Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 13

Psychophysiology, ]]] (2010), 1–13. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA.

Copyright r 2010 Society for Psychophysiological Research


DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01075.x

The effect of musical experience on emotional self-reports


and psychophysiological responses to dissonance

DELPHINE DELLACHERIE,a,b MATHIEU ROY,c LAURENT HUGUEVILLE,d,e,f


ISABELLE PERETZ,c and SÉVERINE SAMSONa,b
a
Laboratoire de Neurosciences Fonctionnelles et Pathologies, CNRS-UMR 8160, University of Lille-Nord de France, Lille, France
b
Epilepsy Unit, La Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, France
c
BRAMS Laboratory, University of Montréal, Montréal, Québec, Canada
d
Université Pierre et Marie Curie-Paris 6, Centre de Recherche de l’Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, UMR-S975, Paris, France
e
Inserm, U975, Paris, France
f
CNRS, UMR 7225, Paris, France

Abstract
To study the influence of musical education on emotional reactions to dissonance, we examined self-reports and
physiological responses to dissonant and consonant musical excerpts in listeners with low (LE: n 5 15) and high (HE:
n 5 13) musical experience. The results show that dissonance induces more unpleasant feelings and stronger phys-
iological responses in HE than in LE participants, suggesting that musical education reinforces aversion to dissonance.
Skin conductance (SCR) and electromyographic (EMG) signals were analyzed according to a defense cascade model,
which takes into account two successive time windows corresponding to orienting and defense responses. These
analyses suggest that musical experience can influence the defense response to dissonance and demonstrate a powerful
role of musical experience not only in autonomic but also in expressive responses to music.
Descriptors: Emotion, Music, Dissonance, Musical experience, Psychophysiology, SCR, EMG, HR

Since Helmholtz (1877), musical dissonance has been considered isolation (Tramo et al., 2001). This phenomenon should be dis-
by psychologists and neuroscientists as a puzzling topic at the tinguished from ‘‘musical’’ dissonance, which refers to disso-
interface between auditory perception and emotional processing. nance manipulated by composers in a harmonic or melodic
The simplest definition of dissonance refers to an unpleasant context in order to induce tension and expectation effects
sensation induced by the simultaneous presentation of two (Meyer, 1956). In the present study, we did not consider musi-
sounds. As proposed by Pythagoras two and half millennia ago, cal dissonance but focused rather on sensory dissonance.
this affective sensation produced by a pair of tones is related to According to neuropsychological, electrophysiological, and
the ratio of the fundamental frequencies of the sounds that are brain imaging studies, it has been suggested that perceptual and
played together. When the ratio is simple, consonance is heard emotional processing of dissonance depends on distinct cerebral
(e.g., octave 2/1; perfect fifth 3/2) and when the ratio is more structures. Whereas perception of dissonance requires the func-
complex, dissonance occurs (e.g., tritone chord, 45/32) (Plomp & tion of auditory cortical areas within the superior temporal gyrus
Levelt, 1965; Tramo, Cariani, Delgutte, & Braida, 2001). This (Fishman, Volkov, Noh, Garell, Bakken, et al., 2001; Peretz,
physical component of dissonance results in a perception of Blood, Penhune, & Zatorre, 2001), its emotional processing in-
beating that is perceived as unpleasant and is referred to as volves various brain structures including mesial temporal lobe
‘‘sensory’’ dissonance, which appears when chords are played in and precuneus regions (Blood, Zatorre, Bermudez, & Evans,
1999; Gosselin, Samson, Adolphs, Noulhiane, Roy, et al., 2006;
The authors are grateful to Laurence Conty, Daniela Sammler, Séve- Koelsch, Fritz, Von Cramon, Muller, & Friederici, 2006).
rine Farley, and Sean Hutchins for their helpful assistance and comments At the behavioral level, it is well known that dissonant chords
on previous versions of the manuscript. This study was supported by a induce more negative valence judgments than consonant chords
PhD scholarship from the Regional Council of Nord-Pas de Calais to (Blood et al., 1999; Brattico, Pallesen, Varyagina, Bailey, Anour-
Delphine Dellacherie and by a grant from ‘‘Agence Nationale pour la ova, et al., 2009; Gosselin et al., 2006; Khalfa, Roy, Rainville,
Recherche’’ of the French Ministry of Research (project no. NT05-3- Dalla Bella, & Peretz, 2008; Koelsch et al., 2006; Koelsch, Remp-
45987) to Séverine Samson and from Eisai Inc. Isabelle Peretz is pis, Sammler, Jentschke, Mietchen, et al., 2007; Pallesen, Brattico,
supported by grants from the Canada Research Chair in Neurocognition Bailey, Korvenoja, Koivisto, et al., 2005; Passynkova, Neubauer,
of Music and Mathieu Roy by a Canadian fellowship from NSERC.
& Scheich, 2007; Peretz et al., 2001; Sammler, Grigutsch, Fritz, &
Address correspondence to: Séverine Samson, Department of Psy-
chology, Université de Lille 3, BP 60 149, 59653 Villeneuve d’Ascq Koelsch, 2007; Schoen, Regnault, Ystad, & Besson, 2005). Even
Cedex, France. E-mail: severine.samson@univ-lille3.fr infants prefer consonance to dissonance, suggesting a natural
1
2 D. Dellacherie et al.

aversion to dissonance very early in life (Masataka, 2006; Trainor Krumhansl, 1997; Steinbeis, Koelsch, & Sloboda, 2006; Witvliet
& Heinmiller, 1998; Trainor, Tsang, & Cheung, 2002; Zentner & & Vrana, 2007), but only three studies specifically explored the
Kagan, 1996). However, it has also been suggested that emotional effect of musical valence on autonomic responses (Nater,
responses to dissonance can be influenced by musical experience. Abbruzzese, Krebs, & Ehlert, 2006; Roy, Mailhot, Gosselin,
As reported by Pallesen et al. (2005) and Schoen et al. (2005), Paquette, & Peretz, 2008; Sammler et al., 2007). A greater HR
adults with musical experience considered dissonant chords as deceleration in response to aversive (heavy metal) music was re-
more unpleasant than did musically untrained individuals. These ported by Nater et al. (2006). Sammler et al. (2007) also found a
self-reported results suggest that aversion to dissonance could in significant decrease of HR induced by dissonant music in com-
some instances be reinforced by formal musical training and pos- parison to consonant music. This cardiac deceleration does not
sibly by learning mechanisms. Further lines of evidence suggest fit with the classic cardiac defense response obtained with aver-
that, at the neurophysiological level, musical experience could fa- sive loud noises, which is mainly characterized by a late accel-
cilitate the processing of dissonance in musicians in comparison to eration (Cook & Turpin, 1997). However, when unpleasant
nonmusicians (Brattico et al., 2009; Minati, D’Incerti, Pietrocini, pictures or sounds are presented, a marked cardiac deceleration
Valentini, Scaioli, et al., 2009; Regnault, Bigand, & Besson, 2001; has been repeatedly reported (Bradley et al., 2001; Cook & Tur-
Schoen et al., 2005). However, the effect of musical education on pin, 1997; Sanchez-Navarro, Martinez-Selva, & Roman, 2006).
emotional responses to dissonance is not a well-established phe- Such a cardiac deceleration is characteristic of an orienting re-
nomenon (Brattico et al., 2009). sponse (Bradley et al., 2001; Cook & Turpin, 1997). Following
To test the effect of musical experience in emotional responses Cacioppo et al. (1997, 2000), this greater response to unpleasant
to dissonance, we recorded psychophysiological correlates of than to pleasant affective stimuli could be interpreted as resulting
musical listening, which is an appropriate method to examine from the negativity bias. According to these results, we could
objective measures of emotional experiences (Bradley, Codispoti, predict a higher HR deceleration in response to dissonant stimuli
Cuthbert, & Lang, 2001; Bradley & Lang, 2000; Lang, Bradley, than to consonant stimuli.
& Cuthbert, 1998; Sanchez-Navarro, Martinez-Selva, & Ro- Although SCR has been traditionally linked to variation of
man, 2006; Witvliet & Vrana, 1995). Several lines of evidence arousal (Bradley et al., 2001; Bradley & Lang, 2000), some ev-
show that physiological changes in skin conductance responses idence suggests that SCR duration and amplitude can also be
(SCRs), heart rate (HR) and electromyographic (EMG) re- influenced by emotional valence (Cacioppo et al., 1997, 2000;
sponses vary systematically with judgments of affective valence Norris, Larsen, & Cacioppo, 2007; Ohman & Wiens, 2003). In
or arousal to emotional stimuli, although evidence in the musical the musical domain, Baumgartner et al. (2006) and Nater et al.
domain is more mixed. (2006) found that SCR was more elevated for unpleasant or
In general, SCR and HR responses that are indexes of au- negatively valenced music than for pleasant or positively va-
tonomic activity are shown to be higher for negative than for lenced music. Therefore, SCR could be larger for dissonant than
positive emotion (Cacioppo, Bernston, Klein, & Poehlmann, for consonant music.
1997; Cacioppo, Bernston, Larsen, Poehlmann, & Ito, 2000; According to the defense cascade model (Bradley et al., 2001;
Taylor, 1991). For example, by comparing the results obtained in Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1997; Ohman & Wiens, 2003), emo-
13 physiological studies, Cacioppo et al. (2000) found that heart tional autonomic responses to aversive stimuli display a two-step
rate was significantly greater during negative than positive emo- dynamic characterized by an initial orienting response followed
tions. The authors interpreted this effect as part of a ‘‘negativity by a subsequent defense response. This model describes an aver-
bias,’’ which was defined in behavioral studies as ‘‘the greater sive motivational circuit that triggers reactions ranging from
impact of negative information on people’s evaluations than orienting to fight/flight. The orienting response elicits a large
comparably extreme positive information’’ (Peeters & Czapinski, SCR response as well as a HR deceleration in an early time
1990). Classical animal and behavioral studies (for review, see window and is part of an automatic lower level appraisal that
Cacioppo et al., 2000; Taylor, 1991) as well as more recent phys- could be processed in a short period of time, without any ap-
iological studies (Ito, Larsen, Smith, & Cacioppo, 1998) inves- parent implication of cortical association areas or of awareness
tigated the concept of ‘‘negativity bias,’’ which can be considered (Ohman & Soares, 1994; Ohman & Wiens, 2003). The defense
as a normal manifestation of adaptive functioning. Cacioppo and response defined as a controlled appraisal of the emotion is re-
colleagues (Cacioppo & Bernston, 1994; Cacioppo, Gardner, & flected by further increase of SCR and HR acceleration in a
Berntson, 1997) have then incorporated the negativity bias in a subsequent time window. In addition to these two steps of re-
more general model of evaluative space in which positive and sponse, HR responses to emotional stimuli typically show a third
negative evaluative processes are assumed to involve separate decelerative component that corresponds to the return to base-
motivational substrates (Lang et al., 1998). In this context, neg- line. Because this third component is not part of the affective
ativity bias refers to a tendency for the negative motivational response per se, we focused here on the two first components of
system to respond more intensely than the positive motivational the HR response. Here, we hypothesized that negative emotions
system to comparable amounts of activation (Ito, Cacioppo, & created by musical dissonance should elicit a similar pattern with
Lang, 1998; Ito et al., 1998). This is the definition that we two steps of responses (an orienting response, followed by a
adopted in the present study, and we hypothesized that this neg- defense response).
ativity bias exists also with music. EMG responses of facial expression provide another index of
In the musical domain, several studies have focused on the somatic activity related to emotional valence (Cacioppo, Klein,
effect of various musical variables on autonomic responses to Bernston, & Hatfield, 1993). More specifically, corrugator ac-
emotional stimuli (Baumgartner, Esslen, & Jancke, 2006; tivity (used in frowning) is increased for unpleasant or negatively
Chapados & Levitin, 2008; Grewe, Nage, Kopiez, & Altenmull- valenced stimuli (pictures: Bradley et al., 2001; mental imagery:
er, 2007; Khalfa, Peretz, Blondin, & Robert, 2002; Khalfa, Roy, Witvliet & Vrana, 1995; sounds: Bradley & Lang, 2000; films:
et al., 2008; Koelsch, Kilches, Steinbeis, & Schelinski, 2008; Ellis & Simons, 2005). Conversely, zygomatic activity increases
Psychophysiology of musical emotion 3

with pleasantness (Ellis & Simons, 2005; Lang et al., 1998; in this study. Based on their responses on a musical experience
Witvliet & Vrana, 1995). Although the corrugator activity in questionnaire, participants were sorted into Low Experience
response to emotional valence in music has been already reported (LE, n 5 15) and High Experience (HE, n 5 12) groups. The
(Roy et al., 2008; Witvliet & Vrana, 2007), no study to our musical experience questionnaire included items related to their
knowledge has recorded corrugator and zygomatic EMG activity music listening habits (listening subscale) as well as their level of
in response to dissonance. Based on previous results in non- musical education and actual practice (practice subscale), in ac-
musical domains, we can hypothesize that corrugator muscles cordance with a multi-dimensional definition of musicianship
known to be influenced by negative valence should be more ac- (Ehrle, 1998). All HE participants, except one who was an au-
tivated when listening to dissonance than to consonance. How- todidact, received training in classical music, and none of them
ever, zygomatic muscles generally modulated by positive valence reported listening to music genres which generally have a lot of
should be more activated in response to consonance than to dis- dissonance, such as contemporary music or free jazz. As dis-
sonance. Since no study interpreted facial EMG responses in played in Table 1, the mean scores on both subscales were higher
relation to the defense cascade model, no specific prediction for HE (mean listening score 5 6, SD 5 1.65 and mean practice
about the dynamic changes of such responses was proposed. score 5 6.92, SD 5 2.57) than LE (mean listening score 5 3.87,
Finally, it has also been demonstrated that automatic phys- SD 5 1.25; mean practice score 5 3.87, SD 5 1.04) participants
iological responses to aversive stimuli can be modulated by our (t(25) 5 3.829, po.005 for listening and t(25) 5 9.16, po.001 for
own experience with the material. Some evidence in a non-mu- practice). Finally, the two groups did not differ on variables
sical domain suggests that the relevance of the task for a given unrelated to musical experience, such as age (mean age for
subject could modulate SCRs. For example, parachutists exhibit LE 5 31.2, (SD 5 9.08); for HE 5 27.2 (SD 5 2.18);
larger skin conductance orienting response to parachutist-rele- t(25) 5 0.94, n.s.), years of education (mean years of education
vant words and pictures than do inexperienced controls (Epstein for LE 5 14.33 (SD 5 3.24); for HE 5 15.83 (SD 5 2.25);
& Fenz, 1962; Fenz & Epstein, 1962). Since musical experience t(25) 5 0.92, n.s.), and gender (for LE: 9M, 6F; for HE: 4M,
acquired through lessons or practice can influence the emotional 8F; w2(1) 5 1.90, n.s). The results remain the same when only
response to dissonance, it seems relevant to explore the effect of participants kept in the SCR and HR analyses were included in
musical experience on emotional reaction to dissonance by com- the analyses.
bining subjective rating judgments with objective autonomic
(SCR and HR) and somatomotor (EMG) measurements in re- Mood Questionnaires
sponse to musical listening. Two mood questionnaires were administered: the State and Trait
For this purpose, consonant musical excerpts and their dis- Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger, 1983) and the Profile of
sonant counterparts of the same musical pieces matched for Mood Scales (POMS; McNair et al., 1992). The STAI comprises
arousal were presented to participants with low and high musical two subscales, one assessing the individual general level of anx-
experience that were carefully controlled in terms of age, sex, iety (i.e., trait anxiety), which is presumed to be stable over time,
education, and mood. Subjective ratings of emotional valence and the other one assessing the present level of anxiety (i.e., state
(according to pleasantness) as well as autonomic (i.e., HR, phasic
SCR) and somatomotor (i.e., facial EMG) indexes related to
Table 1. Description of the Musical Experience of the Two Groups
valence judgments were simultaneously recorded. We predicted
of Participants with Low (LE) and High Experience (HE)
that dissonance will induce more negative valence judgments
than consonance, and this effect would be larger in high than in Low High
low musically experienced participants. For all the participants, experience experience
we also predicted higher SCR and larger HR deceleration in (LE) (HE)
response to dissonance as compared to consonance, these effects (A) Scores on the musical experience questionnaire
being modified by musical experience as well. Based on the de- Global score 4.13 (1.25)n 12.91 (3.21)n
fense cascade model with two-step responses, two successive Listening subscale score 3.87 (1.24)n 6 (1.65)n
patterns of autonomic responses were expected as a function of Practice subscale score 3.87 (1.03)n 6.92 (2.57)n
(B) % of the participants reporting
the time window, early and late time windows corresponding to
Listening Habits
orienting and defense responses, respectively. Therefore, we can Listen music every day 80 91.60
suppose that SCR and HR can be modified by musical experi- Listen music with attention 20 66.60
ence differently in early and late time window. We also predicted Listen classical music 33 58.30
that facial EMG would be modulated by emotional valence: co- Go to concert 46.60 75
Practice
rrugator muscles known to be influenced by negative valence Practice actually 0 33
should be more activated when listening to dissonance than to Have received an institutional training 6.60 75
consonance, whereas zygomatic muscles generally modulated by Have received lessons during at least 3 6.60 92
positive valence should be more activated in response to conso- years
Self-educated 0 8.30
nance than to dissonance.
(C) Details on practice for HE
Age at start of study – 8.54 (3.56)
Duration of the practice – 8.41 (4.30)

Notes: (A) Mean scores on the musical experience questionnaire (stan-


Methods dard deviation in parentheses). (B) Percentage of participants reporting
different listening habits and musical practice details. (C) For HE par-
Participants ticipants, mean age at start of study and mean number of years of musical
Twenty-seven participants (13 men and 14 women), aged be- practice (standard deviation in parentheses).
tween 19 and 31 years (mean age 5 29.41. SD  8.53) took part n
po.05.
4 D. Dellacherie et al.

anxiety). The POMS is a questionnaire comprising 30 items, each Each block consisted of 10 excerpts, including 5 consonant
consisting of an emotional adjective for which subjects have to and 5 dissonant excerpts presented in an alternating order. The
rate to what extent it describes their current mood (0F‘not at orders of presentation of the consonant and dissonant trials
all,’ 4F‘extremely’). Six emotional sub-scales can be derived within each block were counterbalanced within subjects follow-
from the 30 items: Tension, Depression, Anger, Vigor, Fatigue, ing an ABBA design, as well as between subjects with half of the
and Confusion. participants starting with a consonant excerpt and the other half
with a dissonant one. All blocks started with a loud and sudden
Stimuli burst of white noise (50 ms, 85–90 db) intended to elicit a startle
Ten musical excerpts of classical music were taken from a set used response. These startle responses served as another index of in-
in previous studies (Gosselin et al., 2006; Khalfa, Guye, Peretz, dividual electrodermal reactivity and of habituation effect on the
Chapon, Girard, et al., 2008; Peretz et al., 2001; Peretz, Gagnon, signal throughout the experiment.
& Bouchard, 1998). They were instrumental in that they were not Each trial started with a warning signal asking the subject not
originally sung with lyrics. The excerpts were selected to evoke to move in order to avoid contamination of physiological re-
happiness and were played in major mode (e.g., ‘‘Brindisi’’ from cordings by movement artifacts. This signal was followed by a
Verdi’s ‘‘Traviata’’), with a fast tempo (the quarter note value rest period lasting between 7.1 s and 7.7 s, in which subjects were
varies from 80 to 255, conventionally written by M.M. for Met- asked to do nothing. The length of the rest period varied ran-
ronome Marking). All stimuli were transcribed for piano, com- domly in order to reduce subject’s expectancies and response
puter-generated, and delivered with a piano timbre. Each excerpt habituation. The musical excerpt was then played for 7 s and was
had duration of 7 s. The dissonant versions of each original followed by another rest period of 1 s, in which subjects were
consonant excerpt were created by shifting the pitch of the tones again asked not to move. This second rest period allowed the
of the leading voice by one semitone either upward or downward, recording of music-related physiological activity overflows and
leading to 20 dissonant versions of the 10 original consonant also prevented the activity recorded at the end of the musical
excerpts. During the experiment, each consonant version was excerpt from being contaminated by response preparation arti-
presented twice to match the number of dissonant excerpts. facts. The end of this 1-s rest period was indicated by another
signal allowing participants to move. Then, participants were
Experimental Procedure
asked to judge the excerpt they just heard using a rating scale for
The general time course of the experiment is depicted in Figure 1.
emotional valence (from 5 5 ‘unpleasant’ to 5 5 ‘pleasant’)
All participants started the experiment by filling out the musical
and arousal (from 5 5 ‘relaxing’ to 5 5 ‘stimulating’). No time
experience and the mood questionnaires. Then, the physiological
constraints were given for the ratings. All participants signed
sensors were attached while the participants sat comfortably in a
informed consent to participate in the study.
quiet room. Before the experiment started, participants per-
formed a handgrip squeeze task in which they were asked 4 times
to squeeze repeatedly on a handgrip for 7 s. This control task was Data Acquisition and Transformation
performed in order to obtain an index of individual electrodermal SCR, HR, and EMGs were monitored continuously using an
reactivity in each participant before the beginning of the exper- MP150 Biopac system (Biopac Systems, Inc., Goleta, CA) at a
iment. The experimental task comprised four testing blocks. sampling rate of 2000 Hz and processed using AcqKnowledge

A musical
expertence
expert
+ handgrip
mood squeeze Experimental Experimental Experimental Experimental
questionnaires task block 1 block 2 block 3 block 4

startle
l
probe CDCCDCDDCD
+ Musical or
recovery ti l
trials
period DC D C C D C D D C

10 sec ≈ 350 sec

valence
“ l
“please “
“you and
don’tt
don rest can arousal
move” rest period musical excerpt period move” ratings

2 sec 7 1 to 7
7.1 7.7
7 sec 7 sec 1 sec 1 5 sec
1.5 ≈ 10 sec

Figure 1. Experimental procedure: (A) Participants first filled out the musical experience and mood questionnaires and performed the handgrip squeeze
task. The musical excerpts were then presented in four experimental blocks. (B) Each block started with a startle probe. Five consonant and 5 dissonant
excerpts were presented in an alternate order. The blocks either started with a consonant ‘‘C’’ or dissonant ‘‘D’’ excerpt. (C) Each musical trial started
with a rest period in which participants were asked not to move. At the end of each musical excerpt, participants were asked to remain still for 1 s before
they had to rate the valence and arousal of the excerpt.
Psychophysiology of musical emotion 5

software. Skin conductance was recorded on the index and mid- sponses to the musical excerpts. Finally, we used regression an-
dle finger of the non-dominant hand using the EDA finger alyses to explore the relationship between valence ratings and
transducer BSL – SS3LA filled with an isotonic conducting gel. personal (mood and musical experience) and physiological
The signal was filtered between 0.05 and 10 Hz. Then, the SCRs (SCR, facial EMGs, HR) variables. For all analyses, the Fmax
to each musical trial were visually inspected and checked for statistic was used to test if the homogeneity of variance assump-
failures of the measuring device as well as movement-related ar- tion was met. Following the guidelines of Tabachnick and Fidell
tifacts. Trials in which there was electrodermal activity prior to (2001) for similar samples sizes (i.e., within a ratio of 4 to 1), the
the onset of the musical excerpts were excluded. Responses were Fmax was in an acceptable range (i.e., below 10) for all analyses,
selected if they occurred in a 1–4-s latency window following indicating that the variances between samples were sufficiently
stimulus onset. Three participants from the low expertise group homogeneous to proceed with the analyses. Partial eta-squared
that showed no or very feeble responses to grasping and startle (Z2) were used as the effect sizes for the analyses of variance
trials were considered as non-responders and excluded from the (ANOVAs). According to Cohen’s (1988) guidelines, Z2 5 .01
analysis. corresponds to a small effect, Z2 5 .09 to a medium effect, and
Facial EMGs were recorded over the left corrugator and Z2 5 .25 to a large effect.
zygomatic sites as recommended by Fridlund and Cacioppo
(1986), using 8 mm Ag/AgCl shielded electrodes, and were fil- Mood Questionnaires
tered between 100 and 500 Hz. After the recording, EMG was The mean ratings of the mood questionnaires STAI and POMS
rectified using the root mean square function of the software and for the LE and HE groups are displayed in Table 2, along with the
then smoothed by a factor of 200 samples. HR was recorded results of the t-tests to verify whether baseline mood levels dif-
using a bipolar montage with an electrode on the right carotid fered between the LE and HE groups. As can be seen in this table,
artery and another below the left ribs. The signal was filtered (0.5 there were no differences on any of the mood parameters that
to 35 Hz). Instantaneous inter-beat (RR) intervals (in ms), cor- were assessed, confirming that both groups did not differ in this
responding to the inverse of HR, were calculated from the elec- regard.
trocardiogram using a peak detection algorithm to detect
successive R-waves and obtain a continuous RR tachogram.
Careful examination of the electrocardiogram and the tacho- Valence and Arousal Ratings
gram ensured that the automatic R-wave detection procedure Separate ANOVAs with one repeated measure (consonance vs.
had been performed correctly. Careful examination of each par- dissonance) were carried out on the valence and the arousal rat-
ticipant’s electrocardiogram signal led to the exclusion of four ing scores. Figure 2 shows the mean ratings of valence and
participants (3 LE and 1 HE) due to technical failures. Electro- arousal for consonant and dissonant excerpts as a function of
cardiograms of the remaining participants were then cleaned by musical experience. As expected, dissonant excerpts were rated as
removing the trials in which the presence of artifacts prevented more unpleasant than consonant ones (F(1,25) 5 73.19, po.05,
the exact extraction of RR Intervals (RRI). The participants who Z2 5 0.75). This effect was modulated by the degree of musical
were excluded from the SCR analyses were not the same ones experience of the listener, as revealed by the significant Musical
who were excluded from the HR analyses. experience by Dissonance interaction (F(1,25) 5 8.32, po.05,
Z2 5 0.25). HE participants rated dissonant excerpts as more
unpleasant than LE participants did (F(1,25) 5 8.81, po.05,
Results Z2 5 0.26), whereas their ratings of consonant excerpts did not
differ across groups (F(1,25) 5 0.02, p 5 n.s., Z2 5 0.001). The
Before analyzing the data, we checked for group differences be- analysis of arousal ratings showed no effect of Dissonance
tween LE and HE participants on mood questionnaires. Then, (F(1,25) 5 0.28, p 5 n.s., Z2 5 0.015) or of Musical experience
we tested the effects of dissonance and musical experience on (F(1,25) 5 0.97, p 5 n.s., Z2 5 0.51) nor any interaction
valence and arousal ratings, as well as on the physiological re- (F(1,25) 5 0.08, p 5 n.s., Z2 5 0.003).

Table 2. Inter-Group Comparisons of the Mean ( SD) Ratings on the Mood Questionnaires and Electrodermal Reactivity Tests

Musical experience

Dependent variable Low experience High experience Result of t-test


STAI subscales
Trait anxiety 37.33 ( 8.86) 34.83 ( 7.63) t(25) 5 1.51, p 5 n.s.
State anxiety 34.40 ( 7.15) 30.58 ( 5.65) t(25) 5 0.77, p 5 n.s.
POMS subscales
Anger 1.40 ( 1.45) 0.73 ( 1.10) t(25) 5 1.28, p 5 n.s.
Anxiety 1.73 ( 1.62) 1.55 ( 2.25) t(25) 5 0.25, p 5 n.s.
Depression 1.33 ( 1.80) 0.91 ( 1.64) t(25) 5 0.62, p 5 n.s.
Confusion 4.27 ( 1.87) 4.90 ( 0.83) t(25) 5 1.06, p 5 n.s.
Vigor 9.80 ( 2.68) 11.55 ( 3.59) t(25) 5 0.42, p 5 n.s.
Fatigue 3.13 ( 2.92) 4.18 ( 2.27) t(25) 5 0.99, p 5 n.s.
Electrodermal reactivity
Handgrip squeeze 0.45 ( 0.46) 0.48 ( 0.23) t(22) 5 0.28, p 5 n.s.
Startle 0.38 ( 0.34) 0.58 ( 0.40) t(22) 5 0.86, p 5 n.s.

Note: n.s. 5 not significant.


6 D. Dellacherie et al.

Figure 2. Mean valence and arousal ratings of the dissonant and consonant musical excerpts for the low and high experience groups.

Physiological Recordings window, the baseline period was the mean skin conductance level
Electrodermal Responses of the 1-second baseline preceding the onset of the excerpt. For
After exclusion of three non-responder participants, there the second time window, the mean skin conductance level be-
were 12 LE and 12 HE left in the SCR analyses. To make sure tween the 5th and 6th second served as the baseline. The resulting
that the LE and HE groups exhibited similar levels of electrode- values were transformed in log (SCR11) and averaged for each
rmal reactivity, the mean SCRs for the grasping and startle trials participant.
were compared by means of t-tests (Table 2). The results showed An ANOVA with Dissonance (consonance vs. dissonance)
that there were no differences in baseline electrodermal reactivity and Time window (early or later) as within-subjects factors were
between the two groups (Handgrip: t(22) 5 0.28, p 5 n.s.; Star- carried out on the SCR values for the two groups of participants
tle: t(22) 5 0.86, p 5 n.s.). The SCRs to the consonant and dis- (LE vs. HE). The averaged peaks for the LE and HE groups are
sonant musical excerpts were then analyzed for the two groups of displayed in Table 3. SCRs were higher when participants lis-
participants. First, the mean SCR values were extracted by slices tened to dissonant than to consonant excerpts (F(1,22) 5 9.85,
of 500 ms and averaged by musical condition and experimental po.05, Z2 5 0.31), and this effect was influenced by musical ex-
group in order to provide average response curves for the dis- perience, as revealed by the significant interaction
sonant and consonant excerpts and LE and HE participants (see (F(1,22) 5 4.32, po.05, Z2 5 0.16) which demonstrates that the
Figure 3A). Visual inspection of the resulting graph led to the difference in SCRs between consonance and dissonance was
identification of two response periods according to the guidelines higher for HE (F(1,11) 5 7.89, po.05, Z2 5 0.42) than LE
proposed by Dawson (2007). The first response ranged from 1.5 (F(1,11) 5 2.05, p 5 n.s., Z2 5 0.16) group.
to 6 s and corresponded to the orientation response triggered by The effect of Dissonance also marginally interacted with Time
the onset of the excerpt. The second response ranged from 6 to 8 window (F(1,22) 5 4.04, p 5 .057, Z2 5 0.001), the effect of Dis-
seconds and appeared to reflect later responses to the musical sonance being more pronounced in the early (F(1,22) 5 9.01,
excerpts. The amplitude of each SCR was then computed by po.05, Z2 5 0.29) than in the late time window (F(1,22) 5 1.63,
extracting the maximum value of the response in each time win- p 5 n.s., Z2 5 0.066). Finally, the interaction between Disso-
dow and subtracting it from the baseline level. For the first time nance, Time window, and Musical experience was not significant

Table 3. Mean Values ( SD) of the Physological Recordings for Consonant and Dissonant Excerpts by Time Window and Level of Musical
Experience

First window Second window

Consonant Dissonant Consonant Dissonant


SCR amplitude Low experience 0.08 ( 0.08) 0.10 ( 0.07) 0.01 ( 0.03) 0.01 ( 0.03)
Log (maximum 11), mS High experience 0.12 ( 0.08) 0.20 ( 0.17) 0.04 ( 0.05) 0.07 ( 0.03)
Corrugator EMG Low experience 0.03 ( 0.28) 0.06 ( 0.24) 0.03 ( 1.19) 0.94 ( 1.53)
Area under the curve, mVnsec High experience 0.36 ( 0.90) 0.43 ( 0.65) 0.10 ( 01.17) 1.92 ( 4.53)
Zygomatic EMG Low experience 0.12 ( 0.22) 0.03 ( 0.56) 0.05 ( 1.43) 0.10 ( 2.51)
Area under the curve, mVnsec High experience 0.26 ( 0.42) 0.11 ( 0.26) 0.26 ( 2.44) 3.03 ( 6.70)
RR interval
In the first window : Maximum acceleration, msec Low experience 33.73 ( 13.78) 29.50 ( 11.02) 36.82 ( 28.55) 35.92 ( 26.96)
In the second window : Minimum deceleration, msec High experience 24.41 ( 12.64) 29.27 ( 11.44) 32.06 ( 20.57) 32.38 ( 14.66)

Note: All displayed values are differences from baseline.


Psychophysiology of musical emotion 7

A B
Skin Conductance Responses Corrugator EMG
0.150 0
0.60

0.125 0.50
2nd
0.100
window
0.40 1st 2nd window
window
ns

03
.0
30

olt s
μsie men

0.075
1st window

μvo
0.20
0.050
0.10
0.025
0
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 −0.10
−0.025
Time in seconds Time in seconds

C D
Zygomatic EMG R-R interval
1.20 10
8
1.00
2nd window 6 2nd window
0.80

nds
4

eco n
2
0.60 1st
olts

0
window mil llis e
μvo

0.40 −2 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

0.20
−4 1 window
−6
0 −8
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
−10
−0.20
−12
Time in seconds Time in seconds

Figure 3. Average time-courses of the physiological responses during the presentation of consonant and dissonant excerpts for low and high musical
experience groups. Each physiological measure was divided in two phases of responding. (A) For skin conductance responses, a first orientation response
(0–6 s) was followed by a second slower response (6–8 s). Note that the averaging of slight inter-trials and inter-subjects differences in the onsets of the
second non-orientation response smoothes the shape of the mean response curves, compared to individual trials, from which the analyzed responses were
extracted. (B) Corrugator EMG started to diverge between consonant and dissonant excerpts after the 2nd second. (C) Zygomatic EMG started to
diverge between consonant and dissonant excerpts after the 2nd second. (D) RR Intervals first increase between 0–3.5 s (slowing of heart rate) and
decrease between 3.5–7 s (speeding of heart rate).

(F(1,22) 5 0.17, p 5 n.s., Z2 5 0.075). For exploratory reasons, acceleration of heart rate), and then by a final increase of RRI.
we tested if the interaction between Dissonance and Musical ex- The amplitude of the first increase and of the following decrease
perience was different in the two time windows by performing were then extracted and subtracted from a 1-s baseline preceding
ANOVAs in the first and second time window with Dissonance the onset of the musical excerpts. An ANOVA with Dissonance
and Musical experience as factors. The results showed that the and Phase of response was carried out on the RRI for the two
effect of Dissonance did not interact with Musical experience in groups of participants. The amplitude of the first increase and
the early time window (F(1,22) 5 1.78, p 5 n.s., Z2 5 0.075), but secondary decrease in RRI for the HE and LE groups are dis-
it did interact with Musical experience in the second time window played in Table 3. No significant effect of Dissonance
(F(1,22) 5 4.11, po.05, Z2 5 0.16), indicating that SCRs were (F(1,21) 5 0.001, p 5 n.s., Z2 5 0.00) or of Musical experience
higher for dissonant than for consonant excerpts in the late time (F(1,21) 5 0.01, p 5 n.s., Z2 5 0.00) was observed. Only the ex-
window. This effect holds for HE (t(11) 5 2.39, po.05, pected effect of Phase was observed (F(1,21) 5 162.53, po.05,
Z2 5 0.25) but not for LE (t(11) 5 1.91, po.05, Z2 5 0.05) par- Z2 5 0.87), confirming that RRI were higher during the first in-
ticipants. crease than during the following decrease.

Heart Rate Facial Electromyography


There were 12 LE and 11 HE participants left in the HR The corrugator and zygomatic EMG responses to musical
analyses. RRI was extracted by slices of 500 ms and averaged by excerpts were calculated as the difference between the raw signal
musical condition and experimental group in order to provide during each musical excerpts and the mean activity observed in
average response curves for the dissonant and consonant ex- an 800-ms baseline preceding the onset of the excerpts. The re-
cerpts and LE and HE participants (see Figure 3D). Inspection sulting response curves were then averaged by slices of 100 ms for
of the mean RRI curves revealed a typical triphasic pattern the dissonant and consonant excerpts and for LE and HE groups
(Bradley et al., 2001) comprising an initial increase of RRI (i.e., (see Figures 3B and 3C). Visual inspection of the resulting graphs
deceleration of heart rate), followed by a decrease of RRI (i.e., indicated that facial muscles appeared to show two phases of
8 D. Dellacherie et al.

responding: a first phase ranging from 0 to 2 s, where there were showing the largest decreases in valence ratings in response to
no differences between the consonant and dissonant trials, and a dissonance. A final correlational analysis indicated that musical
second phase from 2 to 7 s, where responses to consonant and experience was also correlated to the amount of zygomatic
dissonant musical excerpts started to diverge. The area under the activity in response to dissonance (r(26) 5 0.59, po.05),
curve was extracted within these two time windows and was suggesting that musical experience predicted zygomatic
averaged for consonant and dissonant versions for each partic- responses to dissonance, which in turn strongly predicted the
ipant. The averaged area under the curve for the LE and HE decreases in valence ratings in response to dissonance.
groups are displayed in Table 3. An ANOVA with two repeated
measures, Dissonance and Time window, was carried out on the
averaged area under the curve for the LE and HE groups. Discussion
Corrugator activity differed between consonant and disso-
nant excerpts as a function of Time window, as revealed by the The purpose of our study was to determine the effect of musical
significant interaction between Dissonance and Time window experience on emotional reaction to dissonance by recording
(F(1,25) 5 4.09, p 5 .05, Z2 5 0.14). Decomposition of the inter- emotional self-reports as well as their psychophysiological corre-
action revealed that corrugator activity was higher for dissonant lates. For this purpose, we compared the emotional responses of
than for consonant excerpts in the second window two categories of listeners. One consisted of nonmusicians with
(F(1,25) 5 4.06, p 5 .05, Z2 5 0.14), but not in the first window low musical experience (LE). The other one was composed of
(F(1,25) 5 0.40, p 5 n.s., Z2 5 0.02). Zygomatic activity differed musicians with high musical experience (HE) who had received at
as a function of Dissonance, Time window, and Musical expe- least 3 years of formal training or who were still practicing a mu-
rience, as revealed by the significant interaction between the three sical instrument although they did not reach professional levels.
factors (F(1,25) 5 4.09, po.05, Z2 5 0.14). Decomposition of Therefore, we were able to investigate the interaction between
the interaction showed that there was no difference between musical experience and emotional pleasantness judgments on au-
consonant and dissonant excerpts in the first time window tonomic and somatic responses by measuring SCR, HR, and fa-
(F(1,25) 5 0.01, p 5 n.s., Z2 5 0.00) whereas zygomatic activity cial EMG for the zygomatic and corrugator muscles in response to
was higher for dissonant than for consonant excerpts in the sec- consonant and dissonant musical excerpts. Before we discuss the
ond window for the HE (F(1,11) 5 4.84, p 5 .05, Z2 5 0.31), but results in further detail, we should first point out that the two
not for the LE group (F(1,14) 5 0.01, p 5 n.s., Z2 5 0.00). groups did not differ in terms of age, sex, education, or mood.
Moreover, given that the only difference between the stimuli con-
Regression Analyses cerned the pitch shift of the melodic line, tempo differences could
Stepwise Regressions not have contaminated physiological measurements, in contrast to
In order to assess the relationship between personal variables, most previous studies on musical emotions where different musical
valence ratings, and physiological responses, we performed two excerpts were used to test different emotions.
stepwise regressions with the effects of dissonance on valence Behavioral Measures
ratings as the dependent variable and physiological responses As predicted, the behavioral results confirm previous find-
(SCR, facial EMGs, and RRI within all time windows) and per- ings, indicating that listening to dissonance induces a more neg-
sonal variables (Musical experience and STAI and POMS sub- ative affect than listening to consonance (Blood et al., 1999;
scales) as the independent variables. The goal of these regressions Gosselin et al., 2006; Koelsch et al., 2006; Pallesen et al., 2005;
was to identify the physiological responses and personal variables Passynkova et al., 2007; Peretz et al., 2001; Sammler et al., 2007).
that best predicted the effects of dissonance on valence ratings. Arousal ratings, which were used as a proxy for motivational
For all the variables included in the analysis, the difference be- activation (Ito, Larsen, et al., 1998) did not differ between con-
tween the mean value for the dissonant and consonant excerpts sonant and dissonant excerpts. In agreement with Pallesen et al.
(dissonant–consonant) was used as an index of the effects of (2005) and Schoen et al. (2005), we also found that the effect of
dissonance on these variables. Because the regression models in- dissonance on emotional valence judgments is more salient in HE
cluded all the physiological measurements, we only kept the than in LE participants. Given that arousal judgments obtained
participants for which SCR and HR data were available (9 LE for consonant and dissonant stimuli did not differ between the
and 11 HE). two groups of listeners, we can therefore suggest that musical
The results of the first stepwise regression revealed that experience modulates valence judgments. Moreover, regression
zygomatic activity in the second time window was the best pre- analyses showed that musical experience was a very good pre-
dictor of the decrease in valence in reaction to dissonant excerpts dictor of valence ratings in the present study, suggesting that
(r 5 0.61, po.05). None of the other variables (SCRs, corrugator musical experience may have enhanced the participants’ sensi-
activity, RRI, and zygomatic activity in the first time window) tivity to dissonance. However, since almost all HE participants
significantly improved the proportion of variance explained by were trained in classical music, it remains to be determined if the
the model once the variance explained by the zygomatic activity observed results can be generalized to training in other musical
in the second time window was taken into consideration. The genres more marked by dissonance, such as free jazz or contem-
results of the second stepwise regression showed that musical porary music. Nevertheless, this result appears to be consistent
experience was the best predictor of the decrease in valence in with data of another study (Bigand, Parncutt, & Lerdahl, 1996)
reaction to dissonant excerpts (r 5 0.53, po.05). Again, all showing that dissonant chords induce stronger ratings of musical
other personal variables (STAI and POMS subscales) did not tension than consonant chords, this effect being more pro-
significantly improve the proportion of variance explained by the nounced in musicians than in nonmusicians. We can therefore
model. Thus, the participants who scored higher on the musical conclude that musical experience can modulate emotional judg-
experience questionnaire or who had the largest increases in ment of music even if the role of the specific background remains
zygomatic activity in reaction to dissonant excerpts were the ones to be clarified.
Psychophysiology of musical emotion 9

Physiological Responses response in the first time window and the (more controlled) de-
The analysis of physiological measures revealed that disso- fense response in the second one. Curiously, such a biphasic re-
nance elicited stronger responses than consonance in the two sponse is rarely seen in most psychophysiological studies of
groups of participants. Given that dissonant excerpts were emotion, where visual stimuli are used to induce emotions. For
judged by all participants as more unpleasant than consonant instance, in Bradley and Lang’s study (2000), although the affec-
excerpts but equally arousing, we interpret this reaction to dis- tive pictures had a 6-s duration, no second phasic response after
sonance as resulting from the emotional negativity bias, defined the initial orienting response was observed. Classically, a ‘‘more
as the tendency for the negative motivational system to respond sustained’’ electrodermal response is observed as defense re-
more intensely than the positive motivational system to compa- sponse (Norris et al., 2007; Ohman & Wiens, 2003). One expla-
rable amounts of activation (Cacioppo et al., 2000; Taylor, 1991). nation for this original finding could be that musical emotions
The main finding of our study confirmed that autonomic and unfold in time (Grewe et al., 2007) whereas for pictures, all the
somatomotor responses to musical dissonance are influenced by relevant emotional information is available immediately. Inter-
musical experience, with HE participants presenting stronger estingly, musical experience seems to have contributed to the
physiological responses to dissonance in comparison to conso- enhancement of this second, more controlled response. Indeed,
nance than LE participants. Since HE participants judge disso- after verifying that baseline electrodermal reactivity did not differ
nance as even more unpleasant than consonance as compared to between the two groups of participants, we showed that the SCR
LE participants, the differential reactivity to dissonance between difference between dissonance and consonance is more pro-
the two groups seems to be related to the subjective ratings of nounced in HE than in LE group, as predicted. Subsequent SCR
emotional valence. analysis showed that musical experience interacted with disso-
nance in the second but not in the first time window, suggesting
HR Measures that these orienting and defense responses were differently mod-
HR is normally responsive to the affective valence of the ulated by musical experience.
stimuli. Emotional stimuli generally prompt a triphasic HR re- The orienting response is known to be processed at a pre-
sponse characterized by an initial brief deceleration, followed by attentive level and without implication of cortical association
a short acceleration and a late moderate deceleration (Lang et al., areas (Ohman & Wiens, 2003). The orienting SCR to dissonance
1997; Sanchez-Navarro et al., 2006). Although we found the that we observed in the present study could be interpreted as an
predicted triphasic response when listening to the musical ex- automatic response reflecting an emotional unconscious pro-
cerpts, it was not modulated by dissonance. This result does not cessing. Given that this orienting SCR was higher for dissonance
seem to be consistent with previous findings obtained with un- than for consonance in HE and LE participants, we suggest that
pleasant pictures (Bradley et al., 2001). However, these authors nonmusicians can discriminate dissonance from consonance on
found an effect of valence on HR response only with high (e.g., the basis of emotional feeling as musicians do. We were not able
erotic pictures or mutilation scenes) but not with low arousal to demonstrate an interaction between musical experience and
pictures from International Affective Picture System (Lang, dissonance in this first time window, although larger SCRs were
Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1999). Similarly, Sammler et al. (2007) recorded for HE than for LE participants. This finding indicates
found an effect of dissonance on HR deceleration with real but that LE as well as HE participants showed stronger orienting
not synthetic excerpts that might have been more arousing than responses to dissonance than to consonance. We note that this
our computer-synthesized versions of the stimuli. It might be result was observed despite the lack of formal musical training in
therefore possible that the musical stimuli used in the present LE participants.
study do not induce a sufficiently high emotional response to At a more evaluative level (defense response in the second
affect HR. Notwithstanding the fact that we did not observe any time window), we also observed larger responses to dissonance
significant difference in HR responses between the two emotional (unpleasant) than to consonance (pleasant) but only in HE par-
conditions, the successive phases of decelerative and accelerative ticipants. This response seems to be enhanced in musicians. The
cardiac responses clearly argue in favor of the two-step defensive influence of musical valence on the SCR in the second time win-
cascade model (Bradley et al., 2001; Lang et al., 1997). dow is compatible with previous results demonstrated by re-
cording the tonic (and not phasic) SCRs when listening to
SCR musical stimuli for more than 1 min (Baumgartner et al., 2006;
The analysis of phasic SCR revealed two different peaks of Nater et al., 2006). The fact that a sustained and higher response
response to emotional musical excerpts in the 2–6 s and in the was obtained for dissonance than for consonance suggests that
6–8 s windows. As the late response could not be ascribed to the dissonance affects a more elaborate processing in addition to the
onset of the musical excerpt, this second bump indicates a sec- automatic response. This later processing of musical stimuli
ond, superimposed skin conductance response. Indeed, SCRs are could be specific to musicians. An interpretation of this result is
monophasic, and a secondary increase indicates that another that formal musical learning led musicians to consciously reject
SCR has been triggered after the first one (Dawson, 2007). These dissonant stimuli in a controlled defense response that follows the
two successive responses match our hypothesis based on a two- pre-attentive orienting one. Taken together, analysis of SCR
step cascade model and are consistent with the HR response suggests that musical experience could modulate musical disso-
pattern (Bradley et al., 2001). The initial response, which occurs nance processing, particularly in the late SCR. This result indi-
immediately after the onset of the stimulus (early time window) cates that musical education and training enhance emotional
and the following response beginning a few seconds later (late response to dissonance.
time window) are both larger for dissonant than for consonant An alternative interpretation of these results could be that
excerpts. Based on the cascade defense model (Bradley et al., stronger SCRs to dissonance than to consonance may be related
2001; Lang et al., 1997; Ohman & Wiens, 2003), these two suc- to an effect of unfamiliarity of dissonance. It is clear that con-
cessive responses could correspond to the (automatic) orienting sonant excerpts are more frequent or familiar than dissonant
10 D. Dellacherie et al.

ones because we are immersed in a consonant musical environ- ms. Therefore, this late response cannot be interpreted as an
ment. Moreover, Terhard (1984) hypothesized that prenatal ex- orienting automatic response. Following the two-step cascade
posure to the overtone structure of maternal speech could serve model and given the coherent HR and SCR responses in regard
as the basis for developing preferences for consonance. It is also to this model, we thus propose to interpret a posteriori the late
clear that musical experience contributes to the fact that some EMG reactivity (more than 2 s) as reflecting more controlled
people are more exposed to consonance than others are, even psychological processes related to conscious emotional evalua-
more so if the formation is classical as it is in the present study. It tion, which is characteristic of the defense response (Ohman &
is therefore impossible to differentiate the pleasantness induced Wiens, 2003). This interpretation is confirmed by the regression
by consonance from the frequency or exposure effects (Zajonc, analysis, which shows a strong relation between EMG responses
1980) and to dissociate the unpleasantness to dissonance from a and valence ratings. Indeed, the zygomatic activity in the second
novelty or incongruity effect. However, the aim of the study was window was the best predictor of valence ratings.
not to explain the origin of aversion to dissonance, which is still a Moreover, 2 s after the onset of the aversive stimulus, co-
hotly debated topic (Hauser & McDermott, 2003), but rather to rrugator activity was higher for dissonant than for consonant
explore the relationships between physiological and self-re- excerpts, confirming that such a measure is relevant to the study
ported measures of emotional responses to dissonance (Bradley of emotional reaction to music (Roy et al., 2008; Witvliet &
et al., 2001; Ohman & Wiens, 2003). Because subjective emo- Vrana, 2007). One probable reason why this facial response
tional ratings and physiological responses converge, the inter- seems to take more time to be initiated in the case of music
pretation of physiological responses as reflecting emotional compared to pictures (Bradley et al., 2001) is the fact that the
reactions to dissonance appears highly probable, even if this musical emotions unfold in time whereas for pictures, all the
emotion might be at least partly explained by the unfamiliarity relevant emotional information is immediately available. More
of dissonance. studies on the dynamics of the response to musical emotion are
Several lines of evidence in non-musical domains showed that needed to clarify the specificity of emotional responses to music
the relevance of the task for a given subject plays a role in mod- in comparison to other types of stimuli, such as faces or brief
ulating the SCR (Dindo & Fowles, 2008; Epstein & Fenz, 1962; sounds.
Fenz & Epstein, 1962; Lang et al., 1998; Ohman & Soares, 1994; In addition, we discovered that the late increase of zygomatic
Perpina, Leonard, Bond, Bond, & Banos, 1998; Stormark, La- contraction 2 s after the stimulus onset was specific to experienced
berg, Nordby, & Hugdahl, 2000). These studies, carried out with participants when they were listening to dissonant excerpts. This
psychiatric patients and normal volunteers, revealed that higher unexpected result seems to be in contradiction with the responses
SCRs specific to a given object were dependent on individual ex- observed in other studies showing such a response with pleasant
perience. For example, studies examining phobic individuals stimuli (Ellis & Simons, 2005; Lang et al., 1998; Witvliet & Vrana,
(Lang et al., 1998; Ohman & Soares, 1994) showed hyperactivity 1995). This zygomatic activity might be interpreted as an ironic
to phobic objects, by expressing an intense and sustained fear in smile or as a grimace related to the displeasure induced by disso-
response to stimuli that were not necessarily dangerous. According nance. Since we witnessed that some participants paradoxically
to Mineka and Ohman (2002), the learning of fear could arise laughed (Ansfield, 2007; Craig & Patrick, 1985; Keltner & Bon-
from a classic Pavlovian conditioning: phobic patients would learn anno, 1997; Papa & Bonanno, 2008; Prkachin & Solomon, 2008) at
to be frightened by a neutral stimulus because of an association the dissonant excerpts, we tend to favor the former interpretation,
between such a stimulus and an aversive event. Because formal although we lack objective empirical data to confirm it. Regression
musical education trains listeners to detect and reject dissonance analysis revealed that more unpleasant dissonances were associated
when it is not integrated in an appropriate musical context, we can with stronger zygomatic activities and that this smile/grimace is the
hypothesize by analogy that musical experience leads to a hyper- best predictor of the subjective responses to dissonance. The
sensitivity to dissonance. Consequently, HE participants could zygomatic response could therefore constitute not only a controlled
produce larger SCRs to dissonance than do LE participants in the but also a communicative part of the emotional response to dis-
same way as phobic subjects respond to phobia-relevant stimuli sonance, and this reaction could be more developed in HE partic-
(Lang et al., 1998). In other terms, even if aversion for dissonance ipants than in LE participants. Mimics to dissonance would have a
is natural (Schellenberg & Trainor, 1996; Zentner & Kagan, 1996) communicative function that musical learning might have contrib-
or influenced by an exposure effect (Witvliet & Vrana, 2007), HE uted to create in HE participants. We could hypothesize that ex-
participants could be seen as conditioned to react to dissonance, perience enhances the ability to communicate musically induced
resulting in an enhanced autonomic reactivity. negative emotions. Taken together, these results show that musical
experience plays a role in physiological and communicative re-
EMG Measures sponses induced by dissonance that are linked to emotional valence.
Another index of the emotional appraisal of the stimuli was Based on our results, it seems that dissonance produces larger
the EMG responses. Corrugator and zygomatic muscles are SCR and EMG responses in HE than LE listeners. The results
known to accompany unpleasant and pleasant emotions, re- could be further explained by brain reorganization induced by
spectively. In the present study, we found a response of these musical experience. Indeed, even in amateur musicians or in
muscles that arose late (during the second time window), which children, musical training can produce functional and morpho-
probably reflected a controlled mechanism. We can clearly see logical brain changes in auditory areas (Gaser & Schlaug, 2003;
that there is no zygomatic activity before 2 s after the onset of the Schneider, Scherg, Dosch, Specht, Gutschalk, & Rupp, 2002) as
stimulus. Thus, the zygomatic response to musical excerpts is well as in emotional structures such as the amygdala and the
particularly late. This cannot be explained by the characteristics insula (James, Britz, Vuilleumier, Hauert, & Michel, 2008), the
of EMG latencies. Indeed, unlike skin conductance responses, anterior cingulate cortex (Foss, Altschuler, & James, 2007) and
which generally take around 2 s to develop because of sweat the frontal-lobe areas (Koelsch, Fritz, Schulze, Alsop, & Schl-
gland physiology, EMG responses can be triggered within 100 aug, 2005; Minati et al., 2009).
Psychophysiology of musical emotion 11

Taken together, based on the obtained physiological results, Conclusion


we argue for the existence of a first automatic response to dis-
sonance, which is mainly characterized by an orienting SCR. The data of the present study emphasize the effect of musical
This initial physiological reaction seems to be followed by a sec- experience on emotional responses to music and the role of emo-
ond phase, which is less automatic and more controlled. This tional valence in determining the autonomic and somatomotor
second step is characterized by facial EMG responses and a sec- components of these responses. The results indicate that SCR and
ond non-orienting SCR. The first response suggests the existence EMG responses are sensitive to dissonance. These measures
of an alarm system that can automatically interpret dissonant therefore appear to be appropriate tools to assess emotional re-
music as an aversive stimulus and then evaluate this stimulus in action to unpleasant music. Stronger responses to dissonant than
terms of displeasure. In the nonmusical domain, an alarm system to consonant music were observed in the orienting SCR, and there
in response to fear was shown to involve the amygdala (Liddell, were no differences between HE and LE participants in the am-
Brown, Kemp, Barton, Das, et al., 2005). Given that responses plitude of this response, suggesting that the first step of emotional
of the autonomic nervous system are linked to the amygdala and response to dissonance could be independent of musical experi-
other structures such as the cingulate cortex, the effect of dis- ence. Late skin conductance and EMG responses (corrugator and
sonance on SCR is compatible with neuroimaging studies sug- zygomatic) confirmed the presence of a subsequent, more con-
gesting that affective responses to dissonance could mainly trolled, response to dissonance. Moreover, whereas corrugator
involve mesio-temporal lobe structures (Blood et al., 1999; Ko- activity when listening to dissonance was found in all participants,
elsch et al., 2006, 2007). The stronger late responses obtained in late SCRs and zygomatic activity were found only in experienced
HE than in LE participants (second phase response in SCR and musicians, suggesting a specific emotional response in this group.
zygomatic response) seem consistent with the more important This specific reaction expressed by stronger physiological late re-
involvement of cortical structures such as the cingulate cortex or sponses to dissonance in HE than in LE participants was con-
frontal lobe areas in musicians than in nonmusicians in response firmed by self-report responses. This suggests that experience
to dissonance and consonance (Foss et al., 2007; Minati et al., could influence the negativity bias. An interpretation is that mu-
2009). Considering that the cingulate cortex is known to con- sical education could have reinforced the representation of dis-
tribute in regulating autonomic functions as well as in attentional sonance for musicians by a long and sustained associative
control (Critchley, Mathias, Josephs, O’Doherty, Zanini, et al., learning between dissonance and unpleasant emotions. These re-
2003; Devinsky, Morrell, & Vogt, 1995), we may hypothesize sults add arguments in favor of both physiological and psycho-
that the SCR and the activity observed with fMRI in the cingu- logical origins of the feeling of dissonance. Moreover, the smile
late cortex in musicians may reflect a similar mechanism, result- observed in musicians represents an original finding indicating
ing in a hypersensitivity to dissonance. This hyperactivation may that musical experience influences not only conscious appraisal of
result in a stronger negativity bias observed in autonomic reac- emotional significance but also its means of communication. In
tivity in HE than in LE participants in the second phase of pro- other terms, learned aesthetic preference might play a powerful
cessing dissonance. role in affective and expressive response to music.

REFERENCES

Ansfield, M. E. (2007). Smiling when distressed: When a smile is a frown Cacioppo, J. T., Bernston, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlmann, K. M., &
turned upside down. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, Ito, T. A. (2000). The psychophysiology of emotion. In R. Lewis & J.
763–775. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), The handbook of emotion (2nd Edition, pp.
Baumgartner, T., Esslen, M., & Jancke, L. (2006). From emotion per- 173–191). New York: Guilford Press.
ception to emotion experience: Emotions evoked by pictures and Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1997). Beyond
classical music. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60, 34–43. bipolar conceptualizations and measures: The case of attitudes and
Bigand, E., Parncutt, R., & Lerdahl, F. (1996). Perception of musical evaluative space. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 3–25.
tension in short chord sequences: The influence of harmonic function, Cacioppo, J. T., Klein, D. J., Bernston, G. G., & Hatfield, E. (1993). The
sensory dissonance, horizontal motion, and musical training. psychophysiology of emotion. In Handbook of emotions (pp. 119–
Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 125–141. 142). New York: Guilford Press.
Blood, A. J., Zatorre, R. J., Bermudez, P., & Evans, A. C. (1999). Emo- Chapados, C., & Levitin, D. J. (2008). Cross-modal interactions in the
tional responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with experience of musical performances: Physiological correlates. Cogni-
activity in paralimbic brain regions. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 382–387. tion, 108, 639–651.
Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in pic- (2nd Edition). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
ture processing. Emotion, 1, 276–298. Cook, E. I., & Turpin, G. (1997). Differentiating orienting, startle, and
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Affective reactions to acoustic defense responses: The role of affect and its implications for psycho-
stimuli. Psychophysiology, 37, 204–215. pathology. In Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational pro-
Brattico, E., Pallesen, K. J., Varyagina, O., Bailey, C., Anourova, I., cesses (pp. 137–164). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Jarvenpaa, M., et al. (2009). Neural discrimination of nonprototyp- Craig, K. D., & Patrick, C. J. (1985). Facial expression during induced
ical chords in music experts and laymen: An MEG study. Journal of pain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1080–1091.
Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 2230–2244. Critchley, H. D., Mathias, C. J., Josephs, O., O’Doherty, J., Zanini, S.,
Cacioppo, J. T., & Bernston, G. G. (1994). Relationship between atti- Dewar, B. K., et al. (2003). Human cingulate cortex and autonomic
tudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the control: Converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. Brain, 126,
separability of positive and negative substrates. Psychological 2139–2152.
Bulletin, 3, 401–423. Dawson, G. J. (2007). The electrodermal system. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G.
Cacioppo, J. T., Bernston, G. G., Klein, D. J., & Poehlmann, K. M. Tassinary, & G. G. Bernston (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology
(1997). The psychophysiology of emotion across the lifespan. Annual (Third Edition, pp. 159–181). Cambridge: Cambridge University
Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 17, 27–74. Press.
12 D. Dellacherie et al.

Devinsky, O., Morrell, M. J., & Vogt, B. A. (1995). Contributions of Krumhansl, C. L. (1997). An exploratory study of musical emotions and
anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain, 118, 279–306. psychophysiology. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51,
Dindo, L., & Fowles, D. C. (2008). The skin conductance orienting re- 336–353.
sponse to semantic stimuli: Significance can be independent of Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Motivated at-
arousal. Psychophysiology, 45, 111–118. tention: Affect, activation, and action. In P. J. Lang, R. F. Simons, &
Ehrle, N. (1998). Traitement temporel de l’information auditive et lobe M. T. Balaban (Eds.), Attention and orienting: Sensory and moti-
temporal. Unpublished thesis, Université de Reims Champagne- vational processes (pp. 97–135). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Ardennes. Associates.
Ellis, E., & Simons, R. F. (2005). The impact of music on subjective and Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1998). Emotion, mo-
physiological indices of emotion while viewing films. Psychomusicol- tivation, and anxiety: Brain mechanisms and psychophysiology.
ogy, 19, 15–40. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 1248–1263.
Epstein, S., & Fenz, D. (1962). Theory and experiment on the measure- Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1999). International
ment of approach-avoidance conflict. Journal of Abnormal and Social Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical manual and affective rat-
Psychology, 64, 97–112. ings. Gainsville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology,
Fenz, D., & Epstein, S. (1962). Measurement of approach-avoidance University of Florida.
conflict along a stimulus dimension by a thematic apperception test. Liddell, B. J., Brown, K. J., Kemp, A. H., Barton, M. J., Das, P., Peduto,
Journal of Personality, 30, 613–632. A., et al. (2005). A direct brainstem-amygdala-cortical ‘alarm’ system
Fishman, Y. I., Volkov, I. O., Noh, M. D., Garell, P. C., Bakken, H., for subliminal signals of fear. NeuroImage, 24, 235–243.
Arezzo, J. C., et al. (2001). Consonance and dissonance of musical Masataka, N. (2006). Preference for consonance over dissonance by
chords: Neural correlates in auditory cortex of monkeys and humans. hearing newborns of deaf parents and of hearing parents. Develop-
The Journal of Neurophysiology, 86, 2761–2788. mental Science, 9, 46–50.
Foss, A. H., Altschuler, E. L., & James, K. H. (2007). Neural correlates McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1992). Profile of mood
of the Pythagorean ratio rules. NeuroReport, 18, 1521–1525. states (Revised). San Diego, CA: EdITS: Educational and Industrial
Fridlund, A. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Guidelines for human elect- Testing Service.
romyographic research. Psychophysiology, 23, 567–589. Meyer, L. B. (1956). Emotion and meaning in music. Chicago: University
Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Brain structures differ between of Chicago Press.
musicians and non-musicians. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, Minati, L., Rosazza, C., D’Incerti, L., Pietrocini, E., Valentini, L.,
9240–9245. Scaioli, V., et al. (2009). Functional MRI/event-related potential
Gosselin, N., Samson, S., Adolphs, R., Noulhiane, M., Roy, M., Has- study of sensory consonance and dissonance in musicians and non-
boun, D., et al. (2006). Emotional responses to unpleasant music musicians. NeuroReport, 20, 87–92.
correlates with damage to the parahippocampal cortex. Brain, 129, Mineka, S., & Ohman, A. (2002). Phobias and preparedness: The selec-
2585–2592. tive, automatic, and encapsulated nature of fear. Biological Psy-
Grewe, O., Nage, F., Kopiez, R., & Altenmuller, E. (2007). Emotions chiatry, 52, 927–937.
over time: Synchronicity and development of subjective, physiolog- Nater, U. M., Abbruzzese, E., Krebs, M., & Ehlert, U. (2006). Sex
ical, and facial affective reactions to music. Emotion, 7, 774–788. differences in emotional and psychophysiological responses to
Hauser, M. D., & McDermott, J. (2003). The evolution of the music musical stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 62,
faculty: A comparative perspective. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 663–668. 300–308.
Helmholtz, H. (1954/1877). On the sensation of tones. New York: Dover. Norris, C. J., Larsen, J. T., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). Neuroticism is
Ito, T. A., Cacioppo, J. T., & Lang, P. J. (1998). Eliciting affect using the associated with larger and more prolonged electrodermal responses to
International Affective Picture System: Trajectories through evalua- emotionally evocative pictures. Psychophysiology, 44, 823–826.
tive space. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 855–879. Ohman, A., & Soares, J. J. (1994). ‘‘Unconscious anxiety’’: Phobic re-
Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative sponses to masked stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103,
information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in 231–240.
evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Ohman, A., & Wiens, S. (2003). On the automaticity of autonomic re-
chology, 75, 887–900. sponses in emotion: An evolutionary perspective. In R. J. Davidson,
James, C. E., Britz, J., Vuilleumier, P., Hauert, C. A., & Michel, C. M. K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective
(2008). Early neuronal responses in right limbic structures mediate sciences (pp. 256–275). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
harmony incongruity processing in musical experts. Neuroimage, 42, Pallesen, K. J., Brattico, E., Bailey, C., Korvenoja, A., Koivisto, J.,
1597–1608. Gjedde, A., et al. (2005). Emotion processing of major, minor, and
Keltner, D., & Bonanno, G. A. (1997). A study of laughter and disso- dissonant chords: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
ciation: Distinct correlates of laughter and smiling during bereave- Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1060, 450–453.
ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 687–702. Papa, A., & Bonanno, G. A. (2008). Smiling in the face of adversity: The
Khalfa, S., Guye, M., Peretz, I., Chapon, F., Girard, N., Chauvel, P., et interpersonal and intrapersonal functions of smiling. Emotion, 8,
al. (2008). Evidence of lateralized anteromedial temporal structures 1–12.
involvement in musical emotion processing. Neuropsychologia, 46, Passynkova, N., Neubauer, H., & Scheich, H. (2007). Spatial organiza-
2485–2493. tion of EEG coherence during listening to consonant and dissonant
Khalfa, S., Peretz, I., Blondin, J. P., & Robert, M. (2002). Event-related chords. Neuroscience Letters, 412, 6–11.
skin conductance responses to musical emotions in humans. Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in eval-
Neuroscience Letters, 328, 145–149. uations: The distinction between affective and informational nega-
Khalfa, S., Roy, M., Rainville, P., Dalla Bella, S., & Peretz, I. (2008). tivity effects. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review
Role of tempo entrainment in psychophysiological differentiation of of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 33–60). New York: Wiley.
happy and sad music? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 68, Peretz, I., Blood, A. J., Penhune, V., & Zatorre, R. (2001). Cortical
17–26. deafness to dissonance. Brain, 124, 928–940.
Koelsch, S., Fritz, T., Schulze, K., Alsop, D., & Schlaug, G. (2005). Peretz, I., Gagnon, L., & Bouchard, B. (1998). Music and emotion:
Adults and children processing music: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, Perceptual determinants, immediacy, and isolation after brain
25, 1068–1076. damage. Cognition, 68, 111–141.
Koelsch, S., Fritz, T., Von Cramon, D. Y., Muller, K., & Friederici, A. Perpina, C., Leonard, T., Bond, J. T., Bond, A., & Banos, R. (1998).
D. (2006). Investigating emotion with music: An fMRI study. Human Selective processing of food- and body-related information and au-
Brain Mapping, 27, 239–250. tonomic arousal in patients with eating disorders. The Spanish Journal
Koelsch, S., Kilches, S., Steinbeis, N., & Schelinski, S. (2008). Effects of of Psychology, 1, 3–10.
unexpected chords and of performer’s expression on brain responses Plomp, R., & Levelt, W. J. (1965). Tonal consonance and critical band-
and electrodermal activity. PLoS ONE, 3, 2631. width. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 38, 548–560.
Koelsch, S., Remppis, A., Sammler, D., Jentschke, S., Mietchen, D., Prkachin, K. M., & Solomon, P. E. (2008). The structure, reliability and
Fritz, T., et al. (2007). A cardiac signature of emotionality. European validity of pain expression: Evidence from patients with shoulder
Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 3328–3338. pain. Pain, 139, 267–274.
Psychophysiology of musical emotion 13

Regnault, P., Bigand, E., & Besson, M. (2001). Different brain mech- Tabachnick, B., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th
anisms mediate sensitivity to sensory consonance and harmonic con- Edition). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
text: Evidence from auditory event-related brain potentials. Journal of Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events:
Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 241–255. The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin,
Roy, M., Mailhot, J. P., Gosselin, N., Paquette, S., & Peretz, I. (2008). 110, 67–85.
Modulation of the startle reflex by pleasant and unpleasant music. Terhard, E. (1984). The concept of musical consonance: A link between
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 71, 37–42. music and psychoacoustics. Music Perception, 1, 176–195.
Sammler, D., Grigutsch, M., Fritz, T., & Koelsch, S. (2007). Music and Trainor, L. J., & Heinmiller, B. M. (1998). The development of evaluative
emotion: Electrophysiological correlates of the processing of pleasant responses to music: Infants prefer to listen to consonance over dis-
and unpleasant music. Psychophysiology, 44, 293–304. sonance. Infant Behavior & Development, 21, 77–88.
Sanchez-Navarro, J. P., Martinez-Selva, J. M., & Roman, F. (2006). Trainor, L. J., Tsang, C. D., & Cheung, V. H. W. (2002). Preference for
Uncovering the relationship between defence and orienting in emo- sensory consonance in 2- and 4-month-old infants. Music Perception,
tion: Cardiac reactivity to unpleasant pictures. International Journal 20, 187–194.
of Psychophysiology, 61, 34–46. Tramo, M. J., Cariani, P. A., Delgutte, B., & Braida, L. D. (2001).
Schellenberg, E. G., & Trainor, L. J. (1996). Sensory consonance and the Neurobiological foundations for the theory of harmony in Western
perceptual similarity of complex-tone harmonic intervals: Tests of tonal music. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 930,
adult and infant listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of Amer- 92–116.
ica, 100, 3321–3328. Witvliet, C. V., & Vrana, S. R. (1995). Psychophysiological responses as
Schneider, P., Scherg, M., Dosch, H. G., Specht, H. J., Gutschalk, A., & indices of affective dimensions. Psychophysiology, 32, 436–443.
Rupp, A. (2002). Morphology of Heschl’s gyrus reflects enhanced Witvliet, C. V., & Vrana, S. R. (2007). Play it again Sam: Repeated
activation in the auditory cortex of musicians. Nature Neuroscience, 5, exposure to emotionally evocative music polarises liking and smiling
688–694. responses, and influences other affective resports, facial EMG, and
Schoen, D., Regnault, P., Ystad, S., & Besson, M. (2005). Sensory con- heart rate. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 3–25.
sonance: An ERP Study. Music Perception, 23, 105–117. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no infer-
Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety. Palo Alto: ences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.
Consulting Psychologists Press Inc. Zentner, M. R., & Kagan, J. (1996). Perception of music by infants.
Steinbeis, N., Koelsch, S., & Sloboda, J. A. (2006). The role of harmonic Nature, 383, 29.
expectancy violations in musical emotions: Evidence from subjective,
physiological, and neural responses. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 18, 1380–1393.
Stormark, K. M., Laberg, J. C., Nordby, H., & Hugdahl, K. (2000).
Alcoholics’ selective attention to alcohol stimuli: Automated pro-
cessing? Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 61, 18–23. (Received April 20, 2009; Accepted April 13, 2010)

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi