Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 16

Mother-Child Play: Sequential Interactions and

the Relation between Maternal Beliefs and


Behaviors

Amy Melstein Damast


Albert Ehistein College of Medicine

Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda
New York University

Marc H. Bornstein
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development

DAMAST, AMY MELSTEIX. T.\Mrs-LEMoxDA, CATHERINE S., and BOHNSTEIX, MARC H . Mother-Child
Play: Sequential Interactions and the Relation between Maternal Beliefs and Rehaviors. CHILD
DEVELOPMENT, 1996, 67, 17.52-1766. This investigation of mother and toddler play had 2 goals.
The primary goal was to examine the t>'pes of play mothers introduce in direct response to their
tpddlers' play. A secondary' and exploratory goal was to examine the relation between maternal
knowledge about child play and actual maternal play behaviors. 50 mothers and their 21-month-
old toddlers were observed at home duimg free play. Mother and child explorator>', nonsymbolic,
and symbolic play were coded. Sequential analyses revealed that mothers adjusted their play to
their children's play level by responding to their children with play that was either at the same
level or at a higher level than their children's play. Furthermore, mothers who were more
knowledgeable about early play development more often responded to their children's play by
introducing higher level play. These flndings suggest that mothers tend to play with their tod-
dlers in ways that might promote their child's development, and that mothers with more knowl-
edge about play development provide their children with appropriately challenging play interac-
tions.

In Western cultures, play often occurs dren's play? Second, if mothers do respond
in a social setting (Haight & Miller, 1993). differentially to their children's play, does
In the child's first few years, when new cog- mothers' knowledge about early play devel-
nitive and social skills are developing, a opment relate to mothers' actual play with
more knowledgeable social partner, like their children?
mother, is thought to facilitate the develop-
ment of more sophisticated play. Two re- In the first year, children learn about ob-
lated questions about maternal play behav- jects m their environment through explora-
iors were investigated in the present study, tion—they mouth, look at, and manipulate
First, are mothers sensitive to their chil- them (e.g., Belsky & Most, 1981; Bornstein
dren's play abilities, adjusting their own & Lamb, 1992; Fein, 1981; Nicolich, 1977;
play as a function ofthe level of their chil- Piaget, 1962). Near the end ofthe first year.

This article is based on a dissertation submitted by the flrst author m partial fulfillment of
the requirements for the doctoral degree in the Department of Psycholog\' at New York Univer-
sity. A.M.D. was supported by an N.Y.U. Predoctoral Fellowship; by research grants HD20559,
HD20807, and MH48915, by an IKTA Fellowship from the National Institute of Child Health
and Human Development, and by post-doctoral fellowship grant T32HD07384 C T.-L was
supported by research grants HD20559. HD20807, and MH48915. M.H.B. was supported by
research grants HD20559 and HD20807. and by a Research Career Development Award
HD00521 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. A.M.D. thanks
the members of her dissertation committee, D. Ruble, D. Frye, H. Oster, and J. Daws, for
their helpful comments and suggestions. We also thank L. Baumwell, L. Cyphers, H. Comes, J.
Jankowski, R. Kahana-Kalman, S. Kazas, J. McClure, S. Rose, H. Ruff, and C Schmidt for their
assistance. Requests for reprints should be addressed to Amy Melstem Damast or Catherine
Tamis-LeMonda, Department of Applied Psychology', New York Universitv, 239 Creene Street-
5th Floor, New York, NY 10003.
[Child Development. 1996.67,1752-1766. © 1996 by the Society- for Research in Child Development, Inc.
All rights resented. 6o09-3920/96/6704-0026S01 00]
Damast, Tamis-LeMonda, and Bornstein 1753

children begin to engage in nonsymbolic first and second years (Belsky, Goode, &
play as they examine the unique qualities Most, 1980), as well as greater nonsymbolic
and functions of objects (e.g., pushing but- play (and less simple exploration) during
tons on toy phones, stacking blocks, nesting the toddler years (Fiese, 1990). Maternal
cups). During the second year, play more of- involvement and turn-taking behaviors are
ten takes on a nonliteral "as if" quality, as negatively associated with simple explora-
children incorporate difFerent forms of sym- tion during toddlerhood (Fiese, 1990). In
bolic play into their repertoires (see Belsky addition, Tamis-LeMonda and Bornstein
& Most, 1981; Bornstein & O'Reilly, 1993; (1991) found that mothers' own play behav-
Fein, 1981; Fenson, Kagan, Kearsley, & Zel- iors (i.e., gestural and verbal solicitations
azo, 1976; Fenson & Ramsev, 1980; and demonstrations of play activities) with
McCune-Nicolich, 1981; Nicohch, 1977; their children relate to their children's play
Schaefer, Gitlin, & Sandgrund, 1991; Tamis- behaviors: Mothers who engage in more
LeMotida & Bornstein, 1995; Tamis- symbolic play with their 13- and 20-month-
LeMonda, Damast, & Bornstein, 1994; Un- oids have toddlers who engage in more sym-
gerer, Zelazo, Kearsley, & O'Leary, 1981; bolic play at these ages.
Watson & Fischer, 1977).
The literature on maternal respon-
\'\'hiile children's play increases in so- siveness and child cognitive development
phistication with age, at any one age chil- implies that contingency in maternal inter-
dren exhibit different levels of play in differ- active behaviors might also be central to
ent social contexts (e.g., Fiese, 1990; children's play behaviors (e.g., Bakeman,
O'Connell & Bretherton, 1984). One possi- Adamson, Brown, & Eldridge, 1989; Baum-
ble explanation for this obsen'ation can be well. Tamis-LeMonda, & Bornstein, 1996;
found in Vygotsky's theory of the zone of Beckwith & Cohen, 1989; Bornstein, 1989;
proximal development Vygotsky (1978) sug- Bornstein & Tamis-LeMonda, 1989; Born-
gested that children may function between stein, Tamis-LeMonda, et al., 1992; Olson,
two levels of development—their actual de- Bates, & Bayles, 1984; Olson, Bayles, &
velopmental level and their potential devel- Bates, 1986). This literature demonstrates
opmental level (i.e., the level at which they that it is not merely the Frequency of positive
can function iii collaboration with a more ex- maternal interactive behaviors that Facili-
perienced partner). Vygotsky and others tates children's development, but rather
(e.g., RogofF, 1990; RogofF & Wertsch, 1984; both the contingency (i.e., the temporal and
Wertsch, 1984, 1985; Zukow, 1986) suggest sequential relation) and the appropriateness
that more advanced partners structure inter- (i.e., the content, sophistication, or topic) oF
actions that exceed children's actual level mothers' behaviors in the context oF the on-
and approach children's potential level, going interaction. In other words, maternal
thereby helping to advance children's actual behaviors have a positive influence on chil-
level of development. For example, O'Con- dren's development when those behaviors
nell and Bretherton (1984) demonstrated are both appropriate to and contingent on
increases in children's play diversity (a children's behaviors.
measure fqund to relate to later play
sophistication and intelligence) during col-
labora;tive play with mother as compared to With respect to the appropriateness oF
solitary play. Similarly, Fiese (1990) showed maternal play behaviors, developmental
tl^at toddlers spent a greater percentage of studies suggest tliat the level and the Fre-
their time in nonsjTnbolic and symbolic quency oF mothers' play relate to their chil-
play, and a lower percentage of their time in dren's age and ability (Belsky et al., 1980;
exploratory piay, during mother-child play Brooks-Gunn & Lewis, 1984^ Fiese, 1990;
than during play alone. Tamis-LeMonda & Bornstein, 1991). As a
group, mothers tend to suggest more sophis-
O'Connell and Bretherton {1984) re- ticated play to children when their children
ported, that it is not simply a mother's pres- are older and more capable oF this type oF
ence, but her active participation, that ac- play (Tamis-LeMonda & Bornstein, 1991).
counts for differences in children's play In addition, mothers tend to support their
across social contexts. Maternal physical at- children's autonomy by offering fewer play
tention-focusing behaviors during interac- suggestions as children grow older (Belsky
tion have been found to predict greater ex- et al., 1980; Fiese, 1990). Thus, it appears
ploratory competence (i.e., a weighted that mothers are generally sensitive to their
composite measure of exploration, nonsym- children's play abilities in that they change
bolic play, and symbolic play) during the their behaviors appropriately with child age.
1754 Child Development
Little is known, however, about the con- eventful perinatal histories and were
tingency (i.e., responsiveness) of maternal healthy and free of any known develop-
play to children's play. Are mothers ad- mental delays during the course ofthe study.
justing their play responsively to the indi- They were all from middle- and upper-
vidual play behaviors of their children (i.e., middle-class intact families (M = 57.7, SD
on an episode-by-episode basis)? The first = 7.6, on the Hollingshead Four Factor In-
goal of this investigation was to address this dex of Social Status, 1975; Gottfried, 1985),
Qiiestion. Our expectation was that mothers with 90% of mothers hiiving completed 4 or
woiild respond appropriately and contin- more years of college. At the 21-month visit,
gently to their children's play on an episode- 45 of the 50 were only children, one had a.
by-episode basis. lO-month-old sibling, and four had a sibling
younger than 4months.
Within the context of group sensitivity
to child play behaviors, we expected to find Play at Home
variability in the extent to which individual Procedures.—Dyads were visited m
mothers adjust their play to their children's their homjas at a time convenient to mother
play. The second, exploratory goal ofthis in- and child and when only the two would be
vestigation was, therefore, to examine present. Data from a 10-min mother-child
whether rnothers' ability to respond sensi- free-play session, part ofa larger home visit,
tively on an episode-by-episode basis to are the focus ofthe present report.^ Mother
their children's play is, at least in part, ex- and child were filmed while playing with a
plained by their knowledge ofthe progres- standard set of toys that included cups,
sive nature of children's play. It has been plates, spoons, teapot and cover, doll, baby
suggested that mothers' knowledge about blanket, baby battle, bus with removable
eertain developing abilities in children re- peopie, sponge, telephone, blocks, and a set
lates to their behaviors with their children of nesti:ng cups. The toy set represented ob-
(e,g., Goodnow, 1988; Goodnow & Collins, jects With whieh all children were familiar
1990; Hunt & Paraskevopoulos. lOSO; Ko- in their everyday routines and which lend
phanska, 198|0; McGltlicuddy-DeLisi, 1982; tiiemselves to a variety of age-appropriate
Miller, 198S'. Sigel, McGiliicuddy-DeLisi, & actMties. Each mother was instructed to sit
Goodnow, 1992; Tamis-LeMonda et al., with her child and behave as she normally
1!J94). In a comprehensive review of paren- would when she and her child played. In
tal knowledge of development. Miller (1988) additioii, mothers were asked to ignore the
note^ that parents are gene .rally accurate in expenmenter''s presence to the extent possi-
their knowledge of developmental mile- ble. Mothers were told that they could use
stones, and tliat variatioi:i among parents in any or all of the toys provided, but that the
the aepuraey of their knowledge is pre- chiM's own ttoys should not be included in
dictive (albeit modestly) of their behaviors die play session.
wifh their children. Accordingly, we as-
sessed the a^ccuracy of mothers' knowledge Coding.—Mother play and child play
ofthe relative difficultj,' of'vrarious chilcl play were fioded from Tiddeotapes using an object-
acts and asked whether this knowledige ac- centered, event-sampling procedure. Both
pounts for variation among mothers in their partaers' play was coded using; the mutually
responsive play behaviors. eiclusive codes of exploration, nonsymbolic
play, and symbolic play; during times when
chiiireri were not engaged in one of these
Method three types of play for 2 or more seconds,
theywere assigned a code af off-task. Mater-
Participants nal play could be verbal (e.g., saying "drink
Fifty mothers (M age = 33 years, SD = some tea"), gestural (e.g., pretending to pour
3.4) and their firstborn, 21-month-oId chil- from a teapot into a cup and handing the cup
dren (Mage = 651 days, SD = 8.5; 26 males, to tlae qhild to drink), or a combination ofthe
24 females) participated in this study. Fami- (eig., saying "let's drink tea" and pre-
lies were recruited from a pediatric unit in l to (feink from a teaeup). The onset
a large urban university hospital and from i occurred with the start ofa
Ioeal obstebic groups. All children had un-

1 The entire home visit consisted of four videotaped sessions occumng in a standardized
order- 10 min ofthe child playing alone with a standard set of toys, 10 mm of mother-child tree
play with another set of toys, 10-15 min of experimenter-child play using a third set of toys, and
45 min of naturalistic interaction (during which mdtfeers were free to do whatever they pleased
with or without their childien).
Damast, Tamis-LeMonda, and Bornstein 1755
mother's vocalization (e.g., "Call daddy on and mothers' play separately: percent agree-
the phone") in the case of verbal or verbal- ment was 84% for both child and mother
gestural play, or with the start of her gesture play events. For play acts on which both
(e.g., handing an object to her child, pointing coders agreed, reliabilities were calculated
to an object) or demonstration (e.g., pushing for the level of child play (i.e., for all ehild
buttons on a telephone) in the case of ges- acts, whether or not mother responded), the
tural play. Children's play was always based level of maternal play, and the level of the
on gestures accompanied by visual regard child act that preceded maternal play in the
and could occur with or without vocaliza- sequence (i.e., only child acts to which
tions. For example, if a child pretended to mother responded). Cohen's kappa was used
drink from a teacup, the child's action was as a conservative measure of reliability
coded as a single, self-directed symbolic act, (Bakeman & Gottman, 1986). Kappas were
whether or not the child said "drink tea." good to excellent for each code: K = .67 for
Children's play was coded from the onset of child play level (range = .62 to .76), K = .77
the child's regard. Children's voadizations for maternal play level (range = .58 to .95),
in the absence of gestures were not coded and K. = .66 for preceding child level (range
as play to avoid confounding symbolic play = .54 to .73).
ability with language ability.
Maternal Knowledge of Play
For both mother and child, play events Mothers' knowledge of child play was
terminated when the player looked at an- assessed using a questionnaire mailed to
other object, engaged in a thematically unre- them 1 week prior to the home visit and col-
lated action vvith the same object, or tumed lected by the experimenter at the comple-
attention from the toys altogether for a pe- tion of the visit. The play activities on the
riod of 2 or more seconds. As mentioned, questionnaire represented one example of
when a child directed attention away from each ofthe levels of a hypothesized 24-level
the toys for 2 or more seconds, the behavior play scale (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 1994; Ap-
was coded as off-task. pendix A). This 24-level play scale was de-
Based on the above mother and child veloped by integrating previousfindingsex-
play codes, sequences of play acts were ob- tant in the literature in order to create a more
tained by assigning each maternal play act a comprehensive description of early develop-
code indicating the child's activity' prior to mental progressions in play (see Belsky &
or at the onset of the act. For example, if Most, 1981; Fenson et al., 1976; Fenson &
mother s^id "talk on the phone" after the Ramsey, 1980; McCune-Nicolich, 1981; Ni-
child manipuiated the phone, the mother's colich, 1977; Tamis-LeMonda & Bornstein,
play was coded as symbolic play preceded 1991; Ungerer et al., 1981; Watson &
by child exploration. Likewise, if mother put Fischer, 1977). Seven important transitions
the lid on the teapot while the child was were incorporated into the scale: (1) the
"drinldng" from a teacup, the motlier's play transition from simple exploration to non-
was coded as nonsymbolic play preceded by symbolic play; (2) the transition from non-
a child symbolic act.^ Due to the sequential symbolic play to sy-mbolic play; (3) the tran-
nature of interaction, coders made one pass sition from seif-directed pretense to
thrpugh the videotapes, coding both mother other-directed pretense; (4) the transition
and toddler play togettier. Coders were en- from animate-directed pretense to inani-
couraged to stop and replay segments of in- mate-directed pretense; (5) the transition
teraction to maximize coding accuracy. from single acts of pretense to sequenced
acts of pretense; (6) the transition from
Interrater agreement.—Interrater reli- agentive pretense to vicarious pretense; and
abilities were obtained by having two coders (7) the transitionfromliteral pretense to sub-
independently score ail mother and child stitutive pretense. Moreover, there is some
play behaviors on 10% ofthe sampie for the consensus that eertain transitions occur be-
entire 10-min session. A.s a first step to as- fore others; for example, the transition from
sessing interrater agreement, we examined self-directed to other-directed play is ex-
how often coders agreed on the identifica- pected to occur before the transition from
tion of an event, calculated for children's literal to substitutive play, on account ofthe

This code system does not imply that the child is always the "initiator" and the mother
always the "respondent" in the interaction. Sequences provide no information conceming which
partner established the topic focus; instead, they describe only how a mother responded to a
particular behavior on the part ofthe child.
1756 Child Development
more complex cognitive demands of substi- play and specific patterns of maternal re-
tutive play (i.e., substituting one object for sponsive play were related to one another.
another requires decontextualization from
the object, whereas other-directed play does For each set of analyses, dyads that did
not). Additionally, the scale incorporates the not have complete data on all pertinent vari-
notion that combinations of components of ables were not included. For example, in
play are more difficult to master than are sin- ANOVAs investigating matenial nonsym-
gle components of play. For exainpie, pre- bolic play, mothers who did not engage in
tending to feed a doH with a block (Le., a nonsymbolic play were necessarily ex-
combination of pretense toward an inani- cluded. As a consequence, reported results
mate otiier and substitutive pretense) is are based on samples of 42 to 50 dyads. Prior
jmore difficult than pretending to feed a doll to analysis, data were examined for extreme
with a toy bottle (i.e., a single act of pretense bivariate outliers using scatterplots. Cook's
toward an inanimate other), D, Stud.enti2ed Deleted Residual, and Le-
verage statistics. Four dya:ds whose individ-
preliminary' analyses support the hy- ual data altered the significance ofthe corre-
pothesized ordering of these 24 levels of lations (either positively or negatively)
play (Tamis-LeMonda, Albright, Damast, between maternal knowledge about play
Fox, & Bornstein, 1995). In a sample of 108 arid ma^ternal play behaviors were excluded
21-month-old cMtdren (including the 50 from tfte correlation analyses (Judd &
children in the present study) playing alone MeClelland, 1989). Finally, because the
with the same standard set of toys, the pres- AMO'teAs reVealed no consistent differences
ence or absence of each ofthe 24 play levels in the base rates of maternal and child play
waS coded. The play levels were then rank- by chiid gerider, and there were no gender
ordered based ofli Uie percentage of children differetiees in any ofthe sequential analyses,
engaging in each level. The linearity ofthe results are presented for the entire sample.
hypothesized scale was tested by comparing
these rank-orderings with the ordering in Descriptive Analyses of Base Rates of
the proposed scale using Kendairs tau. The Mother and Child Play
tau was signifiGant, tau = .72, p < .001, indi- The mean frequencies and proportions
CE^ting substaiitial agreement between the (number of maternal play acts at each level
hypothesized play scale and the order ob- of play divided by the total number of mater-
served during toddlers' solitary play. nal plaj' acts, calculated separately for each
mpthelr) of tlie three levels pf maternal play
Examples ofthe 2,4 play levels were pre- are presented in Table 1. A within-subjects
seiited in random order on the question- rejaeatfed-measures ANOVA identified sig-
naire, (The labels of the play levels that ap- nificMt differences in the base rates (i.e.,
pear in Appendix A were not provided.) frequebiCies) of the differerit play levels, F(2,
Mothers were asfed to rarik the 24 different 98) = lCP.94,p < .001. Specifically, mothers
play activities iix order of increasing diffi- pidtflptfid more symbolic play than either
cillti,', wfith 1 being the easiest and 24 the expiorkton or nonsymbolic play, is(49) =
most difficult Xi.64 aitd 10.67, respectively, ps < .001.
They showed no difference in their rates of
esplofation and nonsynabolic play, f(49) <
Results I. (Ptfesnits for all descriptive analyses were
Results are discussed in the following identicsil when transfortned proportions
order. f;irst, base rates of the three levels of were used as the dependent measure.)
maternal play, the four levels of child play,
and the rates with which these child levels The mean frequencies and proportions
precedecl maternal play are presented and (nurnt>er of child play acts- at each play level
analvzed using analyses of variance. Second, diyi&d by the total rmmber of child play
analyses of the sequences of mother and acts, paJeuiated separately for each child) of
child piay behaviors are reported. The goal the three childi play levels and off-task epi-
of these analyses was to describe dependen- sodes are also presented in Table 1. These
cies of maternal play on preceding child rates rgpi-esent the tdtal number of child acts
play. Finally, relations between maternal diujdg Ilie play session (iiot just the subset
knowledge of child play development and of chiilcl acts to which mcfjhars responded). A
observed maternal play are reported. For within-subjects repeated-fioeasures ANOVA
these analyses, measures of the accuracy of e differences in rates, F(3, 147) =
materiial knowledge of progressions in child , p < .001^ Child(eii engaged in explo-
Damast, Tamis-LeMonda, and Bornstein 1757
TABLE 1
BASE R \ T E S OF MATERNAL PL.4IY AND C H I L D ACTIVITIES BY LEVEL

Level of Play Mean Range SD


Mother:
Exploratory 9.5 (.20) 0-24 (.00-.46) 5.0 (.10)
Nons>'mbolic 8.8 (.18) 0-27 (.00-.52) 6.6 (.12)
Symbolic 30.8 (.63) 4-63 (.14-.93) 12.7 (.16)
Child:
Explorator\' 35 9 (.43) 13-51 (.24-.59) 8.4 (.08)
Nonsymbolic 11.8 (.14) 0-30 f.00-.28) 7.4 (.08)
Symbolic 18.2 (.22) 5-43 (.07-.40) 7.5 (.08)
OfF-task 18 2 (.22) 10-.32 (.11-.35) 5.4 ( 05)
NOTE —Proportions in parentheses.

ration more often than they were off-task or levels of child play, F(3, 147) = 42.14, p <
engaged in nonsymbolic or symbolic play, .001. Specifically, greater numbers of mater-
ts(49) = 13.50, 17.89, and 11.60, ps < .001, nal play responses were offered when chil-
respectively. (This may occur because chil- dren were either off-task or exploring than
dren often orient to and explore objects be- when they were engaged in either nonsym-
fore engaging in nonsymbolic and symbolic bolic or symbolic play, ^(49) = 9.67, p <
play.) In addition, children were off-task or .001. Additionally, mothers responded more
engaged in symbolic play more frequently frequently when children were engaged in
than they engaged in nonsymbolic play, exploration than when they were off-task or
ts(49) = 5.04 and 3.93, ps < .001, respec- engaged in nonsymbolic or symbolic play,
tively. There was no difference in the rates ts{49) = 4.12, 11.17, and 6.05, respectively,
at which children were off-task or engaged ps < .001. Mothers also responded more fre-
in symbolic play, f(49) < 1. quently when children were off-task than
when they were eagaged in nonsymbolic or
The mean frequencies and proportions symbolic play, ts{49) = 8.30 and 2.72, ps <
(number of maternal piay acts in response to .001 and .01, respectively. Lastly, mothers
each level of child play divided by the total responded more frequently when children
number of maternal play acts, calculated were engaged in symbolic play than when
separately for each dyad) of mothers' play they were engaged in nonsymbolic play,
responses to each level of child play are pre- t(49) = 3.95, p < 001.
sented in Table 2. Maternal play responses
were collapsed across level of maternal play The Sequential Nature of Mother-Child
in order to question what mothers were re- Play Interactions
sponding to in their children's play, exclu- Mother-child play sequences were ana-
sive of how they were responding. A within- lyzed by first calculating transitional proba-
subjects repeated-measures ANOVA bilities for all combinations of the three lev-
indicated a difference in the number of els of maternal play (i.e., exploration,
times mothers responded to the different nonsymbolic play, symbolic play) and four

TABLE 2
M.\TEHNAL PLAY BY LEVEL OF PRECEDING CHILD PL^Y ACT

Level of Preceding
Child Play Act Mean Range SD
Off-task" . 12.6 (.29) 3-28 (.12-.53) 5.5 (.10)
Exploratory 17 0 (.38) .3-39 (.15-.61) 7.7 (.10)
Nonsymbolic 56 (.12) 0-16 (.00-33) 4.4 (.08)
Symbolic 9.7 (.22) 0-25 (.00-..50) 6.1 (.12)
NOTE —Proportions in parentheses
" OfF-task episodes iire included as child play acts because they, like
child play acts, represent children's attention at the time mothers choose
to o£fer play suggestions.
1758 Chad Development
levels of preceding child play (i.e., off-taslc, X 4 (preceding child play level) within-
exploration, norisymbolic play, symbolic subjects repeated-measures ANOVA to com-
play), using the formulas and procedures de- pare the difference from expected probabil-
tailed by Bakeman and Gottman (1986). ity' ofthe differen.t sequences. This ANOVA
These transitional probabilities were then indicated ^ significant interaction of mater-
converted to z scores, and the z scores were nal play level x preceding child play level,
used as dependent variables in subsequent F(6, 258) = 33.73, p < .001. These results
analyses. Z scores control for the base rates are presented in Table 4.
of both the "given" and "target" behaviors.
Table 3 provides descriptive data of the fre- Figure 1 displays z scores for each child
quencies of the 12 child play—mother play play-mather plag sequence. When compar-
sequences. ing the four maternal exploratory z scores to
cbaiice, maternal exploratory play was pre-
Z scores were used to examine differ- ceded by child ofF-task episodes signifi-
ences between t t e observed rates ofthe se- cantly more often than would be expected
queiices and their expected rates given the by chance, f(49) = 8.43, p < .001, and it was
observed base rate probabilities of the indi- preceded by child exploration, nonsymbolic
vidual "given" and "target" events (i.e., play, and symbolic play less often than
Bakeman & Gottman's, 1986, "first-order" would be expected by chance, <s(49) =3.28,
mode! for generating expected probabili- 2.22, and 5.72, ps < .01, .05, and .001, re-
ties). For exampie, a large positive z score spectively. Consequently, when comparing
associated with a particular sequence would the foiir maternal exploratory play s scores to
sign!ffy a greater occurrence of that sequence each otfeer, child off-task episodes increased
than would be expected by chance given the the likelihood of maternal exploratory piay
base rates of both the child level an^ the sigiiificaatly more than did child explora-
mother play level, a large negative z score tion, iioiisj'rnbolic play, and symbolic play,
assoiiiated with the same sequence would *(48| = 7.m,t{44) = 6.60, and *(47) = 8.64,
indicate a lower probability ofthis sequence respectively, ps < .001.
than vvould be expected by chance.
When comparing the four maternal non-
J h e dependency of mother play on symbolic play z scores to chance, maternal
child play was examined in two ways. First, nonsymbolic play was preceded by child
using one-sample t tests (witli the mean z nonsyiabolic play significantly more often
scoris for each sequence as the dependent than would be expected by chance, f(49) =
variable), the probability of each child play-^ 6.43. p < -OOl, by ehild symbolic play sig-
mother play sequence was compared with nificantly less often than expected by
its expected probability (i.e., chance).^ For chance, t(49) = 8.89, p < .001, and by off-
example, the probability' of maternal sym- task episodes and exploration at rates ex-
bolic play subsequent to a ehild engaging in pected by chance, ts(49) =5= .85 and 1.50, N.S.
exploration, adjusted for the probabilities of Consequently, when comparing the four ma-
both child exploration and materrial sym- ternal nonsymbolic play z scores to each
bolic play, was cpnjpflretd With the simple other, child nonsymbolic play increased the
probability of maternal syrtibolic play (i.e., likelihood of maternal nonsymbolic play sig-
the probability of matem^ synibolic play oc- nificantly more than did child off-task epi-
curring by chance). Thus, Hie effect of each sodes., esploration, and symbolic play, ts(44)
child play level on the probability of each = aiiT, 4.SO, and 3.86, respectively, ps <
maternal play level was determined. In ad- .001.
dition, for each level of maternal play, tlie
probabilities of the four child play-mother When comparing the four maternal sym-
piay sequences were also ccjHipared to one bolic play z scores to chance, maternal sym-
another. For example/the lifcelihood of ma- bolic play was preceded by child symbolic
ternal symbolic play given ehiid exploration play significantlv more often than would be
was compared to the likelihood of maternal exp'ected by chance, *(49) = 9.62, p < .001,
symboiic^play given child ndnsy-mbolic play. by off-task episodes and nonsymbolic play
To do so, z scores for all aftbie sequences of significantly less often than would be ex-
maternal play and clillci preceding level pected by chance, *st49) = 6.26 and 4.37,
were entered into a 3 (maternal piay level) ps < .001, respectively, and by child explo-

^ Comparisons to chance can also be made by pooling the data across dyads, and calculating
transitional probabiUties and their associated z scores from the pooled data. Both approaches
provided essentially the same results.
Damast, Tamis-LeMonda, and Bornstein 1759
TABLE 3
MATERNAL PLAY BY LEVEL AXD BY LEVEL OF PRECEDING
CHILD PLAY

Level of Maternal Play and


Level of Preceding Child Play Mean Range SD
Maternal exploratory play;
Child off-task ' 4.4 0-13 3.1
Child exploration 2.8 0-8 2.3
Child nonsjnnbolie play 0.8 0-.5 1.2
Child symbolic play 0.8 0-4 1.1
Maternal nonsymbolic play.
Child off-task 2.1 0-7 17
Child exploration 3.3 0-16 34
Child nonsymbolic play 2.2 0-9 2.5
Child symbolic play 0.6 0-3 0.8
Maternal symbolic play:
Child off-task 6.2 1-16 3.6
Child exploration 10.6 2-23 .5.2
Child Bonsymbolic play 2.6 0-10 2.5
Child symbolic play 83 0-24 5.6

TABLE 4 ration at a rate expected by chance, f(49) =


DiFFEREXCES IN Z SCORES RY M A T E F X A L 1.32, N.S. Gonsequently, when comparing
LEVEL AXD BY PRECEDING CHILD LEVEL the four maternal symbolic play z scores to
each other, child s\TTibolic play increased
Maternal Play Level the likelihood of maternal symbolic play sig-
and Comparison of nificantly more than did child off-task epi-
Preceding Child Level n* t p sodes, exploration, and nonsymbolic play,
t(48) = 9.71, t{4o) = 9.57, and t(48) = 6.31,
Exploratory: respectively, ps < .001.
O-T vs. EXP 49 7.30 .001
O-T vs. NS 45 6.60 .001 In summary, sequential analyses re-
O-T vs. SYM 48 8.64 .001 vealed noteworthy patterns with respect to
EXP vs. NS 45 -0.49 both increases and decreases in the likeli-
EXP vs. SYM 48 180 .078 hood of certain levels of maternal play given
NS vs. SYM 45 2.97 .005
Nonsymbolic: certain levels of child play. When children
O-T vs. EXP 49 -1.42 were off-task or engaged in exploration, the
O-Tvs. NS 45 -4.80 .001 greatest increase in probability (although
O-T vs. SYM 48 4.35 .001 not always a significant increase above the
EXPvs. NS 45 -3.86 .001 rate expected by chance) occurred at a ma-
EXP vs. SYM 48 5.92 .001 ternal play level one level higher than chil-
NS vs. SYM 45 9.37 .001 dren's play level (i.e., mothers suggested ex-
SjTnbolic: ploration when children were off-task, and
O-T vs. EXP 50 -4.95 .001 nonsymbolic plav when children were ex-
O-Tvs. NS 46 -1.45 ploring). In contrast, when children engaged
O-T vs. SYM 49 -9.71 .001
EXP vs. NS 46 4.23 .001 in nonsymbolic and symbolic play, the great-
EXP vs. SYM 49 -6.31 .001 est increase in probability occurred at a ma-
NS vs. SYM 46 -9.57 .001 ternal play level that matched the preceding
child activity (i.e., mothers suggested non-
NOTE —O-T off-task; EXP. exploraticn, NS: non- symbolic play when children engaged in
symbolic play, SYM- symbolic play nonsymbolic pla}/, and symbolic play when
^ n for each analysis vanes as a result of tbe absence
of certain sequences in lndiwdual dyads children engaged in symbolic play). Mater-
nal play that was either lower or much
higher (i.e., three levels higher) than chil-
dren's ongoing play decreased in likelihood.
Exploratory analyses were conducted
next to determine whether mothers were re-
sponding to their children's nonsymbolic
1760 Child Development
z~score
1.50 r

1.00

0.50

0.00

-0.50

-1.00

-1.50
Exploratory Nonsymboiic Symbolic
Maternal Play Level

^B Child Off-task Child Exploratory


i -I Chiid Nonsymbolic Child Symbolic

• p < 06, " p < 01, " • p < 001


FIG 1.—Maternal play level by child play level

and symbolic play with play that was lower of these three categories of response (i.e.,
than, equal to, or higher than their children's lower than, equal to, or higher than the pre-
preceding play act within the nons^inbolic ceding child play level), within nonsymbohc
and symbolic play categories (see Appendix and symbolic play separately, were summed
A). Specifically, for each maternal nonsym- and used in subsequent analyses.
bolic play act that followed a child nonsym-
bolic play act, and for each maternal sym- Within-subjects repeated-measures
bolic play act that followed a child symbolic ANOVAs were used to identify differences
play act, both maternal play and preceding in the frequencies of lower, equal, or higher
child play were coded using the levels ofthe maternal nonsymbolic and symbolic play be-
24-level play scale. Mothers' play was then haviors separately. For both maternal non-
recoded as being lower than, equal to, or symbolic and symbolic play, there were sig-
higher than the level of the preceding child nificant main effects, F(2, 98) = 3.61, p <
play. (Interrater reliabilities, using the cod- .05, and F(2, 98) = 34.50, p < .001, respec-
ings of the same five dyads as above, were tively. Mothers were more likely to suggest
again good to excellent: K = 1.00 for non- nonsymbolic or symbolic play at levels
symbolic levels and K = .63 for symbolic equal to or higher than the level of their chil-
levels.) For example, if a child stirred with dren's preceding nonsymbolic or symbolic
a toy spoon in a cup and the mother stirred play, rather than lower than the preceding
with another spoon in another cup, she was level of child play, ts(49) = 2.44 and 2.25,
matching the child's overall level of sym- ps < .05, and ts(49) = 6.95 and 8.97, ps <
bolic play as well as the more specific sym- .001, respectively. There were no significant
bolic act of self-directed single act pretense. differences in the rates at which mothers
On the other hand, if a child stirred with a suggested nonsymbolic or symbolic play at
toy spoon in a cup and the mother suggested the same level as their children's preceding
that the child "feed the dolly," the mother nonsymbolic or symbolic play or at a higher
was matching the child's overall level of level than the preceding child play level,
symbolic play, but vvas prompting the child ts{4Q) < 1.
to a higher level within symbolic play by Relations between Maternal Knowledge
suggesting that the child include an inani- about Play and Maternal Play
mate other in a sequence of two different Maternal play summary measure.—To
pretense schemes. The frequencies of each examine relations between maternal knowl-
Damast, Tamis-LeMonda, and Bornstein 1761
edge of play and maternal play, a siagle mea- sequenced acts of pretense, literal pretense
sure of maternal play was created. The goal as easier than substitutive pretense, and
was to create a measure of play to index agentive pretense as easier than vicarious
mothers' attempts to "scaffold" their chil- pretense. (All p values for z scores ranged
dren's play (i.e., attempts to help tlieir chil- from .05 to .001.)
dren reach their potential level of perfor-
mance). For each dyad, the mean of the A measure of the overall accuracy of
transitional probabilities of seven play se- each mother's rankings was generated by
quences was calculated; these sequences correlating her ranking of the 24 play acts
are represented in Appendix B.* Sequences with the hypothesized play scale. As a
were selected to represent instances in group, individual mothers' rankings of these
which mothers responded with play at levels acts correlated highly with the empirical
that were higher than the level of the pre- scale, mean r = ,69, SD = .16. There was,
ceding child activity. Thus, higher values on however, substantial individual variation in
this measure indicate a greater tendency of these correlations, with scores ranging from
mothers to engage in more advanced levels .09 to .91.
of play relative to their children's own play. Correlation between maternal knowl-
The theoretical basis of this measure was edge about play and. maternal play sum-
Kuhn's (1972) finding that children are moti- mary measure.— To examine the relation
vated to perform at advanced stages (in Pia- between maternal knowledge of play and
getian tasks) when they witnessed behaviors observed maternal play, mothers' accuracy
performed at stages higher than Ilieir own in ranking play was correlated with the ma-
stage, As a group, mothers offered 45% (SD ternal play summary measure. Mothers
= 7%) of their play responses at levels whose rankings of play corresponded more
higher than the preceding child play level. closely with the hypothesized scale re-
sponded to their children's play with play
Accuracy of maternal knowledge of that was higher in level than the preceding
play progressions.—Because the task of child play level, n41) = .33, p < .05. In addi-
ranking 24 different child play acts might be tion, although four of the seven sequences
difficult for mothers, the first set of analyses included in this score involved mothers'
investigated how accurate mothers, as a suggesting symbolic play to their children,
group, were in their knowledge of early play maternal knowledge of play development
development (see also Tamis-LeMonda et was unrelated to mothers' overall frequency
al., 1994). Correlation analyses revealed of suggesting symbolic play, r(41) = .10,
strong agreement between both ithe mean N.S. Thus, maternal knowledge about play
and modal maternal rankings ofthe play acts is related to the specific coordination of the
and the hypothesized play scale, rs = .88 for levels of mothers' and children's play, rather
both analyses. Further analyses explored the than mothers' overall level of play.
accuracy of mothers' knowledge ofthe tran-
sitions in play overviewed above. Specifi-
cally, the percentage of mothers ranking the Discussion
more difficult play act as higher than the less The present study investigated the level
difficult act (e.g., the percentage of mothers and contingency of maternal play on chil-
ranking other-directed pretense higher than dren's play, as well as the possibility that the
self-directed pretense) was compared to accuracy of mothers' rankings of the diffi-
chance using z scores. These analyses again culty of child play acts accounts, in part, for
revealed that mothers are accurate in their the content of mothers' play. Results indi-
knowledge of play development. Mothers cated that mothers are sensitive to the level
ranked exploratory play as easier ilian non- of their children's play, adjusting the level
symbolic play, and nonsymbolic play as eas- of their own play to their children's play in
ier th^n symbolic play. Moreover, within both a macroanalytic manner (i.e., consider-
symbolic play, mothers ranked pretending ing base rate frequencies and unconditional
toward self as easier than pretending toward probabilities) and a microanalytic manner
Other, single acts of pretense as easier than (i.e., considering episode-by-episode se-

Transitional probabilities were used, rather than frequencies or unconditional probabili-


ties, because they control for tlie base rate of the child act. This was considered necessary
because certain levels of toddler play force mothers' prompts to he higher than child play. For
example, when children are off-+ask, any maternal prompt is necessarily higher than children's
play.
1762 Ghild Development
quences and transitional prohabilities). In Data from the present study advance our un-
addition, mothers who are more knowledge- derstanding ofthis dynam^ic by demonstrat-
able about play development tend to suggest ing that mothers also adjust the frequency of
play to their children at a more sophisticated their suggestions on an episode-by-episode
level than their children's ongoing play. basis at any one age. That is, they provide
more play suggestions when their children
evaluating the generalizability of are engaged in "less cornpetent" (i.e., off-
these findings, it may be important to con- task and simple exploratory) behaviors, and
sider charact:eristics of the sample particular they provide fewer play suggestions when
to this investiga^tion. Specifically, the moth- their children are engaged in "more compe-
ers who participated in this study were tent" (i.e., nonsymbolic and symbolic play)
somewhat older, more educated, and of a behaviors.
higher socioeconomic background. Maternal
knowledge of and sensitivity to play may dif- From a microanalytic perspective,
fer in younger mothers, less educated moth- mothers appear to adjust the level of their
ers, arid mothers from other socioeconomic play contingent on their children's play,
or cultural backgrounds. thereby functioning withiri their children's
"?:One of proximal deyelopment" (ZPD; Ro-
From a macroanalytie perspective, goff & Wertsch, 1984; Vygotsky, 1978).
mothers are sensitive to the level of sophisti- When children are engaged in less sophisti-
cation of their ehildren's play acflvities, and cated activities, mothers are likely to re-
they adjust the frequency and the level of spond with play at one level higher than
their play responses based on the level of their children's level. Mothers attempt to fo-
their children's play. Across the play session cus their children's attention to the toys
as a whole^ motliers suggest play to their \¥hen their children are oJEf-task, and (though
children most frequently at the overall level not significantly above chance levels) to en-
of their children's play; both toddlers and courage their children to engage in nonsym-
motliers engage in more symbolic play than bolie play when they are involved in simple
noasj'mbolic play. In addition, mothers offer objed manipulation. Thus, at these times
significantly more play resporises when their mothtirs appear to use their play to "chan-
children are engaged in less sophisticated nel" their children's behavior toward focus
activities (i.e., when off-task or engaged in on toys and more sophisticated levels of jalay
px|pibration). In contrast, when their chil- (Hodapp, Goldfield, & Boyatzis, 1^84;
dren are engaged in aiore sophisticated ac- TroutiTian, Hazen, & Cook, 1992). When
tivities (i.e., in norisyriibolic or symbolic children are engaged in more sophisticated
play), mothers offer fewer play suggestions. play, mothers are more likely to match their
oVvn play to the level of their children's play,
These findings concur with other evi- or to engage in higher levels of play within
dence of parental sensitivity to develop- the broader categories of nonsymbolic and
mental level and parental support of auton- sjTuboIic play. This strategj^^ enables moth-
omy in ehildren. As described by Macceby ers to elaborate on their children's behaviors
(1984), parents generally appear to be sensi- at the same level.
tiye to the developmental abilities of their
ciiiidren, adjusting their own actions, over Decreases in the likelihood of certain
time, to accommodiafe their children's maternal play behaviors in response to cer-
emerging a(bij:it;ies. Maccoby suggested that tain toddler behaviors also support the no-
paretafaJ nijenitoring of ehildren's nidment- tion that mothers generally function within
by-moment; behaviors is firequent during in-^ their children's "zone of proximal develop-
fancy and tfjdidlerhodd/ Specific exi3«nE>les of ment " Decreases in the likelihood of mater-
parents' ad|iistiHg their behavior across time nal exploiatory play when preceded by child
are (Siyident: in a variety of parentirigd0m:ains nonsvmbolie and child symbolic play, and
(Bornstein. Tal, et al., 1992; lEeckhausen, decreases in the likelihood of maternal non-
1987; O'Cdnneti & Brethertbii, }Mk Pawjnt symbolic play when preceded by child sym-
Moss GoBselin:; & St;-ta«.rtent: 1993; logofi; bolic play, indicate that mothers tend wot to
Ellis, & Gardner, i§M; Tamis-LeiMonda & respoiid to their children's play with play
Bornstein, 199J). For instance, mothers tend that is lower in level (i.e., below their chil-
to show decreases iin particular interactions dren's ZPD). The same pattem holds true
and increases in others as their children when ea;a.minin.g trends within nonsymbolic
grow dlder^attd imore co!rapeteirt(e.f., Belsky and within symbolic play; mothers tend to
et al.. I980;'fieirnstein & Taniis-LeMonda, make suggestions that are equal or higher in
level, rtot lower in level, than their chil-
199b; Rogoff, Malkin, & Gilbride, 1984).
Damast, Tamis-LeMonda, and Bornstein 1763
dren's play. A decrease in the likelihood of likely to respond to their children's play
maternal exploratory play when preceded by- with play that is more sophisticated. The ef^
child exploration suggests that motlbers tend feet size of this correlation is modest and
not to orient their children to a toy when comparable to relations between parental
their children are already examining it, or knowledge and behavior in other domains
shift their children's focus from one toy to reported in the literature (Miller, 1988). On
another. In addition, although mothers en- a macroanalytic level, no relation emerged
gaged in more symbolic play than nonsym- between maternal knowledge about play
bolic play across the play session, they did and overall frequency of maternal symbolic
not tend to suggest symbolic play in re- play. Thus, mothers who are more knowl-
sponse to their children's nonsymbolic play edgeable about early play development re-
or off-task behaviors. Overall, these results spond sensitively on an episode-by-episode
might suggest that mothers' play behaviors basis, rather than simply offering more sym-
with their children serve to scaHbld their bolic play suggestions across the entire ses-
children's play on an episode-by-episode ba- sion, irrespective of tiieir children's current
sis and across the entire play session. Fur- play level. These differences in sensitivity
thermore, the data suggest that children's between macroanalytic and microanalytic
dyadic play behaviors guide their mothers' analyses of knowledge and scaffolding be-
participation in play interactions; mothers haviors reinforce the value of considering
appear to use their children's play cues (e.g., the sequential nature of interactions (see
object focus, actions) as a framework for their also Lewis & Lee-Painter, 1974; Tingley &
own play behaviors with their children. Golden, 1992).
Beyond the empirical contribution, the Research investigating parental cogni-
present investigation expands the extant tions has tended to explore relations be-
play literature methodologically. The use of tween cognitions and developmental out-
sequential analyses allowed for a more de- comes in children (Miller, 1988); these
tailed examination of an important aspect of relations have frequently been examined
sensitive maternal interactive behavior— with little regard to the parental behaviors
contingency. It is evident that motliers' sen- that might mediate them, although the need
sitivity to their children's developmental for such data has been discussed (Hunt &
level is both global (i.e., aeross an entire play Paraskevopoulos, 1980; Kindermann & Skin-
session) and specific (i.e., on an episode-by- ner, 1989). Furthermore, parental cognitions
episode basis). It also became evident, are ofiten investigated in relation to reports
through these analyses, that matches be- of parental behaviors (either self-reports or
tween the levels of children's and mothers' e.xperimenter-completed reports) rather than
play observed in the literature (e.g., Tamis- to direct observations of parental interac-
LeMonda & Bornstein, 1991) are at least par- tions (Sigel et al., 1992, Stevens, 1984). Fi-
tially detennined by mothers' contingent, nally, when parental knowledge about de-
sensitive responding. velopment is related to parenting behavior,
researchers have typically assessed (1)
Analyses of mothers' knowledge of play knowledge of general milestones and devel-
revealed that mothers, as a group, are knowl- opment (e.g., the KIDI, MacPhee, 1981; the
edgeable about development in eaily play. KEID, Stevens, 1984) and/or (2) more gen-
Nonetheless, interesting and in.Formative eral parenting stj'les £tnd practices (e.g., au-
variation among mothers does exist. From a thoritative/authoritarian parenting; Kochan-
microanalytic perspective, mothers' knowl- ska, 1990). By relating parental cognitions in
edge ofthe progressive nature of play relates the domain of play to observed parental be-
to episode-by-episode adjustments in their haviors in the same domain, the present
play with their children. Mothers who are study takes a step toward elucidating speci-
more knpwledgeable about the relative dif- Rcity in the interplay betvveen maternal cog-
ficult>' of various toddler play acts are more nitions and actual mother-child interactions.
1764 Ghild Development
Appendix A
Empirical Play Scale

Level Action on Mothers' Questionnaire


Exploration:
(1) Mouthing (1) Suck block.
(2) Simple rtlanipulatidn (2) Hold spoon and look at it.
Nonsymbolic play:
(3) Unitary functional (3) Turn wheel on toy car.
(4) Inappropriate ci^mbinations (4) Put toy dish on car.
(o) Combinatioiis based on perception (5) Stack toy plates.
(6) Combinations based on function (6) Put toy lid on teapot.
Symbolic play:
(7) Self (7) Feed self with toy spoon
(8) Agentive animate (8) Wash mom with toy sponge.
(9) Agentive inanimate (9) Rock doll.
(10) Sequenced self (10) Stir in toy cup and eat from toy spoon.
(11) Sequenced agentive animate (11) Pour into toy cup from toy teapot and feed
inom.
(12) Sequenced agentive inanimate (12) Cover doll with blanket and pat to sleep.
(13) Vicarious (13) Make; doll wave "hi."
(14) Self substitution (14) Use block as sponge and wash own face.
(15) Agentive animate substitution (15) Put toy plate on mom's head as hat.
(16) Agentive inanimate substitution (16) Use spoon as brush and brush doll's hair.
(17) Sequenced vicarious (17) Make stuffed bear walk to toy car and
drive away.
(18) Sequenced self substitution (IS) Stir in toy pot with comb as spoon and eat
from comb.
(19) Sequenced agentive animate substitution (19) Feed mom with block and wipe her mouth
with t()y sponge.
(20) Sequenced agentive inanimate substi- (20) Wash doll with block as sponge and dry
Itution vvith towel.
(21) Vicarious substitution (21) Jifake toy person drive away in nesting cup
as car.
(22) Sequenced vicarious substitution (22) Fut toy bib on doll as coat and make her
walk,
(23) Self-removed (23) Make one doll kiss another doll.
(24) Emotive (24) Make doll fall down and cry.

Appendix B References
Sequences in Maternal Play Summary Score Bakeman, R., Adamson, L. B., Brown, J. V., & El-
dridge, M. E. (1989). Can early interaction
Mother Suggests When Child Is predict? How and how much? In M. H.
Bornstein & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Stability
(1) Exploration (1) Off-task and continuity in mental developjnent: Be-
(2) Nonsymbolic (2) Off-task havioral and biological perspectives (pp.
play 235-248). New York: Erlbaum.
(3) Nonsymbolic (3) Exploring
play Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. M. (1986). Observing
(4) Symbolic play (4) OfF-task interaction: An introduction to sequential
(5) Symbolic play (5) Exploring analysis. New York: Cambridge University
(6) Symbolic play (6) Engaged in nonsym- Press.
bolic play Baumwell, L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Born-
(7) Symbolic play (7) Engaged in symbolic stein, M. H. (1996). Styles of maternal inter-
l action and toddler language comprehension.
' when a 21-month-old engaged m symbolic play, a The importance of maternal sensitivity. Manu-
mother who suggests symbolic play is suggesting play at script under review.
an appropriately sophisticated level given the age of the Beckwith, L., & Cohen, S. E. (1989). Maternal re-
child and is considered to be scafFoldmg her child's play iponsiveness with preterm infants and later
competency. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Mater-
nal responsiveness. Characteristics and con-
sequences (pp. 75-87). San Francisco.
Jossey-Bass.
Damast, Tamis-LeMonda, and Bornstein 1765
Belsky, J., Goode, M. K., & Most, R. K. (1980). Goodnow, J. J. (1988). Parents' ideas, actions, and
Maternal stimulation and infant explorator>' feelings: Models and methods from develop-
competence: Cross-sectional, correlabonal, mental and social psychology. ChildDevelop-
and experimental analyses. Child Develop- ment, 59, 296-320.
ment, 51, 1168-1178. Goodnow, J. J., & Collins, W. A. (1990). Develop-
Belsky, J., & Most, R. K. (1981). From exploration ment according to parents: The nature,
to play: A cross-sectional study of infant free sources, and consequences of parents' ideas.
play behavior. Developmental Psychology, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
17, 630-639. Gottfried, A W. (1985) Measures of socioeco-
Bornstein, M. H. (Ed). (1989). Maternal respon- nomic status in child development research:
siveness: Characteristics and consequences. Data and recommendations. Merrill-Palmer
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Quarterly, 31, 85-92.
Bornstein, M. H., & Lamb, M. E. (1992). Develop- Haight, W. L., & Miller, P. J. (1993). Pretending
ment in infancy: An introductiott (3d ed.). at home. Albany, NY: SUNY.
New York: McGraw-Hill. Heckhausen, J. (1987). Balancing for weaknesses
Bornstein, M. H., & O'Reilly, A. W. (Eds.). (1993). and challenging developmental potential. A
The role of play in the development of longitudinal study of mother-infant dyads m
thought. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. apprenticeship interactions. Developmental
Bornstein, M. H., Tal, J., Rahn, C , Calperin, Psychology, 23, 762-770.
C. "Z.f Pecheux, M.-G., Lamour, M., Azuma, Hodapp, R. M., Goldfleld, E. C & Boyatzis, C. J.
H., Toda, S., Ogino, M., & Tamis-LeMonda, (1984). The use and effectiveness of maternal
C. S. (1992). Functional analysis of the con- scaffolding in mother-infant games. Child De-
tents of maternal speech to infants of 5 and 13 velopment, 55, 772-781.
months in four cultures: Argentina, France, Hollmgshead, A. B. (1975). The Four-Factor In-
Japan, and the United States. Developm.ental dex of Social Status Unpublished manu-
Psychology, 28, 593-603. script, Yale University.
Bornstein, M. H., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1989). Hunt, J. McV., & Paraskevopoulos, J. (1980). Chil-
Maternal responsiveness and cognitive devel- dren's psychological development as a func-
opment in children. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), tion ofthe inaccuracy of their mother's knowl-
Maternal responsiveness: Characteristics and edge of their Eibilities. Journal of Genetic
con.sequences (pp. 49-61). San Francisco: Jos- Psychology, 136, 285-298.
sey-Bass. Judd, C. M., & McClelland, G. H. (1989). Data
Bornstein, M. H., & Tamis-LeMonda, C S. (1990). analysis: A model-comparison approach (pp
Activities and interactions of mothers and 214-237). New York. Harcourt Brace Jova-
their children in the flrst six months of life: novich.
Covariation, stability, continuity', correspon- Kindermann, T., & Skinner, E. (1989). Mothers'
dence, and prediction. Child Development. perceptions of children's progress in develop-
61, 1206-1217. mental ta.sks: Organizers of contingencies in
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Tal, J., everyday interactions. Paper presented at the
Ludemann^ P., Toda, S., Rahn, C. W., Pe- meeting of the Societ\' for Research m Child
cheux, M., Azuma, H., & Vardi, D. (1992). Ma- Development, E[ansas City, MO.
temal responsiveness to infants in three soci- Kochanska, G. (1990). Maternal beliefs as long-
eties: The United States, France^, and Japan. term predictors of mother-child interaction
Child Development, 63, 808-821. and report. Child Developmsnt, 61, 1934-
Brooks-Gunn, J., & Lewis, M. (1984). Maternal re- 1943.
sponsivity in interactions with handicapped Kuhn, D. (1972). Mechanisms of change in the de-
infants. Child Development, 55, 782-793. velopment of cognitive structures. Child De-
Fein, G. G. (1981). Pretend play In childhood: An velopment, 43, 833-844.
integrative review. Child Development 52 Lewis, M., & Lee-Painter, S. (1974). An interac-
1095-1118. tional approach to the mother-infant dyad. In
Fenson, L , Kagan, J., Kearsley, R. B., & Zelazo, M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The ef-
p. R. (1976). The developmental progression fect ofthe mfant on its caregiver (pp. 21-48).
of manipulative play in the flrst two years. New York: Wiley.
Child Development, 47, 232-236. Maccoby, E. E. (1984) Socialization and devel-
Fenspa, L., & Ramsey, D. S. (1980). Decentration opmental change. Child Development, 55,
and integration of the child's play in the sec- 317-328.
ond year. Child Development, 51^ 171-178. MacPhee, D. (1981), Manual: Knowledge of In-
Fiese, B. H. (1990). Playful relationships: A con- fant Development Inventory. Unpublished
textual analysis of mother-toddler interaction manuscript. University of North Carolina.
and symbolic play. Child Development, 61, McCune-Nicolich, L. (1981). Toward symbolic
1648-1656. functioning: Structure of early pretend games
1766 Child Development
and potential parallels w4th language. ChiM Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Albright, M. B., Damast,
Development, 52, 785-797. A. M., Fox, T., & Bornstein, M. H. (1995,
McGillicuddy-DeLisi, A. V. (1982). Parental be- March). A longitudinal investigation offirst-
liefs about developmental processes. Human and second-year play: Efforts toward val-
Development, 25, 19^-200. idating a 24-level scale. Poster presented at
Miller, S. A. (1988). Parents* beliefs aboiit their the biennial meeting of the Society for Re-
children's cognitive development. Child De- seareh in Child Development, Indianapolis.
velopment, 59, 259-285. TamisieMonda, C. S., & Bdmstein, M. H. (1991).
Nicolich, L. M. (1977). Beyond sensori-motor in- Individual %'arlation, correspondence, stabil-
telligence: Assessment of symbolic maturity itj' and change in mother and toddler play.
through analysis of pretend play. Merrill- Infant Behavior and Development, 14,
Palmer Quarterly, 23, 89-99. 143-162.
O'Gonnell, B., & Bretherton, I, (1984). Toddler's Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bomstein, M. H. (1995).
play, alone and with motber: The role of Variation in children's exploration, nonsym-
piatenial guidance. In I. Bretherton (Ed.), bolic. and symbolic play: An explanatory,
Symbolic play: The develcrpment of social irialii-dimensional framework. In C. Rovee-
unt^rstanding (jpp. 337-368). Orlando, FL. Collier fit L. Lipsett (Series Eds.), Advances
Academic Press. in infancy research (Vol. 10). Norwood, NJ:
Olson, S. L , Bates. J. E., & Bayles, K. (1984). AbJex.
Mother-infant interaction and tlie develop- Tamis-IjsMonda, C. S., Damast, A. M., &
jaent d£ individual differerices in children's Bomstein, M. H. (1994). What do mothers
cognitive competeBce. Developmental Psy- about the developmental nature of
chology, 20, 166-179. ? Infant Behavior and Development, 17,
Olsoiij, S. L., Bayles, E., & Bates, J. E. (1986). Mother- 341-^345.
chilcj inteiactioB and children's speech prog- Tingley, E. C , & Golden, R. M. (1992). Sequence
ress: A longitudinal study of the first two and structure ofstfmboUc play between well
years. MerrM-'PalmerQuarterhi, 32,1-20. and- depressed mothers and their toddlers.
Parent, S., Moss, E., Gosselin, C , & St.-Laurent, -Faiper presented at the Eighth International
D. 11993). Maternal adjustraent io developing Confeience on Infant Studies, Miami, FL.
child compet,encie.s: Is age all that matters? Troutmaa, K., Hazen, N., & Cook, W. (1992).
Paper presented at the meetliig ofthe Societj' Mafkers' scaffolding and guidance discourse:
for Research in Child Development, New Or- Effects of exploratory and proMem.>-solvhig
leans, LA. a€iivitie.s. Paper presented at the Eighth In-
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in ternational Conference on Infant Studies, Mi-
childhood. New York: W. W. Norton. ami, FL.
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking. Ungerei-, J. A., Zelazo, P. R., Kearsley, R. B., &
Cognitive development in social context. O'Leaiy, K. (1981). Developmental changes
New York: Oxford University Press. in the representation of objects ia symbolic
Rogoff, B., Ellis, S., & Gardner, W. (1984). Adjust- play from 18 to 34 months ofage. ChildDevel-
pient of adult-child instruction according to opment, 55, 1448-1455.
cbild's age arid task. Developmental Fsychol- Vygo(:skj-, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The devel-
ogtf, 20,193-199. opment of higher psychological processes.
Rogoft B., Malkin, C., & Gilbride, K. (1984) In- Cambridge, MA: Harv'ard University Press.
teraction with babies as guitknce in develop- Watson, M., & Fischer, K. (1977). A develop-
ment In B. Rogoff & J. V. Wertsch (Eds.), mental sequence of agent use in late infancy.
Children's learmng in the "Zone of Proximal ehild Development, 4B, 828-836.
Development" (pp. 31—44). San Francisco: Wertsch, J. V. (1984). The Zone of Pro.ximal De-
Jossey-Bass. velopment: Some conceptual issues. In B. Ro-
Rogoff, B., & Wertsch, J. V. (Eds.). (1984). Chil- goff & J. V. Wertscb (Eds.), Children's learn-
firen's learning in the "Zone of Proximal De- ing irt the "Zone of Proximal Development"
velopment." San Francisco; Jossev Bass. (pp, 7,.*lB). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Schaefer, C. E., Gitlin, K., & Sandgrund, A. (Eds.). Wertsdi, J. V. (1985). Vygotsky and the social for-
(1991), Play diagnosis and assessment. New mation of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
York: Wiley. Oniversitj' Press.
Sigel, I. E., McGillicuddy-DeLisi, A. V., & Good- Zukow, F, G. (1986). The relationship betvveen
now, J. J. (Eds.). (199Si). Parental belief sys- irrteraction with the caregiver and the emer-
term: the psychological consequences for gence of play activities during the one-word
children (2ded.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. period. British Journal of Developmental
Stevens, J. H. (1984). Child development knowl- ^, 4, 223-234.
edge and parenting skills. Family Relations,
33, 237-244.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi