Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 8

 Articles

The Relationship Between Workaholism and


Marital Disaffection: Husbands’ Perspective
Bryan E. Robinson
Claudia Flowers
Kok-Mun Ng
University of North Carolina at Charlotte

Previous research suggests that wives of workaholics experience affected by workaholism and can develop a set of mental
greater marital estrangement and less positive feelings than do wives health problems of their own (Robinson, 1998d, 2001). The
of nonworkaholics. This study examined husbands’ perception of bulk of clinical data (Robinson, 1998b, 2000a, 2000b, 2000c)
marital disaffection and the relationship to their wives’ workaholic and empirical studies (Carroll & Robinson, 2000; Navarrette,
tendencies. A random sample of 272 male members of the American 1998; Robinson & Carroll, 1999; Robinson & Kelley, 1998)
Counseling Association were mailed a survey that examined their
have focused on adult children of workaholics. Generally,
spouses’ workaholic behavior, marital disaffection, locus of control,
and positive and negative affects. The results suggest that worka-
these findings suggest that adult children of workaholics
holism is positively related to martial disaffection (p < .01). The most have greater psychological problems and more health com-
important workaholism domains for predicting martial disaffection plaints than do adult children of nonworkaholics.
were overcontrolling behavior (r = .36) and impaired communica- Although little attention has been paid to the association
tion (r = .38). The results underscore the need for greater clinical between workaholism and marital relationship, popular press
awareness of marital problems associated with workaholic tendency. surveys (Herbst, 1996; Weeks, 1995) and clinical reports
(Carroll, 2001; Pietropinto, 1986; Robinson, 1998c) com-
Keywords: workaholism; marital disaffection; gender work issues;
pose the literature on spouses of workaholics. Only one
family functioning
study (Robinson, Carroll, & Flowers, 2001) in which female
spouses were directly assessed on their perspectives of

A lthough the literature is sparse, the research on the


psychological and familial correlates of workaholism
indicates that workaholism is associated with family dys-
living in an atmosphere of workaholism has been conducted.
This study compared a sample of wives of workaholics and
wives of nonworkaholics on marital disaffection, positive
function (Robinson, 2001). The term workaholism has numer- feelings toward husband, and locus of control. A random
ous definitions (see Robinson & Flowers, 2004, for a review). sample of 326 participants drawn from the membership list
It is defined in this article as a compulsive and progres- of the American Counseling Association was surveyed.
sive, potentially fatal disorder characterized by self-imposed Wives of workaholics reported significantly greater marital
demands, compulsive overworking, inability to regulate work disaffection and less positive affect toward husbands and
habits, and overindulgence in work to the exclusion and higher external locus of control than did wives of non-
detriment of intimate relationships and major life activities workaholics. Moreover, there was a statistically significant
(Robinson, 1998a; Robinson & Chase, 2001). difference between reports of wives of workaholics and wives
Workaholism and Family Functioning of nonworkaholics in the number of hours their husband
worked (i.e., an average of 9.5 more hours per week). These
The structural and dynamic characteristics of the worka- findings were the first empirical results to corroborate clinical
holic family indicate that all family members can be negatively and case study reports.
Clinical reports indicate that workaholism, like alco-
Authors’ Note: This research was supported in part by funds from
the University of North Carolina at Charlotte. Correspondence holism, takes its toll on other family members living with a
concerning this article should be addressed to Claudia Flowers, workaholic (Robinson, 2000a). No empirical research
Department of Educational Administration, Research, and has examined the relationship between workaholism and
Technology, University of North Carolina at Charlotte, Charlotte, marital disaffection from the husband’s perspective. The
NC 28223-0001; e-mail: CPFlower@email.uncc.edu. present study sought to address this deficit by examining the

THE FAMILY JOURNAL: COUNSELING AND THERAPY FOR COUPLES AND FAMILIES, Vol. 14 No. 3, July 2006 213-220
DOI: 10.1177/1066480706287269
© 2006 Sage Publications

213
214 THE FAMILY JOURNAL: COUNSELING AND THERAPY FOR COUPLES AND FAMILIES / July 2006

relationship of workaholism to the marital dyad utilizing were couples with other locus of control combinations, except
husbands’ perspective and presenting the results of the first those couples of internal husbands and external wives. Internal
empirical study on the subject. To parallel the previous study husbands married to external wives were more satisfied than
of the wives of workaholics, this study was delimited to were all external husbands regardless of who they married.
examining only male and female marital couples and not Internal wives married to external husbands were only more
same-sex couples. satisfied than were individuals in marriages with both external
spouses. Camp and Ganong’s findings indicated that an indi-
Personality and Marital Satisfaction vidual’s internality was more important than the partner’s
For decades, researchers have found associations between internal locus of control. Also, persons with an internal locus
personality variables and marital relationship (Caughlin, of control, in general, were more satisfied than were persons
Huston, & Houts, 2000; Doherty, 1981). Sabatelli (1986) with an external locus of control, regardless of gender or part-
noted that “the importance of these variables for the study of ner locus of control.
interpersonal relationships is reflected in their consistent Smolen and Spiegel’s (1987) findings support the view
influence on the way in which information from one’s social that, in husbands, internality of locus of control serves to
environment is perceived and processed and in turn influences buffer the effects of marital conflict on marital satisfaction.
people’s social orientations” (p. 939). Neurotism, agreeable- Robinson, Carroll, et al. (2001) found that women with
ness, conscientiousness, and positive expressivity were workaholic spouses reported greater marital disaffection and
found significantly related to marital satisfaction (Eysenck & greater external locus of control compared to women with
Wakefield, 1981; Gattis, Berns, Simpson, & Christensen, nonworkaholic spouses. However, there is a gap in the litera-
2004; Olsen, Martin, & Halverson, 1999; Watson, Hubbard, ture that specifically addresses the experience of men married
& Wiese, 2000). Among the personality variables, locus of to workaholic spouses.
control and affectivity have received a great amount of atten- Affectivity. Affectivity is an emotion-based trait dimen-
tion (e.g., Bugaighis, Schumm, Bollman, & Jurich, 1983; sion (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) that creates a dispo-
Camp & Ganong, 1997; Caughlin et al., 2000; Miller, sition to experience emotions influencing cognition and
Lefcourt, Holmes, Ware, & Saleh, 1986; Watson et al., 2000). self-concept. Affectivity is theorized to have two indepen-
Locus of control. Locus of control refers to the extent to dent dimensions: positive and negative. Positive affectivity
which people believe the outcomes of events in their lives are is the tendency to experience positive emotional states and
attributable to their own actions (Rotter, 1966). Individuals negative affectivity, negative ones. Persons with high nega-
with external locus of control orientations believe that ulti- tive affectivity tend to perceive themselves and the world
mate control of their lives rests outside of themselves, more negatively. They also tend to experience greater dis-
whereas those with internal locus of control believe that out- tress than do low-negative affectivity persons, even in the
comes of life events are a function of their own abilities, absence of stressors (Watson & Clark, 1984).
attributes, and skills. Researchers theorized that persons with Watson et al. (2000) found that negative affectivity and
an internal locus of control are most willing to invest time, positive affectivity were consistent predictors of relationship
energy, and effort necessary to keep their marital relation- satisfaction among married and dating men and women.
ships healthy, dynamic, and growing because they believe in Negative affectivity was negatively correlated to relation-
a great sense of personal control over the functioning of their ship satisfaction, whereas positive affectivity was positively
close relationships (Camp & Ganong, 1997). correlated to relationship satisfaction; the correlations
Camp and Ganong (1997) noted that findings in the appeared to be stronger among married women and men.
literature on the relationship between locus of control Davila, Bradbury, and Fincham (1998) also found that, for
orientation and marital satisfaction have been mixed. For both wives and husbands, negative affectivity was neg-
example, Bugaighis et al. (1983) found that the greater the atively correlated to marital satisfaction. Previous research
internal locus of control of the wife, the higher the marital sat- suggests that wives of workaholics had a much lower posi-
isfaction. But, Miller et al. (1986) reported that general locus tive feeling toward their spouse than did wives of non-
of control of individuals did not correlate with their marital sat- workaholics (Robinson, Carroll, et al., 2001). However,
isfaction; however, individuals’ marital locus of control was there is a lack of literature that examines the difference of
significantly related to their marital satisfaction in that inter- affectivity between men who have married workaholic and
nality was positively related to reported marital satisfaction. nonworkaholic spouses. Also, no study has addressed the
These findings were mainly based on studying individuals and relationships among husbands’ affectivity, their spouses’ work
not couples. Hence, Camp and Ganong studied the influence habits, and their marital satisfaction.
of husband-wife locus-of-control combinations on 137 cou-
Research Questions
ples who had been in long-term marriages. Their findings
showed that couples who both had internal locus of control This study examined the relationship of wives’ workaholism
were significantly more satisfied with their marriages than to marital disaffection, locus of control, and affectivity from
Robinson et al. / WORKAHOLISM AND MARITAL DISAFFECTION 215

the husbands’ perspective. The following research questions validity of scores from the WART, the scales demonstrated
guided this study: face validity: overdoing, self-worth, control or perfection-
ism, intimacy, and future reference or mental preoccupation
1. What relationships exist among workaholism, marital (Robinson & Post, 1994). Twenty psychotherapists criti-
disaffection, locus of control, positive affect, and negative cally examined the 25 WART items for content validity.
affect? They were instructed to identify the 25 items from a list of
2. Do the subscales of workaholism, compulsive tendency, 35 test items that most accurately measured workaholism
controlling behavior, impaired communication, inability
(Robinson & Phillips, 1995). The authors reported high align-
to delegate, and self-worth predict marital disaffection after
controlling for locus of control, positive affect, and nega-
ment of the items for the domain of workaholism. Concurrent
tive affect? validity was investigated by correlating the scores on the
WART with four scales on the Jenkins Activity Survey. The
correlation coefficients between the WART and four scales
METHOD
ranged from .20 to .50 (Robinson, 1996). Scores on the
Participants WART also correlated .40 with generalized anxiety on the
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Construct validity of scores
From the membership list of the American Counseling from the WART was examined by analyzing the underlying
Association, 1,000 were randomly selected. Contact with dimensions of the WART and investigating the accuracy of
the selected respondents was attempted through a series of the WART scores to discriminate between workaholics and
two waves of mailing. A total of 376 surveys were returned, a control group (Flowers & Robinson, 2002). The results
resulting in a 37.6% return rate. Respondents who were suggest that workaholism, as measured by the WART, is
never married or whose spouse did not work were excluded not a unidimensional construct. Five subscales were identi-
from the analyses, resulting in a total of 272 participants fied, and the WART scores produced an 88.5% correct clas-
included in all analyses. sification rate.
The male participants had an average age of 49.3 years
(SD = 9.5), ranging from 26 to 69 years. Respondents had Marital Disaffection Scale (MDS). The MDS (Kayser,
been married an average of 17.6 years (SD = 10.7), ranging 1996; Kersten, 1990) is a 21-item inventory that assesses the
from less than 1 year to 40 years. The respondents had an components of emotional estrangement in marriage. The scale
average of 19.6 (SD = 3.1, range 16-24) years of education. measures the loss of emotional attachment, decline in caring,
The ethnicity of the respondents was predominantly White and loss of desire for emotional intimacy with one’s partner.
(n = 244, 89.7%), followed by African American (n = 9, Respondents rate items on a 4-point scale ranging from not at
3.3%), Hispanic (n = 7, 2.6%), Asian (n = 3, 1.1%), and all true (1) to very true (4). Eleven reversal items were recoded
Other (n = 7, 2.6%). The spouses of respondents had an in the direction of higher scores meaning higher martial disaf-
average age of 46.8 (SD = 9.2) and had an average of 17.0 fection. Scores on the MDS can range from 21 to 84. A con-
(SD = 2.8) years of education. The spouses averaged 41.0 current validity study indicated that there was a statistically
(SD = 10.5, range 10-65) hours at work per week. significant positive relationship (r = .93, p < .001) between the
MDS and Snyder and Regts’ (1982) scale of disaffection.
Instruments Moreover, the MDS correlated inversely with general ques-
tions on martial happiness (r = –.56) and marital closeness
Work Addiction Risk Test (WART). The WART (Robinson,
(r = –.86). Internal reliability, as measured using Cronbach’s
1999) is a 25-item inventory that measures addictive work-
coefficient alpha, was .97. A significant correlation was also
ing patterns. Respondents rate items on a 4-point scale rang-
found between the respondents’ overall level of disaffection
ing from never true (1) to always true (4) according to how
and their participation in counseling (r = .61, p < .001).
well each item describes their work habits. Six scores can be
calculated from the WART, five subscale scores and an over- Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control Scale. The Nowicki-
all score. The five subscales are (a) compulsive tendencies, Strickland Locus of Control Scale (Nowicki & Strickland,
(b) control, (c) impaired communication, (d) inability to del- 1973) is considered to be the best general measure of inter-
egate, and (e) self-worth. The subscale scores are calculated nal or external locus of control from grade three through col-
by averaging the item values within each subscale. The over- lege. A total of 40 questions are answered either yes or no
all score is calculated by summing across all the items; the and the respondents’ perceptions are examined concerning
higher the score, the greater the level of workaholic behav- the control they believe they have over events in their lives.
iors. Several studies have examined the reliability of scores Items are scored in an external direction; thus, higher scores
from the WART. In one study, the test-retest reliability coef- indicate an external locus of control, and lower scores indi-
ficients was .85, and Cronbach’s alpha was .85 (Robinson, cate an internal locus of control. The scale was revised for
Post, & Khakee, 1992). In another study using a sample use with adults by deleting items about parents, leaving
of 442 respondents from diverse groups, the split-half relia- 26 questions. Split-half reliability for the scale was .81, and
bility was .85 (Robinson & Post, 1995). In assessing the test-retest reliability was .71 for high school students.
216 THE FAMILY JOURNAL: COUNSELING AND THERAPY FOR COUPLES AND FAMILIES / July 2006

TABLE 1
Coefficient Alphas, Correlation Coefficients, Means, and Standard Deviations for the WART, Marital
Disaffection, Locus of Control, Positive and Negative Affects, and Hours Worked per Week
WART Disaff LOC Positive Negative Hours Worked
WART .90**
Disaff .36** .68**
LOC –.03** .07** .40*
Positive –.08** –.10** –.02* .84**
Negative .13** .19** .12* –.19** .72**
Hours worked .18** .02** .01* .01** –.04** —a
M 49.63 41.71 5.07 3.83 1.82 41.04
SD 10.85 9.54 3.24 0.50 0.60 10.47

NOTE: Coefficient alphas are on the diagonal. WART is Work Addiction Risk Test. Disaff is marital disaffection. LOC is locus of control.
a. Only 1 item in the scale.
*p < .05. **p < .01.

Moreover, construct validity was investigated between the years of education. Respondents were asked to assess their
Nowicki-Strickland adult scales and the Rotter Scale and current or most recent spouse’s work habits (using the
was found to be significant in two studies with college WART). In addition, they were asked to rate their current or
students (r = .61, p < .01; r = .38, p < .01; Nowicki & most recent marriage in terms of emotional closeness toward
Strickland, 1973). spouse (using the MDS). Next the participants were asked to
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). Positive complete the Nowicki-Strickland Locus of Control Scale on
affect reflects the extent to which a person feels enthusiastic, themselves and the positive affect or negative affect as mea-
active, and alert. High positive affect scores indicate a state sured by the PANAS. A business reply envelope was pro-
of high energy, full concentration, and pleasurable engage- vided for return of the completed material. Two weeks after
ment, whereas low positive affect scores are characterized by the initial mailing, postcards were mailed to thank those par-
sadness and lethargy (Watson et al., 1988). In contrast, nega- ticipants who returned the questionnaire and remind those
tive affect is a general dimension of subjective distress and who had not returned the questionnaire to do so.
unpleasurable engagement that subsumes a variety of aver-
sive mood states, including anger, contempt, disgust, guilt, RESULTS
fear, and nervousness, with low negative affect being a state
of calmness and serenity (Watson et al., 1988). To examine the relationships among the variables of
Watson et al. (1988) developed the PANAS as a simple and workaholism, martial disaffection, locus of control, negative
short method of measuring positive affects and negative affect, and positive affect, correlational analyses were
affects. Respondents are asked to rate how they feel on aver- conducted. The correlation coefficients, coefficient alphas,
age on 20 descriptors, 10 positive and 10 negative. A 5-point means, and standard deviations for WART, martial disaffec-
rating scale was used to assess each descriptor: very slightly tion, locus of control, negative affect, positive affect, and
or not at all (1), a little (2), moderately (3), quite a bit (4), and spouse’s hours worked per week are reported in Table 1. The
very much (5). Watson et al. (1988) reported alpha reliability reliability coefficients (see diagonal of the matrix in Table 1)
coefficients ranging from .84 to .90 and test-retest reliability ranged from .40 (locus of control) to .90 (WART).
coefficients ranging from .39 to .71. The intercorrelation Workaholism was positively related to marital disaffection
between the two scales, positive affect and negative affect, (r = .38, p < .01), negative affect (r = .13, p < .05), and
ranged from –.12 to –.23. Principal factor analyses suggested number of hours worked per week (r = .18, p < .01). Negative
two large factors, which accounted for 75.4% of the common affect was positively related to marital disaffection (r = .19,
variance. Convergent correlations ranged from .76 to .92, and p < .01) and external locus of control (r = .12, p < .05) and
the discriminant coefficients had values close to zero. inversely related to positive affect (r = –.13, p < .01). No
other statistically significant relationships were found.
Procedures
The correlation coefficients for the relationships between
Each prospective respondent was mailed a letter contain- the subscales of workaholism and marital disaffection, locus
ing the general nature of the study with an invitation to par- of control, positive affects and negative affects, and hours
ticipate, a consent form, demographic form, and instrument worked per week, and coefficient alpha, means, and standard
package. The demographic form included items that asked the deviations for workaholism subscales are reported in Table 2.
respondents to identify their and their spouse’s year of birth, Coefficient alpha for the workaholism subscales ranged from
race, number of years with current spouse, and number of .53 to .85. All the workaholism subscales were positively
Robinson et al. / WORKAHOLISM AND MARITAL DISAFFECTION 217

TABLE 2
Correlation Coefficients Between the Work Addiction Risk Test (WART) Subscales
and Marital Disaffection, LOC, Positive, and Negative Affects, and Hours Worked
per Week and WART Subscales Coefficient Alpha, Means, and Standard Deviations
WART Subscales
Compul Control Impair Delegate Self-Worth
Disaff .19** .36** .38** .16** .15*
LOC –.09 < .00 .12* .02 –.04
Positive affect –.03 –.10 –.12* –.03 .09
Negative affect .08 .21** .23** –.03 .14*
Hours worked .23 .09** .06** .09 .13*
Coefficient α .84 .85 .62 —a .53
M 2.08 2.02 1.60 2.50 2.37
SD 0.56 0.55 0.47 0.66 0.60

NOTE: LOC is locus of control. Compul is compulsive tendencies. Impair is impaired communication. Delegate is inability to delegate.
a. Only 1 item in the scale.
*p < .05. **p < .01.

related to marital disaffection, ranging from .15 to .38. Locus significantly to the explanation of marital disaffection,
of control was related to impaired communication (r = .12, namely control and impaired communication, with semi-
p < .05); positive affect was inversely related to impaired com- partial correlations of .18 and .21, respectively.
munication (r = –.12, p < .05); negative affect was positively
related to control (r = .21, p < .01), impaired communication DISCUSSION
(r = .23, p < .01), and self-worth (r = .14, p < .01); and hours
worked per week was positively related to compulsive behav- The findings of the present study of husbands of worka-
ior (r = .23, p < .01) and self-worth (r = .13, p < .05). holics support previous research in which women who identi-
To examine the unique contribution of workaholism in the fied their husbands as workaholics also reported greater
explanation of marital disaffection, a hierarchical multiple marital disaffection and less positive feelings toward their
regression analysis was performed. Variables that explain spouse (Robinson, Carroll, et al., 2001). The correlational
marital disaffection were entered in two steps. In Step 1, findings in the present study between workaholism and mari-
marital disaffection was the criterion variable and (a) locus of tal disaffection, negative affect, and number of hours worked
control, (b) positive affect, and (c) negative affect were the per week suggest that the strength and cohesion of a marriage
predictor variables. In Step 2, the subscales of the WART is associated with the presence or absence of excessive work-
were entered into the Step 1 equation. Before the hierarchi- ing on the part of one’s spouse. Moreover, the two most
cal multiple regression analysis was performed, the predictor significant features of workaholism relating to marital dis-
variables were examined for collinearity. Results of the vari- affection were impaired communication and overcontrolling
ance inflation factor (all less than 2.0) and collinearity toler- behaviors, aspects of a marriage that can lead to problems in
ance (all greater than .76) suggest that the estimated βs are marital cohesion (Robinson, Flowers, et al., 2001). These
well established in the following regression model. findings corroborate what marital experts have contended for
The results of Step 1 indicated that the variance years: that control, as exemplified through struggles in
accounted for (R2) with the first three predictor variables marriage (Hendrix, 1988) and spousal withdrawal (Gottman,
(i.e., locus of control, positive affects, and negative affects) 1999), is an indicator of marital disintegration and separation.
equaled .03 (adjusted R2 = .02), which was significantly dif- There were differences between the findings from this
ferent from zero, F(3, 297) = 3.08, p < .05. Negative affect study and earlier research of wives of workaholics. First,
was the only statistically significant predictor variable, β = .13, wives of workaholics reported higher levels of external
p < .05. In Step 2, the five subscales of the WART were locus of control than did wives of nonworkaholics. In this
entered into the regression equation. The change in variance study, there was no relationship between workaholic tenden-
accounted for (∆R2) was equal to .17, resulting in the total cies of spouses and husbands’ external locus of control.
variance accounted for equal to .20, which was significantly This difference reflects the mixed findings regarding the
different from zero, F(8, 292) = 3.08, p < .05. The unstan- relationship between these two variables. Camp and Ganong
dardized regression coefficients (B) and intercept, the (1997) attributed the differences to possibly conceptual and
standardized regression coefficients (β), and semipartial measurement problems in this body of research. The low
correlations (sri) for the full model are reported in Table 3. to moderate coefficient alpha of the locus of control scores
Only two of the subscales of workaholism contributed in this study suggests that there was too much random
218 THE FAMILY JOURNAL: COUNSELING AND THERAPY FOR COUPLES AND FAMILIES / July 2006

TABLE 3
Unstandardized Regression Coefficients (B) and Intercept, the Standardized
β), Semipartial Correlations (sri), t Values, and p Values
Regression Coefficients (β
for Variables as Predictor of Marital Disaffection
Work Addiction Risk Test Subscales Zero-Order r
Compul Control Impair Delegate B β sr
LOC 0.08 .03 .03
Positive affect –0.72 –.04 –.04
Negative affect 1.18 .08 .07
Compulsive –1.52 –.09 –.07
Control .58** 4.15 .25** .18
Impair .45** .54** 5.28 .27** .21
Delegate .25** .21** .21** 1.28 .09 .09
Self-worth .38** .46** .28** .09** –0.88 –.06 –.05

Intercept 27.17
R2 .20

NOTE: LOC is locus of control. Compul is compulsive tendencies. Impair is impaired communication. Delegate is inability to delegate.
**p < .01.

variability in this measure for this population of respondents. relationship. This tendency is partly because of cultural
The development of a more accurate measure of locus of maxims that extol the positive features of workaholism
control for this population may make the measure more sen- while failing to cite the negative aspects (Robinson &
sitive to potential relationships with other variables. A more Chase, 2001). The results of this study underscore the criti-
context-specific locus of control measure may be more cal need for greater clinical awareness of marital problems
suited to help reveal the relationship between marital satis- associated with workaholism and the need to treat it as a
faction and locus of control (Miller et al., 1986). marital issue and not an individual problem. We recommend
Second, there was no relationship between positive affect that marriage and family therapists screen for the presences
and workaholism found in this study; neither was there a rela- of workaholism, just as they would alcoholism.
tionship between positive affect and marital disaffection, Expectations of change in workaholics require that family
as in the previous study examining wives of workaholics members who have built patterns of reactions to their loved
(Robinson, Carroll, et al., 2001). Negative affect was found one’s workaholism be prepared to change. Family practi-
positively correlated to workaholism and marital disaffection. tioners should be prepared for resistance on both sides.
However, findings in this study corroborate with the literature Complaints and cynicism about the workaholic’s constant
that shows that negative affect was a predictor of marital dis- working and family absence often become habitual. As the
affection (e.g., Davila et al., 1998; Watson et al., 2000). workaholic attempts to reconnect to family, some resistance
The findings presented here support a growing body of to integration may be present for some family members.
literature that suggests that the strength and cohesion of a
marriage is associated with the presence or absence of LIMITATIONS
workaholism (Carroll & Robinson, 2000; Robinson, Carroll,
et al., 2001; Robinson, Flowers, et al., 2001). Previous inves- It should be kept in mind that correlation does not imply
tigations indicate that workaholism, like alcoholism, may causation. The results from this correlational study, as with
take its toll on family functioning. It has been reported that any correlational study, do not ensure a causal relationship.
workaholics described a breakdown in their family’s func- Although correlational findings are necessary, it is not suffi-
tioning as the level of workaholism increased (Robinson & cient for a causal relationship. Research that examines
Post, 1995), and workaholics reported greater health prob- temporal sequencing of workaholic behaviors and marital
lems than did control groups (Spence & Robbins, 1992). disaffection would give greater insight into cause and effect
relationships.
IMPLICATIONS The information available on workaholism is based pre-
dominantly on self-report data, namely self-administered
Workaholism and marital relationships continues to be one questionnaires or face-to-face interviews with convenience
of the most ignored areas of clinical and empirical research. samples. These limited approaches, although useful for build-
Well-meaning clinicians compound the problem by minimizing ing a knowledge base, have led to a need for more sampling
the negative effects that workaholic behaviors have on a and methodological specificity. It is important that future
Robinson et al. / WORKAHOLISM AND MARITAL DISAFFECTION 219

research studies be well planned and include randomized, Gattis, K. S., Berns, S., Simpson, L. E., & Christensen, A. (2004). Birds of
representative samples. A multimethod approach (e.g., a feather or strange birds? Ties among personality dimensions, similarity,
and marital quality. Journal of Family Psychology, 18, 564-574.
Gottman, Coan, Carrere, & Swanson, 1998) to data collec- Gottman, J. M. (1999). The seven principles for making marriage work.
tion in which observational techniques are used in conjunc- New York: Crown.
tion with the traditional self-report techniques will yield Gottman, J. M., Coan, J., Carrere, S., & Swanson, C. (1998). Predicting
more reliable information and lead to a better understanding marital happiness and stability from newlywed interactions. Journal of
of workaholic family functioning. Moreover, it is essential Marriage and the Family, 60(1), 5-22.
Hendrix, H. (1988). Getting the love you want: A guide for couples.
that future inquiries employ a systems-oriented approach and New York: Henry Holt.
assess perceptions and behaviors of spouses and children of Herbst, A. C. (1996, November). Married to the job. McCall’s Magazine,
workaholics. pp. 130-134.
Because of the return rate (37%), there is potential for Kayser, K. (1996). The Marital Disaffection Scale: An inventory for assess-
sampling bias. Because the survey was anonymous, there was ing emotional estrangement in marriage. The American Journal of
Family Therapy, 24, 83-88.
no attempt to contact nonrespondents; therefore, it was not Kersten, K. K. (1990). The process of marital disaffection: Interventions at
possible to evaluate differences between respondents and various stages. Family Relations, 39, 257-265.
nonrespondents. Furthermore, the sampling frame used for Miller, P. C., Lefcourt, H. M., Holmes, J. G., Ware, E. E., & Saleh, W. E.
this study, males in the American Counseling Association, (1986). Marital locus of control and marital problem solving. Journal of
represents a highly educated group of males. The findings of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(1), 161-169.
Navarrette, S. (1998). An empirical study of adult children of worka-
this study may not represent those for less-educated males. holics: Psychological functioning and intergenerational transmis-
Also, the findings may not represent minority groups because sion. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, California Graduate Institute,
the sample was predominantly White. Given these limitations, Westwood.
the findings in this study may not generalize to other socio- Nowicki, S., & Strickland, B. (1973). A locus of control scale for children.
economic or racial backgrounds or to same-sex partners. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 40, 148-154.
Olsen, S. R., Martin, P., & Halverson, C. F. (1999). Personality, marital
It is critical that researchers examining family dynamics relationship, and parenting in two generations of mothers. International
give more attention to the subject of workaholism to achieve Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 457-476.
a better understanding of this condition. The findings Pietropinto, A. (1986). The workaholic spouse. Medical Aspects of Human
presented here are sufficient evidence to warrant further Sexuality, 20, 89-96.
research on workaholism and its impact on marital adjust- Robinson, B. E. (1996). Concurrent validity of the Work Addiction Risk Test
as a measure of workaholism. Psychological Reports, 79, 1313-1314.
ment factors to determine more clearly the risks and conse- Robinson, B. E. (1998a). Chained to the desk: A guidebook for worka-
quences of spousal workaholic habits. holics, their partners and children, and the clinicians who treat them.
New York: New York University Press.
Robinson, B. E. (1998b). Children of workaholics: What practitioners need
REFERENCES to know. Journal of the Child and Youth Care, 12, 75-85.
Robinson, B. E. (1998c). Spouses of workaholics: Clinical implications for
Bugaighis, M. A., Schumm, W. R., Bollman, S. R., & Jurich, A. P. (1983). psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 35, 260-268.
Locus of control and marital satisfaction. The Journal of Psychology, Robinson, B. E. (1998d). The workaholic family: A clinical perspective.
114, 275-279. The American Journal of Family Therapy, 26, 63-73.
Camp, P. L., & Ganong, L. H. (1997). Locus of control and marital satis- Robinson, B. E. (1999). The Work Addiction Risk Test: Development of
faction in long-term marriages. Families in Society, 78, 624-631. a tentative measure of workaholism. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
Carroll, J. J. (2001). Spouses of high-performing men: A profile of marital 88, 199-210.
adjustment and psychological outcomes. In B. E. Robinson & N. Chase Robinson, B. E. (2000a). Adult children of workaholics: Clinical and
(Eds.), High performing families: Causes, consequences, and clinical solu- empirical research with implications for family therapists. Journal of
tions (pp. 23-40). Washington, DC: American Counseling Association. Family Psychotherapy, 11, 15-26.
Carroll, J. J., & Robinson, B. E. (2000). Depression and parentification Robinson, B. E. (2000b). A typology of workaholism with implications for
among adults as related to parental workaholism and alcoholism. The counselors. Journal of Addictions and Offender Counseling, 21, 34-48.
Family Journal, 8, 360-367. Robinson, B. E. (2000c). Workaholism: Bridging the gap between work-
Caughlin, J. P., Huston, T. L., & Houts, R. M. (2000). How does personal- place, sociocultural, and family research. Journal of Employment
ity matter in marriage? An examination of trait anxiety, interpersonal Counseling, 37, 31-46.
negativity, and marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Robinson, B. E. (2001). Workaholism and family functioning: A profile of
Psychology, 78, 326-336. familial relationships, psychological outcomes, and research considera-
Davila, J., Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. (1998). Negative affectivity as a tions. Contemporary Family Research, 23, 123-135.
mediator of the association between adult attachment and marital satis- Robinson, B. E., & Carroll, J. (1999). Assessing the offspring of worka-
faction. Personal Relationships, 5, 467-484. holics: The children of workaholics screening test. Perceptual and
Doherty, W. J. (1981). Locus of control differences and marital dissatisfac- Motor Skills, 88, 1127-1134.
tion. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 43, 369-377. Robinson, B. E., Carroll, J. J., & Flowers, C. (2001). Marital estrangement,
Eysenck, H. J., & Wakefield, J. A. (1981). Psychological factors as predictors positive affect, and locus of control among spouses of workaholics and
of marital satisfaction. Advances in Behavior Research and Therapy, spouses of nonworkaholics: A national study. The American Journal of
3, 151-192. Family Therapy, 29, 397-410.
Flowers, C. P., & Robinson, B. (2002). A structural and discriminant analy- Robinson, B. E., & Chase, N. (2001). High-performing families: Causes,
sis of the Work Addiction Risk Test. Educational and Psychological consequences, and clinical solutions. Washington, DC: American
Measurement, 62, 517-526. Counseling Association.
220 THE FAMILY JOURNAL: COUNSELING AND THERAPY FOR COUPLES AND FAMILIES / July 2006

Robinson, B. E., & Flowers, C. (2004). Symptoms of workaholism: Pop Snyder, D. K., & Regts, J. M. (1982). Factor scales for assessing marital
psychology or private pain? In R. H. Coombs (Ed), Handbook of Addic- disharmony and disaffection. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
tive Disorders. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Psychology, 50, 736-743.
Robinson, B. E., Flowers, C., & Carroll, J. (2001). Work stress and mar- Spence, J. T., & Robbins, A. S. (1992). Workaholism: Definition, measure-
riage: A theoretical model examining the relationship between worka- ment, and preliminary results. Journal of Personality Assessment, 58(1),
holism and marital cohesion. International Journal of Stress Management, 160-178.
8(2), 165-175. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: The disposition
Robinson, B. E., & Kelley, L. (1998). Adult children of workaholics: Self- to experience negative emotional states. Psychological Bulletin, 96,
concept, locus of control, anxiety, and depression. The American Journal 465-490.
of Family Therapy, 26, 223-238. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation
Robinson, B. E., & Phillips, B. (1995). Measuring workaholism: Content of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales.
validity of the Work Addiction Risk Test. Psychological Reports, 77, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063-1070.
657-658. Watson, D., Hubbard, B., & Wiese, D. (2000). General traits of personality
Robinson, B. E., & Post, P. (1994). Validity of the Work Addition Risk Test. and affectivity as predictors of satisfaction in intimate relationships:
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78, 337-338. Evidence from self- and partner-ratings. Journal of Personality,
Robinson, B. E., & Post, P. (1995). Split-half reliability of the Work 68, 413-449.
Addiction Risk Test: Development of a measure of workaholism. Weeks, D. (1995, June). Cooling off your office affair. NorthWest Airlines
Psychological Reports, 76, 1226. World Traveler Magazine, pp. 59-62.
Robinson, B. E., Post, P., & Khakee, J. F. (1992). Test-retest reliability of
the Work Addiction Risk Test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 74, 926.
Rotter, J. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external con- Bryan E. Robinson is professor emeritus at the University of North
trol of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80, 609. Carolina at Charlotte.
Sabatelli, R. M. (1986). Locus of control, locus of control differences, and
quality of relationship in married dyads. Psychological Reports, 58, Claudia Flowers is an associate professor in educational research
939-945. at the University of North Carolina at Charlotte.
Smolen, R., & Spiegel, D. (1987). Marital locus of control as a modifier of
the relationship between the frequency of provocation by spouse and Kok-Mun Ng is an assistant professor in the Department of
marital satisfaction. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 70-80. Counseling at the University of North Carolina at Charlotte.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi