Vous êtes sur la page 1sur 232

Fundamental movement skills and associated physical activity

experiences of children with ADHD

William J. Harvey

Department of Kinesiology and Physical Education

McGill University, Montreal

February, 2006

A thesis submitted to McGill University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree

ofDoctor ofPhilosophy in Kinesiology and Physical Education

©William J. Harvey, 2006


Library and Bibliothèque et
1+1 Archives Canada Archives Canada

Published Heritage Direction du


Branch Patrimoine de l'édition

395 Wellington Street 395, rue Wellington


Ottawa ON K1A ON4 Ottawa ON K1A ON4
Canada Canada

Your file Votre référence


ISBN: 978-0-494-25166-9
Our file Notre référence
ISBN: 978-0-494-25166-9

NOTICE: AVIS:
The author has granted a non- L'auteur a accordé une licence non exclusive
exclusive license allowing Library permettant à la Bibliothèque et Archives
and Archives Canada to reproduce, Canada de reproduire, publier, archiver,
publish, archive, preserve, conserve, sauvegarder, conserver, transmettre au public
communicate to the public by par télécommunication ou par l'Internet, prêter,
telecommunication or on the Internet, distribuer et vendre des thèses partout dans
loan, distribute and sell th es es le monde, à des fins commerciales ou autres,
worldwide, for commercial or non- sur support microforme, papier, électronique
commercial purposes, in microform, et/ou autres formats.
paper, electronic and/or any other
formats.

The author retains copyright L'auteur conserve la propriété du droit d'auteur


ownership and moral rights in et des droits moraux qui protège cette thèse.
this thesis. Neither the thesis Ni la thèse ni des extraits substantiels de
nor substantial extracts from it celle-ci ne doivent être imprimés ou autrement
may be printed or otherwise reproduits sans son autorisation.
reproduced without the author's
permission.

ln compliance with the Canadian Conformément à la loi canadienne


Privacy Act some supporting sur la protection de la vie privée,
forms may have been removed quelques formulaires secondaires
from this thesis. ont été enlevés de cette thèse.

While these forms may be included Bien que ces formulaires


in the document page count, aient inclus dans la pagination,
their removal does not represent il n'y aura aucun contenu manquant.
any loss of content from the
thesis.
•••
Canada
2

Abstract

Excessive activity has been a prominent feature in the symptomatology of attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) for more than four decades. While overactivity has helped to

identify persons with ADHD, the movement skill proficiency and physical activity experiences

of children with ADHD have been overlooked (Harvey & Reid, 1997). This dissertation is a

series of four original manuscripts that explore relationships between adapted physical activity

(APA) and ADHD. The fust manuscript, a review paper, discusses important issues related to

ADHD. It suggests many children with ADHD experience poor levels ofphysical fitness and

movement skill difficulties when compared to children without ADHD (Harvey & Reid, 2003).

Numerous reference citations for seminal review articles on ADHD were provided. The second

manuscript, another review paper, explored research methods used in movement performance

studies about ADHD. Twenty new APA research questions about ADHD were posed (Harvey &

Reid, 2005). Issues surrounding identification of ADHD, data collection procedures, and

strategies to improve AP A research about ADHD were also provided. The two review papers,

when combined, are a substantial and original contribution to the ADHD literature. The third

manuscript is a study about the effects of stimulant medication on the fundamental movement

skill performance for 22 children with ADHD and a comparison oftheir movement skills to age-

and gender-matched peers without ADHD. Multivariate statistical analyses revealed no

significant effect ofmethylphenidate on the performance criteria of the TGMD-2 (Ulrich, 2000)

for the children with ADHD. Significant differences between the children with and without

ADHD were found on both locomotor skills and object control skills. A movement skill profile

was developed for each group and they are unique contributions to the movement performance

literature. The fourth manuscript is a study that explores the lived physical activity experiences
3

of six boys with and without ADHD. Semi-structured interviews revealed sorne similarities

among both groups about their physical activity experiences. However, qualitative differences

were apparent on key aspects ofskill proficiency. It is concluded that more in-depth APA

research about people with ADHD is necessary to gain a clearer understanding of the children's

unique physical activity needs.


4

Résumé

L'activité excessive est une caractéristique marquante dans la symptomatologie du trouble

d'attention avec hyperactivité (TDAH) depuis plus de quatre décennies. Si toutefois

l'hyperactivité a aidé à identifier les personnes atteintes de TDAH, la capacité motrice et

l'activité physique des enfants atteints de TDAH ont été négligées (Harvey & Reid, 1997). Cette

dissertation est une série de quatre manuscrits originaux qui explorent les relations entre

l'activité physique adaptée (APA) et le TDAH. Le premier manuscrit, un article de révision,

discute de sujets importants reliés au TDAH. Il suggère que plusieurs enfants atteints du TDAH

ne sont pas en bonne forme physique et vivent des difficultés avec la capacité motrice lorsqu'on

compare à des enfants non-atteints du TDAH (Harvey & Reid, 2003). Plusieurs citations de

référence portant des articles de révision sur le IDAH ont été fournies. Le deuxième manuscrit,

un autre article de révision, explore les méthodes de recherche utilisées pour réaliser des études

sur la capacité motrice sur le TDAH. Vingt nouvelles questions de recherche sur l'APA ont été

posées (Harvey & Reid, 2005). Les points entourant l'identification du TDAH, les procédures

de collecte des données et les stratégies pour améliorer la recherche sur l' AP A et le IDAH ont

également été apportées. Les deux articles, lorsque combinés, sont une contribution substantielle

et originale à la littérature sur le IDAH. Le troisième manuscrit décrit une étude sur les effets

des médicaments stimulants sur la performance fondamentale de la capacité motrice pour 22

enfants atteints du IDAH, et une comparaison de leur capacité motrice à un groupe d'enfants du

même âge et du même sexe qui sont non-atteints du TDAH. Des analyses statistiques

comportant plusieurs variantes n'ont démontré aucun effet significatif de la méthylphénidate sur

le critère du TGMD-2 (Ulrich, 2000) pour les enfants atteints du TDAH. Des différences

significatives entre les enfants atteints et non-atteints du IDAH ont été retrouvées sur les
5

habiletés locomotrices et le contrôle d'objet. Un profil de capacité motrice a été développé pour

chaque groupe et ils sont une contribution unique à la littérature de la performance du

mouvement. Le quatrième manuscrit est une étude qui explore les expériences d'activité

physique vécues de six garçons atteints et non-atteints du TDAH. Des entrevues semi-

structurées ont démontré quelques similitudes entre les deux groupes portant sur leurs

expériences d'activité physique. Toutefois, des différences qualitatives étaient apparentes lors

des aspects clés sur les habiletés. TI a été conclu qu'une recherche plus approfondie sur l'APA et

sur les personnes atteintes d'un TDAH est nécessaire pour bénéficier d'une meilleure

compréhension des besoins uniques de l'activité physique chez les enfants.


6

Acknowledgements

There are many people to thank in this doctoral dissertation. First, 1 would like to thank

aIl of the professors who have served on my doctoral committees and provided much appreciated

mentorship and guidance. 1 had the distinct pleasure and honor to learn from and work with my

dissertation supervisor, Dr. Greg Reid. He has challenged me to learn and perform to the best of

my abilities each and every day. Greg patiently edited my work and, with time and teaching, he

has enabled me to become independent in my research skills and professional practice. Thank

you for your guidance, candor and friendship over these many years.

Three specific committee members have helped me enormously. They assisted in the

design of the two latter studies of the dissertation. Yet, they contributed so much more.

Dr. A.E. (Ted) Wall was an instrumental part ofmy student life by providing much guidance and

support. He always maintained high professional standards and always demanded my very best.

Dr. Carl Frederiksen opened my eyes to the beauty ofbeing a "sleuth" and maintaining a positive

focus on learning. Carl re-instilled a love for learning in me and, importantly, he re-introduced

statistics to me in a way that 1 have acquired a new found interest in the domain and a deep

respect for his teaching skills. Dr. Margaret Downey has long suffered my "5-minute" talks that

would usually last for much longer. Peggy would listen patiently, reflect and always provide

critical feedback that only a master teacher can provide. Thanks are extended to aIl of you for the

tremendous learning experiences and the opportunity to venture into a new and exciting life

journey.

1 would like to thank the foIlowing co-authors for their assistance and contributions to the

thesis. Dr. Natalie Grizenko and Dr. Ridha Joober were open-minded and allowed this physical

activity research to be included in their ADHD research program at the Douglas Hospital. In
7

addition to the opportunity to observe reliably diagnosed persons with ADHD, we were able to

learn much more about research in psychiatry. Dr. Valentin Mbekou and Mrs. Marina Ter-

Stepanian, also from the Douglas Hospital, were vital for the interpretation of psychological tests

as weIl as training me to reliably conduct the required screening interviews. Dr. Gordon Bloom

and Ms. Kerri Staples from McGill University contributed to my understanding of qualitative

research with theoretical and practical feedback on important methodological issues. Thank you

to Human Kinetics for permitting two published review papers to be reproduced and placed in

this thesis. This dissertation was made special by the contributions of Kelly Norman and

Myriam Rabbat at Riverview Elementary School. A very special thank you is especially

extended to aIl participants and their parents for taking part in both studies.

Thanks to great old and new friends who played key roles in data collection (Johanne

Bellingham, Anna Polotskaia), psychological testing (Jenn Felsher, Rebecca Simon), translation
.
of the abstract (Johanne Bellingham) and conceptual discussions (Andrew Chiarelli, Meghann

Llyod, Joe Nolan, & Andrea Prupas). Many thanks are also extended to aIl ofmy colleagues,

friends and family members who had the pleasure of enduring my ranting and raving about the

pros and cons of the latest physical activity studies about children with ADHD. Finally, thanks

Mom for always encouraging me and never losing faith in me. Thanks Chantal for your ongoing

support, dailyencouragement and unconditionallove. 1 dedicate this thesis to both ofyou and to

the memory ofmy late father. 1 miss him more and more every day.
8

Table of Contents

page

Title page 1
Abstract 2
Résumé 4
Acknowledgements 6
Table of Contents 8
List of Figures, Appendices, and Tables 11
Introduction 13

Chapter 1 Theoretical Perspectives 16

Rationale for themy selection 16


The knowledge-based approach 17
Barkley's model of self-regulation 21
Implications for ADHD research 26
References 28
Bridging Manuscripts and Contributions of Authors 32

Chapter 2 Attention-DeficitIHyperactivity Disorder: A Review of 33


Research on Movement SkiU Performance and Physical Fitness

Abstract 33
Issues 34
Definition and Symptomatology 34
Etiology 35
Nomenclature and Classification 36
Comorbidity, Treatment, & Developmental Course 37
Purpose 38
Method 38
Search Strategies 38
Categorization 39
Results and Discussion 40
Motor Processes 40
Movement Performance 42
Retrospective Views 42
Movement peiformance embedded in large descriptive studies 43
Skill Performance 46
Physical Fitness 50
Conclusions 52
References 55
Authors' Notes 67
Acknowledgements 67
Bridging Manuscripts and Contributions of Authors 68
9

Table of Contents, Conf d

page

Chapter Three Attention-DeficitIHyperactivity Disorder:


APA Research Challenges 69

Abstract 69
Method 71
Discussion 73
Research Question 73
Participants 77
Sampling Design 77
Identification & Diagnosis 80
Gender Differences 82
Instruments 83
Data Collection Procedures 84
Methods of Analysis 91
Summary and Future Directions 93
References 95
Acknowledgement 103
Bridging Manuscripts and Contributions of Authors 104

Chapter Four Fundamental movement slrills and children with


attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Stimulant
effects and peer comparisons 105

Abstract 105
Introduction 106
Method 111
Participants 111
Assessment 114
Design and Procedures 116
Data Analysis 118
The effects of stimulant medication 118
Children with and without ADHD 119
Results 120
Reliability 120
The effects of stimulant medication 120
Children with and without ADHD 121
Discussion 124
The effects of stimulant medication 124
Children with and without ADHD 128
References 133
Bridging Manuscripts and Contributions of Authors 141
10

Table of Contents, Cont' d

Page
Chapter Five Physical activity experiences of boys with and without
ADHD 142

Abstract 142
Introduction 143
Method 147
Participants 147
Movement Skill Assessment 148
Interview 149
Data Analysis 151
Trustworthiness 151
Results 153
Movement Skill Assessment 154
Interview 154
Deliberate Play 156
Knowing About Doing 157
Personal Feelings 162
Discussion 164
Conclusions 169
References 171

Chapter Six Summary 178

Introduction 178
References 185
11

List of Figures, Appendices and Tables

page

Chapter 1 Theoretical Perspectives

Figure 1.0. Knowledge About Action 18


Figure 1.1. A knowledge-based model of information processing 20
Appendix A: McGill University ERB certificate 187
AppendixB: Douglas Hospital ERB certificate 188
Appendix C: Consent form for ADHD participants 189
AppendixD: Consent form for control group participants 191

Chapter 2 Attention-DeficitIHyperactivity Disorder: A Review of


Research on Movement SkiU Performance and Physical Fitness

AppendixE: Table 1 Historical Review ofDSM nomenclature 193


AppendixF: Table 2 Movement Performance 194
AppendixG: Table 3 Physical Fitness 198

Chapter Three Attention-DeficitIHyperactivity Disorder: APA


Research Challenges

Appendix H: Table 1 Assessment Instruments for Movement Behavior of 200


Persons with ADHD

Chapter Four Fundamental movement skills and children with


attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Stimulant
eft'ects and peer comparisons

Appendix 1: Table 1 Performance criteria for TGMD-2locomotor skills 201


Appendix J: Table 2 Performance criteria for TGMD-2 object control skills 202
Appendix K: Table 3 Reliability ofTGMD-2 203
Appendix K: Table 4 Means, standard deviations and effect sizes: GMDQ
scores for participants with ADHD 203
Appendix L: Table 5 Means, standard deviations and effect sizes for the
locomotor skills of participants with ADHD 204
AppendixM: Table 6 Means, standard deviations and effect sizes for the object
control skills of participants with ADHD 205
Appendix N: Figure 1 GMDQ scores of children with and without ADHD 206
Appendix 0: Univariate results for GMDQ 207
AppendixP: Univariate results for locomotor and object control skills 209
Appendix Q: Univariate results for individuallocomotor skills 212
AppendixR: Univariate results for individual object control skills 220
12

List of Figures, Tables and Appendices, Cont'd

page

Appendix S: Figure 2 Locomotor skill profiles 228


Appendix T: Figure 3 Object control skill profiles 229

Chapter Five Physical activity experiences of boys with and without


ADHD

Appendix U: Interview Guide 230


Appendix V: Table 1 Physical Activity Categories 233
Appendix W: Table 2 TGMD-2 scores ofboys with and without ADHD 234
Appendix W: Table 3 Deliberate Play 234
Appendix X: Table 4 Knowing about Doing 235
Appendix X: Table 5 Personal Feelings 235
13

Introduction

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a problem of considerable importance,

given the number of people affected, the multi-dimensional nature of the disorder, and the potential

for a lifetime of disability-related impairments. It affects a substantial number of school-aged

children, as prevalence rates of ADHD are between 2% and 14% of elementary school-aged

populations in North America (Scahill & Schwab-Stone, 2000). The disorder constitutes as much

as 50010 of child psychiatry clinical populations (Cantwell, 1996), with a range of associated

developmental problems that may continue into adulthood (JVeiss & Hechtman, 1993). Moreover,

there is converging evidence of physical activity problems linked to children with ADHD

(Harvey & Reid, 2003). For example, children with ADHD lack the ability to regulate their

behavior in physical activity environments (Alexander, 1990) and may experience significant

difficulties in performing both quantitative (Beyer, 1999; Miyahara, Mobs, & Doll-Tepper, 1995,

2001; Piek, Pitcher, & Hay, 1999; Wade, 1976) and qualitative (Harvey & Reid, 1997) aspects of

fundamental movement skill performance. Burton and Miller (1998) suggest that fundamental

skills include locomotor skills that are used to propel a human body through space (e.g., walking,

running, etc.) and object control skills which include manipulating an object in action situations

(e.g., throwing, catching, kicking, etc.).

There were three main research questions in this study. What are the effects of stimulant

medication on the fundamental movement skill performance of children with ADHD? Are there

differences in the movement skills of children with and without ADHD? What are the

similarities and differences in the physical activity experiences of children with and without

ADHD? Since converging evidence has, only lately, suggested that children with ADHD may

experience movement skill problems, adapted physical activity (AP A) researchers have not yet
14

attempted to explore and explain tbis phenomenon (Harvey & Reid, 2003). Furthermore, little

available information suggests that children with ADHD possess the factual information

necessary to successfully regulate their own movement behaviors and complete fundamental

movement skills or sport specific skills. The lack of empirical investigations which explore these

underlying factors is apparent and a meager understanding of the movement behavior of children

with ADHD has resulted. Therefore, tbis study explored the physical activity experiences of

cbildren with and without ADHD by using the knowledge-based approach to motor development

(Wall, McClements, Bouffard, Findlay, & Taylor, 1985). In summary, the three purposes ofthis

study were to assess the effects of stimulant medication on the movement skill performance of

cbildren with ADHD, compare the movement skills of chi 1dren with and without ADHD, and

describe the physical activity experiences of children with and without ADHD.

There are six chapters in the doctoral dissertation. The frrst chapter, theoretical

perspectives, inc1udes a rationale for the two main theoretical approaches that guided the

research and a description ofboth paradigms. A brief review of the knowledge-based approach

to motor development (Wall et al., 1985) supports the use ofthis theory as the major driving

framework. The hybrid model of executive functions (Barkley, 1997) is described as an

additional guiding theory, with paraUels drawn to the knowledge-based approach. Implications

for physical activity research about ADHD are then provided. Chapter two contains a review

manu script about ADHD research in the movement behavior area. The frrst manuscript is a

review of movement behavior studies about ADHD that has been published in the Adapted

Physical Activity Quarterly (Harvey & Reid, 2003). Chapter three, the second manuscript, a

review ofmethods used in physical activity studies about ADHD, has also been published in the

Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly (Harvey & Reid, 2005). The next two chapters are
15

manuscripts that addressed the three main research questions. Chapter four is an assessment of

the effects of stimulant medication on the qualitative components of fundamental movement skill

performance of22 children with ADHD and a comparison of the movement skills to gender- and

age-matched children without ADHD. This manuscript is currently in the peer review process.

Chapter five is a manuscript that describes the physical activity experiences of six boys with and

without ADHD. It is also in the review process. The dissertation concludes with chapter six

where a summary of the relevant findings and recommendations for the future are provided.
16

Chapter One

Theoretical Perspectives

Rationa/e for theory selection

Harvey and Reid (2003) suggested that movement behavior research about ADHD

lacked theoretical models and recommended that scholarly paradigms be incorporated in future

research. The knowledge-based approach to motor development (Wall et al., 1985) and the

hybrid model of executive functions (Barkley, 1997) were used to frame the CUITent doctoral

research project. There were three major reasons for selecting these theories. First, the

knowledge-based framework was the main gui ding theory to analyze and understand

fundamental movement skill performance since it is derived from contexts ofhuman action and

based on principles ofpsychology. Since the theory draws on empirical information and theories

from educational psychology, cognitive science, motor development and sport expertise, it

provided the opportunity to bOITOW paradigms and methods from these domains in order to test

more specific relationships between constructs of interest, for example, self-regulation and

physical activity. In fact, the knowledge-based approach to motor development and sport

expertise (Wall et al., 1985; Wall, Reid, & Harvey, in press) is a unique blend of developmental,

educational and sport psychology that was identified as a significant theoretical framework in

APA (Reid, 1992). Second, Barkley (1997), a renowned medical scientist-practitioner in the area

of ADHD, created a hybrid model of executive functioning to describe relationships between

response inhibition, cognitive functions, and behavioral actions, hypothesized to be responsible

for self-regulation. Specific clinical implications, including aspects of motor control, are

provided in this model for persons with ADHD. Finally, there is overlap between the knowledge-

based approach to motor development (Wall et al., 1985) and the hybrid model ofexecutive
17

functions (Barkley, 1997) as information-processing and cognitive psychology are two major

underlying assumptions for both paradigms. Both grand theories attempt to explain self-

regulation. One framework is related to general psychology and psychiatry, with a specific focus

on ADHD (Barkley, 1997), while the other framework is linked to motor development and sport

psychology (Wall et al., 1985; Wall et al., in press). The strong association ofpsychological

constructs between each framework was a major reason for the theory selection. For example,

the theories share many important commonalities such as the importance of cerebral

development, the relationship of various behaviors to different brain regions, the value of a

developmental perspective, the construction ofvarious forms ofknowledge, the role oflanguage

in child development and cognitive functioning, and a sharing of much technicallanguage. Thus,

appropriate theoreticallinks could be made between the two models in order to gain an in-depth

understanding of the relationship between ADHD and physical activity.

The knawledge-based approach

Wall and his colleagues (1985) created a holistic model to describe and explain human

movement. According to this theoretical framework, there are at least two unique components to

consider for each individual person. The first, structural capacity, refers to the physiological and

genetic factors that a person brings to bear on anY situation (Wall et al., 1985). Acquired

knowledge, also referred to as knowledge about action, is the second important component that

each individual possesses. This knowledge is related to experience and it is stored in long-term

memory. Figure 1.0 illustrates the five interacting components of the knowledge-based model.

Procedural knowledge, dec1arative knowledge, and affective knowledge are the three knowledge

bases that operate at a basic cognitive level while metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive

skills function at the higher, or meta, level of cognition where there may be conscious regulation
18

Metacognitive Knowledge Metacognitive Skills

1
Declarative Knowledge Procedural Knowledge

Affective Knowledge

Structural Capacity

Figure 1.0 Knowledge About Action


Note. From Wall et al. (in press)

of motor control.

Procedural knowledge was described as knowing how to act and includes the entire

process and production ofa motor action (Wall et al., 1985). In other words, an individual's

movement includes both information-processing and a resultant behavioral response. Thus,

procedural knowledge involves both a cognitive component about

knowing about the movement skill and a physical component about the movement skill.

Declarative knowledge was described as knowing about action. It represents factual information

that is highly contextual and specific to various movement skills and sport-specific skills.

Declarative knowledge is stored in schemas where nodes ofknowledge are highly interconnected

by intricate conceptual networks, that is, deep maps of rich semantic information. This type of

knowledge is influenced by morphological, biomechanical, and environmental constraints while

verbal labels for action, or key feedback words, and basic skilllearning strategies are invaluable
19

declarative components of movement skill acquisition and performance. Thus, declarative

knowledge enables the development and execution ofskilled action (Wall et al., 1985). Affective

knowledge was described as feelings about action and it represents subjective feelings attached

to movement (Wall et al., 1985). Emotion plays an enormous role in the acquisition,

development, and performance ofspecific movements and sport skills (Wall et al., 1985; Wall et

al., in press). Affective knowledge is placed directly abovestructural capacity in the knowledge-

based model because it illustrates the close relationship between the person' s cognitive system,

the human physiological system, and the performance environment.

Metacognitive knowledge about action is knowledge about what one knows or does not

know or "knowing about knowing how to move" (Wall et al., 1985, p. 31). It was described as a

higher form of declarative knowledge where there is conscious awareness of all other knowledge

bases, namely; the procedural, declarative, affective, and metacognitive skill knowledge bases.

Metacognitive skills, the higher level of procedural knowledge, incorporates the use of

metacognitive knowledge in proficient movements based on previous successful and

unsuccessful experiences (Wall et al., 1985). Planning, monitoring, and evaluating are the

executive functions that integrate the higher level knowledge, leading to adaptable problem

solving and improved learning and performance (Wall et al., in press). Therefore, knowledge of

results and planning for future actions are part of an iterative process, where both constructs

integrate via reflection before and after skill performance and with respect to any available

knowledge base to enable highly automatized actions. Take for example, a competitive biathlete

who must cross-country ski many kilometers at a very fast pace and then suddenly stop and use a

rifle to shoot at a target as accurately as possible or be penalized with extra laps to ski.

Undoubtedly, the person's respiratory rate, heart rate, blood pressure, and galvanic skin
20

responses are aU elevated during the event. Yet the athlete learns how to control heart rate and

blood pressure by decreasing the skiing pace going into the shooting area. Breathing is also

controUed during target shooting in order to increase upper body stability and assist in visual

acuity. It is hypothesized that these planned measures would have a positive effect on shooting

accuracy, with a positive decrease in anxiety levels, galvanic skin responses, and voluntary

muscular activity. Renee, there can be much conscious control in a performance environment,

reflecting metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive skills.

The knowledge-based approach is the major driving theory of the CUITent research project

because there is empirical support for the model as a vehicle to explore the movement behavior

of children with disabilities. According to the knowledge-based model of information

processing, Wall (2001) hypothesized that relevant environmental information is identified,

coded, and processed with a perceptual mechanism. The perceived information is then forwarded

to a decision making mechanism where a decision is made about which response to select, given

the saliency of the encoded information, finally the response, or action, is executed. This model

interconnects the mechanisms themselves with both feed forward and feed backward loops and

the higher level knowledge process~s of metacognition (self-awareness) and metacognitive skills

(self-regulation)(See Figure 1.1).

Self-Awareness Self Regulation

Figure 1.1 A knowledge-based model of information processing


Note. From Wall, 2001, p. 51
21

Knowledge-based studies that explored fundamental movement skills in relation to persons with

disabilities specifically indicate that children with disabilities demonstrate problems related to

perception (Lefebvre & Reid, 1998), decision making (Bouffard & Wall, 1991), and response

execution (Kourtessis & Reid, 1997; Marchiori, Wall, & Bedingfield, 1987; Todd, 1988). Since

the knowledge-based approach has been used successfully in AP A research, it is a credible

framework to use as the major driving theory of the CUITent research project.

Barkley's model ofself-regulation

Barkley (1997) created a hybrid model of executive functioning that explained the

underlying mechanisms of self-regulation, with specifie implications for persons with ADHD.

The theory was described as a developmental neuropsychological model of human self-

regulation (Barkley, 1997, 1998). The theory is developmental because Barkley refers to

cognition as being developed in the external physical world in an overt form oflanguage (e.g.,

self-talk) and proceeds to be more fully developed with private, covert, and inner thoughts of the

mind (Vygotsky, 1978, 1986). The theory also possesses a neuropsychological component

because Medical research findings indicated deficiencies in the prefrontal, striatal-limbic brain

areas ofpersons with ADHD (Barkley 1997, 1998). Tannock (1998) also suggested that

biological and genetic factors are the two major causes for ADHD.

Response inhibition, executive functions, and motor control are the three main levels of

Barkley' s model of se1f-regulation. While the knowledge-based model and the hybrid model of

executive functioning do not have direct matches between major constructs, links to the

knowledge-based approach will be made in the description of each level of the Barkley model.

Response inhibition includes the inability to: (a) inhibit behavioral responses where

immediate positive or negative reinforcement is available, (b) stop ongoing behavioral responses,
22

and (c) use interference control where the individual can disregard multiple stimuli in order to

inhibit behavioral responses (Barkley, 1997). Response inhibition is considered to have an

indirect, but vital, influence on each individual' s executive functioning and a direct influence on

individual motor behavior.

A conceptual key to thÏs model is the assumption that a person must take necessary and

adequate reflection time when planning and making decisions. Therefore, Barkley (1997, 1998)

suggested that human beings may need to act in at least three different ways in order to meet the

variable demands of the environment. First, in order for a person to be successful in attaining

long-term goals, it is vital to establish short-term goals without being completely gratified by the

successful completion of the short-term goals. In other words, it is important to take extra time to

delay personal gratification untillong-term goals have been successfully completed. Second, the

individual must be able to deduce performance errors, stop the actual behavior under self-

evaluation, and take appropriate reflection time to plan solutions for the perceived error in order

to produce more effective self-regulation. Finally, the person must also be able to shut out

external and internaI sources of stimuli that may negatively affect behavior. A person must use

reflection and planning time efficiently in order to be proficient in the successful self-regulation

of their behavior. Thus, response inhibition has an indirect effect on executive functioning

because the person may or may not choose to use additional time to be reflective or to plan future

actions. The additional time enables the executive functions to be called upon for reflection in an

organized and efficient manner. Thus, the direct influence of response inhibition on individual

motor behavior may or may not be tempered by planning and reflection, with the resultant action

being carried out with either more thought and planning or with minimal cognitive input and a

decreased temporal period. Many of the same constructs are also valued in the knowledge-based
23

approach to motor development. For example, studies about the development of expertise would

suggest the importance of the individual' s ability to identify salient cues from their own

movements and the environmental display in order to perform in a more proficient manner (Wall

et aI., in press). In fact, it is this attention to detail that distinguishes novice and expert

performers (Ericsson, 2003). Error deduction, planning, monitoring, and taking the necessary

time to reflect are aIso important metacognitive components of the knowledge-based approach

(Bouffard & Wall, 1990; Wall et al., in press).

Nonverbal working memory, verbal working memory, self-regulation of

affect/motivation/arousal, and reconstitution are the four executive functions responsible for

much ofhuman self-regulation (Barkley, 1997). Nonverbal working memory was defined as "the

capacity to maintain intemally represented information in mind or on line that will be used to

control a subsequent response" (Barkley, 1998). It was described as a "covert sensing to the self'

where individuals produce mental representations ofbehavioral sequences. Barkley suggested

that imitation is a very important component of this executive function as visual and verbal

information about the sensory-motor world are initially formed in an overt manner, which, in

tum, become directed inward in the form of private thought and speech. This executive function

is closely related to procedural knowledge as proponents of the knowledge-based approach

would also indicate, for example, that imitation, or an image of the act, is vital ta reproduce

skilled movements (Wall, 2001).

Verbal working memory was conceptuaIized as the intemalization of speech. Barkley

(1997, 1998) emphasized the key role oflanguage in the development of self-regulatory

behavior. Self-talk, in particular, is mentioned as a vital part ofverbal working memory. The

individual performs self-directed speech in order to reflect and describe events before responding
24

by using self-talk to label, describe, and verbally question key elements of events and situations.

Self-questioning leads to greater levels of reflection and thoughts that become internalized.

Again, the direction of self-regulatory speech commences in the physical world and, with time

and experience, is directed inward. This external-internal relationship of language and thought

was hypothesized as an iterative process (Vygotsky, 1978, 1986). The verbal working memory

function is closely related to declarative knowledge in the knowledge-based approach because

individuals create verbal labels for action and intricate schematic networks of domain-specifie

knowledge, leading to higher levels of self-awareness or metacognition.

The self-regulation of affectJmotivation/arousal was defined as the ability to self-motivate

in order to achieve goal-directed actions. Barkley emphasized the importance of linking affective

responses to individual covert visual images and covert self-speech. He indicated the importance

of controlling emotional responses to various events that, in tum, may lead to increased time for

reflection and greater individual ability to inhibit responses in the face of environments, highly

charged with negative or positive reinforcers. Thus, a person who can visualize different

solutions, with appropriate and relevant self-directed speech, may be in a better position to

control their emotional responses to varied events. Again, the development of emotional

reactions is expected to be more extroverted when flfst experiencing an event with a progressive

internalization of emotional thoughts. This executive function is almost identical to the affective

component of the knowledge-based approach. For example, subjective feelings are attached to

movements, experiences, and different environmental displays. Emotional control is vital to skill

automatization and the development of expert performers (Wall, 2001).

Reconstitution was defined as the internalization ofplay. Barkley (1997, 1998)

emphasized the importance ofbreaking down behavior into its sequential and component parts
25

(e.g., analysis) and then, in tum, the individual may create new behavioral sequences by using a

novel combination ofpreviously learned behavioral components (e.g., synthesis). This executive

function also has the unique property ofcreating new verbal (e.g., linguistic) and non-verbal

(e.g., gross motor and fine motor skills) behavioral responses for the purposes ofself-regulation.

Furthermore, self-awareness, problem-solving, and creativity are important elements of the

reconstitution function of the hybrid model. Personal awareness, creativity, and self-evaluation

are also key metacognitive skills from a knowledge-based perspective.

Motor behavior is described in the hybrid model of executive functions as motor

control/fluency/syntax to illustrate the complexity ofhuman behavior. Barkley (1997, 1998)

described the importance of internalizing thought when producing goal-directed actions. The

external to internaI process of action leads to purposive, intentional, and future-oriented motor

behaviors. He also suggested that non-essential sensory information and behavior unrelated to an

action, may be suppressed or minimized during physical performance. Motor behavior was also

hypothesized as directly influenced by response inhibition and all four executive functions. This

component of the hybrid model could also be described as procedural knowledge (Wall et al.,

1985) since there is a physical component to this type ofknowledge, also known as motor

memory. Interestingly, proficient movers use this tacit knowledge to their advantage to perform

skills in a highly proficient manner. There is no need for deliberate and conscious control of the

component parts of a skill, rather the proficient moyer is able to perform the physical action

automatically and pay attention to other salient environmental cues in order to successfully

complete the goal-directed action. Again, it is this difference in attention to detail that separates

the novice from the expert performer.


26

Implications for ADHD research

Children with ADHD have been described as having a developmental disability in

response inhibition and self-regulation (Barkley, 1997, 1998). There are two key aspects of

Barkley' s model of self-regulation with regards to movement-related clinical applications. First,

Barkley (1997) mentions that time should be considered as the ultimate disability because people

with ADHD have an impaired sense of temporal reality that may negatively affect their behavior.

For example, a person with ADHD may be appeased by successful completion of short-term

goals and continually seek to obtain immediate gratification from the environment. Yet by not

taking the time to reflect and failing to complete long-term goals, the executive functions do not

fully develop and the intemalization ofbehavior to thought is affected, further delaying the

development of self-regulatory behavior (Barkley, 1997, 1998). Second, he suggests that

children with ADHD may experience an inability to proceduralize their declarative knowledge

which he refers to as point ofperformance problems. In other words, "ADHD is more a problem

of doing what one knows rather than ofknowing what to do" (Barkley, 1997, p. 335).

The CUITent doctoral research project examined the fundamental movement skill

performance of children with ADHD. Converging evidence suggests that children with this

disorder may demonstrate problems related to movement skills (Harvey & Reid, 2003). Thus,

there seems to be a problem of procedural knowledge about action for children with ADHD.

Chapter four is a study that addressed questions related to the movement skills of children with

ADHD, thus expanding the authors' prior research and our CUITent knowledge base about the

movement skill problems ofthis population. Ethics certificates and consent were obtained (See

appendices A, B, C & D).


27

No physical activity researcher has asked why children with ADHD experience

difficulties in perfonning fundamental movement skills nor has anyone studied the decision-

making of children with ADHD in daily physical activities (Harvey & Reid, 2003). Since

Barkley (1997, 1998) alluded to a discrepancy between procedural knowledge and declarative

knowledge, the focus of chapter five was a qualitative approach where the physical activity

experiences ofboys with and without ADHD were explored to gain in-depth knowledge about

individual movement skill proficiency and involvement in various physical activity contexts. Semi-

structured interviews provided rich personal insights into constructs like activity preference, play,

skilllearning, observationallearning, and individual feelings about movement perfonnance. Thus,

chapter five was an initial attempt to describe general aspects of self-regulatory behavior in

physical activity for children with ADHD. The notion that persons with a disability can regulate

their own movement behavior has led physical activity researchers to explore self-regulation in

physical activity, especially for children with DeD (Reid, Harvey, Lloyd, & Bouffard, 2002).

Since children with ADHD seem to have a developmental delay in self-regulation (Barkley 1997,

1998), they seemed to be the perfect candidates to study in order to better understand the

different cognitive processes about action involved in self-regulation.

In summary, this chapter has provided a rationale for the two main theoretical approaches

that guided the CUITent research project. Descriptions of the knowledge-based approach (Wall et

al., 1985) and the hybrid model of executive functions (Barkley, 1997) were provided to support

the use of the theories as the key underlying frameworks. Implications for ADHD research were

provided. Finally, the remaining thesis chapters were described briefly.


28

References

Alexander, J.L. (1990). Hyperactive children: Which sports have the right stuff? The

Physician And Sportsmedicine, 18(4), 105-108.

Barkley, RA (1997). ADHD and the nature of self~ontrol. New York: Guilford Press.

Barkley, RA (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handhookfor diagnosis


nd
and treatment (2 ed.). New York: Guilford Press.

Beyer, R (1999). Motor proficiency ofboys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

AdaptedPhysical Activity Quarterly, 16, 403-414.

Bouffard, M., & Wall, AB. (1990). A problem-solving approach to movement skill

acquisition. In G. Reid (Ed.)., Problems in Movement Control (pp. 283-316). North-

Holland: Elsevier Science Publishers.

Cantwell, D.P. (1996). Attention deficit disorder: Areview of the past 10 years. Journal of

the American Academy ofChild and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 978-987.

Ericsson, K.A (2003). Development of elite performance and deliberate practice: An

update of the expert performance approach. In IL. Starkes and K.A Ericsson (Eds.),

Expert performance in sports (pp. 49-83). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Harvey, W.I, & Reid, G. (1997). Motor performance of children with attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder: A preliminary investigation. Adapted Physical Activity

Quarterly, 14, 189-202.

Harvey, W.I, & Reid, G. (2003). A review offundamental movement skill performance

and physical fitness of children with ADHD. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly,

20, 1-25.

Harvey, W.J., & Reid, G. (2005). Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Ways to


29

improve AP A research. Adapted Physica/ Activity Quarter/y, 22, 1-20.

Kourtessis, T., & Reid, G. (1997). Knowledge ofball catching in children with cerebral

paIsy and other physical disabilities. Adapted Physica/ Activity Quarter/y, 14, 24-42.

Lefebvre, C., & Reid, G. (1998). Prediction in baIl catching by children with and without

a developmental coordination disorder. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 15,299-

315.

Marchiori, G.E., Wall, AE., & Bedingfield, W. (1987). Kinematic analysis ofskill

acquisition in physically awkward boys. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 4,305-315.

Miyahara, M., Mobs, 1., & Doll-Tepper, G. (1995). Subtypes ofclinically identified

children with hyperkinetic syndrome based upon perceptual motor function and classroom

behaviors. In 1. Morisbak: & P.E. Jorgensen (Eds.), 1ff1' International Symposium on

Adapted Physical Activity: Quality of life through adapted physical activity, A lifespan

concept (pp. 278-286). Oslo, Norway: Ham Trykk AIS.

Miyahara, M., Mobs, 1., & Doll-Tepper, G. (2001). Severity ofhyperactivity and the

comorbidity ofhyperactivity with clumsiness in three sample sources: school, support

group and hospital. Child: Care, Bealth and Development, 27, 413-424.

Piek, IP., Pitcher, T.M., & Hay, D.A (1999). Motor coordination and kinaesthesis in boys

with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology,

41, 159-165.

Reid, G. (1992). Editorial on theory, exchange, and terminology. Adapted Physical

Activity Quarterly, 9, 1-10.

Reid, G., Harvey, W.I, Lloyd, M., & Bouffard, M. (2002). A pilot study ofself-directed
30

motor learning in children with DCD. Paper presentation about the microgenetic analysis

of the overhand throwing of boys with and without developmental coordination disorder.

The DCD-V International Conference: Mechanisms, Measurement, and Management. .

Banff, Alberta, Canada .

Scahill, L., & Schwab-Stone, M. (2000). Epidemiology of ADHD in school-age children.

Child and Adolescent Psychiatrie Clinics of North America, 9(3), 541-555.

Tannock, R. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Advances in cognitive,

neurobiological, and genetic research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39,

65-69.

Todd, T. (1988). A comparison of metacognitive and procedural knowledge of bail

catching by physically awkward and non-awkward children. Unpublished master' s thesis,

McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.

Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind In Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Vygotsky, L.S. (1986). Thought and language. (A Kozulin, Trans.). Cambridge, MA:

Harvard University Press. (Original work published as Myslenie 1 rec in 1934).

Wade, M.G-. (1976). Effects ofmethylphenidate on motor skill acquisition of hyperactive

children. Journal ofLeaming Disabilities, 9,443-447.

Wall, AE. (2001). Understanding sport expertise: A knowledge-based introduction (3 rd

rev). Course text for the Psychology ofMotor Performance, McGill University. Montreal,

QC: Dr. Ted Wall.

Wall, AE., McClements, l, Bouffard, M., Findlay, H., & Taylor, M.I (1985). A

knowledge-based approach to motor development: Implications for the physically

awkward. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 2, 21-42.


31

Wall, A.E., Reid, G., & Harvey, W.J. (in press). Interface of the Knowledge-based and

Ecological Task Analysis Approaches. Book chapter for special text dedicated to the

memory of Dr. Allan Burton. Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL.

Weiss, G., & Hechtman, L. (1993). Hyperactive Children Grawn Up, (2nd ed) New York:

The Guilford Press.


32

Bridging Manuscripts and Contributions of Authors

Chapter one was named theoretical perspectives as it described the two major theories that

guided this doctoral dissertation. Implications for physical activity research in ADHD were

provided. In tum, these ideas led to four original papers that explore the relationship between

ADHD and physical activity. The next study is a review of research on the movement performance

and physical fitness of children with ADHD. It is the frrst article in the cohesive series offour

original dissertation manuscripts. Discussions about key issues in ADHD were also highlighted

(definition, symptomatology, nomenclature, etc.) in order to provide an update of information on

the disorder and reference citations for seminal review articles to physical activity researchers and

practitioners. The relative contributions of the two authors were as follows. As frrst author, 1 was

responsible for 85% of the manuscript and involved in all aspects of the research, writing and

editing of the manuscript. Dr. Greg Reid was responsible for the other 15% of the work involved

by ensuring the reliability of the research process, with multiple reads of the text, editing and

suggestions for text improvement.


33

ChapterTwo

Attention-DeficitIHyperactivity Disorder:

A Review ofResearch on Movement Skill Performance and Physical Fitness

From W.J. Harvey and G. Reid, 2003, Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly 20(1): 1-25. © 2003 by
Human Kinetics Publishers. Reprinted with permission from Human Kinetics (Champaign, IL).

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to present a comprehensive review of research on the movement

performance and physical fitness of children with attention-deficitlhyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

and ofIer research recommendations. Movement behaviors of children with ADHD were described

on the basis of 49 empirical studies published between 1949 and 2002. Major results indicated that

(a) children with ADHD are at risk for movement skill difficulties, (b) children with ADHD are at

risk for poor levels ofphysical fitness, (c) comorbidity may exist between ADHD and

developmental coordination disorder (DCD), and (d) few interventions have focused on movement

performance and physical fitness of children with ADHD. Numerous reference citations for

seminal review articles on ADHD are provided so that potential researchers or program planners

might enter the vast ADHD literature with sorne ease.


34

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder:

A Review ofResearch on Movement Skill Performance and Physical Fitness

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a complex combination of

developmental problems with important medical, educational, and social implications (Cantwell,

1996). Little information is available about conducting physical activity programs for persons with

ADHD. Only one literature review, in 1989, focused on children with ADHD in physical activity

(Churton, 1989), and only a few sources address physical education (e.g., Bishop & Beyer, 1995;

Craft, 2000; Sherrill, 1998). Therefore, a comprehensive review of the movement performance and

physical fitness of children with ADHD is justified as substantial changes have been made in

ADHD (e.g., Stubbe, 2000a), and new physical activity studies have been conducted since 1989

(e.g., Harvey & Reid, 1997; Piek, Pitcher, & Hay, 1999; Yan & Thomas, 2002).

At least 50 textbooks and over 6,000 scientific articles are devoted to ADHD (Barkley,

1995), with different nomenclature used over tÏme (Barkley, 1998). Tannock (1998) suggested that

a comprehensive review of ADHD and related knowledge domains was not feasible because of the

sheer volume of published literaturel . Following is an overview of four issues of concem to aIl

educators and researchers. Under each issue, we describe the particular relevance to adapted and

general physical education.

Issues

Definition and Symptomatology

ADHD is defined in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual olMental Disorders (DSM-IV-

TR) of the American Psychiatric Association (AP A, 2000) with five specified diagnostic criteria:

(a) 6 to 12 behavioral symptoms must be identified; (b) symptoms must be evident before the age

of7 years; (c) behaviors must be exhibited in at least two different settings; (d) significant
35

impainnent in social, academic, or occupational functioning should be observed~ and (e) symptoms

should not be better explained by another disorder (APA, 2000). Children who demonstrate these

symptoms are usually identified first by parents or teachers (AP A, 2000) and subsequently referred

to and assessed by clinical professionals (Reeve, 1990). Physical activity specialists should

understand that there is a difference between children with ADHD and children who are simply

overactive, because overactivity is only one indication of the disorder. In fact, the overactivity must

be excessive and inappropriate to enter the definition of ADHD.

The DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000) groups behavioral symptoms into two lists, representing

clusters of inattention (e.g., often has difficulty sustaining attention in tasks or play activities) and

hyperactivity-impulsivity (e.g., is often "on the go" or often acts as if "driven by a motor"). A child

with ADHD must demonstrate six ofnine symptoms for a diagnosis of the inattentive subtype, six

of nine symptoms for a diagnosis of the hyperactive-impulsive subtype, or six symptoms from both

inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity for the combined subtype (APA, 2000). See Craft (2000)

or Sherrill (1998) for explanations of the diagnostic criteria.

Eti%gy

The etiology of ADHD is unclear (Cantwell, 1996) and controversial (Stubbe, 2000b).

Conceptual models about ADHD and associated causal factors depend upon the individual

professional discipline and underlying assumptions of the clinician, researcher, or author. See

Barkley (1997, 1998) for discussions of etiology. However, there is considerable agreement that

ADHD should be considered a multidimensional disorder consisting of interacting neurological,

genetic, and psychosocial causal factors (Tannock, 1998). Physical activity specialists should be

aware of the probable biological basis for the lack of self-control that people with ADHD exhibit,
36

which in tum, may become more erratic in environments with decreasing amounts of social

support (Barkley, 1997).

Nomenclature and Classification

Professionals and researchers should be cognizant of contemporary nomenclature and

know that terminology changes over time. Nomenclature has been the subject of considerable

debate (McBurnett, Lahey, & Pfiffner, 1993), and there is little doubt that the many classification

terms and subtyping terminology can lead to confusion. For example, there have been numerous

classification terms which are further complicated by subtyping terminology (See Table 1,

Appendix E). Comparing findings from studies based on different nomenclature must be done

cautiously. McBumett et al. (1993) offers a discussion ofterminological comparisons using DSM

over time. See Barkley (1998) for an excellent review on nomenclature and history of ADHD.

Two main terms are currently used, depending upon which of two classification systems

are followed. North America is largely influenced by the DSM-lV-IR and the term ADHD. In

Europe, the International Classification ofDiseases (lCD-JO) of the World Health Organization

(WHO, 1992) has a large following, and the term hyperkinetic disorders (HKS) is used. A child

identified with ADHD using DSM-IV-IR criteria may not be diagnosed with HKS using ICD-JO

criteria. Tripp, Luk, Schaughency, and Singh (1999) suggested that identification with thelCD is

more stringent and, therefore, fewer children are diagnosed with HKS than with ADHD. Both

classification systems provide symptom lists for their diagnostic criteria. However, the ICD system

specifies that a person must meet criteria for all three major diagnostic categories (attention,

hyperactivity, and impulsivity), whereas the DSM system identifies persons who present symptom

criteria of either inattention or hyperactivity-impulsivity categories or both. See Rasmussen and

Gillberg (1999) for comparison of symptom lists.


37

Cultural differences also exist between Europe and North America, as sorne Europeans view

ADHD as a rare disorder, with a prevalency rate of approximately 1%, affected by delinquency

problems (Taylor et al., 1998). The disorder is considered to be more prevalent in North America

with estimates ranging between 2% and 14% of elementary school-aged populations (Scahill &

Schwab- Stone, 2000). See Taylor et al. (1998) for a discussion of cross-Atlantic classification

differences.

Comorbidity, Treatment, and Developmental Course

There is no single method of diagnosis or intervention because there is no definitive cause

of ADHD (Cantwell, 1996). Diagnosis of ADHD may be based upon combinations of age-

appropriate symptoms, clinical tests, and observations (Hechtman, 2000). The diagnostic process

should also include comorbidity (Cantwell, 1996) as there is a high probability of ADHD

coexisting with one or more of the following disorders: developmental coordination disorder

(DCD), oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, anxiety, depression, and learning

disabilities. See Pliszka (2000) and Rasmussen and Gillberg (1999) about comorbidity.

Multimodal treatment approaches are recommended for persons with ADHD

(Mercugliano, 1999) because there is not one definitive cause ofthe disorder and comorbidity is so

frequent. For example, Damico, Damico, and Armstrong (1999) suggested combined intervention

approaches that include pharmacological therapy, behavior management, cognitive-behavioral

therapy, direct communicative interventions, and educational programs.

ADHD is considered a lifelong disorder because longitudinal research indicates that adults

are also a:ffected (Weiss & Hechtman, 1993). Previously it was hypothesized to be only a

childhood problem. The next decade promises identification studies that explore the age of onset

criterion and symptomological changes with age (Barkley & Biederman, 1997). See Weiss and
38

Hechtman (1993) and Silver (2000) for information about adult ADHD. Physical activity

interventions and research programs should be developed with the understanding that children with

ADHD may have at least one comorbid disorder (Szatmari, Offord, & Boyle, 1989) and the effects

of ADHD may vary with the age of the participants (Barkley & Biederman, 1997).

Purpose

The purpose of this study was to present a comprehensive review of research on the

movement performance and physical fitness of children with ADHD and offer research

recommendations. The review and discussion attempt to answer two main questions: Is the

movement performance of children with ADHD similar to their age- and gender-matched peers

without disabilities? Do children with ADHD demonstrate similar levels of physical fitness when

compared to their age- and gender-matched peers without disabilities ?

Method

Search Strategies

Computer searches were conducted in the SPORTDiscus (1949- March, 2002), Current

Contents/Ali Editions (Week 26, 1993-Week 07,2002), ERIC (1966-February, 2002), MEDLINE

(1966-March, 2002), and PsychINFO (1967-January, 2002) databases. Key words reflected a link

to human movement science (e.g., motor, fitness, health, exercise, physiology, sport, recreation)

and psychology, with special attention paid to chronology (e.g., attention deficit, attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder [1987-2002], attention deficit disorder [1980-2002], hyperkinesis,

hyperactivity, minimal brain dysfunction [1949-2002]). The footnote chasing approach was also

used to identify studies that may have been missed (White, 1994).
39

Categorization

The categorization of studies was developed according to the taxonomy of movement skills

by Burton and Miller (1998). Studies were placed into motor process and movement performance

categories. Motor process studies were identified by dependent variables reflecting underlying

internal factors believed to affect observable movement. These studies involved measures of

perceptual-motor, psychomotor, or sensorimotor processes which can be referred to as "descriptors

of motor processes rather than movement skills" (Burton & Miller, p. 44). For example,

perceptual-motor processes were inferred from a finger tapping task in one study (Gordon &

Kantor, 1979). AIthough the major purpose of this paper was to review the movement performance

of children with ADHD, motor processes were discussed briefly to provide a global picture of

movement behavior.

Movement performance is observable behavior. Thus, movement performance studies

reflected an "observable act of moving" or a "goal-directed movement that can be described in

terms of quantity or quality" (Burton & Miller, 1998, p. 43; Burton & Rodgerson, 2001). These

studies were subdivided into four categories, reflecting different movement performance and

research methods contexts: (a) retrospective views, (b) movement performance embedded in large

descriptive studies, (c) skill performance, and (d) physical fitness. Retrospective studies have

parents or teachers recall significant developmental events in the lives of children (e.g.,

Ottenbacher, 1979; Rasmussen & Gillberg, 1983). Movement performance embedded in large

descriptive studies emphasize multiple dependent variables associated with general development.

Movement behavior inferences from these studies are limited because very few movement skills or

health-related variables are tested. Movement data embedded in larger studies have been noted also

in DCD research (Sugden & Wright, 1998).


40

Researchers of skill performance usually assessed locomotor and object control skills as the

main dependent variables. Physical fitness studies were defmed by testing variables that measured

health-related components of physical fitness including cardiorespiratory endurance, body

composition, muscular strength and endurance, and flexibility (American College of Sports

Medicine [ACSM], 2000). Physical fitness studies were placed in the movement performance

category because fitness is a foundational component in the movement skills model of Burton and

Miller (1998). For example, five of the eleven commonly assessed foundation areas (e.g., body

composition, -body size and morphology, cardiovascular endurance, flexibility/range of motion,

and muscular strength and endurance) reflect aspects of physical fitness.

Results and Discussion

Forty-nine studies, published between 1949 and 2002, were identified that examined the

movement behavior of children with ADHD. The investigations were categorized as either motor

processes (22) or movement performance (27). Movement performance studies included

retrospective views (5), large descriptive studies (10), skill performance (10), and physical fitness

(4). One study (Harvey & Reid, 1997) was included in the categories of skill performance and

physical fitness, while another study (Doyle, Wallen, & Whitmont, 1995) was included in the

retrospective views and skill performance categories. AlI studies in each category are identified in

the reference list. 2

Motor Processes

The motoT processes of children with ADHD have been examined in studies that

emphasize sensorimotor, motor control, and fine motor variables. Sensorimotor investigations tend

to minimize gross motor output but highlight sensory or perceptual output. For example, tasks of

visual motor performance (Cakirpaloglu & Radil, 1992; Conners & Delamater, 1980; Conrad,
41

Dworkin, Shai, & Tobiessen, 1971; Korkman & Pesonen, 1994; Millichap, Aymat, Sturgis,

Larsen, & Egan, 1968) and fmger tapping (Gordon & Kantor, 1979) have been used to measure

motor processes of males with ADHD. While contributing to our overall understanding of ADHD,

the implications for physical activity curricula and instruction are not always obvious, given that

the assessment tasks are quite distinct from usual ones in physical education.

A number of motor process studies have also explored aspects of motor control. Boys with

ADHD have been linked to motor overflow (Denclda, Rudel, Chapman, & Krieger, 1985; Denclda

& Rudel, 1978), impaired timing of motor responses (Hefley & Gorman, 1986; Rubia, Taylor,

Taylor, & Sergeant, 1999; Werry, Elkind, & Reeves, 1987; Yan & Thomas, 2002), and motor soft

signs (Denclda & Rudel, 1978; McMahon & Greenberg, 1977; Reeves, Werry, Elkind, &

Zametkin, 1987; Sandberg, Rutter, & Taylor, 1978). In a recent motor control study (Yan &

Thomas, 2002), children with ADHD took more time than children without ADHD to complete

rapid arm aiming movements, with more variable speed and accuracy of the arm movements. Yan

and Thomas (2002) suggested that children with ADHD use more on-line corrections when

performing accuracy tasks in comparison to their peers without ADHD. It would appear that

additional research is necessary to investigate aspects of motor control. While such studies often do

not employ ecologically-valid tasks from the viewpoint of adapted physical activity specialists,

collectively these investigations may shed light on unique motor control strategies and the overall

motor behaviour associated with ADHD.

Poor motor coordination has been suggested as a condition experienced by many children

with ADHD (Conners, Rothschild, Eisenberg, Stone-Schwartz, & Robinson, 1968; Gillberg,

Carlstrom, Rasmussen, & Waldenstrom, 1983; Hefley & Gorman, 1986; Knights & Hinton, 1969;

Palkes, Stewart, & Kahana, 1968; Pereira, Eliasson, & Forssberg, 2000; Taylor, Schachar,
42

Thorley, & Weiselberg, 1986a; Taylor et al., 1986b). The term motor coordination may be

misleading, as the identified studies were usually based on neuropsychological tests that required

fine motor skill performance. Motor skills may be classified into fme and gross motor skills by the

precision ofmovement (Burton & Miller, 1998). The occasional confusion between fme and gross

motor skills was underscored when the first author asked a renowned psychiatrist, a specialist for

children with ADHD, what physical educators could do to help children with ADHD improve their

gross motor skills. "Teach them how to hold a pencil" was the response. While Szatmari and

Taylor (1984) also stated that coordinated finger movements reflect skill acquisition, they

questioned the clinical utility of this type of neurodevelopmental sign. Thus, the researchers might

have been more correct to describe their findings as suggesting that children with ADHD have

poor fine motor coordination rather than a general motor coordination deficit.

Movement Peiformance

Retrospective views. Retrospective studies usually report teacher (Ottenbacher, 1979) and

parent observations of poor movement skills in children with ADHD. For example, mothers

reported their children performed poorly in sports, gymnastics, and fine movement skills (Stewart,

Pitts, Craig, & Dieruf, 1966) while other parents have perceived their children as c1umsy

(Rasmussen & Gillberg, 1983; Szatmari et al., 1989).

Although the observations ofboth parents and teachers are invaluable, conclusions are

usually based in a context of memory that is susceptible to decay and alteration (Offer, Kaiz,

Howard, & Bennett, 2000). These perceptions can also be imprecise as excessive activity may

overshadow real problems of coordination (Keogh, 1978). Furthermore, an underlying assumption

of these studies is that most parents possess adequate amounts of motor development knowledge

for comparison purposes. Moreover, Rasmussen and Gillberg (1983) asked parents to answer only
43

one question: "How would you describe your child' s gross motor control (e.g., movement control

in activities like walking, running, climbing, hopping on one leg)? Is if average (0), Better than

average (1), Somewhat clumsy (2), or Awkward (3) (p. 128)." Doyle, Wallen, and Whitmont

(1995) reported that parents underrated their children's performance on a single question in

comparison to the actual results obtained on the Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency or

BOTMP (Bruininks, 1978). While potentially informative, retrospective views hardly represent a

comprehensive analysis of movement behavior.

Empirical investigations should be conducted in concert with retrospective research to

understand the factors underlying the perceptions of parents in relation to their children' s

movement skills. For example, what is the role of social factors (e.g., socioeconomic level, cultural

background, gender, age, etc.) in parental observations? Are the parents proficient movers or do

they also experience problems of movement behavior? How important is physical activity in the

family context? Such research may explore the ability of parents to comment on the skill

proficiency of their children and their ability and experience in fostering skill acquisition.

Furthermore, children with ADHD should be asked about their own perceptions oftheir movement

skill proficiency and involvement in play, physical activity, and sports. Qualitative research studies

have been recommended to document the health-related behaviors of children with ADHD

(Kendall, 1997), and these types of investigations could provide rich and thick personal

descriptions about the movement skills and experience of children with ADHD.

Movement performance embedded in large descriptive studies. Movement performance

has been assessed in large descriptive studies where the research focus is broad, covering multiple

developmental factors, but the examination of specific movement skills is rather narrow. For

example, Ho et al. (1996) collected a wealth of information about children (e.g., developmental
44

history, sociodemographic data, family relationship, intelligence, reading ability, academic

performance, motor activity level, attention performance, neurological statu s, and motor

clumsiness), but movement performance was represented by catching, only one of39 testing items.

In addition, only two items (e.g., catching and rolling a tennis baIl by foot) were used to assess

movement performance ofchildren with ADHD in several studies (e.g., Ho et al., 1996; Luk,

Leung, & Yuen, 1991; Tripp & Luk, 1997).

A number of large descriptive studies have compared the movement performance of

children with and without ADHD. Tripp and Luk (1997) reported that children with ADHD, ages

5.5 to 12.9 years, demonstrated no significant differences on ball catching and rolling a tennis baIl

by the foot, while Ho et al. (1996) found that 7-year-old children with ADHD performed

significantly poorer when catching a tennis ball. Furthermore, in a longitudinal study (Moffitt,

1990) of children from the ages 3 to 15 years, the scores of males with ADHD were significantly

lower on the Bayley Scale (Bayley, 1969) at 3 years of age, and on the McCarthy Motor Scale

(McCarthy, 1972) at 5 years of age than the control groups.

Longitudinal research indicates that children with comorbid ADHD and DCD, known in

Scandinavian countries as deficits in attention, motor control, and perception or DAMP (Blondis,

1999), are at risk for movement difficulties in both fine and gross movement skills (Gillberg, 1985,

Gillberg, Gillberg, & Groth, 1989; Hellgren, Gillberg, Gillberg, & Enkerskog, 1993; Rasmussen,

Gillberg, Waldenstrom, & Svenson, 1983). However, these large epidemiological studies have

focused mainly on the prevalency of ADHD with DCD (Kadesjo & Gillberg, 1998) or the motor

profiles of children with DAMP as measured by a single neurodevelopmental screening test

(Gillberg, 1985; Gillberg et al., 1989; Hellgren et al., 1993; Rasmussen et al., 1983), rather than on

a comprehensive observation of their movement performance.


45

For example, Rasmussen et al. (1983) tested 112 children, ranging from 6.8 to 8.3 years, on

a neurodevelopmental screening test (Gillberg, 1983). Fine and gross movement skills were

hopping on one leg, standing on one leg, walking on lateral sides of the feet, supination and

pronation ofboth forearms (diadochokinesis), cutting out a paper circle, a labyrinth test,

observance of choreatiform movements, a pencil grip, walking on tiptoe for 20 paces, and hopping

with both feet together. Children with DAMP demonstrated significantly lower scores than

children without disabilities over the course of a 10 year period. The actual percentage of persons

with DAMP who demonstrated poor movement performance dropped from 62% (Rasmussen et al.,

1983) to 300!o after 3 years (Gillberg, 1985),25% after 6 years (Gillberg et al., 1989), and 200!o

after 10 years (Hellgren et al., 1993). These results indicate that sorne children with DAMP may

experience movement difficulties into early adulthood. However, it should be noted that

misclassification rates were approximately 14% (Rasmussen et al., 1983), and interrater reliability

for overall clumsiness was only .55 (Gillberg, 1985). Because the same test items were used over

10 years, test demands may have dec1ined with development, partly accounting for the fewer

individuals being c1assified as poor in motor performance. While the developmental validity of the

testing methods can be questioned, the 20% of children who demonstrated movement difficulties

into late adolescence must have had serious movement problems. These fmdings are the only

empirical evidence that supports the notion that movement performance difficulties can be lifelong

in persons with both ADHD and DCD.

Kadesjo and Gillberg (1998) also assessed 409 seven-year-old children on the same

neurodevelopmental screening test and a physical education motor skills test, the Folke Bernadotte

test (Bille et al., 1985 as cited by Kadesjo & Gillberg, 1998). The testing items for the physical
46

education assessment were not provided. Results indicated that 6.6% of the sample had DAMP and

500/0 ofchildren with ADHD also had DeD.

In general, evidence from the large descriptive studies point to movement performance

difficulties associated with ADHD. However, few movement skills are typically assessed, and

these are usually unrelated to physical activity curricula. Practitioners and researchers require more

detailed information to meet the needs ofindividuals with ADHD.

Skill peiformance. Problems with skill performance have been linked also to children with

ADHD between the agesof5 to 18 years (See Table 2, Appendix F). These studies can be

c1ustered into three groups: (a) intergroup comparison, (b) intragroup comparison, and (c)

intervention effectiveness.

Intergroup comparison research assesses the movement skills of children with ADHD in

relation to norms or control groups. Beyer (1999) tested 112 males on items from the long form of

the BOTMP. Boys with ADHD (N = 56), receiving stimulant medication, performed significantly

below their age-matched male peers with a learning disability (N = 56), who were not receiving

stimulant medication, on four items (bilateral coordination, strength, visual motor coordination,

and upper limb speed and dexterity). Significant differences were not found between the groups for

balance, upper limb coordination, and response speed.

Doyle et al. (1995) used the short form of the BOTMP to test 38 children with ADHD. In

contrast to Beyer (1999), they found that most participants performed movement skills

significantly better than the age and gender norms provided in the BOTMP. However, Bruininks

recommended the short form of the BOTMP be used as "a brief survey of general motor

proficiency" (p. 13), or a screening instrument, because it Is not as detailed as the long form. The

results ofBeyer (1999), perhaps, should be accorded higher relevance.


47

The balancing skills of children with ADHD have also been investigated in several studies.

Piek et al. (1999) tested 48 males on both the Movement ABC, or MABC (Henderson & Sugden,

1992), and the Test ofKinaesthetic Sensitivity (Lazlo & Bairstow, 1985 as cited by Piek et al.,

1999). In comparison to age-matched peers, overall balance scores were significantly lower for

boys with ADHD-C (combined subtype), while manual dexterity scores on the MABC were

significantly lower for boys with ADHD-PI (predominantly-inattentive subtype). There were no

significant differences between any of the groups on the Test ofKinaesthetic Sensitivity.

Wade (1976) compared balance skills of 12 children with ADHD and 12 children without

ADHD on a stabilometer task. While no statistical comparisons were performed, children with

ADHD spent less time balancing when compared to their age- and weight-matched peers without a

disability.

The quality of movement skill patterns of children with ADHD, most of whom were taking

stimulant medication, has been described as "below average" (Harvey & Reid, 1997). After testing

19 children with ADHD on the Test of Gross Motor Development or TGMD (Ulrich, 1985),

Harvey and Reid reported that the children performed locomotor and object control skills below

the 35th percentile when compared to age-matched norms.

Other studies have explored intragroup comparisons. Piek et al. (1999) found that both

overall balance and manual dexterity scores on the MABC were significantly lower for boys with

ADHD-C (combined subtype) when compared to boys with ADHD-PI (predominantly-inattentive

subtype). Miyahara, Mobs, and Doll-Tepper (1995) tested 23 children with hyperkinetic disorders

(HKS) on the MABC and then performed cluster analyses that revealed two motor subtypes: "free

from severe motor incoordination" and "manual incoordination." Manual dexterity and overall
48

balance significantly distinguished between the two different motor subtypes. No performance

differences were found between the two groups in ball catching.

Two other studies also employed an intragroup group approach and focused on the

comorbidity ofHKS or ADHD with DCD. The fust study by Miyahara, Mobs, and Doll-Tepper

(2001) examined the overlap between HKS and DCD by the performance on the.MABC of 47

children with HKS. Considerable overlap between HKS and DCD was noted in the school (35%),

support group (54%), and the hospital (55%) sampi es.

The second comorbidity study assessed 162 children with and without ADHD (Kaplan,

Wilson, Dewey, & Crawford, 1998). Thirty-three percent met criteria for one disorder only. Of

these, 5% had ADHD, 16% had DCD, and 12% had a reading disability (RD). Twenty-four

percent met the criteria for two disorders. Ofthese, 14% had DCD and RD, 6% had ADHD and

DCD, and 4% had ADHD and RD. Overall, comorbidity of ADHD, DCD, and RD was reported

for 14% of the children. These figures suggest that the overlap between ADHD and DCD may be

substantial. Future research should explore this comorbid relationship to determine if the precise

movement difficulties are similar in the comorbid condition and when they occur in isolation. In

addition, do comorbid or singular cases respond to intervention similarly? (Henderson &

Henderson, 2002).

Treatment interventions related to movement performance in children with ADHD have

also been investigated. Hodge, Murata, and Porretta (1999) examined the effects ofthree different

preparatory conditions on the skills ofthrowing for accuracy, ball catching, and fUnning for 46

children with ADHD. The preparatory conditions were no warm-up, a task-specific warm-up, and

mental preparation or visual imagery that were carried out during the fIfSt 5 min of a single 60 to

90 min long testing session. This was a randomized posttest-only control group design where
49

participants were randomly assigned to one of the three groups. The participants performed (a) five

trials ofthrowing a beanbag at a target from a distance of 15 ft (4.57 m), (b) five trials of catching a

sponge ball tossed from 6 ft (l.83 m), and (c) two 40-yd dashes (36.6 m). Throwing accuracy was

significantly better in the visual imagery group when compared to the other groups. Significant

differences were not found between the groups on the ball catching task and the dash. There were

no main effects of gender or interactions.

Wade (1976) tested the effects ofmethylphenidate (Ritalin) on the balance skills of 12

children with ADHD with the use of the stabilometer task in a counterbalanced and placebo-

controlled experiment. There was a 3-week time delay between placebo and medication conditions.

The balance skills of the children improved significantly during the .medication condition.

Pelham et al. (1990) examined the effects ofmethylphenidate on attention and baseball

skills of 17 boys with ADHD in a double-blind and placebo-controlled study. The attention of the

boys improved significantly while playing baseball, but their basebali skills did not.

We should investigate the movement performance and physical activity experiences of

children with ADHD from a number of different perspectives. Longitudinal studies need to be

conducted, because movement performance changes over age have not been explored. We aiso

know little about the physical activity patterns of chiidren with ADHD and influencing factors.

Research should be conducted on the efficacy ofthese interventions in enhancing movement

performance and skill acquisition. Because pharmacological treatment interventions are not always

effective for children with ADHD (Wilens & Spencer, 2000), optimal intervention techniques

should be examined for children on and off medication. Furthermore, as self-regulation seems to

be a problem for children with ADHD (Barkley, 1997), future research should address the

effectiveness of self-regulation strategies in movement performance. Even at a descriptive level,


50

we know little about the movement performance of children with ADHD since the most commonly

used tests, the BOTMP and MABC, are quite limited in the number of movement skills assessed

and may not be relevant to top down programming (Block, 2000, p. 120).

Physicalfitness. Few studies have focused on the physical fitness ofchildren with ADHD,

although related phenomena such as heart rate (HR) have been investigated. For example,

methylphenidate significantly elevates HR during rest (porges, Walter, Korb, & Sprague, 1975).

However, resting HR may be affected by interactions between stimulant medication dosage and the

amount oftime after ingestion. Kelly, Rapport, and DuPaul (1988) stated the amount of ingested

medication may elevate posttest resting HR values on tests of attention. They suggested that

premedication HR may also affect postmedication resting HR and the premedication resting HR

value may be used as a covariate. See Kelly et al. for a thorough discussion about the effects of

stimulants upon resting HR.

Other studies have more specifically explored the physical fitness of children with ADHD

between the ages of5 to 12 years (See Table 3, Appendix G). Boileau, Ballard, Sprague, Sleator,

and Massey (1977) found the HRs of 20 children with ADHD were elevated both at rest and

during aS-min submaximal treadmill walk, when medicated with methlyphenidate compared to a

placebo condition. Oxygen consumption was also significantly lower during the exercise condition

when the children were medicated. Thus, medication has an effect on physical fitness assessment.

Ballard (1977) conducted one of the most comprehensive investigations on health-related

fitness of children with ADHD who were all receiving stimulant medication. Twenty-seven

children with ADHD demonstrated significantly higher levels ofbody fat, with significantly higher

blood pressure and HRs at rest, exercise, and recovery conditions when compared to 23 children

without disabilities.
51

Harvey and Reid (1997) found similar results in their physical fitness field testing.

Compared to normative data, 19 children with ADHD, most of whom were taking stimulant

medication, demonstrated high levels of adipose tissue (75 th percentile). Performance was below

the 25th percentile on a V02max field test (Leger, Lambert, Goulet, Rowan, & Dinelle, 1984), a

shuttle run test (CAHPER, 1980), and a sit-up test (Fitness Canada, 1985). Flexibility and push-ups

were also performed below the 40th percentile (Fitness Canada, 1985).

These three studies (Ballard, 1977; Boileau et al., 1977; Harvey & Reid, 1997) provide an

initial picture ofhealth-related fitness. The findings indicate that children with ADHD are at risk to

develop problems associated with health-related physical fitness.

Trocki-Ables, French, and O'Connor (2001) described the effects of different reinforcers

on the performance ofthe 1 mile/1.6 km walk/run test (Cooper Institute For Aerobics Research,

1992) for 5 boys with ADHD. Each boy was asked to complete the walk/run test as quickly as

possible on each of29 trials. One trial was held each day during a 6-week data collection period.

There were three data collection phases. The frrst phase was three initial trials that served as

baseline. The intervention phase consisted of 24 trials where reinforcers were randomly provided

with either a token (primary), verbal praise (secondary), or in combination, when the individuallap

time fell below the baseline average. The last phase of the data collection period was a

generalization period where two trials were conducted without reinforcement. Trocki-Ables et al.

concIuded that boys with ADHD perform the walk/run test in less time when provided with

combined primary and secondary reinforcers rather than primary or secondary reinforcers alone.

Clearly, there is a lack ofphysical fitness research about children with ADHD. Existing

research should be considered as preliminary because the studies have relatively small sample sizes

with wide age range variability. Yet interesting questions do arise from careful observation of the
52

existing findings. For example, the effects of stimulant medication on respiratory rates need to be

examined as methylphenidate produced significant increases in respiratory rate during rest and

significant decreases in respiratory rate during exercise (Ballard, 1977; Boileau et al., 1977).

Furthermore, no researcher has examined the effects of stimulant medication on HR. during high

intensity exercise. Also, we should use longitudinal studies to examine relationships between

persons with ADHD, health-related physical fitness, and exercise. For example, are children with

ADHD "at risk" for developing cardiovascular heart disease? Are the physical fitness findings

reflective of the current sedentary lifestyles of North American children?

Studies about the health-related fitness of adolescents and adults with ADHD are also

needed (Hartmann, 1993). Research questions about adult ADHD may be found in a review about

the properties of stimulant medications, with many ethical implications for adapted physical

activity (Hickey & Fricker, 1999). For example, stimulant medications are banned in many sports

competitions. Hickey and Fricker questioned whether guidelines should be created to accept the

performance of athletes who have ADHD and have stopped taking medication at least 24 hr prior

to competition.

Conclusions

Our fust conclusion deals with the enormity of the ADHD literature. There are many

exciting avenues ofresearch and practice in ADHD and adapted physical activity, but it is difficult

to achieve a thorough conceptual grasp of the disorder. Adapted physical activity will be

influenced by definition, diagnosis, etiology, nomenclature, classification, and treatment in ADHD.

We conclude that the movement skills of children with ADHD are at risk when compared

to chronological age-matched peers, although as noted throughout the paper there is much to be

learned. The reviewed studies present converging evidence that supports the description of children
53

with ADHD as having problems of movement performance from the vantage point of parents,

teachers, and researchers. This performance is not a function of overactivity that masks valid

assessment, given deficits noted in both quantitative (Beyer, 1999; Harvey & Reid, 1997;

Miyahara et al., 1995,2001; Piek et al., 1999) and qualitative (Harvey & Reid, 1997) performance.

Our conclusion is based on aggregate data. We recognize that sorne individuals with ADHD may

excel in movement skills. Moreover, there is growing evidence that ADHD and DCD might be

comorbid conditions. The aggregate is therefore influenced by those who also have DCD. Future

research should describe movement skills in ADHD when those who warrant the specific

designation ofDCD are eliminated from the participant pool. Would ADHD still be associated

with inferior movement skill performance?

We conclude tentatively that children with ADHD are at risk for poor levels ofphysical

fitness in comparison to their chronological age-matched peers. We are more careful with this

conclusion than with movement skills because the literature is not as complete about health-related

fitness. The relationship between the physical activity levels of children with ADHD and physical

fitness needs to be explored. Comprehensive fitness assessment batteries with large sample sizes

are strongly recommended to observe if children with ADHD possess different physiological

markers than their age- and gender-matched peers without disabilities. Clearly, the interaction

between physiological responses and medication should also be addressed in the exercise context.

It will be crucial to develop collaborative research projects with professionals in

psychology, psychiatry, or special education. Detailed physical activity information may be

invaluable for clinical intervention. Increased sample size, diagnostic reliability, and opportunities

to explore movement issues of children with ADHD, DCD, or DAMP may also be realized.

Much of the existing movement behavior research is atheoretical. Future physical activity
54

researchers should use theoretical models to guide their research. For example, Barkley (1997) has

developed a comprehensive theoretical framework to explain the underlying mechanisms of

ADHD. Movement behavior is an element included in this paradigm, which could be tested by

physical activity researchers. The activity-deficit hypothesis (Bouffard, Watkinson, Thompson,

Causgrove Dunn, & Romanow, 1996) could also be an explanation for the movement skill

difficulties of children with ADHD, where poor movement skills may lead to a diminution of

affect, or vice versa, resulting in reduced social interaction, decreased levels of physical activity,

and diminished physical fitness and health.

It is hoped that this article will benefit adapted physical activity practitioners and

researchers. There remain many unexplored physical activity questions to stimulate further

research. In tum, the findings may prove beneficial for the health and welfare of people with

ADHD and their families.


55

References

American College of Sports Medicine. (2000). Guidelinesfor exercise testing and

prescription, (6th ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical marmal ofmental

disorders, (4th ed.). Text Revision. Washington, OC: Author.

Ballard, J.E. (1977). The effects ofmethylphenidate during rest, exercise, and recovery

upon the circulorespiratory responses of hyperactive children. Microform

Publications (University of Oregon Library No. RM666.M545). (MF)

Barkley, RA (1995). Taking charge ofADHD: The complete authoritative guide for

parents. New York: Guilford Press.

Barkley, RA (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New York: Guilford Press.

Barkley, RA (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbookfor diagnosis

and treatment. New York: Guilford Press.

Barkley, RA, & Biederman, J. (1997). Toward a broader definition of the age-of-onset

criterion for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American

Academy ofChild and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1204-1210.

Bayley, N. (1969). The Bayley Scales ofInfant Development. New York: Psychological

Corporation.

Beyer, R (1999). Motor proficiency of boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 16, 403-414. (MS)

Bishop, P., & Beyer, R (1995). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD):

Implications for physical educators. Palaestra, 11(4),39-45.

Block, M.E. (2000). A teacher 's guide to including students with disabilities in general
56

nd
physical education (2 ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

Blondis, T.A. (1999). Motor disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrie

Clinics ofNorth America, 46, 899-913.

Boileau, RA., Ballard, lE., Sprague, RL., Sleator, E.K., & Massey, RH (1977). Effects

of methylphenidate on cardiorespiratory responses in hyperactive children.

Research Quarterly, 47,590-596. (MF)

Bouffard, M., Watkinson, E.I, Thompson, L.P., Causgrove Duon, IL., & Romanow, S.K.E.

(1996). A test of the activity deficit hypothesis with children with movement difficulties.

Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 13, 61-73.

Bruininks, RH. (1978). The Bruininks-Oseretsky Test ofMotor Proficiency. Circle Pines,

MN: American Guidance Service.

Burton, A. W., & Miller, D.E. (1998). Movement skill assessment. Champaign, IL: Human

Kinetics.

Burton, A. W., & Rodgerson, R W. (2001). New perspectives on the assessment of

movement skills and motor abilities. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 18,347-

365.

CAHPER(1980). CAHPERFitness Performance II TestManual. Ottawa, ON: The

Canadian Association for Health, Physical Education, and Recreation.

Cakirpaloglu, P., & Radil, T. (1992). Perceptual-motor coordination and style in healthy

boys and those with minimal brain dysfunction. Perceptual andMotor Skills, 75,448-450.

(P)

Cantwell, D.P. (1996). Attention deficit disorder: A review of the past 10 years. Journal of

the American Academy ofChild and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 978-987.


57

Churton, M.W. (1989). Ryperkinesis: A review of the literature. Adapted Physical Activity

Quarterly, 6, 313-327.

Conners, c.K., Rothschild, G., Eisenberg, L., Stone Schwartz, L., & Robinson, E. (1969).

Dextroamphetamine sulfate in children with leaming disorders. Archives of General

Psychiatry, 21, 182-190. (P)

Conners, c.K., & Delamater, A (1980). Visual-motor tracking by hyperkinetic children.

Perceptual andMotor SkilIs, 51, 487-497. (P)

Conrad, W.G., Dworkin, E.S., Shai, A, & Tobiessen, lE. (1971). Effects ofamphetamine therapy

and prescriptive tutoring on the behavior and achievement of lower class hyperactive

children. Journal ofLearning Disabilities, 4,45-53. (P)

Cooper Institute For Aerobics Research. (1992). FITNESSGRAM. Dallas, TX: Author.

Craft, D.H. (2000). Learning disabilities and attentional deficits. In IP. Winnick (Ed.),

Adaptedphysical education and sport (3 rd ed.)(pp. 127-141). Champaign, IL:

Ruman Kinetics.

Damico, IS., Damico, S.K., & Armstrong, M.B. (1999). Attention-deficit hyperactivity

disorder and communication disorders: Issues and clinical practices. Child and

Adolescent Psychiatrie Clinics ofNorth America, 8,37-60.

Denckla, M.B., & Rudel, RG. (1978). Anomalies ofmotor development in hyperactive boys.

Armais ofNeurology, 3, 231-233. (P)

Denckla, M.B., Rudel, RG., Chapman, c., & Krieger, l (1985). Motorproficiency in

dyslexic children with and without attentional disorders. Archives ofNeurology, 42,

228-231. (P)

Doyle, S., Wallen, M., & Whitmont, S. (1995). Motor skills in Australian children with
58

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. OccupationalTherapy International, 2,

229-240. (MR)(MS)

Fitness Canada (1985). Physicalfitness of Canadian youth. Ottawa, ON: Government of

Canada, Fitness and Amateur Sport.

Gillberg, e. (1983). Perceptual, motor and attentional deficits in Swedish primary school

children: Sorne child psychiatrie aspects. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 24, 377-403.

Gillberg, e., Carlstrom, G., Rasmussen, P., & Waldenstrom, E. (1983). Perceptual, motor

and attentional deficits in seven-year-old children. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica,

72, 119-124.(P)

Gillberg, I.C. (1985). Children with minor neurodevelopmental disorders, ID: Neurological

and neurodevelopmental problems at age 10. Developmental Medicine and Child

Neurology, 27,3-16. (Dl)

Gillberg, I.e., Gillberg, e., & Groth, J. (1989). Children with preschool minor neurodevelopmental

disorders, V: Neurodevelopmental profiles at age 13. Developmental Medicine and Child

Neurology, 31, 14-24. (Dl)

Gordon, N.G., & Kantor, D.R (1979). Effects ofclinical dosage levels ofmethylphenidate on two-

flash thresholds and perceptual motor performance in hyperactive children. Perceptual and

Motor Skills, 48, 721-722. (P)

Hartmann, T. (1993). Attention deficit disorder: A different perception. Grass Valley, CA:

Underwood Books.

Harvey, W.J., & Reid, G. (1997). Motor performance of children with attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder: A preliminary investigation. Adapted Physical Activity


59

Quarterly,14, 189-202. (MS) (MF)

Hechtman, L. (2000). Assessment and diagnosis of attention-deticit/hyperactivity disorder.

Child and Adolescent Psychiatrie Clinics ofNorth America, 9, 481-498.

Hefley, R.D., & Gorman, D.R (1986). Psychomotor performance ofmedicated and

non-medicated hyperactive emotionally handicapped children and normal children.

American Corrective Therapy Journal, 40, 85-90. (P)

Hellgren, L., Gillberg, C., Gillberg, I.C., & Enkerskog, I. (1993). Children with deficits in

attention, motor control and perception (DAMP) almost grown up: General health

at 16 years. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 35, 881-892. (Dl)

Henderson, S.E., & Henderson, L. (2002). Toward an understanding of developmental

coordination disorder. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 19, 12-31.

Henderson, S.E., & Sugden, D.A. (1992). Movement Assessment Battery for Children.

London: Psychological Corporation.

Hickey, G., & Fricker, P. (1999). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, CNS stimulants and

sport. Sports Medicine, 27, 11-21.

Ho, T.P., Luk, E.S.L., Leung, P.W.L., Taylor, E., Lieh-Mak, F., & Bacon-Shone, 1. (1996).

Situational versus pervasive hyperactivity in a community sample. Psychological

Medicine, 26, 309-321. (Dl)

Hodge, S.R, Murata, N.M., & Porretta, D.L. (1999). Enhancing motor performance through

various preparatory activities involving children with learning disabilities. Clinical

Kinesiology, 53(4), 76-82. (MS)

Kadesjo, B., & Gillberg, C. (1998). Attention deticits and clumsiness in Swedish 7-year-

old children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 40, 796-804. (Dl)
60

Kaplan, BJ., Wilson, B.N., Dewey, D., & Crawford, S.G. (1998). DCD may not be a

discrete disorder. HumanMovement Science, 17,471-490. (MS)

Kelly, K.L., Rapport, M.D., & DuPaul, G.J. (1988). Attention de:ficit disorder and

methylphenidate: A multi-step analysis of dose-response effects on children' s

cardiovascular functioning.lnternational Clinical Psychopharmacology, 3, 167-181.

Kendall, J. (1997). The use of qualitative methods in the study ofwellness in children with

attention de:ficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatrie

Nursing, 10(4), 27-38.

Keogh, J. (1978). Movement outcomes as conceptual guidelines in the perceptual-motor

maze. Journal ofSpecial Education, 12,321-329.

Knights, RM, & Hinton, G.G. (1969). The effects ofmethylphenidate (ritalin) on the

motor skills and behavior of children with learning problems. Journal of Nervous

andMental Disease, 148,643-653. (P)

Korkman, M., & Pesonen, AE. (1994). A comparison ofneuropsychological test profiles of

children with an attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and/or learning disorder. Journal

ofLeaming Disabilities, 27, 383-392. (P)

Leger, L.A, Lambert, J., Goulet, A, Rowan, c., & Dinelle, Y. (1984). Capacite aerobie
des Quebecois de 6 a 17 ans- Test navette de 20 metres avec paliers de 1 minute.

Canadian Journal ofApplied Sport Sciences, 9, 64-69.

Luk, S.L., Leung, P.W.L., & Yuen, J. (1991). Clinic observations in the assessment of

pervasiveness of childhood hyperactivity. Journal ofChild Psychology and Psychiatry and

Allied Disciplines, 32, 833-850. (Dl)


61

McBumett, K, Lahey, B.B., & Ptiffner, L.J. (1993). Diagnosis of attention deticit disorders in

DSM-IV: Scientitic basis and implications for education. Exceptional Children, 60.. 108-

117.

McCarthy, D. (1972). MeCarthy Seales of Children 's Abilities. New York: Psyehologieal

Corporation.

MeMahon, S.A., & Greenberg, L.M. (1977). SeriaI neurologie examination of hyperactive

ehildren. Pediatries, 59, 584-587. (P)

Mercugliano, M. (1999). What is attention-detieitl hyperactivity disorder? Pediatrie Clinies

of North America, 46,831- 843.

Milliehap, IG., Aymat, F., Sturgis, L.H., Larsen, KW., & Egan, RA. (1968). Hyperkinetie

behavior and learning disorders. American Journal ofDiseases of Children, 116,

235-244. (P)

Miyahara, M., Mobs, 1., & Doll-Tepper, G. (1995). Subtypes ofclinieally identified

ehildren with hyperkinetie syndrome based upon perceptual motor function and

classroom behaviors. In 1. Morisbak & P.E. Jorgensen (Eds.), 10th International

Symposium on Adapted Physical Aetivity: Quality of life through "adapted physical

aetivity, A lifespan concept (pp. 278- 286). Oslo, Norway: Ham Trykk NS. (MS)

Miyahara, M., Mobs, 1., & Doll-Tepper, G. (2001). Severity ofhyperactivity and the

comorbidity ofhyperactivity with clumsiness in three sample sources: sehool,

support group and hospital. Child: Care, Health and Development, 27, 413-424. (MS)

Moffit, T.E. (1990). Juvenile delinqueney and attention defieit disorder: Boys'

developmental trajectories from age 3 to age 15. ChildDevelopment, 61, 893-910. (Dl)

Offer, D., Kaiz, M., Howard, KI., & Bennett, E.S. (2000). The altering ofreported
62

experiences. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent

Psychiatry, 39, 735-742.

Ottenbacher, K. (1979). Hyperactivity and related behavioral characteristics in a sample of

learning disabled children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 48, 105-106. (MR)

Palkes, H, Stewart, M., & Kahana, B. (1968). Porteus maze performance of hyperactive

boys aftertraining in self-directed verbal commands. ChildDevelopment, 39,817-

826. (P)

Pelham, W.E., McBurnett, K., Harper, G.W., Milich, R, Murphy, D.A., Clinton, J, &

Thiele, C. (1990). Methylphenidate and baseball playing in ADHD children: Who's

on tirst? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 130-133. (MS)

Pereira, HS., Eliasson, A-C., & Forssberg, A (2000). Detrimental neural control of precision grip

lifts in children with ADHD. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 42,454-553.

(P)

Piek, J.P., Pit cher, T.M., & Hay, D.A (1999). Motor coordination and kinaesthesis in boys with

attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41,

159-165.(MS)

Pliszka, S.R (2000). Patterns of psychiatric comorbidity with attention-

deticitlhyperactivity disorder. Child and Adolescent Psychiatrie Clinics ofNorth

America, 9, 525-540.

Porges, S.W., Walter, G.F., Korb, RJ, & Sprague, RL. (1975). The influences of

methylphenidate on heart rate and behavioral measures of attention in hyperactive

children. Child Development, 46, 727-733. (D2)

Rasmussen, P., & Gillberg, C. (1983). Perceptual, motor and attentional deticits in seven-
63

year-old children. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica, 72, 125-130. (MR)

Rasmussen, P., & Gillberg, C. (1999). AD(H)D, hyperkinetic disorders, DAMP, and

related behavior disorders. In K. Whitmore, H. Hart, & G. Willens (Eds.), A

neurodevelopmental approach to specific leaming disorders (pp. 134-156).

London, England: Cambridge University Press.

Rasmussen, P., Gillberg, C., Waldenstrom, E., & Svenson, B. (1983). Perceptual, motor

and attentional deficits in seven-year-old children: Neurological and

neurodevelopmental aspects. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 25,

315-333.(Dl)

Reeve, RE. (1990). ADHD: Facts and fallacies. Intervention in School and Clinic, 26, 70-

78.

Reeves, Je., Werry, JS., Elkind, G.S., & Zametkin, A (1987). Attention deficit, conduct,

oppositional, and anxiety disorders in children: ll. Clinical characteristics. Journal

of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 144-155. (P)

Reid, G. (1992). Editorial. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 9, 1-4.

Rubia, K., Taylor, A, Taylor, E., & Sergeant, JA (1999). Synchronization, anticipation,

and consistency in motor timing of children with dimensionally defined attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder. Perceptual andMotor Skills, 89, 1237-1258. (P)

Sandberg, S.T., Rutter, M., & Taylor, E. (1978). Hyperkinetic disorder in psychiatric clinic

attenders. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 20, 279-299. (P)

Scahill, L., & Schwab-Stone, M (2000). Epidemiology of ADHD in school-age children.

Child andAdolescent Psychiatrie Clinics ofNorth America, 9, 541-555.

Sherrill, C. (1997). Past, present, future. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, J4, 1-7.
64

Sherrill, C. (1998). Adapted physical activity, recreation and sport: Crossdisciplinary and

lifespan (5th ed.). Dubuque, lA: WCBlMcGraw-Hill.

Sherrill, C., & O'Connor, l (1999). Guidelines for improving adapted physical activity

research. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 16, 1-8.

Silver, L.B. (2000). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adult life. Child and

Adolescent Psychiatrie Clinics ofNorth America, 9, 511-523.

Stewart, M.A., Pitts, F.N., Craig, AG., & Diemf, W. (1966). The hyperactive child

syndrome. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 36, 861-867. (MR)

Stubbe, D.E. (2000a). Preface. ChildandAdolescent Psychiatrie ClinicsofNorthAmerica,

9, xiii-xvii.

Stubbe, D.E. (2 0 OOb). Attention-deficitlhyperactivity disorder: Historical perspective,

current controversies, and future directions. Child and Adolescent Psychiatrie

ClinicsofNorthAmerica, 9,469-47?

Sugden, D.A, & Wright, HC. (1998). Motor coordination disorders in children. In AE.

Kazdin (Series Ed.) Developmental Clinical Psychology and Psychiatry: Vol. 39,

(pp. 1-129). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Swanson, lM., McBurnett, K., Wigal, T., Pffifner, L.I, Lerner, M.A, Williams, L., Christian,

D.L., Tamm, L., Willcutt, E., Crowley, K., Clevenger, W., Khouzam, N., Woo, C.,

Crinella, F.M., & Fisher, T.D. (1993). Effect of stimulant medication on children with

attention deficit disorder: A "review ofreviews." Exceptional Children, 60, 154-162.

Szatmari, P., & Taylor, D.C. (1984). The neurologica1 examination in child psychiatry: A

review ofits uses. CanadianJournal ofPsychiatry, 29, 155-162.

Szatmari, P., Offord, D.R, & Boyle, M.H (1989). Correlates, associated impairments and
65

patterns of service utilization of children with attention deficit disorder: Findings from the

Ontario child health study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 205-217.

(MR)

Tannock, R (1998). Attention deticit hyperactivity disorder: Advances in cognitive,

neurobiological, and genetic research. Journal ofChild Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 65-

99.

Taylor, E., Sergeant, l, Doepfner, M., Gunning, B., Overmeyer, S., Mobius, Hl, & Eisert,

HG. (1998). Clinical guidelines for hyperkinetic disorder. European Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 7, 184-200.

Taylor, E., Schachar, G., Thorley, G., & Weiselberg, M. (1986a). Conduct disorder and

hyperactivity: I. British Journal ofPsychiatry, 149, 760-767. (P)

Taylor, E., Everitt, G., Thorley, R, Schachar, M., Rutter, M., & Weiselberg, M. (1986b).

Conduct disorder and hyperactivity: II. British Journal ofPsychiatry, 149, 768-777.

(P)

Tripp, G., & Luk, S.L. (1997). The identification ofpervasive hyperactivity: Is clinic

observation necessary? Journal ofChild Psychology and Psychiatry, 38,219-234.

(Dl)

Tripp, G., Luk, S.L., Schaughency, E.A., & Singh, R (1999). DSM-IV and ICD-ID: A

comparison of the correlates of ADHD and hyperkinetic disorder. Journal of the American

Academy ofChild and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 156-164.

Trocki-Ables, P., French, R, & O'Connor, 1 (2001). Use ofprimary and secondary

reinforcers after performance of al-mile walk/run by boys with attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Perceptual andMotor Skills, 93,461-464. (MF)


66

Ulrich, D.A. (1985). Test of Gross Motor Development. Austin, TX: PRO-ED, Inc.

Wade, M.G. (1976). Effects ofmethylphenidate on motor skill acquisition of hyperactive

children. Journal ofLearning Disabilities, 9,443-447. (MS)

Weiss, G., & Hechtman, L. (1993). Hyperactive children grawn up (2nd ed.). New York:

Guilford Press.

Werry, IS., Elkind, G.S., & Reeves, J.c. (1987). Attention deficit, conduct, oppositional, and

anxiety disorders in children: ill. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 15, 409-428. (P)

White, H.D. (1994). Scientific communication and literature retrieval. In H. Cooper & L.v.

Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis (pp. 41-55). New York: Russell Sage

Foundation.

Wilens, T.E., & Spencer, T.I (2000). The stimulants revisited Child and Adolescent

Psychiatrie Clinics ofNorth America, 9, 573-603.

Wilson, B.N., Kaplan, B.I, Crawford, S.G., Campbell, A., & Dewey, D. (2000). Reliability

and validity of a parent questionnaire. American Journal of Occupational Therapy,

54, 484-493.

World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 Classification ofMental andBehavioral

Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva: Author.

World Health Organization. (1993). The ICD-10 Classification ofMental and Behavioral

Disorders: Diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva: Author.

Yan, IH., & Thomas, IR. (2002). Arm movement control: Differences between children with and

without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport,

73, 10-18. (P)


67

Authors' Note

1- In fact, the published literature is so large that there has been at least one meta-analysis of

meta-analyses performed (Swanson et al., 1993).

2 - Identifiers include motor processes (P), movement performance - retrospective views (MR),

movement performance - large descriptive studies: movement skills (D 1) and physical fitness

(D2), movement performance - skill performance (MS), movement performance - physical

fitness (MF).

The tirst author wishes to thank Tom Carson, Shirley Edwards, Elaine Mancina, and Sharon

Whelton for their valuable searching skills, input, and assistance. Thanks are also extended to Dr.

A.E (Ted) Wall, Dr. Natalie Grizenko, Dr. Mike Dixon, and Dr. David Kaminester for their

discussions about research.


68

Bridging Manuscripts and Contributions of Authors

Chapter two is a published review paper on the movement skill performance and physical titness

ofchildren with ADHD. The review demonstrated that children with ADHD are at risk for

movement skill difficulties and poor levels of physical titness. A link with developmental

coordination disorder (DCD) was suggested. The next chapter is the second dissertation

manuscript of four original papers that explore the relationship between ADHD and physical

activity. It is a review ofresearch challenges and methods in adapted physical activity and children

with ADHD. The paper is based on the studies and information from the previous chapter and it

also builds on the information, laying the foundation for the two research studies that complete the

dissertation. The relative contributions of the two authors were as follows. As tirst author, 1 was

responsible for 90% of the manuscript and involved in all aspects of the research, writing and

editing of the manuscript. Dr. Greg Reid was responsible for the other 10% of the work involved

by ensuring the reliability of the research process, with multiple reads of the text, editing and

suggestions for text improvement.


69

Chapter Three

Attention-DeficitlHyperactivity Disorder:

AP A Research Challenges

From W.J. Harvey and G. Reid, 2005, Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly 22(1): 1-20. © 2005 by
Human Kinetics Publishers. Reprinted with permission from Human Kinetics (Champaign, IL).

Abstract

The purpose of this paper is to present a critical analysis of the research methods in adapted

physical activity studies about children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

The strengths and weaknesses ofvarious research methods are discussed by (a) three main types

ofresearch questions, (b) identification and description ofresearch participants, (c) reliability and

validity of assessment instruments, (d) data collection procedures, and (e) quantitative and

qualitative methods of data analysis. Strategies to improve research are embedded in each of the

five main categories. It is concluded that substantial methodological inconsistencies exist in the

current ADHD physical activity literature base. Future research would be strengthened by

incorporating recommended suggestions.

.
70

Attention-DeficitIHyperactivity Disorder: AP A Research Challenges

There is a growing awareness of the physical activity challenges of children with

attention-deficitlhyperactivity disorder (ADHD) because they demonstrate poor motor planning

(e.g., Yan & Thomas, 2002), movement skill problems (e.g., Beyer, 1999) and poor levels of

physical fitness (e.g., Ballard, 1977). Adapted physical activity (APA) researchers may have

difficulty designing studies about the movement behavior of children with ADHD because little

information is readily available about pertinent research methods used in ADHD physical activity

studies. For example, few reviews have been written about the movement performance and

physical fitness ofpersons with ADHD (Churton, 1989; Harvey & Reid, 2003; Hickey & Fricker,

1999), with no book chapters or reviews devoted specifically to conducting physical activity

research for persons with the disorder. The lack of information is not surprising, given our field

borrows much research methodology from other professional domains (Reid & Stanish, 2003).

This paper builds upon our previous work (Harvey & Reid, 2003) where we provided an

overview of issues germane to persons with ADHD (e.g., a contemporary definition of ADHD,

symptomatology, etiology, nomenclature, classification, comorbidity, treatment and developmental

course). We suggested that children with ADHD were at risk for problems of movement skill

performance and physical fitness. Readers can observe important characteristics of each study in

the tables provided for the review (e.g., year, purpose, number of participants, diagnostic

framework, prescribed medications if any, statistical analyses, and findings). By careful scrutinyof

these tables, AP A researchers may notice that considerable methodological inconsistencies and

weaknesses exist in the physical activity literature base about ADHD. We wanted to provide

insights to these research issues but, unfortunately, space did not permit these methodological

extensions. For example, stimulant medication is prescribed as a common treatment modality for
71

a substantial number of children with ADHD (Wilens & Spencer, 2000) and it has been part of

many AP A research designs. Yet we will demonstrate that the use of this medication has been

inconsistent and not well controlled in approximately 45% of the identified movement

performance studies. Results may be confounded by extraneous factors, like medication, leading

to questions about the quality ofphysical activity research conducted in ADHD. Since a critical

and detailed review ofresearch methods is warranted, the purpose ofthis paper is to anaIyze the

following question. What research methods were used to explore the movement performance of

children with ADHD in APA studies? New physical activity research questions and strategies to

improve future APA research designs are included in the review, with links to proven methods

used in ADHD research by other professional domains. Interested readers are encouraged to

consult our previous review article in order to acquire generaI information about issues in ADHD

and observe specifie findings about the movement performance of children with ADHD.

Method

Categorization and identification of pertinent studies followed those identified by Harvey

and Reid (2003). The taxonomy ofmovement skills, developed by Burton and Miller (1998), was

used to categorize motor process and movement performance studies. Motor process studies were

identified by dependent variables reflecting underlying internaI factors believed to affect

observable movement (Burton & Miller, 1998). Movement performance studies were identified

with dependent variables that reflected observable or goaI-directed movements that could be

described in quantitative or qualitative terms (Burton & Miller, 1998). These categories were used

to identify studies only from AP A research in order to include investigations that focused on

variables directly related to functionaI movement skill performance and to exclude a number of
72

studies embedded in large behavioral observation research investigations where the primary focus

or interest was not on movement-related behavior (Harvey & Reid, 2003).

An extensive search strategy, similar to Harvey and Reid (2003), was used. The

SPORTDiscus (1830-April, 2004), CUITent Contents/AIl Editions (Week 27, 1993-Week 20,

2004), ERIC (1966-April, 2004), MEDLINE (1966-April, week 4,2004), and PsychINFO (1967-

May, week 1,2004) databases were searched electronicaIly. Keywords linked human movement

science and psychology. Specific search terms included attention deficit, attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder, attention deficit disorder, deficits in attention, motor control and perception

(DAMP), hyperkinesis, hyperactivity, minimal brain dysfunction, motor, fitness, health, exercise,

physiology, sport, and recreation. The footnote chasing approach was also employed to search for

relevant studies that may have been overlooked (White, 1994).

Twenty studies are identified within the time frame of 1830 to May, 2004. Five studies

explore the motor pro cesses of children with ADHD from a contemporary motor control approach

(Eliasson, Rosblad, & Forssberg, 2004; Pit cher, Piek, & Barrett, 2002; Sheppard, Bradshaw,

Georgiou, Bradshaw, & Lee, 2000; Vickers, Rodrigues, & Brown, 2002; Yan & Thomas, 2002).

Fifteen studies examine the movement performance of children with ADHD. Twelve ofthese

studies focus on movement skills (Beyer, 1999; Christiansen, 2000; Doyle, Wall en, & Whitmont,

1995; Harvey & Reid, 1997; Hodge, Murata, & Porretta, 1999; Kaplan, Wilson, Dewey, &

Crawford, 1998; Miyahara, Mobs, & Doll-Tepper, 1995,2001; Pelham et al., 1990; Piek, Pitcher,

& Hay, 1999; Pit cher, Piek, & Hay, 2003; Wade, 1976) and four studies explore physical fitness

(Ballard, 1977; Boileau, Ballard, Sprague, Sleator, & Massey, 1977; Harvey & Reid, 1997;

Trocki-Ables, French, & O'Connor, 2001). Each study is evaluated on the basis of the research

question, participants, instruments, specific data collection procedures, and methods of analysis
73

because strong research methodology includes the careful use ofthese categories (Thomas &

Nelson, 2001). Each category is presented separately because prediction of future research

conditions is difficult at best, with specific conditions varying from one situation to the next,

leaving sorne decisions to make only after a critical analysis by each professional (Bouffard &

Strean, 2003). Therefore, we provide as much information as possible for the APA researcher to

make informed decisions throughout the research process.

Discussion

Research Question

One of the most important aspects of research is the choice of an interesting research

question (Loehle, 1990) that emanates from the review(s) of relevant literature, identification of

a phenomenon ofinterest and a related problem(s). The ensuing hypotheses help to frame the

research study and answer questions related to the research problem. Sherrill and O'Connor

(1999) stated that strong APA methodology should include an explicit statement of the research

question. Since the research questions and hypotheses were not written explicitly in a majority of

the AP A studies about ADHD, these statements can be only inferred from the general purpose

stated for each study. Therefore, a brief summary of the literature is necessary here to provide

explicit statements for three main types of research questions posed, with the associated findings,

in order to grasp the questions and findings that have guided the majority of the published

research.

The fust type of research question involves an intergroup comparison where researchers

ask if performance differences exist between children with and without ADHD on various

dependent variables in motor processes and movement performance. There is converging

evidence that children with ADHD demonstrate poor motor skills, movement skills and levels of
74

physical fitness in comparison to children without ADHD (Ballard, 1977~ Beyer, 1999~

Christiansen, 2000; Doyle et al., 1995; Eliasson et al., 2004; Piek et al., 1999; Pitcher et al., 2002,

2003; Sheppard et al., 2000; Vickers et al., 2002; Wade, 1976; Yan & Thomas, 2002). An

intragroup comparison is the purpose of the second main research question. Do motor skills

and/or movement skills differ between children with ADHD ofvarying subtypes? Motor process

and movement performance differences were found between children with the predominantly-

inattentive (PI), predominantly hyperactive-impulsive (ID), and combined (C) ADHD subtypes

(Eliasson et al., 2004; Kaplan et al., 1998; Miyahara et al., 1995,2001; Piek et al., 1999; Pitcher

et al., 2002, 2003). The third type of research question involves treatment. First, what are the

effects of stimulant medication on movement skill performance and physical fitness? Children

with ADHD have increased the amount oftime spent in balance on a stabilometertask (Wade,

1976), attention during baseball games (Pelham et al., 1990) and gaze frequency during a ping

pong task, with no effect on visual tracking (Vickers et al., 2002) and the performance of

baseball skills (Pelham et al., 1990). Blood pressure and heart rates of children with ADHD

increased significantly during a submaximal treadmill test. Second, what are the effects of

movement-specific interventions on movement performance? Children with leaming disabilities

and ADHD have demonstrated significant improvements in throwing accuracy with the use of a

mental preparation intervention (Hodge et al. 1999) while children with ADHD significantly

improved running performance times when provided with both primary and secondary

reinforcement (Trocki-Ables et al., 2001).

Research questions and hypotheses should be clearly indicated in AP A studies about

ADHD in order to gain a clear understanding of the major questions that drive the exploration,

description, and conceptualization of the movement behavior ofchildren with ADHD. For
75

example, we recommend posing two general questions. First, we need to ask how children with

ADHD develop movement skills over time because, as we will indicate, all of the identified AP A

studies are cross-sectional in nature. Second, no APA researcher has explored the reasons for the

observed inadequate skiU performance so we also need to ask why children with ADHD perform

movement skills poorly when compared to their peers without ADHD?

The lack of research questions and hypotheses provides the unique opportunity to

recommend the development of a variety of AP A research programs, based on different

theoretical approaches, to acquire a more stiuctured understanding about movement performance

from an ADHD perspective. In other words, we recommend research programs that are based on

conceptual meaning (e.g., constructs) where the basic unit ofanalysis (e.g., persons with ADHD)

can be observed from different perspectives and hypotheses can be tested in a variety of contexts.

For example, the inhibitory model of executive functioning (Barkley, 1997) and the knowledge-

based approach to motor development (WalL McClements, Bouffard, Findlay, & Taylor, 1985)

are potential frameworks to explore the movement behavior of children with ADHD. First,

Barkley (1997) hypothesized two of the central problems of ADHD to be an impaired

conceptualization of time and point of performance problems or, in other words, an inability to

perform what one knows. For example, how would the performance ofchildren with ADHD be

atTected on time constrained tasks? Would performance differences between children with and

without ADHD become smaller or larger when movement tasks were slowed down or speeded

up? Barkley (1997) contends that the timing of motor responses and the anticipation of future

events are impaired in children with ADHD. Second, Wall and bis colleagues (1985) developed a

heuristic framework to explore the development of skilled performance in physical activity and it

could be used to inquire about the potentiallinks between movement skill performance and
76

acquired knowledge. For example, are children with and without ADHD able to perform what

they know? Do they share similar or dissimilar experiences that guide the acquisition and

development of movement skills? Are there differences in knowledge and experience between

children with and without ADHD who demonstrate low and high levels of movement skill

proficiency?

Other theoretical paradigms may be of value in understanding ADHD. For example,

motivational theories will be important vehicles to explore the relationship of ADHD and

movement performance and it would be beneficial to describe and explore the causal attributions

of children with ADHD when placed in conditions of movement success and failure. For

example, do children with ADHD attribute their success in physical activity contexts to external

causes (e.g., task was too easy) or internaI causes (e.g., great amount of effort was expended)?

Do they employ internaI or external causal attributions when they face failure in physical activity

situations? Researchers could also ask what impact these causal attributions may have on

adherence to participation in different activities (e.g., leisure and recreation pursuits, physical

education, competitive sports, exercise programs, etc.). In turn, researchers could explore

perceptions ofself-efficacy and self-regulatory processes (Bandura, 1997). For example, how do

children with ADHD feel about their participation in physical activities? What physical activities

do they prefer and why? Do children with ADHD feel confident about their own movement and

sport specific skills? Do children with ADHD feel confident when they participate in individual

or team sports? Do children with and without ADHD regulate their own skill leaming and

behavior in similar ways? Finally, the accumulation and use of more research information may

lead to greater amounts of intervention studies. For example, what types of instructional

programs are most successful for children with ADHD and why? Would a task-specific approach
77

be best for children with ADHD to learn movement skills? Would children with ADHD perform

movement skills better after attribution re-training programs that build upon success and

encourage full participation? What types of recreation and sport programs are most important to

children with ADHD and why? Can children with ADHD be taught how to regulate their

personal involvement in physical activity, recreation, and leisure? What are the opportunities and

barri ers to participation that may affect the personal empowerment of children with ADHD?

Clearly, a paradigmatic approach is recommended for structured understandings ofpersons with

ADHD and their movement skills. Furthermore, there is a need to develop lines of research that

focus on the specific conceptual issues relevant to children with ADHD and their movement skills.

Thus, there are many opportunities to develop basic and applied AP A research programs about

ADHD.

Participants

A thorough description of research participants is generally recommended in journal

articles and texts devoted to physical activity research methodology (e.g., Sherrill & O'Connor,

1999; Thomas & Nelson, 2001). The following three sub-sections deal with a) descriptive

information about the participants in sampling designs, b) participant identification and

diagnosis, and c) issues related to gender and ADHD.

Sampling Design. Sampling designs were not usually reported in the identified studies. AlI

investigations were purposive and cross-sectional. Small sample size is a common problem in AP A

research (Bouffard, 1993) and a similar situation may be observed in APA research about persons

with ADHD. The average total sample size was 51.3 participants. While the average sample size

seems large, there are a few important considerations to note. First, four studies had large numbers

of participants (e.g., N = 112: Beyer, 1999; N = 169; Kaplan et al., 1998, N = 143: Pitcher et al.,
78

2002, 2003) that inflated the average sample size. The average total sample size is 31.3 when the

four large studies are excluded. Thus, 80% of the identified studies had smaller total sample sizes.

While a sample size of 31 participants appears to be adequate, or even robust, the age range of

most studies is usually extensive and, therefore, the number of participants per chronological age

level is quite low.

A similar pattern of inflated average sample size can be observed in the 12 AP A studies

that employed control groups (Ballard, 1977; Beyer, 1999; Christiansen, 2000; Eliasson et al.,

2004; Kaplan et al., 1998; Piek et al., 1999; Pitcher et al., 2002, 2003; Sheppard et al., 2000;

Vickers et al., 2002; Wade, 1976; Yan & Thomas, 2002). For example, the average total sample

size of 64.5 participants (ADHD avg. = 43.4, control avg. = 21) drops to an average of35.6

participants (ADHD = 18.5, control avg. = 15.9) when the four large studies are excluded. Six of
the 12 APA studies utilized control groups with an equal number of participants (Beyer, 1999;

Eliasson et al., 2004; Piek et al., 1999; Sheppard et al., 2000; Wade, 1976; Yan & Thomas, 2002).

Thus, six studies had unequal numbers of participants in each group (Ballard, 1977; Christiansen,

2000; Kaplan et al., 1998; Pitcher et al., 2002, 2003; Vickers et al., 2002) that is problematic

because matching groups on gender and age was not performed. In faet, only two studies overall

(Piek et al., 1999; Sheppard et al., 2000) incorporated matching on gender, age and IQ in their

research designs. The appropriateness of comparing groups with unequal numbers of males and

females in studies related to movement performance is questionable (Sherrill & Williams, 1996).

There were unequal numbers offemales and males in the analysis oftwo studies (Ballard, 1977;

Kaplan et al., 1998) which lead to confounds between gender and the nature of ADHD. For

example, are differences a funetion of typical gender influences on movement performance or

ADHD per se? However, this problem may be best addressed by using equal numbers of males
79

and females in the research design in order to eliminate cross gender analysis and increase both

sample size and the robustness of research designs. The eight remaining AP A studies did not

employ control groups because a norm-referenced test was used (Doyle et al., 1995; Harvey &

Reid, 1997; Miyahara et al., 1995,2001) or an intervention method was assessed (Boileau et al.,

1977; Hodge et al. 1999; Pelham et al., 1990; Trocki-Ables et al., 2001). The average total sample

size for these eight studies was 27 participants.

Thus, generalizations about the movement performance of children with ADHD are not

based on large numbers of children with the disorder. For example, the number of participants with

ADHD, who were assessed in the 20 AP A studies, ranged from 5 to 169 participants per study for

a total of 697 participants with ADHD out of the total 924 participants. Furthermore, the ages of

the participants varied between 4 to 18 years, with an average age range of 4.1 years. Since

movement skills change with age, it is important to seek groups of children within restricted age

ranges. Again, the issue is not total sample size but rather that the samples are typically small,

given the wide age ranges of the participants. Therefore, we must take into account the limitations

of small sample size and age range variability when composing samples and understanding the

results of our research. At the same time, we must realize the challenges to be expected in

obtaining large numbers of participants with ADHD, especially with restricted age ranges. For

example, it might not seem difficult to obtain a large sample of children with ADHD because they

represent 3 to 7 percent of the school-aged population (APA, 2000). Yet access to samples of

children with ADHD may be challenging because they are often involved in large clinical

observation studies where the primary focus or interest is not on movement-related behavior

(Harvey & Reid, 2003). Thus, it is important to collaborate with teaching, clinical and research

professionals in education, psychology and psychiatry in order to gain access to greater numbers of
80

children with ADHD. For example, a strategy to obtain larger sample sizes can be observed in

multi-modal intervention studies where data is collected in many difTerent sites across North

America (Hechtman, 2000). While such a grand scale operation may not always be possible, the

use of a similar approach on a smaller scale at the level of a state, province, or municipality may be

feasible. The AP A researcher should try to control the sample size and age range issues by : a)

including as many participants as possible, and b) using a narrow age range as possible. Thus, APA

researchers must create opportunities that will increase their sample sizes. Sample size will also be

afTected by the identification process of ADHD and gender difTerences that exist with the disorder.

Identification and Diagnosis. The AP A researcher needs to know if participants with

ADHD are diagnosed in a valid and reliable manner. Usually, children with ADHD are initially

identified by their parents and teachers (AP A, 2000) and they are referred to physicians,

psychiatrists, pediatricians, neurologists, and psychologists for assessment and diagnosis. Multiple

diagnostic methods and sources of information are recommended when making the diagnosis of

ADHD (Anastopoulos & Shelton, 2001). Thus, diagnoses should be based on combinat ions of

age-appropriate symptoms, clinical tests, and observations (Anastopoulos & Shelton, 2001~

Hechtman, 2000). Researchers should be aware that heterogeneous samples are more likely the

norm rather than the exception. For example, it is common for persons with ADHD to be

diagnosed with at least one other disorder (Barkley, 1998) and it can take approximately 2 years to

obtain a sample of 20 participants with pure ADHD (personal communication, Dr. L. Hechtman,

1997). Refer to Harvey and Reid (2003) for a list of potential comorbid conditions.

The diagnostic process is not as simple and straightforward as APA researchers might

expect. For example, a comprehensive assessment and a determination of comorbid disorders are

involved in the difTerential diagnosis where the diagnostic professional must be able to take into
81

account the assessment findings and rule out any disorders that are similar in nature to ADHD

(Weiss & Hechtman, 1993). Two different diagnostic frameworks are used to identify children

with either ADHD or hyperkinetic disorders (HKS). Thirteen AP A studies used the criteria from

the Diagnostic and Statisfical Ma11Ual ofMental Disorders, or DSM, (APA, 2000) to identify

children with ADHD (e.g., Beyer, 1999; Christiansen, 2000; Doyle et al., 1995; Eliasson et al.,

2004; Harvey & Reid, 1997; Kaplan et al., 1998; Pelham et al., 1990; Piek et al., 1999; Pitcher et

al., 2002, 2003; Sheppard et al., 2000; Vickers et al., 2002; Yan & Thomas, 2002). Two studies

(e.g., Miyahara et al., 1995,2001) identified children with HKS with the criteria of the ICD-10, the

International Classification ofDiseases (WHO, 1992). Diagnostic frameworks were not reported

in five studies (e.g., Ballard, 1977; Boileau et al., 1977; Hodge et al., 1999; Trocki-Ables et al.,

2001; Wade, 1976). Thus, the reporting of diagnostic framework has been variable.

Pertinent questions about identification and diagnosis can be asked when composing

samples ofpeople with ADHD. One of the main identification and diagnostic research issues about

participants with ADHD lies in a difference between the educational and medical systems. For

example, Hodge et al. (1999) defined their participants as persons with learning disabilities and

attention deficit disorder who were identified by qualified educational personnel. In fact, all

children had been coded by educational diagnostic teams in the appropriate manner as required by

the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act of the United States of America. Since the study

was conducted in North America, it is assumed that the school psychologist(s) used the DSM

framework to diagnose ADHD. Yet, this may not always be the case and, if possible, it would be

beneficial to state which diagnostic criteria and assessment instruments were used to define the

disorder. For example, Harvey and Reid (1997) reported that the DSM framework and information

gathered on the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983) were used in ADHD
82

diagnoses from a clinical center. The main point is that AP A researchers need to secure sampi es of

children who are identified by qualified diagnostic dinicians. In fact, it is strongly recommended to

incorporate experienced professionals for improved reliability of ADHD diagnoses (Hennessey,

1982; cited by McBumett, Lahey, & Pfiffner, 1993). We recommend that researchers state the

relevant framework used to diagnose participants with ADHD. Furthermore, it will be beneficial to

describe the identification process and the physical site where the participants are drawn trom (e.g.,

school, community dinic, hospital, etc.) for replication purposes. The procedures may allow AP A

researchers to identify samples of children with ADHD trom a variety of sources, enabling the

study of children who are affected by the disorder to varying degrees.

Gender differences. The CUITent understanding of movement performance for children with

ADHD is mainly trom a male perspective, with little female representation. For exainple, more

males than females have been included in the majority of movement performance studies on

ADHD. There was approximately 35 female participants (e.g., ADHD = 31, No-ADHD = 4) ofthe

924 participants who were included in APA research designs. Unfortunately, one large study did

not include the number of females with ADHD, assessed in movement performance (Kaplan et al.,

1998), which would have slightly increased the number offemale participants.

Gender differences should be expected because more males are affected with ADHD than

females by ratios of approximately 2: 1 and 9: 1 in general and clinical populations respectively

(AP A, 2000). Potential reasons for these gender differences follow. The DSM symptom criteria

may identify more boys as they were the majority of the participants in the DSM-IV field trials

(Barkley, 2003). AIso, boys are more often referred for and identified with ADHD since they

exhibit more extemalizing and disruptive behaviors when compared to girls who may be diagnosed

at later ages, with intemalized ADHD symptoms and a greater trend toward inattentive behaviors
83

than males (Biederman et al., 1999~ Gaub & Carlson, 1997). Since gender differences in

identification have been recognized, we expect more females to be diagnosed with ADHD over the

next few decades. APA researchers should explore the role of gender in movement skiU

acquisition and performance of children with ADHD. Therefore, when possible, we recommend

that females be included in movement performance studies on ADHD in order to provide a

thorough description of the movement skills of aIl people with ADHD and to represent general

and clinical ADHD populations. Since boys and girls may have different social experiences in

movement and sport activities (Sherrill & Williams, 1996), we also recommend in-depth studies

offemales with ADHD in a variety ofphysical activity contexts. Applied behavior analysis and

qualitative research designs may be more easily applied since it may be difficult to access large

numbers of females with the disorder.

Instruments

The choice of a measurement instrument is linked to the question of interest, area of

investigation, and psychometrie properties of the selected instrument(s). While it is important

that measurement instruments are reliable (Thomas & Nelson, 2001), it is also important that

valid references are made from the measurement results (Yun & Ulrich, 2002). Reliability and

validity seem to be achieved in a majority of the identified AP A studies. One test of movement

behavior was usually administered in each identified AP A study. A finger tapping task (pitcher

et al., 2002), a linear positioning task (Yan & Thomas, 2002), a digitizing tablet for arm

movements (Eliasson et al., 2004), and a kinematical analysis of a ping pong task (Vickers et al.,

2002) are examples of the types of tests used in motor control studies. Background research

information is often provided about the properties of each motor control testing instrument, thus

indicating credible testing methods and results. The majority of movement performance studies
84

also used a single measurement instrument (See Table l, Appendix H). Given the difficulties

observed with the composition of participant samples, the identification of ADHD, and gender

differences~ we must question the depth of our understanding of the movement skill learning and

performance of children with ADHD, especially since few comprehensive batteries of movement

behavior have been performed. For example, only one study (Harvey & Reid, 1997) used a series

of tests to describe the movement skills of children with ADHD by administering the Test of

Gross Motor Development (Ulrich, 1985), a V02max test (Leger, Lambert, Goulet, & Dinelle,

1984), a shuttle run test (CAHPER, 1980), and tests of sit-ups, push-ups, and flexibility (Fitness

Canada, 1985). Yet this study was limited by a small sample size and the lack of a control group

(Harvey & Reid, 1997). Control groups of children without ADHD may not have been necessary

when investigating a treatment intervention for children with ADHD (e.g., Boileau et al., 1977;

Hodge et al. 1999; Pelham et al., 1990; Trocki-Ables et al., 2001). Yet there are potential

generational effects when, instead of control groups, norm-referenced tests were used for

comparison purposes in movement performance (e.g., Doyle et al., 1995~ Harvey & Reid, 1997).

Thus, there may be a superficial understanding of the movement performance of children

with ADHD because the research has been limited by the depth of each investigation or the

significant limitations indicated above. There is a need to incorporate multiple testing batteries to

explore a variety of fundamental movement skills and specific sport skills to obtain a better

understanding of the movement performance ofchildren with ADHD.

Data Collection Procedures

The research environment, motivation, and medication use are three major factors in the

organization of APA studies in ADHD. Since children with ADHD can behave quite differently

than their peers without ADHD (Cooper & Bilton, 2002), the AP A researcher should structure
85

research environments so children may perform experimental tasks at optimal performance levels

(Houghton et al., 2004; Lawrence et al., 2002). For example, variable task performance may be

expected due to problems of response inhibition, or delayed responding, and understanding of tasks

(Barkley, 1997, 1998). In an attempt to control forthese effects, children with ADIID should be

allowed to perform at their own pace with additional demonstrations provided when they do not

seem to understand (Douglas, 1980a, 1980b). The AP A researcher should also be aware of other

leaming conditions of children with ADHD to optimize performance conditions. For example, the

children may experience less behavioral problems with novel tasks or environments, but this may

diminish with time (Zentall, 1993). Children with ADHD may talk excessively during non-

instructional times (Zentall, 1993) and spend less time on-task than controls (Seidel & Joschko,

1990). Therefore, it would be beneficial to provide instructions in a clear, brief, and overt manner

to address the behavioral issues or listening difficulties of children with ADHD (pfiffner &

Barkley, 1990). It is recommended to collect data individually since the unique behavioral

problems and learning styles of children with ADIID may affect optimal movement performance

(Harvey & Reid, 1997). For example, the frrst author attempted to collect physical activity data on

four children with ADIID at the same time to maximize the efficiency of data collection. This

aggregate data collection strategy was quickly changed to an individual approach after the

observation was made that the children would tease others during task performance, and in tum,

not pay attention to their own specifie task demands.

Motivation is an important factor when designing APA research (Wall, 1990). For

example, learned helplessness has been linked to children with ADHD (Milich & Okazaki, 1991)

and its influence could have detrimental effects on the children's movement performance. The

APA researcher may assist the individual with ADHD in optimal performance by being positive,
86

encouraging, and using successful reinforcement strategies during the research process (Harvey &

Reid, 1997). For example, successfullearning experiences may be facilitated by consistent and

immediate rewards for appropriate compliance to tasks (Douglas & Parry, 1994). However,

children with ADHD may become very fiustrated when expected rewards are not provided

(Douglas & Parry, 1994). Reinforcers must be changed from time to time because the children may

experience satiation to behavior management consequences (pfiffner & Barkley, 1990). Immediate

individual performance feedback and academic tasks that are sensitive to an individual's ability and

learning pace should also facilitate learning (Cooper & Bilton, 2002). Children with ADHD may

perform better on demanding tasks during the morning since their performance can be affected by

fatigue and the time of day (Zagar & Bowers, 1983). Therefore, we recommend that researchers try

to (a) exhibit positive behaviors during the data collection period, (b) allow participants with

ADHD to perform experimental tasks at their own pace, (c) keep a steady pace during data

collection by providing instructions in a clear and direct fashion, (d) maximize the time efficiency

of data collection by not getting involved in long discussions or argumentative behaviors, and (e)

collect data during the morning.

Medication usage is an extremely complex factor to consider when designing research

studies about ADHD. Stimulant medication has been the most widely used treatment approach for

children with ADHD (Wilens & Biederman, 1992; Wilens & Spencer, 2000). In fact, stimulants

are used to treat over 1 million school-age children per year in the United States (Greenhill,

Halperin, & Abikoff, 1999; Jensen et al. 1999). Thus, AP A researchers should be aware of

stimulant medication administration as research findings may be affected by dosing schedules,

individual practices, and side-effects. These practices are individualized and differential

intervention modalities may affect the results ofan investigation if the researcher is not aware. For
87

example, methylphenidate (Ritalin), dextroamphetamine (Dexedrine), and amphetamine

compounds (Adderall) seem to be the most popular stimulant prescriptions due to rapid clinical

effects and short half-lives (Wilens & Spencer, 2000). These stimulants are usually administered

two to three times per day in an oral pill form. The medications are also available in a slow release

format to avoid noon dosing regimens, ensure that medication is taken, have lasting effects into the

aftemoon, and avoid social stigma (Weiss & Hechtman, 1993). The APA researcher shouid note

individual dosage levels, time of medication ingestion, and, if necessary, search for experimental

differences due to these practices.

Yet, stimulant medications are not the only prescription option for the pharmacological

treatment of the chiidren (Wilens & Spencer, 2000). Therefore, researchers should inquire about

the different types of medications that the participants are taking. For example, the anti-

hypertensive medication, Cionidine, has aiso been prescribed for children with ADHD who are

poor responders to psycho stimulant treatment and for children with motor tics (Steingard,

Biederman, Spencer, Wilens, & Gonzalez, 1993). Clonidine may be used to decrease the problems

of insomnia associated with methylphenidate use because sleepiness, sedation, lowered blood

pressure, and decreased motor activity are possible si de effects of Clonidine use (Steingard et al.,

1993; Wilens, Biederman, & Spencer, 1994). Therefore, researchers are well-advised not to

assume that stimulant medications are the only prescriptions they need to be aware of when

designing their studies. Thus, it is very important for the AP A researcher to ask if participants are

receiving medication and, if so, what types of medication are prescribed?

Each prescribed medication has a potential side-effect that may have an influence on the

outcome of a research study. Thus, awareness of medication side effects is important for the AP A

researcher because the results of an investigation may be affected by individual reactions to


88

medication. The four most commonly reported side effects of stimulant medication, likely to affect

children with ADHD to varying degrees, are sleep disturbance, appetite suppression, headaches,

and stomachaches (Barkley, McMurray, Edelbrock, & Robbins, 1990). It is also important to be

aware that other less commonly reported side-effects have been identified with the use of stimulant

medication. For example, these side-effects are growth disturbance, weight loss, mood

disturbances, chronic tic disorders, behavioral rebound, seizure disorder, increased lethargy and

fatigue, and increases in heart rate and blood pressure (Barkley, 1977; Barkley et al., 1990;

DuPaul, Barkley, & Connor, 1998). Sorne children with ADHD may receive drug-free periods

during weekends, school vacations, and holidays as a consequence of severe reactions to

medication (DuPaul et al., 1998; Wilens & Spencer, 2000). However, an experimental confound

may arise if the AP A researcher is unaware that a participant(s) may be receiving these drug

holidays during the data collection period. Thus, side-effect information is important to inquire

about and understand before data are collected so the researcher can plan and prepare research

designs that can best explain the influence of medication on their results.

It is evident that the control of medication is vital to research designs since children with

ADHD may experience numerous side-effects from prescribed medication. Unfortunately, there is

variability across AP A studies with regard to medication usage. Stimulant medication was

controlled in the research design for I1of20 APA studies. For example, all children with ADHD

received stimulant medication in seven studies (e.g., Ballard, 1977; Beyer, 1999; Boileau et al.,

1977; Pelham et al., 1990; Sheppard et al., 2000; Vickers et al., 2002; Wade, 1976) and they did

not receive it at all during four other studies (e.g., Eliasson et al., 2004; Pitcher et al., 2002, 2003;

Yan & Thomas, 2002). Experimental control was exerted over the medication factor, thus

maximizing or minimizing the influence of the stimulant medication on the results. However, there
89

is less control over medication in the remaining nine AP A studies. For example, we could not

determine if the children with ADHD received stimulant medication in five studies because

medication usage was not reported by the authors (e.g., Christiansen, 2000; Hodge et al., 1999;

Kaplan et al., 1998; Miyahara et al., 2001; Trocki-Ables et al., 2001). Furthermore, the use of

stimulant medication was variable during the data collection in four other studies (e.g., Doyle et al.,

1995; Harvey & Reid, 1997; Miyahara et al., 1995; Piek et al., 1999). For example, 17 out of 19

participants received medication in one study (Harvey & Reid, 1997) while only 8 out of 48

participants received medication in another investigation (Piek et al., 1999).

It would be preferable to have either all children under medication or none at all during

data collection in order to maximize or minimize the extraneous influence of medication on

movement performance. However, potential conflicts ofinterest between research methodology

and ethics are possible. The major consideration when renioving children with ADHD from

medication is that the children may experience severe negative effects in daily functioning due to

the medication withdrawal. Thus, the AP A researcher may not always be able to exert control over

the administration ofmedication as it might be considered unethical to have a participant(s)

withdraw from a much needed pharmacological treatment. Yet the inability to control the

independent variable (e.g., medication) may impinge on the results.

Therefore, we recommend a variety of investigations to explore the effects of stimulant

medication on the performance and learning ofmovement skills for children with ADHD. For

example, do the movement skills of children with ADHD improve while they are receiving

stimulant medication? Do children with ADHD learn movement skills better when they are or are

not receiving stimulant medication? Double-blind and placebo-controlled designs may help to

answer such questions. For example, the participants are unaware whether or not they are receiving
90

a tablet of either medication or a non-medicinal ingredient (e.g., placebo-controlled) and both the

participants and all researchers are unaware of the specifie assignment of medication or placebo to

individual participants (e.g., double-blind).

Placebo-controlled investigations are possible because of the short half-lives and quick

washouts of stimulant medication. Since methylphenidate is eliminated (e.g., metabolized

completely, washed out) from the body within 24 hours (DuPaul et al., 1998), children with

ADHD may refrain from medication for short periods oftime with minimallifestyle disturbance.

However, the AP A researcher must be aware that inconsistent medication use could cause negative

effects upon the lives of the participants and their families. For example, Wade (1976) employed a

three week wash-out period for a study that examined the effects of methylphenidate on a

balancing task. While the research design was appropriate for the timeframe (e.g., 1970's), it is

now understood that the washout period could be shortened, with less stress placed on the children

with ADHD and their families. The personal intrusion, created by removing medication for

extended periods oftime, may be too high a priee for the children and their families to pay.

Therefore, we must be sensitive to the needs of participants while, at the same time, attempting to

control for the influence of medication. Therefore, the AP A researcher can design cross-sectional

studies oflimited time duration, with placebo-control conditions because of the short-halflife of

methylphenidate. Applied behavior analyses may also be useful when observing the effects of

receiving or removing medication on the skilllearning and performance of children with ADHD.

With time and research evidence, we should know which types of studies are possible to conduct in

relation to the strict control of stimulant medication.

ln summary, AP A researchers need to ask pertinent questions about medication and their

participants with ADHD. First, are the participants receiving medication as a treatment for ADHD?
91

We expect that stimulant medication will be an important design factor to consider since it has

been proven very effective for children with ADHD in the short-term and it is used by a significant

number of children with ADHD (Jensen et al. 1999). Second, the AP A researcher needs to ask if it

is necessary or possible for the participants with ADHD to refrain from taking their medication(s)?

The AP A researcher should provide evidence that stimulant medication will have a negligible

effect on movement performance if it is deemed unnecessary to have participants with ADHD

refrain from taking their medication during data collection. Third, specifie prescriptions and dosage

levels should be reported to provide more accurate participant information if medications are

administered during a study. Researchers should also report if no medications are taken by

participants. Fourth, the AP A researcher should ask if participants reported any side-effects during

the data collection period. Since the side-effects of medication are variable and idiosyncratic

(DuPaul et al., 1998), it may be beneficial to investigate if side effects can aecount for individual

skill performances that, in turn, may be considered as outlier data. Finally, the APA researcher

should ask if participants are experiencing severe behavioral reactions due to the withdrawal of

medication. If so, the decision to exc1ude the participant may be made since the health and welfare

of the individual is the primary ethical concern in our research designs. Consult the following

literature reviews concerning the pharmacological treatment of persons with ADHD (Wilens &

Biederman, 1992; Wilens & Spencer, 2000).

Methods ofAnalysis

Most of the identified studies used a quantitative approach for data analysis. For example,

between-group (Ballard, 1977; Beyer, 1999; Boileau et al., 1977; Christiansen, 2000; Doyle et

al., 1995; Eliasson et al., 2004; Piek et al. 1999; Pitcher et al., 2002; Sheppard et al., 2000;

Vickers et al., 2002; Yan & Thomas, 2002) and within-group (Hodge et al., 1999; Miyahara et
92

al., 1995~ 2001~ Kaplan et al., 1998~ Pelham et al., 1990; Piek et al., 1999; Pitcher et al., 2002,

2003; Vickers et al., 2002; Wade, 1976) statistical approaches were used to analyze the

movement performance of children with ADHD. The movement performance of children with

ADHD has also been explored with an applied behavior analysis approach on the etfects of

reinforcement on performance in a field-based physical fitness test (Trocki-Ables et al., 2001)

and a description of fundamental movement skill performance (Harvey & Reid, 1997). While

these standard analyses have provided reliable movement-related information, there are a few

methodological concerns that are of importance during the analysis phase.

First, we know little about the influence of potentially important independent variables on

the movement behavior of children with ADHD. For example, what are the etfects of

socioeconomic status (SES) on the fundamental movement skills of children with ADHD?

Family income level could have an effect on individual participation in physical activity and

sport due to the rising financial costs associated with organized sports and community recreation

programs. Furthermore, parentallevels of education could be important when determining the

value and emphasis placed on physical activity, health and wellness that, in turn, may have

effects on participation levels. Therefore, it may be important to match groups of children with

and without ADHD on SES, family in come, and parent education levels. It may also be

important to inquire whether or not the parents have ADHD as there is a genetic link with the

disorder (APA, 2000). Perhaps parents also experience difficulty in movement performance,

leading to a decreased family value placed on physical activity. Next, few studies have been

designed to observe the effects of intervention programs. Since intervention studies are important

and unique contributions to AP A research about all children with ADHD, it may be beneficial to

use applied behavior analysis (e.g., times series designs, reversaI designs) to measure
93

intervention outcomes at both individual and group levels. Clearly, more applied research should

be conducted on the fundamental movement skills and physical fitness of children with ADHD.

Additionally, small sample size is an important ADHD issue related to analysis and it may be

beneficial to use repeated measures designs to increase statistical power. FinaIly, the qualitative

paradigm has not been used to explore and explain the movement behavior of children with

ADHD. Thus, it is important to have children with ADHD express their own feelings and

perceptions about their movement and sport behaviors.

Summary and Future Directions

ADHD is a significant challenge in the schools, clinics, and hospitals of communities aIl

over North America. Converging evidence suggests that children with ADHD demonstrate

movement skill problems (Harvey & Reid, 2003). Yet substantial methodological inconsistencies

exist in the AP A research database and they must be addressed in order to confirm the results of

past research. Generally, the use oflongitudinal designs, with developmentally-sensitive

assessment instruments, is recommended since aU of the AP A studies have been cross-sectional

in nature. The following specific methods are suggested to strengthen AP A designs and to

answer the main questions of interest. First, research questions and hypotheses should be clearly

articulated. Two general questions are suggested to guide future ADHD research. For example,

we need to ask how children with ADHD develop movement s10lls over time and why children

with ADHD perform movement s10lls poorly when compared to their peers without ADHD?

Theoretical frameworks and the development of a line of research are recommended to guide

future research about children with ADHD and movement behavior. Next, it is recommended

that as much general descriptive information about the participants with and without ADHD be

collected. For example, the sampling design could be explicitly stated, with a description of the
94

process of participant identification and a naming of the relevant diagnostic frameworks used to

diagnose ADHD, thus improving the credibility of the participant identification and diagnosis.

The inclusion offemales is suggested for a representative description of the movement behavior

of general and clinical populations for all people with ADHD. Further, the influence ofkey

independent variables needs to be explored in order to identify plausible explanatory factors for

the poor movement behavlor of children with ADHD. Since our current AP A research base

seems to lack a depth ofunderstanding of the movement problems with ADHD, we recommend

the use of multiple assessment batteries to acquire greater amounts of information about the

phenomenon. AIso, it is crucial to understand specific methods used to aid data collection in the

organization of APA studies in ADHD (e.g., research environment, motivation, & medication).

We recommend the combination of quantitative and qualitative methods, along with the use of

repeated measures designs and applied behavior analysis designs, as necessary and

complimentary analytical approaches to understand the various correlates of movement behavior

associated with ADHD.

There is a unique opportunity to have a significant AP A treatment impact on a substantial

number of people atTected by ADHD and to gain much insight about the movement behavior,

skilllearning and individual perceptions of the movement experiences ofpersons with ADHD.

As such, physical activity research about ADHD will continue to be a fertile area of

investigation.
95

References

Achenbach, T.M., & Edelbrock, C. (1983). The child behaviour checklist and revised child

behavior profile. Burlington, VT: University ofVennont, Department of

Psychiatry.

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual ofmental

disorders, (4th ed.). Text Revision. Washington, OC: Author.

Anastopoulos, AD., & Shelton, T.L. (2001). Assessing Attention-Deficit / Hyperactivity

Disorder. New York: Kluwer Academic / Plenum Publishers.

Ballard, J.E. (1977). The effects ofmethylphenidate during rest, exercise, and recovery

upon the circulorespiratory responses of hyperactive children. Microform

Publications (University of Oregon Library No. RM666.M545).

Bandura, A (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman and

Company.

Barkley, RA (1977). A review of stimulant drug research with hyperactive children. Child

PsychologyandPsychiatry, 18, 137-165.

Barkley, RA (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New Yorlc Guilford Press.

Barkley, RA (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handhookfor diagnosis

and treatment (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.

Barkley, RA (2003). Issues in the diagnosis of attention-deficitlhyperactivity disorder.

Brain & Development, 25, 77-83.

Barkley, RA, McMurray, M.B., Edelbrock, C.S., & Robbins, K. (1990). Side effects of

methylphenidate in children with attention de:ficit hyperactivity disorder: A

systemic, placebo-controlled evaluation. Pediatries, 86, 184-192.


%

Beyer, R (1999). Motor proficiency ofboys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 16, 403-414.

Biederman, l, Faraone, S.v., Mick, E., Williamson, S., Wilens, T., Spencer, T.l, et al. (1999).

Clinical correlates of ADHD in females: Findings trom a large group of girls ascertained

from pediatric and psychiatric referral sources. Journal of the American Academy of Child

and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38,966-975.

Boileau, RA., Ballard, lE., Sprague, RL., Sleator, E.K., & Massey, B.H. (1977). Effects

of methylphenidate on cardiorespiratory responses in hyperactive children.

Research Quarterly, 47, 590-596.

Bouffard, M. (1993). The perils of averaging data in adapted physical activity research.

Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 10,371-391.

Bouffard, M., & Strean, W.B. (2003). Critical thinking and professional preparation. In

RD. Steadward, G.D. Wheeler, & EJ. Watkinson (Eds.), Adapted Physical

Activity. (pp. 1-10). Edmonton, Alberta, Canada: The University of Alberta Press.

Bruininks, RH. (1978). The Bruininks-Oseretsky Test ofMotor Proficiency. Cirele Pines,

MN: American Guidance Service.

Burton, AW., & Miller, D.E. (1998). Movement skill assessment. Champaign, IL: Human

Kinetics.

CAHPER (1980). CAHPER Fitness Peiformance II Test Manual. Ottawa, ON: The

Canadian Association for Health, Physical Education, and Recreation.

Christiansen, A. S. (2000). Persisting motor control problems in 11- to 12-year-old boys previously

diagnosed with deficits in attention, motor control and perception (DAMP). Developmental

Medicine & ChildNeurology, 42,4-7.


97

Churton, M.W. (1989). Hyperkinesis: A review of the literature. Adapted Physical Activity

Quarterly, 6, 313-327.

Cooper, P., & Bilton, K.M. (2002). Attention deficitlhyperactivity disorder: A practical guide for

teachers (rd Ed) .London, UK: David Fulton Publishers.

Cooper Institute For Aerobics Research. (1992). FITNESSGRAM. Dallas, TX: Author.

Douglas, VI. (1980a). Higher mental processes in hyperactive children: Implications for

treatment. In R Knight & D. Bakker (Eds.), Treatment of hyperactive and leaming

disordered children (pp. 65-92). Baltimore: University Park Press.

Douglas, VI. (1980b). Treatment and training approaches to hyperactivity: Establishing

internal or external control. In C. Whalen & B. Henker (Eds.), Hyperactive

children: The social ecology of identification and treatment (pp. 283-318). New

York: Academic Press.

Douglas, VI., & Parry, P.A. (1994). Effects ofreward and nonreward on frustration and

attention in attention deficit disorder. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 22,

281-302.

Doyle, S., Wallen, M., & Whitmont, S. (1995). Motor skills in Australian children with

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. OccupationalTherapy International, 2,

229-240.

DuPaul, G.J., Barkley, RA., & Connor, D.F. (1998). Stimulants. In RA. Barkley (Ed.),

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (2nd

ed.)(pp. 510-551). New York: Guilford Press.

Eliasson, A-C., Rosblad, B.R, & Forssberg, H. (2004). Disturbances in programming goal-

directed movements in children with ADHD. DevelopmentalMedicine & Child


98

Neurology, 46, 19-27.

Fitness Canada (1985). Physicalfitness ofCanadian youth. Ottawa, ON: Government of

Canada, Fitness and Amateur Sport.

Gaub, M., & Carlson, c.L. (1997). Gender differences in ADHD: A meta-analysis and

critical review. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent

Psychiatry,36, 1036-1044.

Greenhill, L.L., Halperin, J.M., & Abikoff, H. (1999). Stimulant medications. Journal of the

American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 503-512.

Harvey, W.J., & Reid, G. (1997). Motor performance of children with attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder: A preliminary investigation. Adapted Physical Activity

Quarterly, J4, 189-202.

Harvey, W.J., & Reid, G. (2003). A review offundamental movement skill performance

and physical fitness of children with ADHD. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly,

20, 1-25.

Hechtman, L. (2000). Assessment and diagnosis of attention-deficitlhyperactivity disorder.

Child and Adolescent Psychiatrie Clinics ofNorth America, 9, 481-498 ..

Henderson, S.E., & Sugden, D.A. (1992). Movement Assessment Battery for Children.

London: Psychological Corporation.

Hickey, G., & Fricker, P. (1999). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, CNS stimulants,

and sport. Sports Medicine, 27, 11-21.

Hodge, S.R, Murata, N.M., & Porretta, D.L. (1999). Enhancing motor performance

through various preparatory activities involving children with learning disabilities.

Clinical Kinesiology, 53(4), 76-82.


99

Houghton, S., Milner, N., West, 1, Douglas, G., Lawrence, v., Whiting, K., et al. (2004). Motor
control and sequencing ofboys with Attention-DeficitIHyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

during computer game play. British Journal ofEducational Technology, 35,21-34.

Jensen, P.S., Kettle, L., Roper, M.T., Sloan, M.T., Dulcan, M.K, Hoven, c., et al. (1999). Are
stimulants overprescribed? Treatment of ADHD in four U. S. communities. Journal of the

American Academy ofChild and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 797-804.

Kaplan, B.I, Wilson, B.N., Dewey, D., & Crawford, S.G. (1998). DCD may not be a

discrete disorder. HumanMovement Science, 17,471-490.

Lawrence, V, Houghton, S., Tannock, R, Douglas, G., Durkin, K, & Whiting, K (2002).

ADHD outside the laboratory : Boys' executive function performance on tasks in

videogame play and on a visit to the zoo. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology,

30,447-462.

Leger, L.A., Lambert, 1, Goulet, A, Rowan, c., & Dinelle, Y. (1984). Capacite aerobie
des Quebecois de 6 à 17 ans- Test navette de 20 metres avec paliers de 1 minute.

Canadian Journal ofApplied Sport Sciences, 9, 64-69.

Loehle, C. (1990). A guide to increased creativity in research-Inspiration or perspiration.

BioScience, 40, 123-129.

McBumett, K., Lahey, B.B., & Pfiffner, L.I (1993). Diagnosis of attention deficit disorders in

DSM-IV: Scientific basis and implications for education. Exceptional Children, 60.. 108-

117.

Millich, R, & Okazaki, M. (1991). An examination ofleamed helplessness among attention-deficit

hyperactivity disordered boys. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 19, 607-623.

Miyahara, M., Mobs, 1, & Doll-Tepper, G. (1995). Subtypes of clinically identified


100

children with hyperkinetic syndrome based upon perceptual motor function and

c1assroom behaviors. In 1. Morisbak & P.E. Jorgensen (Eds.), 1dh International

Symposium on Adapted Physical Activity: Quality of life through adapted physical

activity, A lifespan concept (pp. 278- 286). Oslo, NOIway: Ham Trykk A/S.

Miyahara, M., Mobs, 1., & Doll-Tepper, G. (2001). Severity ofhyperactivity and the

comorbidity of hyperactivity with c1umsiness in three sample sources: school,

support group and hospital. Child: Care, Health and Development, 27, 413-424.

Pelham, W.E., McBurnett, K., Harper, G.W., Milich, R, Murphy, D.A, Clinton, J., &

Thiele, C. (1990). Methylphenidate and baseball playing in ADHD children: Who's

on first? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 130-133.

Pfiffner, L.l, & Barkley, RA (1990). Educational placement and c1assroom management.

In RA Barkley (Ed.), Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A handbookfor

diagnosis and treatment. (pp. 498-538). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Piek, lP., Pitcher, T.M., & Hay, D.A (1999). Motor coordination and kinaesthesis in boys with

attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41,

159-165.

Pitcher, T.M., Piek, J.P., & Barrett, N.C. (2002). Timing and force control in boys with attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder: Subtype differences and the effect of comorbid

developmental coordination disorder. HumanMovement Science, 21,919-945.

Pitcher, T.M., Piek, lP., & Hay, D.A (2003). Fine and gross motor ability in males with ADHD.

Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 45, 525-535.

Reid, G., & Stanish, H. (2003). Professsionai and disciplinary status of adapted physical

activity. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 20,213-229.


101

Seidel, W.T., & Joschko, M. (1990). Evidence of difficulties in sustained attention in

children with ADDH. Journal ofAbnormal ChildPsychology, 18,217-229.

Sheppard, D.M., Bradshaw, J.L., Georgiou, N., Bradshaw, lA., & Lee, P. (2000).

Movement sequencing in children with tourette's syndrome and attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Movement Disorders, 15, 1184-1193.

Sherrill, c., & O'Connor, l (1999). Guidelines for improving adapted physical activity

research. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 16, 1-8.

Sherrill, C., & Williams, T. (1996). Disability, and sport: Psychosocial perspectives on

inclusion, integration, and participation. Sport Science Review, 5(1),42-64.

Steingard, R, Biederman, J., Spencer, T., Wilens, T., & GonzaIez, A. (1993). Comparison

of clonidine response in the treatment of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

with and without comorbid tic disorders. Journal of the American Academy of

Child andAdolescent Psychiatry, 32,350-353.

Thomas, IR, & Nelson, IK. (2001). Research Methods In Physical Activity (4th ed.).

Champaign, TI: Ruman Kinetics.

Trocki-Ables, P., French, R, & O'Connor, l (2001). Use ofprimary and secondary

reinforcers after performance of al-mile walk/run by boys with attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Perceptual andMotor Skills, 93,461-464.

Ulrich, D.A. (1985). Test of Gross Motor Development. Austin, TX: PRO-ED, Inc.

Vickers, IN., Rodrigues, S.T., & Brown, L.N. (2002). Gaze pursuit and arm control of

adolescent males diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

and normal controls: evidence of a dissociation in processing visuaI information of

a short and long duration. Journal ofSport Sciences, 20,201-216.


102

Wade, M.G. (1976). Effects ofmethylphenidate on motor skill acquisition of hyperactive

children. Journal ofLearning Disabilities, 9,443-447.

Wade, M.G., & NewelL K.M. (1972). Performance criteria for stabilometer learning.

Journal ofMotor Behavior, 4,231-239.

WalL A.E. (1990). Skill acquisition research with persons with developmental disabilities:

Research design considerations. In G. Reid (Ed.), Problems ofMovement Control. North

Holland: Elsevier Science Publishers B. V.

Wall, A.E., McClements, l, Bouffard, M., Findlay, H, & Taylor, M.l (1985). A

knowledge-based approach to motor development: Implications for the physically

awkward. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 2, 21-42.

Weiss, G., & Hechtman, L. (1993). Hyperactive Children Grawn Up, (2nd ed) New York:

The Guilford Press.

White, HD. (1994). Scientific communication and literature retrieval. In H Cooper & L.v.

Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis (pp. 41-55). New York: Russell Sage

Foundation.

Wilens, T.E., & Biederman, 1 (1992). The stimulants. Psychiatrie Clinics ofNorth

America, 15, 191-223.

Wilens, T.E., Biederman, l, & Spencer, T. (1994). Clonidine for sleep disturbances

associated with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American

Academy ofChild and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33,424-426.

Wilens, T.E., & Spencer, T.l (2000). The stimulants revisited Child and Adolescent

Psychiatrie Clinics ofNorth America, 9, 573-603.


103

World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-IO Classification ofMental and Behavioral

Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva: Author.

Yan, IH., & Thomas, IR (2002). Arm movement control: Differences between cbildren with and

without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport,

73, 10-18.

Yun, l, & Ulrich, D.A. (2002). Estimating measurement validity: A tutorial. Adapted Physical

Activity Quarter/y, 19, 32-47.

Zagar, R, & Bowers, N.D. (1983). The effects of day on problem-solving and classroom

behavior. Psychology in the Schools, 20, 337-345.

ZentalL S.S. (1993). Research on the educational implications of attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Exceptional Children, 60, 143-153.

Aclmowledgement

The authors thank Dr. David Porretta and the reviewers for their critical feedback and valuable
suggestions. The first author extends many thanks to bis mentors at McGill University and Dr.
Claudine Sherrill for the encouragement to search and attempt to understand as much as possible
about persons with ADHD in physical activity.
104

Bridging Manuscripts and Contributions of Authors

Chapter three is a published manuscript that explored research challenges and methods in

adapted physical activity and children with ADHD. The strengths and weaknesses ofvarious

research methods in the identified adapted physical activity research studies were discussed.

While substantial methodological inconsistencies were identified in the CUITent ADHD physical

activity literature base, strategies to improve research were provided. Chapter four is the third and

next manuscript offour original papers that explore the relationship between ADHD and physical

activity. It was designed on the basis of the information provided in the two preceding review

papers. The study, a placebo controlled and double blind research design, represents one of the

more tightly controlled research investigations in physical activity and persons with ADHD. There

were six co-authors and their relative contributions were as follows. As frrst author, 1 was

responsible for 80% of the manuscript and involved in aIl aspects of the research, data collection,

statistical analyses, writing and editing of the manuscript. Dr. Greg Reid was responsible for the

other 10010 of the work involved by ensuring the reliability of the research process, with multiple

reads of the text, editing and suggestions for text improvement. The remaining 10010 of the work

was shared by the following people. Dr. Natalie Grizenko performed the diagnoses of ADHD

while also providing feedback on key design issues. Dr. Valentin Mbekou verified the

interpretation of psychological tests. Mrs. Marina Ter-Stepanian trained the first author to

conduct the required screening interviews and assisted in the coordination of data collection. Dr.

Ridha Joober contributed by providing valuable feedback on research design issues. AlI four co-

authors also provided feedback on the text as part of this collaborative process.
105

Chapter Four

Fundamental movement skills and children with ADHD:

Stimulant effects and peer comparisons

Co-authors: William J. Harvey, Greg Reid (McGill University), Natalie Grizenko , Valentin
Mbekou, Marina Ter-Stepanian, and Ridha Joober (Douglas Hospital)

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to assess the effects of stimulant medication on the fundamental

movement skill performance of22 children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

(ADHD), from 6 to 12 years of age, and to compare their movement skills to gender- and age-

matched peers without ADHD. Results from the placebo-controlled and double-blind study

indicated stimulant medication had no significant effect on the movement skill patterns of

children with ADHD. Repeated measures analyses revealed significant performance differences

between children with and without ADHD (p::::; .001). It is concluded that children with ADHD

may be at risk for developmental delays in movement skill performance. Future research avenues

are discussed.

Key words: attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, boys, methylphenidate, movement skills


106

Fundamental movement skills and children with ADHD:

Stimulant effects and peer comparisons

Many children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) do not fare weIl in

movement-related activities (Alexander, 1990; Berenyi, 1996; Harvey, Fagan, & Kassis, 2003) but

the problems associated with their movement skills are an enigma for physical activity

professionals. Movement skills are prerequisites for the performance of sport-specifie skills and

functional involvement in many physical activities (Burton & Miller, 1998). Since 2% to 14% of

North American school-aged children are affected by ADHD (Scahill & Schwab-Stone, 2000), a

considerable number of children with the disorder may be experiencing movement skill

difficulties (Gillberg, 2003).

Burton and Miller (1998) distinguished between movement skill assessments that are

product-oriented or process-oriented. Product-oriented assessments are concerned with the

movement outcome, or product, usually the quantitative result of a movement skill performance.

For example, movement skills may be measured in time (e.g., seconds on a balance task), distance

(e.g., the length of a standing long jump), or number of successful attempts (e.g., the number of

hockey pucks striking specific locations of a hockey net). Process-oriented assessments usually

involve the qualitative aspects, or process, ofhow a skill is performed (Burton & Miller, 1998).

Movement skills may be measured qualitatively through the identification of specifie behavioral

criteria related to successful performance. For example, there are three performance criteria

required for catching a ball in the Test of Gross Motor Development-2, or TGMD-2, (Ulrich,

2000). First, there is a preparation phase where the performer's hands are in front of the body, with

the elbows tlexed. Next, the arms extend from the body and reach for the ball as it arrives. Finally,
107

the ball is caught by the hands only rather than trapped against the body. Thus, movement skills are

broken down into component parts that reflect skilled performance.

The majority of adapted physical activity (APA) research about ADHD suggests the

movement skills of children with the disorder are quantitatively different from their peers without

ADHD (Beyer, 1999; Christiansen, 2000; Doyle, Wallen, & Whitmont, 1995; Harvey & Reid,

1997; Hodge, Murata., & Porretta., 1999; Kaplan, Wilson, Dewey, & Crawford, 1998; Miyahara,

Mobs, & Doll-Tepper, 1995,2001; Pelham et al., 1990; Piek, Pitcher, & Hay, 1999; Pitcher, Piek,

& Hay, 2003; Wade, 1976). This underscores the importance of distinguishing between movement

skills and the general overactive behavior that typifies ADHD. Excessive activity may not lead to

proticient movement skill performance (Keogh, 1978) or high levels of physical fitness (Harvey &

Reid, 1997). In fact, overactivity is simply one indication of the clinical disorder (AP A, 2000). Yet,

only one investigation has explored the process, or qualitative components, of movement skill

performance for children with ADHD. Harvey and Reid (1997) reported that 19 children with

ADHD demonstrated fewer performance criteria on the locomotor skills (e.g., run, gallop, hop,

leap, horizontal jump, skip, and slide) and object control skills (e.g., two-hand strike, stationary

bounce, catch, kick, and overhand throw) subtests when compared to the norms of the original

TGMD (Ulrich, 1985). However, the study was limited by a small sample size, lack ofa control

group, and an inability to control for medication. These facts are not surprising as small sample

sizes, an inability to control for important independent variables related to movement skills, and

other methodological weaknesses have been identified in AP A studies about movement skills

and children with ADHD (Harvey & Reid, in press). For example, the use of stimulant

medication for children with ADHD was not well-controlled in approximately 45% of the

research studies on ADHD and movement skill performance.


108

Stimulant medication is one of the most common treatment modalities for children with

ADHD (Wilens & Biederman, 1992; Wilens & Spencer, 2000). In fact, it is an effective short-

term treatment intervention approach that is prescribed for over 1 million school-aged children

(Greenhill, Halperin, & Abikoff, 1999; Jensen et al., 1999). The effects of stimulant medication

on the movement behavior of children with ADHD include improved fine motor skills and

reaction times (Knights & Hinton, 1969; Rapport & Kelly, 1991; Shephard, Bradshaw,

Georgiou, Bradshaw, & Lee, 2000), improved performance on lower extremity response time

performance (pedersen, Surburg, Heath, & Koceja, 2004) and higher gaze frequency during a

ping pong task, with no effects of medication on visual tracking (Vickers, Rodrigues, & Brown,

2002). DuPaul, Barkley, and O'Connor (1998) suggest that positive effects of stimulant

medication intervention should be expected on most behaviors for children with ADHD,

especially when contextuallimits are set where individual behaviors are restricted and attention

focused on specifie tasks. Furthermore, increased attention to task demands (Barkley &

Cunningham, 1978) and improved on line accuracy of body movements (Yan & Thomas, 2002)

may be potential mechanisms through which movement skiU performance may be improved in

relation to stimulant medication usage.

Only two studies used a product-oriented assessment approach to examine the effects of

methylphenidate (Ritalin) on the observable movement skill performance of children with

ADHD. The first study investigated the effects ofRitalin on the attention and baseball skills of

17 boys, ages 7.8 to 9.9 years, in a double-blind and placebo-controlled design (Pelham et al.,

1990). A product-oriented assessment was used to observe the skills of catching a fly ball,

fiel ding a ground ball, throwing to fIfSt base, and batting a pitched baseball during pre-game

sessions. Game readiness, game awareness, batting judgment, and batting skills were measured
109

during actual situations where the boys were involved in leisurely games ofbasebaU. Hence, the

performance environment was cooperative rather than being highly competitive and time-

constrained. Ritalin had a significant etfect on the measures of attention (e.g., game readiness

and game awareness) but not on the performance measures (e.g., four pre-game skills, batting

judgment, and batting skills).

Performance results may have been atfected by the operational definitions of the baseball

skills. For example, the definition of a hit, or "the percentage of times at bat the boy hit in fair

territory, divided by the total number oftimes at bat" (Pelham et al., 1990, p. 13), is not in

keeping with traditional methods of calculating batting averages. The traditional definition of a

hit is that the baseball must not only be hit into play but it must also land in fair territory with the

batter reaching base safely. The reported averages for hits were 78% (placebo), 78% (0.3 mglkg

dose ofRitalin) and 81% (0.6 mglkg dose ofRitalin). These batting averages demonstrate the

number of times that the ball was put into the field of play and we would expect to see such high

success rates in less competitive and leisurely baseball games. Yet, without the use of traditional

batting averages, it would be difficult to detect ditferences due to an intervention because the

task was most certainly aneasy one. Thus, a ceiling etfect may be quite possible. Also, raw data

and change scores were not reported so it was impossible to observe the direction of performance

change due to the stimulant medication intervention. While this study was counterbalanced, there

was no mention of statistical testing for a learning etfect where the children with ADHD may

have improved their skills over time. We must also consider that the children with ADHD may

have performed very weil during the fIfSt testing sessions and changes in skill performance

would not be expected because there would be little opportunity for skill improvement. Since

means and standard deviations were not reported, it was impossible to calculate the etfect size of
110

the intervention. Unfortunately, the qualitative components of the movement skill patterns were

not recorded or analyzed.

A second study used a placebo-controlled and double-blind design to examine the effects

ofRitalin on a movement skiU (Wade, 1976). It was found that Ritalin had a positive effect on

the static balance of 12 children with ADHD between 7.7 to 1l.8 years. The children spent

significantly more time in balance on a stabilometer during medication trials than the placebo

trials. In other words, the same 12 children with ADHD performed better when they crossed over

from the placebo condition to the medication condition. Statistical analysis revealed a significant

improvement in the timed balance task when they were receiving stimulant medication. The time

spent in balance, during the medication condition, was similar to the balance time of their 12

peers without ADHD. However, statistical analysis was not conducted between the groups of

children with and without ADHD. Also, with only one movement skill studied, a comprehensive

understanding of the relationship between medication and movement skill performance of children

with ADHD could not emerge. Finally, the real world significance of the balance task (e.g.,

stabilometer) can also be questioned as it unlikely represents the day to day fundamental

movement skills used by a majority of children.

ln summary, converging evidence suggests that children with ADHD may be at risk for

movement skill difficuIties. However, there are many methodological shortcomings associated

with these results. There has been only one process-oriented movement skill assessment of

children with ADHD and only two studies about the effects of stimulant medication on their

movement skills. Thus, an investigation into the effects of stimulant medication on the

fundamental movement skills of children with ADHD is warranted as well as a process-oriented

assessment of the movement skills ofchildren with and without ADHD.


III

The main purpose ofthis study was to (a) assess the effects of stimulant rnedication on

the qualitative cornponents of 12 fundamental movement skills for children with ADHD and (b)

compare the movement skill performance between children with and without ADHD. Two

hypotheses were made. First, it was hypothesized that the gross motor development quotient

(GMDQ), the locomotor skill subtest scores, and object control skill subtest scores on the

TGMD-2 (Ulrich, 2000) would be significantly greater for children with ADHD wh en they were

receiving methylphenidate (Ritalin) than when they were not receiving the medication. Second, it

was hypothesized that the GMDQ scores, the locomotor skiU subtest scores, and object control

skiU subtest scores would be significantly lower for children with ADHD, in the rnedication and

placebo conditions, when compared to their gender- and age-matched peers without ADHD.

Method

Participants

Twenty-two children with and without ADHD participated. The 22 children with ADHD

were part of a larger investigation about the effects of stimulant medication on children with

ADHD at an urban Canadian psychiatrie hospital. A formaI clinical diagnosis of ADHD was

made for each child by a qualified child psychiatrist. Thé protocol for identification of these

children was a consensus between two child psychiatrists from several sources of information.

For example, the children met the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for ADHD (Lahey et al., 1994).

Information was derived from the parent report of the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for

Children, computerized version, or DISe-IV, (National Institute of Mental Health, 1998), and the

Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991). Additionally, the parent and teacher questionnaires

of the Conners Global Index were administered (Conners, 1997a, 1997b). Diagnosed subtypes

for the children included inattentive (2), hyperactive-impulsive (2), and combined (18) types.
112

Comorbid diagnoses were oppositional defiant disorder (8), separation anxiety disorder (1), and

depression (1). Thus, ail children with ADHD had behavioral problems. They were also

identified as having generallearning problems as indicated by reports of clinical psychologists.

The primary author was blind to prior medication usage and this fact is a limitation to the CUITent

study.

The 22 children without ADHD were identified in an elementary school, located in close

proximity to the hospital. These children were recommended by the appropriate school personnel

(e.g., physical education teacher, c1assroom teacher) to have no prior history of major cognitive,

behaviora~ emotional, or learning problems. More specifically, a screening process was

developed to identify the control participants. The physical educator created a list of males and

females, from grades 1 to 6, who she perceived to have no major cognitive, behavioral,

emotional, or learning difficulties and be considered of average intelligence. Movement skill

level was not a criterion for inclusion in the control group. Each potential name was then

presented to the student's classroom teacher to verify if the identified child would meet the

established inclusion criteria. Consent forms were only sent home to the parents after agreement

on potential participants was reached between the classroom and physical education teachers and

a close age match was established with each child with ADHD. The physical education teacher

identified a total of 124 children. Agreement was reached with the classroom teacher on 105

children. The genders and ages of ail the children were noted and 66 consent forms were sent

home. There was a 74.2% positive retum rate (e.g. 49 of 66 replies), leaving 49 participants from

whom to select the control group.

Control participants were selected on the basis of gender and age matched to each child

with ADHD. There were 20 males and 2 females in each group, reflecting a representative
113

gender ratio in the population of children with ADHD (AP A, 2000), matched with individual

control participants by age in years and months. Participant ages ranged from 6.6 to 12.5 years.

The average age for children with ADHD was 9.7 years while peers without ADHD were 9.8

years. Matching ofages was within 6 months, with an average age difference of2.5 months at

the time oftesting. There were no significant differences between the groups for age (t (42) =-

.124, P = .902). Family income level was matched by group, with salary levels ranging between

$18,000 and $99,000.00. There were no significant differences between the groups on family

income level (t (42) = .092, P = .928).

Twenty-five potential control participants were tested on the TGMD-2. After testing was

completed, telephone interviews were conducted with one parent of each control participant

using the DISC-IV, (Shaffer et al., 2000) and the Conners Global Index Parent Questionnaire, or

CGI-P (Conners, 1997). These measures provided more evidence, other than the teacher reports

alone, that ADHD could be ruled out for each control participant. Two control participants were

excluded on the basis of the DI SC-IV testing as the parent responses indicated that two boys

might be at risk for significant attention problems. The school principal was notified so that a

professional follow-up could be conducted by the appropriate school professionals. Another boy

was excluded from the study because he refused to finish aIl the required testing. There were

significant differences between the two groups on the restless-impulsive subtest (t (42) = 12.19, P

< .001), emotional-Iability subtest (t (42) = 10.94, P < .001) and the total score (t (42) = 13.12, P

< .001) of the CGI-P. Thus, we contend that separate populations of children with and without

ADHD were assessed.

AlI participants demonstrated a total IQ score of70 or greater on the Wechsler

Intelligence Scale for Children, or WISC-III, (Wechsler, 1991) to ensure individual


114

understanding of test instructions and tasks. The IQ scores ofboys without ADHD were

calculated with a short form version of the WISC-III (Donders, 1997). There was no significant

difference between the groups on IQ (t (42) = - 1.65, P = .107). Approval was received from the

university, hospital, and school board ethics committees, with informed consent obtained from

the participants and their parents, prior to data collection.

Assessment

The fundamental movement skills of aIl participants were videotaped on two separate

occasions with a SONY Mini DV Digital Handycam (DCR-TRV18 NTSC) video recorder.

Locomotor skills and object control skills were assessed with the TGMD-2. This commonly-used

physical education test was designed to assess six locomotor skills (e.g., run, gallop, hop, leap,

horizo~taljump, and slide) and six object control skills (e.g., striking a stationary baIl, stationary

dribble, catch, kick, overhand throw, and underhand roll) where evidence ofthree to four

performance criteria may be observed on each of 12 fundamental movement skills for children

from ages 3 to 10 years (Ulrich, 2000). Tables 1 and 2 provide the specifie criteria for the 12

movement skills of the TGMD-2 (See Appendices 1 & J).

Administration of the TGMD-2 followed the testing protocol outlined by Ulrich (2000).

The test procedures were to: (a) fill in the appropriate participant information on the assessment

form, (b) provide an accurate verbal description and demonstration of each movement skill

before assessment, (c) allow one practice trial per movement skill to ensure individual

understanding, (d) provide one additional demonstration if the child appears to misunderstand

the task, and (e) administer two test trials per movement skill. A score of 0 (absent) or 1 (present)

was assigned to each performance criterion on each of the 2 trials. These scores were then

summed to obtain a raw score for each skill. Raw subtest scores were obtained by the aggregate
115

of raw scores in the six locomotor skills and the six object control skills. The raw scores of all 12

movement skills are then added to produce a total score which, in turn, is converted into the

GMDQ.

The TGMD-2 is a reliable and valid movement skill assessment instrument. Ulrich (2000)

reported acceptable coefficients ofintemal consistency, stability, and interrater reliability for the

locomotor skills subtest, the object control skills subtest, and the GMDQ ofthe TGMD-2 (See

Table 3, Appendix K). In fact, Burton and Miller (1998) acknowledged the reliability of the

TGMD as a fundamental movement skill assessment instrument. Ulrich (2000) reported different

indicators ofvalidity. Content validity was established by a panel ofthree experts who had (a)

academic training at a doctorallevel in motor development, (b) 3 years of elementary physical

education teaching experience, and (c) 3 years of experience in gross motor development

assessment. A conventional item analysis indicated that the TGMD-2 distinguished correctly

between people of different skillievels. Criterion-predictive validity was demonstrated by the

moderate to strong correlations between the TGMD-2 and the Basic Motor Generalizations

subtest of the Comprehensive Sea/es of Student Abilities (Hammill & Hresko, 1994 as cited by

Ulrich, 2000). Construct validity of the TGMD-2 was established by (a) significant correlations

between the subtests and age, (b) performance differentiation between groups of children with

high and low movement skill abilities, (c) the significant moderate correlations between the two

subtests, and (d) the loading of the two subtest scores on two separate factors in both unrotated

and rotated exploratory factor analyses and a confirmatory factor analysis. Discriminative

validity is defined as the degree that a measurement instrument distinguishes between groups on

the basis of independent criteria (Haynes, 2001). The TGMD-2 demonstrates discriminative

validity because the test items are independent movement skill criteria that can distinguish
116

between people on the basis of skillievei. For the present study, it was reasoned if significant

movement skill difTerences on the TGMD-2 were found between the groups of children with and

without ADHD, then the inferences from the results would be valid (Yun & Ulrich, 2002).

Design and procedures

A placebo-controlled and double-blind design was used to observe the efTects of

stimulant medication on the fundamental movement skills of children with ADHD. The design

was placebo-controlled because participants with ADHD received a total of 0.5 mg!kglbody

weight of methylphenidate or placebo each day. Also, the design was counterbalanced with the

participants randomly assigned to the medication condition on one week and to the placebo

condition on the next week or vice versa. The design was considered as double-blind because the

children and the first author were not aware (e.g., blind) of the specific assignment ofmedication

or placebo. The first author remained blind to this assignment during movement skill testing and

test coding.

The first author administered the TGMD-2 to children with and without ADHD between

October, 2002 and June, 2003. The locomotor skills and object control skills ofall children were

tested oh the same day (e.g., Tuesday, Wednesday, etc.) for two consecutive weeks. It was

administered to individual children between 9:40 a.m. and Il :30a.m. each testing day because

children with ADHD seem to perform better on demanding tasks during the morning as their

performance can be afTected by fatigue and the time of day (Zagar & Bowers, 1983). Since

behavioral efTects can be observed within 30 to 60 minutes after ingestion of stimulant

medication (DuPaul, Barkley, & Connor, 1998), there was between 40 to 90 minutes of elapsed

time from ingestion of the stimulant medication to the start of each skill testing session. The first

author also tested the control participants with the TGMD-2 on the same morning for two
117

consecutive weeks in order to maintain a consistent and balanced design. Ali children were

tested in an isolated gymnasium by the tirst author because individual data collection procedures

were recommended to address the unique behavioral problems and learning styles of children with

ADHD that may affect optimal movement performance (Harvey & Reid, in press). Testing time

was approximately 20 to 40 minutes per child.

Interrater reliability was obtained on the videotaped TGMD-2 trials performed by ail

children. This reliability was established through the use of an observer, trained by the primary

investigator who had much experience in movement skill assessment. For example, he

administered the original TGMD (Ulrich, 1985) approximately 500 times over the course of a 10-

year period as an adapted physical educator. The observer was a graduate student in adapted

physical activity, with five years of coaching experience in high school softball and track and

field. Since the observer earned an undergraduate degree in kinesiology and physical education,

she was familiar with the observation of important elements of movement skill patterns. The

primary investigator taught the observer to identify the criteria for each of the 12 movement

skills with a videotape that showed a 7 year-old boy performing the 12 required TGMD-2 testing

items. The videotaped performances of the children with and without ADHD were coded by the

observer only after 80% agreement had been reached with the primary investigator on a second

videotape where a 10 year-old boy performed the TGMD-2. When this training was complete,

25% oftests, spaced evenly throughout (e.g., every 4th test), were analyzed for interrater

reliability. In order to control for potential errors related to experimenter drift (Haynes, 2001),

both the primary researcher and the observer were re-evaluated on the second training video after

every 25th test. Each coder surpassed the 80% criterion level of rating agreement with the

training video during this coding pro cess, thus maintaining the reliability of each rater' s
118

observations (Cone, 1999). The physical activity researchers were blind to the assigned testing

order for children with and without ADHD during the coding of the videotaped skill

performances (e.g., no medication vs. medication, trial 1 vs. trial 2).

Data analysis

The effects of stimulant medication. The Gross Motor Development Quotient (GMDQ) and

the raw scores from the locomotor skills and object control skills subtests were the dependent

measures. AlI analyses were conducted on the statistical software from the SAS System for

Windows (SAS, 2001). One repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) and three

doubly-multivariate repeated measures analyses of variance (MANOVA) were used to test the

first hypothesis that the GMDQ scores, locomotor skill subtest scores, and object control skill

subtest scores on the TGMD-2 were significantly higher for children with ADHD when they

were receiving methylphenidate rather than not receiving the medication. First, a 2 X 2 (Age x

Treatment) ANOVA with repeated measures on the last factor was conducted on the GMDQ

scores. Age was considered as an important developmental factor. Therefore, children with

ADHD were placed into either a 6 to 9 year group (n = 7) or a 10 to 12 year group (n = 15). The

adjusted sums of squares (SS3) were used for the statistical analyses since the groups were not of

equal size. Hence, the parameter estimates were based on the least squares means in order to

account for the unique contribution of each dependent variable to the total variance and control

for alpha inflation (Stevens, 1996). The repeated condition was the children receiving or not

receiving stimulant medication.

Next, a 2 X 2 (Age x Treatment) doubly-multivariate repeated measures procedure was

used to analyze the data from the total raw scores of the locomotor skill and object control

subtests. This approach was used because the dependent variables were significantly correlated
119

within and across each measure. For example, the locomotor and object control skill scores of

each individual were correlated within each measure because the same person was performing

each skill on two occasions. Furthermore, scores were also expected to be significantly

correlated between the total sub-skill measures because significant correlations between

locomotor skills and object control skills were reported in the TGMD-2 (Ulrich, 2000).

Additionally, a 2 X 2 (Group x Time) doubly-multivariate repeated measures analysis was

conducted on the data from the total raw scores of the locomotor skill and object control subtests

to test for an order effect on skill performance due to testing sequence. Finally, separate 2 X 6

(Age x Treatment) doubly-multivariate repeated measures analyses were conducted on the raw

scores for the 6 locomotor skills and the 6 object control skills to explore any differences due to

individual movement skills.

Children with and without ADHD. A repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVA)

and doubly-multivariate repeated measures analyses of variance (MANO VA) were used to test

the second hypothesis that the GMDQ scores, locomotor skill subtest scores, and object control

skill subtest scores on the TGMD-2 were significantly lower for children with ADHD in both the

placebo and medication conditions when compared to their peers without ADHD. First, a 2 X 2

X 2 (Group x Age x Condition) ANOV A with repeated measures on the last factor was

conducted on the GMDQ scores. There were 22 children with and without ADHD (N = 44) who

were placed into groups of6 to 9 year-olds or 10 to 12 year-olds. The repeated factor was the

scores on the two TGMD-2 trials. Next, a 2 X 2 X 2 (Group x Age x Condition) doubly-

multivariate repeated measures analysis was conducted on the total raw scores for the locomotor

skills and object control skills subtests. Finally, separate 2 X 2 X 6 (Group x Age x Condition)
120

doubly-multivariate repeated measures analyses were conducted on the 6 locomotor skills and

the 6 object control skills to explore any differences due to individual movement skills.

Results

Reliability. Interrater reliability was determined by Kappa (Cohen, 1960). The Kappa

statistic ranges from 1.0 (no disagreement) to -1 (less than chance agreement), with the

agreement calculated on data by taking chance into account (Cone, 1999). The formula for

Kappa is K = Po - Pc / 1 - Pc where K is Kappa, Po is the proportion of agreement between

raters, Pc is the proportion of chance agreements (Cone, 1999). Rater agreements on the total

criterion scores were used to calculate separate proportions of agreement (po) for each of the

subtests and the gross motor development quotient (GMDQ). Rater agreements and

dis agreements were used to calculate the proportion of chance agreements (pc) for the locomotor

skills (48), object control skills (48), and the GMDQ (96). Interrater reliability was strong for

locomotor skills (K = .99), object control skills (K = .99) and the GMDQ (K = .99).

The effects of stimulant medication. Table 4 provides the means, standard deviations and

effect sizes (See Appendix K). There were no significant differences between or within effects

on the two-way repeated measures ANOV A. Only age approached significance (P(l,20) = 3.09, P

= .09) as the older children with ADHD scored higher on the GMDQ than their younger peers.

Treatment effect size (ES) was calculated for each group to interpret the effects of stimulant

medication on the components of fundamental movement skills. The method of Thomas and

Nelson (2001) was used where the means of the treatment levels were subtracted (e.g., off

medication - on medication) and divided by the pooled standard deviation (Sp). Thus, ES = Ml -

M 2/ Sp where Sp equals the square root ofS21 (nl- 1) + S22(n2-1) / nl- n2 - 2. Thomas and
121

Nelson (2001) suggested the following guidelines for ES interpretation: ~.2 (smalt), .5

(moderate) and 2: .8 (large). The effect sizes were small.

Similar results were found with the four subsequent analyses. A significant overall effect

of age (F(2, 19) = 8.22, p. = .003) was found on the second analysis, the 2 X 2 (Group x Treatment)

doubly-multivariate repeated measures MANDV A. The total raw scores for locomotor skills and

object control skills subtests for the older children with ADHD were consistently higher than

their younger peers. There were no other significant effects. The 2 X 2 (Group x Time) doubly-

multivariate analysis also revealed a significant effect of age (F(2, 19) = 8.36, P = .003), with no

significant order or interaction effects found on skill performance from the first to the second

movement skill assessment for the boys with ADHD.

The doubly-multivariate analysis for the 6 locomotor skilts revealed no significant

effects. However, the older children with ADHD scored higher on alilocomotor skills than their

younger peers with ADHD. Finally, the doubly-multivariate analysis on the 6 object control

skills revealed a significant overall effect of age as the older children with ADHD scored

significantly higher on aIl object control skills when compared with their younger peer group

(F(6, 15) = 3.06, p. = .0366). No other significant effects were revealed. Since there were no

significant differences for the within subjects factor on all four analyses, we did not accept the

hypothesis that the gross motor development quotient (GMDQ), the locomotor skill subtest

scores, and object control skill subtest scores on the TGMD-2 (Ulrich, 2000) would be

significantly higher for children with ADHD when they were receiving methylphenidate

(Ritalin) than when they were not receiving the medication ..

Children with and without ADHD. Ulrich (2000) created a descriptive scale to interpret

fundamental movement skill performance on the lGMD-2. The means for the movement skills
122

of children with ADHD can be described as "poor" to "very poor" while the same skills of

children without ADHD can be described as "average". Tables 5 and 6 provide the means,

standard deviations and effect sizes for locomotor skills and object control skills (See

Appendices L & M).

The first series of analyses revealed no significant effect of medication on the movement

skill performance of the boys with ADHD and no testing order effect on their skill performance

from the first to the second movement skiU assessment. The second set of analyses focused on

the movement skills of children with and without ADHD. The repeated factor, condition, for

these analyses was the movement skill assessment scores of children with ADHD when they

were receiving placebo or stimulant medication and the movement skill assessment scores of

children without ADHD on the two consecutive TGMD-2 trials. The 2 X 2 X 2 (Group x Age x

Condition) ANOVA with repeated measures on the last factor revealed significant effects of

group (F(l,40) = 63.61, p. = .0001) and age (F(l,40) = 4.10, p. = .05). Children with ADHD

consistently scored lower than their peers without ADHD. Generally, older children scored

higher on the GMDQ than their younger peers (See Figure 1, Appendix N). The results of the

univariate tests were similar (See Appendix 0).

Three subsequent analyses round similar results. An overall effect of group (F(2,39) =

28.09, p. = 0001) and an overall effect of age (F(2,39) = 14.4, p. = .0001) were revealed in the

doubly-multivariate repeated measures analysis for the total raw scores of the locomotor skills

and object control skills subtests. The results of the univariate tests were similar (See Appendix

P). The magnitude of the group differences on locomotor skills and object control skills were

also calculated by ES (See Table 5, Appendix L; Table 6, Appendix M). Group means (e.g.,

Mnoadhd - hladbd) and divided by the pooled standard deviation (Sp). Thus, ES = Mnoadhd - hladbJ
123

Sp. Again, Thomas and Nelson (2001) suggested the following guidelines for ES interpretation:

~ .2 (small), .5 (moderate) and ~ .8 (large). The effect sizes were large.

Next, there was an overall effect of group (F(6,35) = 12.1, p. = 0001) and age (F(6,35) =

3.53, p. = .0078) in the doubly-multivariate analysis for the 610comotor skills. The results of the

univariate tests were similar (See Appendix Q). Finally, the doubly-multivariate analysis on the 6

object control skills revealed an overall effect ofgroup (F(6,35) = 7.34, p. = .0001) and age (F(6,35)
= 5.53, p. = .0004). The results of the univariate tests were similar (See Appendix R). The

significant differences in locomotor skills and object control skills between children with and

without ADHD can be observed by the average number of performance criteria achieved by each

group in relation to the other and the maximum achievable skill performance criteria (See Figure

2, Appendix S; Figure 3, Appendix T). The average number ofperformance criteria attained for

each skill is presented in the parentheses (e.g., children with ADHD, children without ADHD,

maximum number of criteria per skill). The innermost curves for both locomotor skills and

object control skills illustrate the performance of children with ADHD which, for ail skiIls, fall

within the curves for children without ADHD. The children without ADHD do not meet full

criteria for each assessed movement skiIl, reaffrrming that movement skillievel was not a

criterion for inclusion in the control group. Clearly, the shape and depth of the movement skill

proftles for the two groups are quite different for both locomotor skills and object control skills.

Therefore, we accepted the hypothesis that significant performance differences in the qualitative

components of fundamental movement skills exist between children with and without ADHD

since these differences were found on aIl analyses.


124

Discussion

The effects of stimulant medication. The results ofthis study shed light on the movement

performance problems linked to children with ADHD. It was hypothesized that TGMD-2 scores

(e.g., GMDQ, individual and totallocomotor skills, individual and total object control skill

scores) would be significantly higher for children with ADHD when they were receiving

methylphenidate rather than not receiving it. The first hypothesis is not plausible since the results

indicated the stimulant medication had no significant effect on the fundamental movement skills

of children with ADHD.

These results suggest children with ADHD should be provided the opportunity to

participate in as many physical activities as possible in order to learn basic play skills like most

children oftheir age and gender. Thus, there should be few, if any, activity restrictions placed on

children with ADHD due to stimulant medication usage. The results also suggest that various

movement skill interventions, designed to improve the skilllearning and performance of children

with ADHD, may not be affected by stimulant medications. Thus, adapted physical activity

researchers need to explore the relationships between stimulant medication and different

intervention methods. For example, we concur with Pelham et al. (1990) who suggested the

study of sport performance with combined methods of nonpharmacological interventions and

stimulant medications. Yet, we also recommend the study of a number of factors, other than

stimulant medication, influencing the low skill proficiency levels of children with ADHD.

Children with ADHD often experience problems in relationship development and

prosocial behaviors (Litner, 1999) which may lead to few opportunities to engage in free play and

deliberate play contexts where many children spend large amounts of time developing specific

movement skills (Cote, Baker, & Abemathy, 2003; Cote & Hay, 2002). Thus, we suggest that the
125

children may lack necessary domain specifie practice time and instruction (Ericsson, 2003), with

various implications on movement skill acquisition and performance. For example, the children

may experience problems in sequencing various components of movement skills (Kowalski &

Sherrill, 1992; Wall, 1990). Furthermore, they may be unaware of the required need for shifting

levels of conscious control during various movement performance situations (Wall, McClements,

Bouffard" Findlay, & Taylor, 1985). Thus, there is most likely a mismatch between conceptual

knowledge about movement skills and performance ofmovement skills (Wall, 1990,2004), with

limited amounts of problem"'-solving capabilities (Bouffard & Wall, 1990). Similar to the typical

movement skill problems and limited self-regulated learning skills of children with developmental

coordination disorder (Reid, Harvey, Lloyd, & Bouffard, 2002), children with ADHD may be less

inclined to self-regulate their movement skilllearning and performance. Thus, we need to explore

how children with ADHD understand and learn to use their movement skills in the appropriate

play and sport situations.

However, the sensitivity of the measurement instrument and the sample size may have

been influential to our finding that stimulant medication had no effect on the movement skills of

the children with ADHD. Movement skill analysis with the TGMD-2 is conducted at a molar

level that may not be sensitive enough to pick up minute body segment changes that may occur

at an unobservable level of human action. Perhaps kinematical anal ysis of entire body segments

during movement skills may provide a finer grain of analysis and more insight to the effects of

stimulant medication on fundamental movement skills.

The sample size of 22 children with ADHD was small, with a 7-year age range. While the

sample size and age range are similar to the other process-oriented assessment of the movement

skills of children with ADHD (Harvey & Reid, 1997), we emphasize the results of the clinical
126

medication trials must be interpreted with caution due to the sample size. Statistical power was

considered in advance as part of the research design (Hall ahan & Rosenthal, 1996). In orderto

determine an appropriate number of participants, the researcher needs to estimate the parameters

of power, significance level, and effect size (Hall ahan & Rosenthal, 1996). The significance

level was set at .05 and the goal for power was .80. The estimation of an effect size was difficult

since no other research has presented effect size results about the effects of stimulant medication

on the qualitative components offundamental movement skills for children with ADHD.

However, the only available movement skill research (Wade, 1976), where effect size could be

calculated, suggested that there should be a medium effect of stimulant medication on specifie

movement skill performance. Since MANOV A procedures would be conducted for the

locomotor and object control skill total sub-test scores, a power analysis table was consulted

(Stevens, 1996) where it was estimated that a sample of 15 participants would provide adequate

power (.80) for a repeated measures analysis, with two repeated measures, a medium effect size

and alpha set at .05. Thus, we estimated that a sample size of 15 participants or more would be

necessary and our power would be adequate, given the use of repeated measures analyses where

each participant acts as her or his own control. The sample size of22 participants with ADHD

seemed reasonable, given the results of the previous research and the use ofrepeated measures

analyses.

The results of our study indicate that there was no effect of stimulant medication on the

components of the TGMD-2 movement skills since the actual effect sizes of the medication

treatment were small (e.g., .07, .16). The findings may be the result of the short time period in

which the movement skills were tested. For example, fundamental movement skills are

considered to be universal skills which most children develop (Burton & Miller, 1997), with an
127

understanding that tremendous amounts of deliberate practice time are required to develop highly

proficient movement skills (Cote et al., 2003; Cote & Hay, 2002). Thus, it may be beneficial to

assess the effects of stimulant medication on movement skilllearning and performance over an

increased time period. For example, a non-equivalent groups design (Creswell, 2003) could be

utilized where participants with and without ADHD are pre-tested on the TGMD-2 and then are

post-tested on the TGMD-2 periodically over the course oftime (e.g., 3, 6, and 9 months). AlI

children with ADHD would refrain from stimulant medication ingestion for a period of 48 hours

before the pretest, after which, they would resume taking their medication. This longitudinal

design would help to explore if the significant movement skill differences between children with

and without ADHD remain stable over time, with the medication factor taken into account.

Furthermore, there would be a high likelihood for increased power of the results due to larger

sample size or a longer assessment time frame (Kazdin, 1997; Stevens, 1996).

The power of the experiment was also small at 0.14. Since reduced power may lead to

making a type-II error, we suggest that future research should continue to examine the effects of

stimulant medication on the movement skills of children with ADHD in case a type-II error was

made. For example, if a similar study was to be conducted about the effects of stimulant

medication on other age-appropriate movement skills (e.g., baseball, hockey, football, golf, etc.),

we estimate that at least 80 participants would be required to achieve adequate power (.80), with

alpha set at .05 (Cohen, 1992; Stevens, 1996). However, it may be difficult to access such large

sampi es of children with ADHD (Harvey & Reid, 2003) and many years of data collection may

be needed to obtain large sample sizes. Other strategies to increase statistical power might be

required. Thus, it may be useful to collect data in multiple sites in order to increase sample size

(Harvey & Reid, in press).


128

The only significant effect observed in the first series of statistical analyses was for the

age factor as older children with ADHD performed better at locomotor and object control skills

when compared to their younger peers with ADHD. These results suggest that children with

ADHD may demonstrate developmental progression in movement skill performance, an

encouraging finding.

Chi/dren with and without ADHD. There were significant differences in the qualitative

components of fundamental movement skills between children with and without ADHD.

Therefore, the second hypothesis was accepted as plausible since the results indicated

significantly lower scores (e.g., GMDQ, total and individuallocomotor skills, total and

individual object control skills) for children with ADHD in both the placebo and medication

conditions when compared to children without ADHD. AlI analyses demonstrated significant

group and age effects. Unlike the stimulant medication trials, the effect sizes between children

with and without ADHD were large (e.g., ES range = 1.4 to 2.6), resulting in adequate statistical

power (e.g., ~ .80). Thus, more confidence can be placed in these results.

The significant differences between the groups lend added support to suggest children

with ADHD demonstrate qualitative movement skill difficulties in comparison with their peers

without ADHD (Harvey & Reid, 1997). Additionally, the significant effect of age illustrates that

older children performed fundamental movement skills better than the younger children in both

groups, strongly suggesting intragroup developmental progressions for both children with and

without ADHD. Thus, longitudinal observations of the movement and sport skills for children

with ADHD are recommended.

The present study improved on a number of methodological weaknesses of previous

research. First, acceptable levels of 80010 or greater agreement were found for interrater reliability
129

in the CUITent study. Next, it was difficult to generalize the findings of past research because of

small sample sizes with wide age range variability (Harvey & Reid, 1997; Miyahara et al., 1995;

Wade, 1976). Although the present study employed a small sample size, it was larger than the

other process-oriented assessment performed about the movement skills of children with ADHD

(Harvey & Reid, 1997). Also, the CUITent results should be considered as more robust than the

previous movement investigations since power was considered a priori and repeated measures

analyses were conducted. Finally, there has been minimal experimental control over independent

variables, like stimulant medication, in past physical activity research about ADHD (Harvey &

Reid, in press). The present study addressed this concem by utilizing a placebo-controlled design,

with a control group matched by (a) individuals on gender and age and (b) group on family income

level. While the reliability, use of repeated measures designs, and control of independent variables

may be viewed as strengths, there are two limitations to consider. First, all children with ADHD

also had leaming problems that may have affected the results (Lazarus, 1990). We speculate there

will be more substantial movement skill problems for children with ADHD and comorbid

disorders. Since comorbidity seems to be the norm rather than the exception for children with

ADHD (Szatmari, Offord, & Boyle, 1989), adapted physical activity researchers should further

explore the relationship of movement skill performance to children with varying levels of

comorbid psychopathology. For example, a relationship between participation and depressive

symptoms may exist as physically inactive children without ADHD, 8 to 12 years-old, were found

to be at an increased risk for depression (Thomson, Pangrazi, Friedman, & Hutchison, 2003). Since

converging evidence suggests children with ADHD perform movement skills poorly and

movement skills underlie participation in various activities, the movement skill performance of

children with ADHD may also be related to depressive symptomatology. Perhaps future research
130

could use movement skill perfonnance as a predictor of various comorbid disorders. The second

limitation to the present study was the inclusion of five children with and without ADHD who

were older than 10 years old, the eut-off age for the IGMD- 2. Yet the older children, who scored

poorly on the GMDQ, could be considered as having significant movement skill problems given

their age.

The current study demonstrates that children with ADHD may be at risk for developmental

delays in fundamental movement skill perfonnance. Thus, they may be experiencing problems

associated with the developmental skill-Iearning gap hypothesis. Wall (2004) suggests that children

with less physica1 skill may be affected by a developmental skill-Iearning gap where the children

experience minimal success and few opportunities to participate in physical activity. Limited

amounts of declarative knowledge about action and minimal movement experiences may lead the

child with less physical skill to decreased perfonnance ability as the environmental and other

situational demands increase in various movement contexts. The skill proficiency gap between

children with more physical skill and their peers with less physical skill would increase over time.

For example, the more skilled person becomes a highly proficient moyer as a result of more

experience in more complex tasks while the less skilled person continually attempts to master the

basic components of specific tasks.

Furthermore, there is little empirical data available about specific patterns of recreational

and sport behavior for children with ADHD. For example, there is no information available to

suggest that children with ADHD attempt to regulate their own movement behaviors. Again, it is

simply not known ifthey possess factual information necessary to successfully complete

fundamental movements or sport specific skills, nor to know when to use these skills in the

appropriate movement contexts. Yet, sorne children with ADHD may excel in sport skills
131

(Harvey & Reid, 2003) and it will be important to explore relationships between sport

performance and ADHD. However, children with ADHD, who possess highly proficient

movement skills and take prescribed stimulant medications, will most likely come under

increasing levels of scrutiny with age and the level of competition. For example, Hickey and

Fricker (1999) state that athletes with ADHD may discontinue medication use within a short time

frame before competitions (e.g., 24-48 hours) so their blood tests would not test positive for

banned substances at sporting events. However, elite athletes with ADHD might still be

perceived as receiving an unfair competitive advantage. It may prove useful to develop a list of

sports that may be provided with a direct physiological benefit (e.g., performance time in 100 m.

run) rather than an indirect psychological benefit (e.g., improved attention while driving Grand

Prix race cars) with the use of stimulant medication. In tum, sorne athletes with ADHD may

argue that they are put at a disadvantage when disallowed their medication. Thus, there are many

opportunities to explore the construct of self-regulation in relation to varying levels of ADHD.

In conclusion, this study demonstrated no significant effects of stimulant medication on

the movement skill patterns of 22 children with ADHD. The findings provided support for the

growing body of evidence that suggests the movement skills of children with and without ADHD

are significantly different. Yet, more well-controlled research is clearly needed to compare the

movement skills of children with and without ADHD. For example, future research needs to

provide more-evidence of the process-oriented differences in skill performance and identify

explanations for the performance difficulties. An exploration of extraneous factors on skill

performance and the quaIity of physical activity patterns is aIso needed in order to develop a better

understanding of the movement skills of children with ADHD. Piecing together the puzzle of the

movement problems associated to persons with ADHD may lead to a greater understanding of
132

their learning processes and perfonnance abilities, in turn, leading to improvements in adapted

physical activity service delivery and professional perceptions of persons with ADHD and their

unique physical activity needs.


133

References

Achenbach, T.M. (1991). The child behavior checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile. Burlington,

VT: University of Vermont.

Alexander, J.L. (1990). Hyperactive children: Which sports have the right stutr? The

Physician And Sportsmedicine, 18(4), 105-108.

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manualofmental

disorders, (4th ed). Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual ofmental

disorders, (4th ed). Text Revision. Washington, DC: Author.

Barkley, RA, & Cunningham, C.E. (1978). Do stimulant drugs improve the academic

performance of hyperkinetic children? A review of outcome research. Clinical

Pediatrics, 17, 85-92.

Berenyi, V. (1996). Coming to terms with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Health

Savry, 1(2), 16-21.

Beyer, R (1999). Motor proficiency of boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 16,403-414.

Bouffard, M., & Wall, AE. (1990). A problem-solving approach to movement skill

acquisition. In G. Reid (Ed.)., Problems in Movement Control (pp. 283-316). North-

Holland: Elsevier Science Publishers.

Burton, A W., & Miller, D.E. (1998). Movement skill assessment. Champaign, IL: Human

Kinetics.
l34

Christiansen, A. S. (2000). Persisting motor control problems in 11- to l2-year-old boys previously

diagnosed with deficits in attention, motor control and perception (DAMP). Developmental

Medicine & Child Neurology, 42, 4-7.

Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and

PsychologicalMeasurement, 20,37-46.

Cohen, l (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159.

Cone, lD. (1999). Observational assessment: Measure development and research issues. In

P.C. Kendall, lN. Butcher, and G.N. Holmbeck (Eds.), Handhook ofresearch

methods in clinical psychology (2 nd ed)(pp. 183-223). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Conners, C.K (1997a). Conners Global Index-Parent Version. North Tonawanda, NY:

Multihealth Systems Inc.

Conners, C.K (1997b). Conners Globallndex-Teacher Version. North Tonawanda, NY:

Multihealth Systems Inc.

Cote, l, & Hay, l (2002). Children's involvement in sport: A developmental perspective.

In lM Silva and D.E. Stevens (Eds.), Psychological foundations of sport. (pp. 484-

502). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Cote, l, Baker, l, & Abernathy, B. (2003). From play to practice: A developmental

framework for the acquisition of expertise in team sports. In lL. Starkes and KA.

Ericsson (Eds.), Expert performance in sports. (pp. 89-133).Champaign, IL: Human

Kinetics.

Creswell, lW. (2003). Research Design. Qualitative, Quantitative andMixedMethods

Approaches (2 nd ed). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Inc.

Donders, l (1997). A short form of the WISC-ID for clinical use. Psychological
135

Assessment, 9, 15-20.

Doyle, S., Wallen, M., & Whitmont, S. (1995). Motor skills in Australian children with

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Occupationallherapy International, 2,

229-240.

DuPaul, G.l, Barkley, RA., & Connor, D.F. (1998). Stimulants. In RA. Barkley (Ed.),

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment

(2 nd ed(pp. 510-551). New York: Guilford Press.

Ericsson, K.A. (2003). Development of elite performance and deliberate practice: An

update of the expert performance approach. In lL. Starkes and K.A. Ericsson

(Eds.), Expert performance in sports. (pp. 49-83).Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Gillberg, C. (2003). Deficits in attention, motor control, and perception: a briefreview. Archives of

Disease in Childhood, 88,904-910.

Greenhill, L.L., Halperin, J.M, & Abikoff, H. (1999). Stimulant medications. Journal of the

American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 503-512.

Hallahan, M., & Rosenthal, R (1996). Statistical power: Concepts, procedures, and applications.

Behavioral Research lherapy, 34, pp. 489-499.

Harvey, W.l, & Reid, G. (1997). Motorperformance ofchildren with attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder: A preliminary investigation. Adapted Physical Activity

Quarterly,14, 189-202.

Harvey, W.J., & Reid, G. (2003). A review offundamental movement skill performance

and physical fitness of children with ADHD. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly,

20, 1-25.

Harvey, W.l, & Reid, G. (in press). Attention-DeficitJHyperactivity Disorder: APA


136

Research Challenges. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly.

Harvey, W.J., Fagan, T., & Kassis, J. (2003). Enabling students with ADHD to use self-

control in physical activities. PALAESIRA 19(3), 32-35.

Haynes, S.N. (2001). Clinical applications of analogue behavioral observation: Dimensions

ofpsychometric evaluation. Psychological Assessment, 13, 73-85.

Hickey, G., & Fricker, P. (1999). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, CNS stimulants and

sport. Sports Medicine, 27, 11-21.

Hodge, S.R, Murata, N.M., & Porretta, D.L. (1999). Enhancing motor performance through

various preparatory activities involving children with learning disabilities. Clinical

Kinesiology, 53(4), 76-82.

Jensen, P.S., Kettle, L., Roper, M.T., Sloan, M.T., Dulcan, M.K., Hoven, C., et al. (1999). Are

stimulants overprescribed? Treatment of ADHD in four U. S. communities. Journal of the

American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 797-804.

Kaplan, B.J., Wilson, B.N., Dewey, D., & Crawford, S.G. (1998). DCD may not be a

discrete disorder. HumanMovement Science, 17,471-490.

Kazdin, A.E. (1997). A model for developing effective treatments: Progression and

interplay oftheory, research, and practice. Journal ofClinical ChildPsychology,

26, 114-129.

Keogh, J. (1978). Movement outcomes as conceptual guidelines in the perceptual-motor

maze. Journal ofSpecial Education, 12,321-329.

Knights, RM., & Hinton, G.G. (1969). The effects of methylphenidate (ritalin) on the

motor skills and behavior of children with learning problems. Journal ofNervous

andMental Disease,148, 643-653.


137

Kowalski, E.M., & Sherrill, C. (1992). Motor sequencing ofboys with learning disabilities:

Modeling and verbal rehearsal strategies. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 9,

261-272.

Labey, RR, Applegate, R, McBumett, K., Biederman, l, Greenhill, 1., Hynd, G.W., et al.

(1994). DSM-IV field trials for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children

and adolescents. American Journal ofPsychiatry, 151, 1673-1685.

Lazarus, le. (1990). Factors underlying inefficient movement in learning disabled

children. In G. Reid (Ed.), Problems ofMovement Control (pp. 241-282). North

Holland: Elsevier Science Publishers R V.

Litner, R (1999). Understanding attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and leaming

disabilities: Considerations for child and youth care workers. Journal ofChild and

Youth Care Work, 14,29-42.

Miyabara, M., Mobs, 1., & Doll-Tepper, G. (1995). Subtypes of clinically identified

children with hyperkinetic syndrome based upon perceptual motor function and

classroom behaviors. In 1. Morisbak & P.E. Jorgensen (Eds.), 1 ri' International

Symposium on Adapted Physical Activity: Quality of life through adapted physical

activity, A lifespan concept (pp. 278-286). Oslo, Norway: Ham Trykk NS.

Miyabara, M., Mobs, 1., & Doll-Tepper, G. (2001). Severity ofhyperactivity and the

comorbidity of hyperactivity with clumsiness in three sample sources: school,

support group and hospital. Child: Care, Health andDevelopment, 27,413-424.

National Institute of Mental Health (1998). NlMH DISC-IV Joy and William Ruane Center

to Identify and Treat Mood Disorders. Washington, DC: Columbia University

Pedersen, S.l, Surburg, P.R, Heath, M., & Koceja, D.M. (2004). Fractionated lower
138

extremity response time performance in boys with and without ADHD. Adapted

Physical Activity Quarterly, 21, 315-329.

Pelham, W.E., McBurnett, K., Harper, G.W., Milich, R, Murphy, D.A., Clinton, J., et al.

(1990). Methylphenidate and baseball playing in ADHD children: Who's on tirst?

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 130-133.

Piek, J.P., Pitcher, T.M., & Hay, D.A. (1999). Motor coordination and kinaesthesis in boys

with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child

Neurology, 41, 159-165.

Pitcher, T.M., Piek, J.P., & Hay, D.A. (2003). Fine and gross motor ability in males with ADHD.

Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 45, 525-535.

Rapport, M.D., & Kelly, K.L. (1991). Psychostimulant effects on learning and cognitive

function: Findings and implications for children with attention deficit hyperactivity

disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 11,61-92.

Reid, G., Harvey, W.J., Lloyd, M., & Bouffard, M. (2002). A pilot study of self-directed

motor learning in children with DCD. Paper presentation about the microgenetic

analysis of the overhand throwing of boys with and without developmental coordination

disorder. The DCD-V International Conference: Mechanisms, Measurement, and

Management. Banff, Alberta, Canada .

SAS (2001). SAS System for Windows, release version 8.02., Cary, NC: SAS Institute

Inc.

Scahill, L., & Schwab-Stone, M. (2000). Epidemiology of ADHD in school-age children.

Child and Adolescent Psychiatrie Clinics ofNorth America, 9, 541-555.

Sheppard, D.M., Bradshaw, J.L., Georgiou, N., Bradshaw, J.A., & Lee, P. (2000).
139

Movement sequencing in children with tourette's syndrome and attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Movement Disorders, 15, 1184-1193.

Stevens, J. (1996). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences. (3 rd ed).

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

Szatmari, P., Offord, D.R, & Boyle, M.H. (1989). Correlates, associated impairments and

patterns of service utilization of children with attention deficit disorder: Findings from the

Ontario child health study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 205-217.

Thomas, J.R, & Nelson, J.K. (2001). Research Methods In Physical Activity (4th ed.).

Champaign, Il: Human Kinetics.

Thomson, L.M., Pangrazi, RP., Friedman, G., & Hutchison, N. (2003). Childhood

depressive symptoms, physical activity and health related fitness. Journal ofSport

Psychology, 25,419-439.

Ulrich, D.A (2000). Test of Gross Motor Development (r ed). Austin, TX: PRO-ED, Inc.
Ulrich, D.A (1985). Test of Gross Motor Development. Austin, TX: PRO-ED, Inc.

Vickers, J.N., Rodrigues, S.T., & Brown, L.N. (2002). Gaze pursuit and arm control of

adolescent males diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

and normal controls: evidence of a dissociation in processing visual information of

a short and long duration. Journal ofSport Sciences, 20,201-216.

Wade, M.G. (1976). Effects ofmethylphenidate on motor skill acquisition of hyperactive

children. Journal ofLearning Disabilities, 9,443-447.

Wall, AE. (1990). Skill acquisition research with persons with developmental disabilities:

Research design considerations. In G. Reid (Ed.)., Problems in Movement Control (pp.

283-316). North-Holland: Elsevier Science Publishers.


140

Wal~ A.E. (2004). The developmental skill-leaming gap hypothesis: Implications for

children with movement difficuIties. Adapted Physical Activity Quarter/y, 21, 197-

218.

Wall, A.E., McClements, J., Bouffard, M., Findlay, H., & Taylor, M.J. (1985). A

knowledge-based approach to motor development: Implications for the physically

awkward. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 2,21-42.


rd
Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scalefor Children (3 ed): Manual. San

Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Wilens, T.E., & Biederman, l (1992). The stimulants. Psychiatrie Clinics ofNorth

America, 15, 191-223.

Wilens, T.E., & Spencer, T.J. (2000). The stimulants revisited Child and Adolescent

Psychiatrie Clinics ofNorth America, 9, 573-603.

Yan, lH., & Thomas, lR (2002). Arm movement control: Differences between children with and

without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport,

73, 10-18.

Yun, l, & Ulrich, D.A. (2002). Estimating measurement validity: A tutorial. Adapted Physical

Activity Quarterly, 19, 32-47.

Zagar, R, & Bowers, N.D. (1983). The effects of day on problem-solving and classroom .

behavior. Psych%gy in the &hools, 20,337-345.


141

Bridging Manuscripts and Contributions of Authors

Chapter four was a placebo controlled and double blind study that indicated stimulant

medication had no significant effect on the movement skill patterns of children with ADHD.

Repeated measures analyses revealed significant performance differences between children with

and without ADHD (p ~ .001). The next research study, chapter five, is the final manuscript of

four original papers that explore the relationship between ADHD and physical activity. It was also

designed on the basis of the information provided in the two preceding review papers. The study is

the first time that a mixed method research design was used to explore the physical activity

experiences ofboys with ADHD. There were seven co-authors and their relative contributions

were as follows. As first author, 1 was responsible for 80010 of the manuscript and involved in all

aspects of the research, data collection, interviewing, coding, analyses, writing and editing of the

manuscript. Dr. Greg Reid was responsible for 5% of the work involved by ensuring the reliability

of the research process, with multiple reads of the text, editing and suggestions for text

improvement. Dr. Gordon Bloom and Ms. Kerri Staples contributed 5% ofworkload by

providing theoretical understanding of qualitative paradigms and practical feedback on important

issues of coding, interpretation and trustworthiness. The remaining 100/0 of the work was shared

by Natalie Grizenko, Dr. Valentin Mbekou, Mrs. Marina Ter-Stepanian and Dr. Ridha Joober.

The contributions of aIl four co-authors were the same as stated in chapter fOUf. Again, they also

provided feedback on the text as part of this collaborative process.


142

Chapter Five

Physical activity experiences ofboys with and without ADEID

Co-authors: William J. Harvey, Greg Reid, Gordon Bloom, Kerri Staples (McGiU University),
Natalie Grizenko, Valentin Mbekou, Marina Ter-Stepanian, and Ridha Joober (Douglas
Hospital)

Abstract

Similarities and differences in physical activity experiences ofboys with and without attention-

deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) were explored by converging information from movement

skill assessments on the Test of Gross Motor Development (Ulrich, 2000), semi-structured

interviews, and discussions with parents. Six boys with ADHD conveyed many different

experiences than age-matched peers without ADHD. The results indicated boys with ADHD

were not as proficient movers as their peers without ADHD. As weIl, qualitative analysis

resulted in the creation ofthree categories: (a) deliberate play, (b) knowing about doing, and (c)

personal feelings. Taken together, the findings revealed that boys with ADHD had inadequate

attention to detail, superficial knowledge about movement skiIls, and negative feelings about

physical activity. The findings enabled a deeper understanding of skill performance and ADHD.
143

Physical activity experiences ofboys with and without ADHD

Excessive movement and persons with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

have a long-standing historicaI connection. The German author, Heinrich Hoffman, wrote one of

the earliest known references to ADHD in 1865 with his poem "Fidgety Phil" about a boy with

behavior and attention problems who could not stay still (Miyahara, Mobs, & Doll-Tepper, 1995).

Barkley (1998) stated that excessive activity was important to understand hyperactivity, the term

used for ADHD during the time period of 1960 to 1970, because a state of constant motion was a

key element of the accepted definition ofhyperactivity at that time (Chess, 1960). The definition

"emphasized activity as the defining feature of the disorder, as other scientists of the time would

aIso do" (Barkley, 1998, p. 9). However, greater emphasis has been placed on deficits in attention

and a lack of self-control to define ADHD overthe past few decades (Barkley, 1997, 1998;

Douglas, 1999).

Yet, excessive movement continues to be a haIlmark of the ADHD definition from the

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual ofMental Disorders, or DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000). Six

hyperactive symptoms are currently used in the DSM diagnostic framework; often fidgets with

hands or feet or squirms in seat, often leaves seat in c1assroom or in other situations in which

remaining seated is expected, often runs about or c1imbs excessively in situations in which it is

inappropriate, often has difIiculty playing or engaging in leisure activities quietly, is often "on the

go" or often acts as if"driven by a motor", and often taIks excessively. While these symptoms are

related to behavior in generaI, they are not linked to specific movement skills in physical activity

contexts. Thus, for the purposes ofthis paper, we distinguish between movement-related behaviors

and movement skills. Movement-related behaviors have a personaI-social nature where there is

limited goaI-directed movement associated with performance (Keogh, 1978) while movement
144

skills are observable, goal-directed movements that can be described in quantitative or qualitative

terms (Burton & Miller, 1998). For example, there is a difference between standing up and moving

around the room while waiting for a tum in agame of monopoly (e.g., movement-related behavior)

and using an overhand throw to propel a baseball towards homeplate to strike out a batter (e.g.,

movement skill). Appropriate movement-related behaviors, as typified in the DSM-IV- IR, serve a

much needed social purpose that many children with ADHD may lack. However, these behaviors

are not the same as specifie movement skills. Thus, the traditional focus on excessive activity as a

core component of ADHD may have hampered the investigation of the movement skills of

children with ADHD (Harvey & Reid, 1997).

Converging evidence suggests a considerable number of children with ADHD demonstrate

difficulties when performing locomotor and object control skills (Harvey & Reid, 1997~ Harvey et

al., in review~ Hodge, Murata, & Porretta, 1999). They also may lack the ability to regulate their

skill performances in different movement contexts, especially in competitive situations (Harvey,

Fagan, & Kassis, 2003). Barkley (1997) developed the inhibitory model of executive functions to

describe relationships between the cognitive system and human behaviors vital for self-

regulation. For example, individuals usually take necessary and adequate reflection time when

planning and making decisions in relation to their behavior. However, Barkley suggested

children with ADHD have difficulties in self-regulation and, more specifically, they may

experience an inability to proceduralize (e.g., perform) their declarative knowledge (e.g.,

personal facts and acquired knowledge about performance). In other words, "ADHD is more a

problem of doing what one knows rather than ofknowing what to do" (Barkley, 1997, p. 335).

Starkes, Helsen and Jack (2001) suggested the combination ofpersonal knowledge and

movement skill development to better understand the performance of movement skills which, in
145

tum, underlie sport performance. Thus, personal knowledge is an important factor that facilitates

skill performance and expertise, with a strong emphasis placed on the individual's tlexibility of

linking performance and knowledge (Wall, Reid, & Harvey, in press). Yet, there is little

information available to suggest that children with ADHD possess the factual information

necessary to successfully complete movement skills, or when to proceduralize their declarative

knowledge about action in appropriate movement contexts (Harvey & Reid, 2005).

The knowledge-based approach (Wall, McClements, Bouffard, Findlay, & Taylor, 1985)

was used as the gui ding theory, or theoreticallens, for this study since it is a significant

conceptual framework in adapted physical activity (APA) research (Reid, 1992). It is a unique

theoretical blend of developmental, educational and sport psychology that emphasizes the

importance of developmental factors, theories of expertise, and self-regulation for the individual

acquisition ofmovement skills (Wall et al., in press). For example, expertise research suggests

the importance of an individual' s ability to identify salient cues from the physical environment

and their own movements in order to perform in a more proficient manner (Wall et al., in press).

In fact, it is this attention to detail that distinguishes novice and expert performers (Ericsson,

2003). The theory was linked with the inhibitory model of executive functions (Barkley, 1997)

by exploring a movement-based relationship between declarative knowledge and procedural

knowledge of children with and without ADHD. For example, taking the necessary time to

retlect on movement skills, which may be a difficulty for sorne children with ADHD, is a factor

related to an awareness of the important metacognitive skills of error deduction, planning, and

monitoring of actions (Bouffard & Wall, 1990). Since both theories are linked to subjective

feelings about action, or affective knowledge (Wall et al., 1985), feelings about physical activity

experiences were also explored in the current study. Furthermore, these theories share many
146

important commonalities such as the value of a developmental perspective, the role of language

in child development and cognitive functioning, and a sharing of much technicallanguage. Thus,

a deep understanding of the relationship between ADHD and physicai activity was sought in the

present study.

This exploratory study was aiso deemed necessary since there have been few attempts to

understand various influences on the movement skill performance of children with ADHD (Harvey

& Reid, 2003). For example, Harvey and colleagues (in review) found methylphenidate (Rital in)

had no significant effect on the movement skill patterns of 22 children with ADHD, between the

ages of 6.6 to 12.5 years, who were assessed on the Test of Gross Motor Development-2, or

TGMD-2 (Ulrich, 2000). However, several recent motor control studies suggested that stimulant

medication improved (a) fine motor skills and reaction times (Shephard, Bradshaw, Georgiou,

Bradshaw, & Lee, 2000) , (b) performance on lower leg response times (Pedersen, Surburg, Heath,

& Koceja, 2004), and (c) gaze frequency on a ping pong task (Vickers, Rodrigues, & Brown,

2002).

Parents and teachers of children with ADHD have aIso suggested that the children have

poor movement and sport skills (Harvey et al., 2003; Rasmussen & Gillberg, 1983; Szatmari,

Offord, & Boyle, 1989). Yet, children with ADHD have not spoken about their perceptions of

movement skill proficiency and involvement in play, physicai activity, and sports. Thus, it seems

that children with ADHD have not been given the opportunity to have their physical activity voices

heard. Consistent with a postmodern tradition, it was reasoned that a qualitative study would

enable a better understanding of the daily physical activity experiences of children with disabilities

and the contextual meanings associated with physical activity (Goodwin, Krohn, & Kuhnle,

2004). A qualitative research approach was considered beneficial since qualitative studies aIso
147

shed light on process-related questions or why something happens (Merriam, 1990). Therefore,

the purpose oftbis study was to explore the physical activity experiences of children with and

without ADHD. The central question posed in tbis study follows. What are the similarities and

differences in the physical activity experiences of children with and without ADHD?

Method

Participants

Twelve boys with and without ADHD, ages 9 to 12 years, participated. The six boys with

ADHD were part of a larger clinical investigation about the effects of stimulant medication on

various behaviors of children with ADHD at an urban Canadian psychiatrie hospital. Each child

with ADHD had a formaI DSM-IV diagnosis of ADHD made by a qualified child psychiatrist

(Labeyet al., 1994). Identification of each child with ADHD was also based on consensus

between two child psychiatrists on a variety of diagnostic information that included the parent

report of the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children, computerized version~ or DISC-IV,

(National Institute of Mental Health, or NIHM, 1998)~ the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach,

1991)~ the Conners Global Index Parent Questionnaire, or CGI-P (Conners, 1997a)~ and the

Conners Global Index Teacher Questionnaire, or CGI-T(Conners, 1997b). ). AlI boys with

ADHD were diagnosed with the combined subtype of ADHD. Thus, all the boys with ADHD

had behavioral problems. They were also identified as having generallearning problems as

indicated by reports of clinical psychologists. Thus, the sample is best described as purposive

and unique since children with ADHD were sought (Merriam, 1998). Ali children with ADHD

were medicated with methylphenidate during the study.

Each boy with ADHD was matched by age (+ 6 months) to a boy without ADHD from a

local elementary school. AlI boys were between the ages of9.7 and 12.5 years to improve the
148

reliability of verbal reports given their age (Schneider & Pressley, 1997). The average age for the

boys with ADHD was 10.9 years while the average age oftheir peers without ADHD was 11.1

years. There was an average age difference of 3.2 months ai the time of interviewing.

The six boys without ADHD were identified by the physical educator and classroom

teacher as having no major cognitive, behavioral, emotional, or learning difficulties and were

considered of average intelligence. One parent of each control participant was interviewed on the

DISe-IV (NIMH, 1998) and the CGI-P (Conners, 1997a) to provide more information and rule

outADHD.

To ensure individual understanding of the interview questions, the 12 children

demonstrated a total IQ score of 70 or greater on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, or

WISC-III, (Wechsler, 1991). The IQ scores ofboys without ADHD were calculated with a short

form version of the WISC-ID (Donders, 1997). The IQ scores for boys with ADIID ranged from

88 to 117 (M = 100.2) and the IQ scores for boys without ADHD ranged from 88 to 127 (M =

110.7). University, hospita~ and school board ethics approval was received, with informed

consent obtained from the participants and their parents, prior to data collection.

Movement Skill Assessment

AlI participants were tested on the TGMD-2 (Ulrich, 2000) which includes six locomotor

skills (e.g., run, gal10p, hop, leap, horizontaljump, and slide) and six object control skills (e.g.,

striking a stationary baIl, stationary dribble, catch, kick, overhand throw, and underhand roll).

The test is designed for children 3 to 10 years (Ulrich, 2000). There are three to four

performance criteria on each of the 12 movement skills. Two test trials were administered per

movement skill (Ulrich, 2000). A mark of 0 (absent) or 1 (present) was scored for each

performance criterion observed on each of the 2 trials. These marks were then added to obtain a
149

raw score for each individual skill. The raw scores were summed to obtain separate subtest

scores for locomotor skills and object control skills. The raw scores of all 12 movement skills

were then added to produce a total score which, in turn, is converted into the Gross Motor

Development Quotient or GMDQ (Ulrich, 2000). AlI TGMD-2 assessments were videotaped

with a SONY Mini DV Digital Handycam (DCR-TRVI8 NTSC) video recorder.

Interview

The initial plan was to use a completely open-ended interview approach and inductive

data analysis (Denzin & Lincoln, 2000; Sparkes, 2003). However, pilot interviews indicated that

children with and without ADHD, between the ages of9 and 12 years, could not articulate specific

examples when asked open-ended questions about their physical activity experiences. Thus, an

interview guide was created to assist children to talk about their physical activity experiences (See

Appendix U). The 17 questions in the guide were developed from both the Physical Activity

Monitor Questionnaire, or P AMQ (Craig, Cameron, Russell, & Beaulieu, 2000) and theories of

expertise (Wall et al., 1985). The PAMQ represented the most current information about the

physical activity experiences of Canadian children at the time of data collection. Theories of

expertise, underlying the knowledge-based approach (Wall et al., 1985), provided insights to the

physical activity experiences of the children. The complete interview guide was reviewed by three

experts in physical education (e.g., 3 university professors with doctoral degrees in physical

education [PE], with a minimum of 10 years PE teaching experience at the university level and

qualitative research experience). After full agreement was reached with the expert panel on the

final format of the interview guide, interviews were conducted with 2 of the original 4 pilot

participants to verify if the children would respond to the new interview format. They spoke in

detail about their individual physical activity experiences. The fIfst author verified the accuracy
150

of individual responses with one parent of each child in a telephone interview format and, since

the approach was deemed successful, the interview guide was then ready for use with the study

participants.

A sequential mixed methodology was used in the development and application of the

interview guide (CresweIl, 2003), with a mixture ofhighly structured questions (e.g., Likert

scale) and less structured questions. Schneider and Pressley (1997) suggested a successful two-

part strategy for interviewing children. The approach began with structured questions that were

designed to acquire descriptive information about a participant' s prior knowledge and was

followed by less-structured questions that were designed to acquire deeper levels ofknowledge.

Twelve highly-structured questions focused on individual participation in, and

preferences for, specifie individual and group activities as weIl as various aspects of play (e.g.,

the time spent in play, practice habits, observationallearning, etc.). Probes, or follow-up

questions, were created to provide clarity and more information about individual responses

(Merriam, 1998). Thus, the boys were probed for examples of games (question 2), targets

(question 10), domain-specifie vocabulary (question Il), modeling (question 12) and about the

rationale for their choices (e.g., why do you like tbis game, targe!, model, etc.).

A less-structured approach was used for the five other questions (See questions 7, 8, 15,

16, & 17). For example, open-ended questions inquired about methods of practicing games (See

questions 7 & 8) and personal feelings about PE (Questions 15 to 17). Questions 15 and 16

inquired about individu al positive and negative feelings about PB. The boys spoke about their

individual feelings towards activities and peers in PE c1ass. Furthermore, follow-up probes were

asked to c1arify individual responses to questions 15 and 16.


151

Data Analysis

There were two levels of data analysis (Merriam, 1998). First, each interview was treated

as part of a comprehensive within-case analysis where the researcher learned as much as possible

about the individuals at each site (e.g., hospital & school) to build a separate profile of

abstractions for each group ofboys. Second, a cross-case analysis was performed where the

researcher compared their experiences.

Verbatim transcriptions were performed on the open-ended questions and probes. A line-

by-line analysis was conducted where similar words and phrases were identified as data units

(Merri am, 1998). Similar data units were then labeled with tags to reflect their underlying

meaning (Côté, Salmela, & Russell, 1995~ Ryan & Russell Bernard, 2000). There were 17 tags

identified for the 2,188 data units. Similar tags were grouped together to form properties which,

in turn, formed dimensions ofhigher abstract categories (Côté et al., 1995). Thus, there is a

resultant increase in the magnitude of abstract conceptualization from data units to categories.

These categories were reflective of the expertise lens (Wall et al., 1985), guiding interpretations

in this study. The categories, properties, and sample data units are provided in Table 1 (See

Appendix V).

Trustworthiness

Various methodological procedures may be incorporated in qualitative research designs

to gain the reader's trust of the research findings (Brantlinger, Jimenez, Klingner, Pugach, &

Richardson, 2005). For example, the strategie use of triangulation, member checks, and explicit

statements of the researcher' s biases helped demonstrate the trustworthiness of the research

(Brantlinger et al., 2005~ Merriam, 1998~ Sparkes, 1998,2003).


152

The tirst component of trustworthiness was the triangulation of data, or the use of various

data sources, to help establish the internaI validity ofthis study and provide a holistic

understanding of the phenomenon ofinterest (Brantlinger et al., 2005; Merriam, 1998). More

specifically, three sources of information described the physical activity experiences of the boys

with and without ADHD. For example, the fundamental movement skillievei of each child was

identified by their performance on the TGMD-2 (Ulrich, 2000). Next, the 17-question interview

was coIiducted. The final source of information for data triangulation was parental input. Each

parent was asked to complete various questions based on the 2000 P AMQ (Craig et al., 2000) to

gain a more complete description of the physical activity experiences of children with and

without ADHD.

The second component oftrustworthiness was child and parent member checks, where

they confirmed the accuracy of the summary statements and interview, important for establishing

internaI validity (Brantlinger et al., 2005; Merriam, 1998). For example, the content of each

interview was summarized into a single page summary statement and read back to each child

within three to four days after the conclusion of the interview. Each individual was provided the

opportunity to verify the accuracy of the summary statement and to add·or deleteany

information they felt necessary. Furthermore, each child' s summary statement was made

available to a parent so she or he could verify the accuracy oftheir child's verbal reports.

The third and final component of trustworthiness for tbis study was a description of the

primary author' s life experiences and bis underlying assumptions about movement skill

performance and ADHD as they guided and molded the methods and interpretations ofthis

exploratory study. Biographical information was shared because his lived experience lends

credibility to the interpretations of the cbildren's interviews but the accuracy and trustwortbiness of
153

interpretation May be justifiably questioned (Sparkes, 1998, 2003). The openness, honesty and

credibility of the research May also be questioned due to the mixed methodological approach.

As the primary author, 1 am a Caucasian male, who has been a life-long resident of the

same working neighborhoods as the participants. My childhood background was similar to Many

of the participants. For example, 1 lived and went to elementary school and high school within an 8

city block radius ofboth data collection sites. In fact, some parents of the participants were in my

elementary and high school cohorts. Our family income level and social status would match Many

of the participants' families. There is little doubt that 1 am familiar with Many local mannerisms,

colloquial expressions and physical activity practices. 1 also performed three years ofPE teacher

training in this same geographical area. Thus, 1 possess extensive first-hand knowledge of the

specific culturally-normative skills and habits relative to this well-defioed area. As an adult, 1

continue to live in this same community. Moreover, 1 taught PE to children with ADHD for

approximately 12 years. This variety of experiences about children with ADHD leads to the

contention of a trustworthy research instrument.

Results

The results of the movement skill assessment and interview data were compiled

concurrently. The movement skill assessment was conducted to indicate individual and group

movement skill proficiency levels. First, information from the 12 interviews was gathered

individually and Likert scale responses were then averaged. Next, transcriptions were coded into

data units and developed into categories of physical activity experience. This sequential pro cess

led to a deeper understanding from a narrow focus on specific physical activity information (e.g.,

structured questions) to a general exploratory approach about personal physical activity


154

experiences (e.g., less-structured questions). The results are reported in a similar sequential

manner to remain consistent with the analytic process.

Movement Ski/l Assessment

The fundamental movement skills of the boys with and without ADHD were assessed on

the TGMD-2. Interrater reliability was established on the videotaped TGMD-2 skill

performances, with Kappa (Cohen, 1960) strong for locomotor skills, (K = .99), object control

skills (K = .99) and the Gross Motor Development Quotient (K = .99) as reported in a prior study

(Harvey et al., in review). The boys with ADHD were not as proficient movers as their peers

without ADHD (see Table 2, Appendix W). Note that an names used in the text and Table 2 are

fictional to ensure participant confidentiality. Common names for males were used to keep a

human touch to the boys' stories. According to the descriptors provided in the TGMD-2 (Ulrich,

2000), the overall fundamental movement skill performance of the boys with ADHD ranged

between poorto average (e.g., Gross Motor Development Quotient, or GMDQ), with below

average to poor locomotor skills and average to very poor object control skills. The overall

fundamental movement skills of the boys without ADHD ranged from average to above average,

with average to above average locomotor and object control skills.

Interview

Each Likert-scale question was averaged to compare responses between the boys with

and without ADHD. There were 2,188 data units for all of the transcribed interview data, with

1,044 data units forthe boys with ADHD and 1,154 data units for the boys without ADHD. The

complete transcriptions of four participants, two boys from each group, were randomly selected

. for peer review by a graduate student in adapted physical activity. Thus, 570 data units (26.1 % of
155

total data units) were recoded for interrater reliab ility, with 81. 6% complete agreement reached

(e.g., 465 of570 data units).

Each category is described below with specifie references made to the appropriate

properties. The first category was named deliberate play to convey the importance of play in the

development ofmovement skill proficiency (Côté, Baker, & Abemathy, 2003; Côté & Hay, 2002)

and reflect the deliberately planned activities of the six boys without ADHD. Both groups spoke

about different types of play experiences. For example, the boys indicated who they played with

and where they played (e.g., solitary play, group play, no play and play settings). The second

category was labeled knowing about doing as the boys spoke about their skill performance and

learning (e.g., leisure skills, movement skills, modeling, game situations, personal theories, and

self-awareness). The third category was called personal feelings where the boys spoke about

their feelings related to physical activity skills and activities (e.g., positive, negative affective

responses and prosocial, asocial behaviors). AlI data units were summed to support the

observations and comparisons made in each categorical analysis (See Tables 3,4, & 5;

Appendices W & X). Responses on the 12 Likert scale questions were also noted for each

individual and averaged for each group.

Of interest, a fourth category was named "other" for data units and properties that did not

fit into the three categories named above. First, data units reflected the transition between

thoughts property if participants required additional interview time to gather their thoughts.

There were a similar number of data units for the groups (ADHD = 72, No-ADHD = 81). Next,

the clarifications property included data units where a participant asked a question during the

interview or responded with either a yes or no to the interviewer' s attempt to clarify a point. The

boys without ADHD sought more clarifications than their peers with the disorder (ADHD = 306,
156

No-ADHD = 451). Finally, sorne data units indicated a person was not responding to the posed

question. This "off-task" property illustrated, to no surprise, the boys with ADHD were off-task

more often during the interviews (ADHD = 33, No-ADHD = 5). This category did not contribute
overall to the understanding of the boys' experiences but it was important to observe because

both groups of boys had approximately the same amount of total data units (ADHD = 1,044, No-

ADHD = 1,154). This might not have been expeeted, given excessive talking and off-task

behaviors are typical of children with ADHD (AP A, 2000). While the boys with ADHD were

more off-task during the interviews, they did not talk more than the boys without ADHD. In faet,

they spoke slightly less, indicating that excessive talking did not affect the results.

Deliberate Play. Questions one to nine explored the children' s perceptions of individual

aetivity preferences, participation in various activities, organizational aspects of play, and

participation time. There are three major similarities between boys with and without ADHD.

First, most of the boys did not consider their formaI and informaI physical aetivity time as

practice. Instead they referred to this time as play; a fin ding consistent with the retrospective

views of experts who recalled their childhood physical aetivity experiences (Côté et al., 2003).

Next, aIl boys were asked to indicate which individual activities and group activities they played,

at least once, within the last 12 months, and how much they liked playing this activity. Most of

the boys had participated in all of the activities in the past 12 months. However, the boys with

ADHD reported a greater preference for and participation in individual aetivities (e.g., swings

and slides, bicycling, swimming and roller-blading) than their peers without ADHD who, in turn,

preferred and participated in more group aetivities (e.g., hockey, football, and basebaIl). FinaIly,

both groups ofboys reported that they took part in PE class in school most of the time (ADHD

M = 5.0, No-ADHD M = 4.8).


157

The Likert-scale questions revealed more differences between the groups. The boys with

ADHD spent approximately 30 minutes less time in daily physical activity than boys without

ADHD as indicated by both the child and parent questionnaires. The children with ADHD also

reported the organization offewer activities (ADHD avg. = 3.8; No-ADHD avg. = 4.3),

engagement in less spontaneous play (ADHD avg. = 3.5; No-ADHD avg. = 4.3), and

participation in fewer organized sports compared to the boys without ADHD (ADHD = 2/6; No-

ADHD=4/6).

In relation to the less-structured questions and probes, the boys spoke about times and

places where they played or practiced their movement skills alone, with friends, not at all or in

specifie physical activity settings. Table 3 illustrates the number of data units for the four

different properties of deliberate play (See Appendix W). The boys with ADHD made more

references to playing with peers. Yet they also indicated that they often play alone whereas the

children without ADHD made no such references. The boys without ADHD differed from their

peers with the disorder because they referred more to a variety of physical activity settings that,

in tum, linked their practice and play experiences with substantial involvement oftheir family or

friends. For example, Mike without ADHD mentioned, "WeIl, 1 don't usually practice by myself

1 take my sister or my mom. Because, for most of the sports you need more than one person to

practice" .

Knowing about Doing. The knowing about doing category is related to questions 10 to

12, where the children were asked about various dec1arative knowledge factors that may have

affected skiIl performance. Table 4 illustrates the number of data units for the knowing about

doing category and the interviews indicated many differences in dec1arative knowledge about

physical activity (See Appendix X).


158

The boys with ADHD spoke about their lei sure skills, the first property of knowing about

doing, when they mentioned playing cards and creating make-believe games with their siblings

while the boys without ADHD did not refer to leisure participation. The second property of

knowing about doing was movement skills where data units reflected general descriptions of

movement skill performance, including examples and explanations about special practice targets

during play and specific vocabulary for particular actions (e.g., domain-specific terms). The boys

with ADHD reported that they often use targets to practice (M = 3.0) whereas boys without

ADHD responded that they do not use targets very often (M = 2.2). However, as specified during

the probes, the practice targets of the boys with ADHD were often associated with inappropriate

social behaviors like throwing objects at others in an aggressive manner. For example, Larry

with ADHD mentioned that his special target in football is to get someone to stand up so he

could throw a football to hit the person. On the other hand, the boys without ADHD used more

socially desirable and appropriate practice targets. For example, Jim without ADHD spoke about

his football experience. "WeIl. 1 take a bucket and 1 hang it to the fence and shoot a football

through it". When asked for clarification, he responded, "1 said 1 take a bucket, 1 shoot football in

it. Sometimes 1 run to shoot the football in it, cause just in case there' s somebody rushing you".

When the issue was probed further, he stated, "Like say if someone' s running after you and you

are like trying to shoot the baIl. You are going to have to run left if he is coming at you by that

way (points to his right), so you're gonna have to shoot it diagonaIly". Jim mentioned, after his

summary statement, that he tries to practice just like he plays in agame. Thus, while the

participants with ADHD reported the greater use of practice targets in play than their peers

without ADHD, our discussions indicated that the targets were often socially inappropriate.
159

The boys without ADHD used more domain-specifie vocabulary than their peers with

ADHD (No-ADHD M = 2.8, ADHD M = 2.0). The differences between the groups were
associated with the depth and use of the action tenns articulated. The boys with ADHD reported

knowing specifie action tenns but they either did not use them or forgot the words entirely. For

example, Bob with ADHD stated,

"WeIl like the meaning of slap shot. 1 know what it means, but yeah sometimes l'Il use
words like that, but that' s the only thing. Like in football people say dive and 1 know
what it means but 1 don't use it. When 1 try to tell somebody something 1 don't reaIly use
special words".

Furthermore, Randy mentioned, "WeIl sometimes 1 name sorne of my things like 1 give it a name

and afterwards 1 forget the name that 1 give it. So 1 have to give it a new name over and over and

over and over and over". Y et, the boys without ADHD knew and used many specific action

tenns in the appropriate contexts. For example, Chad without ADHD used the tenn Cross 34 and

after being asked what the word meant to him, he elaborated:

"Yeah. There's two running backs in the back (field) and the first goes tbis way (motions
across the table with his left hand) so the defence tbinks l'm going to go tbis way
(motions right hand in same direction as the left hand) but l'm going tbis way (motions
right hand across table and left arm in opposite direction)".

Jim without ADHD aIso used football-specific tenninology. He stated, "Only when l'm playing

with my mend Tom cause he tells me plays to do. And 1 just go down like, say in, it's like a

post pattern. It tells you to go up and turn right or on the left side, turn left".

The third property of knowing about doing was modeling. The boys without ADHD

reported watching someone play a game very often (M = 4.0) to learn how to play and improve

compared to boys with ADHD (M = 3.5). Data units reflected either a general observation of a

group of people playing agame to improve movement skills or a specific focus on the

performance of an individual person, or a particular group of people, to improve movement skill


160

performance. There were a similar number of data units from both groups about the general

observation of models to learn movement skills. However, the boys without ADHD made

approximately 50% more references to the specific observation of particular models than the

boys with ADHD. For example, the boys with ADHD mentioned they would watch a sport on

television but not usually observe any one specifie person as frequently as the boys without

ADHD. The boys without ADHD observed specifie learning models (e.g., mother, siblings, sport

celebrity) and provided the reasons for selecting these people to watch. For example, Dale

watched his oIder brother play hockey because he was leaming how to perform body checks.

AIso, he mentioned the observation of a teenager, a 15 year-old elite hockey player, because that

person was good at stick-handling and checking. Bob without ADHD also provided sorne deep

insight about his observationallearning practices. "After 1 play, it's a lot ofgames, there's

another team that plays after me, an older team and 1 watch them play and 1 see how they use the

stick and shoot. They're older than me and better skilled". When asked for clarification, he

replied, "Yeah. 1 don't watch someone with lower skills than me because 1 won't learn that

much". Thus, the boys with ADHD seemed unaware of the benefits of observation to skill

improvement.

The question about observational learning sometimes led the participants to imagine

themselves in different game situations, the fourth property of knowing about doing, and

personal theories, the fifth property. The two groups ofboys made a similar number of

verbalizations about game situations and personal theories. For example, the boys with and

without ADHD recalled (a) play or game situations, with reference to the specifie movement

skills they used or were trying to learn and (b) causal attributions for practice effects on

performance.
161

The sixth and final property of knowing about doing was self-awareness where data units

reflected an awareness of individual skilllevels, game regulations and action strategies. There

were a similar number of data units for both groups about self-awareness. Overall, the self-

reflections accurately reflected the individual skiIllevels of the boys with and without ADHD on

the movement skill assessment. For example, the fundamental movement skills of boys with

ADHD ranged between poorto average on the GMDQ. Randy's performance on the TGMD-2

was described as below average in locomotor skills and average in object control skills (See

Table 2, Appendix W). He knew that he could not perform movement skills like many peers of

bis own age group. In fact, he stated, "1 know how to catch a baIl and throw a baIl, but

sometimes when 1 throw, it doesn't go far because, like in a baseball game, 1 have to throw the

ball to somebody. 1 can't throw it that far." Bob's performance on the TGMD-2 was described as

below average in locomotor skills and poor in object control skills (See Table 2, Appendix W).

He knew that he was less proficient in sport skills when compared to bis friends. "My friends

like soccer and 1 like playing soccer, but l'mjust not good at it. 1 like playing. l'Il play. It doesn't

bother me, but l' m not good at it."

The overall fundamental movement skills of the boys without ADHD on the TGMD-2

ranged between average to above average and their statements reflected their individual skill

levels. For example, Fred's performance on the TGMD-2 is described as above average in

locomotor skills and average in object control skills (See Table 2, Appendix W). He compared

bis movement skills to his peers in the following way. "It depends what l'm playing. Sometimes,

weIl the same or better". Furthermore, he also knew that parts of skill performance may be

automatized. For example, Fred remembered domain-specific terms for particular moves but

then forgot the terms after being accustomed to performing the related actions. While aIl of the
162

boys without ADHD referred to their movement skilllearning strategies, most of their peers with

ADHD (e.g., 5 of6 boys) did not do so during the open-ended interview questions. In contrast to

the boys with ADHD, the boys without ADHD deliberately planned specifie practice procedures

to improve their play skills. For example, Chad without ADHD spoke about throwing a football

at a black mark on a park fence and a basketball into plastic bins to deliberately improve his

throwing and shooting accuracy.

Another interesting difference between the boys with and without ADHD was

observed in question 14 where they were asked to compare their movement skills to other

children in their PE classes. The boys without ADHD reported their skills to be the same

or better when compared to their peers (M = 3.7) while the boys with ADHD reported

their movement and sport skills were better to much better when compared to other

children in PE class (M = 4.3), whether the comparison group was children with ADHD'

or not. Thus, in the context of peer comparisons in PE class, it seems that the boys with

ADHD either overrated their skill performance or provided socially desirable responses

while the boys without ADHD were fairly accurate about their movement skills.

Personal Feelings. Questions 15 to 17 explored feelings associated with movement skills

in PE. Table 5 illustrates the number of data units for the personal feelings category (See

Appendix X). Positive affective responses indicated enjoyable mental states while negative

affective responses indicated distressing mental states. There were a similar number of positive

affective statements related to specific activities and behaviors ofpeers in PE class. For example,

Claude with ADHD stated that he liked playing (a) basketball because "1 like bouncing it a lot. 1

like dribbling the baIl" and (b) volleyball "because it's fun". He mentioned that playing together

with other children was important to him because of "making new friends and its fun". Mike
163

without ADHD also focused on baIl sports and making mends. For example, he said, "1 really

like soccer because it's fun to play and it's a competitive sport, but ail other, like all friends can

play soccer. And it's a very calm game".

Both groups also made negative affective responses related to specifie activities and

behaviors oftheir peers in PE class. There were almost twice as many data units indicating

negative feelings about specific activities for the boys with ADHD than boys without the

disorder. For example, Randy with ADHD found many games to be boring and often his

negative feelings were related to a lack of skill, with children singling him out for his poor skill

proficiency. He mentioned that he would be picked last for football games and he did not like to

be "tackled and knocked down to the ground", "hit in the stomach by the football" and avoided

being passed to by the quarterback. Boys without ADHD also made negative affective responses

related to specific movement skills but they did not seem to get ridiculed. For example, Jim

without ADHD mentioned that he did not like gymnastics activities and when other children

were "picked on" in physical education class. Yet, he did not feel personally singled out for

ridicule.

Responses about prosocial behavior demonstrated an individual's positive social

behaviors related to movement skills and responses about asocial behavior indicated an

individual' s negative social behaviors related to movement skills. There were a similar number

of data units from both groups about prosocial behavior. For example, all of the boys spoke

about Ca) children who would encourage peers by cheering and clapping, Cb) having sorne choice

in activities, with fun games to play, and Cc) trying to behave in PE class in order to play as much

as possible. There were approximately 50% more data units about asocial behaviors related to

the movement skills of boys with ADHD than their peers without ADHD. The boys with ADHD
164

would often refer to being reprimanded for their misbehavior. They also blamed other children

for behavior problems that would decrease their own personal enjoyment of physical activity and

time spent in PE. Yet, both groups spoke about children insulting each other, calling each other

names, talking over the teacher' s voice, fooling around in class, cheating, fighting and hurting

others. For example, Larry with ADHD mentioned that he liked to play agame called "British

BuIldog" because you get to run into people. This type of aggressive behavior was also

mentioned by the children without ADHD. For example, Mike without ADHD mentioned the

"Pirate Ship" game, "Because there's always the, weIl, people are sharks and they're ronning

after people and they don't care. They'Il run into them and they'Illike hurt them to get them.

They'lllike go smack into them".

Discussion

Distinct group profiles of physical activity experiences were developed from the

interview data of the boys with and without ADHD. Our results indicated more differences than

similarities between the two groups ofboys. Both groups spoke about playing with peers, a

pleasant surprise because children with ADHD are well-known for having difficulty with social

relationships (Litner, 1999). Since the accuracy of the boys' statements could be questioned, we

verified with the parents and guardians that the boys with ADHD participated in physical

activities with other children.

There were several differences between the boys with and without ADHD. For example,

the boys with ADHD rarely organized opportunities to play or be active with other children. In

fact, they seemed to enjoy being involved in a variety of different activities, especially

individually-oriented sports or lei sure activities. These may be areas of strength for children with

ADHD to successfully explore their unique interests. Furthermore, the boys with ADHD devoted
165

little time to acquire the specifie details of physical activities whereas their peers without ADHD

would purposely learn much about their activity of interest. Perhaps the boys' testimonials are

retlections of the nature of ADHD. As Barkley (1997) would suggest, the boys with ADHD lack

the necessary problem-solving abilities required of carefully planned behavior; namely attention

to detail, adequate retlection time, and the need to delay immediate gratification. For example, a

lack of attention to detail emerged in our interview data and most of the boys with ADHD

resemble novices in movement skill performance. We would hardly expect the boys to

demonstrate highly proficient movement skillieveis when it seems that they participated in many

different activities, spending minimal amounts of deliberate time in learning and practicing the

intricacies of basic physical movements, sport skills, and recreational activities.

Planning and monitoring movements are critical to ensuring skilled performance over

time (Wall et al., in press). If the boys are novice-like in their planning and monitoring of

physical activity because they did not prepare or plan activities as our data suggest, their

movement skills are likely to remain low. Ifthis is correct, many children with ADHD may be

affected by a developmental skill-learning gap where children with less physical skill may have

limited opportunities to become involved in physical activities and experience minimal success as

they grow oIder (Harvey & Reid, 2005). The skill proficiency gap between children with less and

more physical skiU would widen over time as environmental constraints and other situational

demands become more complex in various movement contexts (Wall, 2004). Other important

factors, identified by researchers as important in the context of skill proficiency and physical

activity, include: (a) practice methods in free play and deliberate play contexts (Côté et al., 2003;

Côté & Hay, 2002), (b) necessary domain-specifie practice time and instruction (Ericsson, 2003),

(c) problem-solving (Bouffard & Wall, 1990), (d) self-regulated learning (Reid, Harvey, Lloyd, &
166

Bouffard, 2002) and (e) high levels of motivation (Côté et al., 2003). These five areas may be

starting points to develop a thorough understanding of the relationship between ADHD and skiU

performance and learning. Thus, there are opportunities to design research about the influence of

these factors and devise practical application strategies for persons with ADHD.

Our interview discussions about domain-specific vocabulary also demonstrate substantial

differences between the two groups with regard to selective attention. The boys with ADHD

knew different action terms but clearly indicated that they would not use these terms with their

peers. The boys without ADHD, on the other hand, knew that they would use the action terms in

the appropriate contexts of play or sports and they would also stop using certain terms after a

skill had been automatized. The boys with ADHD are likely to experience communication

problems in play if they do not realize, as their peers without ADHD understand, that domain-

specific terminology is required for proficiency in both individual skill performance and team

game performance. It seems the boys with ADHD know many action terms but they possess a

superficial understanding ofboth movement skills and the purposes for the actions within

broader contexts. For example, the boys with ADHD spoke about practice targets which, in tum,

were often socially undesirable. A person with ADHD will have difficulty making new friends if

he tries to hit people with a football for the sake of fun and practice. Also, this same person will

not be able to fine-tune throwing skills to other people and will lack sufficient deliberate play

and practice time to become a highly proficient performer (Cote et al., 2003). Furthermore, the

boys with ADHD seemed unwilling to use their domain-specific knowledge about action in the

appropriate contexts.

Our interviews demonstrate substantial qualitative differences in the depth of declarative

knowledge between the boys with and without ADHD, leading to the contention that the boys
167

with ADHD leaned heavily on using superficial domain-specifie tenninology to convince others

that they know what to do in different sports and at a high level. For example, the boys with

ADHD thought that they were more proficient movers and learners than suggested by the data.

This behavior is hypothesized to be a "faking" phenomenon where a person is not highly

proficient at a skill but can provide a socially desirable response. The tenn is not meant to be

pejorative rather it reflects a distinct form of affective knowledge required when one may try to

detract others' attention away from a personallack of specifie skill or skills (e.g., a defense

mechanism).

Another example of the superficiality of acquired knowledge about action for the boys

with ADHD can be observed in the choice of observational learning models. Not surprisingly,

boys without ADHD were able to demonstrate their preference for particular observational

learning models. However, most of the boys with ADHD did not name a specifie person

observed in order to learn how to become a more proficient moyer or player. Even when probed

for more infonnation, the boys did not articulate why it would be important to watch others to

learn from. Yet, the boys without ADHD did mention the rationale for choosing specifie learning

models. Most likely, the boys with ADHD were unaware that proficient skill performance also

includes a depth ofskill acquisition knowledge. Clearly, they were unaware of the benefits of

observational learning to skill improvement. It would seem the typical symptom combinations of

impulsivity, inattention and overactivity present formidable impediments to the development of

the movement skill perfonnance and proficiency of children with ADHD.

The boys with ADHD also seemed to incorporate an external attributional response style

in physical activity. In fact, the boys expressed many negative aspects of physical activity and

were over-reliant on external causes for movement success and failure. For example, they
168

blamed other children for behavior problems that would decrease their own personal enjoyment

of physical activity and PE. Barkley (1997) suggested persons with ADHD may experience

difficulties when trying to gain emotional self-control and, in turn, negative affective thoughts

would likely lead to avoidance behaviors. Thus, physical awkwardness and leamed helplessness

are suitable constructs to explain the boys' expressions. For example, one of the boys with

ADHD said that he would feel embarrassed if he performed a skill incorrectly because the other

children might laugh or the teacher might get angry. He also mentioned that he gets hurt often

when attempting novel physical skills and activities. While knowing he could not throw a baIl in

a direct line or accurately at a target, he did not seek help to learn for fear ofbeing ridiculed.

When teaching sessions were offered on how to throw a baIl, the boy suggested that the first

author would not be able to make available the sufficient time to help. Thus, the effects of

physical awkwardness and learned helplessness or low skill proficiency and intense feelings of

inability seemed to be present. Interestingly enough, it took approximately 90 minutes to teach

this boy how to throw a ball (e.g., 3 sessions of30 minutes). He did not understand the ideas of

force regulation and point of release in the skill of overhand throwing. Perhaps it would be

beneficial to devise task-specific intervention programs (e.g., Hodge et al., 1999; Trocki-Ables,

French, & O'Connor, 2001) for children with ADHD to improve their movement skills and, in so

doing, they may participate more readily in physical activity.

However, this recommendation is grounded by the realization that the boys with ADHD

named few specifie learning models or other people from which to draw valuable information.

Highly skilled performers often have parents or significant others who exert a significant

influence on their skiU development (Côté et al., 2003). In fact, these adults often guide and lead

the child to become involved in a variety of activities, with a gradual withdrawal of direct
169

involvement as individual skill and sport proficiency increase (Côté et al., 2003). Since many

children with ADHD experience social problems at home (Grizenko & Pawliuk, 1994) and have

trouble establishing and maintaining social relationships (Litner, 1999), there may be minimal

guidance and support from significant others in order to learn important skills (e.g., play, social,

academic, etc.). Clearly, children with ADHD would benefit from the watchful and attentive eyes

of significant others like parents, friends, teachers, coaches, and volunteers. Participation in

ADHD-sensitive PE classes and after school or recreational programs may provide the opportunity

to participate in supportive learning and performance environments. Future research should

investigate the influence ofthese mentors and community-based programs on the functional skill

performance of children with ADHD. Renee, there may be positive and practical benefits ofusing

an expertise approach when understanding the skill development of children with ADHD.

Conclusions

The groups ofboys spoke about very different physical activity experiences and they did not

regulate their physical activityinvolvement in a similar manner. Contrary to the suggestion that

children with ADHD possess sufficient factual domain-specifie knowledge but cannot perform

what they know (Barkley, 1997), limited physical activity experiences and acquired knowledge

were apparent in the children with ADHD. They possessed a superficial understanding ofvarious

knowledge factors underlying both movement skills and sport skills. For example, the children

with ADHD knew the domain-specifie terms for different actions and games but they lacked a

deeper understanding of associated concepts. In other words, the children may indicate to their

peers and adults appropriate movement skill terminology but may not possess a complete

understanding of the factors underlying movement performance. Such superficial differences in

skiI~ procedural knowledge, and declarative knowledge have been found in soccer between adults
170

with physical disabilities who were experienced spectators of soccer and their peers without

disabilities who had 14 to 15 years of playing experience (Williams & Davids, 1995).

To our knowledge, this is the tirst qualitative study to provide a voice for children with

ADHD in relation to their movement skills. We should increase the range of qualitative methods

used to gain more insightful information about the skilllearning of children with ADHD. For

example, the use of the probes in our interview format was important since the quality of 8 to 16

year-old children's recall ofphysical activity improves with verbal prompting (McKenna, Foster,

& Page, 2004). Yet there is also the possibility ofusing a wide range of inductive qualitative

research methods in ADHD research (e.g., drawing, collage, art wode, etc.) to provide more

opportunity for the children' s physical activity experiences to be voiced from a hermeneutic

phenomenological approach (e.g, Goodwin et al., 2004). Our fmdings willlead to more in-depth

research about the self-regulatory behavior of children with and without ADHD in different

physical activity contexts. Most importantly, this exploratory study has led to a preliminary

understanding of the concomitant factors associated with the physical activity experiences of

children with ADHD. This research area promises to be an exciting avenue ofresearch inquiry.
171

References

Achenbach, T.M. (1991). The child behavior checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile. Burlington,

VT: University of Vermont.

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical marmal ofmental

disorders, (4th ed.). Text Revision. Washington, DC: Author.

Barkley, R.A (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-<:ontrol. New York: Guilford Press.

Barkley, R.A (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbookfor diagnosis

and treatment (2"" ed). New York: Guilford Press.

Bouffard, M., & Wall, AB. (1990). A problem-solving approach to movement skill

acquisition. In G. Reid (Ed.)., Problems in Movement Control (pp. 283-316). North-

Rolland: Elsevier Science Publishers.

Brantlinger, E., Jimenez, R., Klingner, J., Pugach, M., & Richardson, V. (2005).

Qualitative studies in special education. Fxceptional Children, 12 (2), 195-207.

Burton, AW., & Miller, D.B. (1998). Movement skill assessment. Champaign, IL: Human

Kinetics.

Chess, S. (1960). Diagnosis and treatment of the hyperactive child. New York State

Journal ofMedicine, 60,2379-2385.

Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and

PsychologicalMeasurement, 20,37-46.

Conners, C.K. (1997a). Conners Global1ndex-Parent Version. North Tonawanda, NY:

Multihealth Systems Inc.

Conners, c.K. (1997b). Conners Globallndex-Teacher Version. North Tonawanda, NY:

Multihealth Systems Inc.


172

Côté, 1., & Hay, 1. (2002). Children's involvement in sport: A developmental perspective.

In lM. Silva and D.E. Stevens (Eds.), Psychological foundations of sport. (pp. 484-

502). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Côté l, Baker, l, & Abernathy, B. (2003). From play to practice: A developmental

framework for the acquisition of expertise in team sports. In IL. Starkes and KA.

Ericsson (Eds.), Expert peiformance in sports. (pp. 89-133).Champaign, IL: Human

Kinetics.

Côté, J, Salmela, IH., & Russell, S. (1995). The knowledge of high-performance

gymnastics coaches: Methodological framework. The Sport Psychologist, 9, 65-

75.

Craig, CL., Cameron, C, Russell, S.l, & Beaulieu, A. (2000). 2000 Physical Activity

Monitor. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Fitness and Lifestyle Research Institute.

Creswell, J.W. (2003). Research Design. Qualitative, Quantitative andMixedMethods


nd
Approaches (2 ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Inc.

Denzin, N.K, & Lincoln, YS. Handbook of Qualitative Research (rd ed). Thousand

Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Inc.

Donders,l (1997). A short form of the WISC-ID for clinical use. Psychological

Assessment, 9, 15-20.

Douglas, YI. (1999). Cognitive control processes in attention-deficitlhyperactivity

disorder. In HC. Quay & A.E. Hogan (Eds.), Handbook ofdisruptive behavior

disorders. (pp. 105-13 8). New York: Kluwer Academic / Plenum Publishers.

Ericsson, KA. (2003). Development of elite performance and deliberate practice: An

update of the expert performance approach. In IL. Starkes and KA. Ericsson
173

(Eds.), Expert performance in sports (pp. 49-83). Champaign, IL: Ruman Kinetics.

Goodwin, D.L., .Krohn, 1., & Kuhnle, A. (2004). Beyond the wheelchair: The experience of dance.

Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 21, 229-247.

Grizenko, N., & Pawliuk, N. (1994). Risk and protective factors for disruptive behavior disorders

in children. American Journal ofOrthopsychiatry, 64,534-544.

Harvey, W.1., & Reid, G. (1997). Motor performance of children with attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder: A preliminary investigation. Adapted Physical Activity

Quarterly, 14, 189-202.

Harvey, W.1., & Reid, G. (2003). A review offundamental movement skill performance

and physical fitness of children with ADHD. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly,

20, 1-25.

Harvey, W.J., & Reid, G. (2005). Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Ways to

improve AP A research. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 22, 1-20.

Harvey, W.J., Fagan, T., & Kassis, 1. (2003). Enabling students with ADHD to use self-

control in physical activities. PALAESIRA 19(3),32-35.

Harvey, W.1., Reid, G., Grizenko, N., Mbekou, v., Ter-Stepanian, M., & Joober, R. (in

review). Fundamental movement skills and children with ADHD: Stimulant

effects and peer comparisons. Submitted to the Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology.

Hodge, S.R., Murata, N.M., & Porretta, D.L. (1999). Enhancing motor performance through

various preparatory activities involving children with learning disabilities. Clinical

Kinesiology, 53(4), 76-82.

Keogh, 1. (1978). Movement outcomes as conceptual guidelines in the perceptual-motor

maze. Journal of Special Education, 12,321-329.


174

Labey, B.B., Applegate, B., McBumett, K., Biedennan, J, Greenhill, 1., Hynd, G.W., et al.

(1994). DSM-IV field trials for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children

and adolescents. American Journal ofPsychiatry, 151, 1673-1685.

Litner, B. (1999). Understanding attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and learning

disabilities: Considerations for child and youth care workers. Journal ofChild and

Youth Care Work, 14,29-42.

McKenna, l, Foster, L.l, & Page, A. (2004). Exploring recall ofphysical activity in young

people using qualitative interviewing. Pediatrie Exercise Science, 16, 5-14.

Merriam, S.B. (1998). Qualitative research and case study applications in education. San

Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers.

Miyabara, M., Mobs, 1., & DolI-Tepper, G. (1995). Subtypes ofclinically identified

children with hyperkinetic syndrome based upon perceptual motor function and

classroom behaviors. In 1. Morisbak & P.E. Jorgensen (Eds.), 1(jh International

Symposium on Adapted Physical Activity: Quality of life through adapted physical

activity, A lifespan concept (pp. 278- 286). Oslo, Norway: Ham Trykk NS.

National Institute of Mental Hea1th (1998). NIMH DISC-IV Joy and William Ruane Center

to Identify and Treat Mood Disorders. Washington, DC: Columbia University.

Pedersen, S.l, Surburg, P.R, Heath, M., & Kojeca, D.M. (2004). Fractionated lower

extremity response time perfonnance in boys with and without ADHD. Adapted

Physical Activity Quarterly, 21, 315-329.

Pelham, W.E., McBurnett, K., Harper, G.W., Milich, R, Murphy, D.A., Clinton, l, &

Thiele, C. (1990). Methylphenidate and baseball playing in ADHD children: Who' s on

fIfSt? Journal ofConsulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 130-133.


175

Rasmussen, P., & Gillberg, C. (1983). Perceptual, motor and attentional deficits in seven-

year-old children. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica, 72, 125-130.

Reid, G. (1992). Editorial on theory, exchange, and terminology. Adapted Physical

Activity Quarterly, 9, 1-10.

Reid, G., Harvey, W.I, Lloyd, M., & Bouffard, M. (2002). A pilot study of self-directed

motor learning in children with DCD. Paper presented at the DCD-V International

Conference: Mechanisms, Measurement, and Management. Banff, AB: Canada.

Ryan, G.W., & Russell Bernard, H. (2000). Data management and analysis methods. In

N.K. Denzin and YS. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook ofqualitative research (r ed).
(pp. 769-802). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1997). Memory Development Between Two and Twenty.

(2 nd ed). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

Sheppard, D.M., Bradshaw, IL., Georgiou, N., Bradshaw, lA, & Lee, P. (2000).

Movement sequencing in children with tourette' s syndrome and attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Movement Disorders, J5, 1184-1193.

Sparkes, AC. (1998). Validity in qualitative inquiry and the problem of criteria:

Implications for sport psychology. The Sport Psychologist, J2, 363-386.

Sparkes, AC. (2003). Telling tales in sport and physical activity: A qualitative joumey.

Champaign, ll.-: Human Kinetics.

Starkes, J.L., Helsen, W., & Jack, R (2001). Expert performance in sport and dance. In

RN. Singer, HA Hausenblas, & C.M. Janelle (Eds.), Handhook of sport

psychology (2 nd ed., pp. 174-201). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Szatmari, P., Offord, D.R, & Boyle, M.H (1989). Correlates, associated impairments and
176

patterns of service utilization of children with attention deficit disorder: Findings from the

Ontario child health study. Journal ofChild Psychology and Psychiatry, 30,205-217.

Trocki-Ables, P., French, R, & O'Connor, l (2001). Use ofprimary and secondary

reinforcers after performance of al-mile walk/run by boys with attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Perceptual andMotor Skills, 93(2),461-464.

Ulrich, D.A (2000). Test of Gross Motor Development (ZW ed). Austin, TX: PRO-ED, Inc.

Vickers, lN., Rodrigues, S.T., & Brown, L.N. (2002). Gaze pursuit and arm control of

adolescent males diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

and normal controls: evidence of a dissociation in processing visual information of

a short and long duration. Journal ofSport Sciences, 20,201-216.

Wall, AE. (2004). The developmental skill-Ieaming gap hypothesis: Implications for

children with movement difficulties. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 21, 197-

218.

Wall, AB., McClements, l, Bouffard, M., Findlay, H, & Taylor, M.l (1985). A

knowledge-based approach to motor development: Implications for the physically

awkward. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 2,21-42.

Wall, AB., Reid, G., & Harvey, W.J. (in press). Interface of the Knowledge-based and

Ecological Task Analysis Approaches. Book chapter for text dedicated to the memory of

Dr. Allan Burton. Human Kinetics: Champaign, ll..

Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scalefor Children (3 rd ed): Marmal. San

Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Williams, M., & Davids, K. (1995). Declarative knowledge in sport: A by-product of


177

experience or a characteristic of expertise. Journal ofSport and Exercise Psychology, J 7,

259-275.
178

Chapter 6

Summary

This doctoral dissertation is a series of manuscripts devoted to understanding the

movement skills ofpersons with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). There are six

chapters. The first chapter was called theoretical perspectives and it provided a rationale for

combining the two main theoretical approaches that guided the research. Thus, the knowledge-

based approach to motor development (Wall et al., 1985) and the inhibitory model of executive

functioning (Barkley, 1997) were described. Chapters two and three are two manuscripts,

published in the Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, which form a comprehensive review of the

literature. Chapter two, the first review paper, provided a discussion about important issues

related to ADHD that included a contemporary definition of the disorder, etiology,

nomenclature, classification, comorbidity, treatment, and developmental course (Harvey & Reid,

2003). This review of 49 movement behavior studies about ADHD, published between 1949 and

2002, indicated that children with ADHD may experience poor levels of physical fitness and

movement skiU difficulties when compared to children without ADHD, with a likelihood of

experiencing coinorbid developmental coordination disorder (DCD). While few intervention

studies on the movement performance and physical fitness of children with ADHD were

retrieved, future physical activity researchers and program planners in ADHD were referred to

numerous reference citations for seminal review articles on ADHD.

Chapter three, the second review paper, explored the research methods used in movement

performance studies about ADHD and provided useful information for adapted physical activity

(APA) researchers interested in ADHD research (Harvey & Reid, 2005). It is a unique

manuscript because approximately 20 new AP A research questions about ADHD were provided.
179

AIso, there was extensive discussion of the identification ofpersons with ADHD, the importance

of providing complete descriptions of research participants with ADHD as well as discussions and

recommendations about key data collection issues related to the organization of the research

environment, associated motivational concerns, and medication use. Strategies to improve AP A

research about ADHD were provided to strengthen research designs because substantial

methodological inconsistencies were identified in the CUITent ADHD physical activity literature.

The two review papers, when combined, are a substantial and original contribution to the ADHD

literature.

Chapter four was a study about the effects of stimulant medication on the qualitative

components of fundamental movement skill performance for 22 children with ADHD and a

comparison oftheir movement skills to age- and gender-matched peers without ADHD. This

investigation was based on the information from the two review manuscripts and a strict research

design resulted. It is one of the first AP A studies to explore the influence of an external factor,

methylphenidate, on the movement skills of children with ADHD. Furthermore, experimental

control was exerted over more independent variables than achieved in previous AP A research

(e.g., gender, age, and family income level). The use ofrepeated measures designs and

multivariate statistical analyses was also considered as an important aspect of the study. There

was no significant effect of methylphenidate on the performance criteria of the TGMD-2 (Ulrich,

2000). Again, the clinical trial component of the study would have benefited from a larger

sample size. However, there were significant differences between the children with and without

ADHD on both locomotor skills and object control skills. The development and presentation of

the movement skill profiles of children with and without ADHD is an original and unique
180

contribution to the movement skill performance literature. This manuscript is currently in the

peer review process.

Chapter five is a study that explores the perceptions of six boys with and without ADHD

about their physical activity experiences. This study is one of the first attempts to explore the

physical activity experiences of children with ADHD with the use of qualitative research

methodology. In fact, the children's physical activity voices were sought because the published

literature mentions only the concerns of parents and teachers, with no representation from the

children. The interpretation of the interviews was an important element of the study where a

mixed methodology was utilized to obtain a deeper understanding of the physical activity

experiences of the boys with and without ADHD. There were sorne similarities expressed by the

two groups about their physical activity experiences. However, differences were apparent on key

aspects of skiU proficiency (e.g., attention to detail, knowledge about action, and affective

knowledge) that may be problematic for the boys with ADHD. This manuscript is also in the

peer review process.

This sixth and final chapter provided a brief summary of the relevant findings and future

recommendations follow. The doctoral dissertation represents a new research li ne in AP A. For

example, the four manuscripts are a cohesive series of papers that build on each other. The two

review papers provide a comprehensive understanding of ADHD in relation to physical activity,

with the two subsequent studies building on the reviewed information as well as contributing to

the current physical activity knowledge base about ADHD.

The combination oftheories to guide the research project was unique as each theory

provided a needed link between sport expertise and psychology in order to explore the movement

skills of children with ADHD. The review manuscripts have already been cited in the ADHD
181

literature and form a comprehensive overview in relation to physical activity. They are original

contributions to the ADHD literature as we were able to gather together research findings that

compared children with and without ADHD and, in tum, gain a clearer understanding of the

movement skill performance of children with ADHD. The research challenge review manuscript

provided general guidelines for performing research studies for children with ADHD in physical

activity. Most of the methodological suggestions were borrowed from key psychology and

psychiatry researchers in ADHD. Thus, in combination, these review papers should guide the

research in this area of inquiry for many years to come.

Future research studies about persons with ADHD and movement skills will be created as

my research program will continue to develop and grow. There is a lifetime of research

opportunities in this relatively new and exciting APA area. For example, the series of20 new

research questions, generated in the review papers, could not be conducted over the lifetime of a

single researcher. It will be exciting and interesting to study particular constructs related to

ADHD. For example, can children with ADHD self-regulate their learning and performance in

physical activity? A mixed methods procedure emerged from this research experience that, in

tum, willlead to an in-depth understanding of the self-regulatory processes ofmovement skill

acquisition and proficiency of aU children, including persons with ADHD. Thus, future

investigations of movement skill proficiency and children with ADHD are anticipated by

combining the expertise approach with interviewing methods. However, a greater range of

qualitative research methods will be incorporated to gain much deeper insight to persons with

ADHD and movement-related phenomena. Therefore, we may be able to achieve greater

understanding of the metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive skills of children with ADHD

by further describing various degrees of procedural and declarative knowledge about action. We
182

may gain much insight about the skilllearning and physical activity experiences of more people

with ADHD. Thus, the CUITent study provides the reader with a greater range ofmethodological

tools to draw from. This fact is important and a valuable contribution to research in the area as

we demonstrate that different methodologies help to answer different questions and illuminate

the skill sets and specifie interests of children with ADHD. AIso, there may be a unique

opportunity to have a significant AP A treatment impact on a substantial number of people

affected by ADHD. For example, it may be possible to develop, use, or learn cognitive strategies

to meet physical activity task demands for persons with ADHD as this type of intervention

method has been successful for persons with learning disabilities (Kowalski & Sherrill, 1992) or

intellectual disabilities (Reid, 1980a, 1980b). While these intervention types need to be

developed, it is clear from our research data that clinical populations of children with ADHD are

"at-risk" for movement skill problems. Practitioners will benefit from reading the dissertation

manuscripts by understanding the skill performance based issues, the personal and environmental

constraints sUITounding people with ADHD, effective teaching and coping strategies, and

practical solutions for active and effective living.

There is much more to explore and learn about persons with ADHD and physical activity.

It may take many years for answers to surface in response to the research questions raised in the

dissertation. Isolated studies, indicating similar results, were drawn together in this doctoral

research project. Clearly, many people with ADHD experience movement skill difficulties. Since

overactivity has been an integral component of the ADHD definition for the past 40 years

(Barkley, 1998), perhaps APA researchers may help to better understand the nature of ADHD by

examining the influence ofkey constructs in physical activity contexts. For example, what type

of causal attribution response styles do people with ADHD employ in different physical activity
183

contexts and what are the associated impacts on specific physical activities and exercise

adherence? Also, we may be able to devise regression models for ADHD, with movement skill

performance as a predictor variable.

A lifespan perspective is also recommended as ADHD and its behavioral correlates may

continue to be present throughout a person's lifetime (Weiss & Hechtmann, 1993). Our data

suggest that there is sorne type of difficulty in the learning and performance of movement skills

for many cbildren with ADHD. Yet, the developmental course ofmovement skills and sport-

specifie skills for people with ADHD is not well understood because most AP A investigations

about ADHD have been cross-sectional research designs. Furthermore, we have a limited

understanding of the physical activity experiences of adolescents and adults with ADHD.

Perhaps we may find interesting activity patterns that other researchers have yet to discover and

explore. For example, we may observe the various types ofphysical activities and recreational

pursuits that adolescents and adults with ADHD prefer and perform. We may then inquire if

these people with ADHD feel empowered in the choice of activities. Moreover, we can ask about

the rationale for activity selection and observe if there are tendencies to adhere to specific

activities or participate sporadically in a variety of activities. Thus, cross-sectional and

longitudinal investigations about sport, exercise, and physical activity are recommended for

people with ADHD of all ages. Again, we re-emphasize the importance of conducting sound and

etbical research projects wbile also collaborating at a multidisciplinary team level. Thus, tbis line

of inquiry has much to offer scholars and field professionals if they choose to study or work in

the area. We believe tbis work will be cited in much of the ADHD and physical education

literature circles due to the complexity of the research designs and the robust findings which

emerged.
184

In summary, this dissertation is a cohesive series of original papers that have led to a

better understanding of the movement skills of children with ADHD. However, there is a need

and ample opportunity for more in-depth AP A research about people with ADHD.
185

References

Barkley, R.A. (1997). ADHD and the nature ofself-control. New York: Guilford Press.

Barkley, R.A. (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handhook for diagnosis

and treatment (r ed.). New York: Guilford Press.


Harvey, W.l, & Reid, G. (2003). A review offundamental movement skill performance

and physical fitness of children with ADHD. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly,

20, 1-25.

Harvey, W.l, & Reid, G. (2005). Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Ways to

improve AP A research. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 22, 1-20.

Kowalski, E.M., & Sherrill, C. (1992). Motor sequencing ofboys with learning

disabilities: Modeling and verbal rehearsal strategies. Adapted Physical Activity

Quarterly, 9,261-272.

Reid, G. (1980a). The effects ofmemory strategy instruction in the short-term memory of

the mentally retarded. Journal ofMotor Behavior, 12,221-227.

Reid, G. (1980b). Overt and covert rehearsal in short-term motor memory ofmentally

retarded and nonretarded pesrons. American Journal ofMental Deficiency, 85, 69-75.

Ulrich, D.A. (2000). Test of Gross Motor Development (rd ed). Austin, TX: PRO-ED, Inc.

Wall, A.E., McClements, l, Bouffard, M., Findlay, H., & Taylor, M.l (1985). A

knowledge-based approach to motor development: Implications for the physically

awkward. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 2,21-42.

Weiss, G., & Hechtman, L. (1993). Hyperactive Children Grown Up, (2nd ed) New York:

The Guilford Press.


186

Appendices
189

Appendix C
HÔPIT.uDOUGLAS HOSPITAL
6875, boulevard Lasalle, Verdun (Québec!. Canada H4H 1 R3 Tél. : (514) i61·6131 Téléc.: (514) 8.88-1067

September, 2002

Dear Parent or Guardian,

My name is Billy Harvey and 1 am a doctoral srudent at the Depamnent of Kinesiology and Physical
Education of McGill Univer;ity. This lener requests the permission for both you and your sonldaughter to panicipare
in a physical acrivity research srudy to be conducted at the hospital. 1 believe that the cmldren will find the
experiencc to be enjoyable.

My research has been designed to describe the fundamental movement skills used by childreD with
anention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in everyday play activities such as running and throwing. There are
rwo main purposes of my research. Fir;!, 1 will ask children with ADHD to perform a 10 minute movement skills
test. Second, 1 wou Id also Iike 10 interview some of the chilciren so that they can describe their unique experiences in
everyday play and sport senings.

Vour sonidaughter is currently part of a research srudy at the hospital where heishe performs a series of tests
when he/she is on and offmedication. 1 will administer the movement skills test once per week durine their rwo week
testing period. The test is commonly used by physical education teachers to assess locomotor skills ai;d object
control skills. The locomotor skills evaluaied are: run. gallop. hop, leap. horizontal jump. and slide. The object
control skills evaluated are: striking a stationary bail (base baIl batting), basketball dribble. catching, kicking,
overhand throw. and underhand roll. The chilciren will be able to perform the rasks at their own pace because we are
observing the quali~ of the children's movement ski Ils not their speed and accuracy. 1 will videotape cach
performance in order to accurately evaluate the movement skills.

1 will also conduct an interview with your child ifheishe is berween 9 and 12 years old 50 that he/she may
describe his/her ph~'sical activity experiences. The interview will last approximately 45 minutes to 1 hour and each
interview will be videotaped. About 1 week afrer the interview has been conducted. 1 will read a summary of the
interview to both you and your child for your feedback and input. Children berween 6 to 8 years will participate in
the movement skills test but they are not decmed old enough for the interview.

1 will not show the videotapes to anyone without your wrinen permission. Please be assured that no child's
name will be associated with any presentation or publication that might cmerge from this research. Vour child's
identity is secured. 1 have 12 years of teaching experience in physical education and 1 am currently the department
head ofrecreation and leisure services at the hospÎtal. 1 am weil aware of the importance ofconiidentiality. respect
for each participant. and the right of each chi Id to witheiraw at any time from the study without any reservation
whatsoever. Even ifyou sign the form below, your child is !Tee to witheiraw at any point later on \vithout question. In
fact. 1 have also arrached a place on the form for your child to agree to and sign.

io43ou~.1 "·C".JC1rncmcm "1n:;iJlL",.~ry Ccntn Ile cDila~IOft dE l'O~tS )CDI'Urcu'" H..O C.,./aocJl'un; Ccnrn:
.:c11':nu~III·~MI Tcac!'II"! HcaPicU Oc ~lonft21 pow' 13 l'II:'~ jOtiù:::tarr.l.and7~"r
ri la ionnaUCMI en IoIIIlIIt nwncaJc "'~c"Q.lHalUI
191

AppendixD

~ McG1ll
e

~/

Depertment of Klnesiology and Phys1cal Education Département ~f kmfs1olog1e et d'éducation physique (514) 398-4184
McGill University Université McGl1i (514)398-4186 Fax
475 Pi ne Avenue West 475 avenue des Pins Ques1
Montreal. PO. Canada H2W 154 Montréal IOuébec) Canada H2W 1$4

April, 2003

Dear Parent or Guardian,

My narne is Bi1Iy Harvey and 1 am a doctoral student at the Department of Kinesiology and
Physical Education of McGiIl University. This lener requests the permission for both you and your
sonldaughter to participate in a physical activity research study to be conducted at the school. 1 believe
that the children will find the experience to be enjoyable.

The main purpose of my research is to describe the movement skills used by children with
anention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADIID). In order to do this, 1 need to compare the movement
skill performance of children with and without ADI-ID. Your sonldaughter has been identified as a person
without a disability by the school principal and hislher teacher. Therefore, your child would be a part of
the important comparison group.

Three different tests will he administered to your child: a) a standard intelligence test which will
require 1 to 2 hours, b) a 20 minute academic achievement test, and c) a 20-30 minute movement skiIls
test. The movement skills test is commonly used by physical education teachers to assess locomotor
skills and object control skiIIs_ The locomotor skiIIs evaluated are: run, gaIlop, hop, leap, horizontal
jump, and slide. The object control skills evaluated are: striking a stationary baIl (baseball baning),
basketball dribble, catching, kicking, overhand throw, and underhand roll. The children will perform the
tasks at their own pace. These performances wiII be videotaped hecause we are observing the quality of
the children's movement skills not their speed and accuracy. Please rest assured that we will notif)' the
school principal immediately if the test scores indicate any major problems. 1 do not believe this will he
the case since your child has been identified as a person without problems. In fact, your child should he a
good candidate to be in our important comparison group.

1 will also conduct an interview with your child ifhelshe is between 9 and 12 years old so that
he/she may describe hislher physical activity experiences~ The interview williast approximately 45
minutes to 1 hour and it will be videotaped. About 1 week after the interview, 1 will read a summary of it
to both you and your child for your feedback and input.

1 will not show the videotapes to anyone without your wrinen permission. Please be assured that
no child's narne will be associated with any presentation or publication that might emerge from this
research. Your child's identity is secured. 1 have 12 years ofteaching experience in physical education
and! am current1y the department head ofrecreation and leisure services at a hospital in Montreal. 1 am
weil aware of the importance of confidentiality, respect for each participant, and the right of each child to
withdraw at any time from the study without any reservation whatsoever. Even ifyou sign the form
below, your child is free to withdraw at any point later on without question. ln fact, 1 have also anached a
place on the fonn for your child to agree to and sign.
193

AppendixE

Table 1 Historical Review ofDSM Nomenclature

Manual Classification Terms Major Subtypes

DSM-I (1952) None None

DSM-ll (1968) Hyperkinetic Syndrome None


of Childhood

DSM-ID (1980) Attention Deficit Disorder ADDH,ADD-H

DSM-ID-R (1987) Attention-Deficit pervasive, situational


Hyperactivity Disorder

DSM-N (1994) Attention-Deficit Inattentive,


Hyperactivity Disorder Hyperactive-Impulsive,
& Combined Types

DSM-N-TR (2000) Attention-Deficit Inattentive,


Hyperactivity Disorder Hyperactive-Impulsive,
& Combined Types

Note. DSM = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders publi shed by the American
Psychiatric Association, Washington, D.C., ADDH = Attention Deficit Disorder with
Hyperactivity, ADD-H = Attention Deficit Disorder without Hyperactivity, Situational subtype =
only symptomatic at one setting or another (APA, 1987, p. 50), Pervasive subtype = symptomatic
in at least two or more settings (APA, 1987, p. 50). See Hechtman (2000) for thorough review of
diagnostic practices.
Table 2

Movement Performance

Study Purpose Sample Statistical Findings


Analysis
Intergroup Comparison

Beyer Compared movement skills of 112 boys MANOVA, Significant multivariate age (p < .001) and
(1999) boys with ADHD and boys with ADHD = 56, taking Ritalin ANOVA condition (p < .001) main effects were found on
LD LD = 56, no medication the BOTMP-LF. ANOVAs revealed boys with
7-12 years ADHD performed bilateral coordination (p <
DSM-IV .001), strength (p < .001), visuaI motor
coordination (p < .001), and upper limb speed ~
"CS
and dexterity (p < .001) significantly worse than g
their peers with LD. &.
~
ITJ
Doyle et al. (1995) Exarnined movement skills of 38 children multiple 82% of children with ADHD performed gross
children with ADHD and 33 boys, 5 girls independent motor skills above the norms of the BOTMP-SF.
parental perceptions about their 7-12 years t tests Parents underrated the movement skill
children' s skills DSM-III-R performance oftheir children with the use of 5-
stimulant medication use was point Likert scaIe.
variable

Wade (1976) Described differences on 24 children descriptive Cbildren witbout a disability spent, on the
stabilometer task between 12=ND statistics average, more time on the static balancing task
children with and without 12 = hyperactive and performed more consistently tban children
ADHD and examined the effects 7.7-11.8 years withADHD.
ofRitalin on children with genders not reported
ADHD performing a diagnostic framework not reported
stabilometer task stimulant medication taken Table continues

-
\0
.j::o.
î

Harvey & Reid Described fundamental 19 children Descriptive Children with ADHD performed locomotor
(1997) movement skills and fitness 17 boys, 2 girls statistics, skills (22.3 percentile) and object control skills
conditions of children with 5-12 years graphs (33.4 percentile) below the 35th percentile when
ADHD DSM-III-R compared to the age-matched norms of the
89% of sample took stimulant TGMD.
medication

Intragroup Comparison
Piek et al. Investigated movement skills and 48 boys MANOVA, Boys with ADHD (ADHD-PI, ADHD-C)
(1999) kinaesthetic processes ofboys 16 = ADHD-PI ANOVA performed significantly poorer (p < .002) on the
with and without ADHD 16 = ADHD-C MABC than boys without disabiIities. In
16=ND comparison with the other groups, boys with
matched on age & verbal IQ ADHD-PI demonstrated significantly worse
8.7-11. 7 years manual dexterity skills (p < .01) while boys with
DSM-IV ADHD-C demonstrated significantly worse
8 children from (ADHD-C) balance skills (p < .01).
received stimulant medication

Miyahara et al. Identified movement and 23 children Cluster Significant univariate results were found
(1995) behavioral subtypes ofHKS 21 boys, 2 girls analyses, between the free-from-severe motor impairment
while estimating the comorbidity 4-12 years ANOVA and mannal incoordination motor c1usters on
ofHKS with DCD diagnostic framework for HKS manual dexterity (p < .01) and balance (p < .01)
from a German psychiatric subtests of the MABC. 52% of the sample, or 12
textbook out of 23 participants, fell in the manual
stimulant medications taken incoordination subtype and were thus
considered as having DCD.

Table Continues
....
\0
Ul
Kaplan et al. Described comorbidity between 162 children Descriptive Assessment instruments were the BOTMP,
(1998) DCD, ADHD, and RD Specific age and gender statistics, MABC, and an initial version of the DCDQ. Of
information was not provided. Pearson the 162 participants with comorbidity, 47
However, the participants were correlation children had no disabilities. There were pure
selected because complete data coefficients, cases of ADHD (N = 8), DCD (N = 26), and RD
were available from an initial ,l, (N = 19). Comorbidity was identified for
sample of379 children ANOVA ADHDIRD (N = 7), ADHDIDCD (N = 10),
169 boys, 55 girls = ADHD + LD DCDIRD (N= 22), and ADHDIDCDIRD (N =
105 boys, 50 girls = ND 23).
8-18 years
DSM-III-R
Medication not reported

Miyahara et al. Examined the severity of HKS 47 children ANOVA Children with HKS, attending a school, were
(2001) behaviors and comorbidity 38 boys, 9 girls rated with significantly greater conduct
between HKS and DCD from average age = 8.4 years problems (p < .02) than their hospitalized peers
school, support group, and diagnostic framework for HKS with HKS and greater hyperactive behaviors (p
hospital sample sources from a German psychiatric < .09) when compared to other peers with RKS
textbook from a support group. No significant differences
stimulant medications were not were found between the groups on the MABC'
reported Substantial amounts of overlap between HKS
and DCD were found at the school (35%),
support group (54%), and hospital (55%).
Intervention Effectiveness
Hodge et al. Examined the effects of warm-up 46 children Two-way Children with ADHD (visual imagery warm-up
(1999) activities on fundamental 36 males factorial condition) demonstrated significantly better
movement skill performance of 10 females ANOVAs throwing accuracy (p < .001) than their peers
children with LD + ADHD 9-11 years (group skill who participated in task-specific warm-up or no
diagnostic framework not reported x gender) warm-up conditions. No performance
medications were not reported differences were found between the groups on a
timed 40 yd-dash and a bail catching task. No
main effects for gender or interaction effects
werefound.
......
Table Continues ~
24 children ANOVA The static balance of children with ADHD on
Wade (1976) Described differences on
12=ND the stabilometer improved significantly (p <
stabilometer task between
12 = hyperactive .01) when methylphenidate was being used.
children with and without
ADIID and examined the effects 7.7-11.8 years
of Ritalin on children with genders not reported
ADHD performing a diagnostic framework not reported
stabilometer task stimulant medication taken

17 boys MANOVA The attention of boys with ADHD, when


Pelham et al. Examined the effects of
7.8-9.9 years medicated with methylphenidate, improved
(1990) methylphenidate on the attention
DSM-III-R significantly during baseball games (p < .001).
and baseball skills of boys with
Stimulant medications taken However, their baseball skills did not improve.
ADHD

Note. ADHO = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, ADHO -PI


= attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-predominantly inattentive
subtype, DCD = developmental coordination disorder, DCDQ =
subtype, ADHO-C = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-combined
, Crawford, Campbell, & Dewey, 2000), HKS = hyperkinetic
Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire (Wilson, Kaplan
disability, MABC = Movement Assessment Batter y for Children
disorders, RD = reading disability, LD = learning disability, ND = no
of Motor Proficiency (Bruininks, 1978), TGMD = Test of Gross
(Henderson & Sugden, 1992), BOTM P = Bruininks-Oseretsky Test
Motor Development (Ulrich, 1985)

....
\0
.......
Table 3
Physical Fitness

Study Purpose N Statistical Findings


~~---:--_ _ _ _ _--:------:-~----=-_--:---:---:-_ _ _-:-:-:-:--_ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _::-::Anc-:-alysis
Ballard Examined the effects of Ritalin on 50 children MANOVA, No differences in weight, height, and
(1977) the health-related fitness of 27 = ADIID ANOVA lean body mass between children
children with ADIID under rest, a 24 boys, 3 girls with ADIID and contraIs. Children
submaximal treadmill walk, and 23 = ND with ADIID had significantly more
recovery conditions 19 boys, 4 girls body fat (p < .02) and demonstrated
7-13 years increased heart rates and mean blood
diagnostic framework not pressure at rest (p < .01, .01)*,
reported exercise (p < .01, .03)*, and recovery
stimulant medication (p < .01, .003)* conditions. ~
"0

Boileau et al. Examined the effect of Ritalin on 20 children ANOVA Heart rates of children with ADIID [
(1977) the heart rates and oxygen 17 boys, 3 girls were significantly higher during rest x·
consumption (V02) of children 4-12 years (p < .01) and exercise (p < .01) when Q
with ADIID during rest and a diagnostic framework not reported compared to placebo conditions. V02
submaximal treadmill walk stimulant medication during exercise was significantly
condition for children with ADIID lower (p < .01) for the children in the
medication condition.
Harvey & Reid Described fundamental movement 19 children descriptive statistics, Children with ADIID performed
(1997) skills and fitness conditions of 17 boys, 2 girls graphs below the 25 th percentile compared to
children with ADIID 5-12 years the norms of a V02 Max test (Leger
DSM-III-R et al., 1984), shuttle fUn test
89% of sample took stimulant (CAHPER, 1980), and sit-up test
medication (Fitness Canada, 1985). Flexibility
and push-ups scores (Fitness Canada,
1985) were below the 40th percentile
of the respective nonns. Excess body
fat was found (75 th percentile) on a
skinfold test (Fitness Canada, 1985)
....
\0
Table Continues 00
Trocki-Ables, Described the effects of 3 different 5 boys single subject Visual inspection of the individual
French, & types of reinforcement on the ADHD methodology graphs (Time in seconds x Trials)
O'Connor (2001) amount of time spent to complete a 8-10 years descriptive, graphs revealed that boys with ADHD
1 mile/1.6 km walk/run test diagnostic framework not reported perfonned the walk/run test in less
medications were not reported time when provided bath tokens and
verbal praise rather than when
provided just tokens or verbal praise
alone.
Note. ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, ND = no disability, * = (p < heart rate, mean blood pressure)

-
\0
\0
200

AppendixH

Table 1 Assessment Instruments for Movement Behavior ofPersons with ADHD

Movement Performance Test Movement Performance Study

Movement Assessment Battery for Children Christiansen, 2000~ Kaplan et al., 1998~
(Henderson & Sugden, 1992) Miyahara et al., 1995, 2001~ Piek et al., 1999

Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency Beyer, 1999~ Doyle et al., 1995~ Kaplan et al.,
(Bruininks, 1978) 1998

Stabilometer Wade, 1976


(Wade & Newell, 1972)

Test of Gross Motor Development Harvey & Reid, 1997


(Ulrich, 1985)

Submaximal treadmill walk Ballard, 1977~ Boileau et al., 1977


(Ballard, 1977~ Boileau et al., 1977)

1 mile Il.6 km walk/run test Trocki-Ables et al., 2001


(Cooper Institute For Aerobies
Research, 1992)
201

AppendixI

Table 1

Perrormance criteria for TGMD-210comotor skills (Adapted from Ulrich, 2000)

Skill Perrormance Criteria

Run 1. Arms move in opposition to legs, elbows bent


2. Briefperiod where both feet are off the ground
3. Narrow foot placement landing on heel or toe (i.e., not flat footed)
Gallop 1. Arms bent and lifted to waist level at takeoff
2. A step forward with the lead foot followed by a step with the
trailing foot to a position adjacent to or behind the lead foot
3. Brief period when both feet are off the floor
4. Maintains a rhythmic pattern for four consecutive gallops
Hop 1. Nonsupport leg swings forward in a pendular fashion to
produce force
2. Foot ofnonsupport leg remains behind body
3. Arms flexed and swing forward to produce force
4. Takes off and lands three consecutive times on preferred foot
5. Takes off and lands three consecutive times on nonpreferred
foot
Leap 1. Take off on one foot and land on the opposite foot
2. A period where both feet are off the ground longer than
running
3. Forward reach with the arm opposite the lead foot
Horizontal lump 1. Preparatory movement includes flexion ofboth knees with
arms extended behind body
2. Arms extend forcefully forward and upward reaching full
extension above the head
3. Take offand land on both feet simultaneously
4. Arms are thrust downward during landing
Slide 1. Body turned sideways so shoulders are aligned with the line
on the floor
2. A step sideways with lead foot followed by a slide of the
trailing foot to a point next to the lead foot
3. A minimum of four continuous step-slide cycles to the right
4. A minimum offour continuous step-slide cycles to the left
202

Appendix J

Table 2

Performance criteria for TGMD-2 object control skills (Adapted from Ulrich, 2000)

Skill Performance Criteria

Striking a 1. Dominant hand grips bat ab ove nondominant hand


stationary baIl 2. Nonpreferred side ofbody faces the imaginary tosser with
feet parallel
3. Hip and shoulder rotation during swing
4. Transfers body weight to front foot
5. Bat contacts baIl
Stationary dribble 1. Contacts baIl with one hand at about belt level
2. Pushes baIl with fingertips (not a slap)
3. BalI contacts surface in front of or to the outside of foot on
preferred side
4. Maintains control of baIl for four consecutive bounces
without having to move the feet to retrieve it
Catch 1. Preparation phase where hands are in front of the body and
elbows are flexed
2. Arms extend while reaching for the baIl as it arrives
3. BalI is caught by hands only
Kick 1. Rapid continuous approach to the baIl
2. An elongated stride or leap immediately prior to baIl contact
3. Nonkicking foot placed even with or slightly in back of the
baIl
4. Kicks baIl with instep of preferred foot (shoelaces) or toe
Overhand throw 1. Windup is initiated with downward movement ofhand/arm
2. Rotates hip and shoulders to a point where the nonthrowing
side faces the wall
3. Weight is transferred by stepping with the foot opposite the
throwing hand
4. Follow-through beyond ball release diagonally across the
body toward the nonpreferred side
Underhand roll 1. Preferred hand swings down and back, reaching behind the
trunk while chest faces cones
2. Strides forward with foot opposite the preferred hand
towards the cones
3. Bends knees to lower body
4. Releases ball close to the floor sa baIl does not bounce more
than 4 inches high
203

Appendix K

Table 3

Reliability of the TGMD-2 (from Ulrich, 2000)

TGMD -2 Scores InternaI Consistency Stability Reliability Interrater Reliability

Locomotor Subtest .85 .88 .98

Object Control Subtest .88 .93 .98

Gross Motor .91 .96 .98


Development Quotient

Table 4

Means, standard deviations and effect sizes: GMDQ scores for participants with ADHD

Age (years) Medication Mean Standard deviation Effect size

6-9 off 66.1 13.0

on 67.0 14.8 .07

10-12 off 77.0 14.0

on 79.2 16.9 .16


204

Appendix L

Table 5

Means, standard deviations and effect sizes for the locomotor skills of participants with and

without ADHD

Age (years) Group Mean Standard deviation Effect size

6-9 ADHD (off med) 28.2 7.3


No-ADHD (trial 1) 41.1 2.5 2.36

ADHD (on med) 28.1 9.7


No-ADHD (trial 2) 43.9 2.0 2.26

10-12 ADHD (off med) 37.4 5.5


No-ADHD (trial 1) 44.8 2.1 1.60

ADHD (on med) 37.1 7.2


No-ADHD (trial 2) 46.0 1.7 1.70
205

AppendixM

Table 6

Means, standard deviations and effect sizes for the object control skills of participants with and

without ADHD

Age (years) Group Mean Standard deviation Effect size

6-9 ADHD (off med) 24.6 7.1


No-ADHD (trial 1) 39.4 4.2 2.54

ADHD (on med) 23.3 8.9


No-ADHD (trial 2) 38.6 4.5 2.17

10-12 ADHD (off med) 36.5 6.6


No-ADHD (trial 1) 44.3 2.5 1.56

ADHD (on med) 37.5 6.9


No-ADHD (trial 2) 44.9 2.5 1.43
206

AppendixN

1 ADHD (6- 9 years)

IADHD (10-12 years)


120
ëI No- ADHD (6- 9 years)
105.4

100 (!] No- ADHD (10-12 years)


en

~

0
()
en
CI
80
c
~
C)

60

40
1 Trials
2

Figure 1 GMDQ scores of children with and without ADHD


207

Appendix 0

Univariate results for GMDQ

Dependent Variable: condl (otTmedication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr > F

Model 3 9238.29156 3079.43052 23.95 <.0001

Error 40 5142.68571 128.56714

COITected Total 43 14380.97727

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE condl Mean

0.642397 12.95520 11.33875 87.52273

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr > F

gpid 1 8596.022727 8596.022727 66.86 <.0001


age 1 532.848701 532.848701 4.14 0.0484
gpid *age 1 109.420130 109.420130 0.85 0.3618

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr > F

gpid 1 8130.874675 8130.874675 63.24 <.0001


age 1 532.848701 532.848701 4.14 0.0484
gpid*age 1 109.420130 109.420130 0.85 0.3618

Dependent Variable: cond2 (on medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr > F

Model 3 10041.02922 3347.00974 18.53 <.0001

EITor 40 7224.85714 180.62143

COITected Total 43 17265.88636


208

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE cond2 Mean

0.581553 14.95927 13.43955 89.84091

Source DF TypeI SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 9280.022727 9280.022727 51.38 <.0001


age 1 570.157792 570.157792 3.16 0.0832
gpid*age 1 190.848701 190.848701 1.06 0.3102

Source DF Typeill SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 8979.757792 8979.757792 49.72 <.0001


age 1 570.157792 570.157792 3.16 0.0832
gpid*age 1 190.848701 190.848701 1.06 0.3102
209

AppendixP

Univariate results for locomotor and object control skills

Dependent Variable: locomotor slulls (off medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr > F

Model 3 1400.194805 466.731602 22.13 <.0001

Error 40 843.714286 21.092857

Corrected Total 43 2243.909091

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE locoffMean

0.623998 11.76243 4.592696 39.04545

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr > F

gpid 1 927.3636364 927.3636364 43.97 <.0001


age 1 397.9948052 397.9948052 18.87 <.0001
gpid*age 1 74.8363636 74.8363636 3.55 0.0669

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr > F

gpid 1 993.1090909 993.1090909 47.08 <.0001


age 1 397.9948052 397.9948052 18.87 <.0001
gpid*age 1 74.8363636 74.8363636 3.55 0.0669

Dependent Variable: locomotor slUlIs (on medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr > F

Model 3 1755.079654 585.026551 17.27 <.0001

Error 40 1354.647619 33.866190

Corrected Total 43 3109.727273

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE locon Mean

0.564384 14.63180 5.819467 39.77273


210

Source DF Type I SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 13 53.090909 1353.090909 39.95 <.0001


age 1 292.260606 292.260606 8.63 0.0055
gpid*age 1 109.728139 109.728139 3.24 0.0794

Source DF Typeill SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 1449.728139 1449.728139 42.81 <.0001


age 1 292.260606 292.260606 8.63 0.0055
gpid*age 1 109.728139 109.728139 3.24 0.0794

Dependent Variable: object control skills (otT medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Model 3 1894.632035 631.544012 23.05 <.0001

Error 40 1096.095238 27.402381

Corrected Total 43 2990.727273

R-Square CoeffVar RootMSE objcoffMean

0.633502 13.87518 5.234728 37.72727

Source DF TypeI SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 1100.000000 11 00. 000000 40.14 <.0001


age 1 673.527273 673.527273 24.58 <.0001
gpid*age 1 121.104762 121.104762 4.42 0.0419

Source DF Typeill SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 1217.832035 1217.832035 44.44 <.0001


age 1 673.527273 673.527273 24.58 <.0001
gpid*age 1 121.104762 121.104762 4.42 0.0419
211

Dependent Variable: object control siriUs (on medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Model 3 2242.985931 747.661977 22.06 <.0001

Error 40 1355.809524 33.895238

Corrected Total 43 3598.795455

R-Square CoeffVar RootMSE objconMean

0.623260 15.34849 5.821962 37.93182

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 1090.022727 1090.022727 32.16 <.0001


age 1 1007.066883 1007.066883 29.71 <.0001
gpid*age 1 145.896320 145.896320 4.30 0.0445

Source DF Typeill SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 1235.350866 1235.350866 36.45 <.0001


age 1 1007.066883 1007.066883 29.71 <.0001
gpid*age 1 145.896320 145.896320 4.30 0.0445
212

Appendix Q

Univariate results for individual locomotor skills

Dependent Variable: ron (off medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr > F

Mode} 3 21.52619048 7.17539683 4.89 0.0055

Error 40 58.72380952 1.46809524

Corrected Total 43 80.25000000

R-Square CoeffVar RootMSE runoffMean

0.268239 16.71241 1.211650 7.250000

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 14.20454545 14.20454545 9.68 0.0034


age 1 2.12142857 2.12142857 1.45 0.2364
gpid *age 1 5.20021645 5.20021645 3.54 0.0671

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 18.83658009 18.83658009 12.83 0.0009


age 1 2.12142857 2.12142857 1.45 0.2364
gpid*age 1 5.20021645 5.20021645 3.54 0.0671

Dependent Variable: ron (on medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr > F

Model 3 15.37207792 5.12402597 5.06 0.0046

Error 40 40.51428571 1.01285714

Corrected Total 43 55.88636364

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE runon Mean

0.275060 13.70958 1.006408 7.340909


213

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 10.02272727 10.02272727 9.90 0.0031


age 1 2.38636364 2.38636364 2.36 0.1327
gpid*age 1 2.96298701 2.96298701 2.93 0.0949

Source DF TypeIDSS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 12.78116883 12.78116883 12.62 0.0010


age 1 2.38636364 2.38636364 2.36 0.1327
gpid*age 1 2.96298701 2.96298701 2.93 0.0949

Dependent Variable: gallop (off medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Model 3 52.0329004 17.3443001 II.78 <.0001

Error 40 58.8761905 1.4719048

Corrected Total 43 110.9090909

R-Square CoeffVar RootMSE gallopofMean

0.469149 18.79638 1.213221 6.454545

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 40.09090909 40.09090909 27.24 <.0001


age 1 7.32813853 7.32813853 4.98 0.0313
gpid*age 1 4.61385281 4.61385281 3.13 0.0843

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F


gpid 1 44.61385281 44.61385281 30.31 <.0001
age 1 7.32813853 7.32813853 4.98 0.0313
gpid*age 1 4.61385281 4.61385281 3.13 0.0843
214

Dependent Variable: gallop (on medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Modet 3 82.4077922 27.4692641 16.10 <.0001

Error 40 68.2285714 1.7057143

Corrected Total 43 150.6363636

R-Square CoeffVar RootMSE gallopon Mean

0.547064 19.81563 1.306030 6.590909

Source DF TypeI SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 71.27272727 71.27272727 41.78 <.0001


age 1 4.12207792 4.12207792 2.42 0.1279
gpid*age 1 7.01298701 7.01298701 4.11 0.0493

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 77.92207792 77.92207792 45.68 <.0001


age 1 4.12207792 4.12207792 2.42 0.1279
gpid*age 1 7.01298701 7.01298701 4.11 0.0493

Dependent Variable: hop (otT medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Modet 3 128.6829004 42.8943001 20.80 <.0001

Error 40 82.4761905 2.0619048

Corrected Total 43 211.1590909

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE hopoffMean

0.609412 18.42014 l.435933 7.795455


215

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 79.11363636 79.11363636 38.37 <.0001


age 1 42.47813853 42.47813853 20.60 <.0001
gpid*age 1 7.09112554 7.09112554 3.44 0.0711

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 85.63658009 85.63658009 41.53 <.0001


age 1 42.47813853 42.47813853 20.60 <.0001
gpid*age 1 7.09112554 7.09112554 3.44 0.0711

Dependent Variable: hop (on Medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Model 3 97.7186147 32.5728716 11.82 <.0001

Error 40 110.1904762 2.7547619

Corrected Total 43 207.9090909

R-Square CoeffVar RootMSE hoponMean

0.470006 20.62963 1.659748 8.045455

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 71.27272727 71.27272727 25.87 <.0001


age 1 22.44242424 22.44242424 8.15 0.0068
gpid*age 1 4.00346320 4.00346320 1.45 0.2351

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 73.82164502 73.82164502 26.80 <.0001


age 1 22.44242424 22.44242424 8.15 0.0068
gpid*age 1 4.00346320 4.00346320 1.45 0.2351
216

Dependent Variable: leap (otT medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square FValue Pr>F
Model 3 10.34632035 3.44877345 4.26 0.0106
Error 40 32.38095238 0.80952381

Corrected Total 43 42.72727273

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE leapoffMean

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr > F

gpid 1 9.09090909 9.09090909 11.23 0.0018


age 1 1.06060606 1.06060606 1.31 0.2592
gpid*age 1 0.19480519 0.19480519 0.24 0.6264

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr> F

gpid 1 7.01298701 7.01298701 8.66 0.0054


age 1 1.06060606 1.06060606 1.31 0.2592
gpid*age 1 0.19480519 0.19480519 0.24 0.6264

Dependent Variable: leap (on medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr > F

Model 3 13.06385281 4.35461760 5.11 0.0044

Error 40 34.09523810 0.85238095

Corrected Total 43 47.15909091

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE leapon Mean

0.277017 19.62453 0.923245 4.704545

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr> F

gpid 1 10.02272727 10.02272727 11.76 0.0014


age 1 2.47813853 2.47813853 2.91 0.0959
gpid*age 1 0.56298701 0.56298701 0.66 0.4212
217

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr> F

gpid 1 10.38116883 10.38116883 12.18 0.0012


age 1 2.47813853 2.47813853 2.91 0.0959
gpid *age 1 0.56298701 0.56298701 0.66 0.4212

Dependent Variable: jump (otTmedication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr > F

Model 3 59.1937229 19.7312410 8.46 0.0002

Error 40 93.2380952 2.3309524

Corrected Total 43 152.4318182

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE jumpoffMean

0.388329 27.19709 l.526746 5.613636

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 38.20454545 38.20454545 16.39 0.0002


age 1 19.35086580 19.35086580 8.30 0.0063
gpid*age 1 l.63831169 l.63831169 0.70 0.4068

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 38.72922078 38.72922078 16.62 0.0002


age 1 19.35086580 19.35086580 8.30 0.0063
gpid*age 1 l.63831169 l.63831169 0.70 0.4068

Dependent Variable: jump (on medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Model 3 80.0508658 26.6836219 7.00 0.0007

Error 40 152.3809524 3.8095238

Corrected Total 43 232.4318182


218

R-Square CoetrVar RootMSE jumponMean

0.344406 33.15799 1.951800 5.886364

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 63.84090909 63.84090909 16.76 0.0002


age 1 16.13181818 16.13181818 4.23 0.0462
gpid*age 1 0.07813853 0.07813853 0.02 0.8868

Source DF Typeill SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 53.89632035 53.89632035 14.15 0.0005


age 1 16.13181818 16.13181818 4.23 0.0462
gpid*age 1 0.07813853 0.07813853 0.02 0.8868

Dependent Variable: slide (off medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Model 3 23.19307359 7.73102453 9.27 <.0001

Error 40 33.35238095 0.83380952

Corrected Total 43 56.54545455

R-Square CoetrVar RootMSE slideoffMean

0.410167 12.71449 0.913132 7.181818

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 5.81818182 5.81818182 6.98 0.0117


age 1 16.48831169 16.48831169 19.77 <.0001
gpid *age 1 0.88658009 0.88658009 1.06 0.3087

Source DF Typeill SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 6.70476190 6.70476190 8.04 0.0071


age 1 16.48831169 16.48831169 19.77 <.0001
gpid*age 1 0.88658009 0.88658009 1.06 0.3087
219

Dependent Variable: slide (on medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr > F

Model 3 26.3816017 8.7938672 2.60 0.0657

Error 40 135.5047619 3.3876190

Corrected Total 43 161.8863636

R-Square CoeffVar RootMSE slideon Mean

0.162964 25.70925 1.840549 7.159091

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 21.84090909 21.84090909 6.45 0.0151


age 1 . 4.06255411 4.06255411 1.20 0.2800
gpid*age 1 0.47813853 0.47813853 0.14 0.7091

Source DF Typeill SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

gpid 1 21.20541126 21.20541126 6.26 0.0165


age 1 4.06255411 4.06255411 1.20 0.2800
gpid*age 1 0.47813853 0.47813853 0.14 0.7091
220

AppendixR

Univariate results for individual object control skills

Dependent Variable: bat (otT medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Model 3 32.3556277 10.7852092 4.79 0.0061

Error 40 90.0761905 2.2519048

Corrected Total 43 122.4318182

R-Square CoeffVar RootMSE batoffMean

0.264275 18.49522 1.500635 8.113636

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

age 1 Il.75086580 11.75086580 5.22 0.0277


gpid 1 19.11363636 19.11363636 8.49 0.0058
gpid*age 1 1.49112554 1.49112554 0.66 0.4206

Source DF Typeill SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

age 1 11.75086580 11.75086580 5.22 0.0277


gpid 1 20.40021645 20.40021645 9.06 0.0045
gpid*age 1 1.49112554 1.49112554 0.66 0.4206

Dependent Variable: bat (on medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr > F

Model 3 37.6199134 12.5399711 5.48 0.0030

Error 40 91.5619048 2.2890476

Corrected Total 43 129.1818182

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE baton Mean


0.291217 19.23995 1.512960 7.863636
221

-- Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

age 1 20.80086580 20.80086580 9.09 0.0045


gpid 1 15.36363636 15.36363636 6.71 0.0133
gpid*age 1 1.45541126 1.45541126 0.64 0.4299

Source DF TypeIDSS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

age 1 20.80086580 20.80086580 9.09 0.0045


gpid 1 16.72813853 16.72813853 7.31 0.0100
gpid*age 1 1.45541126 1.45541126 0.64 0.4299

Dependent Variable: dribble (otT medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Model 3 108.0820346 36.0273449 18.27 <.0001

Error 40 78.8952381 1.9723810

Corrected Total 43 186.9772727

R-Square CoeffVar RootMSE driboffMean

0.578049 23.31859 1.404415 6.022727

Source DF TypeI SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

age 1 56.96298701 56.96298701 28.88 <.0001


gpid 1 42.02272727 42.02272727 21.31 <.0001
gpid*age 1 9.09632035 9.09632035 4.61 0.0379

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

age 1 56.96298701 56.96298701 28.88 <.0001


gpid 1 50.91450216 50.91450216 25.81 <.0001
gpid*age 1 9.09632035 9.09632035 4.61 0.0379
222

Dependent Variable: dribon

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Model 3 114.3627706 38.1209235 13.76 <.0001

Error 40 110.8190476 2.7704762

Corrected Total 43 225.1818182

R-Square CoeffVar RootMSE dribonMean

0.507869 27.12477 l.664475 6.136364

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

age 1 59.88658009 59.88658009 2l.62 <.0001


gpid 1 44.00000000 44.00000000 15.88 0.0003
gpid*age 1 10.47619048 10.47619048 3.78 0.0589

Source DF Typeffi SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

age 1 59.88658009 59.88658009 21.62 <.0001


gpid 1 54.11255411 54.11255411 19.53 <.0001
gpid*age 1 10.47619048 10.47619048 3.78 0.0589

Dependent Variable: catch otT

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Model 3 19.40670996 6.46890332 6.85 0.0008

Error 40 37.75238095 0.94380952

Corrected Total 43 57.15909091

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE catchoffMean

0.339521 18.66635 0.971499 5.204545


223

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr > F

age 1 8.23051948 8.23051948 8.72 0.0052


gpid 1 10.02272727 10.02272727 10.62 0.0023
gpid*age 1 1.15346320 1.15346320 1.22 0.2755

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr > F

age 1 8.23051948 8.23051948 8.72 0.0052


gpid 1 11.15346320 11.15346320 11.82 0.0014
gpid*age 1 1.15346320 1.15346320 1.22 0.2755

Dependent Variable: catchon

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F VaIue Pr > F

Model 3 18.68809524 6.22936508 9.04 0.0001

Error 40 27.56190476 0.68904762

Corrected Total 43 46.25000000

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE catchon Mean

0.404067 15.81122 0.830089 5.250000

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F VaIue Pr > F

age 1 16.36904762 16.36904762 23.76 <.0001


gpid 1 1.84090909 1.84090909 2.67 0.1100
gpid*age 1 0.47813853 0.47813853 0.69 0.4098

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F VaIue Pr>F


age 1 16.36904762 16.36904762 23.76 <.0001
gpid 1 2.29632035 2.29632035 3.33 0.0754
gpid*age 1 0.47813853 0.47813853 0.69 0.4098
224

Dependent Variable: kick (otT medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Model 3 35.65259740 11.88419913 8.62 0.0002

Error 40 55.14285714 1.37857143

Corrected Total 43 90.79545455

R-Square CoeffVar RootMSE kickoff Mean

0.392669 18.25496 1.174126 6.431818

Source DF TypeI SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

age 1 12.78116883 12.78116883 9.27 0.0041


gpid 1 21.84090909 21.84090909 15.84 0.0003
gpid*age 1 1.03051948 1.03051948 0.75 0.3924

Source DF Typeill SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

age 1 12.78116883 12.78116883 9.27 0.0041


gpid 1 22.30324675 22.30324675 16.18 0.0002
gpid*age 1 1.03051948 1.03051948 0.75 0.3924

Dependent Variable: kick (on medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Model 3 26.7125541 8.9041847 4.82 0.0059

Error 40 73.9238095 1.8480952

Corrected Total 43 100.6363636

R-Square CoeffVar RootMSE kickonMean

0.265436 20.62609 1.359447 6.590909


225

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr > F

age 1 9.00779221 9.00779221 4.87 0.0331


gpid 1 15.36363636 15.36363636 8.31 0.0063
gpid*age 1 2.34112554 2.34112554 1.27 0.2671

Source OF Type ID SS Mean Square FValue Pr>F


age 1 9.00779221 9.00779221 4.87 0.0331
gpid 1 17.70476190 17.70476190 9.58 0.0036
gpid*age 1 2.34112554 2.34112554 1.27 0.2671

Dependent Variable: throw (off medication)

Sumof
Source OF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr > F
Model 3 97.9573593 32.6524531 12.00 <.0001

Error 40 108.8380952 2.7209524

Corrected Total 43 206.7954545

R-Square Coeff Var Root MSE throwoffMean

0.4 73692 27.18328 1. 649531 6.068182

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr > F

age 1 37.63831169 37.63831169 13.83 0.0006


gpid 1 46.02272727 46.02272727 16.91 0.0002
gpid*age 1 14.29632035 14.29632035 5.25 0.0272

Source OF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr> F

age 1 37.63831169 37.63831169 13.83 0.0006


gpid 1 59.20541126 59.20541126 21. 76 <.0001
gpid*age 1 14.29632035 14.29632035 5.25 0.0272
226

Dependent Variable: throw (on medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Model 3 96.4352814 32.1450938 13.29 <.0001

Error 40 96.7238095 2.4180952

Corrected Total 43 193.1590909

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE throwon Mean

0.499253 24.70072 1.555023 6.295455

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr > F

age 1 34.45909091 34.45909091 14.25 0.0005


gpid 1 54.56818182 54.56818182 22.57 <.0001
gpid*age 1 7.40800866 7.40800866 3.06 0.0877

Source DF Typeill SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

age 1 34.45909091 34.45909091 14.25 0.0005


gpid 1 6l.95346320 6l.95346320 25.62 <.0001
gpid*age 1 7.40800866 7.40800866 3.06 0.0877

Dependent Variable: roll (off medication)

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr>F

Model 3 92.6309524 30.8769841 8.72 0.0001

Error 40 14l.6190476 3.5404762

Corrected Total 43 234.2500000

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE rolloffMean

0.395436 32.72374 l.881615 5.750000


227

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr> F

age 1 13.85476190 13.85476190 3.910.0548


gpid 1 73.84090909 73.84090909 20.86 <.0001
gpid*age 1 4.93528139 4.93528139 1.39 0.2447

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr > F

age 1 13.85476190 13.85476190 3.91 0.0548


gpid 1 77.66255411 77.66255411 21.94 <.0001
gpid*age 1 4.93528139 4.93528139 1.39 0.2447

Dependent Variable: rollon

Sumof
Source DF Squares Mean Square F Value Pr > F

Model 3 116.2138528 38.7379509 11.77 <.0001

Error 40 131.6952381 3.2923810

Corrected Total 43 247.9090909

R-Square CoeffVar Root MSE roUon Mean

0.468776 30.47238 1.814492 5.954545

Source DF Type 1 SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F


age 1 24.72813853 24.72813853 7.51 0.0091
gpid 1 87.36363636 87.36363636 26.54 <.0001
gpid*age 1 4.12207792 4.12207792 1.25 0.2698

Source DF Type ID SS Mean Square F Value Pr>F

age 1 24.72813853 24.72813853 7.51 0.0091


gpid 1 89.21298701 89.21298701 27.10 <.0001
gpid*age 1 4.12207792 4.12207792 1.25 0.2698
228

Appendix S

Run
-+--ADHD (6-12 yrs.)
(6.7, 7.8, 8.0)
___ No-ADHD (6-12 yrs.)
-i:r- Maximum Skill Crierion
10

Slide
(6.7,7.8,8.0) Gallop
(5.4, 7.6, 8.0)

Hop
Jump (6.7,9.2,10.0)
(4.7,6.7,8.0)

Leap
(4.3, 5.2, 6.0)

Figure 2 Locomotor skill profiles

Note. The numerals in the brackets indicate (avg. skill criteria for children with ADHD, avg.
skill criteria for children without ADHD, maximum skill criterion per skill)
229

Appendix T

-+--ADHD (6-12 yrs.)


Bat ---- No-ADHD (6-12 yrs.)
(7.4,8.6,10.0) ~ Maximum Skill Cr~erion

Dribble
(5.1, 7.4, 8.0)
Roll
(4.6, 7.3, 8.0)

Throw Catch
(5.2,7.3,8.0) (4.9, 5.6,6.0)

Kick
(5.9,7.2,8.0)

Figure 3 Object control skill profiles

Note. The numerals in the brackets indicate (avg. skill criteria for children with ADHD, avg.
skill criteria for children without ADHD, maximum skill criterion per skill)
230

AppendixU

Interview Guide

1 am now going to ask you about the times when you are playing alone or with your friends.

1. Which of the following activities have you played, at least once, within the last 12
months? (a check indicates a positive response) How mu ch do you like playing this
activity?

Q
"Really don't like it"
g
"It's okay"
g
Really like it a lot"

"Don 't Iike it" "Like it"

( ) Swings, slides, teeter-totters 1 2 3 4 5

( ) Bicycling 1 2 3 4 5

( ) Swimming 1 2 3 4 5

( ) Roller-blading 1 2 3 4 5

( ) Running or jogging 1 2 3 4 5

( ) Skateboarding 1 2 3 4 5

( ) Skating 1 2 3 4 5

( ) Hockey 1 2 3 4 5

( ) Football 1 2 3 4 5

( ) Basketball 1 2 3 4 5

( ) Soccer 1 2 3 4 5

( ) Baseball 1 2 3 4 5
231

( ) What other physical activity games have yOll played in the past 12 months?

1 2 3 4 5

2. Do yOll ever set IIp any ofthese games to play or practice with other children?

Not at aIl Not very often Often Very often Most of the time

1 2 3 4 5

2a. Have you ever tried to set up a game of physical activity?

(a) no (b) yes

2b. Ifyes, what games do you like to set up?

3. Do you like to play pick-up games with your friends?

Not at aIl Not very often Often Very often Most of the time

1 2 3 4 5

4. Do you playon any teams or in any organized sports?

(a) no (b) yes, which sports? List here:

Learning Context

1 am now going to ask you a few questions about how you have learned to play.

5. How much time do you spend in play each day from Monday to Friday?

( a) more than 1 hour (b) 1 hour (c) 30 minutes (d) 15 minutes (e) none

6. How much time do you spend in playon each day during the weekend?

(a) more than 1 hour (b) 1 hour (c) 30 minutes (d) 15 minutes (e) none

7. What is your special way of practicing activities or games on your own?

8. Which activity or games do you practice?

9. For each time that you play, how long do you practice each activity or game?

(a) more than 1 hour (b) 1 hour (c) 30 minutes (d) 15 minutes (e) none
232

10. Do you ever set up special targets to practice? For example, 1 sometimes set up a
glass on my office floor and try to putt a golf ball into it.

Not at all Not very often Often Very often Most of the time

1 2 3 4 5

Il. Do you ever use special words to remember how to practice any skills or moves?
For example, a special play in football is the "Dive 32" or the "switch" is a special
way of moving your hand in swimming the front crawl.

Not at all Not very often Often Very often Most of the time

1 2 3 4 5

12. Do you ever watch someone else play agame so that you can learn how to play
that game better?
Not at all Not very often Often Very often Most of the time

1 2 3 4 5

13. Do you take part in physical education class in school?

Not at all Not very often Often Very often Most of the time

1 2 3 4 5

14. Ifyou compare yourselfto the other children in physical education class, are your

movement and sport skills:

(a) Much worse (b) Not as good (c) Same (d) Better (e) Much better

Affective Questions

1 am now going to ask you a few questions about how you feel when you play.

15. What do you like the most about physical education class?

16. What do you like the least about physical education class?

17. How do you feel if the physical education teacher asks you to show a skill or move in front
ofto the whole class?
233

AppendixV

Table 1 Physical Activity Categories

Categories Properties Sample data units

Deliberate Play Solitary Play "1 play basketball myself'


Group Play "My friend plays with me"
No Play "1 don't play games"
Play Settings "1 bring my sister to the park to play"

Knowing About Doing Leisure Sk.ills "We take a spin on my brother' s bike"
Movement Sk.ills "We hit a ball with racquets"
Modeling "1 watch my brother play hockey"
Game Situations "1 throw to the left when 1 play quarterback
in a real game"
Personal Theories "The more you practice, the better you get"
"The puck goes over the net when 1 aim at
Self-Awareness that spot"

Personal Feelings Positive Affect "You always have fun during gym"
Negative Affect "1 really don't like that game"
Prosocial Behavior "They'll cheer you on"
Asocial Behavior "They always pick on me"
234

AppendixW

Table 2 TGMD-2 scores ofboys with and without ADHD

Participants Locomotor Scores Object Control Scores GMDO Scores

Raw Standard Descriptor Raw Standard Descriptor Total Descriptor

ADHD
Larry 42 9 A 32 3 VP 76 P
Gord 31 4 P 30 3 VP 61 VP
Claude 43 9 A 41 7 BA 88 BA
Randy 39 7 BA 47 12 A 97 A
Bob 38 7 BA 36 5 P 76 P
Billy 43 9 A 45 10 A 97 A

No-ADHD
Dale 47 13 AA 46 11 A 112 AA
Mike 48 13 AA 48 13 AA 118 AA
Fred 47 13 AA 44 10 A 109 A
Jim 40 8 A 47 12 A 100 A
Ted 45 11 A 44 10 A 103 A
Chad 47 13 AA 46 11 A 112 AA

Note. VP = Very Poor, P = Poor, BA = Below Average, A = Average,


AA = Above Average

Table 3 Deliberate Play

Property ADHD No-ADHD

Solitary Play 12 o
Group Play 59 47
No Play 3 4
Play Settings 77 94
235

AppendixX

Table 4 Knowing about Doing

Property ADHD No-ADHD

Leisure Skills 8 0
Movement Skills 128 159
General Models 14 10
Specifie Models 20 47
Game Situation 9 14
Personal Theories 14 15
Strategies & Self-Awareness 107 100

Table 5 Personal Feelings

Property ADHD No-ADHD

Positive Affect 57 52
Negative Affect 70 40
Prosocial Behavior 12 7
Asocial Behavior 43 28

Vous aimerez peut-être aussi