Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 33

Society for American Archaeology

Dimensional Analysis of Behavior and Site Structure: Learning from an Eskimo Hunting
Stand
Author(s): Lewis R. Binford
Source: American Antiquity, Vol. 43, No. 3 (Jul., 1978), pp. 330-361
Published by: Society for American Archaeology
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/279390
Accessed: 09-05-2017 17:29 UTC

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted
digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about
JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms

Society for American Archaeology is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
American Antiquity

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
DIMENSIONAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE:
LEARNING FROM AN ESKIMO HUNTING STAND

Lewis R. Binford

Detailed behavioral observations permitted the dimensional analysis of formation processes operati
the Mask site, a Nunamiut Eskimo hunting stand. Activity structure, technological organization, di
mode, and spatial organization were all seen as behavioral dimensions that could each vary, altering the
terns of assemblage content and spatial disposition at an archaeological site.
These ethnoarchaeological experiences were then contrasted with those recently reported by John Ye
(1977), and a critical evaluation of his "conclusions" was conducted from the perspective of the Eskim
perience. It was pointed out that basic differences in philosophy and approach to research largely condi
the contrasting character of the conclusions drawn from the different experiences.

THE PURPOSE OF THIS PAPER is to describe the relationship between characteristic


behaviors observed on hunting stands and the structured consequences of these behaviors in the
archaeological record.
This article is within the domain of "ethnoarchaeology" in that it describes observations be-
lieved to be of interest to archaeologists but experienced in the context of an ongoing living
system. All of the observations to be reported were made between 1969 and 1973 during
ethnographic work among the Nunamiut Eskimo of north central Alaska. Much of the material
resulting from this work has been previously described (Binford 1976, 1978; Binford and Chasko
1976; Binford and Bertram 1977). This article reprepsents the first of a series that will specifically
treat the formation processes and resulting character of the internal site structure for a number
of different types of Eskimo site.
Hunting stands are a type of site commonly produced by the Nunamiut. They are locations
where men congregate to watch for game and to plan hunting strategies after game is sighted.
They are an integral part of an "intercept" hunting strategy as opposed to an "encounter"
strategy (see Binford 1978). In intercept hunting one employs knowledge of the factors that condi-
tion animal behavior to "predict" where animals will be, given the conditions of the moment such
as weather, seasons of the year, etc. One positions himself to be able to monitor the surrounding
area where game is anticipated. These "stations" from which an area is monitored are hunting
stands. They are commonly occupied only by male hunters or hunters and young men. They are
rarely occupied overnight, and when they are, there is a continuous monitoring of the area rather
than a change of tempo within the site when everyone goes to bed. Sleeping facilities on such loca-
tions are always expedient and individual.
Hunting stands provide an interesting situation relative to the assumptions commonly made by
archaeologists regarding the relationship between attributes of site location and site content and
between the internal pattern of artifact disposition and activities. Frequently an archaeologist
may observe that there is some consistent association between features of the physical geography
and the presence of archaeological sites in a given area. Upon recognition of such a pattern it is
not uncommon to study comparatively the artifactual contents of the sites as a clue to their "func-
tion" and hence a basis for understanding their geographical patterning. The assumption is made
that what is in a site betrays the activities conducted there, and that anticipation of those ac-
tivities was crucial in selecting the location of the site.
In the case of many Eskimo hunting stands, there is no obvious relationship between what is in
the site and the "reasons for its occupancy" and hence the "reasons for its location." The loca-
tion is chosen because it provides maximum visual coverage for a large area considered a likely
place for game to be moving. The location is chosen to maximize the informational input for the oc-

Lewis R. Binford. Department of Anthropology, University of New Mexico. Albuquerque. NM 87131.

330

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Binford] ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 331

Table 1. Flow of Men.

Date of Total Men


observation Total hours No. initially Arrived during Left during using site
(1972) of observation present observation observation (June 6)
June 4 6 3 4 2 7
June 4 3 5 2 4 7
June 5 3 7 2 3 9
June 5 6 9 4 6 13
June 6 4 5 4 7 9
June 6 1 4 1 1 5

Total 23 50/16

Date of Total man hours of Average stay at site Mean no. men per hour
observation (1972) on-site activity per man (hours) present during observation
June 4 29.0 4.14 5.00
June 4 13.0 1.86 4.33
June 5 19.5 2.17 6.50
June 5 52.3 4.02 8.71
June 6 15.5 1.72 3.88
June 6 4.5 0.90 4.50

cupants. This informa


executed from a serie
quently characterized
traps. These location
presence and path of
ly placed "blinds" or
tion, and rarely are a
ing directly from sta
shots" are more freq
directly from huntin
more commonly locat
The location of the h
observation of game a
tivities that take plac
levels of the occupan
vironment relative to

Table 2. Frequency of A

Activity
Game Off-site Eating + Target Playing
watching hunting talking shooting cards Crafts Sleep
Date of
observation No. % No. % No. % No. % No. % No. % No. %

June 4 5.5(19.0) 5.5(19.0) 9.5(33.0) 0.0 - 3.5(12.0) 5.0(17.0) 0.0 -


June 4 3.0(23.0) 0.0 - 4.5(35.0) 3.0(23.0) 1.0(8.0) 1.5(12.0) 0.0 -
June 5 3.0(15.0) 0.0 - 9.0(46.0) 3.0(15.0) 2.0(10.0) 1.0(5.0) 1.5(8.0)
June 5 12.0(22.96) 5.0(10.0) 21.75(42.0) 2.0(4.0) 9.0(17.0) 1.0(2.0) 1.5(3.0)
June 6 6.5(42.0) 0.0 - 3.0(19.0) 3.0(19.0) 0.0 - 3.0(19.0) 0.0 -
June 6 1.5(33.0) 0.0 - 1.5(33.0) 0.0 - 0.0 - 1.5(33.0) 0.0 -

Total 31.5 10.5 49.25 11.0 15.5 13.0 3.0


Percentage 24.0 8.0 37.0 8.0 12.0 10.0 2.0

Note: No. = number of man hours.

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
332 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 43, No. 3,1978

/0 -

9-

8-

7-

N
6'

Figure 1. Relationship be- %


tween mean group size and time 0 5 -
spent eating, playing cards, and
talking.
; 4 -

/-

0
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80

% OF TIME SPENT
EATING, PLAYING CARDS a
TALKING

clues to their function from the inventory of remaining contents. The best way to illustrat
point is by example.
The Mask site is located near the present village of Anaktuvuk (see Binford 1978). It is com
ly used as a hunting stand after the main caribou migration hunting in spring. It is used to m
stragglers and small post migration herds of bull caribou in the area to the south and west o
contemporary village. The period of use is from around May 28 through June 19. The gener
of this site and its place in the overall spring hunting strategy have been previously descr
(Binford 1978). Our interest is in the facts of its internal organization and how they came in
ing behaviorally.
I was present on this site for a total of 34 hours spread over the years 1971 and 1972. In
the site was visited several times. Hunting orchestrated from the site was observed, as wa
general character of site activities. During a lull in the use of the site by the Nunamiut, t
was mapped and inventoried and then cleaned of all portable items not identified by infor
as items "cached" or destined for future use and/or retrieval by the users of the site. Thi
very near the end of the use period in 1971. The site was cleaned so that all items observed t
the following year could be referred to behavior occurring on the site during the spring of
In 1972 the site was visited 3 times during its peak use, and the hunting of bull herds fro
site was recorded. Late in the season I was essentially stranded in Anaktuvuk village while
waiting for the arrival of my archaeological crew. I used this "dead time" to conduct a rather in-
tensive set of observations on the Mask site (June 4-7, 1972). Within 1 week I spent 23 hours on the
site. In addition, the site was inventoried, and all items were mapped according to their exact
location. Prior to the start of "systematic" observation I had observed behavior on the site for ap-

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Binford] ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 333

10

&5 6-

Z5 Figure 2. Relationship be-


Q 5' tween mean group size and time
spent in crafts and target
?^~* ~ shooting.

I4J

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40
% OF TIME SPENT IN
CRAFTS a TARGET SHOOTING

proximately 11 hours. I attempted to make "behavioral observations" (see Whiting and W


1970; Weick 1968) during the observation periods June 4-7, 1972, but my observations w
ased in that no attempt was made to record all action occurring on the hunting stand. I t
record the times of arrival and departure of persons; I recorded the times at which vario
dividuals initiated activities and when they stopped. For instance, a typical record may n
Johnny Rulland seated himself next to Bob Ahgook around hearth A at 8:50 and began eng
conversation and the eating of bone marrow. The record may then pick up some time lat
noting that Johnny got up from the hearth area and announced that he had "had enough"
returning to the village. The description would then note the items he collected and carried
with him. Table 1 summarizes the census data on the persons present on the site and the
hours of activity that were recorded.

ACTIVITIES CONDUCTED

My major interest was in actions that resulted in the discard or placement of ite
entered the archaeological domain. I did, however, as noted above, keep an activity
23 hours of observation. Table 2 summarizes these data regarding the numbers of
the occupants of the site were engaged in the several recognized basic activities.
activities into which the overall actions of the man were tabulated. Game-watchin
ly of scanning the area to the south of the site either with the unaided eye or wit
man was engaged in carving a wooden mold for a mask and occasionally looked o
even picked up binoculars and scanned the area, he was still recorded as enga

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
334 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 43, No. 3,1978

Table 3. Data From the 23 Hours of Behavioral Observation as Regards the


Items Observed and the "Activity" in Which They Were Used.

A. No. of B. No. of man-


Activity and items used observations episodes of use A/B
Eating and talking (18)
Kaotah 2 4 .50
Meat bones 11 4 2.75
Marrow bones 8 6 1.33
Can of hash 1 1 1.00
Can of sardines 5 5 1.00
Can of pop 2 2 1.00
Sardine can key 5 5 1.00
Pop can tab 2 2 1.00
"Coffee" cups 6 19 .32
Coffee pot 1 9 .11
Coffee can 2 6 .33
Spoons 2 21 .10
Can of milk 1 9 .11
Stone anvils 5 11 .45
Bottle of instant coffee 1 7 .14
Bag of coffee 4 11 .36
Bag of sugar 12 27 .44

Watching for game (2)


Binoculars 2 18 .11
Skins 1 18 .06

Playing cards (5)


Deck of cards 3 22 .14
Can of pop 5 5 1.00
Pop can tab 5 5 1.00
Skins 2 22 .09
Coffee cups 4 9 .44

Crafts (11)
Abrader 1 1 1.00
Kitchen knife 1 1 1.00
Metal file 1 4 .25
Hone 1 2 .50
Skinning knife 4 4 1.00
Mask mold 1 1 1.00
Scraper (skin) 1 1 1.00
Scissors 1 1 1.00
Screwdriver 1 1 1.00
Dressed skin 1 (sq.) 1 1.00
Skin 1 3 .33

Target shooting
.22 shells (50)' 3 16.67
Pop can (target) 4 13 .31
Milk can (target) 1 6 .17
Skin 1 2 .50

Off-site huntin
Dog packs 3 3 1.00
Rifle 17 17 1.00
Skinning knife 18 18 1.00
Bullets 25 5 5.00
Short ropes 2 1 2.00
Sleds 5 5 1.00
Snowmobiles 7 7 1.00
Binoculars 2 2 1.00

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Binford] ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 335

Table 3 (continued).
A. No. of B. No. of man-
Activity and items used observations episodes of use A/B
Sleeping (1)
Skins 1 2

Other or nonspec
Funnel Not observed
Pair of gloves Not observed
Soap dish Not observed
Wolf trap Not observed
Sheets of canvas Not observed
Cigarette lighter Not recorded

1parentheses indicate an estimate rather than an actual count.

manufacturing tasks; a man was tabulated as engaged in an activity even though


interrupted his action for other purposes.
The only category of action that might be misleading in Table 2 is hunting. Hunt
actually conducted on the site; during hunting episodes, all the men (including myse
site. This "hunting" category, then, indicates hours during which the site was ab
hunting activities were carried out.
Two other observations regarding the activity categories might be useful. All t
was done with guns that were especially introduced for this purpose. That is, the w
hunting were never used in shooting targets at the Mask site. It was explained th
and shells were too expensive to be wasted on targets. A rimfire .22 was almost a
target shooting. The craft activities observed on the Mask site were (1) carving o
for masks, (2) carving of wooden spreaders for a dog harness, (3) carving of a ho
sold to a collector from Fairbanks, (4) carving of an ivory needle valve for repairin
on a snowmobile, (5) sewing a small skin pouch for carrying rifle bullets, and (6) re
skin socks.
The information in Table 2 should demonstrate that only the activity of "watching for game"
was directly related to the primary function of the site. This represented 24% of the total man-
hours of activity recorded; yet there were no recognizable archaeological consequences of this
behavior. No tools left on the site were used, and there were no immediate material "byproducts"
of the "primary" activity. All of the other activities conducted at the site were essentially
boredom reducers.
Figure 1 illustrates the relationship between the percentage of the total time spent in the co
bined activities of eating, playing cards, and talking and the mean group size present on the
during observation. What is clear is that the relative proportions of these activities are a funct
of the size of the group present on the site. The larger the social unit present, the greater the
portion of the total man-hours spent in "socializing" activities. Figure 2 illustrates the relati
ship between the size of the social unit present and the percentage of the total occupation ti
spent in target shooting and craft activities. The relationship is negatively linear; the fewer m
present, the greater the proportion of time spent in target shooting and craft activities. Th
recognition of these relationships is provocative as regards the problem of interassembl
variability. Insofar as there are relationships between activities and material items, we could
pect that hunting stands which tended to vary in the modal group sizes would also vary in term
the relative frequencies of activity-related archaeological debris.
I have described elsewhere (Binford 1978) a number of different hunting stands, and one of
major differences between them was the modal sizes of the occupying group. Stands used du
peak migration hunting are large, with many men frequenting the stand. After main migration
degree to which individual men continue to hunt is largely a function of the successes durin
migration hunting. Generally, the less skilled hunters are the ones hunting after migra
Animals tend to be dispersed, hence hunting stands tend to be dispersed and are generally oc

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
336 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 43, No. 3,1978

Table 4. Comparison between Numbers of Items Observed Behaviorally and Archaeologically.

Items with values Items with values


greater than 1 of 1
Item BO* AO** Item BO AO
Marrow bones 8 334
Meat bones 11 51 Hash can 1 1
Sardine can 5 7
Shells (ammunition) 25 68 Pop can 7 12
Can key 5 6
Can tab 2 12

Abrader 1 1
Kitchen knife 1 (1)
Skinning knife 18 0
Mask mold 1 0
Skin scraper 1 0
Scissors 1 0
Screw driver 1 0
Dressed skin 1 0

Dog packs 3 0
Rifles 17 0
Skinning knives 18 0
Sled 5 0
Snowmobiles 7 0

Items with values Items with values


.99-.25 less than .25
Item BO AO Item BO AO
Coffee cups 6 (6) Coffee pot 1 (1)
Coffee can 2 1 Spoons 2 (2)
Bag of coffee 4 (2) (an of milk 1 1
Bag of sugar 12 (1) Instant coffee 1 (1)

Pop can target 3 4 Milk can target 1 1

Metal File 1 (1) Binoculars 2 0


Htone 1 (1) Sitting skin 5 (1)
Deck of cards 3 (1)
Stone anvils 5 5
Kaotah 2 2

Note: Values in parenthes


ping but informants wer
again and eventually retur
* Behaviorally observed.
** Archaeologically obser

cupied by 2 men at mo
from those of migrati
shooting is a behavior
task-related difference would correlate with "microseasonal" differences.
Individual hunting stands are occupied more commonly after migration, and they would be
geographically distributed in a different manner (see Binford 1978). Hence there would be a cor-
relation between some facts of content and season of occupancy, as well as geographical location.
Nevertheless, the relevant explanatory variable would be group size. How could one discover that
condition by studying the empirical correlations, spatial patterns, or "typological status" of the

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Binford] ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 337

Table 5. Comparative Inventories for Items Observed Ethnographically and Archaeologically-M

Archaeo
observations
Behaviorally observed items Disposal Modes Inventory of site
1st week
Item June 7, of June
Observed Number Dropped Tossed Rested Placed Dumped 1971 1972

Bullets (75)a (25)a 0 (50)a 0 0 123 61


(shells)
Skinning 18knife
0 0 4 1 0
O
0 0
Rifle 17 0 0 31 0 0 0 0
Bag of sugar
2 0 0 9 6 0 3 1
Meat bones 11 (Io
Ribs 10 (35) 9 4 0 (12)b
(l) 150 (49)b
Scapula 1 0 1 0 0 0 2 2
Marrow bones 8
Splinters (60)a 90% 10% 0 0 0 85
Chips (120)a 100% 0% 0 0 0
284 28
223
Ends 16 1 15 2 0 0 26
Pop cans 7 0 7 3 4 0 8 12
Snowmobiles 7 0 0 0 7 0 0 0
Cigarette lighters 7 0 0 3 0 0 0 0
Cups 6 0 0 4 9 0 7 6
Sleds 5 0 0 0 5 0 0 0
Skins 5 0 0 0 5 0 0 1
Deck of cards 3 0 0 8 1 0 1 1
Sardine cans 5 0 5 2 0 0 8 7
Dog packs 3 0 0 3 0 0 0 0
Metal files 3 0 0 6 1 0 1 1
Anvils 5 0 0 0 5 0 3 5
Coffee cans 2 0 1 4 1 0 1 1
Binoculars 2 0 0 6 0 0 0 0
Coffee pot 1 0 0 7 1 (2)c 1 1
Mask molds 1 0 0 1 0 0 (2' 0 0
Length of rope 1 0 0 1 0 0 2 0
Pop can tabs 204 25 30 42 9 12
Can of hash 0 1
Funnel 0 1
Pair of gloves 1 1
Hone 1 1
Kitchen knife 1 0
Spoon 2
File 1
Milk can 1
Instant coffee bottle 1
Bag of coffee
Soap dish
Wolf trap
Wool glove
Canvas 3
Rope fragments 2

Note: Numbers in italic indicate items that would have been removed when site was abandoned.
a estimate of number.
b fragments.
c grounds only.

assemblages? I do not think the facts of the archaeological record would speak for themselves in
this case. Finally, the typological distinctiveness of assemblages from postmigration hunting
stands would be illusionary since the same variables conditioning large stands would be
operative; the only differences would be the "power" of the different variables as manifest at dif-

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
338 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 43, No. 3,1978

Figure 3. Hearths and glacial boulders.

ferent locations. This is what is meant by "functional" variability, but more importantly this is
what is anticipated by arguments for the multivariable basis of many interassemblage dif-
ferences. Our analytical tools must be sufficient for recognizing such conditions when they exist
and recognizing the effects of such past dynamics or formation processes if we are to achieve ac-
curacy in giving meaning to the archaeological facts we observe. These points will be
demonstrated more clearly with the next body of data.

TECHNOLOGICAL ORGANIZATION

We have now explored some of the factors that condition the performance of dif
tivities on the Mask site and how these activities relate to the "primary function" of
next concern is the relationship between the structure of the activities and the organi
"technology" or material items manipulated in the various activities. Table 3 sum
data from the 23 hours of observation at the Mask site regarding the material items
their relationship to the activities previously described. In addition, the inventory
elaborated to include the number of separate items of each type observed togeth
number of man-episodes of use for the items. A "man-episode" is indicated (1) for eac
man that used the item, (2) for a reuse by the same man during different observation
and (3) if another man had used the item between recorded uses by the same man.
This may sound complicated, and I must admit it was. Working out the conventions

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Binford] ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 339

I I I

MODEL SEATING PLAN

ZONE

SCALE IN METERS
0 I
4
I I i I I

Figure 4. Model seating plan.

ing was not easy. For instance, a man carrying a skinning knife is observed on the si
observation period number 1. The same man is observed with the skinning knife on the
observation period number 2. Do we record 2 men and 2 skinning knives? In this case, n
observation of "use" was made in either case, and since 1 man and 1 knife had been recorded.
Unless some episode of use was observed, no further tabulation was made.
The information in Table 3 is supplemented by the data on the number of items observed on the
site during the period of behavioral observation and the number of items observed "ar-
chaeologically" at the end of the period of behavioral observation. Several characteristics of the
Nunamiut technological system are made explicit in this table. First, all the items with low "use
ratios" were most commonly considered to be "site-specific" artifacts that were generally
available for use by any occupants of the site. They were considered part of the site in much the
same way that facilities such as hearths were considered. These items were generally introduced
during the early phases of use, or they were removed from caches at the site, having remained
there from a previous period of use. They were thought of as the appropriate "artifact" fur-
nishings of the site, the site-specific "hardware." The best analogy to the way the Eskimo con-
ceived of these items is similar to the way we think of furnishings in a room. They are items that go
with the place, not necessarily the persons occupying the place. Items in this category were coffee

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
340 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 43, No. 3,1978

Figure 5. Observed localization of eating and talking activities under variable wind direction.

cups, coffee pot, coffee can, spoons, bottle of instant coffee, bags of coffee and sugar, can of m
stone anvils, kaotah, sitting skins, and decks of cards. Most of these items were contributed
various occupants for the use by others during the period of occupation. Others were consid
to be unowned and simply part of the site, such as the stone anvils and kaotah used in crack
marrow bones. These were collected expediently from immediately available raw materials a
are not identified with any contributor as might be the coffee cups or pot.
The other class of items exhibiting low use ratios were items that were identified as the "pr
perty" of an individual but were widely shared at the site. These items include the metal file,
hone, and the binoculars. The metal file and the hone were observed as items cached on the site
and in this sense were almost considered in the same category as contributed "furnishings." It

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Binford] ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 341

Table 6. Proportion of Man-Hours of Activities Performed by Men Seated in Circular


(Cir.) Fashion Around Hearth.

Game Off site Eating + Target Playing


watching hunting talking shooting cards Crafts Sleep
Date Cir. Oth. Cir. Oth. Cir. Oth. Cir. Oth. Cir. Oth. Cir. Oth. Cir. Oth.

June 4 0 100% 0 100% 93% 7% - - 17% 83% 29% 71% - -


June 4 0 100% 0 100% 95% 5% 0 00% 0 100% 50% 50% - -
June 5 0 100% 0 100% 100% 0 0 100% 0 100% 17% 83% 0 100%
June 5 0 100% 0 100% 90% 10% 0 100% 11% 89% 0 100% 0 100%
June 6 0 100% 0 100% 95% 5% 0 100% - - 36% 64% - -
June 6 0 100% 0 100% 100% 0 22% 78% - - 25% 75% -
M 0 100% 0 100%95.5% 4.5% 4.4% 95.6% 7% 93% 26.2%73.8% 0 100%

was explained that these were not very valuable it


them if someone was using them when the owner dec
son using an item would cache it on the site if the o
finished his task. By way of contrast, the binoculars
would not be unreasonable for the owner to collect th
when he decided to leave the site.

Figure 6. Observed localizations of card playing.

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
I
342 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol.43,No. 1978

_ _ _._
I 1 I '
I1

r---- -
N118 MASK SITE LEGEND
OBSERVED LOCALIZATION OF
CRAFT ACTIVITIES ',/ |
SIDE ACTIVTY AREA)

N 17

V,~ -i CRAFT ACTIVITIES(IN-


SIDE ACTIVITY AREA)

N 116 __

%~~A *~~~ /r \ a ~/ ~/ ~ACTIVITIES (INSIDE I

I
ACTIVITY AREA)
i
CRAFTS AREA I
i

iI
\, CR TAFTS AREA #2

___- / /.' ^
~i:.~)
I
i
iI
I

i
iI

I
I
Nt I I

i1-

i-

N /I/ i
i

i
I
I
I
110 ? . .,' SCALE IN METERS
LI0I 2
r- 1

SIO
Ii

EIIO EItl 112 E113


I

--
E:114 E115 E,? E/7 El8 E 119 E o 120
am I

Figure 7. Observed localizations of craft activ

A final class of items exhibiting low use ratios were


they were recycled into this function. That is, they ha
poses.
We can see that of all the items exhibiting low use ratios, only expediently used items s
the anvils a and kaotah or recycled items such as the targets went into the archaeological re
a manner directly proportional to the number actually used on the site. All of the items havi
use ratios may be thought of as "group" tools or multiple-use containers. These are the item
are apt to occur sporadically on sites if they are curated (Binford 1976) and moved aro
cached contexts, or very regularly as in the case of the anvils, if not curated. These are the
that are apt to appear most commonly in the archaeological record as "de facto garba
Schiffer's (1976) terms, if curated.
Among the items exhibiting a use ratio of 1 there are clearly 3 subclasses of items: (1) ind
serving containers-hash, sardine, and pop cans-all containers for individual servings o
or refreshments, (2) almost all the items used in craft activities, abrader, kitchen knife, etc
(3) all the items introduced as part of the personal gear of hunters and destined for use at h
stands, blinds, and butchering locations-dog packs, rifles, skinning knives, etc. Only items i
first category were systematically represented on the site. These were the immediate bypro
of consumption.
What we are seeing are some of the effects of organizational properties on the archaeolo
record. All items used on the site were not organized within the technology in a similar m

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Binford] ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 343

Figure 8. Observed localization of activities, afternoon, June 5.

The particular patterns of technological organization conditioned the degree to which it


did not go into the archaeological record as a direct consequence of their use. It shoul
much imagination to visualize how the archaeological record might vary if the organiza
technology were different with no difference in either the character of the items used
tivity conditions of their use. Suppose for a moment that all the gear listed in Table 4,
and 4, was introduced to the site as part of "personal gear" and each man had his own cup,
spoon, coffee pot, can of milk, coffee can, etc. Clearly the numbers introduced to the site would be
very different, approaching the values for rifles and skinning knives in column 2. Let us further
suppose that these items were "expendable" as opposed to "curated" (see Binford 1976) or main-
tained within the technology for considerable periods of time. I think the reader can easily ap-
preciate how vastly different the content of the archaeological record would be given such
organizational changes, while activities, site functions, identity of occupants, etc., might remain
the same.
We can appreciate how systems might vary in their organizational properties; such differences
have been explored previously in terms of some of their implications (see Binford 1976). It should
be pointed out that organizational properties may vary within a system situationally and thereby
contribute appreciably to intersite variability within a system. For instance, I have observed
situations in which gear normally curated and carried as part of the personal-gear element of the
technology may be abandoned or scuttled. We may also appreciate that environmental "con-
tingencies" may well situationally condition how otherwise identical items are organized. For in-
stance, in 1974 I had the rare opportunity to observe tool-making behavior of Alyawara-speaking

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
344 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 43, No. 3, 1978

I I I I I r , ""
I

I
i

N 17
MASK SITE . _.
OBSERVED LOCALIZATION OF ACTIVITIES
JUNE 6

N 116

?,,,,. 4 ) 'ACTIVE HEARTH

LEGEND

N 115

TARGET SHOOTING

N 114

* - ^\ ;:.: ^^'''^^\ EATING/TALKING

N113 CRAFT ACTIVITIES

I
I

N 112
< I I
i
i
I

. I~~~~ I

SCALE IN METERS I

- I/s -
I

N 111
l-- V <) I
I

Ell E112 E113 E114 E 115 E 116 E 117 E I8 E 119 E 120


!
Figure 9. Observed localization of activi

Australian aborigines at a stone quarry and the


quantities of material were immediately av
byproducts of flint chipping and discarded ess
the base camp, where stone working was re
transported over a considerable distance, the
the various families as potential sources of ra
cessing into tools. In this case we see how the "
ly responsive to external conditions resulting
remains at different locations.

DISPOSAL MODES

Thus far I have discussed factors in the ongoing behavioral system that differentia
the disposition and use of material items. In this section I am concerned with the im
cess of the transformation of material items from their "systemic" context (see Sch
their "archaeological context." I am interested in describing the modes of dispo
entering the archaeological record at the Mask site. In turn, I am interested in the re
ture, the character of the internal site structure that results from the production
chaeological record at this site. Given such interests, one can appreciate that m
observations would be biased. I made no attempt to record all observed behavior occu
hunting stand, only behavior in the context of which material items were manipu
within this domain I did not record all acts, only those which resulted in the depositi
or in the repositioning of an item already placed within the site. The earlier 12 hour
tion were used as the basis for the development of an observational format. I recognized 5

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Binford] ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 345

Figure 10. Observed localization of activities, noon, June 5.

manipulative acts that resulted in items occurring on surfaces within the site: dropping, tossing,
resting, positioning, and dumping items.

Dropping
Most of the cases of dropping were cases where elements were detached from an item already
held in the hand. The most frequently observed dropping situation was in the context of cracking
marrow bones. The bone was commonly held in the hand and struck with the back of a hunting
knife. The impact resulted in detached chips and splinters of bone that dropped to the ground
directly below the point of impact. Another common situation of dropping was observed during
the manufacture of a mold for a mask. The craftsman was carving a piece of wood, and the wood
shavings dropped to the ground directly below the action. A rare form of dropping was "fumble"
dropping. That is, an item either held in the hand or in some container such as a pocket was drop-
ped during the course of the action or during the removal of another item from the container.

Tossing
This action was very common and most often occurred upon the completion of some action. For
instance, containers such as sardine cans or pop cans were commonly tossed after their contents
were consumed. Similarly, articulator ends of bone processed for marrow were tossed away after
the marrow was removed. The act is simple, an item held in the hand is tossed aside, effectively
removing it from the area of its use.

Resting
Items are set down, normally, in the following contexts: "unpacking" upon entry to the camp,
temporary abandonment of a task due to interruptions, or arraying tools that might all be used in

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
346 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 43, No. 3,1978

I l I I i I I _ I I I

MASK SITE L-
L_

DEFINTION OF ACTMTY AREAS W,XY,Z

_-

/
Nml7

N//A

CRF TS_A *
M14

N 13

I
N109O

AA Z /

' - _ /
IN109

SCALE IN METERS

E109 EllO
I
EI
I
El1/
I
E/12
I I
E113
.
ElI
E1SiE1B6 E 17 I El i
L_.
E 119
I
I E/O I E121
I I
L

Figure 11. Definition of activity areas W, X, Y, Z.

accomplishing a general task. Typical behaviors might be the hunter w


and leans his rifle against a rock or takes off his pack and sets it do
men may be sitting around a hearth eating marrow bones and drink
word" caribou is stage whispered among the men. Almost invariably
processed marrow bones, or half-eaten tins of sardines will be set do
the man, and he will bolt up searching for his binoculars, rifle, etc.
items.

Positioning Items (Placed)


An item was identified as positioned if there was some attempt to (a) aggregate several, (b)
unobtrusively place them so they would not interfere with ongoing activities at the location, and
(c) insure their easy retrieval at some future date. Formally this is a difficult category because
there is an assumed motive-the temporary placement of an item or items in anticipation of future
use. This is what the archaeologist would call caching, although some of these "caches" may be
very short term.

Dumping
This was an infrequent behavior on hunting stands. It consists of the accumulation of dropped
or resting items, normally in a container. The container is then picked up and removed, commonly

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Binford] ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 347

to the periphery of the site, and dumped. This results in a recognizable high density ag
distribution.
The above categories were found to adequately accommodate the observed behaviors in which
items came to rest in a hunting stand. Obviously this is only a partial picture, since items at rest
within a site may be removed and/or reorganized spatially as a result of a variety of actions taking
place within the site. The following classification of acts that resulted in the repositioning of items
already at rest within the site was found useful:

1. Brushing aside-This action was observed in only four behavioral contexts: (a) before sitting
down, (b) in preparation for drawing a map in the dirt, (c) before butchering an animal, and (d)
prior to dismantling a snowmobile carburetor. In no case was a special tool used, such as a
broom. The hand and arm were used to brush aside litter and to smooth out the surface of the
ground in preparation for the performance of the acts listed above.
2. Searching-This action was observed only twice. One of the men would generally ask the
group if anyone had picked up his item. In one case the item was a butchering knife and in the
other it was a fragment of a broken saw blade used in cutting antler. In camps such as the Mask
site, the first assumption normally made when an item is suspected of being missing is that some-
one else has picked up the item and is using it or has placed it somewhere unknown to the owner.
This means that if an individual misses some item that he had at the site he will always query the
other men present, "Did anyone see my ?" If no one acknowledges having seen
the item, it is generally assumed that someone no longer present used the item and has left it
"around the site somewhere." Most of the time, in the absence of information as to where the item
may be found, the men present will get up and begin searching for it. There is a kind of search
priority or scale of likelihood about where a lost item may be. First they look on top of prominent
rocks and boulders, then around the bases of these rocks and boulders. Next they look under any
temporary "ground covers" such as caribou skins, which might have been dragged around and in-
advertently covered up an item, or a stack of recently introduced firewood might be poked
around in. Finally, there is a ground search centered around the hearth areas of the site. If
coverage of these areas fails to turn up the item, the men generally abandon the search, assuming
that someone not present has the item and will return it to the owner at a later date. Importantly,
during the search there is a moving around of items already present, and almost always some
items are picked up to be "recycled" for other purposes. Lost items are "rediscovered" at this
time.

Table 5 tabulates the items observed on the site and the frequency with which various items
were manipulated so that they remained unattended on the site. This does not mean that these
items necessarily became part of the archaeological record, for many of them may have been
repositioned or removed from the site later. This difference is indicated by the comparison be-
tween the items actually inventoried "archaeologically" on the site and the frequency with which
items were "positioned" within the site.
Several important facts are illustrated in Table 5. There are major differences between the in-
ventories of items dropped, tossed, and dumped. For instance, marrow bone splinters and chips,
shell casing, and rib tablets were the items most commonly dropped, while articulator ends of
marrow bones, pop cans, and sardine cans were the items most commonly tossed. Dumping ex-
clusively consisted of coffee grounds and rib tablets. On the other hand, many items were rested
or placed on the site but never or only rarely did these become part of the archaeological record.
Referring the "disposal modes" recognized here to those recognized by Schiffer (1976:30-33),
both dropping and tossing would result in "primary refuse." Similarly, dumping as it was oWb-
served on the Mask site would also result in primary refuse in spite of the fact that Schiffer
generally equates dumping with the generation of secondary refuse (see Schiffer 1976:30). Dump-
ing, as on the Mask site and in other contexts, consists of the disposal of multiple aggregated
elements during food processing and/or food preparation activities. Dumping may consist of the
disposal of the aggregate immediately adjacent to the locus of use. This is quite different from the
situation where aggregates are accumulated during the course of "cleaning up" and removed

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
348 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 43, No. 3, 1978

from the location of primary deposition to a "special area" or specific dumping area. The latter
activity was not observed on the Mask site.
As in the previous sections it is hoped that the reader can appreciate the effect that changes in
the relationships between item classes and modes of disposal may have on the final form of the ar-
chaeological record. For instance, I have described elsewhere (Binford 1978) the general pattern
of disposition of bone elements during the course of a meal within an Eskimo house. Items that are
commonly tossed or dropped on the Mask site, such as articulator ends and bone splinters, are
most often "placed" in small piles along the edges of hearth rocks, around the stove, or on the
edge of serving dishes uring r eating and talking episodes within an Eskimo house. This behavior is
in anticipation of the women of the house "cleaning up" after the meal, accumulating articulator
ends in a cache for later processing into bone grease and "dumping" bone splinters in special
dump areas. In this example we can see that there may well be very different disposal modes
associated with the same item in different site or social contexts.
So far I have discussed 3 basic behavioral categories relative to the "systtemic" context (Sc
fer 1976) of the Mask site. Hopefully, I have demonstrated that a location, the actual spatial lo
of a site, may be selected relative to criteria for optimizing conditions for a single task. In the
of the Mask site this task was the e monitoring of a large area for game movements. The activ
conducted within the site were all secondary accommodations to the situational integration
spring straggler hunting at the Mask site into the overall strategy of spring hunting. If migration
hunting has been very successful and herds were more mixed than normal, straggler hunting at
the Mask site will be conducted by only the most unsuccessful hunters, group sizes will be small
compared to the data reported here and activities conducted there while waiting for game will be
conditioned by size and length-of-stay considerations.
Similarly, the organization of the technology is at least partially conditioned by the understood
or anticipated relationship of the location to use intensity and duration, as well as its actual loca-
tion relative to other sites in use at the time. For instance, preparation of meals was never ob-
served on the Mask site, because it is located less than a mile from the main village of Anaktuvuk.
The only eating observed could best be described as snacking. Only bone marrow and items
specifically introduced to contribute to the "picnic" atmosphere of eating (cans of sardines are a
good example) were consumed. Meat per se was never cooked, and techniques of boiling and
roasting meat were never employed on the site during the periods of controlled observation.
Similarly, the form and numbers of artifactual items that were considered "furnishings" on the
site could be expected to vary with the anticipated modal group sizes and anticipated intensity of
use. In the example that I offered regarding remote sites, I suggested that gear present on the
Mask site as furnishings would be part of personal gear at such a remote location.
Finally, I described the disposal modes observed on the Mask site and pointed out that there
was a regular set of relationships between the form and size of items and the modes of disposal. It
was further argued that in other contexts different disposal modes might well be employed for
identical items.
All of the three behavioral dimensions, activity structure, technological organization, and
disposal modes interact and contribute to the facts of site structure, or the patterning
recognizable in the static disposition of cultural features and items at a location.

ORGANIZATION OF SPACE

Several types of behavioral data were collected during observations on the Mask site. A
were made to observe the position of men on the site during the performance of different ac-
tivities. In addition, I measured the placement of men relative to one another and to basic
facilities on the site during the performance of different activities.
The core area of the site is between 3 relatively large glacial boulders. Among these boulders
were 5 hearths. Figure 3 shows the relationship between the disposition of hearths and the place-
ment of boulders. I never observed all 5 hearths in use at the same time; in fact, I never saw more
than 2 in simultaneous use, and that was rare. The differential use of the hearths is basically

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Binford] ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 349

related to wind direction. When the prevailing winds are from the north, hearths B an
primarily used; winds blowing from the south prompted the preferential use of hearths
sometimes D. Prevailing winds from the north tended to carry scent to animals approach
the south, so large groups of men were never observed at the site when winds were fr
north. South winds carried scent away from approaching animals, and more men tende
the hunting stand under these conditions. For these reasons hearths A and C were the m
monly used.
I actually measured the placement of men and found that for a sample of 35 men sea
groups of 3 or 4, the mean distance from the left kneecap to the ember edge of the fire w
cm. The mean distance between left and right knees of men seated next to one another w
cm. When men were seated in groups of 4 they tended to move back from the fire a littl
room if an additional member of the group arrived. Under these conditions I measured 4
5 (20 measurements) and found the mean distance from the left knee to the ember edge o
to be 71 ? 8.2 cm and the mean distance between left and right knees of adjacent men to
cm. Figure 4 illustrates the mean sitting pattern for groups of 3 men and of 4 men aroun
the Mask site.
The first thing to be noted is that there is always a vacant side of the hearth, depending upon
wind direction. Such a distribution around a hearth, that is, with a side of relatively dense debri
and an opposite, low-density side, has frequently been interpreted as deriving from the organiza
tion of space within a house; the low-density side is seen as representing the sleeping area (see
Leroi-Gourham and Brezillon 1972) and the high-density side as the area of domestic ac-
tivity-eating and food preparation. That such a distribution may in fact arise is shown by Yell
(1977). In his case, the low-density side of the hearth relates to the area adjacent to the shelter
sleeping area (Yellen 1977). I have also observed the low/high density pattern of debris around in
ternal hearths, particularly in the case of the Palangana site (see Binford 1978).

Table 7. Man-Hours of Activity Localized in Different Areas.

Game Eating + Target Playing


Areas Hearths watch talking shoot cards Crafts Sleep Total
W none 3.0(12%) 10.5(42%) 4.0(16%) 7.25(30%) 24.75
X B-E 3.75(34%) 2.5(32%) 0.75(7%) 3.0(27%) 10.0
Y C-D 6.25(32%) 0.5(3%) 9.0(47%) 3.5(18%) 19.25
Z A 39.25(96%) 1.5(4%) 40.75

Total 3.0* 49.25 11.0 15.5 13.0 3.0 94.75

*28.5 hours of game watching done from bo


hearth A.

On the Mask site the "seating plan" conditions the dispersion pattern of items that were drop-
ped versus those that were tossed. I did not measure the position of dropped items relative to
seated persons. However, I noted that dropped items came to rest within 20 cm of the front of a
man sitting cross-legged. They dropped between his legs or fell to the side of his legs within about
20 cm. This means that in the seating plan shown in Figure 4 we may add the probable location of
the "drop zone," the area within which men seated around the hearth would drop items. While
observing on the Mask site, I measured the distance between the knee of the seated man and the
resting place of tossed items. Such items were invariably tossed "over the shoulder" and the
mean distance from the kneecap of a seated man for tossed articulator ends (N = 7) of bones was
1.14 m with a standard deviation of 24 cm. For sardine and pop cans (N = 6) the mean distance
was 2.54 m with a standard deviation of 29 cm. When I asked informants why they tossed cans
more vigorously they simply said they were unsightly and got in the way more than bones! Given
this information we may add to our model seating plan an additional zone of anticipated
debris-tossed items. Figure 4 illustrates the anticipated distributional patterning for items
disposed of by individuals seated around an outside hearth.

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
350 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 43, No. 3,1978

9 -

8-

7-

Nh 6-
()

Figure 12. Relationship be- 3 5- ?


tween mean group size and the 0
k 0
percent of total craft time spent (
within an "eating circle." 2 40
3

2-

o 0o 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 o00
% OF TOTAL CRAFT TIME SPENT
WITHIN "EATING CIRCLE"

The basic organization or seating plan for individuals around a hearth may not change f
typical "outside" situation as observed on the Mask site to an "inside" situation where the
hearth in the center of a house. We might anticipate that the seating plan relative to the
might be essentially identical, and we may even anticipate a vacant side to the distribution
the sleeping area is adjacent to the hearth. A drop zone could be expected where stone work
marrow cracking was going on adjacent to the hearth. However, in the Nunamiut case we
never observe a "toss zone" around a seated group inside a house. (See Binford 1978: Chapt
for a description of food consumption within the house.) Items normally tossed outside wou
placed along the hearth stones or along the edges of serving platters so that the woman o
house could easily clean up the large bone debris from meals consumed inside.
Men seated around the hearths of the Mask site in some "size" phase of the pattern illu
in Figure 5 were observed to be engaged in eating and conversation most commonly. Table 6
marizes the proportion of man-hours of activities, as tabulated in Table 2, which was spat
organized as men seated in a circular fashion around the hearth.
What is very clear is that the particular pattern of space use characterized as a semicirc
men seated around a hearth was almost exclusively related to the activity of eating and ta
All other activities were characteristically conducted in a different place and in a differe
tern of association man-to-man and man-to-facility. In short, there were different areas ass
with the performance of different activities. The single activity exhibiting the most overla
eating and talking, in terms of where it was performed, was craft activity. It is shown tha
of the time during which individuals were engaged in craft activity was spent within the c
men seated around the fire. The actual location and seating plan of men engaged in activit
other than eating and talking is best illustrated graphically. Figure 6 illustrates the locati
card-playing activities. The numbering of the areas roughly coincides with the preferent
for placement. Area 1 would certainly be preferred for several reasons: (1) Because card p
is an activity of large group sizes, it is almost certain that winds would be blowing from the

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 351
Binford]

r r -- - ...-- T l II I '

N 117 MASK SITE


DISTRIBUTION OF BONE

F_
NN , ' " 1 " -: N/15

'I~ IC ,~ 2

AI, 'v . ' . I

E N E 112 E 113 E114 E 115 E 116 E117 E 118 E 119 E 120

NI / SCALE IN METERS

clear that5 th ao raidpneto e etdinhat ice saogtelrerc


Figure 13. Distribution of bone.
where ~ ~ \ card plyn ra1i lolctd(rf ra1 nFgr ) nadtoa rfrai
along~~ ~ ~ ~ th atsd ftesalrglca ole utnrho hat dsgae ra2i

of playing area 3, making it less desirable; (2) Players can se


playing area 1, but this is almost impossible from playing
area 2 because of slope; (3) The players are less likely to b
hearths than would be the case in areas 2 and 3.
Craft activities are occasionally conducted by men seated in an "eating and talking" sem
cle. Figure 7 illustrates the seated position of all men observed engaged in craft activitie
clear that the major area independent of men seated in hearth circles is along the la
where card playing area 1 is also located (craft area 1 in Figure 7). An additional craft ar
along the east side of the smaller glacial boulder just north of hearth C (designated area
Figure 7).
Less commonly performed activities are perhaps best illustrated by internal site arran
observed during different occupational episodes. Figure 8 illustrates the arrangement of
lcla ted. hsite
the Mask ao raideedn fmnsaedi
at the time of my erhcrlsisaogtelrerc
arrival on the site during the afternoon of June 5. Six m
present: 1 sleeping, 1 engaged in carving an antler handle for a "woman's knife" (ulu), 1 wa
for game, and 3 engaged in eating and conversation. The wind was from the south, and o
hearth A was in use. The sleeping man was in an area otherwise used as an eating and talk
area when hearth B is in use. The man working on the antler was in craft area 2, and th
watching for game was seated in the same area where card playing or crafts were also co
located.

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
352 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 43, No. 3,1978

7 t I I I I i

MASK SITE
DISTRAIBTION OF SPENT CARTRIDGES
AND WOOD SHAVINGS

N116

I
N 115

N 114

N 113

N112

f- ----.222 CALBER
N 111

"/
% \

.30

.
,257 CAULBER SCALE IN METERS 1

I NllO
i-- 0 2

ElIl E 112 E113 E114 E?115 ?E116 E117 E 118 El119 El120
I I

Figure 14. Distribution of spent cart

Figure 9 illustrates the disposition of men


6th. Two men are eating and talking aro
sewing up a rip in a pair of caribou skin
was being conducted from the same area
where men sometimes sit to watch for g
the glacial boulder from craft area 2.
To further illustrate the point that the i
Figure 10 illustrates the location of men
talking around hearth A, 1 man was carv
hearth B which was active, and 4 men w
area 1. Two major points are to be empha
activities conducted simultaneously are i
Over time, there is a statistical tendency
places, although these loci would not be
In order to illustrate these facts, I have
ways. First, based on the cumulative act
spatial data from all my episodal maps in
basic use areas on the site (Figure 11).
tend to be localized such that they do no
recognized on the Mask site are designat

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Binford] ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 353

Figure 15. Artifact distribution.

tion schedules, I was able to tabulate the man-hours of the various activities which were con-
ducted in each of the 4 recognized "use areas" on the site. Table 7 summarizes these data.
Even a casual examination of Table 7 illustrates nicely 2 very basic points. (1) The intensity of
use was not evenly distributed among the recognized use areas. (2). The various activities were not
evenly distributed among the several areas. The man-hours spent in game watching were 100%
localized in area W. The hours spent sleeping were 100% localized in area X. Of the total man-
hours spent in eating and talking 80% were localized in area Z. Target shooting man-hours were
95% localized in area W, while 58% of the card playing hours were localized in area Y and 56%
of the hours spent in crafts were localized in area W. Quite clearly there is a basis in "reality" for
seeking patterns in the archaeological remains which derive from spatial segregation of ac-
tivities. This is true in spite of the fact that a casual observer seeing the pattern of site use as il-
lustrated in Figures 8-10 might conclude that there were no special "activity areas" but only a
"generalized use of activity space."' Such an impressionistic conclusion was reached by John
Yellen from his Bushman experience.

I have suggested that it is unfounded to assume that activities are spatially segregated or arranged by type
within a single camp. Most tasks may be carried out in more than one place and in more than one social con-
text; and, conversely, in any single area, one can find the remains of many activities all jumbled together.
Unfortunately, many archaeological analyses are based on just such an erroneous assumption, and their
resulting conclusions must be called into question (1977:134).

In another place Yellen states:

.... one may evaluate models archaeologists often use to examine activity patterning within an excavated
site. What underlies many of these is the a priori assumption that most activities are performed by special-

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
354 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 43, No. 3,1978

purpose, job-specific groups, and that individual tasks are spatially segregated from one an
most misleading aspect of this statement lies not in its overly simplistic nature but its implica
primary nature of an activity itself rather than its social context uniquely determines the loca
it will be performed... (1977:97).

These critical, and to my ears pompous, statements are simply wrong and base
systematic observation that I can detect. I think that most would agree that it is ph
possible for 2 independent entities to occupy identical spaces simultaneously. Theref
possible for 2 persons to occupy identical spaces simultaneously. Insofar as 2 per
gaged in different activities simultaneously they must be localized in different place
clearly illustrated in Figures 8-10 from the Mask site. As it is impossible to imagine dif
sons occupying the same space, so it is impossible to imagine activities carried out sim
by different persons occurring in the identical space.
Given these conditions, the degree that activities will be spatially separated at any on
be expected to vary with the number of different activities simultaneously performed
persons.
The interesting question that arises from this has to do with the degree to which simila
tivities will be conducted in the same places at different times. Stated another way, we may ask
what will be the degree of redundancy in the organization of activities in space? I have illustrated
that on the Mask site, Yellen's generalization is correct (see Figures 8-10), namely:

Most tasks may be carried out in more than one place and in more than one social context and conversely in
any single area one can find the remains of many activities all jumbled together (1977:134).

I have also illustrated that the conclusion drawn from this observation is false. Table 7 il-
lustrates that it is not "unfounded to expect activities to be spatially segregated or arranged by
type within a single camp." Table 7 illustrates nicely that there are meaningful structural facts of
spatial association between activities and different places on the Mask site. What conditions
these facts? Yellen suggests at that the primary conditioner is the social context of performance
rather than "the primary nate of than "the primary nature of the activity itself" (1977:97). Such a generalization makes no
sense, and it is contradicted by Yellen himself when discussing head roasting and skin prepara-
tion (see Yellen 1977:92, 145). The point is that some activities interfere with others. Similarly,
some activities require more or less space, more or less time to completion, and more or less par-
ticipants. In addition, activities vary in the amount of debris or pollution (noise or odor) produced
during the course of performance. They further vary in the relative degree to which the debris or
pollution is noxious and thus inhibits or disrupts the performance of other activities. We see very
few dances performed inside of active hearths; rarely does a person assemble a complicated craft
item in the midst of a group of playing children.
An excellent illustration of this point is provided by the relationship between the modal group
size present on the site and the percentage of total activity time spent in craft activities executed
in the hearth-centered circles of men. Figure 12 illustrates this relationship for the 6 observation
periods documented on the Mask site. It is very clear that as group size increases, the percentage
of the total activity time spent in craft activities localized in the "eating and talking areas"
decreases. This is not to be understood as changes in response to "social context" but simply as a
response to the fact that as more men are present and engaged in talking and eating, there is more
noise, and craftsmen are less able to concentrate. Noise and distractive activity are cited by the
Eskimo as the reasons for abandoning craft-related tasks when many men are present. If they
must be performed, a special location away from the talking and eating area is sought by crafts-
men. I expect activity differentiation in space to relate to both the anticipated future use of the
location and the character of activity incompatibilities. We can build a theory of space use, and
we can understand spatial patterning without recourse to vague notions of "social context."

THE ARCHAEOLOGICAL CONSEQUENCES OF ALL THIS BEHAVIOR

I have tried to illustrate that there were at least 4 basic dimensions of potentially independent
variability that interact to contribute to the archaeological facts generated at the Mask site.

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Binford] ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 355

Figure 13 illustrates the observed distribution of animal bone fragments; Figure 14 illu
distribution of wood shavings and shell casings; and Figure 15 illustrates the distribut
tifacts" as observed on the Mask site. It seems to me that little comment is needed here
most of the facts of the archaeological distribution could be anticipated from the
discussion. Given the level at which the technology is "curated" by the Nunamiut
chaeological remains at the Mask site relate primarily to the activities of eating and t
consequences of the disposal modes is well represented in the site structure. Bone spli
chips cluster in the "drop zone" and articulator ends cluster in the "toss zone" of a se
with men seated in a semicircle around the hearth (see Figure 16). The distribution of s
faithfully betrays the locations of target shooting, while wood shavings similarly sug
primary locations of craft activity.
The "artifact" distribution provides us with our only surprise. The items that were
disposed of on the site-pop cans, sardine cans, segments of rope, worn gloves, etc.-all
clustered and peripheral distribution to the site as a whole, not just to the areas in whic
were used. In fact there are clusters of such items adjacent to areas that were use

I It I TI T II I, I

MASK SITE
BONE DISTRIBUTI0N SUPERIMPOSED
ON MODEL SEATING PLAN

_ N/118

II

N 117
I 111 -N

N/16

N 114

\
\

N 1/1

NI O \ /

SCALE iN METERS
01 21

El12 E113 El 1 E 1//5 E 116 E lI El/8 El/9 El2O E121


I I I* I I I II

Figure 16. Bone distribution superimp

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
/
356 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 43, No. 3,1978

MASK SITE " 1\


ARTIFACT DISTRIBUTION SUPERIMOSED /
VO ACT7VITY AREAS

O p /

~~~~~~~~~~~i; X.

'i AREA Y

\ --'4E

\\ (

0 "' - -)
0

AREA Z '

E 109 EIIOB E It/ E 112 E E 114 E116SCALE N ME TERS

109 E1O1 Etl f | E 12 1 E13 | E114 | EIfS I?11 E.1 | E 18 EI 9 Et I E21

Figure 17. Artifact distribution superimposed on act

Figure 17). This pattern implies a biased disposal of items in


course, presupposes an overall understanding of the use pat
posed to the particular pattern that might be present at th
way, disposal patterns result in a distribution that is essenti
of use intensity.
The consequences of this for different archaeological p
stance, let us imagine the Mask site under changed conditio
only dried meat which would contribute no immediate byp
short, all the chips and splinters seen in Figure 13 would be
the archaeological site would appear quite different; it woul
"high-density zone" with a central low-density area lacking
might remain as de facto garbage. Under these condition
almost exclusively to disposal areas and not activity are
associations between items in disposal areas would be mo
would be as likely to reflect relative frequency relations be
relationship among items used within given activities. Such
artifact association from different areas of a site may well
cesses" relative to different areas of a site and not necessar
on a site. There are strong implications for the analysis of
grid units as the basis for summarizing samples. Perhaps te
based on demonstrable structural properties of a scatter plo
proaches.

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Binford] ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 357

CONCLUSIONS

Long ago I questioned (Binford and Binford 1966) the position that variability in the co
internal plan of sites was referable to the single variable "culture." I argued that variab
assemblage composition, site location, and internal site plans could be expected to vary
activities in which men were engaged. I further suggested that activities were likely to b
sive to seasonal variations, giving rise to archaeological variability which would sc
seasonal monitors. At the same time I suggested that activities could be logically differen
to extractive and maintenance tasks. I also speculated that it was unlikely that tasks dif
such a fundamental way would be conducted in identical locations so that some bet
variability may well be referable to differences in site function. Recent investigations
relationship between site contents and seasonality have been the most discussed, but th
plicating problem of site function has not been faced squarely (see for instance Jochim 1
has been particularly true in European studies of Paleolithic remains. It appears tha
every investigator concludes that his site was a residential location (see Bietti 1977; Delu
1969; Leroi-Gourhan and Brezillon 1966, 1972; Poplin 1976) and frequently boasts of the
presence of a shelter or house, in spite of there being little if any convincing evidence
The most detailed ethnoarchaeological study yet to appear has been that of Yellen (197
has strongly criticized these arguments. Yellen suggests that extractive locations, while
among the !Kung, would leave no visible archaeological remains. He further suggests th
are no seasonal correlates to the execution of craft or manufacturing activities. He goes on to
argue that although seasonal variability in subsistence activities is documented, since there is
also a correlation between season and duration of camps, only large camps would remain ar-
chaeologically visible. This situation would render observation of seasonally related variability
unlikely in the !Kung case. The only conclusion which one may draw from Yellen's assertions is
there are no recognizable differences among !Kung sites that are not simply accommodated by a
graded series of variability largely scaling with size of assemblage or site. As Yellen says:

Any site may be conceived of as a spatial locus containing a sample of the society's total repertoire of ac-
tivities .... I suggest a single scale, ranging from simple to complex, may prove more useful than a typological
approach.... (1977:135).

The implication is that if one demonstrates clear and distinct assemblage types, argum
functional variation within systems are inappropriate. The Mask site experience point
"reality" of functionally specific sites. It indicates sources of variability that may ope
dependently or in various combinations to result in significant between-site patterns of
and structural variability within a single system.
I conclude from the experiences at the Mask site that we may expect systems to exi
there are major distinctions between residential and special-purpose sites. I therefore
that we must seek methods to permit us to distinguish these functional differences from
which refer to independent systems of organization. Some of the interesting aspects
purpose" sites are described here. I conclude that we must have means for recognizing
and disagree with Yellen's (1977:83) conclusion that a "continuum of variability" is mo
among hunter and gatherer sites.
I also conclude from the experiences reported here that we may anticipate meaningfu
differentiations between items used in different activities even in sites where there are
"generalized work areas." In addition, there are likely to be further meaningful differences be-
tween disposal modes associated with different items used in similar or different activities,
and meaningful associations can be reasonably sought among items by virtue of their spatial posi-
tioning. This conclusion is in direct opposition to the conclusions reached by Yellen from his
Bushman experiences. He concludes that such expectations are unrealistic: "A corollary of this
simple area-activity assumption is that associated remains are functionally related.... !Kung
data makes this a priori model untenable" (1977:134).
How can 2 people reach such opposite conclusions? In my opinion the major contributor is not
the very real differences between Eskimos and Bushman but extreme differences between myself
and Yellen in what we consider to be appropriate uses of empirical materials and the role of our
thoughts versus our observations. Given such a conflict I caution the reader to read what I have to

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
358 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 43, No. 3,1978

say and to seek an appreciation for what is attempted rather than assuming that he knows w
am doing and why.
This study is an exercise in theory building. The work reported is the justification or a wa
for thinking about some of the ideas that came to me during the course of the fieldwork an
analysis. The ideas are my inventions; they are not in any way summaries of empirica
perience. They are not empirical generalizations. I am not offering inductive argumen
arguments from ethnographic analogy. am not saying that all men will conduct the same a
tivities in hunting camps. I am not saying that all men will play cards in sites with glacial bou
in them. I am not saying that all target shooting was normally conducted away from the g
because of the noise of rifle fire! I am saying that my study dy has prompted by imagination.
been able to imagine patterns of interaction among variables which could result in different
terning in the archaeological record. In turn, I have been able to imagine different patterns
archaeological record that could be meaningfully interpreted if my imagined understandin
"causal" interaction is correct.

The transition from data to theory requires creative imagination. Scientific hypothesis and theories
derived from observed facts, but are invented in order to account for them (Hempel 1966:15).

Basic differences between Yellen and myself in our use of ethnographic experienc

his dt tat there is a relationship between the number of occupants of a site and the size of
nuclear area of artifactual scatter, and similarly that there is a regular relationship between
length of occupation and the extension of the artifactual scatter-the area of the "absolute limits
of scatter." For instance, Yellen states:

Quantitive analysis indicates that the size of the hut circle, or inner ring, is closely and directly related to
group size, while the outer ring, which encompasses special activity areas, reflects length of occupation. On
this basis I have offered predictive equations for group size and length of occupation and put them in ar-
chaeologically useful form. This constitutes an original piece of research and is perhaps the single most im-
portant contribution of this book (1977:134).

Yellen has observed that there is a relationship between metrical attributes of sites and the
numbers of occupants and the duration of occupancy. He has gone to great lengths to demonstrate
that such a relationship exists within the domain of his ethnographic experience. This is a descrip-
tion of the way the world is or appears. It is not an explanation as he suggests (Yellen 1977:101).
Only in seeking an answer to the question, "Why does such a relationship hold in Yellen's
Bushman experience?" does one seek an explanation or understanding of the world. Only with an
answer to the explanatory question could we anticipate when the world will differ from Yellen's
experience. Yellen ignores this interesting problem and apparently assumes that the number of
people and the duration of their stay "causes" the patterning in size among his sites. If this were
the case, then the descriptive equations which Yellen presents should allow us to give meaning in
a reliable manner to facts of site size when they are observed archaeologically. Clearly this is
what Yellen has in mind: "To establish predictive relationships of this nature can provide a
valuable tool in archaeological interpretation" (1977:100).
When one has demonstrated an empirical condition, the assumption needed in any warranted
projection of the observed condition to situations not previously investigated is simply that there is
a causal identity between the 2 situations-that the same causes are active in both situations and
that the dynamics of causation are the same. Stated another way, the world stays the way it was,
and what we have seen is all there is. We don't have to understand what we have seen, we simply
have to believe that what we have seen is "representative of the world in general" and that this
world will remain unchanged.
I suggest that theory building is the progressive delineation of the "other things" that must be
equal for a given relationship to hold true. In Yellen's case we can reasonably ask what some of
those "other things" might be. For instance, according to my calculations of the Mask site there
are 73.8 m2 in the "ALS" as defined by Yellen (1977:103), and there are 12.7 m2 within the

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Binfordl ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 359

"LMS" limits of most scatter as defined by Yellen (1977:103). Using the formulas given
I would draw the following interpretive conclusions from the archaeological remains on
site: (1) It was occupied for 7.89 days (in fact the archaeological scatter measured
cumulated over a period of 21 days); (2) it was occupied by 2.03 nuclear families (
families were ever present on the site); and (3) the number of occupants present was
mean group size present at any one time was observed to be 5.49 men, but the total num
ferent men using the location was 34). The latter figure is equivalent, I think, to the
counted occupants. Clearly some "other things" are causing the Nunamiut case to be s
ferent from the Bushman cases. For Yellen, my data from the Mask site serve as an em
to the contrary, an example of the "spoiler" approach. They exemplify Yellen's
(1977:113) that, "balloons may be punctured by a single pin." For Yellen, an induc
perception is consistent with his view of investigations, since he seeks patterned regu
empirical generalizations as an end product of his work. For empiricists working indu
there are only 2 basic conclusions to be drawn from investigating the world: (1) the c
similar in some way, justifying generalizations or the recognition of a "regularity," or
different, justifying the definition of a new category or taxonomic unit for subsuming
case and any other similar ones discovered in the future. For me, however, the s
tion-my data versus Yellen's-simply points to an interesting set of differences betwe
and Bushman sites which are in need of explanation. The challenge is to build a sufficie
theory to explaine the differences between the cases. Yellen argues that this may be
pirically:

In an ethnographic situation where the "causes" (answers) are known from the start, one can loo
"effects"-in this case observable remains-and see what techniques offer the best route from the one to
the other ....

This is most obvious . . . where a priori knowledge permitted me to devise and evaluate a number of tech-
niques for estimating group size and length of occupation from debris (1977:132).

If Yellen's belief in his ability to see causes directly was justified, I suppose I would have to con-
clude that all ethnographic work must result in causal arguments which are necessarily
accurate, since the causes "spoke for themselves." Ethnography would be a field based on reveal-
ed truths in no need of scientific methods since problems of verification and confirmation would
be irrelevant! In the light of this "faith" on Yellen's part can we not reasonably ask why his
causes did not work in the Eskimo case? The only answer that I can reasonably give is that he
never isolated a cause.
Let us examine the situation. Yellen equates numbers of persons and lengths of stay w
size of the distribution of discarded materials. For such a 1-to-1 relationship to exist between
numbers of man-days of occupation and scales of debris scatter, we must assume that there is a
constant relationship between the consumer demands of individuals and the production rate of
debris. Is such a "constant" relationship realistic? I must answer, "no." I have experienced situa-
tions where large quantities of foods are procured in a very limited period of time, and then pro-
cessed for storage. The debris and the size of the area used in processing would bear some rela-
tionship to the quantity of materials obtained. For instance, the number of caribou killed in a
single day has little if any relationship to the number of men present and the duration of stay. I
have also experienced the reverse situation, where consumption of foods was primarily from dry
meat stores. Little if any debris results from such a consumption pattern (see Binford 1978:
Chapter 6). What I am suggesting is that the variables which interact to cause variations in densi-
ty and extent of debris are input and entropy variables, not consumer variables directly as
"seen" by Yellen.
Among the Bushman, subsistence is essentially a foraging strategy in which inputs of food are
largely on a daily basis and proportional to the daily consumer demand. Under such conditions,
there is a strong and proportional relationship at any given time between consumer demand and
the quantity of input, hence Yellen's results. (This ignores the problem of debris from other
maintenance activities such as tool manufacture and repair, since Yellen argues that among the

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
360 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 43, No. 3,1978

Bushman this is simply proportional to the length of stay.) This situation does not occur a
Eskimo, where there are seasonal and situational variations in the performance of
tivities. "Gearing up" is the best translation of an Eskimo word referring specifically t
tense craft activity that frequently precedes long expedition hunting trips, typically in s
the tundra for calf skins or to the mountains for sheep. The constant rate at which fo
troduced to the Bushman camps (given a daily foraging strategy) and at which craft activ
performed (as argued by Yellen 1977:82) insures a regular relationship between density
of debris scatter and number of man-hours of occupation. This assumes, of course,
regular debris-to-activity ratio, that is, different kinds of consumption or craft a
generating essentially similar quantities of debris. This qualification is one that I find ha
cept as realistic, yet it must be met if Yellen's correlations are to stand. I am not attem
solve all the problems associated with Yellen's observations versus my own. However, I
I have illustrated that Yellen has not "observed" cause, and that he has not been engaged in
theory building. More importantly, I have argued that empirical generalizations, no matter how
complicated (as, for example, Yellen's observations on site size and group size and occupational
duration), are what we seek to understand, and only with understanding can we anticipate how
observations will vary under changed conditions. The latter is, of course, what we mean by
predictions. Our ability to anticipate variability in the world is in turn a measure of our
understanding. I have tried to move in this direction with the Mask-site study.
The Mask site represents a site where activities conducted are "embedded" in another more
basic schedule. None of the activities can be considered "primary" to the mission of the oc-
cupants. This means that the specifics of the specifics of the activities will be largely conditioned by factors other
than those which prompted the occupation of the site in the first place. I feel that such an "embed-
ded" activity schedule may well be a common phenomenon among hunters and gatherers, who
are logistically organized as opposed to foragers. At this point this is simply a hunch. I have
demonstrated how at least 4 potentially independent dimensions could interact to result in chang-
ed composition and internal spatial organizations. I have further suggested that these condition-
ing dimensions are situationally responsive and not simply "normative" or idealized patterns or
designs for living. Finally, I have suggested that there are organizational facts to be discovered in
distributional data regardless of the degree to which men may have conducted their activities in
"generalized work spaces."
I have suggested that the degree to which activities are regularly conducted in different places
is at least partially conditioned by the ways in which their execution interferes with other ac-
tivities. I suggested that scheduling concerns and bulk properties of both items processed and
debris produced would condition the degrees of functional specificity among activity areas on a
site. These are hints, ideas to be explored in hopes of recognizing or inventing variables that could
be used to explore causal relationships between activities and their organization in space. Pro-
gress can be made by seeking a processual understanding of the dynamics that produce different
forms of archaeological patterning. It will not be achieved by trying to refine empirical
generalizations, arguing that someone else is wrong because they have had different experiences,
and fooling oneself into viewing empirical descriptions, no matter how complicated they may be,
as explanations.
Acknowledgments. Financial support for the fieldwork reported here was provided by grants from the Na-
tional Science Foundation. Funds from the Department of Anthropology, University of New Mexico, aided in
analysis and the preparation of the manuscript. Special thanks are due to Dana Anderson and Tim Seaman,
who prepared the illustrations. Lisa Edelhoff translated the text from my unique writing form to finished
typescript. Johnny Rulland of Anaktuvuk guided me through this work. Jean-Philippe Rigaud, Peggy Schneider,
and Patty Marchiando helped with the original mapping tasks in 1971. Advice on this manuscript was offered
by J. Sabloff and L. Straus, and for this I am most grateful.

REFERENCES CITED

Bietti, A.
1977 Analysis and illustration of the Epigravettian Industry collected during the 1955 excavation at
Palidoro (Rome, Italy). Quoternaria 19:197-387.

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
BinfordI ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR AND SITE STRUCTURE 361

Binford, Lewis R.
1976 Forty-seven trips-A case study in the character of some formation processes of the
Archaeological Record. In Contributions to Anthropology: The Interior Peoples of Northern Alaska,
edited by Edwin S. Hall, Archaeological Survey of Canada Paper No. 49, pp. 299-351, National
Museum of Canada, Ottawa.
1978 Nunamiut ethnoarchaeology. Academic Press, New York.
Binford, Lewis R. and Jack B. Bertram
1977 Bone frequencies-and attritional processes. In For Theory Building in Archaeology, edited by L. R.
Binford, pp. 77-156. Academic Press, New York.
Binford, Lewis R. and Sally R. Binford
1966 A preliminary analysis of functional variability in the Mousterian of Levallois facies. American
Anthropologist 68:238-295.
Binford, Lewis R. and W. J. Chasko Jr.
1976 Nunamiut demographic history: a provocative case. In Demographic anthropology, edited by
Ezra B.W. Zubrow, pp. 63-143. University of New Mexico Press, Albuquerque.
DeLumley, Henry
1969 Une Cabane Acheul6ene Dan La Grotte Du Lazaret. Memoires De La Soci6te Pr6historique
Francaise, Tome 7, C.N.R.S., Paris.
Hempel, C. G.
1966 Philosophy of natural science. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs.
Jochim, Michael A.
1976 Hunter-gatherer subsistence and settlement. Academic Press, New York.
Leroi-Gourhan, Andre and Michael Brezillon
1966 L'habitation Magdalenienne N? 1 de Pincevent Pres Montereau. Gallia Pr6histofie, Tome IX,
Fascicule 2, C.N.R.S., Paris.
1972 Fouilles De Pincevent, Essai D'Analyse Ethnographique D'Un Habitat Magdalenien. C.N.R.S., Paris.
Poplin, Francois
1976 Les Grands Vertebres de Gonnersdorf, Fouilles 1968. Franz Steiner, Verlag GMGH, Weisbaden.
Schiffer, Michael B.
1976 Behavioral archaeology. Academic Press, New York.
Weick, Karl E.
1968 Systematic observational methods. Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2; Rev. Ed.), pp. 357-451.
Addison Wesley, Reading, Mass.
Whiting, Beatrice and John Whiting
1970 Methods for observing and recording behavior. Handbook of methods in cultural anthropology,
edited by Raoul Marell and Ronald Cohen, pp. 282-315. Natural History Press, Garden City, New York.
Yellen, John
1977 Archaeological approaches to the present. Academic Press, New York.

This content downloaded from 132.248.9.8 on Tue, 09 May 2017 17:29:36 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi