Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 13

informs ®

Vol. 29, No. 5, September–October 2010, pp. 815–827


issn 0732-2399  eissn 1526-548X  10  2905  0815 doi 10.1287/mksc.1090.0557
© 2010 INFORMS

Positive Effects of Negative Publicity:


When Negative Reviews Increase Sales
Jonah Berger
The Wharton School of the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104,
jberger@wharton.upenn.edu

Alan T. Sorensen
Stanford Graduate School of Business, Stanford University, Stanford, California 94305,
asorensen@stanford.edu
Scott J. Rasmussen
Stanford University, Stanford, California 94305, scott.rasmussen@stanfordalumni.org

C an negative information about a product increase sales, and if so, when? Although popular wisdom suggests
that “any publicity is good publicity,” prior research has demonstrated only downsides to negative press.
Negative reviews or word of mouth, for example, have been found to hurt product evaluation and sales. Using
a combination of econometric analysis and experimental methods, we unify these perspectives to delineate
contexts under which negative publicity about a product will have positive versus negative effects. Specifically,
we argue that negative publicity can increase purchase likelihood and sales by increasing product awareness.
Consequently, negative publicity should have differential effects on established versus unknown products. Three
studies support this perspective. Whereas a negative review in the New York Times hurt sales of books by well-
known authors, for example, it increased sales of books that had lower prior awareness. The studies further
underscore the importance of a gap between publicity and purchase occasion and the mediating role of increased
awareness in these effects.
Key words: negative publicity; awareness; word of mouth; product success
History: Received: April 23, 2009; accepted: December 1, 2009; processed by James Hess. Published online in
Articles in Advance March 10, 2010.

Introduction redolent of stinky socks,” for example, saw its sales


Negative publicity often hurts. When a rumor circu- increase by 5% after it was reviewed by a prominent
lated that McDonald’s used worm meat in its ham- wine website (O’Connell 2006). Similarly, although
burgers, sales decreased by more than 25% (Greene the movie Borat made relentless fun of the country
1978). Coverage of musician Michael Jackson’s bizarre of Kazakhstan, Hotels.com reported a “300 percent
behavior and brushes with the law destroyed his increase in requests for information about the coun-
career. Viacom Inc. Chairman Sumner Redstone esti- try” after the film was released (Yabroff 2006, p. 8).
mated that negative publicity cost Mission Impossible 3 Although these may just be idiosyncratic examples,
they suggest that negative publicity may not always
more than $100 million in ticket sales (Burrough 2006),
be a bad thing. Can negative publicity actually have
and film pundits have suggested that it is “almost
a positive effect? And if so, when?
impossible to recover from bad buzz” (James 2006).
This paper examines negative publicity. Specifi-
Academic research corroborates this sentiment and
cally, we build on both behavioral and quantitative
casts further doubt on the old adage that “any pub- research to delineate contexts under which negative
licity is good publicity.” Negative publicity about a publicity about a product will have positive versus
product has been shown to hurt everything from negative effects on product choice and sales. In the
product and brand evaluation (Tybout et al. 1981, spirit of connecting consumer psychology and mar-
Wyatt and Badger 1984) to firm net present value and keting science (Winer 1999, Wittink 2004), we then test
sales (Goldenberg et al. 2007, Reinstein and Snyder our predictions using both experimental methods and
2005). Negative movie reviews, for example, decrease an econometric analysis of book reviews and sales.
box office receipts (Basuroy et al. 2003). Finally, we discuss the implications of these findings
A number of intriguing examples, however, seem for advertising and the success of cultural products
to contradict these findings. A wine described “as more broadly.
815
Berger, Sorensen, and Rasmussen: Positive Effects of Negative Publicity
816 Marketing Science 29(5), pp. 815–827, © 2010 INFORMS

Negative Effects of Negative or related cues in the environment can thus increase
Information choice and sales by informing consumers of a prod-
Research from a variety of perspectives has found uct’s existence (Stigler 1961) or increasing product
that negative information hurts. Behavioral work has accessibility (Berger and Fitzsimons 2008, Nedungadi
examined how negative information affects product 1990), respectively (see Lynch and Srull 1982). Simi-
impressions. Not surprisingly, negative reviews, mes- larly, regardless of valence, publicity (and potentially
sages, or rumors hurt product evaluations and reduce word of mouth) may have positive effects if it makes
purchase likelihood and sales (Huang and Chen 2006, consumers more aware or encourages the product to
Wyatt and Badger 1984, Tybout et al. 1981). Quanti- be top of mind.
tative work has investigated how reviews influence This distinction between evaluation and awareness
or accessibility is analogous to previous work on dif-
sales. Focusing on critics, a thumbs down from Siskel
ferent effects of advertising (Mitra and Lynch 1995).
and Ebert (Reinstein and Snyder 2005) or negative
Advertising can affect preferences for various options
movie reviews more broadly (Basuroy et al. 2003)
given that they are considered, but it can also affect
decrease box office revenue. Similar effects have been
choice by changing which options consumers con-
shown for consumer reviews: one-star reviews hurt
sider, independently of their preferences for those
book sales on Amazon.com; see, for example, Cheva-
options.
lier and Mayzlin (2006) (also see Clemons et al. 2006,
Dellarocas et al. 2008, Moe 2009).
Thus, whereas idioms may suggest that any public- The Current Research
ity is good publicity, all existing studies of negative Building on this analysis, we suggest that whether
publicity have found its effects to be negative. negative publicity has positive or negative effects will
Although such negative effects may be lessened for depend on existing product awareness and accessi-
high-commitment consumers (Ahluwalia et al. 2000), bility. When awareness is high, negative publicity
we are unaware of any research that has found should hurt sales. If most consumers already know
upsides of negative press. that Mission Impossible 3 is about to come out on
DVD, or there is a lot of buzz about a new Stephen
King book, the potential increase in awareness or
Routes Through Which Publicity accessibility generated by negative publicity should
Influences Behavior be negligible. Negative publicity should lower prod-
In contrast, we suggest that negative publicity can uct evaluation, however and, consequently, decrease
sometimes increase purchase likelihood and sales. consumer choice.
One way publicity can influence product success is Along these lines, previous quantitative work may
through influencing product evaluations. This can be have found negative effects of negative publicity
thought of as the persuasive impact of publicity (Liu because they examined products that already had con-
2006, Van den Bulte and Lilien 2001). If a critic gives siderable awareness. The average film in the Basuroy
a book a negative review, this may lead consumers et al. (2003) analysis, for example, grossed more than
to think the book is bad and reduce their likelihood $5 million in the first week (in the early 1990s)
of buying it. However, because negative information and was reviewed by more than 30 critics. These
should generally decrease product evaluations—or at numbers place the set of films at the more popular
best, leave them unchanged—attitude change alone end of films released over that period (http://www
cannot explain why negative publicity would ever .boxofficeguru.com). More generally, most previous
increase product choice or sales. work has studied well-advertised cultural products
We argue that negative publicity may have positive that many consumers likely knew about already.
effects, however, by increasing product awareness or In contrast, through increasing awareness, negative
accessibility. Consideration appears to explain much publicity may increase sales when product awareness
of choice (Hauser 1978). Consumers have finite atten- or accessibility is low. If few people know about a
tion, and the sheer multitude of cultural offerings book released by a new author, any publicity, regard-
means that most consumers will not be aware of every less of valence, should increase awareness. Although
book, movie, or album that is released. Similarly, peo- it focuses more on accessibility than awareness, this
ple may know about a product, but information varies suggestion is analogous to work by Nedungadi (1990)
its accessibility (Higgins and King 1981, Wyer and showing that cues, which activate related brands,
Srull 1981) and is less likely to affect behavior when it only have effects in situations where consideration
is not top of mind. Consumers may know that Mission is unlikely without a reminder. In addition, nega-
Impossible 3 recently came out on DVD, for example, tive publicity may be even more likely to boost sales
but if it does not come to mind when they are at the if awareness and publicity valence become dissoci-
video store, they are not going to rent it. Advertising ated in memory. Similar to the sleeper effect (Hannah
Berger, Sorensen, and Rasmussen: Positive Effects of Negative Publicity
Marketing Science 29(5), pp. 815–827, © 2010 INFORMS 817

and Sternthal 1984), where source information tends the review. Hundreds of books are released each
to become dissociated from the message over time, week, so consumers cannot possibly know about all of
people may have a feeling of awareness, or remem- them. Consequently, books by relatively new authors
ber they heard something about the product, but the (e.g., Dirk Wittenborn) have much to gain from the
valence may be forgotten (also see Skurnik et al. awareness that reviews provide. In contrast, books
2005). Work on advertising, for example, theorizes by more well-established authors (e.g., John Grisham
that even negative ads might boost purchase likeli- or Stephen King) should already be somewhat famil-
hood after delay because it increases brand awareness iar, and thus publicity’s ability to boost awareness
(Moore and Hutchinson 1983, 1985). or accessibility should be reduced (see Nedungadi
This dissociation should be particularly likely when 1990). We predict that while positive publicity should
product awareness is low. First, product familiar- increase sales for all types of authors, the effect of
ity increases involvement and motivation (Petty and negative publicity will depend on existing product
Cacioppo 1979), while it decreases the cognitive effort awareness. Negative publicity should hurt book sales
necessary to process information (Alba and Hutchin- of established authors but boost sales of authors who
son 1987). Consequently, lower involvement and are relatively unknown.
higher required effort for unfamiliar products should
decrease processing and encoding of publicity-related Method
information. Second, whereas familiar products have Our data set consists of weekly national sales for
strong memory networks, unknown products have 244 hardcover fiction titles that were released from
little existing cognitive structures, making it harder 2001 to 2003 and reviewed by the New York Times.
to incorporate and recall new information (Alba and For consistency’s sake, paragraph-length reviews and
Hutchinson 1987, Anderson 1995). Both these factors articles mentioning multiple books are omitted; thus
should make publicity valence for unknown products only full-length reviews are included in the sample.
more likely to fade over time. The increased aware- The sales data were provided by Nielsen BookScan,
ness, however, may lead even negative publicity to a market research firm that tracks book sales using
increase purchase. scanner data from an almost-comprehensive panel
Three studies test these hypotheses using a com- of retail booksellers. BookScan collects data through
bination of experimental methods and econometric cooperative arrangements with virtually all the major
analysis. Study 1 examines the performance of books bookstore chains, most major discount stores (e.g.,
reviewed in the New York Times to investigate the Costco), and most of the major online retailers (e.g.,
effect of publicity valence and product awareness on Amazon.com). They claim to track at least 80% of total
actual book sales. Study 2 examines the role that time retail sales. Additional information about individual
between publicity and choice plays in positive effects titles (such as publication date, subject, and author
of negative publicity. Finally, Study 3 directly inves- information) was obtained from a variety of sources,
tigates the mediating role of increased awareness in including Amazon.com and Overbooked.org.
these effects. Unlike movie critics, book reviewers do not use
stars or thumbs-up/thumbs-down systems to sum-
marize their opinions, so we had to do it for them.
Study 1: New York Times Book Reviews To avoid subjective biases, we attempted to do this
Study 1 uses weekly sales data to estimate the impact
in a systematic way. Typical reviews consist primarily
of New York Times book reviews on the sales of
of nonopinionated prose describing the book’s char-
more than 200 hardcover fiction titles. The reviews
acters and plot, so we flagged the sentences likely to
themselves are systematically classified as positive or
be opinionated by using a textual search algorithm
negative to distinguish the effects of good and bad
(looking for keywords such as the author’s name or
publicity. Inferring the impact of book reviews by
the word “writing”),1 randomized them, and then
comparing reviewed books to nonreviewed books is
scored them individually as positive, negative, or neu-
obviously problematic, because the New York Times’
tral. Each potentially opinionated sentence is therefore
decision to review a book may depend on its poten-
evaluated on its own merit. The relative opinion score
tial popularity. To avoid potential endogeneity bias,
for each book was then calculated as the ratio of pos-
our analysis focuses solely on reviewed books, using
itive sentences to opinionated sentences; for example,
time-series variation in sales (for a given book) to
identify the effect of published reviews. Essentially,
1
we measure the spike in sales in the week immedi- Details of the Perl script used to find opinionated sentences are
available on request. The algorithm prioritized Type II rather than
ately following the book review and ask whether the
Type I error minimization to limit the number of opinionated sen-
spike is different for positive and negative reviews. tences excluded from the analysis: 42.3% of flagged sentences were
We also examine whether the effect of being re- opinionated, whereas among a sample of 200 unflagged sentences,
viewed varies based on product awareness prior to only 18 (9%) were opinionated.
Berger, Sorensen, and Rasmussen: Positive Effects of Negative Publicity
818 Marketing Science 29(5), pp. 815–827, © 2010 INFORMS

Table 1 Summary Statistics for Reviews (Study 1) to the nine-week window surrounding each book’s
Percentiles
review week (the review week plus up to four weeks
pre- and post-review). As indicated in Table 1, most
Variable Mean Std. dev. 0.10 0.50 0.90 books are reviewed shortly after their release. In some
Books cases, reviews are published within one week of a
List price ($) 2463 312 2300 2495 2595 book’s release; we were forced to omit such books
Average weekly sales 268550 1083190 4890 36830 357020
Week reviewed 770 820 3 5 12
from our sample because since we would have no
pre-review sales trajectory to use as a benchmark for
Reviews
Number of sentences 4550 2260 12 48 74 measuring the impact of the review.
Number of opinionated 680 440 2 6 13 Suppose that sales of book i in week t are given by
Percent of opinionated 1600 890 710 1460 2500
Percent of positive 5530 3030 1110 5260 10000 si t = si t−1 · expxi t  · i t  (1)

so that  
a book with P positive sentences and N negative sen- si t
tences received a score of P /P + N . In the econo- ln = xi t  + ui t  (2)
si t−1
metric analysis, reviews are categorized as negative
when the ratio is below 50% and positive otherwise. We take Equation (2) as the model to be estimated.4
The resulting estimates are similar if publicity valence The dependent variable is the log of the sales ratio:
is kept as a continuous measure, if publicity valence today’s sales as a fraction of yesterday’s sales. Note
is calculated other ways, or if other negative public- that one advantage of this specification is that it puts
ity cutoffs (e.g., below 33% positive statements) are all books on the same scale, even though the sales
used.2 Summary statistics are reported in Table 1. levels vary dramatically across books in the sample.
Another motivation for this specification relates to
The primary motivation for this approach, which
its predictive power: because measuring the impact
essentially involves removing opinionated sentences
of book reviews requires knowing what sales would
from the larger context of the review, was to avoid
have looked like in the absence of a review, we want
confounding subjective opinions about a book’s con-
a model that does a good job predicting the path of
tent with objective evaluations of the reviewer’s cri-
sales. Allowing sales to depend on lagged sales gener-
tique. For example, a reader enthusiastic about the
ates predicted sales paths that are smoother and more
history of the American West may believe that a
accurate than what would be obtained from a model
review of a Western novel is positive, even though
containing only contemporaneous explanatory vari-
the reviewer criticizes the novel as dull or otherwise
ables. Also, note that because we use time-series varia-
poorly written. We expect our measure to differ some-
tion to identify the impact of reviews, for endogeneity
what from (but still be positively correlated with)
bias to be a problem, it would have to be the case that
scores based on an individual’s subjective reading of
reviews are systematically timed to correspond with
the reviews; our intent is to approximate the average
large unobserved demand shocks, which is a relatively
subjective reading in the population (e.g., averaging
implausible scenario.
over readers who do and do not like Westerns).3
Our vector of covariates, xi t , contains variables
Our analysis focuses on reviewed books, identify-
related to book reviews (e.g., an indicator for whether
ing the impact of reviews from the time-series varia-
a review of book i appeared in week t) as well as
tion in sales for a given book. We restrict our attention
week dummies (one for each of the 110 weeks rep-
resented in the sample, to control for time trends
2
A possible drawback to our method is that it does not give extra and seasonal variation in book demand), weeks since
weight to extreme expressions of opinion: e.g., a review with five the book was released, and indicators for whether
mildly positive sentences and one sentence saying “this is the worst
novel I’ve ever had the misfortune to read” will still be character-
the book was announced as a television book club
ized as a positive review. However, given that reviewers who write pick. The errors, ui t , are assumed to be independent
for the New York Times are given to relatively nuanced prose, and across books but potentially heteroskedastic across
blunt expressions of extreme opinion are rare, this possibility seems books and potentially dependent over time for a
like less of a concern. given book.5
3
Indeed, when a research assistant read 100 reviews and scored
them on a five-point negative–positive scale, her scores were sig-
4
nificantly correlated with our measure (r = 044, p < 002). Again, We used this particular equation because prior work has shown
because individual readers may perceive a review differently, it is that the sales of cultural products tend to peak in the first few
difficult to infer too much from this relationship, but the fact that weeks and then exponentially decline (Hendricks and Sorensen
it is significant supports the notion that our measure adequately 2009). Indeed, in our own data, more than 85% of books had
captured the valence of the review. Furthermore, any imprecision downward-trending sales over the first three months.
5
in our classification should introduce noise and make it harder to In the reported results, we simply present standard errors that
find any effects. are robust to the potential heteroskedasticity and within-group
Berger, Sorensen, and Rasmussen: Positive Effects of Negative Publicity
Marketing Science 29(5), pp. 815–827, © 2010 INFORMS 819

We investigate whether the impact of positive and Table 2 The Impact of Reviews on Sales for Authors with Varying
negative reviews vary based on existing awareness. Amounts of Prior Awareness (Study 1)
Using our measure of reviews’ opinions, we catego- Variable Estimates SE
rize reviews as positive or negative and estimate the
impact of reviews among three categories of authors: Positive review
(1) those who have published one or fewer books × <2 previous titles) 0347∗∗∗ 0103
× (2–9 previous titles) 0277∗∗∗ 0095
prior to the book in question, (2) those who have pub- × >10 previous titles) 0417∗∗ 0179
lished between two and nine, and (3) those who have
Negative review
published 10 or more.6 We then estimate the regres- × <2 previous titles) 0372∗ 0202
sion with separate review dummies for each of the six × (2–9 previous titles) 0137 0162
categories implied by 2 (review valence) × 3 (author × >10 previous titles) −0145∧ 0098
category). Weeks since release −0051∗∗∗ 0012
R2 0.182
Results
As predicted, whether being reviewed increased or Notes. Each specification also includes a full set of week fixed effects, a
variable for weeks since review, and four indicators for whether the book was
decreased sales depended on both review valence and
announced as a television book club pick. There are 1,942 observations. The
existing product awareness (see Table 2). Regardless estimate for negative reviews on books by authors with more than 10 prior
of whether the book was written by a new or estab- titles is slightly imprecise because of the relatively small sample size of only
lished author, being positively reviewed significantly 23 books.

increased sales; a positive review generated between p < 015, ∗ p < 010, ∗∗ p < 005, ∗∗∗ p < 001.
a 32% and 52% increase in demand (no significant
difference between coefficients, z = 10, p > 030). In reviews are truly having a causal effect on sales, one
contrast, estimates indicate that the effect of nega-
would expect the impact to be larger in New York
tive publicity depended on existing author awareness
City. We were able to obtain designated market area
(z = 230, p < 005). For books by established authors,
(DMA)-specific sales data for a small subsample of 33
a negative review led to a 15% decrease in sales (this
estimate is slightly imprecise because of the relatively books that allowed us to test this possibility. Nielsen
small sample size). For books by relatively unknown defines DMAs for the purpose of studying geographic
(new) authors, however, negative publicity has the variation in sales, and the DMAs generally encom-
opposite effect, increasing sales by 45%. pass entire metropolitan areas: e.g., the New York City
Comparing the coefficients for positive and nega- DMA includes Brooklyn, Queens, Manhattan, etc. As
tive publicity for different categories of authors pro- expected, the estimated effect of book reviews is much
vides additional insight. Although positive and neg- stronger in New York City (B = 081, SE = 014) than
ative publicity had different effects on the sales of the other DMAs (B = 044, standard error (SE) = 009:
books by established authors (z = 275, p < 001), they F 1 32 = 481, p < 005). This suggests that reviews
had similar effects on books by new authors (z = 011, have a causal effect on sales.
p > 090). Treating review valence as a continuous
variable underscores this pattern of results. Again, the Discussion
results indicate review valence had different effects on Can negative publicity ever increase sales? An analy-
sales of books by new versus established authors (z = sis of New York Times reviews and book sales suggests
198, p < 005). Whereas review valence had a signifi- it can. Relative to not being covered, being reviewed
cant effect on the sales of books by established authors in the New York Times increased a book’s sales, even
(t = 238, p < 002), it had little effect on the sales in some instances where a reviewer panned the book.
of books by new authors (p > 070). For books that The book Fierce People, for example, was written by a
should have relatively lower awareness, both positive new author and received an unambiguously negative
and negative publicity increased sales. review (e.g., “the characters do not have personalities
Finally, geographic variation in the effects bol- so much as particular niches in the stratosphere” and
sters our suggestion that reviews had a causal effect. “He gets by on attitude, not such a great strategy if
Though the New York Times is read widely through- the reader can’t figure out what that attitude is”), yet
out the nation, its readership is still somewhat con- sales more than quadrupled after the review.
centrated in New York City (New York Times Com-
More importantly, the results delineate conditions
pany 2006). Given this geographical dispersion, if
under which negative publicity will have positive
versus negative effects. Whereas positive reviews
dependence. Testing directly for serial correlation in the errors is
not a trivial exercise in this context; however, when the models are always increased sales, the effect of negative reviews
estimated assuming that u follows an AR(1) process, the results are depended on whether the authors were new or well
largely unaffected. established. As predicted, a negative review hurt sales
6
Results are similar when other similar groupings are used. of books by well-established authors but helped sales
Berger, Sorensen, and Rasmussen: Positive Effects of Negative Publicity
820 Marketing Science 29(5), pp. 815–827, © 2010 INFORMS

of books by relatively unknown authors. This is con- Method


sistent with our suggestion that the effects of negative Participants N = 252 completed two ostensibly unre-
publicity depend on existing product awareness. lated studies as part of a group of experiments. They
In addition, the pattern of results also supports our were given a chance to win a $25 gift certificate as
suggestion regarding the role that product awareness compensation.
plays in the stickiness of publicity valence. Whereas In the first study, we manipulated both the valence
positive and negative publicity had different effects and target of a review. Regarding valence, partici-
on the sales of books by established authors, their pants were told that the researchers were interested
effects were similar for books by new authors. This in how people respond to linguistic devices in writing
provides preliminary evidence for the notion that neg- and were asked to read two book reviews and answer
ative publicity may have positive effects because of questions related to the cover story (i.e., how var-
the dissociation of valence in memory. ied was the sentence structure?). The second review
The pattern also casts doubt on an alternative was always the same, but we manipulated whether
explanation for the results. One could argue that neg- the first review was positive or negative. In the posi-
ative reviews might have a positive effect because tive review condition, participants read that the book
being reviewed is itself a signal of quality. The mere was “a powerful, timely, and shocking story of polit-
fact that a book is reviewed in the New York Times ical and legal intrigue, a story that will leave read-
may signal that it is an outstanding book relative ers unable to think about our electoral process or
to the universe of books available. This explanation, judicial system in quite the same way ever again.”
however, would suggest that being reviewed (regard- It went on to suggest that the book was “overall,
less of valence) should always boost sales, which a delightfully entertaining read by an author who
the results show is not the case. Negative reviews knows his subject matter.” In the negative review con-
decreased the sales of books by established authors. dition, participants read that the book provided as
“vivid an account as the fictional fare sold at air-
Study 2: The Role of Time port kiosks—but it is also, alas, just as oversimplified,
Study 2 has two main goals. First, it provides a and it distorts the justice system in the same way.” It
more controlled examination of the effects of nega- went on to suggest that “sometimes there is so much
tive publicity. We directly manipulated both publicity going on that the novel threatens to spin out of con-
valence and product awareness to examine how they trol. In addition, despite the strength of the novel’s
influence purchase likelihood. Second, it provides a themes, their potential effect is sometimes thwarted
deeper examination of the role that the dissociation by cloying prose.” A pretest confirmed the valence of
of valence in memory over time plays in these effects. the reviews. Participants N = 37 rated the valence
Given that much of the sales data in Study 1 was of one of the reviews (−3 = extremely negative, 3 =
in store, and many readers get the New York Times extremely positive). Results indicated that the posi-
delivered to their home, there should have been a rea- tive review was perceived positively (M = 111, com-
sonable delay between when most consumers read a pared to zero, t17 = 316, p = 0006) and the negative
review and bought books. Study 2 examines the effect review perceived negatively (M = −068, compared to
of time delay more directly. Few consumers want to zero, t18 = 282, p = 001).
read a bad book or see a movie they know will be We varied product awareness by manipulating the
terrible, but over time, the valence of publicity for target of the review. In the high-awareness condi-
unknown products may be forgotten, leading posi- tion, we selected a eagerly anticipated book (The
tive and negative reviews to have a similar effect. To Appeal) by a well-known author (John Grisham). It
test this possibility, we manipulated whether people was the author’s first legal thriller in a number of
reported purchase likelihood either right after reading years and was about to be released, so there was a
a product review or after a delay. good deal of buzz about the book on the Web. Indeed,
For products with high awareness, we predict that on the strength of preorders alone, it had risen into
review valence should influence purchase likelihood the top 1,000 best-selling books on Amazon.com. In
regardless of whether there is a delay. Compared to the low-awareness condition, we created a fictitious
a negative review, a positive review should lead to book. It was given a similar sounding name (The
greater purchase likelihood. In contrast, for products Report) but given that we generated it ourselves, it
with low awareness, the effect of review valence on should be unknown. A pretest confirmed the differ-
purchase likelihood should be moderated by time. ence in product awareness. Of eight books rated (see
Compared to a negative review, a positive review Study 3 for information on the pretest method), par-
should increase purchase likelihood when partici- ticipants were most aware of Grisham’s book (rela-
pants fill out the dependent measures right away. tive to others in the set, t165 = 795, p < 0001) and
After a delay, however, this effect should dissipate. least aware of our fictitious book The Report (relative to
Berger, Sorensen, and Rasmussen: Positive Effects of Negative Publicity
Marketing Science 29(5), pp. 815–827, © 2010 INFORMS 821

others in the set, t165 = 376, p < 0001).7 Thus par- Figure 1 Effect of Review Valence, Product Awareness, and Time
ticipants in each publicity valence condition read the Delay on Purchase Likelihood (Study 2)
same review, but the product in the review varied by 5 Well-known product
condition.

Purchase likelihood
Finally, we manipulated when participants reported 4
purchase likelihood. In the context of an ostensibly
3
unrelated task, participants were given a list of four
books, including the target book, and were asked to 2
rate how likely they would be to purchase each book
1
(1 = not at all likely, 7 = extremely likely). Half the
participants completed the purchase likelihood mea- 0
sures right away, whereas the other half first com- No delay Delay
pleted a long series of filler tasks (adapted from Sela
Negative review Positive review
and Shiv 2009).
4 Unknown product
Results

Purchase likelihood
We examined purchase likelihood in a 2 (review 3
valence: positive versus negative) × 2 (time: delay
versus no delay) × 2 product awareness (well known 2
versus unknown) analysis of variance (ANOVA).
Analysis revealed main effects of both review valence 1
(MPositive = 333 versus MNegative = 226; F 1 244 =
1907, p < 0001) and product awareness (MWell-Known = 0
330 versus MUnknown = 228; F 1 244 = 1735, No delay Delay
p < 0001).
More importantly, these effects were qualified by the likelihood when participants completed the depen-
predicted three-way review valence × time × product dent measures right away (F < 010, p > 085). After
awareness interaction (F 1 244 = 411, p = 004); see a delay, however, there was a significant product
Figure 1. Focusing on known versus unknown prod- awareness × review valence interaction (F 1 121 =
ucts separately helps clarify the pattern of results. For 749, p < 001) such that review valence influenced
well-known products, there was only a main effect the purchase likelihood of well-known products
of review valence (F 1 94 = 1117, p < 0001). Partici- (F 1 121 = 1208, p < 0001) but not unknown prod-
pants reported they would be more likely to purchase ucts (F < 005, p > 080).
well-known products that were positively (rather
than negatively) reviewed, regardless of whether they Discussion
reported purchase likelihood right away or after a Study 2 further illustrates when negative publicity
delay. In contrast, for unknown products, a delay will have positive effects while also underscoring
moderated the effect of review valence on purchase the important role of time in this process. Regard-
likelihood (F 1 150 = 430, p = 004). Review valence less of whether participants reported purchase likeli-
had an effect when participants reported purchase hood right away or after a delay, negative publicity
likelihood right away (F 1 244 = 683, p < 001), but hurt purchase of well-known products. For unknown
this effect dissipated after a delay (F < 050, p > 080). products, however, the effect of publicity valence dis-
Viewed another way, the effect of a negative review sipated over time. Consistent with the notion that pub-
for an unknown product became more positive after licity valence for unknown products fades in memory,
a delay (F 1 244 = 596, p = 001) such that purchase there was no effect of publicity valence on purchase
likelihood after a delay was similar after a positive or likelihood of unknown products after a delay. In addi-
negative review. tion, for unknown products, the effect of a negative
Looked at another way, product awareness did not review became more positive over time. This sug-
moderate the effect of review valence on purchase gests that whereas publicity valence fades over time,
increased awareness may remain, which can boost
7
One could argue that the author is well known but the product
purchase likelihood.
itself is not. However, especially in the case of well-known authors,
the author’s name almost always accompanies publicity about the
book. Furthermore, many people refer to books by well-known
Study 3: Increasing Product Awareness
authors by the author (e.g., “John Grisham’s new thriller”) rather The first two studies support our hypotheses, but
than just the title alone. they only tell part of the story. We have argued
Berger, Sorensen, and Rasmussen: Positive Effects of Negative Publicity
822 Marketing Science 29(5), pp. 815–827, © 2010 INFORMS

that whereas publicity valence fades over time for along the line, but based on where the participants
unknown products, increased awareness may remain, clicked, a value between 0 and 700 was recorded.
which can boost purchase likelihood even when the Prior to rating the books, participants were given
publicity was negative. Study 3 directly tests this pos- some instructions on this rating system, as well as
sibility by examining whether positive effects of neg- some practice ratings. This setup was used to make it
ative publicity are driven by increased awareness. In difficult for participants to just restate their pre-review
addition, we measure product evaluations even when awareness rating on the post-review awareness scales.
the product is not reviewed. This baseline allows us After filler tasks, participants completed a suppos-
to examine how positive and negative publicity shift edly unrelated study in which they read a review
evaluation relative to not being reviewed at all. of one of the target books. The task was similar to
We again manipulated both the valence and tar- the one used in Study 2, and the review valence and
get of a book review, but all participants reported product awareness manipulations were the same.
purchase likelihood after a long delay, and they did After more filler tasks, including the delay task
so for both reviewed and nonreviewed books. Thus from Study 2, participants completed the dependent
we used a 2 (prior product awareness) × 2 (review measures. First, they used a seven-point scale to rate
valence) × 2 (product reviewed) full-factorial mixed how likely they would be to buy each of eight books,
design. This allows us to control for the mere act including the two target books. On a separate page,
of reading a positive or negative review (which may they then completed the same awareness task they
affect evaluations through mood). We predict that had done previously. They clicked somewhere on a
whereas positive publicity should increase purchase nonnumbered line to indicate their current product
likelihoods for both books, existing product aware- awareness. Next, they used a seven-point scale to
ness should moderate the effect of a negative review. rate their product evaluation of the same eight books.
Consistent with Studies 1 and 2, negative publicity Finally, they completed demographic measures and
should hurt purchase likelihood of the well-known were thanked for their time.
product but boost purchase likelihood when the prod-
uct is unknown. Results
We also measured a number of other factors to Preliminary Analyses. We subtracted pre-review
further examine the mechanism behind these effects. awareness from post-review awareness to create an
Both before and after reading the review, participants awareness change score for the two target products.
rated product awareness for the two target books. We We controlled for purchase likelihood variation across
also measured product evaluations after the review. participants by averaging purchase likelihoods for
If the publicity valence fades more for unknown nontarget books, and we used this measure as a
products, as we have suggested, then publicity covariate in subsequent analyses. We examined aware-
valence should affect evaluations when the product ness change, product attitudes, and purchase likeli-
is well known, but not unknown. Publicity should hood for each of the target books using a 2 (prior
only increase product awareness, however, when the product awareness: unknown versus well-known) × 2
product is relatively unknown. Furthermore, whereas (review valence: positive versus negative) × 2 (prod-
product attitudes should influence the purchase likeli- uct reviewed: reviewed versus other book reviewed)
hood of both books, change in awareness should have repeated measures ANOVA. Again, because all partic-
a stronger effect on the previously unknown book. For ipants rated purchase likelihood for both books, even
this previously unknown product, increased aware- though they only read a review for one of them, we
ness should drive the effect of being reviewed on can compare how prior product awareness and review
purchase likelihood. valence affect purchase likelihood relative to a control
situation where the product was not reviewed.
Method Change in Awareness. First, we examined how
Participants (N = 137, mean age = 35) completed a publicity influences product awareness. As pre-
group of studies online. They were told they would dicted, being reviewed affected unknown and well-
participate in a variety of studies from different exper- known products differently, as indicated by a
imenters and were compensated with a chance to win product reviewed × prior product awareness inter-
a $25 gift certificate. action (F 1 134 = 489, p = 003). Compared to
First, participants were asked to rate their aware- not being reviewed, publicity increased awareness
ness of eight books (the two target books from of the unknown product (M = 4821 versus 9.29;
Study 2 plus six fillers, e.g., The Omnivore’s Dilemma). F 1 134 = 591, p = 002). In contrast, being reviewed
Importantly, they provided their ratings by clicking had no effect on awareness of the well-known prod-
on a line anchored by “Not at all aware” and uct (F 1 134 < 1, p > 035). No other effects reached
“Extremely aware.” No numbers appeared anywhere significance (F values < 26). In sum, publicity only
Berger, Sorensen, and Rasmussen: Positive Effects of Negative Publicity
Marketing Science 29(5), pp. 815–827, © 2010 INFORMS 823

increased awareness when the product was not well Figure 2 Effect of Review Valence and Existing Product Awareness on
known already. Purchase Likelihood (Study 3)
Product Evaluations. Second, we examined how Well-known product
publicity influences evaluations. As predicted, review 4
valence affected unknown and well-known products
differently, as indicated by the product awareness ×

Purchase likelihood
3
product reviewed × review valence interaction
(F 1 134 = 347, p = 006). Focusing on each book
type separately helps clarify the pattern of results. 2
For the well-known product, there was a sig-
nificant product reviewed × review valence inter-
1
action (F 1 134 = 416, p = 004). Compared to
cases where the book was not reviewed, a posi-
tive review slightly increased evaluations (M = 482 0
versus 4.67; F 1 134 < 035, p > 050, whereas a neg- Negative review Positive review
ative review decreased evaluations (M = 400 versus
4.86; F 1 134 = 414, p = 004). In contrast, attitudes Reviewed Not reviewed (other book reviewed)
toward the unknown book did not significantly differ
across conditions (F s < 06), consistent with the notion 4
Unknown product
that publicity valence fades over time for unknown
products.
Purchase likelihood
3
Purchase Likelihood. Next, we examined how
publicity influences purchase likelihood. Not surpris-
ingly, people said they would be more likely to pur- 2

chase John Grisham’s new thriller M = 320 than an


unknown book (M = 220; F 1 134 = 679, p = 001) 1
More importantly, as predicted, review valence
affected unknown and well-known products differ-
0
ently, as indicated by the product awareness × product Negative review Positive review
reviewed × review valence interaction (F 1 134 =
410, p < 005); see Figure 2. Relative to not being
reviewed, negative publicity decreased purchase like- awareness change. Results revealed interactive effects
lihood of the well-known book (F 1 134 = 644, of book type with both product evaluations ( =
p = 001) but increased purchase likelihood of the 018, t132 = 338, p = 0001) and awareness change
unknown book (F 1 134 = 347, p = 006). Posi- ( = −012, t132 = 234, p = 002).
tive reviews, on the other hand, had similar effect To clarify the pattern of results, we ran separate
on both types of books. Relative to not being regressions for well-known and unknown products,
reviewed, positive publicity increased purchase like- where product evaluation and change in awareness
lihood of both the well-known (although not signif- predicted purchase likelihood. For the well-known
icantly so—F 1 134 = 037, p = 054) and unknown product, evaluations were linked to purchase likeli-
book (F 1 134 = 420, p = 004). hood ( = 057, t132 = 861, p < 0001) but change
Looked at another way, for the well-known in awareness was not ( = 006, t132 = 101, p >
book, review valence influenced purchase likelihood 030). For the previously unknown product, however,
(F 1 134 = 535, p = 002) such that a positive pub- both evaluations ( = 020, t132 = 233, p = 002)
licity helped more than a negative publicity. For the and change in awareness ( = 032, t132 = 355,
unknown book, however, both positive and negative p < 0001) were linked to purchase likelihood. These
publicity had a similar effect in increasing purchase results indicate that whereas purchase likelihood for
likelihood (F 1 134 < 03, p > 060). the well-known product was driven by product atti-
Link Between Product Evaluations, Awareness, tudes, for the unknown product, increased awareness
and Purchase Likelihood. We also examined how played a more important role.
purchase likelihood varied based on product evalua- A mediational analysis (Baron and Kenny 1986)
tion and change in awareness. A multiple regression further demonstrates that increased purchase likeli-
examined how purchase likelihood for the reviewed hood for the previously unknown product was driven
book varied based on product type (whether the by increased awareness. Being reviewed was linked
product was well-known or unknown), product eval- to increased purchase likelihood ( = 016, t136 =
uations, change in product awareness, and interac- 278, p < 001) and change in awareness ( = 023,
tive effects of book type with product evaluations and t136 = 270, p < 001), but when both review and
Berger, Sorensen, and Rasmussen: Positive Effects of Negative Publicity
824 Marketing Science 29(5), pp. 815–827, © 2010 INFORMS

awareness were included in the regression, change situation when negative publicity might boost pur-
in awareness remained significant ( = 029, t135 = chase. Our investigation is the first to show beneficial
523, p < 0001), whereas being reviewed reduced to effects of negative publicity, and further, to delineate
marginality ( = 010, t135 = 178, p = 008). A Sobel conditions under which negative publicity will have
test confirmed the pattern of mediation (z = 240, positive versus negative effects.
p < 002). Using a combination of experimental methods and
econometric analysis, we demonstrate that the effect
Discussion of negative publicity depends on existing product
Results of Study 3 again demonstrate conditions awareness. Although negative publicity hurt products
under which negative publicity will have positive that already had broad awareness, it helped products
versus negative effects. Compared to no publicity at that were relatively unknown (Studies 1 through 3).
all, whether the same negative review increased or These effects were shown for both purchase likelihood
decreased purchase likelihood depended on existing (Studies 2 and 3), as well as actual sales (Study 1).
awareness about the cultural product being reviewed. The studies further demonstrate the importance of
Whereas a negative review decreased purchase like- a delay between publicity and purchase opportunity
lihood of a book that was already well known, in these effects (Study 2), as well as the mediating
it increased purchase likelihood for a previously role of product awareness (Study 3). Review valence
unknown book. fades over time for unknown products and negative
The findings also demonstrate the important role reviews increase purchase likelihood by making peo-
of increased product awareness in positive effects of ple more aware of the product. Finally, by combining
negative publicity. For a product that was already controlled experiments (Studies 2 and 3) and empir-
well known, publicity valence determined the out- ical analysis of actual product sales (Study 1), we
come. Publicity did not boost awareness, but the examine the underlying mechanism behind positive
valence of the review influenced product evaluations effects of negative publicity while also demonstrating
and led people to be more likely to purchase after their importance for actual product performance.
a positive rather than negative review. The process
differed, however, for an unknown product. Regard- Implications and Future Research
less of whether the publicity was positive or nega- Our theorizing also helps unify these findings
tive, it increased product awareness, which, in turn, with prior results showing that negative publicity
increased purchase likelihood. This is consistent with decreases sales (e.g., Basuroy et al. 2003). For the
the notion that negative publicity increases sales most part, previous work has focused on relatively
through affecting the consideration set. popular cultural products (i.e., major films), which
In addition, the findings cast doubt on a num- should already have at least some awareness among
ber of alternative explanations. Although one could the population. Whereas around 700 feature films are
argue that negative publicity might increase purchase released in the United States every year, the number
likelihood through making the product more famil- of new books numbers is in the hundreds of thou-
iar, and thus boosting liking, the evaluation results sands (Bogart 2001, Motion Picture Association 2006).
suggest this is unlikely. Although being negatively Consequently, existing product awareness should be
reviewed boosted product awareness for the book higher for reviewed movies than reviewed books, and
by the unknown author, it did not increase product negative effects of negative publicity should be more
evaluations. In addition, the fact that review valence likely.
influenced evaluations of well-known products but Even when general awareness is low, however,
not unknown products provides further evidence that we suggest that negative publicity will be unlikely
publicity valence fades over time for products that to have a positive effect when product awareness
are less well known. The mediation by awareness and accessibility are high among the people reached.
also casts doubt on an alternative account based on Chevalier and Mayzlin (2006), for example, exam-
involvement. ine consumer reviews on Internet booksellers and
find that negative reviews hurt sales. Although gen-
General Discussion eral awareness of most books should be low, the
Whereas conventional wisdom suggests that any pub- observed effects may have been similar to those found
licity is good publicity, existing research has doc- in work on movie reviews because product aware-
umented only downsides of negative reviews and ness is necessarily high among people reached by the
information, such as decreased product evaluations reviews. Reviews on sites like Amazon.com appear
and reduced sales. This paper helps unify these per- on the same page as the book itself, and to the
spectives. Building on research regarding product degree that consumers have searched for the book by
awareness and evaluation, we theorized one type of name, product awareness is already reasonably high.
Berger, Sorensen, and Rasmussen: Positive Effects of Negative Publicity
Marketing Science 29(5), pp. 815–827, © 2010 INFORMS 825

Consequently, the review should have little ability hurt brand choice among consumers who see them
to increase accessibility or awareness and will be as irrelevant to their own interests (Simonson et al.
unlikely to boost sales. However, in situations where 1994), reviews that contain irrelevant reasons might
people are browsing the New York Times book review also have perverse effects and actually lead to more
section or glancing to see what movies happen to be positive evaluations. Future work might also examine
reviewed that week, the review may be the first time when negative publicity boosts product evaluations.
they hear about the product, and thus negative pub- Repeated exposure to a stimulus can increase liking
licity may still have positive effects. (Zajonc 1968), and thus even negative attention might
More generally, our theorizing helps explain how increase choice and sales through making products
publicity, in general, regardless of valence, will impact more familiar.
product success under different circumstances. Pub- Another interesting question is whether previous
licity should be more likely to have a positive impact evaluations influence exposure to reviews in the first
on sales in situations where existing product aware- place. People who have strong positive attitudes
ness or accessibility is low. Consistent with this toward a product may be less likely to even con-
suggestion, Reinstein and Snyder (2005) report that sider reading a negative review. One could also argue
reviews only influenced demand for more obscure that negative reviews may be more interesting to read
films (e.g., those that were narrowly released). When and more likely to be shared with friends, and if
we estimate separate coefficients for the three author so, this could lead one negative review to generate
categories in our own book sales data, the coefficient greater word of mouth and have a larger overall effect
is largest for the new authors (although the relative than a similar positive review. Gaining deeper insight
imprecision of the estimates prevents us from draw- into the effects of publicity on sales requires under-
ing any strong conclusions). Overall, publicity should standing not only how publicity influences informa-
have less of an impact on widely released books or tion processing and attitudes but also word of mouth,
movies because everyone knows about them already. exposure likelihood, and memory.
Our studies focused on product reviews, but simi- It would also be interesting to examine whether,
lar effects may also hold for other types of publicity through making the product more accessible, negative
such as word of mouth. Researchers have long been attention to concepts that are conceptually linked to a
interested in how word of mouth influences diffusion product can also influence sales (see Berger and Fitzsi-
and sales (Arndt 1967, Coleman et al. 1966, Godes and mons 2008, Berger et al. 2008). Actors, musicians, and
Mayzlin 2009, Iyengar et al. 2008). This research area, even writers sometimes receive negative news cov-
however, generally assumes that negative word of erage for various indiscretions. Could negative press
mouth should hurt product success (e.g., Goldenberg about musicians affect the success of their albums,
et al. 2007; also see numerous popular press books even if the publicity never mentions their music?
on the importance of decreasing negative word of Other data we collected suggest that it may. We
mouth, e.g., Blackshaw 2008). Although this assump- were able to obtain Amazon.com sales rankings for
tion seems fair, the studies reported here indicate a number of Michael Jackson albums over time (Jan-
that it may not always hold. Just as negative reviews uary 2003–December 2004) as well as a measure of
can increase the accessibility of unknown products, negative attention to the star (i.e., the number of
negative word of mouth may have similar effects. top 50 newspaper articles over time that mentioned
Hearing that a friend hated a restaurant should defi- his various run-ins with the law). Analyses indicated
nitely decrease product evaluations, but it should also that negative publicity, even of an indirect nature,
increase product awareness and accessibility, which was linked to sales. Jackson sold more albums in
may have positive downstream effects on sales. times when he was in the news for child molesta-
Future research might examine other factors that tion charges or dangling his baby over a balcony.
shape how publicity and product reviews influence Although these events have nothing to do with the
consumer choice. One such factor is the extremity quality of Jackson’s music, they may influence sales
of the review. Whereas Study 2 illustrated that the through a similar mechanism as product reviews.
effects of negative publicity become more positive Just as product reviews should increase the likeli-
after delay, its immediate effects may depend on hood a product is top of mind, so too should atten-
whether it is weakly or strongly negative. Whereas tion to conceptually related cues in the environment
really negative publicity should turn most people (Berger and Fitzsimons 2008). Increased media atten-
away, slightly negative publicity may increase interest tion to Jackson should make him more accessible in
enough to have positive effects even in the short term. consumers’ minds, which, in turn, should increase
Another factor is whether the review provides reasons the likelihood that they buy his music. Similarly,
that are relevant to the reader. Just as added prod- negative attention to CEOs or other public figures
uct features can sometimes have perverse effects and (e.g., Paris Hilton) may affect the sales of related
Berger, Sorensen, and Rasmussen: Positive Effects of Negative Publicity
826 Marketing Science 29(5), pp. 815–827, © 2010 INFORMS

products because they increase product accessibility. Berger, J., C. Heath. 2005. Idea habitats: How the prevalence of
Consequently, the success of products, ideas, and environmental cues influences the success of ideas. Cognitive
Sci. 29(2) 195–221.
behaviors may vary over time with the prevalence of
Berger, J., M. Meredith, S. C. Wheeler. 2008. Contextual priming:
related cues in the environment (Berger and Heath Where people vote affects how they vote. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
2005). The current findings on this issue are far from USA 105(26) 8846–8849.
conclusive, but they suggest that further research Blackshaw, P. 2008. Satisfied Customers Tell Three Friends, Angry Cus-
should examine not only direct negative publicity tomers Tell 3,000: Running a Business in Today’s Consumer-Driven
(i.e., product reviews) but also publicity that is of a World. Broadway Business, New York.
Bogart, D. 2001. The Bowker Annual Library and Book Trade Almanac.
more indirect nature.
R. R. Bowker, New York.
In summary, although companies and individuals Burrough, B. 2006. Sleeping with the fishes. Vanity Fair (Decem-
often try to quiet negative publicity, our findings indi- ber 1) 244–256.
cate that in some cases, it can actually have positive Chevalier, J. A., D. Mayzlin. 2006. The effect of word of mouth on
effects. Although producers of major motion pictures sales: Online book reviews. J. Marketing Res. 43(3) 345–354.
or highly anticipated books might want to attempt to Clemons, E. K., G. Gao, L. M. Hitt. 2006. When online reviews
deaden negative press, smaller producers might want meet hyperdifferentiation: A study of the craft beer industry.
J. Management Inform. Systems 23(2) 149–171.
to allow, or even fan the flames of, negative pub-
Coleman, J. S., E. Katz, H. Menzel. 1966. Medical Innovation: A Dif-
licity. This does not mean that all negative informa- fusion Study. Bobbs-Merrill, Indianapolis.
tion will have positive effects, and it is still important Dellarocas, C., X. Zhang, N. F. Awad. 2007. Exploring the value of
to understand consumer sentiment (see Goldenberg online product reviews in forecasting sales: The case of motion
et al. 2010). That said, not all negative publicity or pictures. J. Interactive Marketing 21(4) 23–45.
word of mouth should be quieted. Consumers often Godes, D., D. Mayzlin. 2009. Firm-created word-of-mouth com-
munication: Evidence from a field test. Marketing Sci. 28(4)
attempt to discount direct advertising, but because 721–739.
negative publicity does not seem like a direct product Goldenberg, J., B. Libai, S. Moldovan, E. Muller. 2007. The NPV of
appeal, it may slip in under the radar and thus have bad news. Internat. J. Res. Marketing 24(3) 186–200.
a more pronounced effect. In summary, although neg- Goldenberg, J., R. Feldman, M. Fresco, O. Netzer, L. Ungar. 2010.
ative publicity is not always a good thing, in some Using text mining to analyze user forums. J. Online Inform. Rev.
cases, negative can actually be positive. Forthcoming.
Greene, B. 1978. Worms? McDonald’s isn’t laughing. Chicago Tri-
bune (November 20) A1.
Acknowledgments Hannah, D. B., B. Sternthal. 1984. Detecting and explaining the
The authors are grateful to Eric Bradlow, Chip Heath, Wes sleeper effect. J. Consumer Res. 11(2) 632–642.
Hutchinson, John Lynch, Dina Mayzlin, Matthew Salganik, Hauser, J. R. 1978. Testing the accuracy, usefulness, and significance
Christophe Van den Bulte, Christian Wheeler, and partic- of probabilistic models: An information-theoretic approach.
ipants at Stanford’s Café Verona Session for helpful feed- Oper. Res. 26(3) 406–421.
back on various versions of the manuscript; to Lisa Falzone Hendricks, K., A. Sorensen. 2009. Information and the skewness of
for research assistance; to Young Lee for helped implement music sales. J. Political Econom. 117(2) 324–369.
Studies 2 and 3; and to Annalisa Mastri for invaluable data Higgins, E. T., G. King. 1981. Accessibility of social constructs:
and editing assistance. Information-processing consequences of individual and con-
textual variability. N. Cantor, J. F. Kihlstrom, eds. Personality,
Cognition, and Social Interaction. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale,
NJ, 60–81.
References Huang, J.-H., Y.-F. Chen. 2006. Herding in online product choice.
Ahluwalia, R., R. E. Burnkrant, H. R. Unnava. 2000. Consumer Psych. Marketing 23(5) 413–428.
response to negative publicity: The moderating role of com- Iyengar, R., C. Van den Bulte, T. W. Valente. 2008. Opinion lead-
mitment. J. Marketing Res. 37(2) 203–214. ership and social contagion in new product diffusion. Market-
Alba, J. W., J. W. Hutchinson. 1987. Dimensions of consumer exper- ing Science Institute Report 08-120, Marketing Science Institute,
tise. J. Consumer Res. 13(4) 411–454. Cambridge, MA.
Anderson, J. R. 1995. Learning and Memory: An Integrated Approach. James, C. 2006. When the bad buzz arrives before the movie does.
John Wiley & Sons, New York. New York Times (September 26) A1.
Arndt, J. 1967. Role of product-related conversations in the diffu- Liu, Y. 2006. Word of mouth for movies: Its dynamics and impact
sion of a new product. J. Marketing Res. 4(3) 291–295. on box office revenue. J. Marketing 70(3) 74–89.
Baron, R. M., D. A. Kenny. 1986. The moderator-mediator vari- Lynch, J. R. Jr., T. K. Srull. 1982. Memory and attentional factors in
able distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, consumer choice: Concepts and research methods. J. Consumer
strategic, statistical considerations. J. Personality Soc. Psych. Res. 9(1) 18–37.
51(6) 1173–1182. Mitra, A., J. G. Lynch Jr. 1995. Toward a reconciliation of market
Basuroy, S., S. Chatterjee, S. A. Ravid. 2003. How critical are critical power and information theories of advertising effects on price
reviews? The box office effects of film critics, star power, and elasticity. J. Consumer Res. 21(March) 644–659.
budgets. J. Marketing 67(4) 103–117. Moe, W. W. 2009. How much does a good product rating help a bad
Berger, J., G. M. Fitzsimons. 2008. Dogs on the street, Pumas on product? Modeling the dynamics of online product ratings and
your feet: How cues in the environment influence product their effects on sales. Working paper, University of Maryland,
evaluation and choice. J. Marketing Res. 45(1) 1–14. College Park.
Berger, Sorensen, and Rasmussen: Positive Effects of Negative Publicity
Marketing Science 29(5), pp. 815–827, © 2010 INFORMS 827

Moore, D. L., J. W. Hutchinson. 1983. The effects of ad affect on Simonson, I., Z. Carmon, S. O’Curry. 1994. Experimental evidence
advertising effectiveness. R. P. Bagozzi, A. M. Tybout, eds. on the negative effect of product features and sales promotions
Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 10. Association for Con- on brand choice. Marketing Sci. 13(1) 23–40.
sumer Research, Ann Arbor, MI 526–531. Skurnik, I., C. Yoon, D. C. Park, N. Schwarz. 2005. How warnings
Moore, D. L., J. W. Hutchinson. 1985. The influence of affective about false claims become recommendations. J. Consumer Res.
reactions to advertising: Direct and indirect mechanisms of 31(4) 713–724.
attitude change. L. F. Alwitt, A. A. Mitchell, eds. Psychological Stigler, G. J. 1961. The economics of information. J. Political Econom.
Processes and Advertising Effects: Theory, Research, and Applica- 69(3) 213–225.
tion. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 65–90.
Tybout, A. M., B. J. Calder, B. Sternthal. 1981. Using information-
Motion Picture Association of America. 2006. 2006 U.S. theatrical processing theory to design marketing strategies. J. Marketing
market statistics. Retrieved September 1, 2006, http://pds4 Res. 18(1) 73–79.
.egloos.com/pds/200708/28/11/2006-US-Theatrical-Market
-Statistics-Report.pdf. Van den Bulte, C., G. L. Lilien. 2001. Two-stage partial observability
models of innovation adoption. Working paper, The Wharton
Nedungadi, P. 1990. recall and consumer consideration sets: Influ-
School, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia.
encing choice without altering brand evaluations. J. Consumer
Res. 17(3) 263–276. Yabroff, J. 2006. Coming of age. Newsweek (December 18) 8.
New York Times Company. 2006. 2006 annual report. Retrieved Winer, R. S. 1999. Experimentation in the 21st century: The
May 24, 2007, http://www.nytco.com/pdf/annual_2006/ importance of external validity. J. Acad. Marketing Sci. 27(3)
2006NYTannual.pdf. 349–358.
O’Connell, V. 2006. Ripe for change: Wine sales thrive as old barri- Wittink, D. R. 2004. Journal of Marketing Research: 2 Ps. J. Marketing
ers start to crumble. Wall Street Journal (August 25) A1. Res. 41(1) 1–6.
Petty, R. E., J. T. Cacioppo. 1979. Issue involvement can increase or Wyatt, R. O., D. P. Badger. 1984. How reviews affect interest in and
decrease persuasion by enhancing message-relevant cognitive evaluation of films. Journalism Quart. 61(4) 874–878.
responses. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 37(10) 1915–1926. Wyer, R. S., T. K. Srull. 1981. Category accessibility: Some theoret-
Reinstein, D. A., C. M. Snyder. 2005. The influence of expert reviews ical and empirical issues concerning the processing of social
on consumer demand for experience goods: A case study of stimulus information. E. T. Higgins, C. P. Herman, M. P. Zanna,
movie critics. J. Indust. Econom. 53(1) 27–51. eds. Social Cognition: The Ontario Symposium, Vol. 1. Lawrence
Sela, A., B. Shiv. 2009. Unraveling priming: When does the same Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 161–197.
prime activate a goal versus a trait? J. Consumer Res. 36(3) Zajonc, R. B. 1968. Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. J. Personality
418–433. Soc. Psych.: Monograph Supplement 9(2, Part 2) 1–27.

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi