Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 7

Pronouns

one or body; everything, someone, anybody, etc. Note that with no + one the
compoundirl'g is generally not reflected in the orthography; syntactically
8
and phonologically we regard no one as a single word in No one came but as a
sequence of two words in No one ofthe texts would suffice alone - compare anyone
Adjectives and adjective
and any one ofthe texts. (As a separate word, one contrasts here with two, etc.: phrases
any two ofthe texts; as part ofa compound, ofcourse, it does not.) Also included
in the indefinite pronouns is none, as in Ed had little money and 1 had none. As we
have seen, none is the pronominal equivalent of the determinative no; it is not
usefully analysed as no + one, for while one forces an individuated interpreta­
tion, none does not, as can be seen from the examplesjust given, where it has a
mass interpretation. 8. I Adjectives
For the rest, the indefinite subclass will contain many, much, several,jew, all, The most central members of the word-class adjective have the
both, etc., if these are in fact to be analysed as pronouns at all: see §3 above. following four properties:
Because of the doubtful status of these, the set of indefinite pronouns is the (a) Functional potential, I. They occur as head in phrases functioning as
least clearly defined of the pronoun subclasses, and considerable differences predicative complement in clause structure: this is what we have called the
will be found from grammar to grammar with respect to its membership, its predicative use of adjectives. Thus the prototypical adjectives careless, intel­
name and further subdivisions within it. ligent and tiresome are used predicatively in He was careless, She seemed very
intelligent, They fOund it rather tiresome.
FURTHER READING
On the grammar of pronouns and the various subclasses, see Quirk et al. (b) Functional potential, II. They occur as head in phrases functioning as
197 I:§§4. 106-28. Anaphora has attracted a great deal of attention in the last twenty pre-hea~ modifier in NP structure: this is the attributive use. It is illus­
years: see, for example, Hankamer & Sag 1976 (followed up in Sag 1979), Halliday & trated in a careless mistake, a very intelligent woman, that rather tiresome politician.
Hasan 1976, Lyons 1977:§15.3, Wasow 1979, Evans 1980, Carden 1982, Reinhart (c) Functional poten tial, III. They occur as head in phrases functioning as
1983. Some scholars, e.g. Quirk et al. 197 I, Halliday & Hasan 1976, restrict the term
post-head modifier in NP structure: this we will call the postpositive use.
'anaphora' to what I am calling unmarked-order anaphora (with the antecedent pre­
ceding the anaphor), using 'cataphora' for marked-order anaphora (where the ante­
Thus the above adjectives are used postpositively in people careless in their
cedent follows). Where 'anaphora' covers both, the two orders are distinguished in attitude to money, someone very intelligent, something rather tiresome. The
the transformational literature as 'forwards anaphora' vs 'backwards anaphora', postpositive use is much less frequent than the first two, and subject to
while Lyons uses respectively 'backward-looking' and 'forward-looking' (or quite restrictive conditions (see 6.10), but it is nevertheless a significant
'anticipatory'); the conflict arises from the fact that the transformational terminology property of central adjectives that they can be used in this way.
reflects the early (but discredited) analysis where an anaphor is transformationally
(d) Modification and inflection. Prototypical adjectives are 'gradable' (see
substituted for a copy of the antecedent: this leads one to focus on the antecedent and
move 'forwards' in the case where the anaphor follows or 'backwards' when it pre­
6·7) and as such take modifiers indicating degree, notably very, rather, quite,
cedes; Lyons' terminology focuses on the anaphor and looks backwards to a preceding so, too, how, etc.: very careless, rather intelligent, how tiresome. More particularly,
antecedent or forwards to a following one - although this is surely a better way of gradable adjectives either enter into inflectional contrasts of comparison,
looking at it the conflict with the earlier terms is potentially a source of confusion and I asm
have accordingly preferred to speak of unmarked order vs marked order, where there (I) Absolute Comparative Superlative
is no possibility of misinterpretation. Some writers extend the term anaphora to cases
where there is no antecedent and the pronoun or whatever is interpreted deictically­ big bigger biggest
Hankamer & Sag, for example, call this 'pragmatically controlled anaphora', as op­ pretty prettier prettiest
posed to 'syntactically controlled anaphora' for cases where there is an antecedent; good better best
Halliday & Hasan, by contrast, distinguish these cases as respectively exophora vs or occur in the analytic comparative and superlative constructions marked
endophora (with the latter equivalent to anaphora in the sense in which I have used
by more and most: careless, more careless, most careless.
it). On deixis, see Lyons 1977: Ch. 15, 198Ia:22&-35. On gender as a general category,
see Lyons 1968:§7.3. On reciprocals, see Dougherty 1974. No one of these properties is unique to adjectives, but only adjectives
29 8 299
Adjectives and adjective phrases 8.1 Adjectives

possess all four. For example, nouns generally have properties (a) and (b), The morphological rules forming comparatives and superlatives are rela­
and to a more limited extent (c) too, butthey lack (d) - cr. the noun bastard in tively simple: for regular adjectives, the suffixes /;}r/ (see footnote I) and
He is a bastard, his bastard son, his son the bastard, *a very bastardchild. This is not to /Ist/ respectively are added. There are one or two concomitant
suggest, however, that it is only property (d) that distinguishes adjectives phonological modifications: stem-final syllabic /1/ loses its syllabicity (so
from nouns - we must of course also take account of the other properties of that the marked forms ofJeeble are disyllabic, like the absolute form) and
nouns discussed in 6.1: their ability to take determiners as dependents, to final /1]/ becomes /I]Q/ (young, / jAI]/ in most varieties, is paired withyounger,
head phrases functioning as subject, object, etc. Thus, to continue with the / jAI]Q;)/, and so on). There are only a handful of irregular adjectives. Better
same example, bastard shows clear noun properties in He is a bastard and best are the comparative and superlative forms ofthe adjective good, and
(determiner a), That bastard needs watching (determiner that, and subject also of well, which can be used as an adjective or an adverb; adjectival well
function), in its inflectional contrast with bastards, and so on. We will take up means "in good health" and the meaning of better is then not fully predictable
the distinction between nouns and adjectives in the next chapter, but it will from its analysis as the corresponding comparative, for I am now better can be,
be clear from what has been said already that the central members ofthe two and usually is, interpreted as "I am now recovered, i.e. well again" (cr. the
classes are very sharply distinct in terms of the kinds ofdependent they take semantic irregularity of the adverb later mentioned in 1.7). Worse and worst
and the range offunctions realised by the phrases they head. We should add likewise serve as the marked forms ofthe adjective bad, the adverb badly and
that property (d) does not distinguish adjectives from adverbs: note in par­ of ill, which has both adjectival and adverbial uses. Far, which again be­
ticular that a number of adverbs enter into inflectional contrasts of longs to both adjective and adverb classes, has the irregular forms farther/
comparison (badly ~ worse ~ worst; soon ~ sooner ~ soonest, etc.). further and farthest/furthest. Elder and eldest might be analysed as irregular
As in earlier chapters, the above properties have been formulated so as to forms of old, coexisting with the regular forms in a specialised use - or else we
apply to words, but it is a trivial matter to reformulate them so as to apply to might treat them as forms ofa defective lexeme lacking an absolute form. In
lexemes. There are, however, very many adjective words which carry no either case they require ad hoc lexical description: they are used only
inflectional property and where the concept oflexeme is consequently inap­ attributively with a kinship term like brother or daughter or (for elder) the
plicable- the examples used above, careless, intelligent and tiresome, are of this noun statesman as head - and they cannot take modifiers of their own (cr. a
type. As argued in 3.2, we will invoke the concept oflexeme only where we much older/*elder brother). One comparative form that very clearly has no
have inflectional, as opposed to analytic, contrasts of comparison. absolute or superlative counterpart is other- note that this behaves syntac­
Whether an adjective stem can undergo the inflectional processes that tically like an ordinary comparative in that it enters into construction with
yield comparative and superlative forms is to a large extent predictable from than (anyone other/ taller than Ed). In upper (cr. the upper level) the suffix is added
the morphological and phonological properties of the stem. Lexical stems to a stem which does not belong to the adjective class: upper is not the com­
like bored or worried that are derived by conversion from the -en forms ofverbs parative form of an adjective up, but an adjective derived from an adverb/
(see 9.3) do not permit these processes to apply: *Hefelt boreder than he had ever preposition. Its opposite, lower, is the comparative form oflow, and forms
felt before (cr. Hefelt more bored .. .). For the rest, if the stem is monosyllabic, like upper (cr. also inner and outer) thus provide further illustration of the lack
inflection is normally possible: big ~ bigger, sad ~ sadder, tall ~ taller, and so ofa sharp division between inflectional and lexical morphology.
on. Disyllabic stems that take inflectional suffixes are normally either: (i) Let us turn now to the central cases oflexical morphology. Although there
morphologically simple, ending in syllabic /1/ (gentle, simple), /;}r/ or are many adjectives with simple lexical stems, a high proportion have stems
/ (j )u;}r / I (clever, obscure), /;}U / (hollow, narrow) and certain other unaccented derived by affixation, conversion or compounding. As in the verb and noun
syllables (common, quiet) - polite is a rare example where the second syllable is chapters, we will give only a brief outline of the processes involved; not all
accented; (ii) morphologically complex, ending in one of the suffixes -ly the examples cited will be prototypical adjectives - for example, a number
(deadly,friendly) or -=Y (funny, noisy) or beginning with the prefix -un (unkind). will lack property (d) (see §2 below).
Stems of more than two syllables do not inflect, except for a few (such as
unfriendly) formed by adding a negative prefix to a disyllabic stem that (a) Class-changing suffixation. There are a considerable number of suffixes
inflects. forming denominal adjectives: -ful (careful); -less (careless); -ly (friendly);
-like (childlike); -ish (childish); -esque (Picassoesque); -al, -ial, -ical, -ic (musical,
I In many varieties of English, including for example English Received Pronunciation, the
final Ir I is dropped unless followed without an intonational break by a vowel (in some styles
editorial, philosophical, heroic); -ous (grievous); -ian, -ese (Christian, Japanese);
by a vowel within the same word). and so on. The suffix -ed also forms denominal adjectives, as in walled (a

300 301
Adjectives and adjective phrases 8.2 Some non-central subclasses ojadjectives
walled garden, "a garden with a wall around"), but more often it is added to object in clause structure (cf. a swim that broke thel some recordl records) or the
adjective + noun sequences, as in long-haired, flat-chested, simple-minded, complement of a preposition (a law-abiding citizen may be compared with a
giant-sized, etc.: the process is relatively productive. Such forms make it citizen who abides by the law). In -en form compounds, the verb component is
impossible to maintain a rigid division between morphology and syntax: almost always understood passively: a much-debated question is equivalent to
we have here a morphological element added to a syntactic construction a question that islwas much debated (we have to say 'almost always' because of
(but note that the noun is always in the singular form, the form that examples like newly-arrived); again, see Ch. 9 for further elaboration. In
coincides with the lexical stem: thus a blue-eyed baby, not "'a blue-eyesed baby). addition there are a considerable number of compounds formed from
noun + adjective: tax-.free, carsick, etc.; a particularly productive subtype
(b) Class-preserving suffixation. Two of the suffixes mentioned in (a), -ish
has a colour adjective as second component: sky-blue, blood-red. Finally,
and -ly, can also derive adjectives from simpler adjectives. With -ish the
there are adjectives compounded from a pair of adjectives, or from a bound
class-preserving use is relatively productive, applying especially to adjec­
stem in -0 plus an adjective, where neither stem is subordinate to the other:
tives of colour, shape and the like: greenish, squarish, longish, etc.; class­
bitter-sweet, Sino- Tibetan, socio-historical.
preserving -ly is found in a few stems such as kindly, lowly, poorly where the
resultant meaning is of rather low predictability.
8.2 Some non-central subclasses of adjectives
(c) Prefixation. Prefixation is predominantly class-preserving. Most of the The four properties given at the beginning of the last section ­
exclusively class-preserving prefixes are negative or else involve degree. ability to be used predicatively, attributively, postpositively and to take
Among the negative prefixes, un- (unkind) and non- (non-scientific) are ofhigh modification pertaining to degree - characterise the prototypical adjective,
productivity, while a_2 (amoral), dis- (dishonest) and in- (intolerant) are much
but they are not necessary conditions for membership of the adjective class.
more restricted; im- (impossible) and it-I-ir, phonologically /II (illegal,
In this section we will mention briefly some subclasses of adjective that
irrelevant), are variants of -in, occurring with stems beginning with Ipl and lack one or more of the properties.
III or Ir I. Among the degree prefixes, super- (superhuman), hyper- (hypersen­
sitive), ultra- (ultraconservative), over- (overconfident) grade upwards, whereas (a) In the first place, we must recognise a quite large subclass of non­
the only one of any productivity grading downwards is sub- (subhuman). A gradable adjectives like anthropological, philatelic, phonetic, set-theoretic ("per­
few prefixes involving temporal or spatial relations, pre-, post-, inter-, trans-, taining to set theory"), which do not take modifiers of degree. Thus we
attach to adjective stems (especially denominal ones) or else to nouns, so migh t have a philatelic geml magazineI rarity but not '" a very philatelic geml
that they can be either class-preserving (pre-classical, post-classical, intercon­ magazine/rarity. It should be emphasised, however, that gradability is pri­
tinental, transcontinental) or class-changing (pre-war, post-war, inter-university, marily a semantic matter: the possibility of adding a degree modifier
trans-Mississippi) . depends on the meaning of the adjective, not on some semantically-arbi­
(d) Conversion. Given that we are not postulating conversion in cases like trary syntactic property. From a semantic point of view, a gradable adjec­
the Queensland government, where we take the modifier Queensland to be a noun, tive denotes a scalar property as opposed to a categorial one - where a
not an adjective, adjectives derived by conversion are virtually restricted to scalar property is one that can be possessed in varying degrees; and pre­
-ing and -en forms ofverbs: amusing, interesting, bored, distressed, and so on. We cisely because the property denoted can be possessed in varying degrees the
will take up this matter in Ch. 9. adjective can take degree modifiers. Many adjectives, however, are poly­
semous, denoting a categorial property in one sense and a scalar one in
(e) Compounding. The most productive kinds of adjective compound have another. For example, a nationality adjective like British denotes a
an -ing or -en form of a verb as the second component: [a] record-breaking categorial property in its central sense, as in a British passport, the British
[swim], [a] much-debated [question]. The first component may be a noun Parliament, but also has an extended sense denoting a scalar property ("like
(hair-raising, hand-made), including the self that appears in reflexive pro­ typical or stereotypical British people or things"), as in He's very British; the
nouns (self-fUlfilling [prophecies], self-addressed [envelopes]); an adjective primacy of the categorial sense is reflected in the fact that the adjective will
(good-looking, true-born); or an advcrb (well-meaning, well-meant). With -ing not normally be interpreted in the scalar sense unless there is some grading
form compounds, the noun may have a semantic role corresponding to an modifier present. To a significant extent, therefore, the gradable/non­
2 The prefix a- also forms adverbs or prepositions like aboard in He went aboard (the ship); d. gradable contrast applies to uses of adjectives, rather than simply to the
Ch.lo. adjectives themselves.

302 30 3
Adjectives and adjective phrases 8.4 Dependents in AdjP structure: complements
(b) As we have noted, a few adjectives cannot be used attributely: afraid, occurring as pre-head dependent in NP structure - though even here we
asleep, awake, loath, tantamount .. ., and for many speakers well "in good have made a functional distinction between determinatives (determiners)
health", unwell, content, and a few others. Compare They were aftaid (predi­ and adjectives (modifiers). Nevertheless we have just noted that main,
cative), a'!Y0ne afraid ofheights (postpositive), *some aftaid sheep (attributive). principal, mere, etc., are restricted to pre-head dependent position - and
We will see below (§4) that AdjPs containing complements do not some of the determinatives have additional adjective-like properties: one
normally occur attributively: the inclusion of loath and tantamount in the or two are gradable (cf. very much money), and many at least can, under rather
present subclass is then related to the fact that with them a complement is restricted conditions, be used predicatively, as in How many were you? As
normally obligatory. In one or two cases the inability to appear in predica­ noted in 3.4, the classification of closed class items is typically much more
tive position applies to some but not all senses of an adjective: thus glad and problematic than that of open class items, and the informal framework
sorry are not used attributively in the sense "pleased" and "regretful", but developed in this book is certainly not sophisticated enough to allow us to
they can be in other senses, as in glad tidings, a sorry sight. choose in a well-motivated way between an analysis where determinatives
are a subclass of adjective, and one where they constitute a separate class.
(c) Conversely, there are a few adjectives (all non-grad able) that are used
ONLY attributively: main, principal, mere, utter, etc.
8.4 Dependents in AdjP structure: complements
(d) Finally, a small number of adjectives (again non-gradable) are used The range of dependents found in AdjPs is somewhat less
neither attributively nor predicatively: designate, elect, etc., as in the bishop varied and complex than in NPs or EVPs. We can again distinguish,
designate; these are very peripheral members of the adjective class- but they however, between complements and modifiers along the same lines as with
are clearly not candidates for inclusion in any of the other primary word­ dependents of nouns and verbs. Let us begin with a brief consideration of
classes. complements.
A clear example is found in example (gii) in 5.2, Ed is fond ofKim, where
8.3 Determinatives the PP of Kim is complement of the adjective fond. As we suggested in the
The term 'determiner' is commonly used both as a functional earlier discussion,Jond expresses a two-place semantic predicate (like the
label (like 'modifier', 'subject', etc.) and as a class label (like 'adjective', verb love): one of the arguments is expressed by the subject Ed, the other
'NP', etc.). It is evident, however, that we do not have here a one-to-one by the NP Kim within the PP complement. Apart from the semantic
relation between function and class, and I am accordingly restricting the property of including the expression of an argument, of Kim has the two
term to one of the above senses, the functional. The determiner function syntactic properties that we have seen to be characteristic of complements:
has been discussed in our analysis ofNP structure, but at that point we paid
(a) It depends for its occurrence on the presence in head function of an
little attention to questions of class. The easiest case to deal with is illus­
adjective of the appropriate subclass: we could replaceJond by aftaid, say,
trated in, say, the bishop's proposal, where the determiner is realised by a
but not by keen, sorry, tall, etc. Where the complement is a PP, the choice
PossP, the bishop's. Some of the other forms given in list (7) in 6.4 rather
of preposition is determined by the adjective: Jond oj, keen on, sorry for,
clearly have the internal structure ofNPs: one-third, three-quarters, three times,
similar to, etc.
afew (cf. a goodfew) , a little, a dozen, etc. This then leaves us with the closed
class items the, all, a, some, every, ma'!Y - the items to which 'determiner' as a (b) It is obligatory: we cannot have *Ed is fond. However, very few adjec­
class label is commonly applied, and which I am instead referring to as tives take obligatory complements - compare afraid (ofthe dark), keen (on the
'determinatives' (see 3.1). Traditional grammar has the term 'article', but idea), sorry (for the inconvenience), etc., where the parenthesised PP comple­
it is generally restricted to the (the 'definite article') and a (the 'indefinite ments are not syntactically obligatory.
article'): I have preferred to use the relatively unfamiliar term 'determina­ Complements of adjectives are generally realised either by PPs, as in the
tive' rather than stretch 'article' so far beyond its traditional application. above examples, or by subordinate clauses, as in
The determinatives are traditionally analysed as a subclass of adjec­
tives ('limiting adjectives' as opposed to 'descriptive acljectives' like big, (2) Ed was angry that he had gone
good, beautiful), whereas modern grammars more often treat them as a (3) I am unsure whether she can do it
distinct primary class. Clearly they have little in common with proto­ where angry has a declarative content clause as its complement, unsure an
typical adjectives. In general their only adjective-like property is that of interrogative. Examples like (2) and (3) are to be distinguished from It was
30 4 30 5
Adjectives and adjective phrases 8.4 Dependents in AdjP structure: complements
odd that he had gone and It is questionable whether she can do it: these are non­ were keen to see the manuscript is not equivalent to Both scholars were keen jOr both
kernel constructions derived by extraposition from That he had gone was odd scholars to see the manuscript). Conversely, likely expresses a one-place semantic
and Whether she can do it is questionable, so that the adjective and content predicate: "That Ed would see the manuscript was likely"; this is why The
clause do not here go together to form a phrase. manuscript was likely to be seen by Ed has the same propositional meaning as (6)
In addition to finite clauses like those in (2) and (3), we also find non­ (while the pragmatically anomalous The manuscript was keen to be seen by Ed is not
finite clauses functioning as complement to an adjective - more specifically propositionally equivalent to (5)), why we can say There were likely to be too many
infinitival clauses with to: 3 people with vested interests on the committee (but not *There were keen to be .. .), and so
(4) Ed was keen for me to see the manuscript on. As with thc catcnative examples, I leave open the question ofwhether, and
if so how, this initially semantic distinction should be reflected in our syntac­
Some details of the structure of a form like for me to see the manuscript are tic analysis. Eager, reluctant, glad, sorry, etc. belong with keen, while certain,
problematic: I am assuming thatjOr is a 'subordinating conjunction' (see sure and one or two others belong with likely. With the likely class, the
10·4) and that me is subject - it differs from a prototypical subject, of course, infinitival clause cannot contain an overt subject (* Ed was likely jOr me to see the
in that the case-variable pronoun takes thc accusative form in infinitival manuscript), whereas with the keen class it gencrally can, as in (4).
clauses. More often, the infinitival complement has no subject expressed,
asm (c) It may well be that (7) and (8) differ from each other in the same way.
Ready certainly expresses a two-place semantic predicatc (d. Ed was ready jOr
(5) Ed was keen to see the manuscript
the confirence) , and indeed it can also enter into the construction of (5), as in Ed
(6) Ed was likely to see the manuscript
(7) The dye was ready to use
was ready to go: ambiguities can then arise between the two constructions, as in
(8) The dye was easy to use The lamb was ready to eat, where "the lamb" can be taken as either the under­
stood subject-argument of"eat" ("The lamb was ready to eat its food or what­
There are several different kinds of infinitival complement; the four illus­ ever") or its understood object-argument ("The lamb was ready for them/us/
trated here may be distinguished along the following lines: ... to eat it"). The semantic analysis of (8) is more problematic. The case for
treating easy as expressing a one-place semantic predicate rests primarily on
(a) In (5) and (6) the understood subject (or subject-argument) of the
infinitival clause is recovered from the su bject of the superordinate clause­ the relation between (8) and To use the dye was easy (or, equivalently but more
i.e. Ed. Thus the semantic relation between Ed and see the manuscript is the naturally, It was easy to use the dye). On the other hand, the easy of (8) can be
used attributively, as in an easy dye to use, which suggests an interpretation of (8)
same as in the tensed clause Ed saw the manuscript, in which Ed is the actual
as predicating a property "easy to use" of "the dye", i.e. one where "the dye"
subject of see the manuscript. In (7) and (8), by contrast, the infinitival
is an argument of "easy". We will therefore leave open the nature of the sem­
clauses lack not only a subject but also an object- and it is the understood
object (or object-argument) that is recovered from the subject of the super­ antic distinction between (7) and (8); syntactically they differ precisely in that
ordinate clause (cf. We used the dye). To be subsumed under the same con­ it is only the AdjP in (8) that has an attributive use - cf. *a ready dye to use.
struction are those where it is the complement of a preposition that is We have been looking at complements with the form of PPs or clauses:
missing from the infinitival clause, as in The knife was ready/ ea~ to cut with. NPs, by contrast, cannot normally function as complement of an adjective.
The understood subject-argument in (7) and (8), on the other hand, is In this res pect, adjectives resem ble nouns: verbs take a greater range ofcom­
recovered pragmatically ~ "The dye was ready for us/ me/him/her/ one ... plements, with NPs being among the most central. There are just two or
to use", depending on the contex.t. three adjectives that do take NP complements, such as like and worth, as in He
is very like hisfatherand The land was worth ajOrtune, but they lie on the periphery
(b) (5) and (6) differ from each other in just the same way as the catenative
of the adjective class and are not sharply distinguishable from prepositions
constructions Ed expects to amuse Kim and Ed seems to amuse Kim discussed at
(seeCh.IO).
some length in 5.6.2. Thus keen expresses a two-place semantic predicate:
Most AdjPs contain no complement at all or just one, but a small number
"x was keen for x to see the manuscript, for x = Ed" (note that Both scholars
of adjectives allow two: critical of Max jOr his indecisiveness, responsible to the
3 Normally, -ing class clauses require a preposition, rather than entering directly into con­ directors jOr implementing the proposal. 4
struction with the adjective: He's not very keen on marking assignments. An exception is worth, as 4 In the first example,for has to be replaced by ofirthe other complement is omitted: critical of
in The idea is worth pursuingJUrther.
his indecisiveness.
306 30 7
Adjtctivts and adjtctivt phram
8.5 Dtpmdmts in AdjP structurt: modifim
In general, AdjPs containing complements cannot be used attributively to go too, and similarly in (10) the than clause could not remain if more were
- even in the discontinuous construction mentioned in 6.8. Thus we can dropped. There is thus something to be said for analysing the subordinate
have, for example, She is keen on hockey (predicative) or a woman keen on hockey clauses here as dependents of so and more (rather than of the adjectives),
(postpositive) but neither *a keen on hockey woman nor *a keen woman on hockey; with the AdjPs each containing a single discontinuous modifier. I have
similarly with finite clause complements: He was angry that he had gone but preferred to take the clauses as separate modifiers of the adjective for two
not *an angry that he had gone man nor *an angry man that he had gone. The reasons. Firstly, the analytic comparative in (10) may be matched with the
exception involves infinitival complements of the kind found in (8) as inflectional comparative of It was longer than I had expected. If we replace
noted above: an easy car to drive, a diJIicult person to get on with. longer by long we must again drop the than clause, yet the latter does not form
a syntactic constituent with the comparative inflection: within the
8,5 Dependents in AdjP structure: modifiers
framework we have adopted, the les of longer than I had expected could not be
Modifiers normally occur only with grad able adjectives and are
long + -er than I had expected, because -er is part of the word longer. Secondly,
concerned primarily with the expression ofdegree. In pre-head position we
it would be very difficult to distinguish the discontinuous construction
find a large number of de-adjectival adverbs in -ly (absolutely, completely,
from that where there is a post-head dependent of the adjective. In (12), for
enormously, incredibly, profoundly, utterly, etc.) and a small number of other
example, the infinitival clause seems to be closely linked to the too: the
adverbs whose stems are for the most part morphologically simple (how,
meaning is that the degree of sleepiness was excessive relative to the goal of
quite, so, too, very and the non-simple rather, somewhat): absolutely useless, quite
concentrating, and if we drop the too while retaining the infinitival clause
good, rather thin. Most of these can take their own modifiers (as profoundly
the result sounds quite unnatural. Yet if we change the lexical content to,
wrong, ever so pretty,)ust how intelligent), so that we should have the pre-head
say, He was too old to be doing that kind ofwork, it is certainly possible to drop
modifier function realised by phrases rather than, immediately, by words.
just the too. Similarly in ([ [) we could not drop the first as, but in examples
Among the morphologically simple items, special mention should be
like She was as slim as a reed we could.
made of those which belong to both adverb and determinative classes: the,
Examples (12)-(14) illustrate three distinct kinds of infinitival clause
this, that, much, no, any, all. These are adverbs by virtue of their potential to
modifier. In (12) the understood subject-argument of the infinitival clause
modify adjectives and other adverbs, determinatives by virtue of their
is recovered from the subject of the main clause (it is a matter of Ed's
potential to function as determiner in NP structure. Thus the adverbial use is
concentrating), whereas in (13) and ([ 4) it is recovered pragmatically ­
illustrated in He was much the wiserfor it, It wasn't all that satisfactory, It was no
and these latter may contain an overt subject afterfor: ratheryoungfor them to
difftrent }Tom last time, Was it a~ good?, and so on, while the determinative
send on such a mission, and so on. The difference between (13) and (14) is that
use is found in He hadn't much patience, He lost all that money, It was no mean.feat,
the infinitival clause in (13) is structurally incomplete in that it lacks an
Do you want a~ milk? For the most part nouns and adjectives differ with
object (or the complement ofa preposition, as in The wall was too high to climb
respect to the pre-head dependents they take, but these examples show that
over), with the understood argument being recoverable from the subject of
there is some overlap - so that the presence of the, this, etc. cannot be taken
the main clause (in (13) it is a matter of sending Ed on the mission).
as sufficient to indicate a nominal construction. Except for the demon­
Infinitival clauses, as we saw in the last section, also function as comple­
stratives, however, these adverbs can modify only a quite restricted range
ment in AdjP structure: let us therefore pause briefly to clarify the distinc­
of adjectives/adverbs - the, for example, occurs only with comparatives
tion between the two constructions as exemplified in, say,
and with same: I'm all the more gratefUl toyou, My.feelings are the same as before.
Post-head modifiers are exemplified in ( [5) He was anxious to be a minister Complement
(9) Ed was so tall that he could see over the wall II He was young to be a minister ModifierS
(10) It was more usefUL than I had expected
( 1I) It was as long as six feet (a) Semantically, the complement corresponds to an argument of the
( I 2) Ed was too sleepy to concentrate adjectival semantic predicate: (i) may be compared to He wanted to be a
( [3) Ed was ratheryoung to send on such a mission
([ 4) It was warm to be wearing an overcoat 5 As with verb and noun heads, we can probably not make a completely sharp distinction
between complements and modifiers; it is possible, for example, that the constructions of (7)
In some cases the post-head modifier is closely linked to a pre-head
and (8) above should also be differentiated as involving a complement and modifier
modifier: in (9), for example, ifwe dropped the so the that clause would have respectively.
308
30 9
805 Dependents in AdjP structure: modifiers
Adjectives and adjective phrases
natural to analyse obviously as a modifier of discreet. With be, the analysis is
minister. The modifier, on the other hand, is concerned with the specific­
more difficult. One might argue that we can again distinguish between an
ation of degree. Gradable adjectives generally involve some kind of
interpretation where obviously relates to the being indiscreet ("It is obvious
comparison - explicit, as in Ed is smaller than Max, or implicit, as in Ed is
that he was indiscreet") and one where it relates just to the indiscretion
small. That there is implicit comparison in the latter becomes evident when
"He was blatantly indiscreet"). He obviously was indiscreet allows only the
we imagine what sizes the clause would attribute to Ed if he were a six­
former interpretation, but (17) allows either - and it is then tempting to say
month-old baby, a ten-year old boy, an adult human or, say, a circus
that (17) is structurally ambiguous. Notice, however, that obviously can
elephant: the standard with which he is being compared and judged small
enter into construction with an adjective used attributively, as in his
would be quite different. The semantic effect of the modifier in (ii) is thus to
obviously indiscreet ex-secretary- and this allows either of the above interpreta­
specify the standard - "young in comparison with the standard age for
tions. It seems that attributive AdjPs permit a wider range of modifiers,
ministers" .
with some of them corresponding to adjuncts in clause structure in the
(b) The complement, as noted earlier, depends for its occurrence on the matching predicative construction - compare her at that time still industrious
selection of an adjective head allowing an infinitival complement: we could husband with Her husband was at that time still industrious.
replace anxious in (i) by afraid, eager, reluctant, etc., but not by considerate,
responsible, similar and the like. The modifier,. however, can occur with all FURTHER READING
gradable adjectives, though it will often sound more natural if there is also On adjectives in general, see Quirk et al. 197 1: Ch. 5, Bowers 1975· On the
a pre-head modifier, as in our sleepy exampIe, (12). adjective + infinitive construction, see Bolinger 1961, Silva & Thompson 1977,
Nanni 1980.
(c) The two can combine, with the complement preceding the modifier: He
was too anxious to win to appreciate such niceties (some pre-head modifier is just
about obligatory in such cases).
(d) There is, potentially, a prosodic distinction between (i) and (ii): a
natural pronunciation of the latter would have two intonation contours,
the first having the stress on young, the second on min( ister) , but (i) would
not be spoken in this way.
It is not always easy to distinguish clearly between modifiers in Adj P
structure and adjuncts in clause structure. Consider the examples
(16) He was too indiscreet
(17) He was obviously indiscreet

The first is straightforward: too, in the sense "excessively", cannot function


as an adjunct - we could not replace excessively by too in this sense in She had
loved him excessively and the like (the corresponding degree adj unct is too
much); in (16) the too is thus clearly a modifier of indiscreet. Obviously, on the
other hand, can be an adjunct - what we have called a modal adjunct, as in
She obviously loved him - and it clearly has this function in Obviously he was
indiscreet or He obviously was indiscreet. If we take a copulative verb other than
be, such as become or seem, we find a clear semantic contrast between He
obviously became indiscreet ("It is obvious that he became indiscreet") and He
became obviously indiscreet ("He came to exhibit obvious indiscretion"): in the
first it is the becoming indiscreet that was obvious, and here obviously is an
adjunct, in the second there is a change from one state to another, with
1 obviously characterising the indiscretion of the second state - and here it is
3I I
1

310
1

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi