Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 3

Acta Psychiatr Scand 2018: 137: 85–87 © 2017 John Wiley & Sons A/S.

n Wiley & Sons A/S. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
All rights reserved ACTA PSYCHIATRICA SCANDINAVICA
DOI: 10.1111/acps.12837

Editorial
Thinking fast and slow – in clinical
psychiatry
He walked into my rooms slowly, barely swinging else? Now that he thought about it, there was also
his arms and showing no light in his eyes in some left-sided facial numbness.
response to my welcome. Parkinson’s Disease? The prescription pad was retracted and instead a
Melancholia? neurology referral form completed. An MRI that
He provided a succinct reason for the appoint- afternoon showed a subdural haemorrhage which
ment. ‘I developed depression 3 months ago. Just was successfully drained that evening. At review a
after my 63rd birthday. Never had it before. My fortnight later, he appeared completely normal
mother developed depression at exactly the same and no symptoms of ‘melancholia’ were present.
age. She needed ECT to get better’. We both judged that it might be best not to rein-
I undertook a review of his depressive symptoms troduce the antidepressant at that time.
before taking a general history covering other pos- This vignette serves as an introduction to clinical
sible diagnostic issues and his personal narrative. reasoning and one of its key components: pattern
He was clearly depressed, but my primary objec- analysis. Pattern analysis? As medical students, we
tive was to ‘subtype’ his depression. His response marvelled at how a senior physician might in a
to a set of questions that I have progressively minute or two – and perhaps only after absent-
weighted over the years (e.g. anhedonic and non- mindedly tapping a kneecap or transiently applying
reactive mood, marked anergia, impaired ‘foggy’ a stethoscope and asking a brief set of disparate
concentration, mood and energy worse in morn- questions – offer a diagnosis that would be con-
ings) argued for a melancholic depression as a ‘no firmed by a definitive laboratory test (or autopsy).
brainer’ diagnosis and as a stretched metaphor. I We students, by contrast, might have spent days
commenced him on an antidepressant and, at interviewing and examining the patient, as well as
reviews, his depression had significantly improved poring over textbooks, and remained bothered and
by 2 weeks and fully remitted at 6 weeks. I recom- bewildered by our failure to obtain diagnostic clo-
mended he remains on the same dose of the medi- sure. What intuitive art – for it surely was not
cation in the light of the gravity of his initial science – did our seniors possess? And how might it
presentation and as he was not experiencing dis- be acquired, as it was not listed in our syllabus and
tinct side-effects. no lecturer ever took us into this arcane world. It
A year later he returned to say that ‘it’ had lies at the heart (and perhaps the art) of psychiatry,
returned in the last fortnight. He again showed dis- reflecting psychiatry’s domains and the absence of
tinct psychomotor retardation and affirmed the any gold standard laboratory tests.
same set of depressive features. Asked about his Malcolm Gladwell (1) proselytized the concept
medication, he stated that he had stopped it that it took 10 000 h of deliberate practice to
6 weeks previously. My ‘fast brain’ encouraged me develop a skill set. In his monograph, ‘Thinking,
to state the obvious – melancholia is highly likely Fast and Slow’, Daniel Kahneman noted (2) how
to return if maintenance medication is ceased, and chess masters are able to move down an assembly
that he should immediately restart it. However, I line of chess boards slaved over for hours by ama-
held such censuring back as my ‘slow brain’ kicked teur chess tragics to make rapid and brilliant chess
in and I broadened my questioning. Were there moves seemingly intuitively. But Kahneman dis-
any new symptoms? Well, he had had a disastrous missed intuition, arguing more for the masters see-
golf round that week – his swing just didn’t work ing each chess board differently, reflecting the
at all. Anything else? He had been embarrassed to 10 000 h of dedicated performance to become
drop a grocery bag at the supermarket that morn- familiar with thousands of configurations and
ing. Anything else? Well, yes. He had noticed some with each involving arrangements of related
numbness in two fingers in his left hand. Anything pieces that could defend or threaten each other. In

85
Editorial

essence, pattern recognition acquired by repeated diagnostic disease groups such as validly diagnos-
experience. ing melancholia or schizophrenia, or say in
Of further relevance to clinical reasoning, Kah- differentiating schizophrenia from bipolar disorder
neman distinguished ‘fast’ (or implicit) from ‘slow’ and from schizoaffective disorder. Conversely, it
(or explicit) thinking. He detailed how fast think- could well be argued that it is potentially of high
ing operates automatically and without voluntary utility in determining whether an individual has a
control, seeks categories or prototypes and looks non-disease condition such as a personality disor-
to construct the best possible narrative or formula- der and that its application is not limited to the
tion. Such thinking is, however, gullible, possesses interpretation of symptoms and signs but from
a bias to believe, neglects ambiguity, and focusses other sources such as the psychiatrist’s interpreta-
on existing and supporting evidence rather than tion of their own counter-transference and other
ignoring absent evidence. By contrast, he judged subjective information.
‘slow’ thinking as intrinsically lazy but that it As the science suggests that clinical pattern anal-
can be called on (as an executive control system) ysis may not be readily ‘teachable’, I doubt that it
after the fast thinking phase to consider alternate will become an undergraduate or postgraduate
explanations. course in psychiatry. Are we then left with the
Is one style (i.e. slow or fast thinking) optimal necessity of requiring the psychiatrist (and those in
for psychiatric diagnosis and formulation? I doubt other clinical fields) to assess thousands of patients
it. The slow brain would be called on in a psychia- over decades to ensure the ten thousand hours
trist’s early training years and when reliance on threshold has been exceeded? Is there not a place
formalised diagnostic criteria may dictate clinical for computer-assisted or machine learning diag-
judgement. Later, after (say) ten thousand hours, nostics even if limited to the diagnosis of psychia-
the fast brain may be dominant for some practi- try’s disease states? While such computer
tioners. The optimal endpoint is more likely to programs have been under development since the
involve an iterative process, with the ‘fast’ brain seventies, none have delivered at an acceptable
automatically generating a formulation or a diag- level. Overviews of the topic still conclude that
nosis and then the ‘slow’ brain effecting its execu- computers cannot replace doctors at the bedside.
tive control system and considering supportive or Computers may just be unable to master the multi-
alternate explanations. I once asked a number of ple domains that are involved in deriving a valid
psychiatrists as to when they had achieved a crest diagnosis. For example, diarrhoea is not an imme-
in clinical reasoning. Most nominated a peak in diate clinical signal for a diagnosis of sarcoid of
their sixties, paradoxically a time when most prac- the hypothalamus, and only a skilled clinician
titioners are planning retirement. And so clinical might wend their way from such a symptom to that
pattern analysis is not innate, requiring the thou- endpoint diagnosis. This suggests that computers
sands of hours described by Gladwell. This does could be designed more to complement clinical rea-
not apply solely to psychiatrists, but is a mature soning than seeking to develop them as definitive
skill required within many professions: other clini- diagnostic tools in and of themselves. Again, how-
cal specialties, chess masters, fire wardens and even ever, the broad domains of psychiatric diagnoses
chicken sexers. would suggest that such application might be lim-
Where do I position psychiatric clinical reason- ited to a small set of diagnostic categories.
ing in a decade? As noted earlier, the psychiatric Can we learn from chess? In 1997, IBM’s com-
field lacks laboratory tests for diagnostic clarifica- puter, Deep Blue, beat Garry Kasparov at chess
tion and this argues for a need to weight clinical and since then computers have consistently beaten
reasoning and refined pattern analysis. However, chess masters. However, while the chess masters
most if not all psychiatric diagnoses lack precise or operate to a pattern analytic approach, computers
even clear boundaries, leading to formalised (and operate by judging the best next moves rather than
informal) diagnostic categories being of limited searching for patterns. Each bring differing ana-
validity, while comorbidity is common. Further- lytic approaches to the table. Currently, the evi-
more, management is not generally simply shaped dence suggests that, to win a chess match, a
by ‘the diagnosis’, but optimally by a formulation computer plus a skilled player will perform better
which asks why ‘this patient has presented at this than either one alone.
time with this condition’, and generates a compos- Thus, rather than seeking to develop a computer
ite of diagnosis, personal narrative, antecedent program that will have diagnostic superiority to an
distal and proximal determinants, as well as ace clinical psychiatrist, it may be more important
personality style. Psychiatric pattern analysis may to develop programs that complement the psychia-
then be relevant across limited and formal trist’s judgement. For example, the psychiatrist

86
Editorial

might diagnose ‘depression’ and then turn to the same antidepressant was reintroduced and his
computer for subtyping the specific depressive con- depression remitted after 5 weeks. He has now pre-
dition. Thus, the computer would assist by deter- sented the same basic phenomenological pattern
mining whether the depressed patient was most on three occasions but with two differing causes
likely to have a unipolar depression (and, if so, a and with each requiring contrasting management
biological melancholic or an environmental non- strategies. To expect a computer to diagnose
melancholic disorder) or a bipolar disorder based melancholia and then exclude the myriad factors
on the presence (and absence) of multiple symp- that would have established a diagnosis of
toms and their weightings. Similarly, the computer ‘pseudo-melancholia’ (and its specific nature)
might be programmed to establish the presence or strikes me as unlikely to be successful; indeed, the
absence of disordered personality function and, if input data required would be enormous and also
affirmed, to generate the particular personality dis- dependent on the validity of its reporting.
order ‘type’ or ‘types’. If such programs were to be Thus, clinical reasoning and pattern analysis lie
developed, then the decision tree computer algo- at the heart of psychiatry. And the injunction
rithms might benefit from incorporating the multi- ‘Physician know thyself’ challenges us individually
ple signals (and the signal weightings) acquired by and collectively to consider how best to reap the
the clinicians who have laboured thoughtfully at riches of the ‘fast’ and our ‘slow’ pieces of our
the relevant coal face for decades. And then, in an brains.
iterative manner, the computer could progressively
refine the signal weightings for patients with – as
Declaration of interest
against those without a particular diagnosis – and
with such Bayesian calibrations then being applied Nil disclosures reported.
to sharpen the diagnostic criteria employed by clin-
icians, and ideally contributing to improving for- G. Parker1,2
1
School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney,
malised diagnostic criteria. In essence, the Australia and
computer would provide ‘fast brain’ information 2
Black Dog Institute, Sydney, Australia
for reflective consideration by the clinician E-mail: g.parker@unsw.edu.au
engaged in a ‘slow brain’ appraisal and review, and
there would be an iterative process between the
clinician and the computer in advancing the key References
diagnostic signals and their weightings. 1. Gladwell M. Outliers: the story of success. New York: Lit-
A year later, my patient’s ‘melancholia’ returned tle Brown and Company, 2008.
abruptly. A neurology review and repeat MRI 2. Kahneman D. Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar,
failed to identify any neurological cause, the Strauss and Giroux, 2011.

87

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi