Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 10

The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 2010; 11(2): 476485

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION

Longitudinal study on perfectionism and sleep disturbance

MARIA HELENA AZEVEDO1, SANDRA CARVALHO BOS1, MARIA JOÃO SOARES1,


MARIANA MARQUES1, ANA TELMA PEREIRA1, BERTA MAIA1,
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Ulster at Jordanstown on 03/04/15

ANA ALLEN GOMES2 & ANTÓNIO MACEDO1


1
Institute of Medical Psychology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal, and 2Department of
Educational Sciences, University of Aveiro, Campus Universitário de Santiago, Aveiro, Portugal

Abstract
Aim. To examine if perfectionism predicts self-reported sleep disturbances over time. Methods. The HewittFlett
Perfectionism Scale was used to assess self-oriented, socially-prescribed (SPP) and other-oriented perfectionism. Sleep
disturbance was evaluated with two items: difficulty in falling asleep and waking up many times during the night. Out of 870
students who participated at baseline, 592 and 305 completed the same measures 1 year (T1) and 2 years later (T2),
respectively. Results. Subjects who reported insomnia at baseline, T1 and T2 (persistent insomnia) had significantly higher
scores of baseline SPP (T1 M 51.5, SD 15.8; T2 M55.0, SD 19.0) than subjects reporting, in all stages of the study,
never/rarely having had sleep problems (T1 M41.9, SD 11.4; T2 M 42.2, SD 12.3, P B0.001 in both cases).
For personal use only.

Regression analyses showed that baseline SPP was the only significant positive predictor of difficulties in falling asleep at T1
and T2 (T1 partial R0.187; T2 partial R0.196, P B0.001) and of difficulties maintaining sleep (T1 partial R0.116;
T2 partial R0.244, P B0.001). Conclusion. SPP was found to be the most reliable predictor of sleep disturbances over
time, which constitutes a new important finding.

Key words: Personality, perfectionism, sleep disturbance, longitudinal study, university students

Introduction (Frost et al. 1990; Hewitt and Flett 1991). These


authors provided perfectionism researchers with two
The harmfulness of perfectionism was acknowl-
different instruments that nevertheless share the
edged by several clinicians long ago (Hollender
same name  the Multidimensional Perfectionism
1965; Burns 1980). Recurrent themes from the
Scale (MPS).
early descriptions of perfectionistic subjects included
Hewitt and Flett (H&F, 1991) described their
their unrealistically high and rigid standards for perfectionism dimensions based on perfectionistic
performance, fear of failure, excessive self-criticism behaviour/expectations direction (self-directed vs.
and the inability to derive satisfaction from their directed to others) and source (e.g., intrapersonal
achievements. Even though Hamachek (1978) pro- vs. interpersonal). The H&F-MPS distinguishes
posed the distinction of two perfectionism forms, three components: Self-Oriented Perfectionism (SOP,
normal and neurotic, in the 1980s, a unidimensional intrapersonal facet involving unrealistic self-imposed
conceptualization of perfectionism dominated, pre- standards and critical self-monitoring); Socially-
senting this trait as dysfunctional and associated with Prescribed Perfectionism (SPP, interpersonal dimension
psychopathology (Pacht 1984). In the early 1990s related to the perception that others significant hold
this conceptual framing began to change. Two excessively high standards and expectations of per-
independent groups with the heuristic intention of fection for oneself); Other-Oriented Perfectionism
capturing the core facets of perfectionism demon- (OOP, interpersonal component which involves the
strated that this trait would be most correctly expectation that others should achieve unrealistic
examined as a multidimensional construct with standards). Hewitt and Flett (1991) suggested that
both intrapersonal and interpersonal dimensions SOP was directed toward avoiding self-criticism,

Correspondence: Maria Helena Azevedo, MD, PhD, Institute of Medical Psychology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Rua
Larga, 3004-504 Coimbra, Portugal. Tel: 351 239 857759. Fax: 351 239 823170. E-mail: mazevedo@fmed.uc.pt

(Received 15 May 2009; accepted 2 July 2009)


ISSN 1562-2975 print/ISSN 1814-1412 online # 2010 Informa UK Ltd. (Informa Healthcare, Taylor & Francis AS)
DOI: 10.3109/15622970903304467
Perfectionism and sleep: longitudinal study 477

whereas SPP was directed toward avoiding disap- insomnia. Vincent and Walker (2000) used both
proval by others. MPS instruments in a small sample of 32 adults who
Frost and colleagues (1990), conversely, proposed responded to an advertisement placed in a commu-
that six perfectionism facets should be differentiated nity newspaper offering treatment for chronic in-
in their instrument (F-MPS): Personal Standards somnia and 26 healthy controls No significant
(PS, reflecting the setting of high personal associations were found between any of the H&F-
demands); Organization (O, emphasizing that per- MPS dimensions and insomnia, although insomniac
fectionists value order and precision); Concern over subjects tended to report more SPP than healthy
Mistakes (CM, indicating the extent to which the controls. Regarding F-MPS, subjects with chronic
person is concerned with making mistakes); Doubts insomnia were more likely to report DA, frequent
about Actions (DA, reflecting the person indecisive- PC, and CM. However, delayed sleep-onset latency
ness about one’s performance quality). The instru- was only related to the perception of heightened PC.
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Ulster at Jordanstown on 03/04/15

ment presents two additional subscales of an In a previous study with undergraduate students of
interpersonal nature which are circumscribed to both genders we found an association between sleep
the familial environment: Parental Expectations (PE) disturbances and perfectionism (Azevedo et al.
and Parental Criticism (PC) (addressing the great 2007). Further correlational and categorial analyses
importance that perfectionists attach to evaluations revealed that SPP was the only dimension associated
by their parents). According to Flett and Hewitt with sleep disturbance in subjects of both genders.
(2002) this more restricted scope is inadequate. The Males with the highest levels of SPP were approxi-
authors believe that it is important to consider other mately twice more likely to report sleep disturbances
people’s role (i.e. peers and teachers) and socio- than those with less SPP. Similar results were found
cultural factors. within the female sample. However, a positive small
The recognition of perfectionism as a multidimen- correlation between SOP and sleep measures was
sional phenomenon, with both adaptive and mala- observed in females.
daptive components, has enhanced the compre- The literature is sparse on the long-term effects of
For personal use only.

hension of normal psychological development. It maladaptive perfectionism on sleep. To our knowl-


has also elucidated the role of perfectionism in the edge only one prospective study examined the
development of several psychopathological problems relationship between perfectionism and insomnia
to which it has been linked and the mechanisms (Jansson-Fröjmark and Linton 2007). In this study
underlying these associations (Shafran and Mansell with 1936 subjects from the general population, who
2001). completed perfectionism, psychological distress and
Regarding the association between perfectionism insomnia measures at baseline and 1-year follow-up,
and sleep problems, in the 1980s some investigators F-MPS dimension CM was significantly associated
pointed out that the arousal characterizing insom- with pre-existing and future insomnia.
niacs is, in fact, an emotional arousal emerging from In the present study, the main objective, following
an anxious perfectionistic personality style (Lacks a longitudinal perspective, was to further investigate
1987). In spite of these early attempts to understand the association between perfectionism and self-
the mechanisms involved in the relationship of reported sleep disturbances in a non-clinical sample
perfectionism with sleep disturbances, since then of undergraduate Portuguese university students of
this topic has received little attention (Lundh et al. both genders in order to identify which perfection-
1994; Vincent and Walker 2000). In the study by ism dimensions might best predict risk of developing
Lundh et al. (1994), perfectionism was assessed with sleep problems.
a shortened version of Frost-MPS (Frost et al.
1990). In their normal sample, perfectionism corre- Methods
lated with a composite score on degree of sleeping
problems. In the clinical sample, obtained from a This research was reviewed and approved by the
sleep disorders clinic, the authors found that patients Medical Ethics Review Committee of the University
with persistent insomnia had significantly higher Hospital of Coimbra (Macedo et al. 2002).
scores of perfectionism than controls, especially on
the subscales CM and PS. Within the insomnia
Procedure
group, patients with a concomitant diagnosis of
major depression differed from the non-depressed Students were approached and measures were com-
insomniacs by having higher scores on the subscale pleted, as described previously, for the study first
DA. Lundh et al. (1994) hypothesized that perfec- stage (Azevedo et al. 2007). Data were collected at
tionism (high PS and high CM) may serve as a three stages/times separated by intervals of one
predisposing factor for the development of persistent academic year (2000/2001, 2001/2002, 2002/2003),
478 M.H. Azevedo et al.

when exams periods were not taking place. Baseline verify if the former groups were representative of the
was considered the first stage, the first year of the whole cohort subjects. Subjects followed up at T1
follow-up was Time 1 (T1) and the second year of had higher baseline SOP than subjects who were not
the follow-up was Time 2 (T2). The cooperation followed up (M 82.5, SD 14.90 vs. M 79.10;
was voluntary, in all stages, and confidentiality was SD 16.79, t 2.820; P 0.005). Apart from this
ensured. Most students returned the questionnaires. observation, no other significant differences were
Participation refusal occurred when questionnaires identified between groups.
were not handed.

Measures
Participants
Perfectionism. Hewitt and Flett-MPS (1991) was
Table I presents participants characteristics at each
used to measure Self-oriented perfectionism,
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Ulster at Jordanstown on 03/04/15

stage of the study. Mean age (standard deviation)


Socially-prescribed perfectionism and Other-or-
at baseline was 19.59 years (SD 1.61; range 
iented perfectionism. The scale has 45 items, and
1725), at T1 was 20.34 years (SD 1.41; range
the subject is asked to rate each item on a seven-
1826) and T2 was 20.88 years (SD 1.08;
point scale ranging from strongly disagree (score 1) to
range 1926). Of the students assessed at baseline,
strongly agree (score 7). Theoretically the scores may
65 and 48.5% completed the same measures 1 year
range from 45 to 315. A higher score is considered
(T1) and 2 years later (T2), respectively. The
an indicator of higher levels of perfectionism. The
attrition rates might be explained by the fact that
Portuguese version of the MPS has already been
in the third year of the Medicine Course a consider-
found to possess good psychometric properties
able number of modules started to take place at the
(Azevedo et al. 2007). Groups of perfectionism
University Hospital instead of at the Faculty. Classes
were formed based on baseline data scores and using
at the Hospital usually require clinical observations/
as cut-off points 1 SD below the mean (group with
interviews of the/to patients. For this reason, it
For personal use only.

low scores) and 1 SD above the mean (group with


became more difficult for students to have free
high scores). This procedure was performed for the
time at the end or beginning of the classes to
scale total score as well as for each perfectionism
participate in research studies. Subjects who were
followed up at T1 (n 592) and T2 (n 305) were dimension scores (SOP, SPP, and OOP).
compared with the remainder cohort subjects (the
Sleep disturbances. Two items, presented as state-
ones who did not participate at T1, n 278; and at
T2, n 565) on perfectionism dimensions means ments, were used to assess sleep disturbances: (1) I
and sleep variables at baseline. This made possible to have difficulty in falling asleep (difficulties initiating
sleep, DIS) and (2) I wake up many times during the
night (difficulties maintaining sleep, DMS). Each
Table I. Sample characteristics at baseline and follow-up.
item is scored on a six-point scale ranging from 0
Baseline (N870) T1 (n 592) T2 (n 305) (never) to 5 (always). In addition to scores obtained
N (%) n (%) n (%) for DIS and DMS (possible range 05), an overall
sleep disturbance score index (SDI) was calculated
Age
1718 241 (27.7) 10 (1.7) 
summing the scores of the individual items (possible
1920 358 (41.1) 344 (58.1) 128 (42.0) range 010). Higher scores indicate greater subjec-
2126 193 (22.2) 230 (38.9) 174 (57.0) tive sleep disturbance.
Gender Based on these items the following groups were
Males 326 (37.5) 210 (35.5) 99 (32.5) formed:
Females 544 (62.5) 382 (64.5) 206 (67.5)
1. Good sleepers: subjects who referred that
Marital status
Single 861 (99.0) 591 (99.8) 305 (100.0) ‘‘never’’ or ‘‘rarely’’ had DIS/DMS at baseline
Married 4 (.5) 1 (.2)  and at follow-up.
Course 2. Persistent insomnia: subjects who reported
Medicine 706 (81.1) 453 (76.5) 277 (90.8) having ‘‘often’’, ‘‘very often’’ or ‘‘always’’ DIS/
Dentistry 161 (18.5) 136 (23.0) 27 (8.9) DMS at baseline and at follow-up.
Course year 3. Onset insomnia: subjects with no sleep com-
1st 444 (51.0)   plaints at baseline (never/rarely/sometimes) but
2nd 187 (21.5) 365 (61.7)  reporting sleep complaints (DIS/DMS) either
3rd6th 200 (22.9) 227 (38.3) 305 (100.0)
at T1 (1 year) or T2 (2 years) of the follow-up.
Note: Numbers may vary due to missing values; T1, Time 1; T2, 4. Insomnia remission: subjects who reported
Time 2. sleep complaints at baseline but who remitted
Perfectionism and sleep: longitudinal study 479

(never/rarely/sometimes responses) either at T1 ‘‘often, very often and/or always’’ was mentioned
or T2. by 5.2% at baseline, 5.4% at T1 and 6.8% at T2 (see
Table III). Both DIS and/or DMS were experienced
Data analyses and statistics at baseline, T1 and T2 by 23.6, 22.2 and 25.6%,
respectively.
To investigate if perfectionism groups (previously
defined) diverged in DIS, DMS and SDI at different
stages of the study (baseline, T1 and T2), Student’s Perfectionism and sleep variables correlations between
t-test were applied. Odds ratio analyses were addi- the same variables at different follow-up time points
tionally performed to explore the likelihood of a
Correlations (Pearson’s Moment Product Correla-
subject with high scores in total perfectionism or in
tions) between Perfectionism variables at baseline
particular perfectionism dimensions experiencing
and at T1 and T2 revealed large correlation coeffi-
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Ulster at Jordanstown on 03/04/15

long-term sleep difficulties, compared with someone


with low perfectionism scores. To examine whether cients (ranging from 0.51 to 0.78) suggesting a good
sleep groups (formerly defined) differed in perfec- temporal stability of perfectionism dimensions (Ta-
tionism mean scores at T1, the one-way ANOVA ble IV). Spearman’s Rank Correlations between
test was applied. The Tamhane’s test and the least sleep variables at baseline and at T1 and T2 revealed
significant difference test were subsequently used to medium to large coefficients (ranging from 0.40 to
investigate differences within specific perfectionism 0.62) suggesting a reasonable temporal stability
groups. At T2 the non-parametric test Kruskal within sleep variables (Table IV).
Wallis was used to explore if sleep groups diverged
in perfectionism scores. MannWhitney tests were
Comparisons between perfectionism groups and sleep
applied to investigate differences between two spe-
variables
cific groups. Finally, correlation analyses were per-
formed followed by linear regressions to evaluate if The group with higher perfectionism total scores at
For personal use only.

the three perfectionism dimensions (baseline scores) baseline revealed significantly higher mean scores in
explained sleep difficulties (DIS, DMS and SDI) at DMS (M 1.33, SD 0.91) and SDI (baseline)
T1 and T2. According to Cohen (1992), correla- (M 3.20, SD 1.86) than the group with lower
tions of 0.10 represent small effect, 0.30 medium, perfectionism total score (baseline) (DMS, M
and 0.50 large effect. 1.01, SD 0.82, P 0.018; SDI, M2.60, SD 
1.56, P 0.024).
Results The former group also revealed significantly
higher mean scores in all sleep variables (DIS,
Descriptive data DMS and SDI) at T2 (2 years after baseline
Perfectionism and sleep variables mean scores and measurements) comparatively to the group with
standard deviations at baseline, T1 and T2 are lower total perfectionism scores (at baseline) (see
presented in Table II. Table V).
Difficulty initiating sleep (DIS) described as With respect to SPP, the group with high scores in
‘‘often, very often and/or always’’ was reported by this dimension (baseline) showed significantly more
18.4% at baseline, 16.8% at T1 and 18.9% at T2. DIS, DMS and SDI in baseline, T1 and T2 than the
Difficulty maintaining sleep (DMS) reported as group with low scores in this dimension (Table VI).
Table II. Perfectionism and sleep variables mean scores and standard deviations (SD).

Baseline (N870) T1 (n592) T2 (n 305)


Mean (SD; range) Mean (SD; range) Mean (SD; range)

Perfectionism
SOP 81.41 (15.60; 18117) 79.61 (14.89; 35119) 77.88 (16.07; 26119)
SPP 44.78 (12.28; 1594) 43.22 (12.12; 1596) 43.37 (12.38; 1897)
OOP 44.84 (6.98; 1863) 45.29 (7.32; 1663) 45.31 (7.16; 2462)
Total scale 186.13 (27.48; 97265) 183.32 (27.18; 103302) 181.92 (29.01; 87277)
Sleep
DIS 1.70 (1.60; 05) 1.61 (1.04; 05) 1. 67 (1.28; 05)
DMS 1.18 (0.80; 05) 1.23 (0.80; 04) 1.13 (0.84; 05)
SDI 2.89 (1.64; 010) 2.84 (1.59; 09) 2.07 (1.39; 06)

SOP, Self-oriented perfectionism; SPP, Socially-prescribed perfectionism; OOP, Other-oriented perfectionism; DIS, Difficulties initiating
sleep; DMS, Difficulties maintaining sleep; SDI, Sleep disturbance index; SD, Standard deviation; T1, Time 1; T2, Time 2.
480 M.H. Azevedo et al.
Table III. DIS and DMS frequency at baseline and follow-up.

Baseline (N870) T1 (n592) T2 (n 305)

DIS DMS DIS DMS DIS DMS


N (%) N (%) N (%) N (%) N (%) N (%)

Never 84 (9.7) 150 (17.2) 56 (9.5) 84 (14.2) 33 (10.8) 41 (13.4)


Rarely 328 (37.7) 467 (53.7) 262 (44.3) 331 (55.9) 124 (40.7) 165 (54.1)
Sometimes 298 (34.3 208 (23.9) 175 (29.6) 145 (24.5) 91 (29.8) 78 (25.6)
Often 100 (11.5) 31 (3.6) 61 (10.3) 22 (3.7) 31 (10.2) 15 (4.9)
Very often 45 (5.2) 9 (1.0) 29 (4.9) 10 (1.7) 20 (6.6) 5 (1.6)
Always 15 (1.7) 5 (.6) 8 (1.4)  6 (2.0) 1 (.3)
NK   1 (.2)   

NK, Not known; DIS, Difficulties initiating sleep; DMS, Difficulties maintaining sleep; T1, Time 1; T2, Time 2.
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Ulster at Jordanstown on 03/04/15

Considering SOP and OOP dimensions, only a results were only found for SPP scores. The persis-
small number of significant results were found. The tent insomnia group showed significantly higher SPP
group with high SOP values revealed higher mean values (n 23, M 55.0, SD 19.0) than the good
scores in DMS in T2 than the group with low SOP sleepers group (n 74, M42.2, SD 12.28, P 
(M 1.47, SD 0.98 vs. M1.09, SD 0.74, P  0.02).
0.033). The group with high OOP revealed less
DMS (M 1.16, SD 0.80) than the group with
low OOP (M 1.39, SD 0.86). Correlational and regression analyses
Exploring SPP in detail, subjects with high values Pearsons’ correlations between perfectionism di-
in this dimension at baseline were approximately 3 mensions and sleep variables showed that SOP at
times more likely to have overall sleep problems baseline was significantly and positively correlated
(x2 9.164; P 0.002; OR 2.696 CI 95%, 1.454
For personal use only.

with DIS, DMS and SDI at T2 (Table VIII). SPP


to 5.001), 3 times more likely to report DIS (x2  was significantly and positively associated with DIS,
11.438; P 0.001; OR 3.031, CI 95%, 1.626 to DMS and SDI at T1 and T2. OOP was significantly
5.650) and 2 times more likely to refer DMS (x2  and negatively associated with DMS at T1 (1 year
4.386; P0.036; OR 2.101 CI 95%, 1.097 to after baseline). The magnitude of the correlation
4.024) 1 year later (T1) than subjects with low SPP. coefficients which were statistically significant varied
Moreover, subjects with high SPP at baseline were between small and medium sizes (r 0.094 and
approximately 7 times more likely to refer overall 0.298). Medium coefficients were observed between
sleep disturbance (x2 4.59; P0.047; OR 7.22, baseline SPP scores and SDI and DMS at T2 (r $
CI 95%, 1.08 to 48.48) than subjects with low 0.3). Finally, a multiple linear regression performed
SPP 2 years later (T2). using the three perfectionism dimensions as predic-
tors of DIS, DMS and SDI at T1 and T2 (Table VIII)
revealed that these dimensions (perfectionism base-
Perfectionism and sleep status at follow-up
Table IV. Perfectionism and sleep variables correlations at differ-
Significant differences in total perfectionism and SPP
ent follow-up time points.
were found between sleep groups (T1). Post-hoc
multiple comparisons tests (least significant differ- Baseline Baseline T1
ence, LSD, comparison test) revealed that the onset Variable vs. T1 vs. T2 vs. T2
group and persistent insomnia group had signifi- Perfectionism
cantly higher baseline total perfectionism scores Self-oriented 0.689** 0.606** 0.784**
(M 193.4, SD 26.3, P B0.05 and M193.0; Socially-prescribed 0.732** 0.728** 0.789**
SD 30.5, P B0.05, respectively) than the good Other-oriented 0.544** 0.513** 0.613**
Total scale 0.716** 0.658** 0.764**
sleepers group (M 183.5; SD 26.3) (Table VII).
Tamhane’s comparisons tests also revealed that Sleep
DIS 0.569** 0.504** 0.619**
persistent insomnia group had significantly higher DMS 0.427** 0.397** 0.502**
SPP baseline scores (M 51.5; SD 15.8) than the SDI 0.584** 0.493** 0.554**
remission (M 44.2, SD 9.8, P B0.05) or good
sleepers groups (M 41.9; SD 11.4, P B0.01). DIS, Difficulties initiating sleep; DMS, Difficulties maintaining
sleep; SDI, Sleep disturbance index.
At T2 comparisons failed to reveal significant Note: Pearson’s correlations were applied to Perfectionism vari-
differences between sleep groups (results not ables and Spearman’s Rank correlations were used with Sleep
shown), in respect to SOP and OOP. Significant variables; T1, Time 1; T2, Time 2.
Perfectionism and sleep: longitudinal study 481
Table V. Comparison between baseline perfectionism total score
enduring personality trait. A reasonable temporal
groups in DIS, DMS and SDI at baseline, Time 1 and Time 2.
stability was equally found when exploring correla-
Group with Group with tions between sleep variables at baseline and at T1
low total score high total score and T2; coefficients ranged from medium to large
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
magnitudes (0.400.62). The highest correlation
n 80 n 88 t P
was found with DIS variable (0.500.57). Consis-
DIS tent with previous findings (review Sateia et al.
Baseline 1.59 (1.12) 1.88 (1.20) 1.599 0.112 2000) sleep complaints were found to be stable
Time 1 1.58 (1.05) 1.76 (1.29) 1.021 0.309
over time, with 51% of students with sleep distur-
Time 2 1.48 (1.02) 2.04 (1.45) 2.120 0.037
bances at baseline also reporting symptoms at
DMS
follow-up.
Baseline 1.01 (.82) 1.33 (.91) 2.382 0.018
Time 1 1.15 (.78) 1.30 (.91) 1.113 0.267 Students with high levels of SPP at baseline
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Ulster at Jordanstown on 03/04/15

Time 2 1.07 (.74) 1.54 (1.04) 2.535 0.013 showed consistently having more DIS, DMS and
SDI SDI than subjects with low levels of SPP through all
Baseline 2.60 (1.56) 3.20 (1.86) 2.274 0.024 assessment times/stages. These students were 3
Time 1 2.72 (1.56) 3.06 (1.91) 1.225 0.222 times more likely to have overall sleep problems or
Time 2 1.76 (1.28) 2.50 (1.41) 2.628 0.010 DIS, 2 times more likely to have DMS 1 year later
DIS, Difficulties initiating sleep; DMS, Difficulties maintaining (T1), and 7 times more likely to report overall sleep
sleep; SDI, Sleep disturbance index; SD, Standard Deviation; t, disturbance 2 years later (T2) than subjects with low
Student’s t-test. SPP. Additionally, students with persistent insomnia
showed higher scores on SPP, at T1 and T2, than
line scores) explained 4% of the total variance of the good sleepers.
SDI [r2 0.040, F(3, 541) 8.505, P B0.001], One of the earliest and most enduring insomnia
3.4% of the total variance of DIS [r2 0.034; F(3, conceptualisations refers to psychophysiological
541) 7.409, P B0.001] and 2.6% of the total arousal (Buysse and Dorsey 2002). From the several
For personal use only.

variance of DMS [r2 0.026, F(3, 542) 5.882, reasons for hyperarousal, psychological distress (PD)
P 0.001] at T1. At T2 the three perfectionism and psychiatric disorders are among the most salient
variables explained 6.4% of the total variance of the causes of insomnia (Hauri 2002). It is well known
SDI [r2 0.064, F(3, 277) 7.274, P B0.001], that the aetiology of PD and psychiatric syndromes
5.6% of the total variance of DIS [r2 0.056, F(3, involves the interplay of many biological (e.g.,
277) 6.516, P B0.001] and 8.1% of the total genetic) and environmental factors (Tandon and
variance of DMS [r2 0.081, F(3, 277) 9.150, McGuffin 2002; Rijsdijk et al. 2003). Therefore, as
P B0.001]. Considering the partial correlations it would be expected in complex transmission
between perfectionism and sleep variables, baseline disorders, the pathway between genetic/develop-
SPP was a significant predictor of DIS, DMS and mental risk factors and the behavioural phenotypes
SDI at T1 and T2. DMS at T1 (1 year after
baseline) could be predicted (positive association) Table VI. Comparison between baseline Socially prescribed
perfectionism groups (SPP) in sleep variables at baseline, Time
by SPP baseline and by OOP (negative association). 1 and Time 2.

Low SPP High SPP


Discussion and conclusions Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
n 83 n89 t P
The main finding of this study, reported for the first
time, is that SPP was found to be the most reliable DIS
predictor of self reported sleep disturbances at 1 year Baseline 1.34 (1.01) 2.00 (1.24) 3.813 B0.001
(T1) and 2 years (T2) of the follow-up. This result Time 1 1.39 (.99) 2.00 (1.20) 3.661 B0.001
Time 2 1.60 (1.15) 2.30 (1.31) 2.756 0.007
confirms our cross-sectional previous results which
indicated that SPP was the only dimension consis- DMS
Baseline .92 (.74) 1.49 (.88) 4.688 B0.001
tently associated with sleep disturbance in under-
Time 1 1.13 (.72) 1.43 (.84) 2.489 0.014
graduate students of both genders (Azevedo et al. Time 2 1.02 (.71) 1.62 (1.00) 3.389 0.001
2007). It also highlights the long-term adverse
SDI
effects of SPP on sleep. Baseline 2.25 (1.39) 3.49 (1.97) 4.790 B0.001
Correlations between perfectionism variables at Time 1 2.52 (1.43) 3.43 (1.74) 3.726 B0.001
baseline with these same variables at T1 and T2 Time 2 1.57 (1.11) 2.66 (1.31) 4.294 B0.001
revealed large correlation coefficients particularly DIS, Difficulties initiating sleep; DMS, Difficulties maintaining
evident when considering SPP (0.730.79), lending sleep; SDI, Sleep disturbance index; SD, Standard Deviation; t,
support to the notion of SPP as a measure of an Student’s t-test.
482 M.H. Azevedo et al.
Table VII. Baseline perfectionism scores by sleep groups at Time 1 (1-year follow-up).

Sleep groups

1 2 3 4
Good Sleepers Remission Onset Persistent
Baseline n 189 n 54 n48 n 57
Perfectionism Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) P Comparisons (1)

SOP 81.4 (14.6) 84.6 (12.4) 85.5 (14.6) 84.3 (17.2) 0.231
SPP 41.9 (11.4) 44.2 (9.8) 46.2 (12.1) 51.5 (15.8) 0.000** 4 1**,2* (2)
OOP 45.2 (7.0) 46.1 (7.2) 46.1 (6.1) 42.8 (8.0) 0.05
Total scale 183.5 (26.3) 189.8 (22.8) 193.4 (27.0) 193.0 (30.5) 0.038* 1 B 3*, 4* (3)

(1) Comparisons between groups performed with one-way ANOVA.


(2) Tamhane’s test.
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Ulster at Jordanstown on 03/04/15

(3) LSD test; *PB0.05; **P B0.01.


SOP, Self-oriented perfectionism; SPP, Socially-prescribed perfectionism; OOP, Other-oriented perfectionism; SD, standard deviation.

is not straightforward. Instead, it is a long and (Stöber and Otto 2006), specifically to depression,
devious road with many crossroads and dead-ends. anxiety, eating and OC disorders (Maia et al. in
Some alternatives ways have been proposed to over- preparation).
come this complexity and to clarify the underlying It is worth mentioning that the main reason for
mechanisms by which genetic susceptibility influ- student’s self-referral in the outpatient clinic for
ence psychopathological conditions. One consists in psychological treatment and counselling of Coimbra
using alternative phenotypic measures (e.g., endo- University is stress-related problems, namely social
phenotypes), including personality traits such as anxiety and psychological distress associated with
perfectionism. In a near future, we may discover as evaluative concerns (Pereira 2004). Likewise, the
For personal use only.

Insel and Collins (2003) have stated about the study of Cabrita et al. (2004), based in a large
aetiology of anorexia nervosa that its genotype is sample of Lisbon University students found that
not linked to a specific disorder but to a perfectio- stress-related complaints (anxiety, depression and
nistic, risk-aversive personality style that confers insomnia) were the most frequently stated health
vulnerability to many syndromes. There is some problems to justify the use of psychoactive drugs
evidence of the involvement of genetic factors in during the fortnight before the interview. Tranqui-
perfectionism (Lilenfeld et al. 2000; Woodside et al. lizers (BZD; valerian) were taken by 7.2% students
2002; Tozzi et al. 2004). Moreover, an association
(with valerian accounting for 35.5% of the total
between perfectionism and insomnia has system-
number of medicines consumed) and 1.7% used
atically been found in the literature (Lundh et al.
antidepressants.
1994; Vincent and Walker 2000; Azevedo et al.
Therefore, it can be argued that the association
2007). Therefore, based in the existing literature, we
between SPP and future insomnia observed in our
believe that perfectionism can be seen as an inter-
study can be a result of PD experienced by students.
mediate phenotype associated with sleep difficulties.
It is noteworthy that, in our study, SPP mean values
To support the case of perfectionism as an
intermediate phenotype of sleep disturbances, it is for the insomnia group and for good sleepers were
also important to insure a strong temporal stability similar to the ones found by Vincent and Walker
of the influence of this trait measure and, also to (2000) in their chronic insomnia and healthy control
assure that it is not predominantly state-related group. In addition, our insomnia group SPP mean
(Rice and Aldea 2006). Research on depression values at T1 were equal to the scores of psychiatric
and sleep disturbances supports that perfectionism outpatients with a clinical diagnosis of depression
is a personality trait with both relative stability and and/or anxiety, and similar, at T2, to the scores of a
state dependence effects (Zuroff et al. 1999; Cox mixed sample of outpatients with eating and/or
and Enns 2003; Rice and Aldea 2006; Maia et al. in obsessivecompulsive disorders found in a previous
preparation). study of our group (Maia et al. in press).
Specific personality traits, including perfectionism It is particularly relevant that certain persona-
have been associated with cognitive arousal and lity traits correlate with particular cognitive aspects,
psychological distress (PD; Kales et al. 1983; Kales which play a central role in the conceptual
and Kales 1984; Shafran and Mansell 2001). Some models of PD (Beck 1976; Clark 1999) and in the
dimensions of perfectionism (e.g., SPP, CM, DA) explanation of complex mechanisms involved in the
have been consistently related to higher levels of PD interplay between personality, PD and insomnia.
Perfectionism and sleep: longitudinal study 483

SOP, Self-oriented perfectionism; SPP, Socially-prescribed perfectionism; OOP, Other-oriented perfectionism; r, Pearson moment-product correlation coefficients; Partial R, partial correlation
According to the cognitive model of insomnia,

0.231**
Partial R

0.013

0.000
worriers and ruminators are more likely to react to
life stressors creating cognitive arousal which, in

T2
turn, will affect sleep, leading to insomnia (Perlis
et al. 2005). There is empirical evidence that some

Sleep disturbances index

0.271**
0.133*
perfectionism negative facets (e.g., SPP) are asso-

0.016
r
ciated with worry, rumination and autonomic arou-
sal (Flett et al. 1995, 1998). Worriers have an
Table VIII. Correlation coefficients and partial correlations between baseline Perfectionism and sleep disturbances scores at Time 1 (n 592) and Time 2 (n 305).

0.181**
elevated and intense fear of failure that makes
Partial R

0.015

0.062
them hesitant about making mistakes. In other
words, they need to be absolutely sure that they
are doing the ‘‘right thing’’ before a response can be
T1
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Ulster at Jordanstown on 03/04/15

0.200** made (Tallis et al. 1991). These are characteristics


0.083*
which overlap with those that have been described in
0.053
r

some of the perfectionism negative facets (DA and


CM; SPP) (Frost et al. 1993; Bieling et al. 2004).
According to Flett et al. (1991) and Frost et al.
(1990) the fact that perfectionists tend to anticipate
0.244**
Partial R

0.042

0.003

stress (worrying about future failures) might be one


of the possible explanations for the association
T2

between SPP and sleep disturbance. Another im-


0.173**
0.298**
Difficulties maintaining sleep

portant mechanism that can explain the relationship


0.094* 0.004

between perfectionism and psychological distress is


Sleep disturbances

the subject coping style. There is a large amount of


literature giving support to the association between
0.116**
Partial R
For personal use only.

0.031

SPP and negative coping responses and negative


cognitive appraisals. Flett et al. (1998) reflect about
rumination as a coping response of perfectionists
which can perpetuate stress in their lives (e.g.,
T1

leading to sleep difficulties).


0.152**
0.097*

Some studies of relevance in the present context


0.065
r

have investigated the association between perfection-


ism and physical health/somatic complaints, includ-
ing sleep problems. Saboonchi and Lundh (2003)
found that SOP and SPP were positively correlated
0.196**
Partial R

0.048

0.049

with somatic complaints such as daytime sleepiness,


headaches, tension, and insomnia. In the study of
T2

Molnar et al. (2006), SPP was associated with


0.141**
0.252**

coefficients; *pB0.05; **p B0.01; T1, Time 1; T2, Time 2.

poorer physical health. This relationship was par-


Difficulties initiating sleep

0.049

tially mediated by low positive affect and high


r

negative affect. Research has demonstrated that


socially prescribed perfectionists tend to rely on
0.187**
Partial R

emotion-focused coping and avoidance-oriented


0.047

0.023

coping, tend to have a negative self-perception of


problem-solving orientation, engage in self-blame,
show a lack of constructive thinking, and deal with
T1

stressful situations in ways that indicate a sense of


0.189**
0.030

0.053

helplessness or hopelessness (Flett et al. 1994, 1996;


r

Hewitt et al. 1994, 1995; Dunkley et al. 2000).


Taken together, these findings suggest that mala-
daptive coping may further contribute to explain the
link between SPP and health complaints.
Perfectionism

In conclusion, a constellation of factors inclu-


ding a negative cognitive style (e.g., proneness to
Baseline

rumination) associated with autonomic arousal and


OOP
SOP
SPP

anxiety (e.g., worry) has been consistently found in


484 M.H. Azevedo et al.

insomniacs (see Harvey 2002). To our knowledge Flett GL, Hewitt PL, Blankstein K, Mosher SW. 1991. Perfec-
tionism, self-actualization, and personal adjustment. J Soc
there is only one prospective study where these
Behav Pers 6:147160.
pathways were analysed. Results showed an associa- Flett GL, Russo FA, Hewitt PL. 1994. Dimensions of perfection-
tion between perfectionism, PD and insomnia ism and constructive thinking as a coping response. J Ration
(Jansson-Fröjmark and Linton 2007). However, Emot Cogn Behav Ther 12:163179.
when emotional distress was accounted for, none Flett GL, Hewitt PL, Endler NS, Tassone C. 1995. Perfectionism
of the perfectionism subscales (CM and PS) con- and components of state and trait anxiety. Curr Psychol
13:326350.
tributed significantly to explain pre-existing and
Flett GL, Hewitt PL, Blankstein KR, Solnik M, Van Brunschot
future insomnia. M. 1996. Perfectionism, social problem-solving ability, and
Future work should examine the mechanisms psychological distress. J Ration Emot Cogn Behav Ther
linking SPP to insomnia/sleep disturbances, specifi- 14:245275.
cally exploring the role of psychological distress, Flett GL, Hewitt PL, Blankstein KR, Gray L. 1998. Psychological
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Ulster at Jordanstown on 03/04/15

worry and rumination. distress and the frequency of perfectionistic thinking. J Pers Soc
Psychol 75:13631381.
Frost RO, Marten P, Lahart C, Rosenblate R. 1990. The
dimensions of perfectionism. Cogn Ther Res 14:449468.
Acknowledgements Frost G, Heimberg R, Holt C, Mattia J, Neubauer A. 1993. A
comparison of two meaasures of perfectionism. Pers Indiv
The data for this report were drawn from a research
Differ 14:119126.
on Perfectionism and ObsessiveCompulsive Spec- Hamachek D. 1978. Psychodynamics of normal and neurotic
trum Disorders, funded by Fundação para a Ciência perfectionism. Psychology 15:2733.
e Tecnologia (FCT  no. 37569/PSI). The co- Harvey AG. 2002. A cognitive model of insomnia. Behav Res
operation of Professors and Students is gratefully Ther 40:869893.
acknowledged. Hauri PJ. 2002. Psychological and psychiatric issues in the
etiopathogenesis of insomnia. Primary care companion. J Clin
Psychiatry 4:1720.
Statement of interest Hewitt PL, Flett GL. 1991. Perfectionism in the self and social
For personal use only.

contexts: conceptualization, assessment, and association with


None to declare. psychopathology. J Pers Soc Psychol 60:456470.
Hewitt PL, Flett GL, Weber C. 1994. Dimensions of perfection-
ism and suicide ideation. Cogn Ther Res 18:439460.
References Hewitt PL, Flett GL, Ediger E. 1995. Perfectionism traits and
perfectionistic self-presentation in eating disorder attitudes,
Azevedo MH, Soares MJ, Bos SC, Gomes AA, Maia B, Marques characteristics and symptoms. Int J Eat Disord 18:317326.
M, et al. 2007. Perfectionism and sleep disturbance. World J Hollender MH. 1965. Perfectionism. Comp Psychiatry 6:94103.
Biol Psychiatry 9:19. Insel TR, Collins FS. 2003. Psychiatry in the genomics era. Am J
Beck AT. 1976. Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. Psychiatry 160:616620.
New York: International University Press. Jansson-Fröjmark M, Linton SG. 2007. Is perfectionism related
Bieling P, Israeli A, Antony M. 2004. Is perfectionism good, bad, to pre-existing and future insomnia? A prospective study. Br J
or both? Examining models of the perfectionism construct. Pers Clin Psychol 46(Pt 1):119124.
Indiv Differ 36:13731385. Kales A, Kales J. 1984. Evaluation and treatment of insomnia.
Burns D. 1980. The perfectionist’s script for self defeat. Psychol-
New York: Oxford University Press.
ogy Today November:3452.
Kales A, Caldwell AB, Soldatos CR, Bixler EO, Kales JD. 1983.
Buysse DJ, Dorsey CM. 2002. Current and experimental ther-
Biopsychobehavioral correlates of insomnia II. Pattern specifi-
apeutics of insomnia. In: Davis K, Charney D, Coyle JT,
city and consistency with the Minnesota Multiphasic Person-
Nemeroff C, editors. Neuropsychopharmacology. The fifth
ality Inventory. Psychosomatic Med 45:341345.
generation of progress. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams
Lacks P. 1987. Behavioral treatment for persistent insomnia.
and Wilkins. pp 19311943.
Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Cabrita J, Ferreira H, Iglesias P, Baptista T, Rocha E, Lopes da
Lilenfeld RL, Stein D, Bulik CM, Strober M, Plotnicov KH,
Silva A, et al. 2004. Patterns and determinants of psychoactive
drug use in Lisbon University students  a population-based Pollice C, Rao R, Nagy L, Kaye WH. 2000. Personality traits
study. Pharm World Sci 26:7982. among currently eating disorder, recovered, and never hill first-
Clark DM. 1999. Anxiety disorders: Why they persist and how to degree female relatives bulimic and control women. Psychol
treat them. Behav Res Ther 37:S5S27. Med 30(6):13991410.
Cohen J. 1992. A power primer. Psychol Bull 112:155159. Lundh LG, Broman JE, Hetta J, Saboonchi F. 1994. Perfection-
Cox BJ, Enns MW. 2003. Relative stability of dimensions of ism and insomnia. Scand J Behav Ther 23:318.
perfectionism in depression. Can J Behav Sci 35(2):124132. Macedo A, Pocinho F, Soares MJ, Dourado A, Campos C,
Dunkley DM, Blankstein KR. 2000. Self-critical perfectionism, Domingues O, et al. 2002. Perfeccionismo  uma dimensão
coping, hassles, and current distress: a structural equation fenotı́pica comum aos fenómenos obsessivos e distúrbios
modeling approach. Cogn Ther Res 24:713730. alimentares? Psiquiatr Clı́n 23:163174.
Flett GL, Hewitt PL. 2002. Perfectionism and maladjustment: an Maia BR, Soares MJ, Gomes AA, Marques M, Pereira AT, Cabral
overview of theoretical, definitional, and treatment issues. In: A, et al. Perfectionism in obsessivecompulsive and eating
Flett GL, Hewitt PL, editors. Perfectionism: theory, research, disorders. Rev Bras Psiquiatr (in preparation).
and treatment. Washington, DC: American Psychological Maia BR, Soares MJ, Pereira AT, Marques M, Bos S, Gomes A,
Association. pp 531. Valente J, Azevedo, MH, Macedo A. 2009. State dependence
Perfectionism and sleep: longitudinal study 485
and relative stability of perfectionism in sleep disturbances. Eur Sateia MJ, Doghramji K, Hauri PJ, Morin CM. 2000. Evaluation
J Pers (submitted). of chronic insomnia. An American Academy of Sleep Medicine
Molnar DS, Reker DL, Culp NA, Sadava SW, DeCourville NH. review. Sleep 23:243308.
2006. A mediated model of perfectionism, affect, and physical Shafran R, Mansell W. 2001. Perfectionism and psychopathology:
health. J Res Pers 40:482500. a review of research and treatment. Clin Psychol Rev 21:879
Pacht A. 1984. Reflections on perfection. Am Psychol 39:386 906.
390. Stöber J, Otto K. 2006. Positive conceptions of perfectionism:
Pereira AMS. 2004. Concepções e práticas metodológicas em approaches, evidence, challenges. Per Soc Psychol Rev 10:295
diferentes paradigmas de investigação. In: Oliveira L, Pereira A, 319.
Santiago R, editors. Investigação em Educação. Abordagens Tallis F, Eysenck MW, Mathews A. 1991. Elevated evidence
Conceptuais e práticas. Porto: Porto Editora. pp 4758. requirements and worry. Behav Person Individ Diff 12:2127.
Perlis ML, Smith MT, Pigeon WR. 2005. Etiology and patho- Tandon K, McGuffin P. 2002. The genetic basis for psychiatric
physiology of insomnia. In: Kryger M, Roth T, Dement WC, illness in man. Eur J Neurosci 16:403407.
Tozzi F, Anderson CB, Bulik CM, Mazzeo SE, Aggen SH, Neale
editors. Principles and practice of sleep medicine. Philadelphia,
BM, Neale MC. 2004. The structure of perfectionism: a twin
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by University of Ulster at Jordanstown on 03/04/15

PA: Saunders Company. pp 714725.


study. Behav Genet 34(5):483494.
Rice KG, Aldea MA. 2006. State dependence and trait stability of
Vincent NK, Walker JR. 2000. Perfectionism and chronic
perfectionism: a short-term longitudinal study. J Couns Psychol
insomnia. J Psychosom Res 49:349354.
53:205212. Woodside DB, Bulik CM, Halmi KA, Fichter MM, Kaplan A,
Rijsdijk FV, Snieder H, Ormel J, Sham P, Goldberg DP, Spector
Berrettini WH, et al. 2002. Personality, perfectionism and
TD. 2003. Genetic and environmental influences on psycho- attitudes toward eating in parents of individuals with eating
logical distress in the population: General Health Question- disorders. Intern J Eat Dis 31:290299.
naire analyses in UK twins. Psychol Med 33:793801. Zuroff DC, Blatt SJ, Sanislow CA, Bondi CM, Pilkonis PA. 1999.
Saboonchi F, Lundh LG. 2003. Perfectionism, anger, somatic Vulnerability to depression: re-examining state dependence and
health and positive affect. Pers Indiv Differ 35:15851599. relative stability. J Abnorm Psychol 108(1):7689.
For personal use only.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi