic classroom settings, shown in the table, five toys scored higher when boys
the number of segments examined for each toy varied, played with them. Only one toy, Tree Blocks, received

123
Early Childhood Educ J

Table 2 Means and standard deviations for PQT scores, frequency Latino or Euro-American backgrounds, and those of low, mid-low or
and percentage of two minute video segments for each toy and mean middle SES backgrounds are playing with them
scores and standard deviations for when boys and girls, those of
Toy PQT Scores Frequency Gender Ethnicity SES Day observed
M SD N % Gender M SD Ethnicity M SD SES M SD Day M SD

Bristle blocks 3.71 1.36 44 5.4 F 2.93 1.46 Latino 3.84 1.29 Low 3.13 1.64 1 3.42 1.62
M 4.12 1.12 Euro-Am NOa Mid NOa 2 4.62 .49
High 3.92 1.24 4 3.52 1.40
Duplo bricks 4.10 1.16 113 13.5 F 3.32 1.37 Latino 4.05 1.17 Low 3.97 1.14 1 4.34 1.07
M 4.25 .98 Euro-Am 4.01 1.06 Mid 3.95 1.14 2 4.19 1.05
High 3.84 1.32 4 3.74 1.40
Lincoln logs 3.53 1.23 48 5.8 F 2.96 1.29 Latino 3.61 1.22 Low 3.50 1.01 1 3.51 1.21
M 3.72 1.15 Euro-Am 3.50 1.09 Mid 3.62 1.26 2 3.78 1.37
High 3.74 1.27 4 3.50 1.23
Measure up cups 3.67 1.18 96 11.6 F 3.40 1.17 Latino 3.97 1.10 Low 3.38 1.20 1 3.84 1.33
M 4.03 1.09 Euro-Am 3.38 1.21 Mid 3.41 1.18 2 3.51 1.25
High 3.90 1.07 4 3.50 .97
Rainbow people 4.08 1.22 32 3.9 F 3.41 1.10 Latino 4.38 1.02 Low 3.13 1.24 1 4.53 .93
M 4.33 1.17 Euro-Am 3.28 1.37 Mid 3.44 1.03 2 3.32 1.38
High 4.00 1.34 4 NOa
Sand castle buckets 3.70 1.20 72 8.7 F 3.64 1.23 Latino 3.57 1.19 Low 3.75 1.22 1 3.69 1.26
M 3.76 1.17 Euro-Am 3.83 1.28 Mid 3.52 1.27 2 3.68 1.41
High 3.69 1.20 4 2.79 1.52
Shape model mold 3.30 1.22 184 22.2 F 3.22 1.18 Latino 3.43 1.20 Low 2.86 1.28 1 3.47 1.28
M 3.35 1.24 Euro-Am 3.17 1.23 Mid 3.32 1.24 2 2.99 1.34
High 3.73 1.05 4 3.58 1.22
Tree blocks 2.90 1.28 60 7.2 F 3.18 1.29 Latino 2.58 1.23 Low 2.56 1.25 1 3.00 1.35
M 2.83 1.28 Euro-Am 3.91 1.06 Mid 2.50 1.19 2 3.32 1.31
High 3.17 1.27 4 2.77 1.66
Wooden train set 3.85 1.03 180 21.7 F 3.68 1.03 Latino 3.91 1.01 Low 3.87 1.00 1 3.88 1.07
M 3.96 1.01 Euro-Am 3.78 1.06 Mid 3.91 1.01 2 3.90 1.09
High 3.78 1.05 4 3.71 1.21
a
Not observed during observation periods

higher scores when they were used by girls. Three toys Model, and Mold set, mean difference = .30, and higher
received nearly equivalent scores for both genders. scores of Euro-American children when using Tree Blocks,
Ethnicity and PQT were examined by comparing scores mean difference = 1.33.
for each toy when Latino versus Euro-American children Play quality scores for toys differed significantly,
were playing with them. (There were too few African depending on the SES of children playing with them.
American, Jamaican, and Filipino participants in the study to Overall, toy scores were significantly lower when used by
examine other ethnic groups.) A significant 2 (ethnicity) 9 9 children of low SES, compared to mid-low and middle SES
(toys) interaction effect was found, F (18, 14,718) = 21.86. levels, F (2, 2,891) = 15.87, p \ .001. This analysis was
This analysis was based on 545 two minute segments with based on 268 coded two minute segments when children of
Latino children playing with toys and 219 segments when low SES were playing with them, 204 segments for chil-
used by Euro-American children. Means and standard dren of middle-low SES, and 344 for children of middle
deviations of play quality scores for toys, when used by SES. However, a significant 3 (SES) 9 9 (Toy) interaction
children of these ethnic backgrounds, are presented in effect was found, F (15, 2,868) = 5.00, p \ .001. Means
Table 2. As shown in the table, the sources of this interaction and standard deviations of PQT scores for each toy and for
are the higher play quality scores for Latino children, when each level of SES are presented in Table 2. As shown in
playing with Measure Up Cups, mean difference = .59, the table, scores for one toy were approximately equivalent
Rainbow People, mean difference = 1.10, the Shape, regardless of the SES of children playing with it (Duplo

123
Early Childhood Educ J

bricks). Other toys scored higher when children of low or the quality of the play they engage in when using it. In
middle-low SES were using them (sand castle buckets and other words, children are not necessarily drawn to materials
wooden trains), and other toys scored higher when used by that are most beneficial for play development. For example,
those of middle SES (Bristle Blocks, Lincoln Logs, Mea- one of our highest scoring toys, Rainbow People, was less
sure Up Cups, Rainbow People, and Tree Blocks). frequently chosen in free play than all other toys. In con-
Table 2 shows that scores for toys varied depending on trast, a toy with only moderate play quality scores—the
how many days they had been available to children in the Shape, Model, and Mold set—was selected often by chil-
classroom. A significant 3 (day studied) 9 9 (toy) inter- dren. This suggests that teachers should observe, not only
action was found, F (15, 14,722) = 28.806, p \ .001. As what children like to play with, but what they do with toys
shown in the table, Duplo bricks, rainbow people, measure once they begin playing. When using a toy, do children
up cups, and sand castle buckets received their highest play show thinking and problem solving behaviors, social
quality scores on the first day they were observed; bristle interaction and language, and creative and autonomous
blocks, Lincoln logs, tree blocks, and the wooden trains play?
received their highest play quality scores on the second day Our study suggests that boys and girls respond to the same
they were available to children. Scores for all of these toys toys in different ways. Gender differences in play are not
declined to varying degrees on by the fourth day they were surprising; these have been reported in previous research
in the classroom. The only exception was for the Shape, (Campenni 1999; Cherney et al. 2004; Miller 1987). What is
Model, and Mold set that received its highest scores on the unique about our findings is that it was not just toy preference
fourth day. It is important to note that some toys that that was different for boys and girls, but the quality with
received lower scores on the fourth day in classrooms still which each gender played with toys. Some toys inspired
maintained relatively high play quality ratings throughout higher quality play for girls, others for boys. A cause for
all observation periods. This suggests that some toys con- concern is that the majority of toys that we studied—all
tinue to inspire children even after several days. nominated by teachers and parents—received their highest
scores when used by boys. This suggests that classroom
observation of toy use should focus on identifying toys that
Discussion equally inspire beneficial play for both boys and girls.
Teachers may need to add or replace toys in a classroom in
Our findings suggest that toys make a difference in children’s order to provide a better balance of materials that inspire play
play. Some of the toys that we studied prompted very high activities for both genders. In addition teachers might watch
quality play, others did not. An implication is that careful for toys that promote more mixed-gender play, which was
thought should go into selecting toys for a classroom. The found to be less frequent in our study.
toys provided may have as great an impact on children’s Based on our findings, teachers should also attend to
development as planned group time activities and other ethnic and SES differences in the use of toys. We found
elements of the curriculum. Observation of toy effects is that some toys scored higher when Latino children played
important in order to identify those materials that promote with them, others when used by Euro-American children.
meaningful play for a particular classroom of children. These likely reflect cultural differences in family play
In our study, Duplo bricks and Rainbow people inspired experiences, social and thinking styles, and even world
higher quality play behaviors than other toys. These two views (Goncu et al. 2000; Trawick-Smith 2010). Similar
materials share common characteristics. They are both rela- data were obtained for children of varying socioeconomic
tively non-realistic, so they do not suggest any one particular groups. Several toys received their highest scores when
play theme. Rainbow People, for example, are simple, mul- children of low SES status used them, others when children
ticolored human figures without features. They can represent of middle SES children were playing. Children of color and
make believe people of any gender or can be sorted or stacked those living in poverty have long been reported to have
like construction toys. Duplos are even more open-ended in deficits in play abilities (Doyle et al. 1991; Saltz et al.
their uses. Children were observed creating buildings, people, 1977; Smilansky 1968). Our findings suggest that these
animals, and pretend play props (e.g., a cell phone). Our play differences may be, in part, a function of the toys
observations show that these toys allow many different kinds available to children. If children of low SES were assessed
of play, particularly construction and pretend play. These while playing with sand castle buckets or toy trains—toys
findings are consistent with previous research on more con- that received higher PQT scores when used by low SES
ventional, classic toys with these same qualities, such as children—would these same conclusions about play defi-
hardwood blocks (Trawick-Smith et al. 2010). cits be reached? Observations will assist teachers in iden-
Our findings also indicate that the frequency with which tifying toys that match the play needs and interests of all
children choose to play with a toy is not always related to children. Our findings suggest that such toys do exist. Both

123
Early Childhood Educ J

Duplo bricks and a toy train set received equally high Cherney, I. D., Kelly, V. L., Glover, K. G., Ruane, A., & Ryalls, B. O.
scores when used by children of lower and higher SES (2004). The effects of stereotyped toys and gender on play-based
assessment in 18–48 months old children. Educational Psychol-
groups and by those of diverse cultural backgrounds. ogy, 23, 95–106.
That toys vary in their impact based on culture and SES Doyle, A., Ceschin, R., & Tessier, O. (1991). The relation of age and
also has implications for working with families. Teachers social class factors in children’s social pretend play to cognitive
might interview family members about toys that hold cul- and symbolic ability. International Journal of Behavioral
Development, 14, 395–410.
tural meaning. They might inquire about family play Elias, C. L., & Berk, L. E.(2002). Self-regulation in young children: Is
experiences that are enjoyed at home and even the types of there a role for sociodramatic play? Early Childhood Research
play materials that they, parents and grandparents, used Quarterly, 17, 216–238.
when they were young. Teachers can also share with Ginsberg, H. (2006). Mathematical play and playful mathematics: A
guide for early education. In D. Singer, R. M. Golinkoff, & K.
families their observations of children’s toy use at school. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.) Play = learning: How play motivates and
This information would be very helpful in guiding the enhances children’s cognitive and social-emotional growth (pp.
selection of toys to promote high quality play at home. 145-165). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Most of the toys we studied inspired the highest Goncu, A., Mistry, J., & Mosier, C. (2000). Cultural variations in the
play of toddlers. International Journal of Behavioral Develop-
quality and frequency of play on the first or second day ment, 24, 321–329.
that they were available to children. This finding is Hale-Benson, J. E. (1986). Black children: Their roots, culture, and
consistent with other investigations showing that novel learning styles. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
toys are more attractive to children (Hunter et al. 1982; Hanline, M. F., Milton, S., & Phelps, P. C. (2009). The relationship
between preschool block play and reading and math abilities in
Rabinowitz et al. 1975; Shutts et al. 2010). A unique early elementary school: A longitudinal study of children with
finding in our study is that novelty not only drew children and without disabilities. Early Child Development and Care,
to toys, but inspired higher quality play with them. 189, 117–147.
Several toys maintained relatively high play quality Hendrickson, J., Tremblay, A., Strain, P., & Shores, R. (1981).
Relationship between toy and material use and the occurrence of
scores even on the fourth day they were available. Some social interactive behaviors by normally developing preschool
toys, then, may not lose their power to promote play as children. Psychology in the Schools, 18, 500–504.
readily as others. For example, scores for Duplo bricks, Howes, C., & Matheson, C. C. (1992). Sequences in the development
Lincoln Logs, Measure Up Cups, and the wooden train of competent play with peers: Social and social pretend play.
Developmental Psychology, 28, 961.
set declined only minimally on day four in each class- Hughes, C., & Carter, M. (2002). Toys and materials as setting events
room. A goal of classroom observation should be iden- for the social interaction of preschool children with special
tifying toys that have what Prescott et al. (1972) call needs. Educational Psychology, 22, 429–444.
‘‘holding power’’—the ability to engage children in Hunter, M. A., Ross, H. S., & Ames, E. W. (1982). Preferences for
familiar or novel toys: Effect of familiarization time in 1-year-
elaborate play over time. Too, teachers might use strat- olds. Developmental Psychology, 18, 519.
egies to keep play materials fresh—rotating toys in and Ivory, J. J., & McCollum, J. A. (1999). Effects of social and isolate
out and sharing materials across classrooms, so that there toys on social play in an inclusive setting. Journal of Special
are always novel materials to choose from. Education, 32, 238–243.
Lloyd, B., & Howe, N. (2003). Solitary play and convergent and
The importance of our findings is not simply identifying divergent thinking skills in preschool children. Early Childhood
specific toys that enhance play. There are broader impli- Research Quarterly, 18, 22–41.
cations in regard to the properties of toys that inspire high McLoyd, V. C. (1983). The effects of structure of play objects on the
quality play and variations in how toys impact play for pretend play of low income children. Child Development, 54,
626–635.
diverse classrooms of children. The most critical conclu- McLoyd, V. (1986). Scaffolds or shackles? The role of toys in
sion from our study is that teachers must be observant, preschool children’s pretend play. In G. Fein & M. Rivkin
reflective, and responsive to individual children’s needs as (Eds.), Young child at play: Reviews of research (pp. 63–78).
they equip their classrooms with toys, just as they are in all Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of
Young Children.
other aspects of teaching. Miller, C. L. (1987). Qualitative differences among gender–stereo-
typed toys: Implications for cognitive and social development in
girls and boys. Sex Roles, 16, 473–487.
Mundy, P., & Newell, L. (2007). Attention, joint attention, and social
cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16,
References 269–274.
Pellegrini, A. D., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2004). The ontogeny and
Bergen, D. (2001). The role of pretend play in children’s cognitive phylogeny of children’s object and fantasy play. Human Nature,
development. Early Childhood Research and Practice, 4, 1–12. 15, 23–43.
Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (2007). Tools of the mind. Upper Saddle Prescott, E., Kritchevsky, S., & Jones, E. (1972). The day care
River, NJ: Pearson. environmental inventory. Pasadena, CA: Pacific Oaks College.
Campenni, C. E. (1999). Gender stereotyping of children’s toys: A Rabinowitz, F. M., Moely, B. E., Finkel, N., & McClinton, S. (1975).
comparison of parents and nonparents. Sex Roles, 40, 121–138. The effects of toy novelty and social interaction on the

123
Early Childhood Educ J

exploratory behavior of preschool children. Child Development, Trawick-Smith, J. (1992). A descriptive study of spatial arrangement
46, 286–289. in a family day care home. Child & Youth Care Forum, 21,
Rettig, M., Kallam, M., & McCarthy-Salm, K. (1993). The effect of 356–397.
social and isolate toys on the social interactions of preschool- Trawick-Smith, J. (1993). Effects of realistic, non-realistic, and
aged children. Education and Training in Mental Retardation, mixed-realism play environments on young children symboliza-
28, 252–256. tion, social interaction, and language. Paper presented at the
Riojas-Cortéz, M. (2001). Preschoolers’ funds of knowledge dis- annual meeting of the American Educational Research Associ-
played through sociodramatic play episodes in a bilingual ation, Atlanta.
classroom. Early Childhood Education Journal, 29, 157–170. Trawick-Smith, J. (2010). Drawing back the lens on play: A frame
Roskos, K. A., Christie, J. F., Widman, S., & Holding, A. (2010). analysis of young children’s play in Puerto Rico. Early
Three decades in: Priming for meta-analysis in play-literacy Education and Development, 21, 1–32.
research. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 10, 55–96. Trawick-Smith, J., & Dzuirgot, T. (2010a). ‘‘Good-fit’’ teacher–child
Rubin, K. H. (1977). The social and cognitive value of preschool toys play interactions and subsequent autonomous play in preschool.
and activities. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 9, Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, 110–123.
382–385. Trawick-Smith, J., & Dzuirgot, T. (2010b). Untangling teacher–child
Saltz, E., Dixon, D., & Johnson, J. (1977). Training disadvantaged play interactions: Do teacher education and experience influence
preschoolers on various fantasy activities: Effects on cognitive ‘‘good-fit’’ responses to children’s play? Journal of Early
functioning and impulse control. Child Development, 48, Childhood Teacher Education, 31, 1–24.
367–380. Trawick-Smith, J., Russell, H., & Swaminathan, S. (2010). Measuring
Shutts, K., Banaji, M. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2010). Social categories the effects of toys on the cognitive, creative, and social play
guide young children’s preferences for novel objects. Develop- behaviors of preschool children. Early Child Development and
mental Science, 13, 599–610. Care, 181, 909–927.
Smilansky, S. (1968). The effects of sociodramatic play on disad- Vygotsky, L. S. (1976). Play and its role in the mental development of
vantaged preschool children. New York, NY: Wiley. the child. In J. Bruner, A. Jolly, & K. Sylva (Eds.), Play: Its role
Sutton-Smith, B. (1986). Toys as culture. New York, NY: Gardner in development and evolution (pp. 6–18). New York, NY: Basic
Press. Books.
Sutton-Smith, B. (2009). The ambiguity of play. Cambridge, MA: Whiting, B. B., & Edwards, C. P. (1988). Children of different worlds.
Harvard University Press. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tizzard, B., Phelps, J., & Plewis, L. (1976). Play in preschool centres: Wolfgang, C. H., Standard, L., & Jones, I. (2001). Block play
Play measures and their relation to age, sex, and IQ. Journal of performance among preschoolers as a predictor of later school
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 251–264. achievement in mathematics. Journal of Research in Childhood
Trawick-Smith, J. (1990). Effects of realistic vs. non-realistic play Education, 15, 173–180.
materials on young children’s symbolic transformation of
objects. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 5, 27–36.

123

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi