Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 10

Research

JAMA Pediatrics | Original Investigation

Associations Between Screen-Based Media Use


and Brain White Matter Integrity in Preschool-Aged Children
John S. Hutton, MS, MD; Jonathan Dudley, PhD; Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus, PhD; Tom DeWitt, MD; Scott K. Holland, PhD

IMPORTANCE The American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) recommends limits on


screen-based media use, citing its cognitive-behavioral risks. Screen use by young children is
prevalent and increasing, although its implications for brain development are unknown.

OBJECTIVE To explore the associations between screen-based media use and integrity of
brain white matter tracts supporting language and literacy skills in preschool-aged children.

DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS This cross-sectional study of healthy children aged
3 to 5 years (n = 47) was conducted from August 2017 to November 2018. Participants were
recruited at a US children’s hospital and community primary care clinics.

EXPOSURES Children completed cognitive testing followed by diffusion tensor imaging (DTI),
and their parent completed a ScreenQ survey.

MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES ScreenQ is a 15-item measure of screen-based media use
reflecting the domains in the AAP recommendations: access to screens, frequency of use,
content viewed, and coviewing. Higher scores reflect greater use. ScreenQ scores were
applied as the independent variable in 3 multiple linear regression models, with scores in 3
standardized assessments as the dependent variable, controlling for child age and household
income: Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing, Second Edition (CTOPP-2; Rapid
Object Naming subtest); Expressive Vocabulary Test, Second Edition (EVT-2; expressive
language); and Get Ready to Read! (GRTR; emergent literacy skills). The DTI measures
included fractional anisotropy (FA) and radial diffusivity (RD), which estimated
microstructural organization and myelination of white matter tracts. ScreenQ was applied as
a factor associated with FA and RD in whole-brain regression analyses, which were then
narrowed to 3 left-sided tracts supporting language and emergent literacy abilities.

RESULTS Of the 69 children recruited, 47 (among whom 27 [57%] were girls, and the mean
[SD] age was 54.3 [7.5] months) completed DTI. Mean (SD; range) ScreenQ score was 8.6
(4.8; 1-19) points. Mean (SD; range) CTOPP-2 score was 9.4 (3.3; 2-15) points, EVT-2 score was
113.1 (16.6; 88-144) points, and GRTR score was 19.0 (5.9; 5-25) points. ScreenQ scores were
negatively correlated with EVT-2 (F2,43 = 5.14; R2 = 0.19; P < .01), CTOPP-2 (F2,35 = 6.64;
Author Affiliations: Division of
R2 = 0.28; P < .01), and GRTR (F2,44 = 17.08; R2 = 0.44; P < .01) scores, controlling for child General and Community Pediatrics,
age. Higher ScreenQ scores were correlated with lower FA and higher RD in tracts involved Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical
with language, executive function, and emergent literacy abilities (P < .05, familywise Center, Cincinnati, Ohio (Hutton,
error–corrected), controlling for child age and household income. Horowitz-Kraus, DeWitt); Reading
and Literacy Discovery Center,
CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE This study found an association between increased Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical
Center, Cincinnati, Ohio (Hutton,
screen-based media use, compared with the AAP guidelines, and lower microstructural Dudley, Horowitz-Kraus, DeWitt,
integrity of brain white matter tracts supporting language and emergent literacy skills in Holland); Pediatric Neuroimaging
prekindergarten children. The findings suggest further study is needed, particularly during Research Consortium, Cincinnati
the rapid early stages of brain development. Children's Hospital Medical Center
and University of Cincinnati College
of Medicine, Cincinnati, Ohio (Dudley,
Horowitz-Kraus, Holland);
Educational Neuroimaging Center,
Biomedical Engineering, Technion,
Israel (Horowitz-Kraus); Medpace Inc,
Cincinnati, Ohio (Holland).
Corresponding Author: John S.
Hutton, MS, MD, Division of General
and Community Pediatrics, Cincinnati
Children's Hospital Medical Center,
3333 Burnet Ave, MLC 15008,
JAMA Pediatr. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.3869 Cincinnati, OH, 45229, (john1.hutton
Published online November 4, 2019. @cchmc.org).

(Reprinted) E1
© 2019 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Western Sydney University User on 11/04/2019


Research Original Investigation Associations Between Screen Use and White Matter Integrity in Preschoolers

I
n a single generation, through what has been described
as a vast “uncontrolled experiment,” 1 the landscape Key Points
of childhood has been digitized, affecting how children
Question Is screen-based media use associated with differences
play, learn, and form relationships. In addition to traditional in the structural integrity of brain white matter tracts that support
programming, rapidly emerging technologies, particularly language and literacy skills in preschool-aged children?
portable electronic devices, provide unprecedented ac-
Findings In this cross-sectional study of 47 healthy
cess to a wide range of media. 2,3 Use begins in infancy 4
prekindergarten children, screen use greater than that
and increases with age, and it was recently estimated at recommended by the American Academy of Pediatrics guidelines
more than 2 hours per day in children younger than 9 was associated with (1) lower measures of microstructural
years, aside from use during childcare and school.3 Accom- organization and myelination of brain white matter tracts that
panying this rise are variables with potential risks and ben- support language and emergent literacy skills and (2)
efits, including access to screens (eg, in bedrooms), fre- corresponding cognitive assessments.
quency of use, content, and grownup-child interaction Meaning These findings suggest a need for further study into the
(eg, coviewing). 5,6 The American Academy of Pediatrics association between screen-based media use and the developing
(AAP) recommends limits on screen-based media, citing brain, particularly during early childhood.
developmental and health risks with excessive and inop-
portune use. 3 These risks include language delay 7, 8 ;
poor sleep2,9 ; impaired executive function10 and general
cognition11; and decreased parent-child engagement, includ- Methods
ing reading together. 12-14 The World Health Organization
recently released even more restrictive recommendations Participants
for children younger than 5 years, discouraging screen time A total of 69 parent-child dyads were recruited through adver-
and advocating greater study of its implications for health tisement at a children’s medical center and surrounding primary
and development.15 care clinics. Inclusion criteria were as follows: children aged 3 to
Recent evidence suggests that screen-based media use 5 years, born at at least 36 weeks’ gestation, living in a household
poses neurobiological risks in children,16-19 yet its associa- of native English speakers, without a history of neurodevelop-
tions with early brain development are largely unknown, mental disorder conferring risk of language delays, no previous
particularly during the dynamic span of development be- or current kindergarten attendance, and no contraindications to
fore kindergarten. 10 Although sensory networks mature magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), such as metal implants. Writ-
relatively early,11 those sensory networks for higher-order ten informed consent was obtained from a custodial parent, and
skills, such as language,12 executive function,20,21 multi- families were compensated for their time and travel expenses.
modal association, 13 and reading, 22,23 exhibit protracted This study was approved by the institutional review board of Cin-
development11,14 and are dependent on constructive stimu- cinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center and was conducted
lation in the home. Specifically, the organization and from August 2017 to November 2018.
myelination of white matter tracts, which enhance the effi-
ciency of signal conduction within and between these net- Screen-Based Media Use Assessment
works, are highly sensitive to environmental factors.21,24-26 Research coordinators administered the ScreenQ35 survey to a
Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) is a powerful means to custodial parent in a private room before or during the child’s MRI,
quantify white matter integrity in the brain and its various and responses were entered into a REDCap (Research Electronic
factors.27 Parameters of DTI include fractional anisotropy Data Capture) database.36 ScreenQ is a recently developed,
(FA) and radial diffusivity (RD), scalar values associated with 15-item composite measure of screen-based media use of chil-
microstructural organization (eg, bundling, packing), and dren aged 3 to 9 years, reflecting the AAP recommendations for
myelination of white matter tracts.28 The aim of this study this age range.5,6 The conceptual model for ScreenQ is derived
was to use DTI to explore the association between composite from the 4 domains of the AAP recommendations: access to
screen-based media use in the context of the domains screens, frequency of use, content viewed, and interactivity or
cited in the AAP recommendations5 (access, frequency, con- coviewing. A preliminary version of the ScreenQ was tested and
tent, and coviewing) and the indexes of white matter integ- psychometrically refined in a previous study.37 Psychometric
rity in preschool-aged children, particularly major tracts analyses for the current version included modern-theory Rasch
involved with language, executive functions, and emergent methods and were favorable, including a range of item difficulty,
literacy (arcuate fasciculus, inferior longitudinal fasciculus, reliability, internal consistency (Cronbach α = .74), and criterion-
and uncinate fasciculus).29-34 Given the evidence of risks related validity referenced to external standards of child cogni-
associated with screen time,5,7 we administered assessments tive skills and home cognitive environment (StimQ-P38).35 The
of expressive language, speed of processing, and emergent ScreenQ scoring range was 0 to 26 points, with higher scores re-
literacy skills to serve as cognitive-behavioral correlates. flecting greater use than the AAP recommendations.
Our hypothesis was that higher use would be associated
with lower integrity in these tracts (ie, lower FA and Behavioral Measures and Analyses
higher RD) and with lower scores on corresponding cognitive Three standardized assessments of language and emergent
measures. literacy abilities were administered to the children before

E2 JAMA Pediatrics Published online November 4, 2019 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

© 2019 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Western Sydney University User on 11/04/2019


Associations Between Screen Use and White Matter Integrity in Preschoolers Original Investigation Research

their MRI: (1) Expressive Vocabulary Test, Second Edition Tract-Based Spatial Statistics
(EVT-2), a norm-referenced measure for children aged 2.5 We performed 2 analyses using FSL’s tract-based spatial
years or older39; (2) Comprehensive Test of Phonological statistics47 pipeline to test for associations between DTI mea-
Processing, Second Edition (CTOPP-2), a comprehensive, sures and ScreenQ scores: (1) whole-brain analysis and (2) tract-
norm-referenced instrument for assessing phonological specific analysis. First, individual FA maps were nonlinearly
processing abilities as prerequisites to reading fluency,40,41 warped into alignment with the FMRIB58_FA template, and the
with a Rapid Object Naming subtest evaluating the retrieval same warp fields were applied to the RD maps to bring them
of information from memory and execution of operations into alignment with standard space. For the whole-brain analy-
quickly (speed of processing) by young children who have sis, an analysis mask was created by skeletonizing the group
not yet mastered letters, numbers, or colors; and (3) Get mean FA map after thresholding at a value of 0.2. Second, a
Ready to Read! (GRTR), a norm-referenced assessment of new analysis mask was created by intersecting the whole-
core emergent literacy skills for children 3 to 6 years of brain mask with binarized probability maps48 of the left arcu-
age that is a factor associated with reading outcomes. 42 ate fasciculus, inferior longitudinal fasciculus, and uncinate
ScreenQ total scores were applied as variables in a series of fasciculus, which are 3 tracts associated with language and lit-
multiple linear regression models, with CTOPP-2, EVT-2, and eracy abilities assessed through the selected cognitive mea-
GRTR scores as the dependent variables. Child age and sures (language, emergent literacy, rapid naming).29-34
household income level were applied in each model as Left-sided tracts were selected given well-described struc-
covariates, with a threshold of statistical significance of tural and functional lateralization supporting these abilities at
P < .05. We used SAS software, version 9.4 (SAS Institute) for this age.49-51 A general linear model approach was used to test
behavioral analyses. for correlations between ScreenQ scores and each of the 2 DTI
measures at each voxel, with ScreenQ scores, demeaned age,
Diffusion Tensor Imaging and demeaned household income level composing the de-
Details of play-based acclimatization techniques used with sign matrix. The randomize function52 of the FSL was used to
children before the MRI are described by Vannest et al.43 All perform 10 000 random permutations of this multiple regres-
children were awake and not sedated during the MRI. The sion analysis, and familywise error (FWE) rate was controlled
MRI protocol involved a T1-weighted anatomical scan that with threshold-free cluster enhancement.53 Substantial clus-
lasted approximately 6 minutes, 4 BOLD (blood oxygen ters in the whole-brain analysis were cross-referenced to known
level–dependent) functional MRI sequences that lasted white matter tracts using the Johns Hopkins University white
approximately 5 minutes each (resting state and 3 active matter tractography atlas,54 including commissural tracts.
tasks), and DTI that lasted approximately 8 minutes; each
procedure was separated by approximately 2 minutes to
allow the child to rest. Children were allowed to watch a
video of their choice during DTI.
Results
The MRI was conducted using a scanner equipped with Demographics
a 32-channel head coil (3 Tesla Achieva; Philips). For DTI, A total of 47 of 69 children (68%) completed DTI (20 [43%] boys
61 diffusion-weighted ( b-value = 2000 seconds/mm 2 ) and 27 [57%] girls; mean [SD] age, 54.3 [7.5] months). Median
images were acquired with a spin echo-planar imaging household income category was $50 001 to $100 000 per year,
sequence with the following parameters: repetition time/ and 37 mothers (78%) were college graduates or had higher
echo time = 5000/88 ms, acquisition matrix: 96 × 96, 66 educational status, reflecting a sample with middle to high
slices, slice thickness = 2 mm, 2 × 2 mm in-plane resolution, socioeconomic status. 55 Participant demographics are
in-plane parallel-reduction factor = 1.5, and multiband summarized in Table 1.
factor = 3. Data were visually assessed for quality issues
(eg, pervasive motion or artifact, inadequate coverage) ScreenQ Survey and Cognitive Test Scores
before processing and analyses conducted using the FMRIB Mean (SD; range) ScreenQ score for children completing DTI
(Functional MRI of the Brain) Software Library (FSL), version was 8.6 (4.8; 1-19) points. Median (range) age for initiating
5.0.11. Diffusion-weighted images were corrected for subject screen use was 18 (0-36) months, and median (range) use was
movement and other artifacts using the eddy function of the 1.5 (0-12) hours per day. Twenty-eight children (60%) had their
FSL.44 Mean (SD) framewise movement across children was own portable device, and 19 (41%) had a television or a por-
0.54 (0.29) mm, and mean (SD) frequency of outlier slices table device in their bedroom. Mean (SD; range) CTOPP-2 score
was 1.3% (1.1%). Slices identified as outliers were replaced was 9.4 (3.3; 2-15) points. Mean (SD; range) EVT-2 scaled score
with estimates by the gaussian process (eddy’s “–repol” was 113.1 (16.6; 88-144) points, with 64% of scores in the mean
option with default parameters, except single-slice and range for age. Mean (SD; range) GRTR score was 19.0 (5.9; 5-25)
groupwise outlier detection for multiband acquisition).45 points. Scores are summarized in Table 1.
The eddy-corrected diffusion-weighted images and rotated ScreenQ scores were negatively correlated with EVT-2
b-vectors were applied to fit a standard diffusion tensor (F2,43 = 5.14; R2 = 0.19; P < .01), CTOPP-2 (F2,35 = 6.64; R2 = 0.28;
model at each voxel using the dtifit function of the FSL. P < .01), and GRTR (F2,44 = 17.08; R2 = 0.44; P < .01) scores, con-
Maps of FA and RD were calculated from the principal eigen- trolling for child age. These associations did not reach statisti-
values of the diffusion tensor using standard methods.46 cal significance when household income was included as a

jamapediatrics.com (Reprinted) JAMA Pediatrics Published online November 4, 2019 E3

© 2019 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Western Sydney University User on 11/04/2019


Research Original Investigation Associations Between Screen Use and White Matter Integrity in Preschoolers

Table 1. Participant Demographics


No. Who
Completed Mean (SD)
Variable No. (%) Measure [Range]
Child age, y
≥3 10 (21) NA NA
≥4 22 (47) NA NA
≥5 15 (32) NA NA
Child sex
Male 20 (43) NA NA
Female 27 (57) NA NA
Annual household income, US $
≤25 000 8 (17) NA NA
25 001-50 000 6 (13) NA NA
50 001-100 000 13 (28) NA NA
100 001-150 000 11 (23) NA NA
>150 000 9 (19) NA NA
Maternal educational level
≤High school 3 (7) NA NA
Some college/associate degree 7 (15) NA NA
College graduate 19 (40) NA NA
>College 18 (38) NA NA Abbreviations: CTOPP-2,
Comprehensive Test of Phonological
ScreenQ total scores NA 47 8.6 (4.8) [1-19]
Processing, Second Edition;
CTOPP-2 Rapid Object Naming subtest scaled score NA 40 9.4 (3.3) [2-15] EVT-2, Expressive Vocabulary Test,
EVT-2 scaled score NA 46 113.1 (16.6) [88-144] Second Edition; GRTR, Get Ready to
Read! assessment; NA, not
GRTR total score NA 47 19.0 (5.9) [5-25]
applicable.

covariate, and this may be associated with the moderate and income (P < .05, FWE-corrected). These associations are
collinearity between income level and ScreenQ scores (rρ = –0.54). shown in Figure 3.
Scatterplots of ScreenQ scores in comparison with EVT-2,
CTOPP-2, and GRTR scores are shown in Figure 1.

Discussion
Diffusion Tensor Imaging
Higher ScreenQ scores were statistically significantly corre- Unprecedented technological upheaval over the past few de-
lated with lower FA and higher RD in extensive aspects of white cades has largely outpaced the ability of scientists to charac-
matter tracts in the whole-brain analysis, controlling for child terize its fueling controversy and anxiety among parents, edu-
age and household income (P < .05, FWE-corrected). Figure 2 cators, and clinicians. To our knowledge, this study is the first
illustrates the extent and degree of these correlations, with pa- to describe structural neurobiological correlates of screen-
rameter estimates from multiple regression analyses show- based media use in preschool-aged children. The findings are
ing the adjusted change in FA or RD per unit increase in ScreenQ in the context of well-described measures of white matter mi-
score. Correlations for FA (35% vs 28%) and RD (33% vs 21%) crostructural integrity, FA and RD.57 Fractional anisotropy is
were generally more extensive in the left hemispheric tracts most clearly associated with organization of white matter in
compared with their contralateral counterparts, as described parallel bundles, whereas RD is inversely associated with de-
in Table 2. Tracts with the most extensive associations were gree of myelination of such bundles57 as well as axonal pack-
involved with language, visual processing or imagery, execu- ing and other microstructural processes. Increases in FA and
tive functions, and multimodal association. The arcuate fas- decreases in RD are consistently observed across develop-
ciculus exhibited the greatest degree of left laterality (35% vs ment in older children and in response to constructive envi-
10% of tract for FA; 44% vs 6% for RD), consistent with its well- ronmental stimulation, such as language exposure.21,58-60 This
described role in language.56 study found higher FA and lower RD associated with more
In the hypothesis-driven analysis, higher ScreenQ scores screen time in major fiber tracts supporting core language and
were correlated with lower FA, covering 16% of the arcuate executive and emergent literacy skills, raising questions as to
fasciculus, 29% of the inferior longitudinal fasciculus, and whether at least some aspects of screen-based media use may
55% of the uncinate fasciculus, controlling for child age and provide suboptimal neurodevelopmental stimulation during
household income (P < .05, FWE-corrected). Higher ScreenQ early childhood.
scores were correlated with higher RD, spanning 20% of the Structural markers for language performance involving FA
arcuate fasciculus, 36% of the inferior longitudinal fascicu- and RD have been described in full-term and preterm children
lus, and 39% of the uncinate fasciculus, controlling for age as young as 3 years.31,61 The arcuate fasciculus, which connects

E4 JAMA Pediatrics Published online November 4, 2019 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

© 2019 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Western Sydney University User on 11/04/2019


Associations Between Screen Use and White Matter Integrity in Preschoolers Original Investigation Research

volving the uncinate fasciculus and inferior longitudinal fascicu-


Figure 1. Scatterplots of ScreenQ Survey Total Scores
vs Cognitive Assessment Scores
lus, which support other aspects of language and literacy, such
as semantic processing, emotional integration, and incorpora-
A ScreenQ vs speed of processing tion of visual imagery.30,31,33,65,66 That these associations were
18 left-lateralized (Table 2) is consistent with reliance on language
16 areas in the left hemisphere that increases with age.51 Lower FA
14
and higher RD in other tracts (eg, superior longitudinal fascicu-
lus, anterior thalamic radiation, and forceps minor) are also con-
12
sistent with the reliance of language processing on a distributed,
CTOPP Score

10
bilateral network that becomes more specialized with age.31,59,61
8
Although these findings are correlational in nature and do not
6
identify causality or long-term developmental risks, they align
4 with findings in previous studies describing the detrimental out-
2 comes of screen time for language abilities in children,7,67 war-
0 ranting further investigation.
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
ScreenQ Total Score
Rapid automatized naming is a critical language and literacy
skill that supports orthographic-phonological translation
B ScreenQ vs expressive vocabulary (eg, nonword reading) and semantic processing (eg, deriving
160 meaning from objects, symbols, or letters) and is highly indica-
150 tive of reading outcomes.68-70 Lesion studies have described ma-
140 jor deficits in word and object naming involving the uncinate fas-
130 ciculus, particularly in the left hemisphere.33,34 The inferior
120 fronto-occipital fasciculus also plays a major role in rapid automa-
EVT Score

110 tized naming, with higher FA estimating better performance.71


100 Substantial decreases in FA in the uncinate fasciculus (55% of the
90 left, and 35% of the right) and fronto-occipital fasciculus (44%
80 of the extent of the left, and 34% of the right) in the context
70 of lower CTOPP-2 scores are concordant neurocognitive
60 findings associated with greater screen use. In addition, rapid au-
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
tomatized naming is intimately associated with broader execu-
ScreenQ Total Score
tive functions, including speed of processing and working
C ScreenQ vs emergent literacy memory,72,73 which are supported by other tracts associated with
35
greater use in the whole-brain analyses (superior longitudinal
fasciculus, forceps minor, and anterior thalamic tracts).74,75
30
Although broader executive functions were not directly assessed
25 in this study, the associations between higher screen use and
lower FA and higher RD for each of these tracts are consistent with
GRTR Score

20
deleterious associations that have been previously described in
15 young children.10,19,76
The children in this study were not yet attending school
10
and not yet reading independently, but we believe these find-
5 ings are highly relevant within the construct of emergent lit-
0 eracy, an incremental process beginning in infancy that in-
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
volves skills, knowledge, and attitudes required for reading and
ScreenQ Total Score
writing.22,77 Emergent literacy development has been associ-
Solid blue line represents least squares fit; dashed blue lines, 95% CI bounds of ated with increases in FA and decreases in RD in the arcuate
slope; dashed orange lines, 95% prediction interval for new observations. fasciculus,62 whereas lower FA in the arcuate fasciculus, in-
CTOPP-2 indicates Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing, Second ferior longitudinal fasciculus, and superior longitudinal fas-
Edition, Rapid Object Naming subtest; EVT, Expressive Vocabulary Test, Second
Edition; GRTR, Get Ready to Read! assessment.
ciculus has been associated with lower prereading (particu-
larly phonological) skills.49,78 Neural microstructure involving
frontal, thalamic, parietal, and temporal areas (eg, through ar-
receptive (Wernicke) and expressive (Broca) language brain cuate fasciculus or superior longitudinal fasciculus) has also
areas,29,30 is associated with language skills,31,62,63 including pho- been shown to be a factor in reading outcomes.79-81 Associa-
nological processing and vocabulary.31,59,64 In this study, in- tions between lower FA and higher RD in the arcuate fascicu-
creased screen time was associated with lower FA in 16% and lus, inferior longitudinal fasciculus, and other tracts with higher
higher RD in 20% of the arcuate fasciculus and also with signifi- ScreenQ scores, in the context of negative correlation be-
cantly lower EVT-2 and CTOPP-2 scores, which are concordant tween ScreenQ and GRTR scores, are concordant findings re-
neurocognitive results. Similar associations were also seen in- lated to this multifaceted skill set.

jamapediatrics.com (Reprinted) JAMA Pediatrics Published online November 4, 2019 E5

© 2019 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Western Sydney University User on 11/04/2019


Research Original Investigation Associations Between Screen Use and White Matter Integrity in Preschoolers

Figure 2. Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) Parameter Maps for Whole-Brain Analysis

A Lower FA

–3.0

–3.5

ΔFA per ScreenQ Point


–4.0

–4.5

–5.0

–5.5

Left Front Right –6.0


×10–3

B Higher RD

8.0 White matter voxels exhibit a

ΔRD (mm2/s) per ScreenQ Point


7.5 statistically significant correlation
7.0 between screen-based media use
6.5 (ScreenQ scores) and lower fractional
6.0 anisotropy (FA; A) as well as higher
5.5
radial diffusivity (RD; B) in
5.0
whole-brain analysis, controlling for
child age and household income level
4.5
(P < .05, familywise error–corrected).
4.0
Color indicates the slope or
3.5
Left Front Right magnitude of correlation (ie, change
3.0
×10–6 in the DTI parameter for every point
increase in ScreenQ score).

Table 2. Extent of Tracts With Statistically Significant Correlations Between ScreenQ Scores and Diffusion Tensor Imaging Parameters
in Whole-Brain Analysis

FA Decrease, % RD Increase, %
White Matter Tract Regions Connected Major Functions Left Right Left Right
Anterior thalamic radiation Thalamus-prefrontal Executive 33a 13 18a 10
Cingulum (cingulate gyrus) Limbic Emotion, learning, memory 28a 18 25 22
Cingulum (hippocampus) Limbic Emotion, learning, memory 4 20a 4 27a
Corticospinal tract Cortex–spinal cord Sensory-motor 29 21 26a 15
Forceps major Occipital-occipital Visual processing/imagery 33 39 38 33
Forceps minor Frontal-frontal Executive 55 56 34 33
a
Inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus Frontal-occipital/temporal Association, language 51 44 48 24
Inferior longitudinal fasciculus Occipital-temporal Visual association/imagery, language 38a 21 48a 6
Superior longitudinal fasciculus Frontal-occipital-temporal-parietal Association, working memory, language 24 40a 29 37
Arcuate fasciculus Frontal-temporal-parietal Language 35a 10 44a 6
Uncinate fasciculus Orbitofrontal-temporal Association, language (rapid naming) 55a 35 53a 20

Abbreviations: FA, fractional anisotropy; RD, radial diffusivity.


a
Substantial lateralization of correlation (extent >50% greater than contralateral tract).

Although establishing causation is beyond the scope of lence of “learning” applications marketed for young children
this study, a critical question is whether neurobiological dif- and the emphasis on technology in early childhood educa-
ferences are directly associated with properties of screen- tional curricula,3 multiyear studies of emergent literacy out-
based media itself or indirectly associated with differences comes are warranted to determine long-term implications.
in human interactive (eg, shared reading) time, which tends
to decrease with greater use.12-14 Aspects of both associa- Limitations and Strengths
tions seem likely, as suggested by a recent MRI study of chil- This study has several limitations. The sample largely com-
dren aged 8 to 12 years that found lower functional connec- prised families with high socioeconomic status, yet house-
tivity within the emerging reading network was correlated hold income was accounted for in the DTI analyses. Associa-
with higher screen time and higher functional connectivity tions between ScreenQ survey and cognitive testing scores
was correlated with more reading time.18 Given the preva- did not meet the threshold for statistical significance when

E6 JAMA Pediatrics Published online November 4, 2019 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

© 2019 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Western Sydney University User on 11/04/2019


Associations Between Screen Use and White Matter Integrity in Preschoolers Original Investigation Research

Figure 3. Tract-Based Spatial Statistical Analysis in the Left Hemisphere

A Lower FA with higher ScreenQ scores

–3.5

–4.0

ΔFA per ScreenQ Point


–4.5

–5.0

–5.5

Lateral view Medial view


–6.0
×10–3

B Higher RD with higher ScreenQ scores

8.0
7.5
The tract-based spatial statistical

ΔRD (mm2/s) per ScreenQ Point


7.0 analysis involves these 3 language-
6.5 and literacy-associated tracts in the
left hemisphere: arcuate fasciculus
6.0 (white), inferior longitudinal
5.5 fasciculus (tan), uncinate fasciculus
(brown). A, Blue represents
5.0
statistically significantly lower
4.5 fractional anisotropy (FA) with higher
ScreenQ scores. B, Red represents
4.0
statistically significantly higher radial
Lateral view Medial view 3.5 diffusivity (RD) with higher ScreenQ
3.0 scores. Both analyses controlled for
×10–6 child age and household income level
(P < .05, familywise error–corrected).

income was included in the model, which may be associated tion skills. However, DTI is a task-free paradigm that is
with the moderate collinearity between income level and not affected by performance factors. Although important,
ScreenQ scores that negatively affected statistical power, the cross-sectional nature of this study precludes the
particularly given the small sample size. Both behavioral discernment of causation or whether the results are attribut-
and DTI findings may have been even more robust in able to screen time or an indirect effect of differences
families with higher socioeconomic status compared with in reading, language, or other constructive experiences.3,28
families with lower socioeconomic status, for whom lan- This study also has strengths. The sample of preschool-
guage, literacy, and other cognitive disparities are well aged children is remarkable given the difficulty of success-
documented.82,83 fully conducting MRI at this age. The DTI results survived
Although exhibiting strong psychometric properties,35 stringent correction for motion and multiple comparisons,
ScreenQ is a new measure reliant on parent reporting controlling for child age and household income, and
and thus is subject to social desirability bias. However, were concordant with behavioral results controlling for
no similar composite measure or direct assessment currently child age. Rather than apply a single screen time variable,
exists, and ScreenQ reflects domains in the current AAP such as minutes per day or violent content as is often
recommendations. 5 Although DTI is a powerful method the case, 84,85 ScreenQ incorporated domains of use into
for studying white matter in the brain, it is vulnerable to a coherent composite, providing a more holistic view of
artifacts attributable to nonuniform directionality of use. 35 The analyses were driven by a hypothesis based
fiber tracts, such as crossing fibers.28 However, the analy- on recent neuroimaging evidence and AAP guidelines,
ses involved well-defined tracts and a hypothesis-driven including characterization of associations for a broad range
approach, and FA and RD findings were consistent with of tracts supporting relevant cognitive abilities and fo-
results in the current behavioral and neuroimaging literature cused analyses with increased statistical power. The DTI
and with each other. Diffusion-weighted images were findings aligned with those on validated cognitive measures,
acquired at the end of the MRI session, resulting in a providing compelling insights into the implications for
lower success rate (68%) than from recent work,17 possibly child development, preschool education, and further
conferring bias toward older children with higher atten- research.

jamapediatrics.com (Reprinted) JAMA Pediatrics Published online November 4, 2019 E7

© 2019 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Western Sydney University User on 11/04/2019


Research Original Investigation Associations Between Screen Use and White Matter Integrity in Preschoolers

Although preliminary, this study fills a gap in evidence


and provides a novel, neurobiological lens through which to Conclusions
view the recommendations of the AAP1,3 and World Health
Organization15 in terms of readiness of brain networks to In this study of 47 preschool-aged children, increased use of
constructively process screen-based media and the market- screen-based media in the context of the AAP guidelines was
ing of such media to children. Given the results of this study, associated with lower microstructural integrity of brain
we can speculate that before age 5 years, when brain net- white matter tracts that support language, executive func-
works are developing rapidly,86,87 caution is warranted, not- tions, and emergent literacy skills, controlling for child age
ing the postulate attributed to Donald Hebb: “neurons that and household income. Screen use was also associated with
fire together, wire together.”42 We believe multiyear studies lower scores on corresponding behavioral measures, control-
are needed to explore whether the associations observed in ling for age. Given that screen-based media use is ubiquitous
this study, related to language and literacy abilities, reflect and increasing in children in home, childcare, and school
longer-term risks and outcomes. Such studies are critical in settings, these findings suggest the need for further study
the context of a child’s early cognitive ecosystem, in which to identify the implications for the developing brain, par-
unprecedented technological shifts may change how chil- ticularly during stages of dynamic brain growth in early
dren live, connect, and learn. childhood.

ARTICLE INFORMATION 2. Carter B, Rees P, Hale L, Bhattacharjee D, 13. Tomopoulos S, Valdez PT, Dreyer BP, et al.
Accepted for Publication: August 4, 2019. Paradkar MS. Association between portable Is exposure to media intended for preschool
screen-based media device access or use and sleep children associated with less parent-child shared
Published Online: November 4, 2019. outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. reading aloud and teaching activities? Ambul Pediatr.
doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.3869 JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170(12):1202-1208. doi:10. 2007;7(1):18-24. doi:10.1016/j.ambp.2006.10.005
Author Contributions: Drs Hutton and Dudley had 1001/jamapediatrics.2016.2341 14. Mendelsohn AL, Berkule SB, Tomopoulos S,
full access to all the data in the study and take 3. Rideout V. The Common Sense Census: Media et al. Infant television and video exposure
responsibility for the integrity of the data and the Use by Kids Age Zero to Eight. San Francisco, CA: associated with limited parent-child verbal
accuracy of the data analysis. Common Sense Media; 2017. interactions in low socioeconomic status
Concept and design: Hutton, Horowitz-Kraus, households. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2008;162
DeWitt, Holland. 4. Hutton JS, Lin L, Gruber R, et al. Shared reading
and television across the perinatal period in (5):411-417. doi:10.1001/archpedi.162.5.411
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data:
Hutton, Dudley, Horowitz-Kraus, Holland. low-SES households. Clin Pediatr (Phila). 2018;57 15. World Health Organization. WHO Guidelines on
Drafting of the manuscript: Hutton, Dudley, (8):904-912. doi:10.1177/0009922817737077 Physical Activity, Sedentary Behaviour and Sleep for
Horowitz-Kraus, Holland. 5. AAP Council on Communications and Media. Children Under 5 Years of Age. Geneva, Switzerland:
Critical revision of the manuscript for important Media and Young Minds. Elk Grove Village, IL: World Health Organization; 2019.
intellectual content: All authors. American Academy of Pediatrics; 2016. 16. Paulus MP, Squeglia LM, Bagot K, et al. Screen
Statistical analysis: Hutton, Dudley, 6. Moreno MA, Chassiakos YR, Cross C; Council on media activity and brain structure in youth:
Horowitz-Kraus. Communications and Media. Media use in evidence for diverse structural correlation networks
Obtained funding: Hutton. school-aged children and adolescents. Pediatrics. from the ABCD study. Neuroimage. 2019;185:
Supervision: Hutton, Horowitz-Kraus, DeWitt, 2016;138(5). 140-153. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.040
Holland. 17. Hutton JS, Dudley J, Horowitz-Kraus T,
7. Anderson DR, Subrahmanyam K; Cognitive
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr DeWitt Impacts of Digital Media Workgroup. Digital screen DeWitt T, Holland SK. Differences in functional
reported serving as chair of the national Reach Out media and cognitive development. Pediatrics. 2017; brain network connectivity during stories
and Read Board of Directors, an organization that 140(suppl 2):S57-S61. doi:10.1542/peds.2016-1758C presented in audio, illustrated, and animated
advocates and supports an early literacy program format in preschool-age children [published online
working with pediatric health care clinicians. 8. Ma J, Birken C. Handheld screen time linked with October 30, 2018]. Brain Imaging Behav. doi:10.
No other disclosures were reported. speech delays in young children. Paper presented 1007/s11682-018-9985-y
at: Pediatric Academic Societies Meeting; May 6,
Funding/Support: This study was funded by a 2017; San Francisco, CA. https://www. 18. Horowitz-Kraus T, Hutton JS. Brain connectivity
Procter Scholar Award from the Cincinnati aappublications.org/news/2017/05/04/ in children is increased by the time they spend
Children’s Research Foundation (Dr Hutton). PASScreenTime050417. Accessed April 2019 reading books and decreased by the length of
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no exposure to screen-based media. Acta Paediatr.
9. Garrison MM, Liekweg K, Christakis DA. Media 2018;107(4):685-693. doi:10.1111/apa.14176
role in the design and conduct of the study; use and child sleep: the impact of content, timing,
collection, management, analysis, and and environment. Pediatrics. 2011;128(1):29-35. 19. Zivan M, Bar S, Jing X, Hutton J, Farah R,
interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or doi:10.1542/peds.2010-3304 Horowitz-Kraus T. Screen-exposure and altered
approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit brain activation related to attention in preschool
the manuscript for publication. 10. Lillard AS, Li H, Boguszewski K. Television and children: An EEG study. Trends Neurosci Educ. 2019;
children’s executive function. Adv Child Dev Behav. 17:1-5. doi:10.1016/j.tine.2019.100117
Additional Contributions: Research coordinators 2015;48:219-248. doi:10.1016/bs.acdb.2014.11.006
Arielle Wilson, BA, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital 20. Rohr CS, Arora A, Cho IYK, et al. Functional
Medical Center, and Amy Kerr, MEd, Cincinnati 11. Walsh JJ, Barnes JD, Cameron JD, et al. network integration and attention skills in young
Children's Hospital Reading and Literacy Discovery Associations between 24 hour movement children. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;30:200-211.
Center, oversaw the data collection, data entry, and behaviours and global cognition in US children: doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.007
quality control for this study, with no compensation a cross-sectional observational study. Lancet Child
Adolesc Health. 2018;2(11):783-791. doi:10.1016/ 21. Sousa SS, Amaro E Jr, Crego A, Gonçalves ÓF,
outside of their normal salaries. Sampaio A. Developmental trajectory of the
S2352-4642(18)30278-5
prefrontal cortex: a systematic review of diffusion
REFERENCES 12. Choi JH, Mendelsohn AL, Weisleder A, et al. tensor imaging studies. Brain Imaging Behav. 2018;
1. Groundbreaking study examines effects of Real-world usage of educational media does not 12(4):1197-1210. doi:10.1007/s11682-017-9761-4
screen time on kids [transcript]. 60 Minutes. promote parent-child cognitive stimulation
activities. Acad Pediatr. 2018;18(2):172-178. doi:10. 22. Horowitz-Kraus T, Hutton JS. From emergent
CBS television. December 9, 2018. literacy to reading: how learning to read changes a
1016/j.acap.2017.04.020

E8 JAMA Pediatrics Published online November 4, 2019 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

© 2019 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Western Sydney University User on 11/04/2019


Associations Between Screen Use and White Matter Integrity in Preschoolers Original Investigation Research

child's brain. Acta Paediatr. 2015;104(7):648-656. development and psychometric refinement of the development in children. Int J Audiol. 2007;46(9):
doi:10.1111/apa.13018 ScreenQ. Paper presented at: Pediatric Academic 533-551. doi:10.1080/14992020701448994
23. Dehaene S, Cohen L, Morais J, Kolinsky R. Societies Meeting; May 4, 2018; Toronto, Canada 52. Winkler AM, Ridgway GR, Webster MA,
Illiterate to literate: behavioural and cerebral 38. Bellevue Project for Early Language Literacy and Smith SM, Nichols TE. Permutation inference for
changes induced by reading acquisition. Nat Rev Education Success. STIMQ Home Cognitive the general linear model. Neuroimage. 2014;92:
Neurosci. 2015;16(4):234-244. doi:10.1038/nrn3924 Environment. http://pediatrics.med.nyu.edu/ 381-397. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060
24. Yeatman JD, Dougherty RF, Ben-Shachar M, developmental/research/the-belle-project/stimq- 53. Smith SM, Nichols TE. Threshold-free cluster
Wandell BA. Development of white matter and cognitive-home-environment. Accessed March 2019. enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing,
reading skills. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109 39. Williams KT. Expressive Vocabulary Test, Second threshold dependence and localisation in cluster
(44):E3045-E3053. doi:10.1073/pnas.1206792109 Edition (EVT-2). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson inference. Neuroimage. 2009;44(1):83-98. doi:10.
25. Keller TA, Just MA. Altering cortical Assessments; 2007. 1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061
connectivity: remediation-induced changes in the 40. Mitchell J. Comprehensive test of phonological 54. Hua K, Zhang J, Wakana S, et al. Tract
white matter of poor readers. Neuron. 2009;64(5): processing (CTOPP). Assess Eff Interv. 2001;26(3): probability maps in stereotaxic spaces: analyses of
624-631. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.10.018 57-63. doi:10.1177/073724770102600305 white matter anatomy and tract-specific
26. Forbes TA, Gallo V. All wrapped up: 41. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA, quantification. Neuroimage. 2008;39(1):336-347.
environmental effects on myelination. Trends Pearson NA. CTOPP-2: Comprehensive Test of doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.053
Neurosci. 2017;40(9):572-587. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2017. Phonological Processing–Second Edition. Austin, TX: 55. United States Census Bureau. Income and
06.009 Pro-Ed; 2013. https://www.proedinc.com/ Poverty in the United States: 2017. Washington, DC:
27. Qiu A, Mori S, Miller MI. Diffusion tensor Products/13080/ctopp2-comprehensive-test-of- US Department of Commerce; 2018.
imaging for understanding brain development in phonological-processingsecond-edition.aspx. 56. Skeide MA, Brauer J, Friederici AD. Brain
early life. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66:853-876. Accessed March 2019. functional and structural predictors of language
doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015340 42. Hebb D. The Organization of Behavior. performance. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26(5):2127-2139.
28. Soares JM, Marques P, Alves V, Sousa N. A Neuropsychological Theory. New York, NY: Wiley; doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv042
A hitchhiker’s guide to diffusion tensor imaging. 1949. 57. Winklewski PJ, Sabisz A, Naumczyk P, Jodzio K,
Front Neurosci. 2013;7:31. doi:10.3389/fnins.2013. 43. Vannest J, Rajagopal A, Cicchino ND, et al; Szurowska E, Szarmach A. Understanding the
00031 CMIND Authorship Consortium. Factors physiopathology behind axial and radial diffusivity
29. Friederici AD. White-matter pathways for determining success of awake and asleep magnetic changes—what do we know? Front Neurol. 2018;9:
speech and language processing. Handb Clin Neurol. resonance imaging scans in nonsedated children. 92. doi:10.3389/fneur.2018.00092
2015;129:177-186. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-62630- Neuropediatrics. 2014;45(6):370-377. doi:10.1055/s- 58. Krogsrud SK, Fjell AM, Tamnes CK, et al
1.00010-X 0034-1387816 Changes in white matter microstructure in the
30. Friederici AD, Gierhan SM. The language 44. Andersson JLR, Sotiropoulos SN. An integrated developing brain—a longitudinal diffusion tensor
network. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23(2):250-254. approach to correction for off-resonance effects imaging study of children from 4 to 11 years of age.
doi:10.1016/j.conb.2012.10.002 and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. Neuroimage. 2016;124(pt A):473-486. doi:10.1016/j.
Neuroimage. 2016;125:1063-1078. doi:10.1016/j. neuroimage.2015.09.017
31. Dodson CK, Travis KE, Borchers LR, neuroimage.2015.10.019
Marchman VA, Ben-Shachar M, Feldman HM. White 59. Romeo RR, Segaran J, Leonard JA, et al.
matter properties associated with pre-reading skills 45. Andersson JLR, Graham MS, Zsoldos E, Language exposure relates to structural neural
in 6-year-old children born preterm and at term. Sotiropoulos SN. Incorporating outlier detection connectivity in childhood. J Neurosci. 2018;38(36):
Dev Med Child Neurol. 2018;60(7):695-702. doi:10. and replacement into a non-parametric framework 7870-7877. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0484-18.2018
1111/dmcn.13783 for movement and distortion correction of diffusion 60. Reynolds JE, Grohs MN, Dewey D, Lebel C.
MR images. Neuroimage. 2016;141:556-572. doi:10. Global and regional white matter development in
32. Jouen AL, Ellmore TM, Madden-Lombardi CJ, 1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.058
Pallier C, Dominey PF, Ventre-Dominey J. Beyond early childhood. Neuroimage. 2019;196(Apr):49-58.
the word and image: II- structural and functional 46. Basser PJ, Mattiello J, LeBihan D. MR diffusion doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.004
connectivity of a common semantic system. tensor spectroscopy and imaging. Biophys J. 61. Walton M, Dewey D, Lebel C. Brain white
Neuroimage. 2018;166:185-197. doi:10.1016/j. 1994;66(1):259-267. doi:10.1016/S0006-3495(94) matter structure and language ability in
neuroimage.2017.10.039 80775-1 preschool-aged children. Brain Lang. 2018;176:
33. Sierpowska J, Gabarrós A, Fernández-Coello A, 47. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H, 19-25. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2017.10.008
et al. White-matter pathways and semantic et al. Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise 62. Thiebaut de Schotten M, Cohen L, Amemiya E,
processing: intrasurgical and lesion-symptom analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage. Braga LW, Dehaene S. Learning to read improves
mapping evidence. Neuroimage Clin. 2019;22:101704. 2006;31(4):1487-1505. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage. the structure of the arcuate fasciculus. Cereb Cortex.
doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101704 2006.02.024 2014;24(4):989-995. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs383
34. Papagno C. Naming and the role of the 48. Rojkova K, Volle E, Urbanski M, Humbert F, 63. Smits M, Jiskoot LC, Papma JM. White matter
uncinate fasciculus in language function. Curr Dell’Acqua F, Thiebaut de Schotten M. Atlasing the tracts of speech and language. Semin Ultrasound CT
Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2011;11(6):553-559. doi:10. frontal lobe connections and their variability due to MR. 2014;35(5):504-516. doi:10.1053/j.sult.2014.
1007/s11910-011-0219-6 age and education: a spherical deconvolution 06.008
tractography study. Brain Struct Funct. 2016;221(3):
35. Hutton JS, Huang G, Sahay R, DeWitt T. 1751-1766. doi:10.1007/s00429-015-1001-3 64. Lebel C, Beaulieu C. Lateralization of the
Ittenbach RF. Assessment of screen-based media arcuate fasciculus from childhood to adulthood and
use in children: preliminary validation of the 49. Yeatman JD, Dougherty RF, Rykhlevskaia E, its relation to cognitive abilities in children. Hum
ScreenQ. Paper presented at: Pediatric Academic et al. Anatomical properties of the arcuate Brain Mapp. 2009;30(11):3563-3573. doi:10.1002/
Societies Meeting; April 30, 2019; Baltimore, MD fasciculus predict phonological and reading skills in hbm.20779
children. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23(11):3304-3317.
36. Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00061 65. Teichmann M, Rosso C, Martini JB, et al.
Gonzalez N, Conde JG. Research electronic data A cortical-subcortical syntax pathway linking
capture (REDCap)—a metadata-driven 50. Panesar SS, Yeh FC, Jacquesson T, Hula W, Broca’s area and the striatum. Hum Brain Mapp.
methodology and workflow process for providing Fernandez-Miranda JC. A quantitative tractography 2015;36(6):2270-2283. doi:10.1002/hbm.22769
translational research informatics support. study into the connectivity, segmentation and
laterality of the human inferior longitudinal 66. Von Der Heide RJ, Skipper LM, Klobusicky E,
J Biomed Inform. 2009;42(2):377-381. doi:10.1016/ Olson IR. Dissecting the uncinate fasciculus:
j.jbi.2008.08.010 fasciculus. Front Neuroanat. 2018;12:47. doi:10.
3389/fnana.2018.00047 disorders, controversies and a hypothesis. Brain.
37. Hutton JS, Xu Y, DeWitt T, Ittenbach RF. 2013;136(Pt 6):1692-1707. doi:10.1093/brain/awt094
Assessment of screen-based media use in children: 51. Holland SK, Vannest J, Mecoli M, et al.
Functional MRI of language lateralization during

jamapediatrics.com (Reprinted) JAMA Pediatrics Published online November 4, 2019 E9

© 2019 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Western Sydney University User on 11/04/2019


Research Original Investigation Associations Between Screen Use and White Matter Integrity in Preschoolers

67. Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN. 74. Smolker HR, Friedman NP, Hewitt JK, 81. Rimrodt SL, Peterson DJ, Denckla MB,
Associations between media viewing and language Banich MT. Neuroanatomical correlates of the unity Kaufmann WE, Cutting LE. White matter
development in children under age 2 years. J Pediatr. and diversity model of executive function in young microstructural differences linked to left perisylvian
2007;151(4):364-368. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2007. adults. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018;12:283. doi:10. language network in children with dyslexia. Cortex.
04.071 3389/fnhum.2018.00283 2010;46(6):739-749. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.
68. Norton ES, Wolf M. Rapid automatized naming 75. Mamiya PC, Richards TL, Kuhl PK. Right forceps 07.008
(RAN) and reading fluency: implications for minor and anterior thalamic radiation predict 82. Hart B, Risley T. Meaningful Differences in the
understanding and treatment of reading executive function skills in young bilingual adults. Everyday Experience of Young American Children.
disabilities. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:427-452. Front Psychol. 2018;9:118. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes Publishing Co; 1995.
doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100431 00118 83. Larson K, Russ SA, Nelson BB, Olson LM,
69. Hulme C, Snowling MJ. Reading disorders and 76. Nathanson AI, Aladé F, Sharp ML, Halfon N. Cognitive ability at kindergarten entry
dyslexia. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2016;28(6):731-735. Rasmussen EE, Christy K. The relation between and socioeconomic status. Pediatrics. 2015;135(2):
doi:10.1097/MOP.0000000000000411 television exposure and executive function among e440-e448. doi:10.1542/peds.2014-0434
70. Lohvansuu K, Hämäläinen JA, Ervast L, preschoolers. Dev Psychol. 2014;50(5):1497-1506. 84. Lissak G. Adverse physiological and
Lyytinen H, Leppänen PHT. Longitudinal doi:10.1037/a0035714 psychological effects of screen time on children and
interactions between brain and cognitive measures 77. Whitehurst GJ, Lonigan CJ. Child development adolescents: literature review and case study.
on reading development from 6 months to 14 years. and emergent literacy. Child Dev. 1998;69(3): Environ Res. 2018;164:149-157. doi:10.1016/j.envres.
Neuropsychologia. 2018;108:6-12. doi:10.1016/j. 848-872. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06247.x 2018.01.015
neuropsychologia.2017.11.018 78. Saygin ZM, Norton ES, Osher DE, et al. Tracking 85. Christakis DA, Zimmerman FJ. Violent
71. Rollans C, Cheema K, Georgiou GK, Cummine J. the roots of reading ability: white matter volume television viewing during preschool is associated
Pathways of the inferior frontal occipital fasciculus and integrity correlate with phonological awareness with antisocial behavior during school age. Pediatrics.
in overt speech and reading. Neuroscience. 2017; in prereading and early-reading kindergarten 2007;120(5):993-999. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-3244
364:93-106. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.09.011 children. J Neurosci. 2013;33(33):13251-13258. 86. Knudsen EI. Sensitive periods in the
72. Papadopoulos TC, Spanoudis GC, Georgiou GK. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4383-12.2013 development of the brain and behavior. J Cogn
How is RAN related to reading fluency? a 79. Borchers LR, Bruckert L, Dodson CK, et al. Neurosci. 2004;16(8):1412-1425. doi:10.1162/
comprehensive examination of the prominent Microstructural properties of white matter 0898929042304796
theoretical accounts. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1217. pathways in relation to subsequent reading abilities 87. Petrican R, Taylor MJ, Grady CL. Trajectories of
doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01217 in children: a longitudinal analysis. Brain Struct Funct. brain system maturation from childhood to older
73. McCabe DP, Roediger HL, McDaniel MA, 2019;224(2):891-905. doi:10.1007/s00429-018- adulthood: implications for lifespan cognitive
Balota DA, Hambrick DZ. The relationship between 1813-z functioning. Neuroimage. 2017;163:125-149. doi:10.
working memory capacity and executive 80. Lebel C, Shaywitz B, Holahan J, Shaywitz S, 1016/j.neuroimage.2017.09.025
functioning: evidence for a common executive Marchione K, Beaulieu C. Diffusion tensor imaging
attention construct. Neuropsychology. 2010;24(2): correlates of reading ability in dysfluent and
222-243. doi:10.1037/a0017619 non-impaired readers. Brain Lang. 2013;125(2):
215-222. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2012.10.009

E10 JAMA Pediatrics Published online November 4, 2019 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

© 2019 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: https://jamanetwork.com/ by a Western Sydney University User on 11/04/2019

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi